Skip to main content
Neurology logoLink to Neurology
. 2018 Apr 10;90(15):e1306–e1315. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000005291

Age-accelerated cognitive decline in asymptomatic adults with CSF β-amyloid

Lindsay R Clark 1,, Sara E Berman 1, Derek Norton 1, Rebecca L Koscik 1, Erin Jonaitis 1, Kaj Blennow 1, Barbara B Bendlin 1, Sanjay Asthana 1, Sterling C Johnson 1, Henrik Zetterberg 1, Cynthia M Carlsson 1
PMCID: PMC5894934  PMID: 29523644

Abstract

Objective

Compare cognitive and hippocampal volume trajectories in asymptomatic middle-aged and older adults with positive CSF markers of β-amyloid (Aβ) or tau to adults without an Alzheimer disease (AD)-associated biomarker profile.

Methods

Three hundred ninety-two adults enrolled in a longitudinal cohort study (Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention or Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center) completed a lumbar puncture and at least 2 biennial or annual neuropsychological evaluations. Cutoffs for Aβ42, total tau, and phosphorylated tau were developed via receiver operating characteristic curve analyses on a sample of 78 participants (38 dementia, 40 controls). These cutoffs were applied to a separate sample of 314 cognitively healthy adults (mean age at CSF collection = 61.5 years), and mixed-effects regression analyses tested linear and quadratic interactions of biomarker group × age at each visit on cognitive and hippocampal volume outcomes.

Results

Two hundred fifteen participants (69%) were biomarker negative (preclinical AD stage 0), 46 (15%) were Aβ+ only (preclinical AD stage 1), 25 (8%) were Aβ+ and tau+ (preclinical AD stage 2), and 28 (9%) were tau+ only. Both stage 1 and stage 2 groups exhibited greater rates of linear decline on story memory and processing speed measures, and nonlinear decline on list-learning and set-shifting measures compared to stage 0. The tau+ only group did not significantly differ from stage 0 in rates of cognitive decline.

Conclusion

In an asymptomatic at-risk cohort, elevated CSF Aβ (with or without elevated tau) was associated with greater rates of cognitive decline, with the specific pattern of decline varying across cognitive measures.


Although most studies of preclinical Alzheimer disease (AD) focus on older adults, recent studies report that middle-aged adults with CSF biomarkers of both β-amyloid (Aβ) and tau exhibit more rapid decline on cognitive and clinical measures than those with only one abnormal biomarker.1,2 These studies support guidelines defining preclinical AD as the presence of Aβ and neurodegeneration, while designating the presence of only one feature as “asymptomatic at-risk for AD.”3 However, prior studies examined change on cognitive composite scores or global screening measures and it remains unclear whether the presence of either Aβ or tau in isolation is associated with decline within specific cognitive domains, such as memory. In addition, although cutoff values defining normal or abnormal levels of Aβ and tau are useful clinically, examining relationships between biomarkers and clinical symptoms along a continuum may provide additional information.

Our analysis was designed to replicate and build on prior work by (1) identifying Aβ and tau positivity in a longitudinal cohort sample of cognitively healthy middle-aged and older adults, (2) comparing biomarker groups on longitudinal neuropsychological performance across multiple measures, and (3) investigating relationships among continuous variables of Aβ, tau, and cognitive performance. We hypothesized that adults with both Aβ and tau positivity would exhibit greater rates of cognitive decline compared to biomarker-negative adults. Based on prior work showing associations between Aβ and cognitive decline,4,5 we further hypothesized that those with Aβ+ would exhibit greater decline on memory measures, whereas tau+ adults would not differ from biomarker-negative adults.

