Abstract
Working memory (WM) skills are closely associated with learning progress in key areas such as reading and mathematics across childhood. As yet, however, little is known about how the brain systems underpinning WM develop over this critical developmental period. The current study investigated whether and how structural brain correlates of components of the working memory system change over development. Verbal and visuospatial short‐term and working memory were assessed in 153 children between 5.58 and 15.92 years, and latent components of the working memory system were derived. Fractional anisotropy and cortical thickness maps were derived from T1‐weighted and diffusion‐weighted MRI and processed using eigenanatomy decomposition. There was a greater involvement of the corpus callosum and posterior temporal white matter in younger children for performance associated with the executive part of the working memory system. For older children, this was more closely linked with the thickness of the occipitotemporal cortex. These findings suggest that increasing specialization leads to shifts in the contribution of neural substrates over childhood, moving from an early dependence on a distributed system supported by long‐range connections to later reliance on specialized local circuitry. Our findings demonstrate that despite the component factor structure being stable across childhood, the underlying brain systems supporting working memory change. Taking the age of the child into account, and not just their overall score, is likely to be critical for understanding the nature of the limitations on their working memory capacity.
RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS
Multiple measures of verbal and visuospatial short‐term and working memory enabling assessment of latent constructs of working memory, instead of using task‐specific scores.
A large sample of children between 5.58 and 15.92 years who completed working memory assessments (n = 153), with a large subset who also completed T1‐weighted MRI (n = 122, age = 5.58–15.92y) and diffusion‐weighted MRI (n = 112, age = 5.58–15.92y).
First study to investigate changes in the association between brain structures and working memory capacity across childhood and adolescence.
1. INTRODUCTION
Working memory is a limited capacity system for retaining and processing information over brief periods of time. It is closely linked with the acquisition of complex cognitive skills (Cowan, 2013) such as reading (Cain, Oakhill, & Bryant, 2004), mathematics (Dumontheil & Klingberg, 2011), and other academic subjects (Clair‐Thompson & Gathercole, 2006; Gathercole, Pickering, Knight, & Stegmann, 2003). Deficits in working memory have been identified across a range of neurodevelopmental disorders, including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Holmes et al., 2014; Martinussen, Hayden, Hogg‐Johnson, & Tannock, 2005), dyslexia (Smith‐Spark & Fisk, 2007), dyscalculia (Rotzer et al., 2009; Szucs, Devine, Soltesz, Nobes, & Gabriel, 2013), and language disorders (Archibald & Gathercole, 2006; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1989; Montgomery, 2000; Weismer, Evans, & Hesketh, 1999).
Working memory develops gradually through early and middle childhood (Gathercole, Pickering, Ambridge, & Wearing, 2004; Huizinga, Dolan, & van der Molen, 2006; Siegel & Ryan, 1988). It is assumed that this development reflects the maturation of the brain system supporting this skill in adulthood (Tamnes et al., 2013). However, understanding the mechanism of working memory development in childhood necessitates a neuropsychological account that incorporates developmental change. Currently, we have no detailed understanding of how age‐related changes in brain organization support specific developmental improvements in working memory. The purpose of this study is to take steps towards redressing this.
1.1. Working memory and its development
There are many theoretical accounts of working memory. The influential multicomponent model of working memory advanced by Baddeley and Hitch (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974) consists of three subcomponents: two domain‐specific stores and a central executive. The stores are specialized for the retention of material in either phonological (Baddeley, 1987) or visuospatial format (Baddeley & Lieberman, 1980; Logie, 1986). The central executive is a system responsible for a range of regulatory functions, including attention, the control of action, and problem solving (Baddeley, 1996).
There have been many refinements of the original model (Baddeley, 2000, 2003, 2012; Burgess & Hitch, 1996), and several new accounts. Some of these focus on specific mechanisms within working memory. For instance, Engle and colleagues propose inhibitory processes that protect activated memory traces from disruption (Engle, 2002; Kane, Conway, Hambrick, & Engle, 2007). Other models integrate short‐term memory with long‐term memory, suggesting that working memory represents long‐term memory in an activated state (Cowan, 1988, 1999; Oberauer, 2002), and activation is guided by an attentional mechanism. Other theorists have extended the scope of WM to encompass other processes that include updating (Ecker, Lewandowsky, Oberauer, & Chee, 2010; Schmiedek, Lövdén, & Lindenberger, 2014; Shelton, Elliott, Matthews, Hill, & Gouvier, 2010), set shifting and relational binding (Oberauer, Lewandowsky, Farrell, Jarrold, & Greaves, 2012; von Bastian & Oberauer, 2013), and fluid intelligence (Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin, & Conway, 1999). In short, there exists a rich literature in which the specific cognitive mechanisms underlying working memory in adulthood, and its relationship with other cognitive processes, are keenly debated.
Considerable progress in understanding the cognitive processes of WM has been provided by the analysis of latent factors underlying the wide range of measures of WM that have been developed. Using this individual differences approach, the three‐factor structure has been robustly reproduced across multiple studies and age groups (Alloway, Gathercole, Willis, & Adams, 2004; Kane et al., 2004; Bayliss, Jarrold, Gunn, & Baddeley, 2003; Hornung, Brunner, Reuter, & Martin, 2011), although studies that have drawn on a wider range of assessments indicate that refinements may be needed in the concept of attentional control within the system (Gray et al., 2017). In general, these analyses have favoured the distinction between domain‐specific storage for verbal and visuo‐spatial material linked with an executive or attentional component. These components are already detectable in children from about 5 years of age (Alloway et al., 2004) and their configuration remains broadly stable throughout childhood (Gathercole et al., 2004). Working memory performance, however, improves substantially over childhood (Gathercole et al., 2004; Huizinga et al., 2006; Siegel & Ryan, 1988), with linear increases until adolescence, when adult levels are reached (Gathercole et al., 2004; Luciana, Conklin, Hooper, & Yarger, 2005). It has been widely recognized that the cognitive mechanisms contributing to improvements across different periods may themselves change (Gathercole et al., 2004; Huizinga et al., 2006; Siegel & Ryan, 1988). Developmental improvements in WM may, for example, be driven by increases in storage capacity (Cowan, Ricker, Clark, Hinrichs, & Glass, 2014) and / or attention (Barrouillet, Gavens, Vergauwe, Gaillard, & Camos, 2009; Tam, Jarrold, Baddeley, & Sabatos‐DeVito, 2010). They may also be the consequences of changes in rehearsal strategies (Gathercole, Adams, & Hitch, 1994; Hitch, Halliday, Schaafstal, & Heffernan, 1991), although it is now understood that limitations in the sensitivity of memory span in pre‐school children may obscure the clear signatures of phonologically based rehearsal in older children and adults (Jarrold, 2016; Wang, Logie, & Jarrold, 2016).
