Abstract
Background
Negative control exposure studies are increasingly being used in epidemiological studies to strengthen causal inference regarding an exposure-outcome association when unobserved confounding is thought to be present. Negative control exposure studies contrast the magnitude of association of the negative control, which has no causal effect on the outcome but is associated with the unmeasured confounders in the same way as the exposure, with the magnitude of the association of the exposure with the outcome. A markedly larger effect of the exposure on the outcome than the negative control on the outcome strengthens inference that the exposure has a causal effect on the outcome.
Methods
We investigate the effect of measurement error in the exposure and negative control variables on the results obtained from a negative control exposure study. We do this in models with continuous and binary exposure and negative control variables using analysis of the bias of the estimated coefficients and Monte Carlo simulations.
Results
Our results show that measurement error in either the exposure or negative control variables can bias the estimated results from the negative control exposure study.
Conclusions
Measurement error is common in the variables used in epidemiological studies; these results show that negative control exposure studies cannot be used to precisely determine the size of the effect of the exposure variable, or adequately adjust for unobserved confounding; however, they can be used as part of a body of evidence to aid inference as to whether a causal effect of the exposure on the outcome is present.
Keywords: Negative control exposure, measurement error, unobserved confounding
Key Messages
Negative control exposure studies can contribute to a triangulation of evidence to estimate the effect of an exposure on an outcome in the presence of suspected unobserved confounding.
In the presence of measurement error, negative control exposure studies do not give a reliable estimate of the causal effect of the exposure on the outcome.
Negative control exposure studies should not be used in effect estimate calibration to obtain an estimate of the causal effect of an exposure on an outcome.
Introduction
In the presence of unobserved confounding, the causal effect of an exposure on an outcome of interest cannot simply be determined by regressing the outcome on the exposure. A method that has often been used to detect confounding and help assessment of whether a causal relationship exists between an exposure and an outcome is a negative control exposure study.1,2 Negative control exposure studies compare the association between an exposure of interest and an outcome with the association between a control variable and the same outcome. The control variable is chosen to be a variable that has no effect on the outcome of interest but is subject to the same unobserved confounding as the exposure of interest. Therefore, any association observed between the negative control and the outcome will be due to confounding in the model.3 If the association observed between the exposure of interest and the outcome is markedly larger than the association between the negative control and the outcome, then this can add to the evidence that the exposure of interest does have a causal effect on the outcome, and can feed into a triangulation of evidence on the causal effect of exposure on the outcome from a wide range of sources.4–6 If triangulation of the results from studies that suffer from different types of potential bias point to the same relationship between the exposure and the outcome, then this provides evidence for a causal association between the exposure and outcome. Fuller discussion of the use of negative controls in epidemiology is available elsewhere.1,2,4,7–13
One area where negative controls have often been used in epidemiology is to determine the effect of intrauterine exposure on later outcomes by comparing the association of a maternal exposure during pregnancy with the outcome of interest, with the association of the paternal exposure with the same outcome.2,7,14 If an intrauterine effect of the mother’s exposure on the child is present, the association of the maternal exposure with the outcome is expected to be larger than the association of the equivalent paternal exposure. Examples of studies where this type of negative control has been used include the effect of maternal and paternal smoking on offspring outcomes,7,15–25 the effect of maternal and paternal body mass index (BMI) on later offspring BMI,26–33 the effect of maternal and paternal diabetes on predisposition to diabetes in offspring,34 the effect of maternal and paternal energy intake on later offspring dietary intake35 and the effect of maternal and paternal BMI on offspring autism spectrum disorder.36
Another example of where negative control exposure studies have been used to evaluate the effect of intrauterine exposure, without using the paternal exposure as a control, is examining the association of mothers taking folic acid supplements in pregnancy compared with the negative control of taking other supplements, with autism37 and language development delays38 in their children. Other examples include the association of maternal smoking during pregnancy compared with maternal smoking after pregnancy, with offspring respiratory outcomes,39 the effect of maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy compared with the negative control of maternal alcohol consumption before pregnancy, on offspring ADHD symptoms40 and the effect of exposure to air pollution before, during and after pregnancy, on autism spectrum disorder in offspring.41 A list of examples of negative control exposure studies is given in Table 1.
Table 1.