Methods

Participants

Participants included 392 middle-aged or older community-dwelling adults enrolled in longitudinal cohort studies of Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention (WRAP)6 (n = 141) or the Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center (WADRC) clinical core (n = 251). These cohorts include cognitively healthy and impaired participants, are enriched for at-risk adults with a family history of AD, and undergo study evaluations on an annual or biennial basis. Cognitive status was determined by consensus conference panel based on National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer's Association criteria.7,8 The current study included participants with dementia in the development of CSF cutoff values, but included cognitively healthy middle-aged and older adults in all remaining analyses. Exclusion criteria consisted of only one study visit completed, relevant CSF or diagnosis data unavailable, diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or impaired–not MCI at baseline or lumbar puncture (LP) visit, or diagnosis of dementia that reverted to MCI at subsequent visits. Participants with incomplete neuropsychological data were included if data for at least 2 visits were available. Participants from the WRAP cohort were younger at baseline than those from the WADRC, but similar in sex distribution, education, and APOE genotype (table e-1, links.lww.com/WNL/A331).

Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents

The inclusion of human subjects in this study was approved by the University of Wisconsin–Madison institutional review board, and all participants provided informed consent.

Procedures

CSF was collected in the morning after a minimum 12-hour fast. A Sprotte spinal needle was inserted into the L3-4 or L4-5 vertebral interspace and 22 mL of CSF was removed via gentle extraction into polypropylene syringes. Within 30 minutes of collection, the CSF was combined, gently mixed, centrifuged to remove red blood cells or other debris, aliquoted into 0.5-mL polypropylene tubes, and stored at −80°C. Samples were sent in batches at 2 time points for analysis at the Clinical Neurochemistry Laboratory at the Sahlgrenska Academy of the University of Gothenburg, Sweden. All samples were analyzed according to protocols approved by the Swedish Board of Accreditation and Conformity Assessment using one batch of reagents (intraassay coefficients of variation <10%) for each batch. Board-certified laboratory technicians blinded to clinical diagnosis performed all analyses on one occasion for each of the 2 batches. CSF samples were assayed for total tau (t-tau), phosphorylated tau 181 (p-tau181), Aβ 1–42 (Aβ42), and Aβ 1–40 (Aβ40) using commercially available ELISA methods (INNOTEST assays, Fujirebio, Ghent, Belgium; Triplex assays, MSD Human Aβ Peptide Ultra-Sensitive Kit, Meso Scale Discovery, Gaithersburg, MD). Additional details on batch-to-batch conversions are provided in the supplemental material (links.lww.com/WNL/A332) and tables e-1 to e-5 (links.lww.com/WNL/A331).

A comprehensive neuropsychological assessment was completed at each visit. Measures of memory (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test [RAVLT] Total Trials 1–5 and Delayed Recall,9 Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised Logical Memory Story A [LM] Immediate and Delayed Recall10) and executive functioning (Trail Making Test Part B [TMT-B],11 Animal Fluency, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised Digit Symbol12) were included based on prior meta-analyses indicating that these cognitive domains demonstrate significant decline and associations with AD biomarkers in preclinical AD.1315 A subset of 205 participants completed at least 2 MRI scans and were included in secondary analyses of hippocampal volume (HCV) change (see supplemental material for MRI details, links.lww.com/WNL/A332).

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted in R version 3.3.1.16 Cutoff values for CSF assays were developed using receiver operating characteristic curve analysis in the pROC package (version 1.8)17 in 38 participants with clinical diagnosis of dementia due to AD based on National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer's Association criteria8 without reference to CSF biomarkers and 40 late middle-aged (48–64 years), stable, cognitively healthy adults at lower risk of AD (APOE ε4 noncarrier, no family history of AD). Youden J (sensitivity + specificity − 1), which maximizes both the sensitivity and specificity of a diagnostic test, was used.

To reduce potential risk of researcher assessment bias, a nonoverlapping sample of 314 cognitively healthy participants (mean LP age 61.5 years) were included in subsequent analyses. We compared biomarker groups on demographic characteristics using χ2 and analysis of variance tests. We compared mean neuropsychological performance and HCV among biomarker groups at the visit closest to the LP using analysis of covariance models with age at LP (mean = 61.5 years), sex (reference group = female), and years of education (mean = 16.3) as covariates. Comparisons of HCV also included total intracranial volume (mean = 1,464.8 mm3) as a covariate.

