Can the brain repurpose neural resources originally developed to support hand function for the control of artificial limbs? By studying individuals with congenital or acquired hand-loss using functional MRI, van den Heiligenberg et al. show that prosthesis usage shapes brain activity and connectivity. Neural resources can be repurposed to support artificial limbs.
Keywords: plasticity, body representation, reorganization, neuroimaging, motor control
Abstract
The human brain contains multiple hand-selective areas, in both the sensorimotor and visual systems. Could our brain repurpose neural resources, originally developed for supporting hand function, to represent and control artificial limbs? We studied individuals with congenital or acquired hand-loss (hereafter one-handers) using functional MRI. We show that the more one-handers use an artificial limb (prosthesis) in their everyday life, the stronger visual hand-selective areas in the lateral occipitotemporal cortex respond to prosthesis images. This was found even when one-handers were presented with images of active prostheses that share the functionality of the hand but not necessarily its visual features (e.g. a ‘hook’ prosthesis). Further, we show that daily prosthesis usage determines large-scale inter-network communication across hand-selective areas. This was demonstrated by increased resting state functional connectivity between visual and sensorimotor hand-selective areas, proportional to the intensiveness of everyday prosthesis usage. Further analysis revealed a 3-fold coupling between prosthesis activity, visuomotor connectivity and usage, suggesting a possible role for the motor system in shaping use-dependent representation in visual hand-selective areas, and/or vice versa. Moreover, able-bodied control participants who routinely observe prosthesis usage (albeit less intensively than the prosthesis users) showed significantly weaker associations between degree of prosthesis observation and visual cortex activity or connectivity. Together, our findings suggest that altered daily motor behaviour facilitates prosthesis-related visual processing and shapes communication across hand-selective areas. This neurophysiological substrate for prosthesis embodiment may inspire rehabilitation approaches to improve usage of existing substitutionary devices and aid implementation of future assistive and augmentative technologies.
Introduction
Our hands are the primary tool of the brain, and the loss of a hand leads to profound changes in individuals’ abilities to interact with their environment (Makin et al., 2013a; Hahamy et al., 2017). Since brain organization is thought to be shaped by experience (Ejaz et al., 2015), real-life constraints on behaviour, such as hand loss, should provide a powerful driver for brain reorganization (Pons et al., 1991; Flor et al., 2006; Makin et al., 2015). It has recently been suggested that the profound reorganization observed following hand loss possibly occurs to accommodate changes in individuals’ abilities to interact with their environment in daily life (Makin et al., 2013a; Hahamy et al., 2015, 2017). Specifically, it has been suggested that the territory of the missing limb could be reappropriated to support the representation of other body parts that substitute the missing hand function as a compensatory strategy. A key strategy for adapting to hand loss is using an artificial limb (hereafter ‘prosthesis’). Prosthesis usage strongly depends on both motor control and visual information, particularly considering the limited somatosensory inputs from the artificial limb.
It is well established that within the primary somatosensory and motor cortex (SI/M1, respectively) specific areas show strong selectivity for inputs and outputs relating to the hand (Penfield and Rasmussen, 1950; Kaas et al., 1979). It has been demonstrated that similar hand-selectivity also exists in the visual system. Specifically, areas in the lateral occipitotemporal cortex show visual selectivity for upper limbs compared to other body parts (Orlov et al., 2010) or object categories (Bracci et al., 2010, 2012). The functional relationship between hand representations in the sensorimotor and visual systems is still unknown (Tal et al., 2016). In recent years, evidence has been accumulating to demonstrate that visual hand-selective areas are involved in action perception and cognition (Gallivan and Culham, 2015; Lingnau and Downing, 2015; Downing and Peelen, 2016). However, it is still unclear to what extent representation in this area is informed by personal daily-life motor experience.
Here we examined individuals with hand-loss to determine how alternative motor strategies through prosthesis usage affect functioning of—and coupling between—visual and sensorimotor hand-selective areas. Participants (hereafter ‘one-handers’; Table 1) with either unilateral acquired arm amputation (n = 16) or unilateral congenital maldevelopment of the hand (n = 16) with varying degrees of prosthesis usage underwent a functional MRI session. The functional MRI session comprised task-based scans in which the participants viewed images of different categories (e.g. upper limbs, prosthetic arms) and a resting state scan.
Table 1.
Subject | Gender | Age | Deprivation age | Level of amputation | Missing hand side | Cause | Usage frequency (MAL) | Usage time | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cosmetic | Mechanical | Myoelectric | ||||||||
PA01 | M | 57 | 20 | Below elbow | Left | Trauma | 0.57 | 5 | 0 | 0 |
PA02 | F | 49 | 0 | Below elbow | Left | Congenital | 0.46 | 4 | 0 | 0 |
PA03 | M | 59 | 40 | Above elbow | Left | Trauma | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
PA04 | F | 52 | 0 | Below elbow | Right | Congenital | 0.15 | 5 | 1 | 0 |
PA05a | M | 58 | 27 | Above elbow | Left | Trauma | 0.09 | 5 | 2 | 0 |
PA06 | M | 53 | 28 | Below elbow | Left | Trauma | 0.24 | 3 | 5 | 0 |
PA07 | M | 52 | 0 | At wrist | Left | Congenital | 0.04 | 0 | 3 | 0 |
PA08 | M | 41 | 27 | Above elbow | Right | Trauma | 0.09 | 2 | 1 | 0 |
PA09 | M | 48 | 17 | Above elbow | Left | Trauma | 0 | 2 | 2 | 0 |
PA10 | F | 25 | 0 | At wrist | Right | Congenital | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
PA11 | M | 49 | 0 | Above elbow | Left | Congenital | 0.26 | 1 | 4 | 0 |
PA12 | M | 37 | 27 | Above elbow | Left | Trauma | 0.28 | 0 | 2 | 0 |
PA13 | F | 46 | 38 | Below elbow | Left | Trauma | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
PA14 | F | 28 | 0 | At wrist | Left | Congenital | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