1.2. Neural correlates of working memory
The developmental period associated with increases in working memory is accompanied by pronounced changes in brain structure. These include decreasing cortical thickness (Sowell, 2004) and increasing myelination of white matter tracts (Dean et al., 2014). Further, functional neuroimaging studies suggest that improvements in working memory are accompanied by some reorganization in brain networks (Houde, Rossi, Lubin, & Joliot, 2010). In adults, a specialized network including bilateral parietal, cingulate, and prefrontal areas has been found to show increased blood oxygenation during working memory tasks (Owen, McMillan, Laird, & Bullmore, 2005; Wager & Smith, 2003). Children show activation in a similar set of regions (Thomason et al., 2009) and also in additional non‐specific areas outside of the core processing network observed in adults (Ciesielski, Lesnik, Savoy, Grant, & Ahlfors, 2006; Vogan, Morgan, Powell, Smith, & Taylor, 2016).
Findings from the more limited research on structural neural correlates of working memory broadly concur with this pattern of change. Frontal and parietal grey matter volume (Mahone, Martin, Kates, Hay, & Horska, 2009; Rossi et al., 2013), and temporal and parietal connections of the corpus callosum (Treble et al., 2013), are significant predictors of a participant's working memory capacity. However, these studies either investigate narrow age ranges or statistically correct for the effect of age. As a result, little is known about how structural brain changes support the development of particular cognitive skills such as working memory. Furthermore, the majority of previous studies have used performance on individual tasks to measure working memory ability (see Poldrack & Yarkoni, 2016, for a detailed discussion). This approach has two key limitations. First, it is widely accepted that multiple underlying components underpin performance (Alloway et al., 2004; Clair‐Thompson & Gathercole, 2006; Conway, Cowan, Bunting, Therriault, & Minkoff, 2002; Oberauer, Süß, Schulze, Wilhelm, & Wittmann, 2000). Second, scores on individual tests also reflect task‐specific components that may be unrelated to WM demands such as proficiency in the stimulus domain from which the stimuli are drawn (Dark & Benbow, 1994). The purpose of the current study was to redress these two gaps in the literature by (i) exploring how structural brain correlates of working memory, in terms of both grey and white matter, differ over developmental time; and (ii) using multiple behavioural assessments alongside factor analysis, to differentiate the neural correlates of robustly determined cognitive components of WM.
2. METHODS
Our analysis approach used data reduction techniques to reduce raw behavioural and neuroimaging measures to underlying statistical components. We then explored how the underlying cognitive factors of the working memory system were associated with structural brain components and the extent to which these relationships were moderated by developmental stage (i.e., age). A schematic summary of this approach can be seen in Figure 1. The computer code used for data processing and statistical analysis is available online (https://github.com/joebathelt/WorkingMemory_and_BrainStructure_Code).
2.1. Participants
The data for the current study were taken from two large‐scale studies at the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences. Both studies employed the same working memory assessments and structural scanning protocols. These two studies had different recruitment criteria but when combined, provide a large sample of children with working memory scores whose distributional properties closely approximated the standardization sample. The first study was the Centre for Attention, Learning, and Memory (CALM) research clinic (n = 111, 78 boys, Age [years]: mean = 9.54, std = 2.109, range = 5.58–15.92). At the clinic, children were recruited on the basis of ongoing problems in attention, learning and memory reported by professionals working in schools or specialist children's community services. Exclusion criteria for referrals were a known history of brain injury, significant or severe known problems in vision or hearing that were uncorrected and having a native language other than English. This study was approved by the local NHS research ethics committee (Reference: 13/EE/0157). Written parental consent was obtained and children provided verbal assent. Children attending the clinic completed a cognitive test battery administered over approximately 3 hours. Here, we report data from the working memory measures in this battery.
The second study investigated the neural, cognitive, and environmental markers of risk and resilience in children, and recruited a broad community sample (n = 42, 24 boys, Age [years]: mean = 9.95, std = 1.528, range = 7.17–12.42). Children attending mainstream school in the UK with normal or corrected‐to‐normal vision or hearing and no history of brain injury were recruited via local schools and through advertisements in public places (childcare and community centres, libraries). Participating families were invited to the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit for a 2‐hour assessment that included the working memory battery reported here. Participants received monetary compensation for taking part in the study. This study was approved by the Psychology Research Ethics Committee at the University of Cambridge (Reference: Pre.2015.11). Parents provided written informed consent.
The final sample for behavioural analysis consisted of 153 children between 5.58 and 15.92 years (96 boys, Age [years]: mean = 9.65, std = 1.975, range = 5–15, see Figure 2). Thirty‐one children were excluded from cortical thickness analysis because the T1‐weighted data were not usable due to participant movement (n = 122, 67 boys, Age [years]: mean = 9.57, std = 2.143, range = 5–15). Forty‐one children were excluded from the analysis of diffusion‐weighted data due to head movement above 3 mm in the DWI sequence (n = 112, 67 boys, Age [years]: mean = 9.64, std = 1.911, range = 5–15). Residual movement estimates were included as a nuisance variable in regression models. As these measures did not influence the results, they were omitted from the reported models.
2.2. Working memory assessment
The Digit Recall, Backward Digit Recall, Dot Matrix, and Mr X task of the Automatic Working Memory Assessment (AWMA) (Alloway, 2007; Alloway, Gathercole, Kirkwood, & Elliott, 2008) were administered individually. In Digit Recall, children repeat sequences of single‐digit numbers presented in an audio format. In Backward Digit Recall, children repeat the sequence in backwards order. These tasks were selected to engage verbal short‐term and working memory, respectively. For the Dot Matrix task, the child was shown the position of a red dot for 2 seconds in a series of four by four matrices and had to recall this position by tapping the squares on the computer screen. In the Mr X task, the child was shown two Mr X figures and had to identify whether they were holding the ball in the same or different hands. One Mr X was rotated in each trial. The child then had to recall the location of the ball in Mr X's hand by pointing to one of eight compass points. These tasks were aimed at tapping short‐term and working visuospatial memory.
Standardized scores established that the sample performed at expected levels for their age, that is, mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15 (Digit Recall: mean = 96.39; std = 16.32; Backward Digit Recall: mean = 94.61, std = 12.671; Dot Matrix: mean = 98.29, std = 15.595; Mr X: mean = 99.32, std = 15.69).
In order to reconstruct the latent variable structure of working memory from the assessment data, principal component analysis was applied. This was carried out using the ‘principal’ function of the psych package v1.5.1 (http://personality-project.org/r) in R v3.1.3 (R Development Core Team, 2008). Varimax rotation was used to create orthogonal factors (Kaiser, 1958). A three‐factor solution provided the best fit with theoretical predictions and explained a large proportion of variance in the assessment scores (92% of the variance in the raw scores). An additional benefit of using the three‐factor solution is that our findings can be readily interpreted alongside, and usefully integrated with, the large behavioural literature on typical and atypical working memory development. Mahalanobis distance was computed to detect outliers in the assessment data, but no data point exceeded the standard cut‐off at 3 degrees of freedom.