To test whether longitudinal change on the 7 neuropsychological measures and HCV varied across biomarker groups, linear mixed-effects models were conducted using the lme4 package version 1.1-12.18 Fixed effects included sex, years of education, practice effects (number of exposures to test19), biomarker group (4 levels), age (at each visit), and the interaction of age × biomarker group. To allow for acceleration of cognitive decline with increasing age, 2 quadratic terms, age2 and age2 × biomarker group, were included in all models and removed if not significant. To minimize collinearity in the linear and quadratic age terms, the age variable was centered on the sample mean. All models included random effects of intercept and slope nested within subject. The overall significance of the interaction term was assessed by likelihood ratio tests comparing the primary model and a model that did not include the interaction term. The p values for fixed-effect coefficients were calculated using asymptotic properties of the estimates.20 Statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05.

To investigate the relationship between cognitive or HCV change and continuous Aβ42 or tau values, we conducted 2 identical models to those above (excluding biomarker group terms). The first included predictors of Aβ42 (centered), p-tau (centered), age × Aβ42, age × p-tau, Aβ42 × p-tau, and age × Aβ42 × p-tau. The second model included effects of p-tau/Aβ42 and age × p-tau/Aβ42. Since t-tau was highly correlated with p-tau (r = 0.85, p < 0.001), we only included p-tau in these models.

Results

Biomarker cutoffs

Table 1 details sample characteristics. All biomarker cutoffs had a minimum sensitivity and specificity of 70% and 90%, respectively (table e-2, links.lww.com/WNL/A331). The ratios of tau to Aβ42 exhibited sensitivities and specificities ≥90% and greater area under the curve values than Aβ42 (p < 0.05), Aβ42/Aβ40 (p < 0.05), and p-tau (p < 0.01).

Table 1.

Sample characteristics

graphic file with name NEUROLOGY2017835660TT1.jpg

Characteristics of biomarker groups

Of the 314 cognitively healthy participants, 53 (17%) had a positive tau biomarker (either p-tau ≥59.5 [n = 40; 13%] or t-tau ≥461.26 [n = 42; 13%]) and 76 (24%) had a positive amyloid biomarker (either Aβ42[ln] ≤6.156 [back-transformed value = 471.54] [n = 44; 14%] or Aβ42/Aβ40 ≤0.09 [n = 67; 21%]).

The majority of participants were negative for both biomarkers of Aβ and tau (stage 0 = 68.5%): 14.6% were positive for Aβ only (stage 1), 8% were positive for tau only, and 8.9% were positive for both Aβ and tau (stage 2). Stages 0 and 1 did not differ on mean t-tau (p = 0.10) or p-tau (p = 0.41). Stage 0 had lower Aβ42 than the tau+ group (p < 0.001) but did not differ on the Aβ42/Aβ40 ratio (p = 0.97). The stage 2 group was the oldest and the stage 0 group was the youngest (p < 0.001). The stage 1 and 2 groups included greater proportions of APOE ε4 carriers (63% and 64%, respectively) compared with the stage 0 or tau+ groups (28% and 38%). There were no differences between biomarker groups in sex, years of education, family history of AD, or source cohort (table 2).

Table 2.

Biomarker group characteristics (n = 314)

graphic file with name NEUROLOGY2017835660TT2.jpg

Cognitive trajectories across biomarker groups

At the visit closest to the LP, there were no significant differences in cognitive performance or HCV across biomarker groups (table 2), with the exception of processing speed (Digit Symbol).

Longitudinal neuropsychological performance for each biomarker group is displayed in figure 1. Results from likelihood ratio tests (χ23) indicated that age2 × biomarker group accounted for a significant amount of variation in change on RAVLT Delay (χ2 = 9.74, p = 0.02) and similar but nonsignificant variation in change on RAVLT Total (χ2 = 7.11, p = 0.07) and TMT-B (χ2 = 6.89, p = 0.08). Compared to the stage 0 group, both stage 1 and 2 groups showed more rapid, nonlinear decline with age on the RAVLT Delay (p values <0.05), whereas the stage 2 group only showed more rapid, nonlinear decline on the RAVLT Total (p = 0.02). Compared to the stage 0 group, the stage 1 group showed more rapid, nonlinear change with age on TMT-B (p = 0.02). In contrast, the tau+ group did not significantly differ from the stage 0 group. Age2 × biomarker group was not significant for the remaining outcomes (p values >0.43). Model parameters are displayed in table 3.