Exposure | Negative control exposure | Outcome(s) |
---|---|---|
Maternal smoking | Paternal smoking | Offspring outcomes: Inattention/hyperactivity15,20 Obesity/adiposity16,22–24 |
Blood pressure17 | ||
Gestational diabetes21 | ||
ADHD symptoms19 | ||
Cognitive development18 | ||
Offspring psychotic symptoms46 | ||
Maternal psychosocial stress | Paternal psychosocial stress | Offspring vascular function54 |
Maternal smoking during pregnancy | Maternal smoking after pregnancy | Offspring respiratory outcomes39 |
Offspring psychotic symptoms46 | ||
Maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy | Maternal alcohol consumption before pregnancy | Offspring ADHD symptoms40 |
Maternal BMI/obesity | Paternal BMI | Offspring BMI/adiposity26–33 |
Offspring cognitive and psychomotor development55 | ||
Length of pre-birth inter-pregnancy interval | Length of post-birth inter-pregnancy interval | Risk of schizophrenia in the offspring56 |
Folic acid supplements in pregnancy | Other supplements in pregnancy | Autism spectrum disorders37 |
Language development delays38 | ||
Prescription for trimethoprim 1–3 months before pregnancy | Prescription for trimethoprim 13–15 months before pregnancy | Offspring congenital malformation57 |
Air pollutant exposure during pregnancy | Air pollutant exposure before and after pregnancy | Offspring autism spectrum disorder41 |
Exposure to childhood infections | Hospital attendance for broken bones | Multiple sclerosis later in life58 |
Adherence to prescribed statins and beta blockers | Adherence to other prescribed medication | Long-term mortality after acute myocardial infarction59 |
Vaccination during flu season | Vaccination outside flu season | Mortality and hospitalization from flu60 |
Swimmers’ exposure to bacteria in water | Non-swimmers | Gastrointestinal illnesses after an increase in bacteria levels in water61 |
That negative controls should be used more routinely in epidemiological studies has been suggested,1,8 and adjustment of the effect or P-value of the exposure on the outcome for the negative control has been suggested.42–45 A number of the studies described in Table 1 adjust the estimated effect of the exposure for the negative control in order to attempt to account for any unobserved confounding in the model.23,25,31,34,36,39,46 As discussed further below, adjusting for the effect of the negative control is not the correct interpretation of negative control exposure studies, as any measurement error in either the exposure or negative control will affect the results obtained from the analysis. The results obtained from the negative control exposure study should instead contribute to a triangulation of evidence from a range of sources which are subject to different sources of potential bias, and feed into the overall result.1,2,11
One key assumption made in negative control studies is that the relationship between the exposure and outcome and the control and outcome are subject to the same confounding. However, in order to obtain a consistent estimate of the effect of the exposure on the outcome, we also need to make the additional assumption that there is no differential measurement error in either the exposure or the negative control. Measurement error in an explanatory variable in a linear regression will lead to a biased estimate of the association between that variable and the outcome.47,48 Therefore, if there is measurement error in either the exposure or the negative control, the estimated coefficients from the regression of the outcome on the exposure and the negative control will be biased estimates of the true association between these variables and the outcome.
It is likely that the exposure and control variables may be subject to different levels of measurement error. For example, a negative control for the intrauterine effect of a mother’s exposure on their child’s outcome, that is often used, is the father’s exposure. However, the father’s data are likely to suffer from a higher level of measurement error than the mother’s if they are collected from the mother (as they often are), as the data will be less accurately recalled. This potential difference in measurement error is important; if there is a higher level of measurement error in the negative control than in the exposure variable, then the association between the negative control and the outcome may appear to be weak or null even when unmeasured confounding factors that relate to both the exposure and the negative control are present.49 In the remainder of this paper, we evaluate the effect of adjusting for a negative control on the identification of a causal effect of the exposure on an outcome when measurement error is present. We adjust for the negative control by examining the difference between the expected value of the coefficient for the exposure and the coefficient for the negative control, in a regression of the outcome on the exposure and the negative control.
Methods
We examine the effect of measurement error on the interpretation of the estimated coefficients when the association between an exposure and an outcome is interrogated in a negative control exposure study. We consider two examples of negative control exposure studies: first, a model with continuous outcome, exposure and negative control variables; and second, a model with a continuous outcome variable and binary exposure and negative control variables. These models reflect two different scenarios which are frequently found in epidemiological studies, and where negative control exposure methods have previously been used. In each setting, the causal relationship between the exposure, the negative control and the outcome are set up as given in Figure 1. This relationship can be written as:
is the true value of the exposure of interest for individual , is the true value of the negative control, is an unmeasured confounder that is correlated with both the exposure and the control and is the outcome. is a normally distributed random error term with mean 0.