2.3. MRI data acquisition
Magnetic resonance imaging data were acquired at the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge UK. All scans were obtained on the Siemens 3 T Tim Trio system (Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany), using a 32‐channel quadrature head coil. The imaging protocol consisted of two sequences: T1‐weighted MRI and a diffusion‐weighted sequence.
T1‐weighted volume scans were acquired using a whole brain coverage 3D Magnetization Prepared Rapid Acquisition Gradient Echo (MP‐RAGE) sequence acquired using 1 mm isometric image resolution. Echo time was 2.98 ms, and repetition time was 2250 ms.
Diffusion scans were acquired using echo‐planar diffusion‐weighted images with an isotropic set of 60 non‐collinear directions, using a weighting factor of b = 1000s*mm−2, interleaved with a T2‐weighted (b = 0) volume. Whole brain coverage was obtained with 60 contiguous axial slices and isometric image resolution of 2 mm. Echo time was 90 ms and repetition time was 8400 ms.
2.4. Processing of diffusion‐weighted data
Diffusion imaging makes it possible to quantify the rate of water diffusion in the brain. In the parallel bundles of white matter, diffusion is stronger along the fibre orientation but is attenuated in the perpendicular direction. This can be summarized by the metric of fractional anisotropy (FA), which is a scalar value between 0 and 1 describing the degree of anisotropy of the diffusion at every voxel. Developmental studies show steady increases in FA between childhood and adulthood (Imperati et al., 2011; Muftuler et al., 2012; Westlye et al., 2009), which is likely to reflect increased myelination (Dean et al., 2014).
A number of processing steps are necessary to derive FA maps from diffusion‐weighted volumes. In the current study, diffusion‐weighted MRI scans were converted from the native DICOM to compressed NIfTI‐1 format using the dcm2nii tool (http://www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/mricro/mricron/dcm2nii.html). Subsequently, the images were submitted to the DiPy v0.8.0 implementation (Garyfallidis et al., 2014) of a non‐local means de‐noising algorithm (Coupe et al., 2008) to boost the signal‐to‐noise ratio. Next, a brain mask of the b0 image was created using the brain extraction tool (BET) of the FMRIB Software Library (FSL) v5.0.8. Motion and eddy current correction were applied to the masked images using FSL routines. The corrected images were re‐sliced to 1 mm resolution with trilinear interpolation using in‐house software based on NiBabel v2.0.0 functions (http://nipy.org/nibabel/). Finally, fractional anisotropy maps were created based on a diffusion tensor model fitted through the FSL dtifit algorithm (Behrens et al., 2003; Johansen‐Berg et al., 2004).
For comparison across participants, we created a study‐specific FA‐template based on all available images using Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs) algorithms (Avants et al., 2014; Lawson, Duda, Avants, Wu, & Farah, 2013), which showed the highest accuracy in software comparisons (Klein et al., 2009; Murphy et al., 2011; Tustison et al., 2014). Individual images were transformed to template space using non‐linear registration with symmetric diffeomorphic normalization as implemented in ANTs (Avants, Epstein, Grossman, & Gee, 2008). Next, the images were eroded twice with a 3 mm sphere using FSL maths to remove brain edge artefacts.
2.5. Processing of T1‐weighted data
Another measure of brain development that can be derived from neuroimaging data is cortical thickness (Giedd & Rapoport, 2010; Gogtay et al., 2004). Cortical thickness is defined as the distance between the outer edge of cortical grey matter and subcortical white matter (Fischl & Dale, 2000). To obtain thickness measures from anatomical MRI data, T1‐weighted volumes were initially co‐registered with MNI152 space using rigid co‐registration to obtain good initial between‐subject alignment and optimal field of view. Next, all images were visually inspected and images with pronounced motion artefact were removed from further analysis (n = 31, 20.25% of the acquired data). The remaining data were submitted to the automatic ANTs cortical thickness pipeline (antsCorticalThickness). Details about the processing pipeline and thickness estimation are described in Tustison et al. (2014) and Das, Avants, Grossman, and Gee (2009). Tissue priors were taken from the OASIS‐TRT‐20 template (http://www.mindboggle.info/data.html#mindboggle-software-data). Subsequently, images in template space were smoothed using a 10 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel and resampled to 2 mm resolution. A thickness mask was created by averaging all images and binarizing the resulting mean image at a threshold of 0.1.
2.6. Eigenanatomy decomposition
Traditional univariate approaches such as voxel‐based morphometry (VBM) fit a statistical model for every voxel in a brain image. The large number of voxels in a typical imaging protocol necessitates correction for a very large number of comparisons (T1‐volumes in the current study contained over 1 million voxels), and this results in a substantial loss of statistical power. However, effects are typically spread over areas that are larger than 1 voxel. Multivariate approaches are better suited to reduce the dimensionality of the data to the information contained in the data themselves before statistical comparisons are applied. Eigenanatomy decomposition is a novel method for data‐driven dimensionality reduction of neuroimaging data that adds sparseness and smoothness constraints for better anatomical interpretability in comparison to standard spatial principal component analysis (Kandel, Wang, Gee, & Avants, 2015). Cortical thickness masks and FA images were processed using the ANTsR v0.3.2 implementation of the eigenanatomy decomposition algorithm (Kandel et al., 2015). Parameters for eigenanatomy decomposition were adopted from published work, that is, decomposition into 32 components with a sparseness of 1/32 with 20 iterations, an L1 penalty with gradient step size 0.5, a smoothing kernel of 1 voxel, and a minimum cluster size of 1000 voxels for each eigenvector. For statistical analysis, the mean value of each brain morphology measure (FA, cortical thickness) within each eigenanatomy component was calculated. See Figure 3 for an illustration of the resulting parcellation.
2.7. Statistical analysis
Our aim was to examine whether and how brain morphology is associated with the components of the working memory system, and the extent to which this relationship is moderated by age. The relationship between these factors was tested in the following set of regression models: (a) age predicting working memory performance, (b) age predicting brain morphology measures, (c) brain morphology predicting working memory; and ultimately (d) the interaction between brain morphology and age predicting working memory (see Figure 4 for an overview of these models). Gender and an intercept term were included as additional regressors in each model. Models for cortical thickness contained intracranial volume as an additional regressor of no interest. Assessment of Cook's distance (Cook, 1977) indicated no particularly influential data points in the regression models. Therefore, all available data points were retained in the analysis. Regression analysis was carried out using the ‘stats’ package v3.1.2 in Rbase. Bonferroni correction was applied to account for multiple comparisons and the adjusted p‐values are reported as p corrected.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Factor analysis of behavioural data
Principal component analysis (PCA) was applied to the raw scores of the working memory battery to derive the latent variable structure of working memory. Assessment of Mahalanobis distance did not indicate outliers in the cognitive scores (Maximum distance D2(4) = 15.542, critical value = 18.47). Correlations between raw scores were moderate to high (range: 0.39 to 0.63). The three‐factor PCA solution explained 92% of the variance in the raw scores. Factor loadings are shown in Table 1.
Table 1.
