Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2018 Apr 13;7:e33456. doi: 10.7554/eLife.33456

Motor selection dynamics in FEF explain the reaction time variance of saccades to single targets

Christopher K Hauser 1, Dantong Zhu 1, Terrence R Stanford 1, Emilio Salinas 1,
Editor: Joshua I Gold2
PMCID: PMC5947991  PMID: 29652247

Abstract

In studies of voluntary movement, a most elemental quantity is the reaction time (RT) between the onset of a visual stimulus and a saccade toward it. However, this RT demonstrates extremely high variability which, in spite of extensive research, remains unexplained. It is well established that, when a visual target appears, oculomotor activity gradually builds up until a critical level is reached, at which point a saccade is triggered. Here, based on computational work and single-neuron recordings from monkey frontal eye field (FEF), we show that this rise-to-threshold process starts from a dynamic initial state that already contains other incipient, internally driven motor plans, which compete with the target-driven activity to varying degrees. The ensuing conflict resolution process, which manifests in subtle covariations between baseline activity, build-up rate, and threshold, consists of fundamentally deterministic interactions, and explains the observed RT distributions while invoking only a small amount of intrinsic randomness.

Research organism: Rhesus macaque

eLife digest

As we examine the space around us our eyes move in short steps, looking toward a new location about four times a second. Neurons in a region of the brain called the frontal eye field help initiate these eye movements, which are known as saccades. Each neuron contributes to a saccade with a specific direction and size. Before a saccade, the relevant neurons in the frontal eye field steadily increase their activity. When this activity reaches a critical threshold, the visual system issues a command to move the eyes in the appropriate direction. So a saccade that moves the eyes to the right requires a specific group of neurons to be strongly activated – but, at the same time, the neurons responsible for movement to the left need to be less active.

Imagine that you have to move your eyes as quickly as possible to look at a spot of light that appears on a screen. Some of the time your eyes will start to move about 100 milliseconds after the light appears. But on other attempts, your eyes will not start moving until 300 milliseconds after the light came on. What causes this variability?

To find out, Hauser et al. recorded from neurons in monkeys trained to perform such a task. When the spot of light appeared many different neurons were active, suggesting there is conflict between the plan that would move the eyes toward the target and plans to look at other locations. That is, when the target appears, the monkey is already thinking of looking somewhere. The time required to resolve this conflict depends on how far apart the target and the competing locations are from one another, and on how much the competing neurons have increased their activity before the target appears.

Similar mechanisms are likely to operate when we sit at the dinner table and look for the salt shaker, for example, and so the results presented by Hauser et al. will help us to understand how we direct our attention to different points in space. Understanding how these processes work in more detail will help us to discern what happens when they go wrong, as occurs in attention deficit disorders like ADHD.

Introduction

The reaction time (RT) represents the total time taken to perform all of the mental operations that may contribute to a particular action, such as stimulus detection, attention, working memory, or motor preparation. Although the importance of the RT as a fundamental metric for inferring the mechanisms that mediate cognition cannot be overstated (Welford, 1980; Meyer et al., 1988), such reliance is a double-edged sword. Under appropriate experimental conditions, differential measurements of RT may be used as a readout for changes in the (mean) time consumed by any one of the aforementioned operations, but a particular RT value is hard to interpret because it may be that not all of the operations involved are known, and those that are relevant may overlap in time to varying degrees. Furthermore, each operation may have its own, independent source of variability, making it very difficult to attribute the measured variance in RT to a particular cause (e.g., Krajbich et al., 2015). In the case of eye movements, this ambiguity is likely to be more severe than previously appreciated. There is a firm mechanistic account that describes how saccades are triggered, but according to the present results, that account lacks a crucial ingredient — ongoing motor conflict — and assumes, incorrectly, that in response to the same stimulus, the fundamental reason why some saccades are triggered very quickly whereas others take much longer simply boils down to noise in the underlying neuronal activity.

The neural dynamics that give rise to eye movements are well established. In essence, the preparation to make a saccade of a particular direction and amplitude is equal to a gradual rise in the activity of oculomotor neurons that are selective for the corresponding movement vector. If this rising activity, referred to as a motor plan, ramps up rapidly, the saccade is initiated quickly; if the motor plan grows more slowly, the saccade starts later. Quantitatively, this corresponds to a negative correlation between saccadic RT and build-up rate (Hanes and Schall, 1996; Fecteau and Munoz, 2007; Ding and Gold, 2012; Heitz and Schall, 2012; Costello et al., 2013). Notably, neurons that encode motor plans seem to reach a consistent firing level just before the onset of a saccade, particularly in the superior colliculus (SC) and the frontal eye field (FEF) (Hanes and Schall, 1996; Brown et al., 2008; Stanford et al., 2010; Ding and Gold, 2012). That is, there appears to be a fixed activity threshold that serves as a trigger for eye movements (Lo and Wang, 2006). Thus, it is widely thought that, for simple saccades to lone, unambiguous stimuli, i.e., reactive saccades, the variance of the RT distribution is predominantly determined by the variance of the FEF/SC build-up rates across trials (Carpenter and Williams, 1995; Hanes and Schall, 1996; Fecteau and Munoz, 2007; Sumner, 2011).

This rise-to-threshold process is of enormous conceptual importance, as it is the key building block of virtually all models of decision-making in which multiple-choice alternatives, typically guided by perceptual information, are evaluated over time (Gold and Shadlen, 2001; Erlhagen and Schöner, 2002; Smith and Ratcliff, 2004; Brown and Heathcote, 2008; Stanford et al., 2010; Krajbich and Rangel, 2011; Thura et al., 2012; Brunton et al., 2013; Miller and Katz, 2013). Nevertheless, the variance of this process in its simplest possible instantiation — reactive saccades — remains a mystery (Sumner, 2011), because it seems too large to reflect noise or intrinsic randomness (in the build-up rates of oculomotor neurons) alone. One possibility is that the randomness is purposeful, that unpredictability in saccade timing somehow entails a behavioral advantage (Carpenter, 1999). Alternatively, the RT of reactive saccades may fluctuate, at least in part, because of underlying neural mechanisms that have simply not been identified yet.

Here we describe such mechanisms. We recorded activity from single FEF neurons in an elegant paradigm (Lauwereyns et al., 2002; Hikosaka et al., 2006) that produces a large spread in saccadic RT simply by varying the subject’s spatial expectation of reward. A model based on competitive dynamics quantitatively reproduced the temporal profiles of the evoked neural responses, as well as their dependencies on RT, reward expectation, and trial outcome (correct/incorrect) — this, while simultaneously matching the monkeys’ full RT distributions across experimental conditions. The results indicate that RTs vary because the stimulus-driven activity does not start from a consistent, neutral state, but rather from a dynamic, biased state in which incipient, internally driven motor plans are already developing. In other words, when the target appears, the monkey is already thinking of looking somewhere. This conflict between motor alternatives (1) requires varying amounts of time to be resolved, depending on how advanced and how congruent the bias-driven plans are relative to the target-driven response, (2) is initially defined by the baseline levels of activity (measured during fixation) across spatial locations, and (3) dictates the build-up rate and threshold of the subsequent rise-to-threshold process. Thus, the noise in the build-up rate is much more modest than that predicted by extant frameworks, and the high variability of saccades to single targets is, to a large degree, deterministic, a direct consequence of motor selection mechanisms that allow voluntary saccades to be driven by both sensory events and internal biases.

Results

Behavioral manifestations of a spatial bias

Two rhesus monkeys were trained on the one-direction rewarded (1DR) task (Figure 1a), in which a saccade to a lone, unambiguous target must be made but a large liquid reward (primary reinforcer) is available only when the target appears in one specific location (Lauwereyns et al., 2002; Hikosaka et al., 2006). The rewarded location remains constant over a block of trials and then changes. Of note, ours is a RT version of the task whereby the go signal (offset of fixation point), which means ‘move now!’ is simultaneous with target onset. Also, it involves four locations and variable block length. This task generates errors and a large spread in RT (Figure 2) under minimalistic sensory stimulation conditions. We exploit this to investigate how variance in saccadic performance relates to variance in FEF activity.

Figure 1. Saccadic tasks used.

Figure 1.

(a) The 1DR task. After a fixation period of 1000 ms, a single eccentric stimulus appears at one of four locations and the subject is required to make a saccade to it. Stimulus location is chosen randomly in each trial. Fixation offset (go signal) and stimulus onset are simultaneous. In each block of trials, only one of the directions yields a large reward; the others yield either no reward (monkey G) or a small reward (monkey K). (b) The ADR task. Same sequence of events as in (a), except that saccades in all directions are equally rewarded. White arrows indicate saccades; they are not displayed.

Figure 2. Asymmetric reward expectation leads to strong spatial bias.

(a–d) RT distributions for correct saccades, for monkeys G (left column) and K (right column). Insets indicate rewarded location (blue drop) and target (red circle). When the two are congruent ([a], red traces), RTs are shorter and less variable than when they are incongruent, that is, either opposite ([b], cyan) or adjacent ([c], [d], cyan). In unbiased trials (ADR task; gray), results are intermediate. (e) RT distributions in correct congruent (red, same data as in [a]), correct incongruent (cyan, data in [b-d] combined), and incorrect incongruent (black) trials. Histograms are normalized to a maximum of 1. The RTs during errors are neither the fastest nor the slowest.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Variations in spatial bias over time.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Analyses of the sequential responses made by the two animals confirmed that they were strongly aware of which location was the rewarded one. (a, b) Time course of bias adaptation after a change in rewarded location. Mean RT (± 1 SE) for saccades to a fixed location as a function of the number of trials before or after a change in reward status (i.e., block transition). Transitions from unrewarded to rewarded (a, red) are faster than from rewarded to unrewarded (b, cyan). Horizontal lines indicate asymptotic RT values. Black lines are exponential fits with time constants as indicated (τ; units are trials). (c, d) Trial history effects. Mean RTs (± 1 SE) to rewarded ([c], red) and unrewarded ([d], cyan) targets are shown conditioned on various preceding sequences of rewarded (R) and unrewarded (U) trials. Note systematic changes in RT, which are somewhat idiosyncratic to each animal, as each condition is repeated. For calculating these trial-history effects, the first 15 trials after each block transition were excluded. All data are from correct responses.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Impact of reward-location bias on saccade metrics.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

All plots compare measurements in congruent (red labels and markers) versus incongruent conditions (blue labels and markers). (a) Endpoints of saccades to a stimulus located at (10, 0) collected during a single session from monkey G. The origin corresponds to the average saccade endpoint. For each data point, the displacement is the distance to the origin. Yellow circles indicate 1 and 2 SDs in displacement from the congruent data. (b) Mean normalized displacement in each experimental session (n=135). Displacement values for each session were z-scored, and then congruent and incongruent trials were averaged separately. Colors indicate monkeys (G, gray; K, white). The session shown in (a) is marked by the green cross. (c) Mean normalized displacement for each monkey, averaged across sessions. Error bars indicate ± 1 SE. (d) Average time course of eye velocity for saccades to stimuli at (8, 4) collected during a single session from monkey K. Inset zooms in on the change in peak velocity of 40/s between congruent (red, n=104 trials) and incongruent (blue, n=427 trials) trials. Shades indicate ± 1 SE across trials. (e) Mean peak velocity in each experimental session. Colors indicate monkeys (G, gray; K, white). The session shown in (d) is marked by the green cross. (f) As in (e) but for saccade amplitude. (g) Average velocity (top) and amplitude (bottom) values averaged across sessions and monkeys. Error bars indicate ± 1 SE. All significance values are for differences in means across experimental conditions, from permutation tests for paired data. The results indicate that saccades to high-reward locations are spatially more precise (a–c) and have higher peak velocities (d, e, g) than those to low-reward locations, whereas saccade amplitude changes little in comparison (f, g). Results are consistent with earlier reports (Lauwereyns et al., 2002; Takikawa et al., 2002; Watanabe and Hikosaka, 2005).

When the target and rewarded locations coincided (congruent trials; Figure 2a, red traces), the monkeys consistently moved their eyes very quickly (monkey G, 158±33 ms, mean RT ± 1 SD; monkey K, 146±21 ms), and essentially never missed (monkey G, 99.8% correct, n=7234 congruent trials; monkey K, 99.6%, n=5837). By contrast, when the rewarded and target locations were either diametrically opposed or adjacent (incongruent trials; Figure 2b–d, cyan traces), both the mean RT and the spread increased dramatically (monkey G, 269±84 ms; monkey K, 236±77 ms), as did the percentage of incorrect saccades away from the target (monkey G, 18.3% incorrect, n=16905 incongruent trials; monkey K, 8.1%, n=12708). The symmetric condition in which all directions were equally rewarded (ADR; Figure 1b) produced RT distributions that were intermediate between those of congruent and incongruent trials (Figure 2a–d, gray; monkey G, 192±40 ms; monkey K, 174±36 ms). These results recapitulate the puzzle mentioned in the Introduction: if saccades can be very fast, why, under identical stimulation conditions, are they sometimes very slow?

Also note that, compared to those of correct saccades, the RTs of incorrect saccades were neither consistently fast, as might be expected on the basis of strong anticipation, nor consistently slow, as might be expected from a protracted deliberation process (Figure 2e). Instead, the RTs during errors fell squarely in the middle of the distributions of correct RTs (for correct trials, 90% of RTs were inside the ranges 158–432 ms and 146–404 ms for monkeys G and K, respectively; for errors, the ranges were 180–336 and 152–388 ms). Mechanistically, it is not obvious how this could be accomplished. One of the aims of the model presented below is to reproduce the RT data shown in Figure 2 and to resolve this conundrum.

In short, monkeys are highly sensitive to the spatially asymmetric value associated with otherwise identical target stimuli (for additional evidence of this, see Figure 2—figure supplement 1). This manifests primarily as large differences in RT between correct congruent, correct incongruent, and incorrect incongruent trials (for other manifestations, see Figure 2—figure supplement 2). The pattern of results suggests that, when a target appears at an unrewarded location, a conflict arises because the monkey wants to look at the rewarded location instead. In what follows, we ask: what features of the evoked FEF activity reflect such conflict, and do they account for the measured variability in the direction and timing of the elicited saccades?

Motor conflict during the rise-to-threshold process

We recorded single-unit activity from 132 FEF neurons in the two monkeys (67 in monkey G; 65 in monkey K) during performance of the 1DR task. The analysis in this section focuses on a population of 62 neurons that satisfied two conditions: they had standard visual and saccade-related responses, and both correct and incorrect trials were collected for them (for details of cell and trial selection procedures, see 'Materials and methods, Neuronal classification and selection').

In this section, we show that, during the rise-to-threshold process associated with reactive saccades, the build-up rate, the apparent threshold, and the baseline firing rates measured during fixation (before the target is presented) exhibit coordinated variations that have not been appreciated before and which, beyond the specifics of our task, are likely to be key for determining RTs in general. We refer to the baseline levels in plural because the target location is not the only relevant one; neurons with RFs at other locations may be activated by internally driven biases, creating motor conflict, that is, activity that competes with and impacts the target-driven response — and the RT.

Before describing these results, a note about nomenclature. Whereas just two distinct experimental conditions, congruent and incongruent, are relevant for behavioral analysis, neurophysiologically there are eight conditions to consider, depending on whether the target, expected reward, and saccadic movement were inside or outside a recorded cell’s response field (RF). Thus, for example, we use the abbreviation IOI to denote the target/reward/saccade combination in/out/in; that is, trials for which the target was in, the reward was expected out, and the saccade was into the RF (see icons in Figure 3a–c). Such IOI trials are incongruent, because target and reward locations do not match, and correct, because the target and saccade locations match. Only six of the eight possible combinations are considered because congruent trials were virtually devoid of errors, so IIO and OOI conditions are absent. With this notation at hand, we now turn to the activity of FEF across the six remaining conditions.

Figure 3. Baseline activity predicts response gain, threshold, and outcome.

(a–c) Normalized firing rate as a function of time for a population of 62 neurons (V, VM, and M) for which both correct and incorrect responses were collected. Icons indicate target (red circle), rewarded location (blue drop), and saccade (white cross) relative to the RF (gray area) in each case. Paired reddish and greenish traces correspond to activity with the target inside or outside the RF, respectively, in the same behavioral condition. For congruent trials (a) only correct responses are shown. For incongruent trials both correct (b) and incorrect (c) responses are shown. The reference line (dotted) is identical across panels. (d) (e) Mean peak activity (d) and mean baseline activity (e) for each target-reward-saccade (TRS) combination, from the same 62 neurons in (a–c). Error bars indicate ±1 SE across cells. (f) Baseline activity (z-scored) for incorrect (y axis) versus correct outcomes (x axis) under identical target and reward conditions. Each point is one neuron. Magenta dots indicate IOO versus IOI trials (target in/reward out); cyan dots indicate OII versus OIO trials (target out/reward in).

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Responses of predominantly visual and predominantly movement-related neurons in FEF across conditions and outcomes.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Each panel shows normalized firing rate as a function of time for different combinations of target (red circle), reward (blue drop), and saccade (white cross) locations relative to the RF (gray area), as indicated by the icons on the right. The dotted reference line is the same for all panels. (a–c) Results are as in Figure 3a–c, except that they are based on the 33% of the cells (n=21) that were most visual, that is, had the lowest visuomotor index values. (d–f) As in panels (a–c) except that the results are based on the 33% of the cells (n=21) that were most motor, that is, had the highest visuomotor index values. Note differences in timing and in absolute baseline levels between visual and movement-related traces, but the similar relationships between motor plans into and away from the RF across conditions.