Figure 1. Biomarker groups and cognitive trajectories.

Figure 1

Graphs depict neuropsychological performance on the y-axis for 6 cognitive measures (A–F) and age at each visit (centered on mean age) on the x-axis. Each line depicts the estimated slope for the 4 biomarker groups, adjusting for covariates of sex, education, and practice effects. Higher scores equate better performance on all measures except TMT-B (higher scores = worse performance). Quadratic terms were retained for the RAVLT and TMT-B. Nonsignificant quadratic terms were removed for other outcomes, and linear effects are depicted. Both the Aβ+ only group (orange) and the Aβ+/tau+ group (green) exhibited significantly greater decline than the biomarker-negative group (black). In contrast, the group with only tau+ (blue) did not differ from biomarker-negative individuals. Aβ = β-amyloid; RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; TMT-B = Trail Making Test Part B.

Table 3.

Parameter estimates from linear mixed-effects models

graphic file with name NEUROLOGY2017835660TT3.jpg

For the remaining outcomes (in which the quadratic term was not associated with cognitive performance), results from likelihood ratio tests (χ23) indicated that the interaction between age × biomarker group accounted for a significant amount of variation in change on LM Immediate (χ2 = 11.74, p < 0.01), LM Delay (χ2 = 12.77, p < 0.01), and Digit Symbol (χ2 = 13.21, p < 0.01). For all 3 outcomes, stages 1 and 2 exhibited greater age-related decline than stage 0 (p < 0.05). In contrast, the tau+ group did not differ from the stage 0 group in rates of cognitive change. Age-related change in HCV did not differ by biomarker group. Sensitivity analyses conducted on WRAP and ADRC cohorts separately revealed similar directions of effects, but slight heterogeneity in magnitude of β-weights possibly because of baseline age differences across cohorts (supplemental material, links.lww.com/WNL/A332).

Cognitive trajectories and continuous CSF values

There were no significant interactions among Aβ42 × p-tau × age2; this term was removed from subsequent analyses. The 3-way interaction between Aβ42 × p-tau × age was statistically significant for LM Delay (B = 0.01, p = 0.03), in which the relationship between p-tau and longitudinal story memory performance was dependent on Aβ42. Similar to the results above, 2-way interactions between age2 × Aβ42 were significant for RAVLT Delay (age2: B = −0.004, p < 0.01; age2 × Aβ42: B = 0.01, p = 0.03), RAVLT Total (age2: B = −0.01, p = 0.05; age2 × Aβ42: B = 0.03, p < 0.01), and TMT-B (age2: B = 0.0002, p < 0.001; age2 × Aβ42: B = −0.0004, p = 0.03), indicating that lower CSF Aβ42 (higher brain amyloid) was associated with greater nonlinear decline. For outcomes for which age2 × Aβ42 was not significant, greater amyloid burden was associated with greater linear decline (significant age × Aβ42 interaction) for LM Immediate (B = 0.22, p = 0.001), LM Delay (B = 0.22, p < 0.01), Digit Symbol (B = 0.48, p < 0.01), and Animal Fluency (B = 0.34, p < 0.01). In contrast, there were no interactions between age (linear or quadratic) × p-tau (figure 2).

Figure 2. Relationships among Aβ42, tau, and longitudinal verbal memory performance.

Figure 2

Two-way interaction between age at each visit and Aβ42 (A) or p-tau (B) on memory performance. Figures depict that although performance generally decreases with age, those with low Aβ42 (high brain amyloid) exhibit most rapid decline, whereas the association between age at each visit and memory performance does not vary by p-tau. Facets depict biomarker level by quartile (1 = lowest quartile [0%–25%], 2 = 25%–50%, 3 = 50%–75%, 4 = highest quartile [75%–100%]). Aβ42 = β-amyloid 1–42; p-tau = phosphorylated tau.