Verbal Factor | Executive Factor | Visuo‐spatial Factor | |
---|---|---|---|
Digit Recall | 0.95 | 0.15 | 0.18 |
Backward Digit Recall | 0.55 | 0.50 | 0.47 |
Dot Matrix | 0.22 | 0.27 | 0.93 |
Mr. X | 0.17 | 0.94 | 0.26 |
Proportion explained | 0.35 | 0.33 | 0.32 |
Cumulative proportion | 0.35 | 0.68 | 1.00 |
3.2. Working memory performance improves with age
Linear regression indicated that age was significantly associated with increases in working memory scores (Effects of age including gender as a nuisance regressor: Verbal factor: F(2, 150) = 4.538, p = .012, R 2 = 0.057, R 2 Adjusted = 0.044, βAge = 0.010, t Age(150) = 2.99, p = .003; Executive factor: F(2, 150) = 6.506, p = .002, R 2 = 0.079, R 2 Adjusted = 0.068, βAge = 0.003, t Age(150) = 3.09, p = .002; Spatial factor: F(2, 150) = 16, p < .001, R 2 = 0.176, R 2 Adjusted = 0.165, βAge = 0.018, t Age(150) = 5.66, p < .001; see Figure 5a). Comparison with alternative quadratic and cubic models using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) as a measure of parsimony (Akaike, 1974) suggested that a linear model provided the best account for the relationship between age and factor scores in the current data (Verbal factor: AIClinear = 433.12, AICquadratic = 432.21, AICcubic = 427.54; Executive factor: AIClinear = 428.46, AICquadratic = 429.52, AICcubic = 431.51; Spatial factor: AIClinear = 408.91, AICquadratic = 409.94, AICcubic = 411.61).
3.3. FA increases with age
In order to assess the relationship between each measure of brain morphology and participant age, a linear regression analysis was carried out (FA: yFA = βAgeXAge + βGenderXGender + βIntercept + ε; Cortical thickness: yThickness = βAgeXAge + βGenderXGender + βICVXICV + βIntercept + ε). For FA, the results indicated a significant effect of age in 30 of the 32 components after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. The effect was marginal for the remaining two components after correction for multiple comparisons (p < .051). The slopes were positive for all components (βAge: mean = 0.22, SD = 0.03, Range = 0.16–0.29, based on z‐scores; see Figure 5b), indicating that FA increased with age for all eigenanatomy components. For cortical thickness, the results indicated no significant relationship with age (βAge: mean = 0.05, SE = 0.01, Range = −0.08–0.18, based on z‐scores).
3.4. FA predicts differences in executive scores
Next, the relationship between brain morphology and factor scores was assessed (FA: yFactor = βFAXFA + βGenderXGender + βIntercept + ε; Cortical thickness: yFactor = βThicknessXThickness + βICVXICV + βGenderXGender + βIntercept + ε). There was no significant effect of FA in any eigenanatomy component for the verbal and visuospatial storage factor after correction for multiple comparisons. There were significant effects of FA in 16 eigenanatomy components for the executive factor (see Table 2). For cortical thickness, the results indicated no significant effect of cortical thickness within any of the 32 eigenanatomy components on scores for any of the working memory factors (corrected‐p > .05). In summary, FA predicted working memory capacity associated with the executive factor, while cortical thickness was not significantly associated with any working memory constructs.
Table 2.
Volume | x | y | z | β | tstat | p | p corrected | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Comp 2 | 46439 | 90.71 | 83.23 | 84.62 | 0.47 | 3.09 | 0.003 | 0.01 | |
Comp 3 | 41374 | 89.78 | 101.47 | 59.97 | 0.48 | 2.77 | 0.007 | 0.03 | |
Comp 4 | 46585 | 91.01 | 107.09 | 85.48 | 0.49 | 2.74 | 0.007 | 0.031 | |
Comp 6 | 32929 | 106.63 | 119.04 | 96.72 | 0.45 | 2.64 | 0.009 | 0.041 | |
Comp 7 | 33840 | 78.00 | 92.32 | 57.81 | 0.48 | 2.68 | 0.009 | 0.035 | |
Comp 8 | 23804 | 74.90 | 152.59 | 82.53 | 0.46 | 2.88 | 0.005 | 0.03 | |
Comp 9 | 24972 | 125.41 | 106.25 | 65.33 | 0.51 | 3.15 | 0.002 | 0.008 | |
Comp 10 | 36812 | 97.46 | 75.41 | 92.97 | 0.5 | 3.24 | 0.002 | 0.007 | |
Comp 13 | 27930 | 98.03 | 82.84 | 99.30 | 0.49 | 2.91 | 0.004 | 0.019 | |
Comp 16 | 20309 | 76.13 | 101.56 | 101.69 | 0.41 | 2.71 | 0.008 | 0.04 | |
Comp 20 | 20608 | 110.17 | 112.02 | 100.88 | 0.46 | 2.79 | 0.006 | 0.033 | |
Comp 22 | 18040 | 86.64 | 98.73 | 71.46 | 0.49 | 2.9 | 0.005 | 0.017 | |
Comp 24 | 21049 | 64.55 | 115.31 | 75.93 | 0.51 | 2.73 | 0.007 | 0.033 | |
Comp 25 | 15867 | 94.10 | 104.76 | 67.27 | 0.53 | 3.11 | 0.002 | 0.01 | |
Comp 27 | 19398 | 66.55 | 116.81 | 99.49 | 0.51 | 2.99 | 0.003 | 0.02 | |
Comp 28 | 13506 | 54.45 | 102.02 | 102.15 | 0.46 | 2.65 | 0.009 | 0.036 |
Finally, the extent to which the relationship between brain morphology and components of the working memory system is moderated by age was investigated. The regression model for this analysis controlled the linear contributions of age, gender, and FA within the eigenanatomy component, and also contained a term for the interaction between FA and age which was the main focus of this analysis (yFactor = βAgeXAge + βFAXFA + d(XAge × XFA) + βGenderXGender + βIntercept + ε). The results indicated a significant effect of the interaction between age and FA on the executive factor in two eigenanatomy components (Corpus callosum component: β = −0.337, t(5) = −3.35, p = .001, p corrected = .036; Occipitotemporal white matter component: β = −0.368, t(5) = −3.32, p = .001, p corrected = .039; see Figure 6).
For cortical thickness, intracranial volume was included as an additional regressor of no interest (yFactor = βAgeXAge + βThicknessXThickness + d(XAge × XThickness) + βGenderXGender + βICVXICV + βIntercept + ε). The results of the regression analysis indicated a significant interaction between age and cortical thickness for one eigenanatomy component (left temporal thickness component: β = 0.56, t(5) = −0.91, p = .002, p corrected = .049).
4. DISCUSSION
The aim of the current study was to explore how structural brain correlates of working memory capacity differ with age. The neural structures associated with the executive component of the working memory system were shown not to be invariant across age, but to interact with it. Specifically, the corpus callosum and bilateral posterior temporal white matter, and cortical thickness in the left occipitotemporal cortex made differential contributions to the executive component of working memory according to age.