Although each saccade in the 1DR task involves a single target, the motor preparation process in FEF can (and should) be understood as a motor selection process involving not one but at least two populations of neurons, those that contribute to the actual saccadic choice and those that favor the opposite choice (Figure 3a–c, see icons). During congruent trials, when the target appears at the rewarded location, the evoked activity rises most rapidly and reaches the highest firing rate (Figure 3a, III trials, red trace). The saccade is essentially always correct and no evidence of conflict is discernible because the neurons favoring the opposite choice, away from the target, show little (if any) response before the eye movement (Figure 3a, OOO trials, green trace). In this case, naively, it would appear as if only the one response that rises to threshold is important.

Notably, though, a difference in activity between the two complementary populations is already evident before the go signal/target onset. This is the internal bias signal created by reward expectation, which in this case is spatially congruent with both the target and the saccade. This baseline signal is consistent with previous neurophysiological studies using the 1DR task (Takikawa et al., 2002; Sato and Hikosaka, 2002; Ding and Hikosaka, 2006), and may be interpreted as a neural correlate of spatial attention (Maunsell, 2004; Peck et al., 2009; Preciado et al., 2017).

By contrast, during incongruent trials, when the target is presented opposite to the rewarded location, a conflict arises early in the trial in the form of a higher baseline favoring the rewarded location (Figure 3b, note green trace above red before go signal). During correct trials this conflict is appropriately resolved as the target-driven activity increases and overtakes the competition (Figure 3b, IOI trials, red trace), but the rise proceeds more slowly, that is, it has a lower build-up rate and ultimately reaches a lower peak firing level (Rp; see 'Materials and methods, Peak response') than that observed when the bias and the target are congruent (Figure 3d, IOI vs. III). Finally, the conflict is even more extreme during incongruent trials that end in erroneous choices toward the rewarded location (Figure 3c). In that case, the initial bias in baseline activity is maximized (Figure 3e, IOO vs. OII) and the evoked target-driven activity (Figure 3c, magenta trace) is considerably weaker than that observed during correct saccades (Figure 3d, IOO vs. IOI). The neural response associated with the (wrong) saccadic choice rises very slowly (Figure 3c, cyan trace) and reaches a modest threshold level prior to saccade onset, but this activity is nonetheless slightly above that associated with the opposite (correct) motor alternative (Figure 3c, right panel; Figure 3d, OII vs. IOO). This ambivalence between motor plans is reminiscent of that observed during choice tasks (Thompson et al., 2005; Ding and Gold, 2012; Costello et al., 2013), as if the monkeys had struggled to make a choice between two competing targets, even though only one was displayed.

These results were based on recordings from 62 neurons with diverse visuomotor properties, but were qualitatively similar when the averaging across cells was restricted to units that were either predominantly visual or predominantly motor (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Those two populations differed in the time at which they fired maximally and in the magnitude of their baseline activity, but qualitatively, their responses changed similarly across conditions and outcomes.

The mean peak and baseline responses, Rp and Rb, demonstrated remarkably similar variation patterns (Figure 3, compare [d] and [e]). Overall, these comparisons based on population responses suggest that the baseline firing rates (at both the target and opposite spatial locations), build-up rate, and threshold of the rise-to-threshold process vary in a coordinated way across experimental conditions. But then a crucial question arises: do such covariations also occur from trial to trial within each condition? We sought evidence of this by analyzing the responses of individual neurons.

First we found that, during incongruent trials, the baseline activity, Rb (firing rate in a 250 ms window preceding target onset), is strongly predictive of outcome. When the rewarded location coincided with the RF and the target was presented outside, most neurons (34 of 53, p= 0.03, binomial test) had a higher baseline rate before incorrect as opposed to correct trials (Figure 3f, cyan dots, OII vs. OIO; p=0.002, permutation test), as if an excessive Rb triggered a (wrong) saccade into the RF. Conversely, when the rewarded location was opposite to the RF and the target was subsequently presented inside, most neurons (33 of 42, p= 0.001, binomial test) had a lower Rb preceding incorrect trials (Figure 3f, magenta dots, IOO vs. IOI; p=0.009), as if the lack of baseline activity precluded a (correct) saccade into the RF.

Second, for each recorded cell (V, VM, and M), we calculated Spearman correlation coefficients between pairs of neural response measures across trials ('Materials and methods, Statistical analyses'). Although some of those correlations were too noisy to resolve (e.g., between baseline activity and build-up rate), that between baseline activity and peak response, ρ(Rb,Rp), was highly robust; it was strongly positive not only in IOI trials (Figure 4a) but also in other conditions (III, OOO, and OIO; p<0.001 in all cases), and there was no evidence of a dependence on the visuomotor properties of the recorded cells (Figure 4a, scatterplot; p>0.27 for linear regression). This confirms that when the baseline activity of a neuron is high, the response evoked later, after the target appears, typically reaches a higher firing level than when the baseline is low.

Figure 4. Characteristic manifestations of the rise-to-threshold process in single FEF neurons.

Figure 4.

Each panel shows Spearman correlation coefficients calculated for 90 V, VM, and M neurons. (a) Correlations between baseline activity and peak response. (b) Correlations between build up-rate and RTals. In each panel, the scatterplot (bottom) shows the same correlation coefficients as the histogram (top) but together with the visuomotor indices of the neurons. Gray shades in the scatterplots demarcate the borders defining the V, VM, and M cell categories. White bars in histograms and red points in scatterplots indicate significant correlations (p<0.05). Pink triangles mark mean values, with significance from a signed-rank test indicated.

At the single-cell level, we also found strong associations between responsivity and RT. In IOI trials, the correlation between build-up rate and RT, ρ(RBU,RT), was strongly negative (Figure 4b). This is in agreement with the common finding reported by previous studies, that the build-up rate is the main link between oculomotor activity and saccade latency. Furthermore, the correlations ρ(Rb,RT) and ρ(Rp,RT) were also predominantly negative, consistent with the observation that, across conditions with movements into the RF, shorter RTs are simultaneously associated with higher baseline, higher build-up rate, and higher threshold (Figure 3a,b). These dependencies, and important deviations observed across trial types, are analyzed in detail below.

In summary, during the 1DR task, the activity in FEF demonstrated characteristic covariations in the three main features of the rising saccade-related activity: the ‘baseline’ firing rates at both the target and opposite spatial locations, the build-up rate of the target-driven response, and the maximum activity reached before movement onset. This suggests a causal relationship between the baseline and the subsequent response, because the baseline signal arises earlier (before target presentation) and because it is predictive of outcome (Figure 3f). Although these covariations were more evident across task conditions than across trials, our contention is that they are always present because they reflect fundamental dynamics of the oculomotor circuitry. Next, to test this, we present a mechanistic model that generalizes these novel interrelationships to all trials, and is thereby able to relate neuronal activity to RT with remarkable detail.

Modeling the rise-to-threshold dynamics

A saccadic competition model was developed to investigate the mechanistic link between the FEF activity and the monkeys’ behavior in the 1DR task ('Materials and methods, Saccadic competition model'). Such a bridge requires that multiple constraints are satisfied. First of all, the model must reproduce the neurophysiological results presented in the previous section. Thus, it considers two neural populations whose responses may rise to a threshold. One population generates saccades toward location T, where the target stimulus is presented, and the other toward D, the diametrically opposite location (Figure 5a, icon). In any given trial, if the target-driven response, RT (Figure 5a–c, red traces), reaches threshold first, a correct saccade to the target is produced, whereas if the bias-driven activity, RD (Figure 5a–c, blue traces), reaches threshold first, the result is an incorrect saccade away from the target. In this way, the RT and RD variables correspond to the population responses recorded with the target inside (Figure 3a–c, reddish traces) and outside (greenish traces) of the RF, respectively.

Figure 5. Simulation results from a saccadic competition model that bridges the neural and behavioral data.

(a–c) Simulated activity in three single trials. Traces show motor plans toward the target location (T, red) and the diametrically opposite location (D, blue). Gray shades mark the period during which the target stimulus suppresses the motor plan toward D. When the target-driven activity, RT (red), reaches threshold first (a, b), the saccade is correct; when the bias-driven activity, RD (blue), reaches threshold first (c), the saccade is incorrect. The scale on the y axes is the same for all three plots. (d–f) Mean firing rate traces RT (red) and RD (blue) averaged across correct congruent (d), correct incongruent (e), and incorrect incongruent (f) simulated trials. Same format as in Figure 3a–c. (g–i) RT distributions from the same simulations as in panels (d–f), with trials sorted accordingly: for correct congruent (g), correct incongruent (h), and incorrect incongruent (i) trials. Colored shades are combined data from the two monkeys; black lines are model results. Dotted vertical lines at 150 ms are for reference.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Variations in baseline and threshold levels in FEF and in the saccadic competition model.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(a) Mean normalized activity preceding saccade onset, Rsac, for each target-reward-saccade (TRS) combination. Data are from the same population and in the same format as those in Figure 3d, but here activity is based on the firing rate of each neuron computed in a fixed 50 ms window ending at saccade onset. Also, each unit’s responses were normalized before averaging across cells. Error bars indicate ±1 SE across cells. (b) Mean baseline activity (preceding the go signal), Rb, for each experimental condition. Data are from the same population and in the same format as those in Figure 3e, but here each unit’s responses were normalized by the same factor as in (a) before averaging across cells. (c, d), As (a) and (b) but from model simulations; same runs as in Figure 5d–f. Presaccadic activity, Rsac, was equated to the response at threshold crossing. Baseline activity, Rb, was equated to the response at the go signal. To compare the FEF and model responses in the same scale, both data sets were rescaled so that Rsac in the IOI condition was equal to 1.

Another important feature of the recorded data is the evident asymmetry between the two motor plans: the target-driven activity is typically strong and never fully suppressed, whereas the internally driven activity favoring the opposite location is typically suppressed and only rarely of moderate strength. In the model, this asymmetry is captured by two suppression mechanisms that constrain when and how RD can rise. One of them ('Materials and methods, Saccadic competition model, Rule 1') simply prescribes that, once RT is rising, it can fully suppress RD. That is, the moment RT advances past RD, RD stops rising altogether. The other mechanism is about the timing of RD. The target-driven response, RT, begins to rise shortly after target onset (after an afferent delay of 35 ms), whereas its counterpart, RD, begins to rise later, partly because of a somewhat longer afferent delay (50 ms) but mostly because of a transient, stimulus-driven suppression. This (partial) suppression is based on abundant evidence (reviewed by Salinas and Stanford, (2018); see also Reingold and Stampe (2002); Dorris et al. (2007); Stanford et al. (2010); Bompas and Sumner (2011); Buonocore and McIntosh (2012); Buonocore et al., (2017)) indicating that ongoing saccade plans, RD in this case, are briefly inhibited by stimuli that appear abruptly, just like the target in our experiment. In the model, the inhibition lasts 115 ms (Figure 5a–c, gray shades), after which RD may rise in full force — if it was not overtaken by RT in the interim. These two suppression mechanisms give the target-driven activity an advantage over its competing, internally driven counterpart, and they are necessary to reconcile the occurrence of errors with the late-onset, weak activity away from the RF seen during correct saccades (Figure 3a,b, greenish traces). For instance, without the transient, stimulus-driven inhibition, the model would produce incorrect saccades at a rate that is vastly higher than that observed experimentally.

Finally, the model must also capture the variations in baseline activity, build-up rate, and threshold observed in the FEF data (Figures 3d,e and 4a), and this is where the crucial conceptual leap takes place. What we found empirically was that, for the target-driven response, a higher baseline was accompanied by both a higher build-up rate and a higher threshold, with the baseline activity of the alternative motor plan having opposite effects. The model generalizes these dependencies by making two assumptions. First, that similar relationships hold across all trials, rather than just across the three experimental conditions examined, and second, that because the baseline signal is present before target onset, any variations in build-up rate and threshold can be formulated mainly as the result of variations in baseline activity. Thus, the model can be thought of as designed to test whether the differences in the rise-to-threshold process observed across experimental conditions (Figure 3) are the average manifestations of similar but more general dynamical relationships between the three relevant variables, where the variance is primarily derived from the baselines. So, in practice, the general idea is that the baselines fluctuate stochastically and determine the ensuing rise-to-threshold excursion in each trial.

The resulting dynamics between competing motor plans can be intuitively appreciated with three example trials (Figure 5a–c). The simplest situation is when, during fixation, the baseline at the target location, BT, is larger than that at the opposite location, BD (Figure 5a). This is typically the case when the target and rewarded locations coincide, but what matters in the model is simply the actual baseline values (more on this below). The condition BT>BD has two specific consequences: (1) it yields a high build-up rate for the target-driven activity, RT (red trace), and (2) it sets the saccade threshold, Θ, to a high value (Equations 6, 7). Thus, because of the high build-up rate, RT rises sharply and quickly triggers a saccade, in spite of the high Θ. The D plan (blue trace) is always suppressed in this case, so no overt conflict is visible. This is how correct saccades with very short RTs are produced.

The more interesting scenario occurs when the bias-driven plan starts with the higher baseline, as typically happens when the reward is expected at the D location (but again, the dynamics are dictated just by the baseline values). In that case, the saccade can be either correct or incorrect, depending on how big the lead is. When BD is much larger than BT (Figure 5c), the consequences are essentially the opposite of those in the previous example: (1) RT has a low build-up rate, so the target-driven response (red trace) rises slowly, and (2) the saccade threshold, Θ, is low. In this way, RD is able to advance steadily after the suppression interval and win the race from wire to wire, reaching a relatively low firing level before saccade onset. This is how incorrect saccades are produced.

By contrast, if the baseline BD is only moderately higher than BT (Figure 5b), then the combined effect of the baselines is intermediate relative to that in the two previous examples: (1) the initial build-up rate of RT is moderate, neither as high as that in the first example nor as low as that in the second, and (2) the value of Θ is also intermediate. The target-driven plan (red trace) rises at a rate that allows it to overtake the competing plan (blue trace) and to win the race by coming from behind. Importantly, in this case, RT slows down as it goes past RD (note the slight change in the slope of the red trace during the shaded interval). Although RT wins the race, overtaking the competing plan exacts a toll, and the lower its initial build-up rate, the higher that toll (Equation 11). This is the one mechanism that was introduced into the model specifically to satisfy key behavioral constraints. In this case, slowing down the winner target-driven plan is necessary to produce correct saccades with very long RTs — longer than those of incorrect saccades.

These examples illustrate how the baseline levels BT and BD quantitatively regulate both the build-up rate of the target-driven activity and the saccade threshold. Nevertheless, it is important to stress that, at the same time, the baselines convey information about the location of the expected reward in a manner that is consistent with the experimental data. In the simulations, the baseline values across trials are characterized by their mean and variance, which are fixed (Equation 5). However, the two mean values are assigned to the T and D locations according to a simple prescription: the rewarded location gets the higher mean ('Materials and methods, Saccadic competition model'). Thus, in simulations of the congruent case, BT is typically — although not always — larger than BD (as in Figure 5a), and the majority of trials are fast and correct; whereas in simulations of the incongruent case, the roles are reversed, BT is (on average) lower than BD (as in Figure 5b,c), which results in a combination of correct and incorrect slower responses. Other than that, the simulations proceed in exactly the same way in the two bias conditions, as they should.

With all of these elements in place, the model parameters were adjusted to fit all of the experimental data discussed so far ('Materials and methods, Correspondence between data and model parameters'). In this way, when trials were sorted by bias and outcome, the simulated neural responses reproduced the covariations in baseline, build-up rate, and threshold across conditions (Figure 5d–f; for quantification, see Figure 5—figure supplement 1). This demonstrates that, as intended, the hypothesized coupling across trials is indeed consistent with the observed coupling across experimental conditions. In addition, the average RT(t) and RD(t) traces matched the trajectories of the recorded population responses in great detail (compare to Figure 3a–c). The proposed interaction mechanisms between the two motor plans resulted in average traces with the appropriate magnitude, time course, and degree of ambivalence. But most critically, at the same time, the model fully accounted for the behavioral data: (1) it generated correct and incorrect saccades in proportions similar to those found experimentally (0% and 10% errors in congruent and incongruent conditions), and (2) it generated simulated distributions of RTs (Figure 5g–i) that closely mimic their behavioral counterparts (as assessed by mean, median, SD, and skewness). Note, in particular, that the RTs in incorrect trials (panel i) are neither too fast, because the stimulus-driven suppression mechanism prevents fast errors, nor too slow, because the slowest responses (which occur when RT slows down) are correct. The model explains the behavioral data in terms of the neural data, accurately replicating both.

The results show that, as an intrinsic part of the motor competition process, the baseline activity, build-up rate, and threshold vary in a coordinated fashion to generate the wide range of RTs observed in the task. In the rest of the paper, we show that this fact explains many other, seemingly odd features of the neural data.

Comparison accross conditions with equalized RTs

A key assumption of the model is that fluctuations in baseline activity result in fluctuations in build-up rate and threshold. To further characterize the interdependencies between these three variables and better understand how they impact the RT, we compared the responses evoked in congruent versus incongruent trials before and after equalizing their RTs.

The FEF responses recorded during III and IOI trials were quite distinct (Figure 3a,b), even though both involved correct saccades in the same direction. The differences could be due to the different expected reward locations in the two conditions and/or to the different RTs generated (Figure 6a). To eliminate the differences due to RT, we devised a simple sub-sampling procedure ('Materials and methods, RT matching') that resulted in IOI and III data subsets with identical numbers of trials and nearly identical RT distributions (Figure 6b). Then, we made comparisons across conditions with and without matching the RTs.

Figure 6. Disentangling the coupling between baseline activity, build-up rate, and threshold.