PA15 | M | 64 | 33 | Below elbow | Right | Trauma | 0.33 | 0 | 2 | 5 |
PA16 | M | 38 | 0 | Below elbow | Left | Congenital | 0.39 | 5 | 0 | 0 |
PA17 | F | 24 | 18 | Below elbow | Right | Trauma | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
PA18 | F | 27 | 0 | Below elbow | Left | Congenital | 0.54 | 5 | 0 | 0 |
PA19 | M | 49 | 37 | Above elbow | Left | Trauma | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 |
PA20 | M | 60 | 0 | At wrist | Left | Congenital | 0.06 | 2 | 0 | 0 |
PA21 | F | 34 | 0 | Below elbow | Right | Congenital | 0.46 | 5 | 0 | 0 |
PA22 | F | 36 | 0 | Below elbow | Right | Congenital | 0.57 | 5 | 0 | 0 |
PA23 | F | 50 | 45 | Above elbow | Left | Tumour | 0 | 0 | 2 | 0 |
PA24 | F | 41 | 0 | Below elbow | Left | Congenital | 0.54 | 0 | 0 | 5 |
PA25 | M | 29 | 24 | Through shoulder | Left | Trauma | 0.09 | 0 | 0 | 2 |
PA27 | M | 25 | 0 | Below elbow | Left | Congenital | 0.59 | 1 | 0 | 5 |
PA28 | M | 34 | 0 | At wrist | Left | Congenital | 0.11 | 0 | 0 | 3 |
PA29 | M | 25 | 18 | At wrist | Left | Trauma | 0 | 0 | 2 | 0 |
PA30 | M | 38 | 0 | Below elbow | Left | Congenital | 0 | 0 | 2 | 1 |
PA31 | F | 49 | 0 | At wrist | Left | Congenital | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 |
PA32 | M | 45 | 20 | Below elbow | Right | Trauma | 0.09 | 2 | 0 | 0 |
PA33 | M | 32 | 31 | Above elbow | Left | Trauma | 0 | 0 | 2 | 0 |
aThis participant was excluded from the functional MRI analysis.
M/F = male/female; MAL = Motor Activity Log scores: how frequently one-handers use their prosthesis in an inventory of 27 daily activities (e.g. taking money out of wallet etc.). Scores of 0 = never, 1 = sometimes, 2 = very often. The sum of all items was divided by the highest possible score, such that individuals were rated on a scale ranging from 0 to 1. Prosthesis usage time relates to wear time, 1–5: the scale for prosthesis usage time: 0 = never; 1 = rarely; 2 = occasionally; 3 = daily, <4 h; 4 = daily, 4–8 h, 5 = daily, >8 h.
We hypothesized that reappropriation of hand-selective cortical resources depends on successful implementation of the prosthesis in daily life to substitute the missing hand’s function. Therefore, we predicted that more frequent habitual prosthesis usage (which strongly depends on visual feedback; Antfolk et al., 2013) would increase processing for prostheses in typically hand-selective visual areas and increase cross-talk between visual and sensorimotor hand-selective areas. To investigate the role of visual experience further, we presented participants with images of both the prosthesis belonging to them (i.e. highly familiar), and images of a prosthesis exemplar belonging to another one-hander, unfamiliar to the observer. This allowed us to determine whether the representation of prosthetic limbs depends on specific experience, or rather more general categorical representation. Finally, we tested 24 able-bodied controls with varying degrees of passive visual exposure to prosthesis usage (Supplementary Table 1). We predicted that the degree to which individuals are passively exposed to prostheses usage should not scale with functioning of—and coupling between—visual and sensorimotor hand-selective areas.
Upper limb prosthetic limbs are broadly classified in two subcategories: active prostheses (affording adjustment of grip), and passive cosmetic prostheses. Active prostheses include: (i) mechanical prostheses, typically having little visual similarity to a hand and operated via the opposite shoulder to adjust grip size; and (ii) myoelectric prostheses, affording adjustment of grip based on ipsilateral arm-muscles while also having a visual appearance resembling a hand. Passive prostheses (hereafter ‘cosmetic’) are typically designed to resemble in visual appearance the human hand and arm but are not operational. Nevertheless, it is important to emphasize that passive prostheses can greatly enhance daily lives functionality, and are in fact preferred by the majority of amputees for daily functioning (Jang et al., 2011; Østlie et al., 2012, see also Table 1). We therefore tested our predictions considering both active and cosmetic prosthesis types.
Materials and methods
Participants
Thirty-two individuals with a missing hand [one-handers, mean age 42.3 years, standard deviation (SD) = 11.8, 12 females, eight missing their right hand] were recruited to take part in the study (Table 1). Sixteen one-handers lost their hand because of amputation (mean years since amputation: 16.7), and 16 had congenital unilateral hand absence (amelia). In addition, 24 age- and gender-matched two-handed controls (mean age 41.7 years, SD = 13.1; 12 female; eight left-handed) took part in the study (Supplementary Table 1). Recruitment was primarily carried out through Opcare (contracting prosthetic providers for National Health Services, UK) in accordance with Oxford University’s Medical Sciences inter-divisional research ethics committee (Ref: MSD-IDREC-C2-2014-003). Informed consent and consent to publish was obtained in accordance with ethical standards set out by the Declaration of Helsinki. One additional one-hander was recruited to the study but did not participate in the scanning session because of claustrophobia. Two control participants did not complete the motor functional MRI task because of time constraints. Another one-hander was excluded from data analysis because of poor quality of neuroimaging data.
Experimental procedures
Participants took part in a single experimental session, involving questionnaires, behavioural tasks [as reported in Hahamy et al. (2017) and van den Heiligenberg et al. (2017); see osf.io/kd2yh for the full study protocol] and an MRI session. Questionnaires included demographic and clinical details (as summarized in Table 1), phantom sensations and pain (as described in Makin et al., 2013b), and prosthesis usage (as described below).
Experience with prosthetic limbs
Prosthesis usage habits
Prosthesis usage habits in one-handers are summarized in Table 1 and Supplementary Table 2. Of the 32 one-handers, two were not prosthesis owners and another three did not use their prosthesis currently. Of the remaining 27, 17 one-handers regularly used an active prosthesis, either body powered (mechanical; n = 11) or powered via electrical muscle signal (myoelectric; n = 4) or both (n = 2). Seventeen one-handers regularly used a cosmetic prosthesis (of which seven were also active prosthesis users, whereas the other 10 used a cosmetic prosthesis exclusively).