Performance on four tasks was used to assess the latent structure of working memory. Distinct factors were identified for verbal and visuo‐spatial storage with an additional factor contributing to tasks with a higher executive demand load regardless of domain (see also Alloway et al., 2004; Bayliss et al., 2003; Kane et al., 2007). Factor scores were linearly related to age for all factors, replicating previous studies that indicate linear increases in short‐term and working memory capacity throughout childhood and adolescence (Conklin, Luciana, Hooper, & Yarger, 2007; Gathercole et al., 2004; Swanson, 1999).
4.1. White matter organization but not cortical thickness show differences with age
Next, the aspects of neurophysiology that show the greatest degrees of age‐related differences were investigated. Of particular interest was the anatomy of white matter. White matter changes are thought to be a key process in postnatal brain development, which continues throughout childhood and adolescence into early adulthood (Barnea‐Goraly, 2005; Muftuler et al., 2012; Qiu, Tan, Zhou, & Khong, 2008; Tau & Peterson, 2010). In particular, the myelination of axons is thought to be a critical mechanism of brain development in this age range (Miller et al., 2012). Differences in microstructural properties, namely FA, as measured by diffusion MRI are directly related to myelination and have been linked to cognitive development (Clayden et al., 2012; Mabbott, Noseworthy, Bouffet, Laughlin, & Rockel, 2006). In the current study, FA was also significantly related to age.
In contrast, cortical thickness was not related to age. This was unexpected as multiple studies have reported decreasing cortical thickness with age (Sowell, 2004; Tamnes et al., 2009; Tamnes et al., 2010; Wierenga, Langen, Oranje, & Durston, 2014). However, these studies included participants from early childhood to adulthood (Tamnes et al., 2009, Sowell et al., 2006, Wierenga et al., 2014), or mapped changes longitudinally over a shorter period (Shaw et al., 2006; Sowell, 2004). Our narrower age range could account for the absence of age‐related differences in cortical thickness in the current study. Even so, our data indicate that FA is a more sensitive indicator of brain development in the 6‐ to 16‐year age range, and that neural differences across this age span are largely mediated by the maturation of structural connections and integration within brain systems.
4.2. Brain morphology and age interact in the development of the executive component of working memory
Many studies indicate that neural correlates of cognitive development show a shift from using general brain systems in younger children to an adult‐like recruitment of specialized networks of regions in older participants (Johnson, 2011). This developmental tendency has been demonstrated for the processing of both faces (Kadosh, Johnson, Henson, Dick, & Blakemore, 2013; Kadosh, Kadosh, Dick, & Johnson, 2010) and language (Weiss‐Croft & Baldeweg, 2015). Functional neuroimaging studies indicate that this developmental progression may also apply to working memory. Children show higher blood oxygenation in additional regions beyond the core working memory areas found in adults (Ciesielski et al., 2006; Crone, Wendelken, Donohue, van Leijenhorst, & Bunge, 2006; Scherf, Sweeney, & Luna, 2006; Vogan et al., 2016). However, differences in the contribution of brain structure with development have not been investigated so far.
The current study indicates that younger children's working memory capacity is more closely associated with the microstructural integrity of white matter components than is the capacity of older children and adolescents. In contrast, higher focal cortical thickness in areas associated with working memory is associated with lower levels of performance in older but not in younger children. In short, the executive aspects of working memory are supported by different brain systems across this age range. The greater importance of large white matter connections in younger children suggests that younger children are relying on a more distributed system. In contrast, the greater importance of cortical thickness in the left posterior temporal lobe demonstrates the importance of this local processing in later stages of working memory development.
There exists a relatively large literature discussing the potential functional role of the particular structures that were implicated in our findings. To summarize briefly: it has been hypothesized that interhemispheric connections of the corpus callosum provide inhibition between functionally homologous areas in the left and right hemisphere (Gazzaniga, 2000). Differences in corpus callosum anatomy such as reduced volume or reduced microstructural integrity are related to lower lateralization of function in typical participants and patient groups (Hinkley et al., 2016; Just, Cherkassky, Keller, Kana, & Minshew, 2007; Persson et al., 2006). In turn, lower lateralization is associated with lower performance on cognitive tasks, including executive function tasks (Hinkley et al., 2012; Just et al., 2007; Nagel, Herting, Maxwell, Bruno, & Fair, 2013). Similarly, posterior temporal white matter may provide connections for integration between specialized regions of temporal lobe for verbal and visuospatial working memory with the posterior parietal executive attention network. Posterior temporal white matter has also been shown to relate to working memory performance in typical adults (Burzynska et al., 2011; Golestani et al., 2014) and in lesion studies (Finke, Bublak, & Zihl, 2006; Palacios et al., 2012).
With structural data alone, we are left to speculate as to the functional role of these connections and their changing contribution to working memory capacity across development. But our findings make a more fundamental point – despite the factor structure of working memory being stable across childhood (Alloway et al., 2004; Gathercole et al., 2004; Luciana et al., 2005), the neural systems associated with it change. Impairments of working memory are a consistent feature of numerous neurodevelopmental disorders (Archibald & Gathercole, 2006; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1989; Holmes et al., 2014; Martinussen et al., 2005; Montgomery, 2000; Rotzer et al., 2009; Smith‐Spark & Fisk, 2007; Szucs et al., 2013; Weismer et al., 1999), in addition to being an important constraint on learning within the typically developing population (Cain et al., 2004; Clair‐Thompson & Gathercole, 2006; Cowan, 2013; Dumontheil & Klingberg, 2011; Gathercole, Tiffany, Briscoe, & Thorn, 2005; Gathercole et al., 2003). Understanding the nature of these impairments, and providing a plausible neuropsychological account for them, will require a developmentally informed model of brain–behaviour relationships. Differences in capacity will likely be underpinned by a different combination of neural systems, depending upon the age of the child.
5. CONCLUSION
The current study investigated whether the relationship between individual differences in brain structure and working memory performance varies with age. There was clear evidence of differences in the neural underpinnings of the executive component of working memory, with a shift from a higher contribution of callosal and temporal white matter in younger children to a greater dependence on left temporal cortex in older children. The current study can be characterized as a developmental progression from an early distributed system supported by long‐range connections to later reliance on specialized local circuitry.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The Centre for Attention Learning and Memory (CALM) research clinic at the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit in Cambridge (CBSU) is supported by funding from the Medical Research Council of Great Britain to Duncan Astle, Susan Gathercole and Tom Manly. The clinic is led by Joni Holmes and managed by Francesca Woolgar. Data collection is assisted by a team of PhD students and researchers at the CBSU that includes Agnieszka Jaroslawska, Sally Butterfield, Amy Johnson, Erica Bottacin, Erin Hawkins, Gemma Crickmore, Laura Forde, Sinead O'Brien, and Sara Gharooni. The authors wish to thank the many professionals working in children's services in the South‐East and East of England for their support, and to the children and their families for giving up their time to visit the clinic.