(a) Original RT distributions for III (magenta) and IOI (black) trials from all FEF recording sessions (non-matched condition, NM). (b) RT distributions after RT matching (yes-matched condition, YM). (c), Firing rate as a function of time for the target-driven activity, RT, in simulated III (magenta) and IOI (black) trials; same as red traces in Figure 5d,e. RTs are not matched. (d) As (c), but with matched RTs. (e) Normalized firing rate as a function of time for a population of 84 V, VM, and M neurons. RTs are not matched. (f) As (e) but with matched RTs.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Comparisons between congruent and incongruent conditions, with and without RT equalization, for individual cell types.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

Each panel shows normalized firing rate as a function of time in III (magenta) versus IOI (black) trials. Plots on the left include all recorded trials in each condition (non-matched), whereas plots on the right only include trials with matched RTs (yes-matched). The format is the same as in Figure 6e,f. (a) Responses of 25 V neurons. (b) Responses of 37 VM neurons. (c) Responses of 22 M neurons. The effect of RT matching is qualitatively similar for the three cell types.

What should be expected on the basis of the model? In the standard case, without RT matching (NM condition), the target-driven response, RT, has a higher baseline and reaches a higher threshold in congruent trials than in incongruent trials (Figure 6c). However, because they have very different build-up rates (note the steeper rise of the magenta curve), when aligned on saccade onset, the corresponding response trajectories intersect each other twice. By contrast, when their respective RT distributions are the same (YM condition), the shapes of the trajectories are much more similar and no longer intersect; now, across the two conditions, the differences in baseline and threshold are smaller, and the build-up rates are nearly identical (Figure 6d). These results make perfect sense within our modeling framework. First, shorter RTs are associated with higher baseline, higher build-up rate, and higher threshold, but the RT equalization procedure only retains the fastest IOI trials, so naturally the resulting IOI trajectory simultaneously increases its baseline, build-up rate, and threshold. Second, the build-up rate changes the most because it relates to RT most directly (Figure 4b). And third, the residual difference between congruent and incongruent conditions, which is exclusively related to the internal bias signal, is consistent with the strong coupling between the threshold and the baseline across trials (Figure 4a; Equation 6).

Now consider the same analysis but for 84 V, VM, and M FEF neurons that had sufficient RT-matched trials. Qualitatively, the results are just as expected from the model: when the RTs are not matched, the responses in III and IOI trials differ patently in baseline, threshold, and build-up rate (Figure 6e), and the corresponding curves intersect each other twice when the spikes are aligned on saccade onset (right panel). In contrast, when the RTs are matched, the differences in build-up rate practically disappear, and those in baseline and threshold become smaller (Figure 6f). These results were qualitatively similar across FEF cell categories (Figure 6—figure supplement 1).

These findings are consistent with the dynamics of the model, which dictate that shorter RTs are generally associated with a higher baseline (at the target location), higher build-up rate, and higher threshold, where the correlation between build-up rate and RT is strongest.

Predicted RT sensitivity of the mean population activity

To test the model more stringently, we exploited the wide range of RTs produced in the 1DR task to generate predictions for how the evoked neural activity should be expected to vary as a function of RT. The rationale for these predictions is straightforward: instead of calculating the mean activity averaged across all trials in, say, the IOI condition (Figure 5e), we consider similar traces based on subsets of trials within narrow RT bins ('Materials and methods, Continuous firing activity'). Assuming that the activity in FEF directly contributes to triggering each saccade, as happens in the model, the resulting response profiles should vary systematically across those RT bins, and any patterns should be consistent with the correlations among baseline, build-up rate, and threshold instantiated by the model, as well as with its other mechanisms (e.g., RD suppression).

Indeed, when the simulated IOI trials are sorted and averaged according to RT and the resulting curves are color-coded, a characteristic pattern emerges (Figure 7a): steeply rising trajectories precede short-latency saccades (black), and more shallow, protracted trajectories precede long-latency saccades (red). This is largely because, in the model, the RT depends critically on the build-up rate of the target-driven activity. Notably, the apparent threshold reached by these curves at saccade onset is also modulated by RT (Figure 7a, right panel), with fast choices (black) leading to higher activity levels than slow ones (red). This, in turn, is consistent with the correlated fluctuations in build-up rate and threshold built into the model. Qualitatively, this prediction for the IOI condition — that is, the pattern resulting from the simultaneous dependencies of build-up rate and threshold on RT — is highly robust to parameter variations (of at least ±30%, Equations 5–11).

Figure 7. RT sensitivity of the average population activity.

(a–d) Firing rate as a function of time for the simulated motor plans RT (a, c) and RD (b, d) during incongruent (a, b) and congruent (c, d) trials. The corresponding experimental conditions are indicated by the icons (far right). Curves are based on the same simulations as in Figure 5d–f. Each colored trace includes 20% of the simulated trials around a particular RT quantile. (e–h) Normalized firing rate as a function of time for a population of 84 FEF neurons (V, VM, and M). Activity is for correct saccades in the four experimental conditions indicated. Each colored trace includes 20% of the trials recorded from each participating cell around a particular RT quantile. Lighter shades behind lines indicate ± 1 SE across cells. Color bars apply to both simulated and recorded data in incongruent (color bar in [f]) or congruent conditions (color bar in [h]). The scale bars in (d) apply to all panels.

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Correlation between RT and activity during correct saccades away from the RF.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

For each row, the corresponding target-reward-saccade configuration is depicted by the icon on the far right. (a) Normalized neural responses. Each colored trace corresponds to population firing rate as a function of time for a subset of trials around a particular RT quantile. Traces are the same as in Figure 7f,h, but with a shorter scale on the y axes, as marked on the bottom panel. The dashed reference line is identical across panels. (b) Distributions of Spearman correlation coefficients between RT and mean activity, ρ(Rm,RT), where Rm is the mean response between the go signal and saccade onset. The data in each histogram are from the same 84 cells used in Figure 7. White bars correspond to significant correlations (p<0.05). Pink triangles mark mean values, with significance from signed-rank tests shown next to them.
Figure 7—figure supplement 2. RT sensitivity of the VM/M population activity.

Figure 7—figure supplement 2.

The format is exactly the same as in Figure 7, except that the results are based only on the activity of the cells classified as VM and M; that is, all of the V neurons (n=26, visuomotor index < 0.46) were excluded. The resulting population averages (e–h) showed only slight differences with respect to the averages obtained with the larger neuronal sample. For the simulations shown (a–d), the model parameters were adjusted so that the model matched those slight differences in activity, while still replicating the corresponding RT distributions.
Figure 7—figure supplement 3. Responses in the ADR task, in which all correct trials were equally rewarded.

Figure 7—figure supplement 3.

(a,b) Firing rate as a function of time for the motor plans RT (a) and RD (b) during simulated ADR trials. Colors correspond to RT quantiles, as in previous figures. (c, d) Normalized firing rate as a function of time for a population of 38 FEF neurons (V, VM, and M) recorded in the ADR task. Activity is for correct saccades into (c) or away from (d) the cells’ RFs, as indicated. (e) RT distributions obtained from the ADR recording sessions (purple shade) and from the simulations (black trace). Model results were generated with the same parameters used to simulate the IOI and III trials in the 1DR task, with three exceptions: the two mean baseline values were equal and less variable (BT=BD=0.2 and σ=0.14 in Equation 5), and the build-up rate of the stimulus-driven response was slightly lowered (by setting the first term in Equation 5 equal to 5.95 instead of 6.16). Otherwise, the model simulations proceeded exactly as described for the 1DR task.

To test this prediction, the recorded trials from 84 FEF neurons were sorted by RT in the same way as the simulated trials, and the corresponding traces were averaged across cells ('Materials and methods, Continuous firing activity'). The population curves that resulted (Figure 7e) showed the same smooth transitions across RT bins as the simulated curves. Both the build-up rate and the threshold increased with shorter RTs as expected from the model. More generally, when comparing across narrow RT bins, the agreement between the simulations and the overall population activity in FEF was always tight and evident — even though the model predictions varied widely across trial types. This was true in three important respects. 

(1) For the activity evoked in III trials. When the target and rewarded locations were congruent, the neural responses into the RF (Figure 7g) were much less sensitive to RT than those in the corresponding incongruent trials (Figure 7e), with the variations in threshold essentially disappearing (Figure 7g, right panel). According to the model (Figure 7c), this much weaker dependence resulted from intrinsic randomness, or noise, in the build-up rate, that is, variability that is independent of the baseline (the term η in Equations 8 and 9). In the model, such randomness is proportionally stronger in III than in IOI trials, and it blurs the effects created by the baseline-dependent fluctuations. This is explained in more detail in the last section of the 'Results'. Here, we simply emphasize that, although the sensitivity to RT manifested quite differently in III and IOI trials, there was close agreement between the simulated and neural data in both.

(2) For the baseline activity. The model postulates that the fluctuations in baseline translate into fluctuations in build-up rate and threshold. As a consequence, when sorted by RT, the simulated baseline levels spread accordingly. For the target-driven response, RT, a higher baseline always corresponds to shorter RTs (Figure 7a,c, left panels, note black lines above red before the go signal), so the correlation ρ(Rb,RT) is negative. By contrast, for the baseline of the opposite motor plan, RD, the correlation is either positive (Figure 7b, note red lines above black) or zero (Figure 7d, note overlapping red and black curves), because of the competitive nature of the interactions between the T and D motor plans. Again, even though the effects were expected to be small for all conditions, the actual baseline activity measured in FEF was highly consistent with the model predictions (Figure 7e–h, left panels). This is easier to visualize when the data are magnified appropriately (Figure 8a). In quantitative terms (Figure 8b), on average, there was a significant negative correlation between baseline activity and RT in both IOI (p=105, signed-rank test) and III (p=0.004) trials, as predicted. There was no net correlation in OOO trials (p=0.8), again as predicted. In OIO trials, although the trend was not strong (p=0.16), it was toward a positive correlation, as expected.

Figure 8. Correlation between RT and baseline activity in FEF.

Figure 8.

For each row, the corresponding target-reward-saccade configuration is depicted by the icon on the far right. (a) Normalized neural responses around the go signal. Each colored trace corresponds to population firing rate as a function of time for a subset of trials around a particular RT quantile. Traces are exactly as in Figure 7e–h (left panels), except at shorter x and y scales, as marked on the bottom panel. The dashed reference line is identical across panels. (b) Distributions of Spearman correlation coefficients between RT and baseline activity, ρ(Rb,RT). The data in each histogram are from the same cells used in Figure 7. White bars correspond to significant correlations (p<0.05). Pink triangles mark mean values, with significance from signed-rank tests indicated.

(3) For the activity elicited during saccades away from the RF. The model predicts that, when the target is outside of the RF, the low-intensity evoked response should display the same dependencies on RT as the baseline activity preceding it (Figure 7b,d). Once again, the neural data were very similar to the model simulations (Figure 7f,h; Figure 7—figure supplement 1), and the agreement was confirmed statistically (Figure 7—figure supplement 1).

In summary, the FEF activity averaged across V, VM, and M cells demonstrated varying degrees of sensitivity to RT, depending on the specific experimental condition considered, but in all cases, the neuronal data conformed closely to the simulation results. Such agreement supports several key features of the model, including the competitive interactions between the target- and bias-driven responses, the limited yet visible impact of intrinsic randomness (in build-up rate) on the evoked responses, and a central hypothesis of the model — that the fluctuations in baseline at the two relevant locations are directly linked and possibly causal to the subsequent movement-related dynamics and, ultimately, to the RTs generated.

Model robustness

We examined two aspects of the model that could potentially limit its significance. The first is the degree to which it depends on the precise mix of V, VM, and M cells included in the analyses. This is an issue because it is unclear whether all cell types contribute equally to the FEF output, that is, to the signal that is thought to cross a threshold to trigger a saccade. For example, the purely visual (V) neurons could have a lesser weight than those with movement-related activity.

To investigate this, we repeated all analyses excluding all of the V neurons (n=26 cells with visuomotor index < 0.46) from the population averages, and re-fitted the model in accordance to this more restricted data set. Quantitatively, the main difference was that variations in baseline were diminished, but qualitatively, the results were similar to those based on the larger population (compare Figure 7 and Figure 7—figure supplement 2). Furthermore, the model was still able to simultaneously replicate the neural activity and the RT distributions accurately (Figure 7—figure supplement 2). Thus, neither the neuronal averages nor the model results are overly sensitive to the magnitude of the visual component of the population response.

The second potential concern is whether the model generalizes to other tasks or experimental conditions. Consistency with prior studies indicates that the reward manipulation in the 1DR task simply exaggerates and exposes mechanisms that are always operating (see 'Discussion'). However, to test this more directly, we explored the conditions needed for the model to reproduce the data in the ADR task, in which all directions are equally rewarded (Figure 1b).

In the ADR condition, the average baseline level was constant (Figure 7—figure supplement 3, compare [c] vs. [d]), as expected given that there was no spatial asymmetry in that case. Accordingly, in the model, the two mean baseline levels were set equal to the measured experimental value (BT=BD=0.2 in Equation 5). Apart from that, matching the model to the ADR data required only two additional parameter adjustments: lowering the variability of the baselines, and slightly decreasing the maximum build-up rate of the stimulus-driven response (Figure 7—figure supplement 3). Everything else was as in the 1DR simulations. With those changes, the model generated less extreme variations in baseline, build-up rate, and threshold, and was able to replicate both the neural activity and the RT distribution measured in ADR trials (Figure 7—figure supplement 3). These ADR results provide an important consistency check, showing that the same competitive dynamics postulated for the congruent and incongruent 1DR conditions are fully compatible with the simpler, unbiased case.

Heterogeneity in RT preference across FEF cells

In characterizing the functional roles of specific brain circuits, one of the main challenges is dealing with the inevitable heterogeneity of cell types and their specializations (Zeng and Sanes, 2017). Not surprisingly, single FEF neurons showed a variety of relationships to RT in the 1DR task. Remarkably, however, the model accounted for much of this diversity on the basis of a simple intuition: the build-up rate of the target-driven activity is determined by two factors, one that is coupled to the baseline and another that is not, and those factors are weighted in different proportions across single neurons. In this section. we first describe the range of RT preferences measured in single FEF cells and then show that those diverse preferences naturally fall out of the elements already built into the model.

We examined the responses of individual FEF neurons during correct saccades into the RF, and found that their dependencies on RT could deviate quite substantially from those of the average population. This is illustrated with two example cells for which the maximum level of activity across trials was modulated strongly — and in opposite directions (Figure 9a–f). To view all the responses recorded from a given neuron simultaneously, these were arranged as activity maps in which color corresponds to firing intensity and trials are ordered according to RT (Figure 9a,d). In this way, it is clear that both cells were most active shortly before the saccade (white marks on the right) and that their firing rates were very different for the fastest versus the slowest responses (top vs. bottom trials). One cell preferred fast trials, that is, it fired at higher rates for short RTs, whereas the other preferred slow trials, that is, it fired at higher rates for long RTs. The contrast is also apparent when the same data are plotted as collections of firing rate traces sorted and color-coded by RT, as done in previous figures (Figure 9b,e; compare to Figure 7e).

Figure 9. RT sensitivity of individual FEF neurons.

Figure 9.

(a–c) Responses of a single FEF cell that fired preferentially during short RTs. All data are from IOI trials (icon). (a) Activity map. Each row corresponds to one trial, and shows the cell's firing rate (represented by color) as a function of time (x axis) with spikes aligned to the go signal (vertical white line). Trials are ordered by RT from fastest (top) to slowest (bottom). For this cell, the time of peak activity (black marks) closely tracks saccade onset (white marks). (b) Firing rate as a function of time, with trials sorted and color-coded by RT, as in Figure 7. (c) Peak response as a function of RT. Each dot corresponds to one trial. The gray line shows linear fit. Numbers indicate Spearman correlation ρ(Rp,RT) and significance. (d–f) As (a–c) but for a single cell that fired preferentially during long RTs. (g) Spearman correlation between peak response and RT for all classified cells (n=132). For standard cell types (V, VM, M), values on the abscissa correspond to visuomotor index; for other cells, they are arbitrary. Shades demarcate ranges approximately corresponding to standard V, VM, and M categories. All correlation values are from IOI trials. Brown points indicate significant neurons (light, p< 0.05; dark p<0.01). Arrows identify example cells in panels above.

For each recorded neuron, sensitivity to RT was quantified by ρ(Rp,RT), the Spearman correlation between the peak response and RT across trials ('Materials and methods, Statistical analyses'). Negative values correspond to a preference for short RTs, as for the first example cell (Figure 9c), whereas positive values correspond to a preference for long RTs, as for the second example cell (Figure 9f). Across our FEF sample, the resulting distribution of correlation coefficients (mean ρ=0.12, p=0.0005, signed rank test) was notable in two ways. First, it contained many more significant correlations, both positive and negative, than expected just by chance (43 of 132 cells 33% were significant with p< 0.05, as opposed to 6.75 expected by chance; p=1022, binomial test). Thus, a substantial fraction of the FEF neurons had robust temporal preferences, with both modulation types represented (Figure 9g, colored points). Second, the distribution was approximately the same for all the standard FEF cell types. The proportion of positive and negative correlations, as well as the fraction of significant neurons, was statistically the same for the V, VM, M, and other categories (Figure 9g; p>0.2, binomial tests). So, as far as we could tell, the sensitivity to RT spanned a similar range for all the elements of the FEF circuitry.

These results explain the moderate RT sensitivity seen in the average population activity during IOI trials (Figure 7e) as the sum of two opposing contributions from subpopulations with temporal preferences that partially offset each other. Within the fast-preferring group (ρ<0), the pattern of response trajectories of many cells was qualitatively similar to that of the average population but showed more extreme variations across RT bins (Figure 10f; compare to Figure 7e). By contrast, only a few of the neurons within the slow-preferring group (ρ>0) demonstrated a strong correlation with RT (Figures 9d–f and 10g); for most of them, the dependence on RT, particularly during the 100 ms before movement onset, was more modest (Figure 10h). Thus, when the responses of the fast- and slow-preferring neurons are combined, their temporal dependencies partially cancel out, and the overall population activity ends up resembling an attenuated version of the fast-preferring responses. Similar results were obtained in the congruent condition. That is, both fast- and slow-preferring cells were also found during III trials (Figure 10i,j), except that in this case, the complementary modulations canceled out more fully upon averaging (Figure 7g).