Usage measurements in one-handers
Daily prosthesis usage was assessed using a revised version of the Motor Activity Log (MAL) as described and validated by Makin et al. (2013a) and Hahamy et al. (2017). In brief, participants rated how frequently they use their prosthesis in an inventory of 27 daily activities, requiring varying degrees of motor control (e.g. taking money out of wallet; zipping up a coat; peeling fruit skin etc.). As this inventory was not exhaustive, and it is possible that participants wear the prosthesis for other purposes than stated in the inventory (e.g. for cosmetic purposes), participants additionally rated how much time they typically spend wearing their prostheses in their daily lives. Individuals’ MAL scores and wear time for the different types of prostheses are detailed in Table 1. Participants primarily using cosmetic (n = 14) and active (n = 13) prostheses similarly use their prosthesis in daily tasks, as reflected in both MAL scores [average ± standard error of the mean (SEM) cosmetic = 0.25 ± 0.06; active = 0.19 ± 0.06; group difference t(25) = 0.67, P = 0.51] and usage time (average ± SEM cosmetic = 3.5 ± 0.45; active = 3.23 ± 0.38; group difference Mann-Whitney U-test = 84.5, P = 0.77).
Both the MAL and maximum wear-time ratings were standardized using a Z-transform and summed to create a usage score that included both wear time and incorporation of the prosthesis in day-to-day activities, as previously implemented (van den Heiligenberg et al., 2017). Note that the two variables composing the usage score highly correlated with each other [n = 32, r(30) = 0.84; P < 0.001].
Passive visual exposure to prosthesis usage in control participants
Fourteen of the control participants were family members or friends of prosthesis-using one-handers, or had professional relationships with prosthesis users. We asked each control participant to rate how frequently they observed artificial limbs being used for daily purposes, using the same procedures as described above. This involved both prosthesis observation log (POL) of the inventory of daily activities included in the MAL, and prosthesis observation time (analogous to wear time). Visual experience was quantified using the same approach implemented for active prosthesis usage, as detailed above. Three of the control participants did not complete the questionnaires and were therefore discarded from this analysis. The remaining 21 control participants showed a diverse range of visual experience of prosthesis usage (Supplementary Table 1).
Experimental design: functional MRI tasks
Visual task
Visual stimuli consisted of photographs from the following five categories (Fig. 2A and B): (I) upper limbs; (II) man-made objects; (III) participants’ ‘own’ prosthesis; (IV) unfamiliar cosmetic prostheses; and (V) unfamiliar active prostheses. Four other categories, not relevant for the purpose of the present study, were also presented during functional MRI scans but are not reported here (see osf.io/kd2yh for full details).
Images of (I) upper limbs (with and without the arm, from both first and third person perspectives); and (II) man-made objects, which are typically non-manipulable, were taken from an online database. To generate stimuli for the prosthesis conditions III–V, one-handers were asked to bring their prostheses to the study with them. Pictures of each participants’ prosthesis were taken by the experimenters prior to the functional MRI session from different angles (both first and third person perspectives). In the (III) ‘own’ prosthesis condition, all one-handers who had brought their prosthesis to the study were presented with images of their own prostheses, either cosmetic or active (n = 26, see Supplementary Table 2). For individuals using several prostheses, we used the prosthesis worn more often. All other participants (i.e. the remaining six one-handers who did not bring a prosthesis and all control participants) were shown instead pictures of their own shoe. This was done to fill gaps in the experimental time course. Shoes were selected as a familiar external object that was intended to exert similar cognitive effects (e.g. in terms of arousal) as the prosthesis, and therefore minimize differences in the scan time course across groups. Consequently, the shoe condition was not included in further analysis.
Images of cosmetic and active prostheses (taken from the one-handers prostheses image pool) were shown to all the study participants in the remaining (IV) and (V) conditions, respectively. This time, however, participants with a prosthesis were presented with images of another person’s prosthesis (i.e. not their own). Note that the subset of myoelectric prosthesis users (Supplementary Table 2) were shown another myoelectric prosthesis. For both upper-limb and prosthesis images, the hand/prostheses were matched to the one-handers’ missing-hand side and the non-dominant hand in controls (e.g. participants missing their left hand were presented with ‘left-handed’ hands/prostheses).
Visual stimuli had their background removed, normalized for size, placed on an equi-luminant grey background and overlaid with a fixation point. The experiment included four separate runs. In each of the runs, each visual condition comprised nine trials (i.e. nine condition repetitions). In each such trial a single image was shown for 1.5 s, followed by 2.5 s of fixation. Eight different exemplars of a particular image category were used in these nine trials: seven images were presented only once while one image was shown twice in succession. Participants were required to detect these repetitions and report them with a button press (one-back recognition task). This design resulted in 36 repetitions of the same condition across all runs (nine trials × four runs). The run order was varied across participants. First-order counterbalancing of the image sequences was performed using Optseq (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/optseq), which returns the most optimal image presentation schedule. The specifics of this design were validated against an event-related design with a jittered interstimulus interval and a block design during piloting (n = 4). Visual stimuli were presented on a screen located at the rear end of the scanner bore and were viewed through a mirror mounted on the head coil. Stimulus presentation was controlled by a Macbook-Pro running the Psychophysics Toolbox in Matlab (The MathWorks, Natick, MA).
Motor localizer
To localize the sensorimotor hand-selective area, participants were visually instructed to move their intact hand (dominant hand in controls) or feet (bilateral toe movements). Other body-part conditions, not relevant for the purpose of the present study, were also included in the scan but not reported here (see Hahamy et al., 2017 and osf.io/kd2yh for full details). The protocol consisted of alternating 12-s periods of movement and rest. Each of the conditions was repeated four times in a quasi-counterbalanced order. Participants received training before the scan on the degree and form of the movements prior to the scanning session.
Resting state scan
During the resting state functional MRI scan, participants were instructed to keep their eyes open, look at a central fixation cross displayed on the screen, and let their mind wander.