Continuous exposure and control variables
The exposure and negative control and are continuous variables measured with error:
where is the observed value of the exposure for individual , measured with error, is the observed value of the negative control for individual , measured with error, and and are normally distributed random error terms with variance and , respectively. Throughout we assume that this measurement error is uncorrelated with the true values of the exposure and negative control. We additionally make the assumption that and are only correlated through the unmeasured confounders ; therefore . In the context of maternal and paternal comparison studies, this assumption implies no assortative mating on the basis of the exposure or control variables. Throughout the analysis, and are set to 0.4, meaning that = 0.16. The values of and are determined according to the desired values of the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC), the proportion of total variance in the observed variable that is due to true variation:
is the variance of the true variable and is the variance of the measurement error. An ICC value of 1 indicates that there is no measurement error in the model and , whereas an ICC value of 0 would indicate that all of the variation in the observed variable was due to the measurement error and . We derive the bias of the ordinary least squares (OLS) estimator of and when the exposure and control are included in the same model. This bias is the difference between the expected value of the estimator and the true value of the parameter. The measurement error in the model will lead to regression dilution bias and so bias the estimated coefficients towards the null; however, the additional presence of an unmeasured confounder that is correlated with the exposure and negative control variables means that the total bias could be towards or away from the null.
In matrix form, the true model considered can be written as:
where , , and and are vectors including all individuals for variables and , respectively. The measurement error can be defined as:
where and V , and and are vectors including all individuals for variables , and , respectively. We make the following assumptions about the distribution of the error terms:
and the asymptotic distribution of the variables is given by:
As the confounders are unobserved, the regression to be estimated is:
(1) |
In matrix form this can be written as:
In this case it can be shown that the bias of the OLS estimator of is given by:
(2) |
The derivation of this is given in Appendix 1, available as Supplementary data at IJE online.
Using this equation for the bias, we calculate the bias of and the bias of the difference between the coefficients for the exposure and the negative control, i.e. , with and without measurement error in each of the exposure and the negative control and for a range of values of, the effect of the unmeasured confounder on the outcome.
Binary exposure and control variables
We then consider a model where the exposure and control are both binary variables which take values of 0 or 1. For this set-up, we conducted Monte Carlo simulations to examine the difference between the estimated association and the true effect for 1, and for , for different levels of measurement error.
In this scenario, the true values of the exposure and the control, and , were dichotomized into binary variables by classing those observations with the highest 20% of values as 1 and the rest as 0. Measurement error was introduced in this model by reclassifying a proportion of the true values for the exposure and the control, to take the opposite value. This proportion was changed in order to change the level of the measurement error in the model, but was applied equally to the ‘true’ (1) and ‘false’ (0) values. Other than this change, the model was set up in the same way as in the continuous case, and the true relationship between the binary exposure and control variables and the outcome variable is given by:
As before, the outcome variable in this model is a continuous variable and so the model is estimated using OLS. Simulations were conducted for two scenarios: where neither the exposure or the control have a true effect on the outcome; and where the exposure has an effect on the outcome and the negative control has no effect on the outcome. In each of these scenarios, different levels of measurement error in each of the exposure and the control were considered: no measurement error; a low level of measurement error where 10% of the observations are misclassified; and a high level of measurement error where 50% of the observations are misclassified.
Results
Continuous exposure and control variables
The results for the bias for different levels of measurement error and effect of the unmeasured confounder are given in Figures 2 and 3. These figures show that in all scenarios where the unmeasured confounder has an effect on the outcome, the estimate of is not equal to the true effect and the inclusion of the negative control in the model does not remove this difference. These figures also show that when there is no measurement erro, (Figure 2A and Figure 3A), then the estimation of the exposure adjusted for the negative control correctly estimates the causal effect of the exposure on the outcome. However, when there is measurement error, then this estimate is only unbiased when both the measurement error and the effect size are the same for both the exposure and the negative control, as shown in Figure 2D.