Bathelt J, Gathercole SE, Johnson A, Astle DE. Differences in brain morphology and working memory capacity across childhood. Dev Sci. 2018;21:e12579 https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12579
REFERENCES
- Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19, 716–723. [Google Scholar]
- Alloway, T. (2007). Automated working memory assessment. Oxford: Pearson Assessment. [Google Scholar]
- Alloway, T.P. , Gathercole, S.E. , Kirkwood, H. , & Elliott, J. (2008). Evaluating the validity of the Automated Working Memory Assessment. Educational Psychology, 28, 725–734. [Google Scholar]
- Alloway, T.P. , Gathercole, S.E. , Willis, C. , & Adams, A.‐M. (2004). A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 85–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Archibald, L.M. , & Gathercole, S.E. (2006). Short‐term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41, 675–693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avants, B. , Epstein, C. , Grossman, M. , & Gee, J. (2008). Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross‐correlation: Evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Medical Image Analysis, 12, 26–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avants, B.B. , Tustison, N.J. , Stauffer, M. , Song, G. , Wu, B. , & Gee, J.C. (2014). The Insight ToolKit image registration framework. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 8, 44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A. (1987). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 49, 5–28. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 417–423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A.D. , & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory In Bower G.H. (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 47–89). Amsterdam: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A.D. , & Lieberman, K. (1980). Spatial working memory In Nickerson R.S. (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 521–539). London and New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. [Google Scholar]
- Barnea‐Goraly, N. (2005). White matter development during childhood and adolescence: A cross‐sectional diffusion tensor imaging study. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1848–1854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrouillet, P. , Gavens, N. , Vergauwe, E. , Gaillard, V. , & Camos, V. (2009). Working memory span development: A time‐based resource‐sharing model account. Developmental Psychology, 45, 477–490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayliss, D.M. , Jarrold, C. , Gunn, D.M. , & Baddeley, A.D. (2003). The complexities of complex span: Explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 71–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behrens, T. , Woolrich, M. , Jenkinson, M. , Johansen‐Berg, H. , Nunes, R. , Clare, S. , … Smith, S.M. (2003). Characterization and propagation of uncertainty in diffusion‐weighted MR imaging. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 50, 1077–1088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess, N. , & Hitch, G.J. (1996). A connectionist model of STM for serial order In Gathercole S. (Ed.), Models of short‐term memory (pp. 51–72). Hove, East Sussex: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Burzynska, A.Z. , Nagel, I.E. , Preuschhof, C. , Li, S.‐C. , Lindenberger, U. , Backman, L. , & Heekeren, H.R. (2011). Microstructure of frontoparietal connections predicts cortical responsivity and working memory performance. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 2261–2271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cain, K. , Oakhill, J. , & Bryant, P. (2004). Children's reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31–42. [Google Scholar]
- Ciesielski, K.T. , Lesnik, P.G. , Savoy, R.L. , Grant, E.P. , & Ahlfors, S.P. (2006). Developmental neural networks in children performing a Categorical N‐Back Task. NeuroImage, 33, 980–990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clair‐Thompson, H.L.S. , & Gathercole, S.E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting updating, inhibition, and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 745–759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clayden, J.D. , Jentschke, S. , Muñoz, M. , Cooper, J.M. , Chadwick, M.J. , Banks, T. , … Vargha‐Khadem, F. (2012). Normative development of white matter tracts: Similarities and differences in relation to age, gender, and intelligence. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 1738–1747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conklin, H.M. , Luciana, M. , Hooper, C.J. , & Yarger, R.S. (2007). Working memory performance in typically developing children and adolescents: Behavioral evidence of protracted frontal lobe development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 31, 103–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conway, A.R. , Cowan, N. , Bunting, M.F. , Therriault, D.J. , & Minkoff, S.R. (2002). A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short‐term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30, 163–183. [Google Scholar]
- Cook, R.D. (1977). Detection of influential observation in linear regression. Technometrics, 19, 15–18. [Google Scholar]
- Coupe, P. , Yger, P. , Prima, S. , Hellier, P. , Kervrann, C. , & Barillot, C. (2008). An optimized blockwise nonlocal means denoising filter for 3‐D magnetic resonance images. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 27, 425–441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 163–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded‐processes model of working memory In Miyake A. & Shah P. (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 62–101). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cowan, N. (2013). Working memory underpins cognitive development, learning, and education. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 197–223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan, N. , Ricker, T.J. , Clark, K.M. , Hinrichs, G.A. , & Glass, B.A. (2014). Knowledge cannot explain the developmental growth of working memory capacity. Developmental Science, 18, 132–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crone, E.A. , Wendelken, C. , Donohue, S. , van Leijenhorst, L. , & Bunge, S.A. (2006). Neurocognitive development of the ability to manipulate information in working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 103, 9315–9320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dark, V.J. , & Benbow, C.P. (1994). Type of stimulus mediates the relationship between working‐memory performance and type of precocity. Intelligence, 19, 337–357. [Google Scholar]
- Das, S.R. , Avants, B.B. , Grossman, M. , & Gee, J.C. (2009). Registration based cortical thickness measurement. NeuroImage, 45, 867–879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dean, D.C. , O'Muircheartaigh, J. , Dirks, H. , Waskiewicz, N. , Walker, L. , Doernberg, E. , … Deoni, S.C. (2014). Characterizing longitudinal white matter development during early childhood. Brain Structure and Function, 220, 1921–1933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumontheil, I. , & Klingberg, T. (2011). Brain activity during a visuospatial working memory task predicts arithmetical performance 2 years later. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 1078–1085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ecker, U.K. , Lewandowsky, S. , Oberauer, K. , & Chee, A.E. (2010). The components of working memory updating: An experimental decomposition and individual differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 170–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engle, R.W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23. [Google Scholar]
- Engle, R.W. , Tuholski, S.W. , Laughlin, J.E. , & Conway, A.R. (1999). Working memory, short‐term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent‐variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finke, K. , Bublak, P. , & Zihl, J. (2006). Visual spatial and visual pattern working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for a differential role of left and right dorsal visual brain. Neuropsychologia, 44, 649–661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl, B. , & Dale, A.M. (2000). Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 97, 11050–11055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garyfallidis, E. , Brett, M. , Amirbekian, B. , Rokem, A. , van der Walt, S. , Descoteaux, M. , & Nimmo‐Smith, I. (2014). Dipy, a library for the analysis of diffusion MRI data. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 8, 8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole, S.E. , Adams, A.‐M. , & Hitch, G.J. (1994). Do young children rehearse? An individual‐differences analysis. Memory & Cognition, 22, 201–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole, S.E. , & Baddeley, A.D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213. [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole, S.E. , Pickering, S.J. , Ambridge, B. , & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole, S.E. , Pickering, S.J. , Knight, C. , & Stegmann, Z. (2003). Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1–16. [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole, S.E. , Tiffany, C. , Briscoe, J. , & Thorn, A. (2005). Developmental consequences of poor phonological short‐term memory function in childhood: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 598–611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gazzaniga, M.S. (2000). Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: Does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain, 123, 1293–1326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giedd, J.N. , & Rapoport, J.L. (2010). Structural MRI of pediatric brain development: what have we learned and where are we going? Neuron, 67, 728–734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gogtay, N. , Giedd, J.N. , Lusk, L. , Hayashi, K.M. , Greenstein, D. , Vaituzis, A.C. , … Thompson, P.M. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 101, 8174–8179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golestani, A.M. , Miles, L. , Babb, J. , Castellanos, F.X. , Malaspina, D. , & Lazar, M. (2014). Constrained by our connections: White matter's key role in interindividual variability in visual working memory vapacity. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 14913–14918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray, S. , Green, S. , Alt, M. , Hogan, T. , Kuo, T. , Brinkley, S. , & Cowan, N. (2017). The structure of working memory in young children and its relation to intelligence. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 183–201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinkley, L.B.N. , Marco, E.J. , Brown, E.G. , Bukshpun, P. , Gold, J. , Hill, S. , … Nagarajan, S.S. (2016). The contribution of the corpus callosum to language lateralization. Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 4522–4533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinkley, L.B.N. , Marco, E.J. , Findlay, A.M. , Honma, S. , Jeremy, R.J. , Strominger, Z. , … Sherr, E.H. (2012). The role of corpus callosum development in functional connectivity and cognitive processing. PLoS ONE, 7, e39804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hitch, G.J. , Halliday, M. , Schaafstal, A.M. , & Heffernan, T.M. (1991). Speech ‘inner speech’, and the development of short‐term memory: Effects of picture‐labeling on recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 220–234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes, J. , Hilton, K.A. , Place, M. , Alloway, T.P. , Elliott, J.G. , & Gathercole, S.E. (2014). Children with low working memory and children with ADHD: Same or different? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hornung, C. , Brunner, M. , Reuter, R.A. , & Martin, R. (2011). Children's working memory: Its structure and relationship to fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 39, 210–221. [Google Scholar]
- Houde, O. , Rossi, S. , Lubin, L. , & Joliot, M. (2010). Mapping numerical processing, reading, and executive functions in the developing brain: An fMRI meta‐analysis on 52 studies including 842 children. Developmental Science, 13, 876–885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huizinga, M. , Dolan, C.V. , & van der Molen, M.W. (2006). Age‐related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2017–2036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imperati, D. , Colcombe, S. , Kelly, C. , Martino, A.D. , Zhou, J. , Castellanos, F.X. , & Milham, M.P. (2011). Differential development of human brain white matter tracts. PLoS ONE, 6, e23437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarrold, C. (2016). The Mid‐Career Award: Working out how working memory works: Evidence from typical and atypical development. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9, 1747–1767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansen‐Berg, H. , Behrens, T.E.J. , Robson, M.D. , Drobnjak, I. , Rushworth, M.F.S. , Brady, J.M. , Smith, S.M. , … Matthews, P.M. (2004). Changes in connectivity profiles define functionally distinct regions in human medial frontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 101, 13335–13340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, M.H. (2011). Interactive specialization: A domain‐general framework for human functional brain development? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 7–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Just, M.A. , Cherkassky, V.L. , Keller, T.A. , Kana, R.K. , & Minshew, N.J. (2007). Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: Evidence from an fMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 951–961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kadosh, K.C. , Johnson, M.H. , Henson, R.N. , Dick, F. , & Blakemore, S.‐J. (2013). Differential face‐network adaptation in children, adolescents and adults. NeuroImage, 69, 11–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kadosh, K.C. , Kadosh, R.C. , Dick, F. , & Johnson, M.H. (2010). Developmental changes in effective connectivity in the emerging core face network. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 1389–1394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaiser, H.F. (1958). The varimax criterion for analytic rotation in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 23, 187–200. [Google Scholar]
- Kandel, B.M. , Wang, D.J. , Gee, J.C. , & Avants, B.B. (2015). Eigenanatomy: Sparse dimensionality reduction for multi‐modal medical image analysis. Methods, 73, 43–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane, M.J. , Conway, A.R.A. , Hambrick, D.Z. , & Engle, R.W. (2007). Variation in working memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control In Conway A.R.A., Jarrold C., Kane M.J., Miyake A., & Towse J.N. (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 21–48). New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kane, M.J. , Hambrick, D.Z. , Tuholski, S.W. , Wilhelm, O. , Payne, T.W. , & Engle, R.W. (2004). The generality of working memory capacity: A latent‐variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 189–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein, A. , Andersson, J. , Ardekani, B.A. , Ashburner, J. , Avants, B. , & Chiang, M.‐C. (2009). Evaluation of 14 nonlinear deformation algorithms applied to human brain MRI registration. NeuroImage, 46, 786–802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawson, G.M. , Duda, J.T. , Avants, B.B. , Wu, J. , & Farah, M.J. (2013). Associations between children's socioeconomic status and prefrontal cortical thickness. Developmental Science, 16, 641–652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logie, R.H. (1986). Visuospatial processing in working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 38, 229–247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luciana, M. , Conklin, H.M. , Hooper, C.J. , & Yarger, R.S. (2005). The development of nonverbal working memory and executive control processes in adolescents. Child Development, 76, 697–712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mabbott, D.J. , Noseworthy, M. , Bouffet, E. , Laughlin, S. , & Rockel, C. (2006). White matter growth as a mechanism of cognitive development in children. NeuroImage, 33, 936–946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mahone, E.M. , Martin, R. , Kates, W.R. , Hay, T. , & Horska, A. (2009). Neuroimaging correlates of parent ratings of working memory in typically developing children. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 31–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinussen, R. , Hayden, J. , Hogg‐Johnson, S. , & Tannock, R. (2005). A meta‐analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 377–384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, D.J. , Duka, T. , Stimpson, C.D. , Schapiro, S.J. , Baze, W.B. , McArthur, M.J. , … Sherwood, C.C (2012). Prolonged myelination in human neocortical evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 16480–16485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montgomery, J.W. (2000). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 293–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muftuler, L.T. , Davis, E.P. , Buss, C. , Solodkin, A. , Su, M.Y. , Head, K.M. , & Sandman, C.A. (2012). Development of white matter pathways in typically developing preadolescent children. Brain Research, 1466, 33–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy, K. , van Ginneken, B. , Reinhardt, J.M. , Kabus, S. , Ding, K. , & Deng, X. (2011). Evaluation of registration methods on thoracic CT: The EMPIRE10 challenge. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 30, 1901–1920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagel, B.J. , Herting, M.M. , Maxwell, E.C. , Bruno, R. , & Fair, D. (2013). Hemispheric lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory during adolescence. Brain and Cognition, 82, 58–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer, K. (2002). Access to information in working memory: Exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 411–421. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer, K. , Lewandowsky, S. , Farrell, S. , Jarrold, C. , & Greaves, M. (2012). Modeling working memory: An interference model of complex span. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19, 779–819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer, K. , Süß, H.‐M. , Schulze, R. , Wilhelm, O. , & Wittmann, W. (2000). Working memory capacity: Facets of a cognitive ability construct. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 1017–1045. [Google Scholar]