Data acquisition
All data were acquired using a 3 T Verio scanner (Siemens) with a 32-channel head coil. Anatomical data were acquired using a T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo sequence (MPRAGE) with the parameters: repetition time = 2040 ms; echo time = 4.7 ms; flip angle = 8°, voxel size = 1 mm isotropic resolution. Blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) functional MRI during the resting state and visual task was acquired using a multiband-6 sequence (Uğurbil et al., 2013) with the parameters: voxel size = 2 mm isotropic, repetition time = 1300 ms; echo time = 40 ms; flip angle = 66°. Seventy-two slices with 2 mm thickness and no slice gap were acquired in the oblique axial plane, covering the whole cortex and most of the cerebellum. Two hundred and fifty-six volumes in each of the visual runs and 230 volumes in the resting state scan were acquired. Additional dummy volumes were acquired before the start of each scan to achieve equilibrium. The first dummy volume was saved and later used as a reference for co-registration.
During the motor task, BOLD functional MRI was acquired using a multiple gradient echo-planar T2*-weighted pulse sequence, with the parameters: voxel size = 3 mm isotropic, repetition time = 2000 ms; echo time = 30 ms; flip angle = 90°; imaging matrix = 64 × 64; field of view = 192 mm axial slices. Forty-six slices with slice thickness of 3 mm and no gap were acquired in the oblique axial plane, covering the whole cortex, with partial coverage of the cerebellum. Additional dummy volumes were acquired before the start of the scan.
Neuroimaging data processing and low-level analysis
All imaging data were processed using FMRIB’s Expert Analysis Tool (FEAT; version 6.0) of FMRIB’s Software Library (FSL; version 5.0). Subsequent analyses were performed using Matlab (version 7.11, The Mathworks Inc, Natick, MA).
Preprocessing
The following preprocessing steps were applied to each participant’s task data: motion correction using FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool (MCFLIRT; Jenkinson et al., 2002), B0-unwarping, brain extraction using BET (Smith, 2002), high-pass temporal filtering of frequencies below a cycle of 100 s and 130 s for visual and resting scans, respectively, grand-mean intensity normalization of the entire functional run by a single multiplicative factor, and spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel with a full-width at half-maximum of 3 mm for visual and resting scans and 5 mm for motor scans. Functional images were aligned to structural images initially using FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool (FLIRT; Jenkinson and Smith, 2001; Jenkinson et al., 2002) and then optimized using Boundary-Based Registration (Greve and Fischl, 2009). Structural images were transformed into MNI space using FMRIB’s Nonlinear Image Registration Tool (FNIRT).
Individual level statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was performed using FMRIB’s Improved Linear Model (FILM). We applied a general linear model (GLM), as implemented in FEAT, to each functional run, as detailed below.
Task-based scans
Each of the experimental conditions was modelled separately against rest (fixation). Regressors were created by convolving stimulus presentation with a double-gamma haemodynamic response function (HRF). For each run, six head motion parameters were included as regressors of no interest. In case of large movement between volumes (>1 mm) additional regressors of no interest were included in the GLM to account for each of these instances individually. Contrasts for the conditions of interest were defined either against a control condition (objects, to control for interindividual variations in visual activity; or feet, to control for motor task demands which are not hand specific, for the visual and motor task, respectively) or against the baseline. For the visual task, second-level analyses were conducted on each participant's four experimental runs using a fixed-effects analysis.
Resting state scan
To account for non-neuronal noise that might bias functional connectivity analyses (Behzadi et al., 2007; Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012), we extracted the BOLD time series underlying white matter and CSF in the resting state scans. For this purpose, the T1-weighted structural scans were segmented into grey matter, white matter, and CSF, using the segmentation algorithm available in the SPM12 software package. The white matter maps were restricted by the white matter standard mask from the Harvard-Oxford atlas and thresholded to select 30 000 voxels with the highest intensity values. The resulting maps were eroded by one voxel in each direction to minimize partial voluming with grey matter. This yielded white matter maps that contained 16 105± 68 voxels. CSF maps were created by thresholding the individual CSF maps to select the 2000 voxels with the lowest intensity. No erosion was applied.
For white matter and CSF maps, the first five eigenvectors were calculated using the preprocessed resting state time series, as they best characterize the majority of observed signal variation across a set of voxels within a region (Behzadi et al., 2007; Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon, 2012). Additionally, we extracted time series representing head motion throughout the scan in six directions. The 16 regressors of no interest were regressed out from the preprocessed resting state time series. The resulting time series (residuals) were z-transformed and subsequently used in region of interest-based connectivity analyses (see below).
Regions of interest
Since the focus of the study was on hand-selective areas, our main analysis was restricted to the individualized regions of interest.
Visual regions of interest
Bilateral visual hand-selective regions of interest were selected in lateral occipito-temporal cortex using the contrast hands versus objects (Fig. 1A). Note that visual hand representation is unchanged by hand loss (Striem-Amit et al., 2017). For each participant, the 250 most hand-selective voxels were selected in each hemisphere (see Supplementary Fig. 1 for group probabilistic maps). Only voxels with a Z-score > 2 were included. Voxel selection was restricted to the superior temporal, middle temporal, inferior temporal, fusiform and parahippocampal gyri, as well as the lateral occipital cortex and occipitotemporal cortex (all bilateral), as defined by Harvard-Oxford atlas (Desikan et al., 2006). Contrary to the motor hand representation, there is no clear laterality of visual hand representation in the occipitotemporal cortex (Shmuelof et al., 2006). Voxels from both hemispheres were therefore combined in a single region of interest for use in subsequent analyses. We confirmed this by splitting the regions of interest across the hemispheres and performing the same analyses outlined below, in which we found no significant differences between the hemispheres.
Motor regions of interest
The putative sensorimotor missing-hand territory (or controls’ non-dominant hand territory) was identified in S1/M1 by mirror-projecting the intact hand region of interest (or controls’ dominant hand) across the mid-sagittal plane (Fig. 1A). To delineate the intact/dominant hand regions of interest, we used the contrast of intact or dominant hand (in one-handers or controls, respectively) versus feet movements. For each participant, the 200 most active voxels (with a Z-score > 2) were selected during intact/dominant hand movements in the contralateral sensorimotor cortex. For the two control participants who did not complete the motor functional MRI task, sensorimotor hand regions of interest were defined based on the corresponding group statistical maps, while taking into account participants’ hand-dominance. Voxel selection was restricted by the precentral and postcentral gyri, as defined by the Harvard-Oxford atlas.