These results show that the difference between the estimated effect of the exposure and the estimated effect of the negative control is not equal to the true difference in the causal effect of the two variables, except under a strong set of assumptions. Therefore, we cannot reliably estimate the size of the effect of the exposure on the outcome, when there is measurement error, by looking at the difference in the association between the exposure and the outcome and the negative control and the outcome. Figure 2B and Figure 2C show that differing measurement error in the exposure and the control can lead to different sizes of estimated coefficients for and , even when neither of the variables has a direct effect on the outcome.
Binary exposure and control variables
We ran Monte Carlo simulations with a binary exposure and binary control and a continuous outcome variable, for different levels of association between the variables and for different proportions of the exposure and the control misclassified. Table 2 shows the results for a scenario where there is no true association of either the exposure or the negative control with the outcome. Table 3 shows the results where the exposure has a positive effect on the outcome and the negative control has no causal effect on the outcome.
Table 2.
Error in exposure | Error in negative control | Bias for | Bias for | Bias for |
---|---|---|---|---|
None (0%) | None (0%) | 0.140 | 0.140 | 0.000 |
None (0%) | Low (10%) | 0.140 | 0.093 | 0.046 |
None (0%) | High (50%) | 0.140 | 0.000 | 0.140 |
Low (10%) | None (0%) | 0.093 | 0.140 | −0.047 |
Low (10%) | Low (10%) | 0.093 | 0.093 | 0.000 |
Low (10%) | High (50%) | 0.093 | 0.000 | 0.093 |
High (50%) | None (0%) | 0.000 | 0.140 | −0.140 |
High (50%) | Low (10%) | 0.000 | 0.093 | −0.094 |
High (50%) | High (50%) | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 |
Bias in the estimated values of , and ) when the exposure and negative control variables are binary and the outcome is continuous. Measurement error is the proportion of observations misclassified: . Effect of the unmeasured confounder, : = = 0.4.
Table 3.
Error in exposure | Error in negative control | Bias for | Bias for | Bias for |
---|---|---|---|---|
None (0%) | None (0%) | 0.140 | 0.157 | −0.017 |
None (0%) | Low (10%) | 0.140 | 0.104 | 0.035 |
None (0%) | High (50%) | 0.140 | 0.000 | 0.140 |
Low (10%) | None (0%) | 0.026 | 0.157 | −0.131 |
Low (10%) | Low (10%) | 0.026 | 0.104 | −0.078 |
Low (10%) | High (50%) | 0.026 | 0.000 | 0.026 |
High (50%) | None (0%) | −0.200 | 0.157 | −0.357 |
High (50%) | Low (10%) | −0.200 | 0.104 | −0.305 |
High (50%) | High (50%) | −0.200 | 0.000 | −0.200 |
Bias in the estimated values of , and ) when the exposure and negative control variables are binary and the outcome is continuous. Measurement error is the proportion of observations misclassified: . Effect of the unmeasured confounder, : = = 0.4.
The results here show the same pattern as the analytical results for the continuous example and show that, in all of the scenarios considered, the effect of is only unbiased when the effects of the exposure and of the negative control on the outcome are the same, and either there is no measurement error in the model or the measurement errors for each of the exposure and the negative control are the same. These results additionally show that when the level of misclassification is high, is always zero, leading to a large difference between and when is large but no difference when is zero.
Discussion
Due to the unmeasured confounding that is inherent in studies in which it is necessary to use a negative control, the estimates of regression coefficients are always expected to reflect the confounded association rather than the causal relationship. In the analysis above, we have shown that measurement error in the exposure and negative control will add a bias which may increase or decrease the difference between the estimated coefficient and the causal relationship. The implication of this is that adjusting the estimated effect of the exposure for the estimated effect of the negative control variable, as suggested as a way to account for the bias created by confounding42–45 and implemented in a number of studies described in Table 1,23,25,31,34,36,39,46 does not necessarily improve our estimates of the associations between the exposure and the outcome. This indicates that the results from a negative control study cannot be used to estimate the size of a causal effect directly, but instead can feed into a triangulation of evidence which is subject to different sources of bias, to strengthen evidence regarding whether or not a causal effect of the exposure on the outcome is present.6,13 In cases where measurement error in the exposure and negative control variables is likely to be minimal, such as where they are germline genetic variants, the total error in the estimate of the effect of the exposure adjusted for the negative control will be small. However, this will only be the case if it is known that the measurement error in both the exposure and negative control is minimal, and so will only apply to a very small proportion of studies. Control outcome calibration is a method which has been proposed to estimate the size of an effect of an exposure in a negative control outcome study, even when the outcome of interest and control outcome do not have the same scale,50–52 and has been suggested for negative control exposure studies.50 The results given here show that any calibration based on the results from a negative control study should be used with caution. The results presented here also show that such calibration methods will not transfer directly to models with a negative control exposure variable as measurement error in either the exposure, or the negative control will distort the obtained results.