Age2 × p-tau/Aβ42 was significant for TMT-B (B = 0.004, p < 0.01) and marginal for RAVLT Delay (B = −0.1, p = 0.08). Age × p-tau/Aβ42 was significant for all other outcomes with the exception of HCV, indicating that elevated AD biomarkers were associated with greater decline on RAVLT Total (B = −3.6, p < 0.01), LM Immediate (B = −2.3, p < 0.001), LM Delay (B = −2.3, p < 0.001), Digit Symbol (B = −3.0, p = 0.02), and Animal Fluency (B = −2.6, p < 0.01).

Discussion

In 314 cognitively healthy, middle-aged and older adults enriched for AD risk, approximately one-third were positive for CSF biomarkers of AD (Aβ or tau). Those with Aβ positivity (with or without tau positivity) exhibited significantly greater decline on neuropsychological measures than biomarker-negative adults, whereas those with only tau positivity did not differ from biomarker-negative adults.

These results have potentially important implications pertaining to AD during the asymptomatic or preclinical period. First, 24% of the sample was Aβ positive and 17% was tau positive using the selected biomarker threshold at relatively young ages of 59.3 and 59.6 for the Aβ-only and tau-only groups, respectively, and 65.9 for the Aβ and tau positive group. While the age of the latter group was significantly older than other groups, the ages were overall quite young and empirically support the hypothesis21 that AD neuropathology changes begin well in advance of MCI and dementia syndromes.

Second, elevated Aβ in the absence of tau was associated with cognitive decline in late middle-age. This is an important finding because it adds to the debate on whether Aβ or tau more strongly contributes to early symptoms of cognitive decline. Although emerging evidence indicates that elevated Aβ on a PET scan is associated with increased risk of cognitive decline,4,5,22 simultaneous measures of tau have not always been available, and therefore it is unclear whether results from prior studies are attributable to elevated Aβ alone or elevated Aβ and tau. Neuropathology studies demonstrating correlations between patterns of cognitive impairment in older adults with dementia and regional distribution of neurofibrillary tangle development23,24 suggest that tau distribution drives major cognitive symptoms. However, the current results suggest that elevated Aβ independent of tau in late middle-age is associated with cognitive decline. Decline in this context was significant but mild (e.g., using our regression results, we estimate that 5-year decline on the RAVLT Total from age 61.5 to age 66.5 for the Aβ-only group would be 3.2 points compared to 1.6 points for the biomarker-negative group), and few individuals declined to a cognitively impaired diagnosis during the visits included in this study (e.g., only 4 participants declined from cognitively normal to MCI at the most recent visit). This finding in the context of the literature suggests that Aβ may be associated with subtle decline in midlife, whereas tau may contribute to more pronounced clinical symptoms as the disease progresses.

Third, the pattern of decline with age and Aβ varied across cognitive measures. Prior investigations of preclinical biomarker stage and longitudinal cognition in late middle-age have examined change on a global cognitive screener2 or composite score1; current results suggest examination of multiple cognitive domains may be useful in parsing out subtle patterns of decline related to Aβ. Specifically, performance on story memory and processing speed measures declined linearly with age and Aβ burden, whereas nonlinear decline on list-learning and set-shifting tasks indicated faster rates of decline on these measures with advancing age in the presence of Aβ burden. These results have potentially important implications for choosing appropriate outcome measures in clinical trials. For example, if a trial is enrolling older adults, it may be more optimal to choose a list-learning memory measure since it would be expected to decline more rapidly in older adults with AD pathology. Moreover, our results suggest that a neuropsychological measure of processing speed and working memory (Digit Symbol) may be a very early predictor of decline as this was the only cognitive measure that distinguished biomarker groups cross-sectionally at the biomarker visit. This is consistent with a prior study in a separate middle-aged cohort, which reported that baseline performance on Digit Symbol and 3 additional measures best predicted conversion from cognitively normal to cognitively impaired.25 Lastly, results across the majority of models including continuous CSF markers were similar to those using a group variable based on cutoffs (e.g., lower CSF Aβ42 was associated with worsening performance, whereas elevated tau was not). This finding suggests that dichotomizing continuous biomarker variables does not result in significant loss of information.