To validate this flipping procedure, we repeated the analysis described in Hahamy et al. (2017). Specifically, we created a new region of interest for the non-dominant hand in each of our controls who completed the motor localizer scan (n = 22). We generated this region of interest by selecting the 200 most active voxels in the contralateral sensorimotor cortex during non-dominant hand movements (versus feet movements), as described above. We then extracted the activation level, i.e. contrasts of parameter estimates (COPEs) for non-dominant hand movement under both the dominant-hand flipped region of interest and the non-dominant hand unflipped region of interest. The correlation coefficient across control participants between both regions of interest was high [r(20) = 0.82, P < 0.001] suggesting that the flipped region of interest captured most of the interindividual variance of the relevant representation.
Region of interest analysis
Region of interest analysis of visual experiment
The mean COPEs across voxels were extracted for each condition of interest (own/cosmetic/active prostheses) and control condition (objects) versus baseline. For the analysis of unfamiliar (others’) prostheses shown in Fig. 2E, we only included one-handers who regularly use cosmetic or active prostheses (n = 26, see Supplementary Table 2 for full details). To account for differences in non-category-specific visual activity, as well as other sources of variance across participants of no interest, we regressed out object activity from prostheses activity (Van Breukelen, 2006). We performed a semi-partial (hereafter ‘part’) Pearson correlation between activity values for each condition of interest and prosthesis usage (or visual exposure to prosthesis usage in controls). For the unfamiliar (others’) conditions we included all amputees eligible for analysis (n = 31; Supplementary Table 2), including the few individuals who do not use a prosthesis, allowing us to best assess usage-related variance. For the ‘own’ condition we could only include the participants who brought their own prosthesis to the study (n = 25, Supplementary Table 2). Note that object activity didn’t correlate with prosthesis usage [r(23) = −0.11 and r(29) = 0.04 for ‘own’ and ‘unfamiliar’ conditions, respectively].
Region of interest analysis of resting state data
The purpose of this analysis was to determine intrinsic functional connectivity between visual hand selective areas and the missing-hand sensorimotor cortex. We focused on the missing hand sensorimotor cortex because the prosthesis is designed to substitute the missing hand’s motor function. However, given the known shared resting state variance across the two hand areas (Biswal et al., 1995), we needed to account for any visuomotor connectivity that is shared between the two sensorimotor hand areas. For this purpose, the mean time series were extracted from the sensorimotor intact/dominant and missing/non-dominant hand regions of interest independently (in one-handers/controls, respectively). Next, we calculated the Pearson’s bivariate correlation between the time series of each of the two sensorimotor hand regions of interest and the mean time series of bilateral visual hand regions of interest. The correlation coefficients, after applying a Fisher’s Z-transform, represented the coupling between sensorimotor and visual areas in each participant. To assess the relationship between prosthesis usage (or visual exposure to prosthesis usage) and functional connectivity, we calculated the part correlation between prosthesis usage and missing hand sensorimotor-to-visual connectivity, while accounting for intact-hand sensorimotor-to-visual connectivity. This was done to ensure that the visuomotor connectivity we are measuring expresses the unique contribution of the missing hand sensorimotor cortex. For this purpose, we first adjusted the visuomotor connectivity strength between the S1/M1 missing hand region of interest with the bilateral visual region of interest, based on visuomotor connectivity with the intact hand region of interest. We then correlated the residual values of missing hand visuomotor connectivity with prosthesis usage. Note that visuomotor connectivity with the intact hand region of interest associated negatively (although not significantly) with prosthesis usage [r(29) = −0.18]. This suggests that visuomotor connectivity described below may also be influenced by prosthesis disuse (resulting in increased reliance on the intact hand, and increased visuomotor connectivity with the intact-hand region of interest).
Statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was calculated using SPSS (version 16.0, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL) and Matlab (version 7.11, The Mathworks Inc, Natick, MA). After verifying normality (Shapiro-Wilks test), we used ANOVA and two-tailed Student’s t-tests to compare between groups and subgroups (or Mann-Whitney U-test, if the assumption of normality was violated). To determine whether the distribution of passive observation and prosthesis usage is overlapping, an independent-samples Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was applied. To examine relationship between brain measurements and prosthesis usage we ran permutation tests. To generate a chance distribution, the usage/visual experience scores were permuted 10 000 times, and a part Pearson correlation was repeated. We then assessed the significance of the true correlation coefficient, by calculating the two-sided P-values in the generated chance distributions (Figs 1C, D and 3B, E and G). To compare between two correlation coefficients, we used the Fisher’s r to Z test.
To examine joint versus unique relations between prosthesis usage, prosthesis activity and visuomotor connectivity, we performed a hierarchical linear regression analysis—a comparison of nested regression models. The dependent variable was visual activity to (unfamiliar) active prostheses (after accounting for object-related activity using part correlation, as described above). We used this active prosthesis condition because the active prosthesis least resembles a natural hand. Independent variables were prosthesis usage and resting state functional connectivity between the sensorimotor missing hand region of interest and the bilateral visual regions of interest (after accounting for intact hand visuomotor connectivity, as described above). A series of linear regression analyses was performed. By comparing the explained variance of a model containing both usage and connectivity (‘full model’) with regression models containing these variables as separate predictors (‘reduced models’), we assessed the unique and shared variance explained by these predictors.
Finally, to determine whether the correlations with prosthesis usage were affected by (i) primary prosthesis type for daily usage (active, cosmetic); and (ii) developmental period of hand-loss (congenital, adulthood), the main correlation analyses were repeated as above but with subgroup affiliation as a covariate. For this purpose, we carried out a one-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) on activity/connectivity values (after applying part correlation to account for objects activity/intact hand connectivity, as described above), with prosthesis usage as a main factor, and subgroup as a covariate, in separate analyses.