Figure 10. RT sensitivity in subpopulations of FEF neurons.

Figure 10.

All panels show firing rate as a function of time, with each colored curve based on a different subset of trials selected according to RT, as in Figure 7. Only activity associated with correct saccades into the RF is displayed. (a–e) Simulated activity demonstrating a clear preference for either short (a, d) or long RTs (b, c, e). The model generated fast- and slow-preferring responses in both IOI (a–c) and III trials (d, e) with varying modulation strengths. (f–j) As in (a–e) but based on the responses of various subsets of FEF neurons with similar RT preferences, as quantified based on ρ(Rp,RT). Each subset is a selection from the same pool of 84 recorded neurons used in Figure 7 (in [f] ρ<0 and p<0.05; in [g] ρ>0 and p<0.05; in [h] ρ>0; in (i) ρ<0; in [j] ρ>0; visuomotor index >0.4 in all cases). Numbers of participating cells are indicated in each panel. Lighter shades behind lines indicate ± 1 SE across cells. Color bars apply to both simulated and recorded data, either in IOI (color bar in h) or III trials (colorbar in j).

The temporal heterogeneity just discussed was readily replicated by the model. For incongruent trials, strong modulation could be simulated favoring either short (Figure 10a) or long RTs (Figure 10b), but more modest temporal sensitivity like that exhibited by the majority of slow-preferring neurons could be reproduced too (Figure 10c). In all of these cases, the simulated response trajectories matched the experimental results extremely well (compare to Figure 10f–h). The most visible (but still minor) discrepancy between the simulated and neuronal trajectories was due to the discontinuity of the threshold-crossing event in the former, as opposed to the sharp but smooth turn around the peak of activity of the latter. Analogous results were obtained in congruent trials, for which both fast- (Figure 10d) and slow-preferring (Figure 10e) model responses similar to the experimental ones were also generated (compare to Figure 10i,j). In both congruency conditions, the simulated slow-preferring responses are particularly notable because, although they are still target-driven, they would seem to require mechanisms that are completely different from those described earlier.

How did the model capture such wide-ranging heterogeneity? The short answer is that, without changing any of the parameters, the target-driven activity in the model, RT, can be naturally expressed as the sum of two components:

RT(t)=RTU(t)+RTC(t) (1)

where the equality holds at every point in time. For one component (indicated by the C superscript), the variations in build-up rate are coupled to the fluctuations in baseline (BT), whereas for the other component (U superscript), the variations in build-up rate are random, uncoupled from the baseline. These two components correspond to the two neuronal types with opposite RT preferences.

To see the correspondence, consider once more the RT-sensitive responses of the FEF cells, now focusing on how the response trajectories fan out when they are aligned on the go signal: for the fast-preferring examples, the slopes of the curves increase progressively as the RTs become shorter, and the spread is visible from the moment the activity begins to rise (Figure 10a,d,f,i, left panels); by contrast, for the slow-preferring examples, all the curves begin to rise with approximately the same slope, and the modulation by RT begins to manifest only later, 100 ms after the go signal (Figure 10b,e,g,j, left panels). According to the model, this feature, the variability of the initial build-up rate, is the fundamental mechanistic distinction between the fast- and slow-preferring FEF neurons.

A more elaborate intuition can be gleaned from the analytical expression that determines the initial build-up rate of the target-driven activity, GT, in each trial. This build-up rate can be written as

GT=f1+ϕ+f2BT (2)

(see Equations 8, 9). Here, the terms f1, f2 and ϕ are not necessarily constant, but what matters is that they do not depend on the baseline at the target location, BT. The term ϕ, which for now is assumed to be relatively small, represents noise in GT, that is, the random fluctuations in build-up rate mentioned earlier. Intuitively, then, Equation 2 says that the initial build-up of RT is the result of two influences, a term that depends on the baseline BT and a relatively constant drive that is independent of it. The former, f2BT, leads to much higher variability in build-up rate across trials — and stronger covariance with RT — than the latter, f1+ϕ.

Now, the coupled and uncoupled components in Equation 1 differ exclusively in their initial build-up rates, which are given by the two terms just discussed:

GTU=αf1+f1+ϕ
GTC=αf1+f2BT. (3)

A key property of these build-up rates is that their sum, GTU+GTC, is always equal to GT, as given by Equation 2. This is true for any value of the newly introduced parameter α, which serves as an offset by means of which the weights of the two components may be adjusted. Splitting the target-driven activity in this way allowed us to simulate neuronal responses RTU and RTC that had opposite RT preferences but were otherwise identical, as they had the same initial conditions, afferent delays, evolution equations, and so on. Furthermore, by varying α, we could vary the strength of the resulting modulation — without altering either the original target-related activity, RT(t), or the outcomes and RTs of the competitions in any way. In other words, the split via Equations 1–3 produces paired sets of target-driven responses with different RT sensitivities, parameterized by α, but all the pairs thus generated are compatible with the same summed activity (Figure 7) and the same distributions of outcomes and RTs (Figure 5g–i). On the basis of this simple decomposition of RT into pairs of components, the model can generate a wide range of RT-sensitive responses, which are strikingly similar to those found in the FEF population.

In closing this section, we emphasize the distinct role that intrinsic randomness plays in the model, and why it is necessary. During incongruent trials (or, more precisely, when BTBD). the variance in RT is so strongly coupled to the fluctuations in baseline activity that the noise in the build-up rate has a negligible impact (in Equation 2, ϕf2). In that case, even large variations (of 100%) in noise have relatively little consequence, and RTC and RTU correspond almost perfectly to the fast- and slow-preferring neurons, respectively. By contrast, during congruent trials (or, more precisely, when BT>BD), a relatively large amount of independent noise (ϕ>f2) is necessary to reproduce the lack of sensitivity to RT in the average population activity (Figure 7g). In that case, the preferences for short and long RTs of the simulated responses (Figure 10d,e) are noticeably sensitive to modest variations (of 20%) in noise, and the temporal heterogeneity of the RTU and RTC components is more complicated; the details are beyond the scope of this report. Nevertheless, the conclusion is clear: the large variance in RT observed during incongruent trials vastly exceeds that associated with intrinsic noise in the rising activity, and is fundamentally determined by the covert conflict between competing saccade plans and the ensuing dynamics; whereas the much smaller variance in RT observed during congruent trials is best explained by noise that is independent of the competitive interactions, which make just a modest contribution in that case.

Discussion

Reactive saccades are to voluntary behavior what the harmonic oscillator is to classical mechanics — the simplest non-trivial problem. And yet, the fundamental mechanisms that determine their timing have remained poorly understood. We revisited the established account of how saccades are programmed, the rise-to-threshold process, and found new dynamics that explain why the variance of saccadic RTs is high even under minimalistic stimulation conditions. Key to this was the saccadic competition model, which connected numerous empirical observations and provided a deeper understanding of the full RT distributions in terms of circuit interactions.

Motor conflict is critical

We examined single-neuron activity in FEF, a cortical area whose role in saccade generation and attentional deployment is firmly established (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985; Tehovnik et al., 2000; Squire et al., 2012), and where the activity of movement-related neurons is perhaps most emblematic of the idealized rise to threshold (Hanes and Schall, 1996; Fecteau and Munoz, 2007; Stanford et al., 2010; Ding and Gold, 2012; Costello et al., 2013). We found that the three main components of this process — the baseline activity preceding target presentation, the build-up rate of the evoked response, and the saccade threshold — fluctuate in a coordinated fashion. This is already a significant departure from the simpler, standard framework in which the only source of variability (within a given experimental condition) is the build-up rate. But the problem is more complicated, because by themselves, the interrelationships between these three variables are insufficient to explain the variations in RT accurately; for that, it is critical to consider not only the target-driven response but also the weaker, internally driven activity favoring saccades to alternative locations.

During reactive saccades, it may seem as if only one motor plan is possible, but this is rather illusory. When the target appears, oculomotor activity begins to grow in response to it, but this ramping process (represented by RT in the model) does not start from the same neutral state every time; instead, it occurs while other incipient, internally driven motor plans (represented by RD) are also developing, and the time necessary to resolve the ensuing conflict depends on how advanced and how congruent those budding, bias-driven plans are relative to the target-driven response. Thus, the high variance in RT results not from noisy representations or sloppy computations, but rather from the normal operation of a well-oiled motor-selection machine (Najemnik and Geisler, 2005; Oostwoud Wijdenes et al., 2016; Tian et al., 2016). Making the target unique, highly visible, and task-relevant minimizes potential variance related to the sensory detection step and enhances the priority of the target-related plan, but still leaves those alternative internal plans relatively unconstrained. What the 1DR task does is to align the internal biases with a specific direction — that of the expected reward — and it is under those conditions that the motor selection process becomes more apparent.

Contrary to current ideas, we found that noise in the build-up rate is not the main source of RT variance. Such noise was discernible but only during congruent trials, when the target-driven activity typically starts with the higher baseline and there is minimal motor ambiguity to begin with (Figure 3a). More typically, the proportion of variance in RT due to such intrinsic randomness is smaller (ADR task), and may become negligible (1DR task, incongruent trials) as motor conflict increases.

The model revealed completely novel mechanistic details of the motor selection process but, on average, its manifestations during the rise to threshold were nevertheless quite subtle, particularly during correct saccades. Consider, for example, how the longest RTs in correct trials are produced (Figure 5b,h). That was quite a puzzle. In that case, the target-driven activity, which starts with a lower baseline than the opposing plan, must somehow rise at about the lowest possible build-up rate and yet still win the competition by a large margin. The solution is for the target-driven response to rise quickly initially, suppress the opposing plan early on, and then slow down immediately afterward — all of which happens when the competing plans are far from threshold. This is a major departure from standard choice models, in which the outcome is not determined until activity is close to or at threshold. It means that critical dynamical interactions may occur at quite low levels of activity, where they are less readily apparent and much more difficult to characterize without prior knowledge of their signature features.

The model has numerous moving parts, but consider the scope of the data that it reconciles ('Materials and methods, Correspondence between data and model parameters'). Behaviorally, the model generated errors at the appropriate rates and reproduced three distinct RT distributions in their entirety (Figure 5g–i). Neurophysiologically, it replicated the average response trajectories in all conditions (Figure 5d–f), the isolated effect of the spatial bias (Figure 6c–f), the dependence on RT of the average activity (Figure 7), and most remarkably, the responses of individual FEF neurons, which showed a wide range of RT preferences (Figure 10). With minimal adjustment, the model also replicated the empirical results in the ADR task (Figure 7—figure supplement 3). Thus, a large number of disparate observations are subsumed into one coherent framework for resolving the kind of saccadic conflict that must typify naturally occurring oculomotor behaviors.

The threshold varies — but not as we thought

Our results indicate that the saccade threshold is not constant. Instead, it fluctuates quite dramatically, and in tandem with other elements of the circuitry. Although the proposed dynamics remain to be tested directly, substantial agreement can already be found with extant data. For instance, in the model, a stronger internally driven activity promotes a lower threshold, and indeed, movement-related activity preceding memory-guided saccades, anti-saccades, or saccades triggered by blinks is considerably weaker than that for stimulus-driven saccades (Edelman and Goldberg, 2001; Jantz et al., 2013; Jagadisan and Gandhi, 2017). In addition, the presaccadic activity measured during visual search is less vigorous for incorrect than for correct responses to the same location (Thompson et al., 2005), presumably because the former involve a stronger internal (and erroneous) drive that promotes a lower threshold, just as in our data. And, again in the context of visual search, one study (Heitz and Schall, 2012) showed multiple differences in FEF activity across task conditions similar to those found here; that is, when the mean RT was shorter, the baseline, build-up rate, and threshold were all higher.

These findings place significant constraints on the trigger mechanism that converts a saccade plan into a committed, uncancelable command. For instance, a popular idea is that adjustments in threshold may serve to trade speed against accuracy during choices (Lo and Wang, 2006; Bogacz et al., 2010; Heitz and Schall, 2012; but see Salinas et al., 2014; Thura et al., 2014; Thura and Cisek, 2016, 2017). This is simply because, everything else being equal, activity that ramps toward a higher threshold should take longer to reach it, thus providing more time for deliberation. However, the results in the 1DR task are entirely antithetical to these notions: first, congruent trials produce shorter RTs and higher accuracy than incongruent ones, and second, the observed variations in threshold are linked to variations in baseline and build-up rate in ways that, according to the standard characterization of the speed-accuracy tradeoff, are entirely inconsistent. A higher threshold, which lengthens the deliberation period, is typically accompanied by a higher baseline and a higher build-up rate, both of which shorten that period.

These considerations are significant because, although the threshold is a universal feature of decision-making models, the strongest evidence of its existence is precisely the behavior of oculomotor neurons in FEF and SC (Hanes and Schall, 1996; Lo and Wang, 2006; Brown et al., 2008; Stanford et al., 2010; Ding and Gold, 2012). In other related circuits, either no threshold is apparent (Ding and Gold, 2010; Stuphorn et al., 2010) or its implementation is much less evident (Afshar et al., 2011; Hayden et al., 2011).

It is all about the base

Sumner (2011) has pinpointed why explaining saccadic latency distributions has been so challenging: it is well established that Gaussian variability in the build-up rate of a rise-to-threshold process accurately reproduces their characteristic skewed shapes (Carpenter and Williams, 1995; Hanes and Schall, 1996; Fecteau and Munoz, 2007), but most factors that are known to affect saccadic RT in simple tasks are normally modeled as baseline shifts (Sumner, 2011). Our results suggest that the dichotomy is false. The fluctuations in baseline, build-up rate, and RT are inextricably linked (Figures 4, 6, 7 and 8); they involve alternate, covert motor plans and depend on multiple neural mechanisms acting in concert.

According to the model, the major source of randomness across trials is the variability of the baselines (Equation 5). The noise in the target-driven build-up rate (ϕ in Equation 2) makes a distinct contribution, but in the incongruent condition, in particular, nearly all of the variance observed experimentally — in RT, in saccadic choice, in threshold level, and in the build-up rates and peak responses of the neurons — is determined by the computational amplification of the initial baseline fluctuations.

This is a simplification, of course. First, it is possible that the link is not strictly causal, that is, that an unidentified factor drives the fluctuations in baseline and in the other dynamic variables. And second, other sources of variability are likely to contribute too; for instance, variations in response onset (Pouget et al., 2011; Peel et al., 2017), which would correspond to fluctuations in the afferent delays of the model. These contributions, however, are likely to be very small in comparison (Pouget et al., 2011). It is certainly possible to add noise to the afferent delays or to other components of the model without substantially altering the results, but what is notable is that this is not necessary.

Arguably, the baselines reflect multiple cognitive elements, including expectation, anticipation, and the allocation of attention and other resources (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985; Kastner et al., 1999; Coe et al., 2002; Maunsell, 2004; Rao et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2014; Thura and Cisek, 2016). In the model, these elements set the initial spatial priorities such that the neurons with the higher baseline have a higher probability of triggering the next saccade. This probability is a complicated function of the baseline values and the upcoming stimulus, but qualitatively, the effect is very much in agreement with the findings of other single-neuron studies (Everling et al., 1998; Everling and Munoz, 2000) and with the results of subthreshold microstimulation experiments (Glimcher and Sparks, 1993; Dorris et al., 2007). It is also in line with theoretical studies showing that the background activity in recurrent circuits can have profound dynamical and amplification effects on evoked responses (Salinas and Abbott, 1996; Chance et al., 2002; Salinas, 2003; York and van Rossum, 2009).

The model proposes a tight relationship between initial state and subsequent oculomotor dynamics, and interestingly, mounting evidence demonstrates a similar phenomenon in motor cortex, whereby the initial neural state is predictive of an ensuing arm movement and of the trajectories of the underlying neural signals over time (Churchland et al., 2006, 2010, 2012; Afshar et al., 2011; Ames et al., 2014; Sheahan et al., 2016; Stavisky et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2018). In that case, the dynamics develop within a very high-dimensional space, such that the preparatory activity is only weakly related to specific kinematic parameters (Churchland et al., 2006, 2010). Saccades are simpler because they are lower dimensional and largely stereotyped, and because the activity of any given cell generally corresponds to a fixed movement vector. However, we propose that their dynamical behavior is qualitatively similar in that the initial state of the system — that is, the configuration of baseline levels across RFs during fixation — fundamentally determines its subsequent temporal evolution, including its interaction with new incoming sensory information (Sheahan et al., 2016) and the eventual outcome (Churchland et al., 2006; Afshar et al., 2011).

Overall, the current results suggest that, when oculomotor circuits receive new visual information, ongoing saccade plans and internal settings (e.g., threshold level and attentional locus) radically shape the impact of that information, even when it is behaviorally relevant, expected, and unambiguous. Deeper understanding of the underlying network dynamics will be critical for further elucidation of how saccades are triggered and, more generally, of how perceptually guided choices are made.

Materials and methods

Subjects and setup

Experimental subjects were two adult male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). All experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, USDA regulations, and the policies set forth by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of Wake Forest School of Medicine.

An MRI-compatible post served to stabilize the head during behavioral training and recording sessions. Analog eye position signals were collected using a scleral search coil (Riverbend Instruments, Birmingham, AL) and infrared tracking (EyeLink 1000, SR Research, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada). Stimulus presentation, reward delivery, and data acquisition were controlled by a purpose-designed software/hardware package (Ryklin Software, New York, NY). Target stimuli were displayed by a 48×42 array of tri-color light-emitting diodes. Saccade onset was identified as the time at which eye velocity exceeded 50/s; having detected the start of a saccade, its end was identified as the time at which eye velocity fell below 40/s. Eye movements were scored as correct if the saccade endpoint fell within 5 of the target stimulus.