In the context of the recently proposed amyloid/tau/neurodegeneration (A/T/N) biomarker classification system,26 our findings suggest that those characterized as A+/T− exhibit similar decline to those characterized as A+/T+ in late middle-age. However, we have not yet fully examined neurodegeneration. Total and phosphorylated tau were incorporated into the tau positivity classification and as they are highly correlated in this sample (r = 0.85, p < 0.001), it was not feasible to disambiguate neurodegeneration from neurofibrillary tau in this analysis. Furthermore, we did not observe differences among biomarker groups in HCV, unlike a prior study.27 It is possible these differences are attributable to the younger age of our cohort, who may not be expected to show structural brain changes at this stage, or that incorporation of additional structural imaging markers (e.g., cortical thickness) is needed to provide additional sensitivity and specificity to early neurodegeneration in AD.

Based on prior meta-analyses of cognitive decline in preclinical AD,14 we focused on episodic memory and executive functioning measures; however, different patterns may be observed in other domains such as visuospatial function. It should be noted that factors that may be unrelated to AD can contribute to poor performance on cognitive tests (e.g., depression, sleep disorders, cerebrovascular disease), and continued longitudinal observation will be needed to parse the effects due to slowly evolving Aβ and tau pathology vs other explanations. Future analyses should examine additional differences between Aβ+ and Aβ− asymptomatic adults to determine whether other factors (e.g., vascular risk factor burden) exacerbate decline in Aβ+ asymptomatic adults. An important limitation was inclusion of only CSF AD biomarkers, and future analyses will incorporate CSF and molecular neuroimaging biomarkers to provide greater reliability in classification of preclinical AD. Our sample contained a smaller proportion of adults with markers of only tau+ (8%) compared to other studies (11%–23%), perhaps because of the younger mean age of our cohort, the method by which we defined the cutoffs, or the relatively small sample from which the cutoffs were derived. These results are based on longitudinal cohorts that include a majority of Caucasian, highly educated adults from the Midwest region of the United States and may be less generalizable to other populations.

Acknowledgment

The authors gratefully acknowledge the assistance of researchers and staff at the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention and Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center for assistance in recruitment and data collection. Most importantly, the authors thank the dedicated WRAP and WADRC participants for their continued support and participation in this research.

Glossary

β-amyloid

AD

Alzheimer disease

HCV

hippocampal volume

LM

Logical Memory

LP

lumbar puncture

MCI

mild cognitive impairment

p-tau

phosphorylated tau

RAVLT

Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test

TMT-B

Trail Making Test Part B

t-tau

total tau

WADRC

Wisconsin Alzheimer's Disease Research Center

WRAP

Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention

Author contributions

Lindsay Clark: drafting/revising the manuscript, study concept or design, analysis or interpretation of data, statistical analysis. Sara Berman: drafting/revising the manuscript, study concept or design, analysis or interpretation of data, statistical analysis. Derek Norton: analysis or interpretation of data, statistical analysis. Rebecca Koscik: drafting/revising the manuscript, analysis or interpretation of data, statistical analysis. Erin Jonaitis: drafting/revising the manuscript, analysis or interpretation of data, statistical analysis. Kaj Blennow: analysis or interpretation of data, contribution of vital reagents/tools/patents. Barbara Bendlin: analysis or interpretation of data, obtaining funding. Sanjay Asthana: study concept or design, study supervision or coordination, obtaining funding. Sterling Johnson: drafting/revising the manuscript, study concept or design, analysis or interpretation of data, study supervision or coordination, obtaining funding. Henrik Zetterberg: analysis or interpretation of data, contribution of vital reagents/tools/patents. Cynthia Carlsson: study concept or design, analysis or interpretation of data, acquisition of data, study supervision or coordination.

Study funding

The project described was supported by NIH grants UL1TR00427, ADRC P50 AG033514 (S.A.), R01 AG027161 (S.C.J.), R01 AG021155 (S.C.J.), R01AG037639 (B.B.B.), F30 AG054115 (S.E.B.), T32 GM007507 (S.E.B.), and T32 GM008692 (S.E.B.).