Group level analysis of task-based data
To further confirm and extend our main region of interest results and to determine whether the reported effects were specific to lateral occipitotemporal cortex, we also carried out whole-brain analyses (note, however, that such analysis is not appropriate for regions showing strong lateralization with respect to the amputation side). Whole-brain activity maps across all participants [for hands versus objects and unfamiliar (other’s) active prostheses versus objects, Fig. 2C and D] were calculated using FMRIB’s Local Analysis of Mixed Effects (FLAME1). We also estimated voxel-wise correspondence between prosthesis usage and activity (versus baseline) in each of the three prosthesis conditions in one-handers, after partialling out voxel-wise activity in response to objects (versus baseline). As in the region of interest-based analysis, we only used one-handers eligible for functional MRI analysis (n = 31). One-handers who brought their prosthesis to the study (n = 25) were included in the ‘own’ prosthesis correlation analysis. Z-statistic images were minimally thresholded (Z > 2.3) and adjusted for multiple comparisons using whole-brain family-wise error (Gaussian Random Field) cluster size correction, and a corrected cluster significance threshold of P < 0.05. For visualization purposes activation maps were projected onto an inflated cortex using FreeSurfer (Dale et al., 1999; Reuter et al., 2012) and Workbench (Van Essen et al., 2013).
Results
We first searched for group differences in visual hand-selective regions when participants were shown images of other people’s prosthesis images (active or cosmetic, Fig. 2A and B). Across all participants, unfamiliar active prosthesis images activated lateral occipito-temporal cortex, overlapping with hand-selective activity (Fig. 2C and D). Importantly, activity in the visual hand-selective regions of interest (Fig. 1A) was greater in one-handers, and prosthesis users in particular, compared to controls. A 2 × 2 ANOVA with factors group (all one-handers, n = 31, controls, n = 24) and prosthesis type (cosmetic, active) revealed a significant group difference [F(1,106) = 6.5, P = 0.012], with one-handers showing stronger activity than controls. Although activity tended to be stronger for the cosmetic prostheses compared to active prostheses [F(1,106) = 3.5, P = 0.066], the interaction term was not significant [F(1,106) = 0.4, P = 0.538], indicating that this trend is likely driven by the visual features of the cosmetic prosthesis, which strongly resembles a hand. When focusing the analysis specifically on one-handers who are prosthesis users (n = 26, Supplementary Table 2), we found significantly increased activity for images of unfamiliar active prostheses (who share little visual similarity with natural hands) in the hand-selective visual region of interest, compared with controls [n = 24, t(48) = 2.8, P = 0.008; Fig. 2E]. These results suggest that prosthesis usage leads to increased activity for prosthesis images in hand-selective regions.
We next determined whether prosthesis usage can account for interindividual differences in activity levels for prosthesis images in visual hand-selective regions of interest. Across one-handers, greater prosthesis usage in daily life positively correlated with increased activity in visual hand-selective cortex. This correlation was found when one-handers viewed images of both their own prosthesis [n = 25, r(23) = 0.52, Pperm = 0.007, Fig. 3A–C], and another person’s active prosthesis, which shares the functionality of the hand but not its visual features [n = 31, r(29) = 0.51, Pperm = 0.004, Fig. 3D–F]. Although usage showed only a trending correlation with activation to another’s cosmetic prostheses [n = 31, r(29) = 0.33, Pperm = 0.068], correlations with active and cosmetic prosthesis images were not significantly different (two-tailed Fisher Z = 0.82, P = 0.41). To examine the consistency of the effect across one-handers’ subgroups primarily using cosmetic or active prostheses, we repeated the analysis while accounting for prosthesis usage type (Table 1). We found that the relationship between prosthesis usage and visual activity remained significant [ANCOVA (n = 26): images of active prostheses: F(1,23) = 7.1, P = 0.014; images of cosmetic prostheses: F(1,23) = 6.0, P = 0.022]. Importantly, the subgroup factor (prosthesis usage type) failed to show significance [F(1,23) = 0.13, P = 0.72 and F(1,23) = 0.06, P = 0.81, respectively]. As the effects were not restricted either to individual’s own prosthesis or to a prosthesis type primarily used by the one-hander (active versus cosmetic), these results hint at categorical changes in representation (van den Heiligenberg et al., 2017), rather than familiarity with the specific visual features of the user’s prosthesis. This interpretation is consistent with the fact that one-handers routinely replace their prosthesis, and often use multiple prosthesis types (Table 1).
To determine whether the increased activity in prosthesis users could be ascribed to passive visual experience alone, we repeated the analysis in the control participants, based on their passive observation log scores (Supplementary Table 1). Although prosthesis-related experience is likely to be less intensive in controls compared to one-handers, the difference between the overall distribution of the POL and MAL scores was non-significant, as indicated by the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (Z = 0.750, P = 0.627). Therefore, the correlation analysis is potentially suitable for measuring interindividual differences that scale with passive observation. Correlation found between prosthesis activity and controls’ visual experience was significantly smaller than the reported correlation between prosthesis activity and prosthesis usage in one-handers [n = 21, active prosthesis: r(19) = 0.03, Pperm = 0.93, group difference: one-tailed Fisher Z = 1.76, P = 0.039, Fig. 3G; cosmetic prosthesis: r(19) = −0.10; Pperm = 0.68, group difference: one-tailed Fisher Z = 2.13, P = 0.017]. This result indicates that activity in hand-selective visual areas in the adult visual system scales with active everyday visuomotor experience.
To assess intrinsic coupling between individuals’ sensorimotor missing-hand area and bilateral hand-selective visual areas, we measured resting state functional connectivity. Visuomotor connectivity was correlated with prosthesis usage, while accounting for visuomotor connectivity with the intact hand area (see ‘Materials and methods’ section). One-handers who use their prosthesis more in daily life showed stronger relative connectivity than those with lower usage [n = 31, r(29) = 0.55; Pperm = 0.001; Fig. 1B and C]. Here again, no significant correlation was found with controls’ visual experience [n = 21, r(19) = −0.28, Pperm = 0.22, group difference: one-tailed Fisher Z = 3.0 P = 0.001; Fig. 1D]. This result suggests that successful prosthesis usage is associated with increased visuomotor communication with the missing-hand’s territory.