Throughout this analysis we have assumed that there are no measured confounders in the model. The inclusion of measured confounders that were not subject to any measurement error would not change the results in the analysis above. However, as has been discussed previously,53 the addition of measured confounders to the model, which are also measured with error, will lead to a more complex bias in the estimated effects of the exposure on the outcome. The addition of such variables, however, would not change the overall conclusion that caution should be used when interpreting results from negative control studies, as any additional confounders will add to the potential for the results obtained to be very different from the true effect of the exposure on the outcome. We have also assumed no direct correlation between the true values of the exposure and control; however, relaxing this assumption will also not change the conclusions that can be drawn from the results.
The results presented above show that negative controls can be useful in contributing to the triangulation of evidence regarding whether or not a causal relationship is likely to exist between an exposure and outcome of interest.7 However, negative control exposure studies should not be used to obtain a point estimate of the causal effect of the exposure of interest on the outcome as, in the presence of measurement error in either the exposure or the control, this effect estimate will also be subject to bias. The results we have found mean we cannot give a general statement about the direction of any bias caused by measurement error in a negative control exposure study.
Supplementary Data
Supplementary data are available at IJE online.
Funding
This work was supported by the Medical Research Council and the University of Bristol [MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit (IEU) MC_UU_12013/1 and MC_UU_12013/9].
Conflict of interest: None declared.
Supplementary Material
References
- 1. Lipsitch M, Tchetgen Tchetgen E, Cohen T. Negative controls: a tool for detecting confounding and bias in observational studies. Epidemiology 2010;21:383–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Davey Smith G. Negative control exposures in epidemiological studies. Epidemiology 2012;23:350–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Lipsitch M, Tchetgen Tchetgen E, Cohen T. Negative control exposures in epidemiologic studies. Epidemiology 2012;23:351–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Dusetzina SB, Brookhart MA, Maciejewski ML. Control outcomes and exposures for improving internal validity of nonrandomized studies. Health Serv Res 2015;50:143–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Brookhart MA, Patrick AR, Shrank WH, Dormuth CR. Validating studies of adherence through the use of control outcomes and exposures. Am J Hypertens 2010;23:110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Lawlor DA, Tilling K, Davey Smith G. Triangulation in aetiological epidemiology. Int J Epidemiol 2016;45:1866–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Davey Smith G. Assessing intrauterine influences on offspring health outcomes: can epidemiological studies yield robust findings? Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol. 2008;102:245–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Arnold BF, Ercumen A, Benjamin-Chung J, Colford JM. Brief report: negative controls to detect selection bias and measurement bias in epidemiologic studies. Epidemiology 2016;27:637–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Arnold BF, Ercumen A. Negative control outcomes: a tool to detect bias in randomized trials. JAMA 2016;316:2597–98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Lipsitch M, Jha A, Simonsen L. Observational studies and the difficult quest for causality: lessons from vaccine effectiveness and impact studies. Int J Epidemiol 2016;45(6):2060–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Lawlor DA. The Society for Social Medicine John Pemberton Lecture 2011. Developmental overnutrition—an old hypothesis with new importance? Int J Epidemiol 2013;42:7–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Richmond RC, Al-Amin A, Davey Smith G, Relton CL. Approaches for drawing causal inferences from epidemiological birth cohorts: A review. Early Hum Dev 2014;90:769–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Gage SH, Munafò MR, Davey Smith G. Causal inference in Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD) research. Annu Rev Psychol 2016;67:567–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Keyes KM, Davey Smith G, Susser E. Commentary: Smoking in pregnancy and offspring health: early insights into family-based and ‘negative control’ studies? Int J Epidemiol 2014;43:1381–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Keyes KM, Davey Smith G, Susser E. Associations of prenatal maternal smoking with offspring hyperactivity: causal or confounded? Psychol Med 2014;44:857–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Leary SD, Davey Smith G, Rogers IS, Reilly JJ, Wells JC, Ness AR. Smoking during pregnancy and offspring fat and lean mass in childhood. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2006;14:2284–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Brion MJ, Leary SD, Davey Smith G, Ness AR. Similar associations of parental prenatal smoking suggest child blood pressure is not influenced by intrauterine effects. Hypertension 2007;49:1422–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Julvez J, Ribas-Fitó N, Torrent M, Forns M, Garcia-Esteban R, Sunyer J. Maternal smoking habits and cognitive development of children at age 4 years in a population-based birth cohort. Int J Epidemiol 2007;36:825–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Langley K, Heron J, Davey Smith G, Thapar A. Maternal and paternal smoking during pregnancy and risk of ADHD symptoms in offspring: testing for intrauterine effects. Am J Epidemiol 2012;176:261–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Kovess V, Keyes KM, Hamilton A, et al. Maternal smoking and offspring inattention and hyperactivity: results from a cross-national European survey. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2014;24:919–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Bao W, Michels KB, Tobias DK, et al. Parental smoking during pregnancy and the risk of gestational diabetes in the daughter. Int J Epidemiol 2016;45:160–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Durmuş B, Kruithof CJ, Gillman MH, et al. Parental smoking during pregnancy, early growth, and risk of obesity in preschool children: the Generation R Study. Am J Clin Nutr 2011;94:164–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. von Kries R, Bolte G, Baghi L, Toschke AM; GME Study Group. Parental smoking and childhood obesity—is maternal smoking in pregnancy the critical exposure? Int J Epidemiol 2008;37:210–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Howe LD, Matijasevich A, Tilling K, et al. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring trajectories of height and adiposity: comparing maternal and paternal associations. Int J Epidemiol 2012;41:722–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Matijasevich A, Brion M-J, Menezes AM, Barros AJ, Santos IS, Barros FC. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring growth in childhood: 1993 and 2004 Pelotas cohort studies. Arch Dis Child 2011;96:519–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Lawlor DA, Davey Smith G, O’Callaghan M, et al. Epidemiologic evidence for the fetal overnutrition hypothesis: findings from the mater-university study of pregnancy and its outcomes. Am J Epidemiol 2007;165:418–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Lawlor DA, Timpson NJ, Harbord RM, et al. Exploring the developmental overnutrition hypothesis using parental-offspring associations and FTO as an instrumental variable. PLoS Med 2008;5:e33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Sharp GC, Lawlor DA, Richmond RC, et al. Maternal pre-pregnancy BMI and gestational weight gain, offspring DNA methylation and later offspring adiposity: findings from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol 2015;44:1288–304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Davey Smith G, Steer C, Leary S, Ness A. Is there an intrauterine influence on obesity? Evidence from parent child associations in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Arch Dis Child 2007;92:876–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Kivimaki M, Lawlor DA, Davey Smith G, et al. Substantial intergenerational increases in body mass index are not explained by the fetal overnutrition hypothesis: the Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. Am J Clin Nutr 2007;86:1509–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Fleten C, Nystad W, Stigum H, et al. Parent-offspring body mass index associations in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study: a family-based approach to studying the role of the intrauterine environment in childhood adiposity. Am J Epidemiol 2012;176:83–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Patel R, Martin RM, Kramer MS, et al. Familial associations of adiposity: findings from a cross-sectional study of 12, 181 parental-offspring trios from Belarus. PloS One 2011;6:e14607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Vik KL, Romundstad P, Carslake D, Davey Smith G, Nilsen TI. Comparison of father-offspring and mother-offspring associations of cardiovascular risk factors: family linkage within the population-based HUNT Study, Norway. Int J Epidemiol 2014;43:760–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Sobngwi E, Boudou P, Mauvais-Jarvis F, et al. Effect of a diabetic environment in utero on predisposition to type 2 diabetes. Lancet 2003;361:1861–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Brion MJ, Ness AR, Rogers I, et al. Maternal macronutrient and energy intakes in pregnancy and offspring intake at 10 y: exploring parental comparisons and prenatal effects. Am J Clin Nutr 2010;91:748–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Gardner RM, Lee BK, Magnusson C, et al. Maternal body mass index during early pregnancy, gestational weight gain, and risk of autism spectrum disorders: Results from a Swedish total population and discordant sibling study. Int J Epidemiol 2015;44:870–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Suren P, Roth C, Bresnahan M, et al. Association between maternal use of folic acid supplements and risk of autism spectrum disorders in children. JAMA 2013;309:570–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Roth C, Magnus P, Schjolberg S, et al. Folic acid supplements in pregnancy and severe language delay in children. JAMA 