Disclosure

L. Clark, S. Berman, D. Norton, R. Koscik, and E. Jonaitis report no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. K. Blennow holds the Torsten Söderberg Professorship in Medicine. B. Bendlin, S. Asthana, and S. Johnson report no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. H. Zetterberg is a Wallenberg Academy Fellow. C. Carlsson is a site principal investigator for a project jointly funded by the NIH and Lilly (A4 study). Go to Neurology.org/N for full disclosures.

References

  • 1.Soldan A, Pettigrew C, Cai Q, et al. Hypothetical preclinical Alzheimer disease groups and longitudinal cognitive change. JAMA Neurol 2016;73:698–705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Vos SJB, Gordon BA, Su Y, et al. NIA-AA staging of preclinical Alzheimer disease: discordance and concordance of CSF and imaging biomarkers. Neurobiol Aging 2016;44:1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Dubois B, Hampel H, Feldman HH, et al. Preclinical Alzheimer's disease: definition, natural history, and diagnostic criteria. Alzheimers Dement 2016;12:292–323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Clark LR, Racine AM, Koscik RL, et al. Beta-amyloid and cognitive decline in late middle age: findings from the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention Study. Alzheimers Dement 2016;12:805–814. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lim YY, Maruff P, Pietrzak RH, et al. Effect of amyloid on memory and non-memory decline from preclinical to clinical Alzheimer's disease. Brain 2014;137:221–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Johnson SC, Koscik RL, Jonaitis EM, et al. The Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention: a review of findings and current directions. Alzheimers Dement 2017;10:130–142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement 2011;7:270–279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement 2011;7:263–269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Schmidt M. Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test: A Handbook. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised. New York: The Psychological Corp., Harcourt Brace Jovanovich; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Reitan RM, Wolfson D. The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Therapy and Clinical Interpretation. Tucson: Neuropsychology Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Backman L, Jones S, Berger AK, Laukka EJ, Small BJ. Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer's disease: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 2005;19:520–531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hedden T, Oh H, Younger AP, Patel TA. Meta-analysis of amyloid-cognition relations in cognitively normal older adults. Neurology 2013;80:1341–1348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Duke Han S, Nguyen CP, Stricker NH, Nation DA. Detectable neuropsychological differences in early preclinical Alzheimer's disease: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev 2017;27:326–327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing [computer program]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Robin X, Turck N, Hainard A, et al. pROC: an open-source package for R and S+ to analyze and compare ROC curves. BMC Bioinformatics 2011;12:77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 2015;67:1–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Jonaitis EM, Koscik RL, La Rue A, Johnson SC, Hermann B, Sager MA. Aging, practice effects, and genetic risk in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention. Clin Neuropsychol 2015;29:426–441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bates DM, Pinheiro JC. Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-Plus. New York: Springer; 2000:81–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Jack CR Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, et al. Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer's disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol 2013;12:207–216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Donohue MC, Sperling RA, Petersen R, et al. Association between elevated brain amyloid and subsequent cognitive decline among cognitively normal persons. JAMA 2017;317:2305–2316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Braak H, Braak E. Frequency of stages of Alzheimer-related lesions in different age categories. Neurobiol Aging 1997;18:351–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Nelson PT, Alafuzoff I, Bigio EH, et al. Correlation of Alzheimer disease neuropathologic changes with cognitive status: a review of the literature. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2012;71:362–381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Albert M, Soldan A, Gottesman R, et al. Cognitive changes preceding clinical symptom onset of mild cognitive impairment and relationship to ApoE genotype. Curr Alzheimer Res 2014;11:773–784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Jack CR Jr, Bennett DA, Blennow K, et al. A/T/N: an unbiased descriptive classification scheme for Alzheimer disease biomarkers. Neurology 2016;87:539–547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Gordon BA, Blazey T, Su Y, et al. Longitudinal beta-amyloid deposition and hippocampal volume in preclinical Alzheimer disease and suspected non-Alzheimer disease pathophysiology. JAMA Neurol 2016;73:1192–1200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Neurology are provided here courtesy of American Academy of Neurology

RESOURCES