- Owen, A.M. , McMillan, K.M. , Laird, A.R. , & Bullmore, E. (2005). N‐back working memory paradigm: A meta‐analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 46–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palacios, E.M. , Sala‐Llonch, R. , Junque, C. , Roig, T. , Tormos, J.M. , Bargallo, N. , & Vendrell, P. (2012). White matter integrity related to functional working memory networks in traumatic brain injury. Neurology, 78, 852–860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Persson, J. , Lind, J. , Larsson, A. , Ingvar, M. , Cruts, M. , Van Broeckhoven, C. , Adolfsson, R. , Nilsson, L.‐G. , & Nyberg, L. (2006). Altered brain white matter integrity in healthy carriers of the APOE epsilon4 allele: A risk for AD? Neurology, 66, 1029–1033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack, R.A. , & Yarkoni, T. (2016). From brain maps to cognitive ontologies: Informatics and the search for mental structure. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 587–612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qiu, D. , Tan, L.‐H. , Zhou, K. , & Khong, P.‐L. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging of normal white matter maturation from late childhood to young adulthood: Voxel‐wise evaluation of mean diffusivity fractional anisotropy, radial and axial diffusivities, and correlation with reading development. NeuroImage, 41, 223–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Development Core Team (2008). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: http://www.R-project.org
- Rossi, S. , Lubin, A. , Simon, G. , Lanoë, C. , Poirel, N. , Cachia, A. , … Houde, O. (2013). Structural brain correlates of executive engagement in working memory: Children's inter‐individual differences are reflected in the anterior insular cortex. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1145–1150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rotzer, S. , Loenneker, T. , Kucian, K. , Martin, E. , Klaver, P. , & von Aster, M. (2009). Dysfunctional neural network of spatial working memory contributes to developmental dyscalculia. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2859–2865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scherf, K.S. , Sweeney, J.A. , & Luna, B. (2006). Brain basis of developmental change in visuospatial working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1045–1058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmiedek, F. , Lövdén, M. , & Lindenberger, U. (2014). A task is a task is a task: Putting complex span, n‐back, and other working memory indicators in psychometric context. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw, P. , Greenstein, D. , Lerch, J. , Clasen, L. , Lenroot, R. , Gogtay, N. , … Giedd, J. (2006). Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature, 440, 676–679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shelton, J.T. , Elliott, E.M. , Matthews, R.A. , Hill, B.D. , & Gouvier, W.D. (2010). The relationships of working memory, secondary memory, and general fluid intelligence: Working memory is special. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 813–820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel, L.S. , & Ryan, E.B. (1988). Development of grammatical‐sensitivity phonological, and short‐term memory skills in normally achieving and learning disabled children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 28–37. [Google Scholar]
- Smith‐Spark, J.H. , & Fisk, J.E. (2007). Working memory functioning in developmental dyslexia. Memory, 15, 34–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sowell, E.R. (2004). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 8223–8231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sowell, E.R. , Peterson, B.S. , Kan, E. , Woods, R.P. , Yoshii, J. , & Bansal, R. (2006). Sex differences in cortical thickness mapped in 176 healthy individuals between 7 and 87 years of age. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1550–1560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson, H.L. (1999). What develops in working memory? A life span perspective. Developmental Psychology, 35, 986–1000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szucs, D. , Devine, A. , Soltesz, F. , Nobes, A. , & Gabriel, F. (2013). Developmental dyscalculia is related to visuospatial memory and inhibition impairment. Cortex, 49, 2674–2688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tam, H. , Jarrold, C. , Baddeley, A.D. , & Sabatos‐DeVito, M. (2010). The development of memory maintenance: Children's use of phonological rehearsal and attentional refreshment in working memory tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 306–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamnes, C.K. , Ostby, Y. , Fjell, A.M. , Westlye, L.T. , Due‐Tonnessen, P. , & Walhovd, K.B. (2009). Brain maturation in adolescence and young adulthood: Regional age‐related changes in cortical thickness and white matter volume and microstructure. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 534–548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamnes, C.K. , Ostby, Y. , Walhovd, K.B. , Westlye, L.T. , Due‐Tonnessen, P. , & Fjell, A.M. (2010). Neuroanatomical correlates of executive functions in children and adolescents: A magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) study of cortical thickness. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2496–2508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamnes, C.K. , Walhovd, K.B. , Grydeland, H. , Holland, D. , Ostby, Y. , Dale, A.M. , & Fjell, A.M. (2013). Longitudinal working memory development is related to structural maturation of frontal and parietal cortices. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 1611–1623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tau, G.Z. , & Peterson, B.S. (2010). Normal development of brain circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 147–168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomason, M.E. , Race, E. , Burrows, B. , Whitfield‐Gabrieli, S. , Glover, G.H. , & Gabrieli, J.D.E. (2009). Development of spatial and verbal working memory capacity in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 316–332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treble, A. , Hasan, K.M. , Iftikhar, A. , Stuebing, K.K. , Kramer, L.A. , Cox, C.S. , … Ewing‐Cobbs, L. (2013). Working memory and corpus callosum microstructural integrity after pediatric traumatic brain injury: A diffusion tensor tractography study. Journal of Neurotrauma, 30, 1609–1619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tustison, N.J. , Cook, P.A. , Klein, A. , Song, G. , Das, S.R. , Duda, J.T. , … Avants, B.B. (2014). Large‐scale evaluation of ANTs and FreeSurfer cortical thickness measurements. NeuroImage, 99, 166–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogan, V. , Morgan, B. , Powell, T. , Smith, M. , & Taylor, M. (2016). The neurodevelopmental differences of increasing verbal working memory demand in children and adults. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 19–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Von Bastian, C.C. , & Oberauer, K. (2013). Distinct transfer effects of training different facets of working memory capacity. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 36–58. [Google Scholar]
- Wager, T.D. , & Smith, E.E. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of working memory. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 255–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, X. , Logie, R.H. , & Jarrold, C. (2016). Interpreting potential markers of storage and rehearsal: Implications for studies of verbal short‐term memory and neuropsychological cases. Memory and Cognition, 44, 910–921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weismer, S.E. , Evans, J. , & Hesketh, L.J. (1999). An examination of verbal working memory capacity in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1249–1260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss‐Croft, L.J. , & Baldeweg, T. (2015). Maturation of language networks in children: A systematic review of 22 years of functional MRI. NeuroImage, 123, 269–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westlye, L.T. , Walhovd, K.B. , Dale, A.M. , Bjornerud, A. , Due‐Tonnessen, P. , & Engvig, A. (2009). Life‐span changes of the human brain white matter: Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and volumetry. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 2055–2068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wierenga, L.M. , Langen, M. , Oranje, B. , & Durston, S. (2014). Unique developmental trajectories of cortical thickness and surface area. NeuroImage, 87, 120–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]