Neural activity was recorded using single tungsten microelectrodes (2–4 MΩ, FHC, Bowdoin, ME) driven by a hydraulic microdrive (FHC). Individual neurons were isolated on the basis of the amplitude and/or waveform characteristics of the recorded and filtered signals (FHC; Plexon, Inc, Dallas, TX). Putative FEF neurons were selected from areas in which saccade-like movements could be evoked by low-current microstimulation (70 ms stimulus trains at 350 Hz, with amplitude equal to 50 μA) (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985; Costello et al., 2013). Neurons were recorded unilaterally in both monkeys. The majority of RFs (76%) were located at 10 of eccentricity.

Behavioral tasks

In the 1DR task (Figure 1a), all trials began with the appearance of a centrally located stimulus. Monkeys had to maintain their gaze on this fixation spot for 1000 ms. The disappearance of the fixation spot (go signal) was simultaneous with the appearance of a second, target stimulus in one of four possible locations (Figure 1a). Subjects were required to make a saccade to the peripheral target within 500 ms of the go signal in order to receive a liquid reward. Target locations varied pseudorandomly from trial to trial. In each block of trials, only one of the four target locations was associated with a large reward; the other three were unrewarded (Monkey G), or yielded a much smaller reward (Monkey K). For brevity, we refer to these simply as the ‘rewarded’ and ‘unrewarded’ locations. The rewarded location changed pseudorandomly from one block to another. Block length was highly variable (range: 10–140 trials); the average was 70 trials per block.

In the all-directions-rewarded task (ADR), the events were the same as in the 1DR, but the four target locations were rewarded equally (Figure 1b). Blocks of ADR trials were run sporadically, interleaved with those of 1DR trials.

In the delayed-saccade task, each trial began with fixation of a central spot, followed by the appearance of a single stimulus at a peripheral location during continued fixation. After a variable delay (500, 750, or 1000 ms), the fixation spot was extinguished (go signal) and the subject received a liquid reward if a saccade was made to the peripheral target. In each experimental session, the delayed-saccade task was run first to locate the RF of the recorded neuron, and subjects performed the 1DR task after the initial spatial characterization.

The four target locations in the 1DR task were chosen on the basis of the RF of each recorded neuron. One location always corresponded to the RF center. The others had equal eccentricity and were 90, 180, and 270 away from the RF relative to fixation.

The RT was always measured from the go signal until the onset of the saccade.

Trial selection

For all analyses not specifically examining sequential effects and block transitions, we discarded the first 8 trials of each 1DR block, during which the monkeys may have been discovering the new rewarded location (Figure 2—figure supplement 1a). This guaranteed that all behavioral and neural metrics reflected a stable expectation, and that erroneous saccades were not due to spatial uncertainty. More stringent exclusion criteria produced qualitatively similar results.

Continuous firing activity

Continuous (or instantaneous) firing rate traces, also known as spike density functions, were computed by convolving evoked spike trains with a Gaussian function (σ=15 ms) with unit area. Continuous mean traces for each neuron were generated by averaging across trials. To produce equivalent population responses (e.g., Figure 3a–c), the continuous traces of individual cells were first normalized by each neuron’s overall maximum firing rate and were then averaged across neurons.

To visualize how RT modulated the activity of each cell, families of firing rate traces ordered and color-coded by RT were generated (Figure 9b,e). For this, the trials in the relevant experimental condition (e.g., IOI, III) were sorted by RT and distributed over 20 evenly spaced, overlapping bins defined by RT quantiles, where each bin contained 20% of the trials. Thus, the first bin was centered on the 10th percentile and included the fastest 20% of the recorded trials, the next bin included the 20% of the trials around the 14th percentile, and so on, with the last bin being centered on the 90th percentile and including the slowest 20% of the recorded trials. Then, a continuous firing rate trace was produced for each of the 20 RT bins/quantiles. To generate equivalent families of curves for populations of neurons (Figure 7e–h), the traces for each participating cell were first normalized by that cell’s overall maximum firing rate, and then, for each quantile, a population trace was compiled by averaging across neurons. This method — based on quantiles, as opposed to standard binning using fixed RT values —reveals more clearly the actual modulation range of the neural responses and their smooth dependence on RT, because the numbers of trials and neurons remain nearly constant across bins. Also, if the dependence on RT is monotonic, as the data indicate, this procedure can only underestimate the magnitude of the modulation. For the simulated data, families of response curves ordered by RT were generated in the same way.

For each neuron, an activity map (Figure 9a,d) for a given condition (e.g., IOI, III) was assembled by aligning all spike trains to the go signal, converting each one to a continuous firing rate, sorting the trials by RT, putting the sorted firing rate traces into a single matrix, and displaying the matrix as a heat map with color indicating intensity. For display purposes, activity maps were also smoothed with a Gaussian function in the vertical direction, that is, across trials (σ=2 trials), but this was exclusively for ease of viewing; trials were kept independent in all analyses.

Statistical analyses

All data analyses were performed using customized scripts in Matlab (The MathWorks, Natick, MA). For comparisons across any two conditions, significance was typically evaluated using permutation tests for paired or unpaired samples (Siegel and Castellan, 1988), as appropriate. Because 100,000 permutations were used, the smallest significance value in this case is reported as p<105.

The relationship between neuronal activity and RT for each neuron was evaluated separately for each experimental condition (e.g., III, IOI) using the Spearman rank correlation coefficient. The Matlab function corr was used for this. This coefficient serves to identify any monotonic relationship between two variables. As measures of activity, for each cell we considered: the baseline response, Rb (firing rate in a 250 ms window preceding the go signal); the mean response, Rm (firing rate computed over the full RT interval); the presaccadic response, Rsac (firing rate in a 50–70 ms window preceding saccade onset); the build-up rate, RBU (described below); and the peak response, Rp (described below). The correlation between Rp and RT is denoted as ρ(Rp,RT). Correlations in other activity measures are denoted similarly). Neurons with ρ(Rp,RT)<0 and ρ(Rp,RT)>0 were designated as fast- and slow-preferring, respectively.

Peak response

The standard presaccadic firing rate, Rsac, which is calculated in a fixed time window anchored to saccade onset, would seem to be the most direct indicator of saccade threshold. However, for a purely visual neuron responding to target onset, the use of Rsac could produce a negative correlation with RT even if that cell was always activated with the same temporal profile. To avoid this spurious correlation due to temporal misalignment between the visually driven spikes and saccade onset across trials, we computed the peak response, Rp, which is insensitive to the alignment of the spike trains. Although, on average, results based on Rp and Rsac were highly consistent (e.g., compare Figure 3d and Figure 5—figure supplement 1a), the former is a more veridical indicator of response modulation on a single-cell basis.

For each neuron, the value of Rp in each trial was equal to the cell’s firing rate computed in an interval centered on the time point Tp, which we call the time of peak response. This is simply the time along a trial (with the go signal at t=0) at which the cell was most likely to fire at the highest rate (Figure 9a,d, black marks). Tp is described as a linear function of RT,

Tp=β0+β1×RT (4)

where the coefficients β0 and β1 characterize the timing of each neuron. For example, for a typical visual cell with β0=80 and β1=0, the maximum rate in a trial is observed 80 ms after the go signal, regardless of RT, whereas for a typical movement-related cell with β0=30 and β1=1, the highest discharge occurs 30 ms before the saccade.

The coefficients β0 and β1 were obtained in two steps: (1) finding, from the activity map of the cell, the maximum instantaneous firing rate in each trial and the time, relative to the go signal, at which that rate was achieved, Tmax, and (2) fitting Tmax as a linear function of RT. All trials in which a saccade was made into the cell’s RF were included, regardless of bias condition. The coefficients resulting from the fit, that is, the intercept and slope, were β0 and β1. Finally, to determine Rp in a given trial, first, the corresponding Tp was found by plugging the RT from that trial into Equation 4; then a firing rate was calculated by counting the spikes in a time window centered on Tp and dividing this by the window length. The result was Rp. The window length was 100 ms for most cells (80%) but was set to 200 ms for a minority that had more prolonged responses (e.g., postsaccadic cells).

Build-up rate

The build-up rate, RBU, was computed for each cell and each trial by calculating the excursion in firing rate from the initial response to the peak, RpRon, and dividing it by the time interval TpTon between the onset of the rise and the time of peak response. For the peak response, Rp and Tp are as defined above, whereas for the onset of activity, Ron and Ton, are as follows.

The onset time corresponds to the latency of the cell’s response, that is, to the time point between target onset and Tp at which the activity of the neuron starts ramping up. This latency, Ton, was computed separately for each trial using the method developed by Rowland et al. (2007). The firing rate Ron was calculated by counting the spikes in a time window centered on Ton and dividing this by the window length (50 ms).

Several other methods were tested for computing the build-up rate in individual trials. The results reported (Figure 4b) were robust across methods.

Neuronal classification and selection

The 132 FEF neurons were classified by comparing their responses (mean firing rate in windows of 100–250 ms) during fixation, during the RT interval, and after the saccade. Multiple-comparison tests were performed by ANOVA. Accordingly, cells that were maximally active before the go signal were classified as fixation neurons (n=12); cells that responded significantly above baseline, but only after the saccade, were deemed postsaccadic (n=18); and neurons that began to respond shortly after the go signal and that were still significantly active after the saccade were deemed wide-profile (n=10). The latter group could conceivably have been included in the visuomotor category described below, but given their peculiar lack of sensitivity to the saccade, they were analyzed separately.

The rest of the neurons (n=92) had standard visuomotor properties and responded significantly above baseline between the go signal and saccade onset. A visuomotor index, which was equated to the coefficient β1 in Equation 4, was used to characterize them (Figure 9g). This coefficient naturally serves as a visuomotor index because it describes the degree to which a neuron is activated by a stimulus in its RF (in which case β10) as opposed to an eye movement toward it (in which case β11) — which is the classic criterion used to classify FEF cells (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985). So, based on their β1 values, the remaining 92 neurons were classified as either visual (V; n=26), visuomotor (VM; n=43), or motor (M; n=23). As expected from previous studies (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985; Costello et al., 2013), during delayed-saccade trials, the neurons thus classified as V responded briskly to the presentation of the target stimulus in the RF and gradually decreased their activity thereafter; M neurons showed no activity linked to stimulus presentation but responded intensely just before movement onset; and the cells in the VM group showed both visual and presaccadic activation.

Two of the 92 neurons with standard visuomotor properties lacked well-defined RFs and were excluded from the population averages. Of the remaining 90 units, only the 62 that had error trials (IOO, OII) were considered for comparisons between correct and incorrect responses (Figure 3). Further comparisons between the model and neural populations presented in the main text were based on 84 neurons, with the 6 units with the lowest visuomotor indices (β1<0.1) also being excluded from the main pool of 90, as these were unlikely to carry any trace of the motor signal described by the model. However, these 6 cells were included in all single-cell analyses and when explicitly dividing the population according to visual versus motor activity (Figure 3—figure supplement 1).

RT matching

To tease apart the effects of RT and spatial bias on FEF activity, we devised a procedure for equalizing the RT distributions of the congruent and incongruent conditions. For each recorded cell, the observed distributions in III and IOI trials (Figure 6a) were sub-sampled as follows. An III trial was selected randomly and the IOI trial with the most similar RT was identified; then, if the RT difference was smaller than 15 ms, the two trials were accepted into the respective sub-samples and removed from the original pools, or else the III trial was discarded. After probing all of the III trials like this, the resulting sub-sampled pools (Figure 6b) contained equal numbers of trials with virtually identical RT sets. To account for variations due to random resampling, all results based on RT matching were repeated 50 times and averaged.

Saccadic competition model

The model consists of two populations of FEF neurons that trigger saccades toward locations T (where the target stimulus appears) and D (diametrically opposite to T), their activities represented by variables RT and RD. After the go signal is given, both motor plans begin to increase, and the first one to reach a threshold Θ wins the competition, thus determining the direction of the evoked saccade and the RT. If RT wins, the saccade is correct, toward T, whereas if RD wins, the saccade is incorrect, toward D. Each simulated race corresponds to one trial of the 1DR task. The actual trajectories followed by RT and RD in each trial are dictated by the dynamics and interactions described below. Matlab scripts for running the model are provided as supplementary source code files.

In each simulated trial, three key quantities need to be specified before the race between RT and RD can take place: the baseline firing levels, which serve as the initial values for RT and RD, the initial build-up rates of the two motor plans, and the threshold, Θ. Simulated neural responses are scaled so that the firing activity at threshold is around 1. The baselines for the target and distracter locations, BT and BD, are specified first, drawn according to the following expressions,

BT=BT[1+σϵT]0
BD=BD[1+σϵD]0 (5)

where the variability across trials is determined by σ=0.28, and ϵT and ϵD are random Gaussian samples (different for each trial) with negative correlation equal to 0.5, zero mean, and unit variance. Here and in the expressions below, the square brackets with a subscript indicate that there is a minimum floor value beyond which the argument cannot drop, that is, [x]a=​max{x,a}. This ensures, for instance, that quantities commensurate with firing activity are not negative. The mean baseline levels in Equation 5, BT and BD, are set to be consistent with the spatial bias: in incongruent trials BT=0.16 and BD=0.34, so the target side has the lower baseline on average, whereas in congruent trials BT=0.34 and BD=0.16, so the target side takes the higher value. Stated differently, the higher mean baseline is always assigned to the rewarded location.

Once the baselines are drawn, the other key quantities, the threshold and the initial build-up rates, can be set for the trial. The threshold is given by

Θ=[1.185+1.2(BTBD)]0.73 (6)

where Θ cannot drop below 0.73, as indicated by the square brackets. This expression means that the threshold for triggering a saccade increases with the baseline level on the target side and decreases with the baseline level on the opposite side, but cannot be less than a certain minimum.

The initial build-up rates (or gains) of the motor plans also depend on the baselines. For the internally driven plan, RD, the build-up rate is

GD=0.001[1.4+1.7(BDBT)]0 (7)

so the activity favoring D rises most steeply when the baseline at that location is high and the baseline at the target location is low. For the target-driven motor plan, RT, the rise in activity has two distinct regimes. In the first one, when BTBD, the initial build-up rate is given by

GT=0.001(6.16+0.55η+2.5BT) (8)

where η is a random Gaussian sample (zero mean, unit variance) that varies stochastically across trials and represents intrinsic, baseline-independent fluctuations in build-up rate. In the second regime, when BT<BD,

GT=0.0013.0+0.3η+23.25BT1+1.3BD (9)

where the numerator has the same form as in Equation 8 but now BD appears in the denominator. The rationale for using two distinct expressions for GT is simply that the rise of the target-driven activity is very different when RT is already above the competition before the race begins compared to when it starts below the competition (Figure 3a,b). In the former case (BTBD, regime 1) the rise is always steep, the dependence on BT is weak, and there is no further opposition from the D plan (note the absence of a BD term in Equation 8). By contrast, when the target side starts at a disadvantage (BT<BD, regime 2), the initial build-up rate depends strongly on the actual baseline level, BT, and there is sizable competition from the D plan, instantiated as divisive suppression (note the dependence on BD in the denominator of Equation 9).

It is important to realize that, because the baselines fluctuate across trials (Equation 5), in general, Equation 8 applies most often, but not uniquely, to congruent trials. Similarly, Equation 9 applies most often, but not uniquely, to incongruent trials. The build-up rate GT depends only on the baseline values themselves, regardless of the label assigned to the spatial configuration of each trial. In other words, the local competition process has no knowledge of what determines the baselines, it simply takes them as input and evolves accordingly.

The main variables, RT and RD, which represent the activities of the competing populations, are updated in each time step Δt (set to 1 ms) as follows

RT(t+Δt)=RT(t)+ΔtVT
RD(t+Δt)=RD(t)+ΔtVD (10)

where VT and VD are the instantaneous build-up rates. Initially (that is, during fixation), the activities are equal to their respective baseline values, and they remain constant until the target/go signal is presented (at t=0). Thus, the initial conditions are RT=BT, RD=BD, VT=0 and VD=0. The motor plans begin to advance thereafter, but not right away, because there is an afferent delay between the go signal and the actual onset of ramping activity. The target-driven plan, RT, begins to rise after a short delay AT=35 ms, and does so with the build-up rate prescribed by Equation 8 or 9, whichever applies (which can be coded as: if tAT, then VT=GT). The bias-driven plan, RD, begins to rise after a slightly longer delay AD=50 ms, but at the beginning, it is partially inhibited by the cue presentation. During this partial inhibition, which occurs between Ion=40 and Ioff=155 ms, RD rises slowly, at 38% of its nominal build-up rate, GD (if tAD and t[Ion,Ioff], then VD=0.38GD). This dynamic is based on evidence indicating that the abrupt appearance of a visual stimulus, the target in this case, briefly interrupts or suppresses ongoing saccade plans (reviewed by Salinas and Stanford, 2018), a phenomenon known as ‘saccadic inhibition’ or the ‘remote distracter effect’. After the inhibition period has elapsed, the bias-driven activity may rise in full force (if t>Ioff, then VD=GD).

The two motor plans then continue to advance until one of them reaches threshold. However, their build-up rates may change mid-flight as one plan goes past the other. These changes are dictated by two rules that describe the two possible ways in which the competition may end.

Rule 1 (T wins): if the target-driven firing rate, RT, exceeds the competing one at any point after its afferent delay has elapsed, then two things happen. First, RD is fully suppressed, so it stops increasing altogether (if t>AT and RT>RD, then VD=0). And second, the build-up rate of the T plan is adjusted so that

VTwin=0.0088+2.6GT (11)

(if t>AT and RT>RD, then VT=VTwin). In this case, RT wins the race and the evoked saccade is correct, toward the target. The coefficients in Equation 11 are such that VTwin is typically smaller than GT. This means that the target-driven motor plan typically slows down after it overtakes the competition, and the lower its initial build-up rate, the more it slows down. This change in build-up rate represents the difficulty, or cost, of resolving the conflict for the T plan.