2011;306:1566–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Haberg SE, Stigum H, Nystad W, Nafstad P. Effects of pre- and postnatal exposure to parental smoking on early childhood respiratory health. Am J Epidemiol 2007;166:679–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Eilertsen EM, Gjerde LC, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, et al. Maternal alcohol use during pregnancy and offspring attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a prospective sibling control study. Int J Epidemiol 2018;46:1633–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Raz R, Roberts AL, Lyall K, et al. Autism spectrum disorder and particulate matter air pollution before, during, and after pregnancy: a nested case-control analysis within the Nurses’ Health Study II Cohort. Environ Health Perspect. 2015;123:264–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Schuemie MJ, Ryan PB, DuMouchel W, Suchard MA, Madigan D. Interpreting observational studies: why empirical calibration is needed to correct p‐values. Stat Med 2014;33:209–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Ryan PB, Stang PE, Overhage JM, et al. A comparison of the empirical performance of methods for a risk identification system. Drug Saf 2013;36:143–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Flanders WD, Strickland MJ, Klein M. A new method for partial correction of residual confounding in time-series and other observational studies. Am J Epidemiol 2017;185:941–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Miao W, Tchetgen Tchetgen E. Invited commentary: bias attenuation and identification of causal effects with multiple negative controls. Am J Epidemiol 2017;185:950–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Zammit S, Thomas K, Thompson A, et al. Maternal tobacco, cannabis and alcohol use during pregnancy and risk of adolescent psychotic symptoms in offspring. Br J Psychiatry 2009;195:294–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Greenland S. The effect of misclassification in the presence of covariates. Am J Epidemiol 1980;112:564–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Hernán MA, Cole SR. Invited commentary: causal diagrams and measurement bias. Am J Epidemiol 2009;170:959–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Weisskopf MG, Tchetgen EJT, Raz R. Commentary: On the use of imperfect negative control exposures in epidemiologic studies. Epidemiology 2016;27:365–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Tchetgen ET. The control outcome calibration approach for causal inference with unobserved confounding. Am J Epidemiol 2014;179:633–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Tchetgen Tchetgen EJ, Sofer T, Richardson D. Negative outcome control for unobserved confounding under a Cox proportional hazards model. Working Paper Series 192. Department of Biostatistics, Harvard University, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 52. Richardson DB, Laurier D, Schubauer-Berigan MK, Tchetgen ET, Cole SR. Assessment and indirect adjustment for confounding by smoking in cohort studies using relative hazards models. Am J Epidemiol 2014;180:933–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Fewell Z, Davey Smith G, Sterne JA. The impact of residual and unmeasured confounding in epidemiologic studies: a simulation study. Am J Epidemiol 2007;166:646–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. van Dijk AE, Dawe K, Deanfield J, et al. The association of maternal prenatal psychosocial stress with vascular function in the child at age 10–11 years: findings from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents And Children. Eur J Prev Cardiol 2014;21:1097–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Casas M, Chatzi L, Carsin A-E, et al. Maternal pre-pregnancy overweight and obesity, and child neuropsychological development: two Southern European birth cohort studies. Int J Epidemiol 2013;42:506–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Gunawardana L, Davey Smith G, Zammit S, et al. Pre-conception inter-pregnancy interval and risk of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 2011;199:338–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Sun Y, Wu CS, Olsen J. Trimethoprim use before pregnancy and risk of congenital malformation: reanalyzed using a case-crossover design and a case-time-control design. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf 2014;23:1076–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Zaadstra BM, Chorus AM, van Buuren S, Kalsbeek H, van Noort JM. Selective association of multiple sclerosis with infectious mononucleosis. Mult Scler 2008;14:307–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Rasmussen JN, Chong A, Alter DA. Relationship between adherence to evidence-based pharmacotherapy and long-term mortality after acute myocardial infarction. JAMA 2007;297:177–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Jackson LA, Jackson ML, Nelson JC, Neuzil KM, Weiss NS. Evidence of bias in estimates of influenza vaccine effectiveness in seniors. Int J Epidemiol 2006;35:337–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Colford JM Jr, Schiff KC, Griffith JF, et al. Using rapid indicators for Enterococcus to assess the risk of illness after exposure to urban runoff contaminated marine water. Water Res 2012;46:2176–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.