To determine the utility of visuomotor functional connectivity in explaining variance in visual activity, a hierarchical regression analysis was implemented (see ‘Materials and methods’ section). Visual activity to (unfamiliar) active prostheses served as the dependent variable. As the sole predictor of such activity, usage accounted for 26% of the variance in the dependent variable [n = 31, F(1,29) = 10.02, P = 0.004], while visuomotor connectivity alone accounted for 22% of such variance [F(1,29) = 8.15, P = 0.008]. When the predictors were combined, the explained variance was increased to 31% [F(2,28) = 6.24, P = 0.006]. Considering this combined variance, we determined the shared versus unique effects of the predictors, i.e. the contribution of usage to activity that is unrelated to connectivity and vice versa. We found that the shared effect accounts for 17% of the variance in the activity. Connectivity uniquely accounted for only 5% of such variance (i.e. above the variance explained by usage), and this effect was not significant [F(1,28) = 2.1, P = 0.161]. Importantly, the prosthesis usage also contributed little additional variance in activity (9%), beyond that explained by the connectivity. As above, the effect failed to reach significance level [F(1,28) = 3.6, P = 0.068]. Although this analysis does not enable causal inferences to be drawn, it nevertheless clearly indicates a threefold coupling between activity, connectivity and usage.
Finally, we determined whether the repurposing of visual hand-selective areas to support prosthesis representation depends on the developmental period during which individuals experienced hand loss. We therefore repeated our main analysis showing correspondence between prosthesis daily usage and visual activity/connectivity (as described above), while accounting for any potential group differences between individuals with congenital and acquired handlessness. We found that the correspondence reported above remained significant, both with respect to activity [ANCOVA: own prosthesis (n = 25): F(1,22) = 6.2, P = 0.021; active prosthesis (n = 31): F(1,28) = 9.0, P = 0.006] and functional connectivity (n = 31): [F(1,28) = 10.3, P = 0.003]. Importantly, the subgroup factor (congenital versus acquired one-handedness) failed to show significance [own prosthesis: F(1,22) = 0.25, P = 0.623; active prosthesis: F(1,28) = 0.06, P = 0.804; functional connectivity: F(1,28) = 0.02, P = 0.891]. This result is consistent with our previous findings showing that brain organization and reorganization in one-handers is best characterized by everyday experience (Makin et al., 2013a; Hahamy et al., 2015, 2017).
Discussion
Here we show that prosthetic limbs, used to substitute the missing hand, can recruit brain resources normally devoted for body representation. We also show that this neurophysiological ‘embodiment’ of artificial limbs depends on prosthesis usage in everyday life—those individuals who rely more on their prosthesis to substitute hand function show stronger activity in hand-selective visual areas when presented with images of a prosthesis. Importantly, the engagement of hand-selective areas in prosthesis representation was not specific to individuals’ own prostheses, but generalized to other, unfamiliar, exemplars of artificial limbs. Furthermore, prosthetic limb representation did not depend on the type of prosthesis primarily used by each one-hander, or on the developmental period in which individuals lost their limb and started using prosthesis (i.e. congenital amelia versus acquired amputation in adulthood). Our findings therefore hint at categorical representation of artificial limbs that primarily depends on everyday experience (see van den Heiligenberg et al., 2017 for related behavioural results). Finally, we show that prosthesis usage also shapes large-scale brain reorganization, specifically intrinsic connectivity between visual and sensorimotor hand-selective areas. Together, our findings provide the first account of how artificial limbs are represented in the brain of amputees.
While it has long been established that the lateral occipito-temporal cortex shows modular visual representation for upper limbs, the inputs guiding this representation, as well as its behavioural relevance, have been debated (Astafiev et al., 2004; Peelen and Downing, 2005; Orlov et al., 2010, 2014; Downing and Peelen, 2011; Bracci et al., 2012; Gallivan and Culham, 2015; Lingnau and Downing, 2015; Zimmermann et al., 2018). Indeed, recent studies have argued that visual body representation is independent of any motor experience (Vannuscorps and Caramazza, 2016; Striem-Amit et al., 2017). We demonstrate that altered motor behaviour in daily life facilitates visual processing and shapes communication between visual and sensorimotor areas. However, since prosthesis usage strongly relies on visual input (due to lack of somatosensory feedback), people who use their prosthesis more in daily life also spend more time and attention while looking at it. It is therefore difficult to tease apart the contributions of pure visual experience as opposed to visuomotor experience. Several pieces of evidence in the current study can inform us on the potential contribution of active, versus passive, everyday experience. First, we found similar evidence when presenting individuals with images of their own prosthesis versus an unfamiliar one. This indicates that the increased activity doesn’t depend on visual familiarity per se. Second, we tested control participants with varying daily exposure to prosthesis usage, including family members and friends of one-handers. Even if these control participants may spend less time observing a prosthesis compared with prosthesis users, the lack of correlation between the degree of passive visual experience and activity in visual hand-selective areas for prosthesis images indicates that the two are not tightly coupled. Finally, our hierarchical regression analysis reveals that the re-appropriation of the visual hand-selective areas to support prosthetic limb representation depends not just on visual exposure to the prosthesis, but rather also on increased connectivity with hand-selective sensorimotor resources, induced by usage. While directionality cannot be inferred from these hierarchical analyses, our findings provide evidence for a tight coupling between daily actions, functional connectivity with sensorimotor cortex, and visual body representation.
In summary, our findings show that neurocognitive resources devoted to representing our body can support representation of artificial body parts. By providing first evidence for a relationship between embodied technology and successful prosthesis usage, our results may aid assistive and augmentative technological development and usage (Makin et al., 2017).
Funding
This work was supported by a Sir Henry Dale Fellowship jointly funded by the Wellcome Trust and the Royal Society (104128/Z/14/Z), an ERC Starting Grant (715022 EmbodiedTech), and a Cogito Foundation grant, awarded to T.R.M. S.N.M. was funded by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada CREATE training program (371161-2009 CREAT) to J.C.C. H.J.B. is funded by a Wellcome Principal Research Fellowship (110027/Z/15/Z).
Supplementary material
Supplementary material is available at Brain online.