Rule 2 (D wins): if the internally driven firing rate, RD, exceeds the competing one at any point after its afferent delay has elapsed and outside of the transient inhibition interval, then RT can no longer advance past RD. In this case, RD simply continues to rise, winning the race without any further change in its build-up rate. The evoked saccade is incorrect, away from the target. The target-driven motor plan also keeps rising, but may suffer a minimal amount of suppression (the amount needed to ensure that RT stays below RD for the remainder of the trial).

Finally, to account for the characteristic fall in activity seen postsaccadically in FEF, after the winner reaches threshold, both motor plans decay exponentially toward a firing level of 0.2 with a time constant of 120 ms.

Note that the model generates different outcomes and RTs based on just three quantities (random numbers) that vary across trials: ϵT and ϵD, which determine the baseline firing levels (Equations 5), and η, which adds independent noise to the build-up rate of RT (Equations 8, 9). There are no other sources of variability.

Correspondence between data and model parameters

All parameter values in the model were adjusted to fit the experimental data. Each parameter typically has multiple effects, often on both the behavioral and neurophysiological responses simulated. For example, the afferent delays determine the short pause between the go signal and the onset of the rise to threshold (seen experimentally in Figure 3a–c, left panels), but they also pin the left tails of the RT distributions (Figure 5g–i) because they determine the shortest possible RTs. Similarly, the variance of the baselines (σ) generally determines the correlation between activity and RT (Figures 3 and 7), but it also influences the widths of the RT distributions during incongruent trials (Figure 5h,i).

With this in mind, note that the parameters in Equations 5, 6 were primarily set to match the baseline and threshold values measured from the FEF population across conditions (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). The parameters in Equations 7, 8, and 9 mainly determine the shapes of the RT distributions for incorrect incongruent, correct congruent, and correct incongruent trials, respectively (Figure 5g–i). The parameters that describe the stimulus-driven suppression of RD determine the frequency of incorrect saccades, the shape of the corresponding RT distribution, and the shape of the simulated response trajectories in those incorrect trials. Finally, the parameters associated with Rule 1 scale the RT distribution for correct incongruent trials, and also determine the steepness of the simulated target-driven rise in activity.

In all, 22 model parameters were adjusted to satisfy 23 basic experimental constraints: six baseline and six threshold values across conditions (Figure 5—figure supplement 1), two error rates, and three RT distributions (Figure 5g–i), each of which requires, at a minimum, three parameters to be characterized. But note that the model accounted for many more features (i.e., degrees of freedom) in the data, pertaining to the specific shapes of response trajectories, the effect of RT equalization, the correlation between firing activity and RT before and after target onset, and the response heterogeneity across individual FEF neurons.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Emilio Salinas, Email: esalinas@wakehealth.edu.

Joshua I Gold, University of Pennsylvania, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Eye Institute R01EY12389 to Terrence R Stanford.

  • National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship to Christopher K Hauser.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Training grant T32NS073553-01 to Christopher K Hauser.

  • National Institute on Drug Abuse R01DA030750 to Terrence R Stanford, Emilio Salinas.

  • National Eye Institute R01EY12389-S1 to Terrence R Stanford.

  • National Eye Institute R01EY021228 to Terrence R Stanford, Emilio Salinas.

Additional information

Competing interests

Reviewing editor, eLife.

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Data acquisition.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Experimental subjects were two adult male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). All experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, USDA regulations, and the policies set forth by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of Wake Forest School of Medicine under protocols A10-192, A13-088 and A16-192. All surgery was performed under sodium pentobarbital anesthesia, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.

Additional files

Source code 1. Matlab script for simulating the simplest version of the saccadic competition model.

This script generates RT distributions and average RT and RD traces as functions of time (as in Figure 5).

elife-33456-code1.m (20.7KB, m)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.33456.021
Source code 2. Matlab script for simulating the saccadic competition model with two types of target-driven response.

This script works in the same way as Source code 1, except that RT is split into separate RTC and RTU components (as defined in Equation 1).

elife-33456-code2.m (26.2KB, m)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.33456.022
Source code 3. Matlab script for sorting the response traces produced by Source code 1 and 2 into RT quantiles (as in Figures 7 and 10).
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.33456.023
Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.33456.024

Data availability

Matlab scripts for running the model are provided as supplementary code files. Experimental data are available from the corresponding author (esalinas@wakehealth.edu) upon reasonable request.

References

  1. Afshar A, Santhanam G, Yu BM, Ryu SI, Sahani M, Shenoy KV. Single-trial neural correlates of arm movement preparation. Neuron. 2011;71:555–564. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ames KC, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV. Neural dynamics of reaching following incorrect or absent motor preparation. Neuron. 2014;81:438–451. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bogacz R, Wagenmakers EJ, Forstmann BU, Nieuwenhuis S. The neural basis of the speed-accuracy tradeoff. Trends in Neurosciences. 2010;33:10–16. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bompas A, Sumner P. Saccadic inhibition reveals the timing of automatic and voluntary signals in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:12501–12512. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2234-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brown JW, Hanes DP, Schall JD, Stuphorn V. Relation of frontal eye field activity to saccade initiation during a countermanding task. Experimental Brain Research. 2008;190:135–151. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1455-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brown SD, Heathcote A. The simplest complete model of choice response time: linear ballistic accumulation. Cognitive Psychology. 2008;57:153–178. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bruce CJ, Goldberg ME. Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1985;53:603–635. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.53.3.603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brunton BW, Botvinick MM, Brody CD. Rats and humans can optimally accumulate evidence for decision-making. Science. 2013;340:95–98. doi: 10.1126/science.1233912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Buonocore A, McIntosh RD. Modulation of saccadic inhibition by distractor size and location. Vision Research. 2012;69:32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buonocore A, Purokayastha S, McIntosh RD. Saccade Reorienting Is Facilitated by Pausing the Oculomotor Program. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2017;29:2068–2080. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carpenter RH, Williams ML. Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements. Nature. 1995;377:59–62. doi: 10.1038/377059a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carpenter RHS. A neural mechanism that randomises behaviour. J Consciousness Studies. 1999;6:13–22. [Google Scholar]
  13. Chance FS, Abbott LF, Reyes AD. Gain modulation from background synaptic input. Neuron. 2002;35:773–782. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00820-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Churchland MM, Afshar A, Shenoy KV. A central source of movement variability. Neuron. 2006;52:1085–1096. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Churchland MM, Cunningham JP, Kaufman MT, Foster JD, Nuyujukian P, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV. Neural population dynamics during reaching. Nature. 2012;487:51–56. doi: 10.1038/nature11129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Churchland MM, Cunningham JP, Kaufman MT, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV. Cortical preparatory activity: representation of movement or first cog in a dynamical machine? Neuron. 2010;68:387–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Coe B, Tomihara K, Matsuzawa M, Hikosaka O. Visual and anticipatory bias in three cortical eye fields of the monkey during an adaptive decision-making task. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:5081–5090. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-12-05081.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Costello MG, Zhu D, Salinas E, Stanford TR. Perceptual modulation of motor--but not visual--responses in the frontal eye field during an urgent-decision task. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:16394–16408. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1899-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ding L, Gold JI. Caudate encodes multiple computations for perceptual decisions. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:15747–15759. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2894-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ding L, Gold JI. Neural correlates of perceptual decision making before, during, and after decision commitment in monkey frontal eye field. Cerebral Cortex. 2012;22:1052–1067. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ding L, Hikosaka O. Comparison of reward modulation in the frontal eye field and caudate of the macaque. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:6695–6703. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0836-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dorris MC, Olivier E, Munoz DP. Competitive integration of visual and preparatory signals in the superior colliculus during saccadic programming. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:5053–5062. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4212-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Edelman JA, Goldberg ME. Dependence of saccade-related activity in the primate superior colliculus on visual target presence. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2001;86:676–691. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.2.676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Erlhagen W, Schöner G. Dynamic field theory of movement preparation. Psychological Review. 2002;109:545–572. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Everling S, Dorris MC, Munoz DP. Reflex suppression in the anti-saccade task is dependent on prestimulus neural processes. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;80:1584–1589. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.3.1584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Everling S, Munoz DP. Neuronal correlates for preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in the primate frontal eye field. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:387–400. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-01-00387.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fecteau JH, Munoz DP. Warning signals influence motor processing. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;97:1600–1609. doi: 10.1152/jn.00978.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Glimcher PW, Sparks DL. Effects of low-frequency stimulation of the superior colliculus on spontaneous and visually guided saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1993;69:953–964. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.69.3.953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gold JI, Shadlen MN. Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2001;5:10–16. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01567-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hanes DP, Schall JD. Neural control of voluntary movement initiation. Science. 1996;274:427–430. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5286.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hayden BY, Pearson JM, Platt ML. Neuronal basis of sequential foraging decisions in a patchy environment. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:933–939. doi: 10.1038/nn.2856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Heitz RP, Schall JD. Neural mechanisms of speed-accuracy tradeoff. Neuron. 2012;76:616–628. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hikosaka O, Nakamura K, Nakahara H. Basal ganglia orient eyes to reward. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2006;95:567–584. doi: 10.1152/jn.00458.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Ikeda T, Hikosaka O. Reward-dependent gain and bias of visual responses in primate superior colliculus. Neuron. 2003;39:693–700. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00464-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Jagadisan UK, Gandhi NJ. Removal of inhibition uncovers latent movement potential during preparation. eLife. 2017;6:e29648. doi: 10.7554/eLife.29648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jantz JJ, Watanabe M, Everling S, Munoz DP. Threshold mechanism for saccade initiation in frontal eye field and superior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2013;109:2767–2780. doi: 10.1152/jn.00611.2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kastner S, Pinsk MA, De Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG. Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron. 1999;22:751–761. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80734-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Krajbich I, Bartling B, Hare T, Fehr E. Rethinking fast and slow based on a critique of reaction-time reverse inference. Nature Communications. 2015;6:7455. doi: 10.1038/ncomms8455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Krajbich I, Rangel A. Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions. PNAS. 2011;108:13852–13857. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1101328108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lauwereyns J, Watanabe K, Coe B, Hikosaka O. A neural correlate of response bias in monkey caudate nucleus. Nature. 2002;418:413–417. doi: 10.1038/nature00892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lo CC, Wang XJ. Cortico-basal ganglia circuit mechanism for a decision threshold in reaction time tasks. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:956–963. doi: 10.1038/nn1722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Maunsell JH. Neuronal representations of cognitive state: reward or attention? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8:261–265. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Meyer DE, Osman AM, Irwin DE, Yantis S. Modern mental chronometry. Biological Psychology. 1988;26:3–67. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(88)90013-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Miller P, Katz DB. Accuracy and response-time distributions for decision-making: linear perfect integrators versus nonlinear attractor-based neural circuits. Journal of Computational Neuroscience. 2013;35:261–294. doi: 10.1007/s10827-013-0452-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Najemnik J, Geisler WS. Optimal eye movement strategies in visual search. Nature. 2005;434:387–391. doi: 10.1038/nature03390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Oostwoud Wijdenes L, Ivry RB, Bays PM. Competition between movement plans increases motor variability: evidence of a shared resource for movement planning. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2016;116:1295–1303. doi: 10.1152/jn.00113.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Peck CJ, Jangraw DC, Suzuki M, Efem R, Gottlieb J. Reward modulates attention independently of action value in posterior parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:11182–11191. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1929-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Peel TR, Dash S, Lomber SG, Corneil BD. Frontal eye field inactivation diminishes superior colliculus activity, but delayed saccadic accumulation governs reaction time increases. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:11715–11730. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2664-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Pouget P, Logan GD, Palmeri TJ, Boucher L, Paré M, Schall JD. Neural basis of adaptive response time adjustment during saccade countermanding. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:12604–12612. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1868-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Preciado D, Munneke J, Theeuwes J. Mixed signals: The effect of conflicting reward- and goal-driven biases on selective attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. 2017;79:1297–1310. doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1322-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Rao V, DeAngelis GC, Snyder LH. Neural correlates of prior expectations of motion in the lateral intraparietal and middle temporal areas. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:10063–10074. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5948-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Reingold EM, Stampe DM. Saccadic inhibition in voluntary and reflexive saccades. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14:371–388. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Rowland BA, Quessy S, Stanford TR, Stein BE. Multisensory integration shortens physiological response latencies. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:5879–5884. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4986-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Salinas E, Abbott LF. A model of multiplicative neural responses in parietal cortex. PNAS. 1996;93:11956–11961. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.21.11956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Salinas E, Scerra VE, Hauser CK, Costello MG, Stanford TR. Decoupling speed and accuracy in an urgent decision-making task reveals multiple contributions to their trade-off. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2014;8:85. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Salinas E, Stanford TR. Pardon the interruption: saccadic inhibition enables the rapid deployment of alternate oculomotor plans. bioRxiv. 2018 doi: 10.1101/285841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Salinas E. Background synaptic activity as a switch between dynamical states in a network. Neural Computation. 2003;15:1439–1475. doi: 10.1162/089976603321891756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sato M, Hikosaka O. Role of primate substantia nigra pars reticulata in reward-oriented saccadic eye movement. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:2363–2373. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-06-02363.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sheahan HR, Franklin DW, Wolpert DM. Motor planning, not execution, separates motor memories. Neuron. 2016;92:773–779. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Siegel S, Castellan NJ. Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  61. Smith PL, Ratcliff R. Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions. Trends in Neurosciences. 2004;27:161–168. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Squire R, Steinmetz N, Moore T. Frontal eye field. Scholarpedia. 2012;7:5341. doi: 10.4249/scholarpedia.5341. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Stanford TR, Shankar S, Massoglia DP, Costello MG, Salinas E. Perceptual decision making in less than 30 milliseconds. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:379–385. doi: 10.1038/nn.2485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Stavisky SD, Kao JC, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV. Trial-by-Trial motor cortical correlates of a rapidly adapting visuomotor internal model. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:1721–1732. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1091-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Stuphorn V, Brown JW, Schall JD. Role of supplementary eye field in saccade initiation: executive, not direct, control. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010;103:801–816. doi: 10.1152/jn.00221.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Sumner P. Determinants of saccadic latency. In: Liversedge S, Gilchrist I, Everling S, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Eye Movements. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 413–424. [Google Scholar]
  67. Takikawa Y, Kawagoe R, Hikosaka O. Reward-dependent spatial selectivity of anticipatory activity in monkey caudate neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2002;87:508–515. doi: 10.1152/jn.00288.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Tehovnik EJ, Sommer MA, Chou IH, Slocum WM, Schiller PH. Eye fields in the frontal lobes of primates. Brain Research Reviews. 2000;32:413–448. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00092-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Thompson KG, Bichot NP, Sato TR. Frontal eye field activity before visual search errors reveals the integration of bottom-up and top-down salience. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;93:337–351. doi: 10.1152/jn.00330.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Thura D, Beauregard-Racine J, Fradet CW, Cisek P. Decision making by urgency gating: theory and experimental support. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;108:2912–2930. doi: 10.1152/jn.01071.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Thura D, Cisek P. Modulation of premotor and primary motor cortical activity during volitional adjustments of speed-accuracy trade-offs. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:938–956. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2230-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Thura D, Cisek P. The basal ganglia do not select reach targets but control the urgency of commitment. Neuron. 2017;95:1160–1170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Thura D, Cos I, Trung J, Cisek P. Context-dependent urgency influences speed-accuracy trade-offs in decision-making and movement execution. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:16442–16454. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0162-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Tian X, Yoshida M, Hafed ZM. A microsaccadic account of attentional capture and inhibition of return in posner cueing. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2016;10:23. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Wang J, Narain D, Hosseini EA, Jazayeri M. Flexible timing by temporal scaling of cortical responses. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:102–110. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0028-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Watanabe K, Hikosaka O. Immediate changes in anticipatory activity of caudate neurons associated with reversal of position-reward contingency. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;94:1879–1887. doi: 10.1152/jn.00012.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Welford AT. Reaction Times. New York: Academic Press; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  78. York LC, van Rossum MC. Recurrent networks with short term synaptic depression. Journal of Computational Neuroscience. 2009;27:607–620. doi: 10.1007/s10827-009-0172-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Zeng H, Sanes JR. Neuronal cell-type classification: challenges, opportunities and the path forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2017;18:530–546. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Zhang M, Wang X, Goldberg ME. A spatially nonselective baseline signal in parietal cortex reflects the probability of a monkey's success on the current trial. PNAS. 2014;111:8967–8972. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1407540111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Joshua I Gold1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Motor selection dynamics in FEF explain the reaction time variance of saccades to single targets" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by Timothy Behrens (Senior Editor) and three reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

In this paper, the authors explored neural activity in the frontal eye field (FEF) in relation to trial-to-trial variability in saccadic reaction times (RT). They used a simple visually guided saccade task, but with the manipulation that one of the saccade directions could have been rewarded more than the other potential directions. This manipulation creates a bias due to reward expectancy that affects RT distributions. The authors explored FEF neural activity in relation to such variability. A major component of the work describes a model that instantiates interactions between an internally represented spatial preference (in this case, where a monkey really wants to look to get a reward) and where a visual target is actually presented. The resolution of these interactions determines where the monkey eventually looks. The comparison of the profile of activation within the model match that observed with neural recordings, as do the simulated and observed profile of RTs.

The neural results show that the location with high reward expectancy has higher baseline activity before target onset than other locations, as might be expected. According to the model, it is essentially baseline activity that dictates the rest of the activity dynamics and the resultant RT distributions.

Essential revisions:

The reviewers all agree that the questions are timely and interesting. Moreover, the idea that it is the baseline activity that matters is intriguing, and so there is value in the model.

However, the reviewers also had a number of major concerns.

1) All three reviewers raised serious concerns about the readability of the manuscript. As one of the reviewers noted, "I had a very hard time understanding a lot of sections of it, and it was therefore difficult to evaluate this paper. I would really like that the authors work much more on clarity in their revision." Another noted that "the text is overly dense in places, overly long-winded in others. Throughout, the key points feel like they are buried under too many details and too few clear demonstrations of the key features of the data that support the model."

Below is a compilation of more specific suggestions provided by the reviewers concerning clarity and readability:

a) Abstract: It is hard to understand why strong co-dependencies imply that intrinsic randomness contributes little to saccade variance. It is also not entirely clear what is meant by non-linear co-dependencies. For a reader who has not read the rest of the paper, these two critical sentences of the Abstract are very difficult to understand. I would recommend making the Abstract, especially these two critical take-home messages, more readable to even someone from outside the field. It might even help to explain what the co-dependencies are in the first place.

b) First paragraph of Introduction: like for the Abstract, some of this paragraph was confusing. For example, why is the saccadic system "singularly" susceptible to this "confound", and what is the confound in the first place? Also, based on the Abstract (i.e. not having read the rest of the paper), the impression I get from the Abstract is that it is not randomness that causes variability in RT but instead a fundamentally deterministic mechanism. Then, in the first paragraph of Introduction, I see that people assume a "seemingly firm, mechanistic account" of variability (which to me is a sentence that can be interpreted as agreeing with the conclusion of the Abstract), but then you say that this is incorrect. Why the inconsistency? These points seem a bit clearer after reading the rest of the paper, but for a reader who starts with the Abstract and Introduction, the very first two paragraphs of the paper are quite hard to parse.

c) Last sentence of Introduction: again, the sentiment is interesting, but there seems to be a disconnect between what is told to the reader up to this point and what the sentence says. For example, the sentence uses the expression "internal biases", but this is not described earlier in the paragraph or the rest of the Introduction. So, the reader has to guess what you mean. e.g. is the concept of internal biases related to the large fluctuations of "baseline activity" that was alluded to earlier in the paragraph? Some more simple and clear description throughout the Abstract and Introduction would greatly help this paper reach a wide audience. For example, something like “if saccades can be very fast, why, under identical stimulation conditions, are they sometimes very slow?” should come earlier in the paper. Around the third paragraph of the subsection “Modeling the rise-to-threshold dynamics” is exactly where the take home message of the paper becomes more clear, and it would be nice if this was made much earlier on.

d) The key, novel finding, as captured by the model, is that the build-up and end-point of the motor plan depend critically on the baseline value, regardless of the task condition that caused that baseline value. It would be useful to show this empirically as clearly as possible. Using, for example, linear regression, how are those quantities related per-neuron and per-condition?

e) It would then be useful to show that the key dynamics of the model (especially Equations 6-9) come directly from these empirical relationships or more generally explain where all the constants in those equations come from and if/how the findings are robust to the particular values used. Also, to what extent is the coupling between baseline and both rate-of-rise and the endpoint necessary to produce the measured results?

f) Finally, explain how heterogeneity in the population of neural responses can be reconciled with the actual RTs that are produced; i.e., what range/proportions of response properties need to be in the population for it to produce the measured errors and RTs? Do the baseline values need to be coupled across neurons for the population to produce a predictable choice/RT on a given trial?

2) A number of questions were raised about the strength of support for the conclusion that baseline activity plays such a primary role in driving the saccade-generation process. Specifically:

a) One of the key observations in this manuscript is of co-dependencies between various parameters. However, there seems to be a (perhaps unintended) primacy given to the baseline rate, seen for example in statements implying that baseline activity is "directly causal" to or predictive of subsequent movement-related dynamics. The notion of causality is evocative when stated this way, inferring that everything proceeds from the starting baselines. An alternative explanation is that all movement-related parameters are influenced by some common factor like reward expectancy or spatial attention. Likewise, reward expectancy is likely not a binary concept, but would itself depend on the animal's level of satiation during a session. Incorporating such internal (and somewhat unknowable) variables complicates the "simplest non-trivial problem" to a considerable degree. In general, it is not clear if the conclusions drawn from the results apply only to a task that is by intent designed to strongly manipulate an internal drive (and hence relates to the question of generalizability, details below).

b) Many of the analyses in the paper rely on a degree of averaging either across the recorded sample (e.g., Figure 3), or within neurons. There are some trial-by-trial analyses that relate baseline to RT, but I did wonder why trial-by-trial correlations between baseline and other saccade-related parameters such as build-up rate and threshold were not explicitly presented. Claims are made (e.g., subsection “Modeling the rise-to-threshold dynamics”, third paragraph) that the variance in buildup rate and threshold stems from baseline at a trial-by-trial level, but I do not believe this was shown at this degree of resolution. Related to this, please separate any trial-by-trial analyses to the functional class of a neuron (e.g., for V, VM, and M-cells); my reasoning here is that baseline seems to barely change in the M-dominant cells shown in Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

c) In all of the figures with fine RT bins, it is ultimately the "visual response" that appears to be most correlated with RT and baseline fluctuations (e.g. Figure 6, 8, and so on). That is, short RT trials have strong early visual bursts, and long RT trials have weak and late visual bursts. The rest just follows. So, maybe the baseline activity dictates the quality of the visual response, and the rest just follows? The authors do not mention any of this in their paper. Of course, with visually guided saccades having very short RT, the visual and motor bursts are hard to distinguish; however, their reward manipulation was done exactly to increase RT, and it is very clear that in a lot of their neural and model results, clear effects on visual bursts alone can be recovered. Can the authors therefore comment on the importance of the visual burst on RT dynamics in their model and data? This idea would actually be consistent with brainstem ideas (e.g. Boehnke and Munoz, 2008, On the importance of visual transients. Also, Chen and Hafed, JNP, 2017 showed that the visual burst in the superior colliculus even during fixation without any saccades can predict RT fluctuation dynamics from completely separate sessions involving saccades). It would also be in line with some ideas from smooth pursuit eye movements (e.g. Lisberger) in which it is sensory variability that almost entirely dictates final motor variability.

3) How generalizable are the results? As the authors cite in this manuscript, there have been any number of studies that have related neurophysiological recordings in the FEF or SC to rise-to-threshold models and/or RT distributions. Indeed, and as found here, many of these neurophysiological results have challenged the idea of a fixed threshold, and instead related changes in RT to variations in baseline, variations in rate of rise, or variations in the onset of saccade-related accumulation (see below); all of these previous studies have relied on different behavioral tasks.

Thus, a key question is: Is the framework proposed here specific to a task that manipulates spatial reward expectancy, uses a high degree of temporal predictability (e.g., the fixation period is fixed at 1,000 ms), and/or requires selection between an internal bias and an externally-cued alternative? The authors may be able to resolve this to some degree by analyzing data collected from the "all direction rewarded" condition, or perhaps even from the delayed saccade task. These tasks can also feature substantial RT variability; would they also show co-dependencies in baseline activity, buildup rate, and threshold? If possible, it would be very informative to see FEF data from these other tasks analyzed like the data shown in Figures 6, 8, and 9.

Other works that have investigated this issue have stressed the importance of the time of onset of saccade-related accumulation in determining RT variance (e.g., Pouget, Loga et al., 2011 in the context of adaptive RT adjustments; Peel et al., 2017 in the context of delayed response tasks; see also the review by Teichert, Grinwald and Ferrera, J Neurophys 2016). In fact, in the Pouget and Peel papers, changes in onset of accumulation, rather than changes in baseline, rate of rise, or threshold, best explained associated changes in RT, even down to the trial-by-trial level; the Pouget paper seems particularly relevant, given that the behavior in that paper (featuring a reactive saccade generated after an error) also involves resolution of an internal bias (in this case to slow down) with an externally-cued alternative. Given this, I was surprised that onset was not considered in the current paper. Indeed, it seems like the onset of activity is varying in the neural activity shown in Figure 7A (3rd row), and in Figure 8A and 8D. In reactive tasks, the onset of accumulation may be particularly important (or easier to extract) for predominantly movement-related neurons (see also the work of Purcell, Schall and colleagues). Some discussion, and ideally analysis, of onset of saccade-related activity would seem to be warranted.

An assumption throughout the manuscript is that the saccades made in the various conditions are the same from the perspectives of saccade metrics or kinematics; at the end of the subsection “Behavioral manifestations of a spatial bias” it is stated explicitly that a conflict between target and reward location will play out in saccade direction and timing. My sense is that this is unlikely to be the case; previous work with the 1-DR task emphasized that saccades to rewarded locations are also faster than those to unrewarded locations (see for example Takikawa, Kawagoe, Itoh, Nakahara and Hikosaka, Exp Brain Res 2002); saccade metrics and kinematics can also influence the firing of SC neurons (e.g., Ikeda and Hikosaka, J Neurophys 2007). My suggestion is that the authors describe any changes in metrics (e.g., endpoint scatter) and velocity for saccades to rewarded vs. unrewarded locations, and consider how any dependencies may or may not influence the interpretation of their data. To be clear, it may be that the FEF is simply far enough upstream that such considerations are not pertinent, but I would like to hear the authors' views on this.

Finally, how do the results relate to recent results from the laboratory of Brian Corneil reversibly inactivating the FEF and exploring SC activity dynamics and RT variability?

eLife. 2018 Apr 13;7:e33456. doi: 10.7554/eLife.33456.028

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) All three reviewers raised serious concerns about the readability of the manuscript. As one of the reviewers noted, "I had a very hard time understanding a lot of sections of it, and it was therefore difficult to evaluate this paper. I would really like that the authors work much more on clarity in their revision." Another noted that "the text is overly dense in places, overly long-winded in others. Throughout, the key points feel like they are buried under too many details and too few clear demonstrations of the key features of the data that support the model."

We really appreciate all the specific suggestions. Also, the manuscript has been revised at various points with this comment in mind.

Below is a compilation of more specific suggestions provided by the reviewers concerning clarity and readability:

a) Abstract: It is hard to understand why strong co-dependencies imply that intrinsic randomness contributes little to saccade variance. It is also not entirely clear what is meant by non-linear co-dependencies. For a reader who has not read the rest of the paper, these two critical sentences of the Abstract are very difficult to understand. I would recommend making the Abstract, especially these two critical take-home messages, more readable to even someone from outside the field. It might even help to explain what the co-dependencies are in the first place.

Got it. We have rewritten the second half of the Abstract to better integrate the take-home messages:

“In studies of voluntary movement, a most elemental quantity is the reaction time (RT) between the onset of a visual stimulus and a saccade toward it. […] The ensuing conflict resolution process, which manifests via subtle covariations between baseline activity, build-up rate, and threshold, consists of fundamentally deterministic interactions, and explains the observed RT distributions while invoking only a small amount of intrinsic randomness.”

We think this now presents a single, more precise and more nuanced conclusion.

b) First paragraph of Introduction: like for the Abstract, some of this paragraph was confusing. For example, why is the saccadic system "singularly" susceptible to this "confound", and what is the confound in the first place? Also, based on the Abstract (i.e. not having read the rest of the paper), the impression I get from the Abstract is that it is not randomness that causes variability in RT but instead a fundamentally deterministic mechanism. Then, in the first paragraph of Introduction, I see that people assume a "seemingly firm, mechanistic account" of variability (which to me is a sentence that can be interpreted as agreeing with the conclusion of the Abstract), but then you say that this is incorrect. Why the inconsistency? These points seem a bit clearer after reading the rest of the paper, but for a reader who starts with the Abstract and Introduction, the very first two paragraphs of the paper are quite hard to parse.

We agree; thanks for going carefully over this. The revised version now says:

“In the case of eye movements, this ambiguity is likely more severe than previously appreciated. There is a firm, mechanistic account describing how saccades are triggered, but according to the present results, that account lacks a crucial ingredient – ongoing motor conflict – and assumes, incorrectly, that in response to the same stimulus, the fundamental reason why some saccades are triggered very quickly whereas others take much longer simply boils down to noise in the underlying neuronal activity.”

This now pinpoints exactly what is missing from the established account, and reinforces the conclusion stated in the Abstract. Also, it incorporates some of the language suggested in the next point.

c) Last sentence of Introduction: again, the sentiment is interesting, but there seems to be a disconnect between what is told to the reader up to this point and what the sentence says. For example, the sentence uses the expression "internal biases", but this is not described earlier in the paragraph or the rest of the Introduction. So, the reader has to guess what you mean. e.g. is the concept of internal biases related to the large fluctuations of "baseline activity" that was alluded to earlier in the paragraph? Some more simple and clear description throughout the Abstract and Introduction would greatly help this paper reach a wide audience. For example, something like “if saccades can be very fast, why, under identical stimulation conditions, are they sometimes very slow?” should come earlier in the paper. Around the third paragraph of the subsection “Modeling the rise-to-threshold dynamics” is exactly where the take home message of the paper becomes more clear, and it would be nice if this was made much earlier on.

Thanks for identifying the specific issues to address. The last paragraph of the Introduction now ends as follows:

“A model based on competitive dynamics quantitatively reproduced the temporal profiles of the evoked neural responses, as well as their dependencies on RT, reward expectation, and trial outcome (correct/incorrect) – this, while simultaneously matching the monkeys’ full RT distributions across experimental conditions. […] Thus, the noise in the build-up rate is much more modest than predicted by extant frameworks, and the high variability of saccades to single targets is, to a large degree, deterministic, a direct consequence of motor selection mechanisms that allow voluntary saccades to be driven by both sensory events and internal biases.”

This is very much in the spirit of the take home message identified by the reviewer, explains what we mean by “internal biases,” and is consistent with the phrasing of the Abstract.

d) The key, novel finding, as captured by the model, is that the build-up and end-point of the motor plan depend critically on the baseline value, regardless of the task condition that caused that baseline value. It would be useful to show this empirically as clearly as possible. Using, for example, linear regression, how are those quantities related per-neuron and per-condition?

Great idea. We had initially carried out extensive linear regression analyses, but prior to the model the relationships between the neural responses, RT, and reward condition were difficult to disentangle and interpret. We have included a new figure, now Figure 4, that demonstrates two things, a very strong association between build-up rate and RT, and a strong association between baseline activity and peak response at the level of single neurons. These data make it easier to set up the model. The ninth paragraph of the subsection “Motor conflict during the rise-to-threshold process” describes these data.

e) It would then be useful to show that the key dynamics of the model (especially Equations 6-9) come directly from these empirical relationships… or more generally explain where all the constants in those equations come from and if/how the findings are robust to the particular values used.

The data in the new Figure 4 demonstrate key qualitative relationships, but nothing more specific that can be directly taken as the value of a model parameter, unfortunately (for several reasons; see next response). The constants in those equations (and Equation 11) were adjusted so that the model would fit both the behavioral data and the neural data in Figure 5. This is explained in the last section of the Materials and methods, and is now explicitly mentioned in the main text as well (subsection “Modeling the rise-to-threshold dynamics”, eighth paragraph).

Regarding the issue of robustness, yes, the results vary in a graded way with all the parameters (and even, to some extent, with the form of the equations). This is now addressed in a new short subsection titled “Model robustness”, and in in the new Figure 7—figure supplement 2.

Also, to what extent is the coupling between baseline and both rate-of-rise and the endpoint necessary to produce the measured results?

Absolutely critical. The empirical relationships demonstrate correlations that are relatively weak (new Figure 4), and yet, according to the model, underlie strong deterministic dependencies. How is that possible? The observed correlations are weak because (1) single neurons are noisy, (2) baseline firing levels are very low, which amplifies the noise problem, (3) there is considerable heterogeneity in the actual neural population, and (4) they do not take into consideration the effect of the competing motor plans. This is exactly why the model is so important.

f) Finally, explain how heterogeneity in the population of neural responses can be reconciled with the actual RTs that are produced; i.e., what range/proportions of response properties need to be in the population for it to produce the measured errors and RTs?

This is an interesting question, as those proportions (of fast- and slow-preferring neurons) could potentially provide another constraint for the model. However, the model is agnostic about this. The choices and RTs are determined by the summed activity of the two components, RTCand RTU, each of which, as suggested by the reviewer, could indeed be thought of as a sum over individual neurons — but these would be weighted sums. If we assumed that there were, say, twice as many C neurons as U neurons, we could counter that by also assuming that their synaptic weights were half as strong as those of the U neurons, so that the factor RTCwould not change. In other words, it is impossible to say something about the numbers of neurons of each type without making additional assumptions about their relative impact.

Do the baseline values need to be coupled across neurons for the population to produce a predictable choice/RT on a given trial?

The requirement is very loose. In the model, the C component of the target-driven response absorbs the main dependency on the baseline (Equation 3). Again, thinking of RTCas a weighted sum over neurons, each C neuron could have a different mean baseline, and their fluctuations could be independent to varying degrees, as long as the sum over cells produced fluctuations in RTCof the appropriate mean and variance. There is a lot of leeway in this case, especially if one is again willing to consider a distribution of weights across the population.

Both of these questions hint at the idea of expanding the model so that it consists of an actual population (or two) of diverse, single neurons. There is certainly room for that, and is something that we will be working on, because some interesting aspects of our data could be matched to that type of model. But that is beyond the scope of the present manuscript, where only two qualitatively different types of response are distinguished.

2) A number of questions were raised about the strength of support for the conclusion that baseline activity plays such a primary role in driving the saccade-generation process. Specifically:

a) One of the key observations in this manuscript is of co-dependencies between various parameters. However, there seems to be a (perhaps unintended) primacy given to the baseline rate, seen for example in statements implying that baseline activity is "directly causal" to or predictive of subsequent movement-related dynamics. The notion of causality is evocative when stated this way, inferring that everything proceeds from the starting baselines. An alternative explanation is that all movement-related parameters are influenced by some common factor like reward expectancy or spatial attention. Likewise, reward expectancy is likely not a binary concept, but would itself depend on the animal's level of satiation during a session. Incorporating such internal (and somewhat unknowable) variables complicates the "simplest non-trivial problem" to a considerable degree. In general, it is not clear if the conclusions drawn from the results apply only to a task that is by intent designed to strongly manipulate an internal drive (and hence relates to the question of generalizability, details below).