Supplementary Material
Glossary
Abbreviation
- MAL
Motor Activity Log
References
- Antfolk C, D’Alonzo M, Rosen B, Lundborg G, Sebelius F, Cipriani C. Sensory feedback in upper limb prosthetics. Expert Rev Med Devices 2013; 10: 45–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Astafiev SV, Stanley CM, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions. Nat Neurosci 2004; 7: 542–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. Neuroimage 2007; 37: 90–101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar mri. Magn Reson Med 1995; 34: 537–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bracci S, Cavina-Pratesi C, Ietswaart M, Caramazza A, Peelen MV. Closely overlapping responses to tools and hands in left lateral occipitotemporal cortex. J Neurophysiol 2012; 107: 1443–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bracci S, Ietswaart M, Peelen MV, Cavina-Pratesi C. Dissociable neural responses to hands and non-hand body parts in human left extrastriate visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 2010; 103: 3389–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 1999; 9: 179–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 2006; 31: 968–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Downing PE, Peelen MV. The role of occipitotemporal body-selective regions in person perception. Cogn Neurosci 2011; 2: 186–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Downing PE, Peelen MV. Body selectivity in occipitotemporal cortex: causal evidence. Neuropsychologia 2016; 83: 138–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ejaz N, Hamada M, Diedrichsen J. Hand use predicts the structure of representations in sensorimotor cortex. Nat Neurosci 2015; 18: 1034–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flor H, Nikolajsen L, Jensen TS. Phantom limb pain: a case of maladaptive CNS plasticity? Nat Rev Neurosci 2006; 7: 873–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallivan JP, Culham JC. Neural coding within human brain areas involved in actions. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2015; 33: 141–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greve DN, Fischl B. Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. Neuroimage 2009; 48: 63–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahamy A, Macdonald S, van den Heiligenberg FMZ, Kieliba P, Emir U, Malach R, et al. Representation of multiple body parts in the missing hand territory of congenital one-handers. Curr Biol 2017; 27: 1350–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahamy A, Sotiropoulos SN, Slater DH, Malach R, Johansen-Berg H, Makin TR. Normalisation of brain connectivity through compensatory behaviour, despite congenital hand absence. Elife 2015; 4: e04605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jang CH, Yang HS, Yang HE, Lee SY, Kwon JW, Yun BD, et al. A survey on activities of daily living and occupations of upper extremity amputees. Ann Rehabil Med 2011; 35: 907–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 2002; 17: 825–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M, Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med Image Anal 2001; 5: 143–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaas JH, Nelson RJ, Sur M, Lin CS, Merzenich MM. Multiple representations of the body within the primary somatosensory cortex of primates. Science 1979; 204: 521–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lingnau A, Downing PE. The lateral occipitotemporal cortex in action. Trends Cogn Sci 2015; 19: 268–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makin TR, Cramer AO, Scholz J, Hahamy A, Slater DH, Tracey I, et al. Deprivation-related and use-dependent plasticity go hand in hand. Elife 2013a; 2: e01273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makin TR, de Vignemont F, Faisal AA. Neurocognitive barriers to the embodiment of technology. Nat Biomed Eng 2017; 1: s41551–016. [Google Scholar]
- Makin TR, Filippini N, Duff EP, Slater DH, Tracey I, Johansen-Berg H. Network-level reorganisation of functional connectivity following arm amputation. Neuroimage 2015; 114: 217–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makin TR, Scholz J, Filippini N, Slater DH, Tracey I, Johansen-Berg H. Phantom pain is associated with preserved structure and function in the former hand area. Nat Commun 2013b; 4: 1570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orlov T, Makin TR, Zohary E. Topographic representation of the human body in the occipitotemporal cortex. Neuron 2010; 68: 586–600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orlov T, Porat Y, Makin TR, Zohary E. Hands in motion: an upper-limb-selective area in the occipitotemporal cortex shows sensitivity to viewed hand kinematics. J Neurosci 2014; 34: 4882–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Østlie K, Lesjø IM, Franklin RJ, Garfelt B, Skjeldal OH, Magnus P. Prosthesis use in adult acquired major upper-limb amputees: patterns of wear, prosthetic skills and the actual use of prostheses in activities of daily life. Disabil Rehabil Assist Technol 2012; 7: 479–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peelen MV, Downing PE. Is the extrastriate body area involved in motor actions? Nat Neurosci 2005; 8: 125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penfield W, Rasmussen T. The cerebral cortex of man: a clinical study of localization of function. New York, NY: Macmillan; 1950. [Google Scholar]
- Pons TP, Garraghty PE, Ommaya AK, Kaas JH, Taub E, Mishkin M. Massive cortical reorganization after sensory deafferentation in adult macaques. Science 1991; 252: 1857–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reuter M, Schmansky NJ, Rosas HD, Fischl B. Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage 2012; 61: 1402–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shmuelof L, Zohary E. A mirror representation of others' actions in the human anterior parietal cortex. J Neurosci 2006; 26: 9736–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp 2002; 17: 143–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Striem-Amit E, Vannuscorps G, Caramazza A. Sensorimotor-independent development of hands and tools selectivity in the visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2017; 114: 4787–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tal Z, Geva R, Amedi A. The origins of metamodality in visual object area LO: bodily topographical biases and increased functional connectivity to S1. Neuroimage 2016; 127: 363–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uğurbil K, Xu J, Auerbach EJ, Moeller S, Vu AT, Duarte-Carvajalino JM, et al. Pushing spatial and temporal resolution for functional and diffusion MRI in the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage 2013; 80: 80–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Breukelen GJ. ANCOVA versus change from baseline had more power in randomized studies and more bias in nonrandomized studies. J Clin Epidemiol 2006; 59: 920–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Heiligenberg FM, Yeung N, Brugger P, Culham JC, Makin TR. Adaptable categorization of hands and tools in prosthesis users. Psychol Sci 2017; 28: 395–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen DC, Smith SM, Barch DM, Behrens TE, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K, et al. The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: an overview. Neuroimage 2013; 80: 62–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vannuscorps G, Caramazza A. Typical action perception and interpretation without motor simulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2016; 113: 86–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A. Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect 2012; 2: 125–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmermann M, Mars RB, de Lange FP, Toni I, Verhagen L. Is the extrastriate body area part of the dorsal visuomotor stream? Brain Struct Funct 2018; 223: 31–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.