We fundamentally agree with this observation. Our model shows that the baselines could be causal to the subsequent movement-related dynamics, but we surely cannot rule out some other common factor(s). The reference to causality spotted by the reviewer has been softened as follows:

“[…] the fluctuations in baseline at the two relevant locations are directly linked and possibly causal to the subsequent movement-related dynamics and, ultimately, to the RTs generated.” In addition, we now clarify what it is that suggests a causal relationship:

“[…] the activity in FEF demonstrated characteristic covariations in the three key features of the rising saccade-related activity. […] This suggests a causal relationship between the baseline and the subsequent response, because the baseline signal arises earlier, before target presentation, and because it is predictive of outcome (Figure 3F).”

The issue is also mentioned in the Discussion, where we explicitly state the caveat:

“This is a simplification, of course. First, it is possible that the link is not strictly causal, i.e., that some unidentified factor drives both the fluctuations in baseline and in the other dynamic variables.”

As to the comment that “reward expectancy is likely not a binary concept,” we totally agree, but also think that the tie to reward expectation is not obligatory. The reward manipulation generates a condition in which the monkey is strongly incentivized to plan an eye movement to a known location. Cueing paradigms such as those used in attention studies likely produce similar effects (consistent with reported changes in baseline activity during the deployment of spatial attention). Studies using subthreshold microstimulation (mentioned in the Discussion) are also in agreement with this. The covert planning is what is key, not the specific factor that happens to drive it. The model describes what happens when there is a conflict between a stimulus-driven and a bias-driven motor plan, whatever the source of the bias. We suggest that the corresponding oculomotor dynamics are likely to be ubiquitous precisely because under natural viewing conditions there are multiple stimuli and multiple endogenous factors (current goals, motivation, etc.) acting simultaneously and associated with numerous movement vectors, thus creating ongoing motor conflict preceding every saccade.

The issue of generalization is addressed in the subsection “Model robustness”, as is explained in more detailed in point 3, below.

b) Many of the analyses in the paper rely on a degree of averaging either across the recorded sample (e.g., Figure 3), or within neurons. There are some trial-by-trial analyses that relate baseline to RT, but I did wonder why trial-by-trial correlations between baseline and other saccade-related parameters such as build-up rate and threshold were not explicitly presented. Claims are made (e.g., subsection “Modeling the rise-to-threshold dynamics”, third paragraph) that the variance in buildup rate and threshold stems from baseline at a trial-by-trial level, but I do not believe this was shown at this degree of resolution.

Yes, those data are now shown in the new Figure 4. Please see the response to point 1d above.

Related to this, please separate any trial-by-trial analyses to the functional class of a neuron (e.g., for V, VM, and M-cells); my reasoning here is that baseline seems to barely change in the M-dominant cells shown in Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

This is an important point, for sure. The average traces mentioned by the reviewer show that the V neurons have a higher baseline level. And indeed, the magnitude of the baseline showed a slight declining trend as a function of visuomotor index (not shown). But crucially, the correlations between baseline and responsivity (i.e., peak response) shown in the new Figure 4 were equally strong across the visuomotor range . The modulations in baseline showed no evidence of a bias in the contribution of V neurons. See also the response to the next point.

c) In all of the figures with fine RT bins, it is ultimately the "visual response" that appears to be most correlated with RT and baseline fluctuations (e.g. Figure 6, 8, and so on). That is, short RT trials have strong early visual bursts, and long RT trials have weak and late visual bursts. The rest just follows.

We are not quite sure what is the proposed scenario here. When comparing “early” and “late,” we think the reviewer is referring to the time of the visual burst relative to saccade onset. If so, it is true that the visual burst tends to produce a correlation between firing rate and RT as the saccade onset becomes more distant from target onset. However, this effect does not explain the RT dependencies of the VM and M neurons (such as those shown in Figure 9, for which the visual burst is minimal). The temporal alignment effect of the visual bursts is consistent with the overall trend in IOI trials, which is for presaccadic responses to be stronger at short RTs, but it is neither necessary nor sufficient for explaining the stratification of the response trajectories with RT, as demonstrated when the V neurons are excluded from the population averages; in that case (see new Figure 7—figure supplement 2), the results are very similar to those obtained with the full population (Figure 7).

The contribution of the visual burst is a bit tricky to interpret, but it does not uniquely account for the results.

So, maybe the baseline activity dictates the quality of the visual response, and the rest just follows?

The correlation between baseline activity and responsivity seems to be pretty much the same for all cell types (Figure 4, second row; Figure 6—figure supplement 1). So, while it may be true that the baseline modulates the gain of the visual responses, the effect is by no means exclusive to the V neurons. It is not the case that the rest just follows.

The authors do not mention any of this in their paper. Of course, with visually guided saccades having very short RT, the visual and motor bursts are hard to distinguish; however, their reward manipulation was done exactly to increase RT, and it is very clear that in a lot of their neural and model results, clear effects on visual bursts alone can be recovered. Can the authors therefore comment on the importance of the visual burst on RT dynamics in their model and data?

It is important to note that the model simulates the total motor activity in FEF, a response that always starts ramping linearly with the same latency and has no separate visual burst. The actual aggregate FEF activity, which combines neurons with a diversity of temporal profiles, behaves very much like this (Figure 7). The visual bursts certainly contribute to this aggregate, but are not the exclusive (or dominant) source of the observed modulations across RTs.

This is now specifically addressed in the first paragraph of the subsection “Model robustness”, where the issue is framed in terms of the following question: to what degree do the results depend on the precise mix of V, VM, and M neurons included in the analyses? When the most visually dominant neurons are excluded (about 30% of the cells; Figures 6—figure supplement 1, Figure 7—figure supplement 2), the observed responses show almost identical modulations with RT as the full population (Figure 7). So, the magnitude of the visual component is not critical.

This idea would actually be consistent with brainstem ideas (e.g. Boehnke and Munoz, 2008, On the importance of visual transients. Also, Chen and Hafed, JNP, 2017 showed that the visual burst in the superior colliculus even during fixation without any saccades can predict RT fluctuation dynamics from completely separate sessions involving saccades). It would also be in line with some ideas from smooth pursuit eye movements (e.g. Lisberger) in which it is sensory variability that almost entirely dictates final motor variability.

Again, the data are not inconsistent with variations in the gain of the visual bursts, as documented in those papers, but that is not the main source of the RT effects in this case; here, RTs vary fundamentally because of motor competition, which is now emphasized in the Abstract and Introduction (see point 1 above).

3) How generalizable are the results? As the authors cite in this manuscript, there have been any number of studies that have related neurophysiological recordings in the FEF or SC to rise-to-threshold models and/or RT distributions. Indeed, and as found here, many of these neurophysiological results have challenged the idea of a fixed threshold, and instead related changes in RT to variations in baseline, variations in rate of rise, or variations in the onset of saccade-related accumulation (see below); all of these previous studies have relied on different behavioral tasks.

We agree that different sources of RT variance may contribute to different degrees depending on the specifics of the task at hand. In general, however, we would argue that the magnitude of the effects in our data is much larger than in previous studies, indicating that motor conflict may generate much larger variations in RT than other factors (as detailed below). Surely, when motor conflict is minimized, as in our congruent condition, those other factors may account for a sizable proportion of the remaining variance, in agreement with our own modeling results.

Thus, a key question is: Is the framework proposed here specific to a task that manipulates spatial reward expectancy, uses a high degree of temporal predictability (e.g., the fixation period is fixed at 1,000 ms), and/or requires selection between an internal bias and an externally-cued alternative? The authors may be able to resolve this to some degree by analyzing data collected from the "all direction rewarded" condition, or perhaps even from the delayed saccade task. These tasks can also feature substantial RT variability; would they also show co-dependencies in baseline activity, buildup rate, and threshold? If possible, it would be very informative to see FEF data from these other tasks analyzed like the data shown in Figures 6, 8, and 9.

This is an excellent point; thanks for bringing it up. We had thought about presenting the ADR data but were not sure about it. Framed as a question of generalization, though, it is clearly important to know whether the model can deal with the simpler, unbiased condition.

The results for the ADR blocks are shown in Figure 7—figure supplement 3 and are discussed in the last paragraph of the subsection “Model robustness”. Indeed, the model replicates the RT distribution and the neural data in the ADR task with what we consider to be the minimal parameter changes to be expected. After adjusting its mean baseline values to the measured one, all we had to do to match the data was lower the variability of the baselines (σ in Equation 5) (the second parameter change mentioned in the text was small and eliminated a small discrepancy in the RT distribution). The ADR neural responses are a bit noisy because we had fewer cells in that condition, but the agreement is evident. The results show that all the mechanisms inferred for the 1DR congruent and incongruent conditions are entirely consistent with the simpler, symmetric case, which, according to the model, differs simply in the degree to which the bias- and stimulus-driven plans differ from each other initially.

Other works that have investigated this issue have stressed the importance of the time of onset of saccade-related accumulation in determining RT variance (e.g., Pouget et al., 2011 in the context of adaptive RT adjustments; Peel et al., 2017 in the context of delayed response tasks; see also the review by Teichert, Grinwald and Ferrera, J Neurophys 2016). In fact, in the Pouget and Peel papers, changes in onset of accumulation, rather than changes in baseline, rate of rise, or threshold, best explained associated changes in RT, even down to the trial-by-trial level; the Pouget paper seems particularly relevant, given that the behavior in that paper (featuring a reactive saccade generated after an error) also involves resolution of an internal bias (in this case to slow down) with an externally-cued alternative. Given this, I was surprised that onset was not considered in the current paper. Indeed, it seems like the onset of activity is varying in the neural activity shown in Figure 7A (3rd row), and in Figure 8A and 8D. In reactive tasks, the onset of accumulation may be particularly important (or easier to extract) for predominantly movement-related neurons (see also the work of Purcell, Schall and colleagues). Some discussion, and ideally analysis, of onset of saccade-related activity would seem to be warranted.

We did look at the time of response onset in our recorded data but found very little evidence of systematic variations in timing in the average population activity. In general, the apparent variations in response onset almost disappear once the different baselines and build-up rates are taken into account. Consider, for instance, the comparison between congruent versus incongruent trials shown in Author response image 1. The traces are the same as in Figure 6e, but limited to a short time period around the go signal, when activity starts rising. The more steeply rising trace was rescaled and shifted vertically and horizontally to maximize the overlap between the two curves, and the optimal time shift for this was only 3 ms. Other comparisons produced similar results. The largest shift was between responses in short- versus long-RT trials in the IOI condition, but it was still only 11 ms.

Author response image 1.

Author response image 1.

Just to be clear, single neurons did show a range of temporal variations in onset, but of course, for individual cells even very large effects are entirely expected. For instance, for an ideal motor neuron the response onset could very well shift in direct proportion to the RT. So, the question is really whether the population activity shifts or not, which is what the aforementioned papers evaluated.

The study by Pouget showed evidence that response onset can change systematically, but the magnitude of the effect they reported was actually quite small. On average, they found differences of approximately 15 ms between two task conditions (conditions that should, in fact, maximize those differences given the timing constraints of the stop signal task). The difference was about 20 ms in one data set but only about 5 ms in the other, which was the one with more trials (about 3 times as many as in the first set). Variations of ∼10 ms are very small in comparison with those we are trying to account for: the mean RT difference across conditions (congruent vs. incongruent) was close to 100 ms, and the SD of the RT distributions for incongruent trials was about 80 ms.

The Discussion now summarizes this issue as follows:

“And second, other sources of variability are likely to contribute [to the observed variance] too; for instance, variations in response onset (Pouget et al., 2011; Peel et al., 2017), which would correspond to fluctuations in the afferent delays of the model. […] It is certainly possible to add noise to the afferent delays or other components of the model without substantially altering the results, but what is notable is that this is not necessary.”

The paper by Peel et al. looked at a very different (non-reactive) task in which motor conflict was probably minimized. Please see the last part of this point (point 3), below, for a more detailed discussion of that result.

An assumption throughout the manuscript is that the saccades made in the various conditions are the same from the perspectives of saccade metrics or kinematics; at the end of the subsection “Behavioral manifestations of a spatial bias” it is stated explicitly that a conflict between target and reward location will play out in saccade direction and timing.

We did not mean to make such implication or assumption; our apologies for the misunderstanding. The relevant passage now reads as follows:

“In short, monkeys are highly sensitive to the spatially asymmetric value associated with otherwise identical target stimuli (for additional evidence of this, see Figure 2—figure supplement 1). This manifests primarily as large differences in RT between correct congruent, correct incongruent, and incorrect incongruent trials (for other manifestations, see Figure 2— figure supplement 2).”

We emphasize the variations in RT because they are enormous (∼three-fold changes in SD) compared to those in peak velocity (variations of ∼5%) or other metrics, but now we show the data (in the new Figure 2— figure supplement 2) and make it clear that the latter are not excluded.

My sense is that this is unlikely to be the case; previous work with the 1-DR task emphasized that saccades to rewarded locations are also faster than those to unrewarded locations (see for example Takikawa, Kawagoe, Itoh, Nakahara and Hikosaka, Exp Brain Res 2002); saccade metrics and kinematics can also influence the firing of SC neurons (e.g., Ikeda and Hikosaka, J Neurophys 2007). My suggestion is that the authors describe any changes in metrics (e.g., endpoint scatter) and velocity for saccades to rewarded vs. unrewarded locations.

Our data (shown in the new Figure 2—figure supplement 2) are entirely consistent with those previous reports, which are now referenced in the caption of the figure.

And consider how any dependencies may or may not influence the interpretation of their data. To be clear, it may be that the FEF is simply far enough upstream that such considerations are not pertinent, but I would like to hear the authors' views on this.

We think that there is no inconsistency really, and that saccade metrics can only affect the interpretation minimally. Critically, there is no fixed relationship between variations in RT and in saccade metrics, and therefore, the (neural) cause of the former can hardly be constrained by measurement of the latter. The same conclusion is reached if we look at this the other way around, that is, if we start from the assumption that the same neural activity (in FEF or SC) influences both the timing and the metrics of saccades. In that case, if we claim that certain variations in neural activity are responsible for the observed variations in RT, then we can expect that the same activity will also produce some variations in saccade metrics. But again, it is difficult to go much further given the loose association between RT and saccade metrics.

It is worth mentioning, however, that based on other recent work from our lab (currently under review), we have a working hypothesis: that whereas the threshold-crossing event determines saccade onset, the build-up rate of the same population activity at the time of threshold crossing is what influences the peak velocity of the impending saccade (i.e., higher build-up rate translates into higher peak velocity). This is speculative at this point, but it is a specific example of a possible link between RT and peak velocity mediated by FEF activity, and one that is consistent with the 1DR results.

Finally, how do the results relate to recent results from the laboratory of Brian Corneil reversibly inactivating the FEF and exploring SC activity dynamics and RT variability?

In the recent study from Brian Corneil’s lab (Peel et al., 2017) they found that the dominant effect of FEF inactivation was to increase saccadic RT and the onset of SC activity, rather than the build-up rate or other parameters of the ensuing motor plan. In our data, the onset of the rise to threshold varied very little across experimental conditions and RT bins, at least in comparison with the variations in other response features (see Author response image 1).

At least one crucial difference between the studies is that the inactivation results were based on memory-guided saccades, which are not reactive and are triggered well after the target location is known. Thus, in a way, the RTs in that task and in ours are rather different phenomena. In the task used by Peel et al., there is ample opportunity for any ongoing motor conflict to be fully resolved during the delay. So, by the time the go signal is given there is no reason for ambiguity in the choice; at that time, the motor plan toward the target can be substantially advanced already. The corresponding RT then simply reflects the speed with which the trigger can be pulled, so to speak, a process that may be generally slower in that case because the triggering event (offset of fixation point) is not spatially overlapping with the onset of the saccade target. Under those conditions, it makes sense that detection of the visual offset at fixation (which partly determines the onset of the rising activity) would dominate the variability of the measured RTs, as other sources of variance associated with motor planning would likely be minimized. In contrast, in our task a strong spatial conflict arises at the time of the go signal, and the observed RT largely reflects the amount of time needed for it to be resolved. In this case the detection of the go signal is likely to contribute much less variance to the RT, not only because the rise-to-threshold process itself varies much more, but also because the onset of the target serves as a go signal that is highly salient and spatially aligned with the required saccade endpoint.

The results of Peel et al. are nevertheless relevant, in that they highlight another potential source of variance in saccadic RTs (which is now mentioned in the Discussion, subsection “It is all about the base”, third paragraph), albeit one that happens to contribute little under our experimental conditions.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Source code 1. Matlab script for simulating the simplest version of the saccadic competition model.

    This script generates RT distributions and average RT and RD traces as functions of time (as in Figure 5).

    elife-33456-code1.m (20.7KB, m)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.33456.021
    Source code 2. Matlab script for simulating the saccadic competition model with two types of target-driven response.

    This script works in the same way as Source code 1, except that RT is split into separate RTC and RTU components (as defined in Equation 1).

    elife-33456-code2.m (26.2KB, m)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.33456.022
    Source code 3. Matlab script for sorting the response traces produced by Source code 1 and 2 into RT quantiles (as in Figures 7 and 10).
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.33456.023
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.33456.024

    Data Availability Statement

    Matlab scripts for running the model are provided as supplementary code files. Experimental data are available from the corresponding author (esalinas@wakehealth.edu) upon reasonable request.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES