Abstract
Sensory cues exchanged during courtship are crucial for mate choice: if they show intraspecific divergence, this may cause or reinforce sexual isolation between strains, ultimately leading to speciation. There is a strong asymmetric sexual isolation between Drosophila melanogaster females from Zimbabwe (Z) and males from all other populations (M). While M and Z flies of both sexes show different cuticular pheromones, this variation is only partly responsible for the intraspecific isolation effect. Male acoustic signals are also partly involved in sexual isolation. We examined strain-specific courtship behaviour sequences to determine which body parts and sensory appendages may be involved in sexual isolation. Using two strains representative of the Z- and M-types, we manipulated sensory cues and the social context; we then measured the consequence of these manipulations on courtship and copulation. Our data suggest that Z females mated best with males whose sensory characteristics matched those of Z males in both quantity and quality. M females were less choosy and much less influenced by the sensory and social contexts. Differences in emission and reception of sensory signals seen between Z and M flies may lead to the concerted evolution of multiple sensory channel, thereby shaping a population-specific mate recognition system.
Keywords: incipient speciation, wing, leg, antenna, ovipositor extrusion, social interaction
1. Introduction
Sensory signals exchanged between individuals of most vertebrate and invertebrate species are crucial for guiding intraspecific interactions, thereby allowing mate recognition and social organization [1–4]. Insects, which often depend on such signals and generally have a short generation time, are suitable model organisms to study the evolution of sensory communication. For example, the alteration of a single gene coding for an enzyme involved in the production of moth pheromones has led to the appearance of a novel species [5,6]. This implies that, during speciation, genetic factors coding for the emission and reception of sensory signals should remain tightly linked, in order to avoid the collapse of sensory communication. Where the emission–reception link of a single sensory signal does initially diverge, this may be followed by the secondary changes that reinforce the incipient isolation between diverging populations or sub-species [7]. This would explain why examples of speciation rarely turn out to be based on the variation of a single signal but rather depend on several signals acting in synergy (in human [8], mouse [9] and cockroach [10]). This suggests that each species possesses a specific mate recognition system (SMRS) combining different sensory modalities, each of which has a species-specific weight in the recognition/isolation process [11,12].
Drosophila constitutes an ideal group of species for studying the role of multiple sensory signals on mate recognition and sexual isolation: their social and sexual interactions depend on vision, sound, olfaction and taste [13–22]. The courtship of Drosophila melanogaster flies consists of a series of stereotyped behaviours [23,24]. While male courtship behaviour largely follows a genetically fixed action pattern [25], it can be modulated by several female behaviours that are correlated with copulation success [26–28]. A useful way of investigating the impact of sensory modalities in species-specific recognition involves comparing the mating patterns of geographically distant populations of the same species [29]. For example, a strong incipient speciation process has been described between D. melanogaster populations found in Zimbabwe (Z strains) and non-overlapping populations from all other geographical regions (M strains). In particular, a strong asymmetric isolation has been observed, whereby a very low copulation frequency is observed between Z females and M males (less than 10% matings in 60 min compared with 60–80% in homotypic pairs) [30,31]. This incipient sexual isolation has been attributed to a small number of genetic loci that have been roughly mapped on the autosomes of females and males (three and four loci, respectively). These loci may have concurrently evolved during intraspecific divergence [32]. The desat2 gene, coding for a Δ9-desaturase thought to be expressed only in Z strains, may be one of the factors involved in this example of incipient speciation [33,34]. A second Δ9-desaturase gene, desat1, which is tightly linked to desat2, controls both the emission and the perception of cuticular pheromones [35,36]. This suggests that the strong sexual isolation between M and Z flies may be due to the interaction between desat1, desat2 and other mating-related genes [30,37,38]. Some of the sensory cues, diverging between Z and M strains and potentially involved in the strong isolation effect, have been evaluated [30,39]. Cuticular pheromones and/or acoustic signals are partly—but not totally—responsible for the sexual isolation between M males and Z females [30,40].
We hypothesized that multiple sensory cues, their combination and their quantitative variation, could explain isolation. To precisely characterize the sensory signals involved in sexual isolation between Z females and M males, we carried out a fine-grain characterization of courtship behaviour in copulating and non-copulating pairs. We then manipulated some of the body parts potentially linked to courtship variation and involved in the production and/or reception of sensory cues. In some of these experiments, we also changed the social environment of the flies.
2. Material and methods
2.1. Strains
The Zimbabwe Z30 line (hereafter Z6 (the Z strain used here), as in [30]) and the M-type Arkansas-Louisiana line (AL) [30,41] were provided by Prof. Jerry Coyne (University of Chicago). These inbred strains were chosen based on their differences in copulation pattern and cuticular pheromones [30]. Flies were raised in 150 ml glass vials containing 30 ml of yeast/cornmeal/agar medium and were kept at 24.5 ± 0.5°C with 65 ± 5% humidity on a 12 L : 12 D cycle (subjective day 8.00–20.00). Adults were transferred every two/three days to avoid larval competition and to provide sufficient progeny for testing. Flies were sexed under brief CO2 anaesthesia 0–4 h after emergence. Males were individually kept in fresh glass food vials for 4 or 5 days before testing; females were kept in single-sex groups of five to ten flies of the same age. Each test was performed over several days and took place 1–4 h after lights on. All experiments were performed under similar conditions.
2.2. Behaviour
We did not use genetically manipulated flies for two reasons: (1) mutations and transgenes often induce undesired pleiotropic effects [17,42], and (2) it is difficult to introduce a sensory mutation into the background of a Z wild-type strain while maintaining the linkage of the courtship-associated alleles that determine the highly polygenic Z behavioural phenotype [32]. All courtship and copulation tests were performed under a 1.6 cm3 watch glass used as a courtship observation chamber, except for the video and social context experiments. Behavioural observations were always performed from 1 to 4 h after lights on.
2.2.1. Locomotor activity of single flies
To determine whether differences in sexual behaviour could be due to differences in general activity, we compared locomotor activity in single female and male flies of the two strains under white and red light (25 W, Kodak ‘Safe-light’ #1 filter; red light wavelength 590–750 nm versus 390–750 nm for white light [43]). Single naive flies were introduced into the observation chamber that was placed over a grid. After a 10 min adaptation period, the fly's locomotor activity was quantified by counting the number of lines crossed during a total of 50 s (5 × 10 s periods with a 50 s interval between each period).
2.2.2. Courtship and copulatory behaviours
Tests performed under white or red light lasted either 10 or 60 min (see below). Depending on the experiment, all or some of the following female × male fly pairs were tested: AL × AL, Z6 × AL, AL × Z6 and Z6 × Z6. A single male was aspirated without anaesthesia into the observation chamber. After 10 min, a virgin female (intact or decapitated) was introduced and we observed male activity and noted both courtship latency and, in the case of intact females, copulation latency. Latency was defined as the interval between introduction into the chamber and behaviour onset. Observations lasted 10 min or until copulation occurred. We calculated the courtship index (percentage of time spent by the male courting the female during the observation period). Pairs were kept together for 60 min to estimate courtship and copulation frequencies; if courtship or copulation did not occur, the observation was used only for the calculation of frequencies, not latencies. The test involving intact females allowed us to estimate female receptivity (acceptance of the male); decapitated females cannot produce active acceptance or rejection signals and never copulate. Virgin CO2-anaesthetized 4-day-old females were decapitated with a Gillette Double Edge Platinum razor blade, 1 h before the test. Decapitated females remained alive for 3 to 4 h but were mostly immobile.
2.2.3. Video analysis of locomotion and courtship behaviours
The behaviour of Z6 × Z6 and Z6 × AL pairs was videotaped for 10 min or until copulation occurred, between 2 and 4 h after lights on. A Panasonic AJ-D230E digital video tape recorder and a Sony SSC-DC38P colour digital video camera were used, connected to a Leica MZ8 binocular microscope. Behaviour was recorded for either 10 min or until copulation occurred. Movies were analysed with Observer Video-Pro 4.0 software (Noldus Information Technology, The Netherlands [44]) on a PC fitted with a PC–VITC Time Code Reader (Adrienne Electronics Corporation, USA) and with a Screen Machine II video overlay board (FAST Multimedia AG, Germany) with a 0.04 sec accuracy, following the description in [24]. Briefly, each state and elementary behaviour (each complex behaviour consisting of a set of elementary behaviours) was noted, and the proportion of time performing that behaviour, relative to the total observation time (expressed as the mean ± s.e.m. [28]), was noted. We also analysed videotaped behaviours to measure locomotor activity: a transparent sheet divided into six equal areas was placed on the video monitor screen, and we scored the number of boundaries crossed by each fly of the tested pairs. Activity during the observation period was measured as the number of boundaries crossed during each one-eighth fraction of the total observation period for each pair; the duration of each fraction of the period was relative, not absolute and could have a different length for different pairs [28].
2.3. Manipulating sensory modalities
2.3.1. Vision
Drosophila melanogaster is a light-independent species [45] which mates less frequently under darkness than under light [46]. To determine strain-specific light-dependence in both Z6 and AL flies, we compared their sexual behaviour under white and red lights.
2.3.2. Chemosensation/audition
In one experiment, we performed bilateral ablation of the maxillary palps (in females), aristae (in males) or funiculi (third antennal segment carrying the arista; in both sexes). All surgical ablations were carried out with Moria Worst Microscissors (#17000) on CO2-anaesthetized flies, under a Leica Wild MZ8 stereomicroscope. Operated flies were kept in food vials for 24 h until the behavioural test. Non-operated control flies were also CO2-anaesthetized and kept in similar conditions.
In another experiment, we manipulated the wings, which are involved in the emission of acoustic signals [47] and in the diffusion and perception of chemosensory signals [16,48,49]. Based on previous studies that showed the influence of different parts of the male wing on copulation [50,51], we tested the effect of different types of wing ablation in males: (1) complete bilateral ablation (cutting next to the wing articulation on the thorax; ‘Bilateral’); (2) complete unilateral ablation (‘Unilateral’); (3) bilateral ablation of the distal region (cutting beyond the posterior crossvein; ‘Distal’); (4) bilateral ablation of the posterior margin (from longitudinal vein L4 to anal vein A2; ‘Posterior margin’); (5) bilateral ablation of the costal margin (cutting from longitudinal vein L4 to radial vein R1; ‘Anterior margin’).
2.4. Manipulation of the social context and contact duration
To test the effect of social context on sexual isolation between Z6 females and AL males, we either changed the number of male flies placed with a single female, and/or we altered the male capacity to produce sensory cues using a regular copulation chamber. To test the effect of additional olfactory and auditory cues provided by extra flies without contact with the focal pair, we used a double-floor chamber separated by a mesh (electronic supplementary material, figure S1).
2.4.1. Competition between two males
The performance of two intact AL and Z6 males placed with one female (AL or Z6) was measured under both red and white light. Both males were introduced into the observation chamber 10 min after the female. To distinguish the males, we slightly clipped (0.5 mm) one wing (alternately for each male genotype), a procedure which does not affect Drosophila copulation success [52,53]. In each test, the genotype of the copulating male was noted together with its copulation latency.
We also placed an intact male (AL or Z6) with a second male of the other genotype that was unable to copulate. Male copulation impairment was achieved by depositing a tiny droplet of Loctite® Super-Glue on the male genitalia using a needle. Males were held still with a small brush until the glue was dry. Single Z6 females were also placed with an intact AL male and a manipulated-wing Z6 male (see above). All surgery was performed under a binocular microscope on CO2-anaesthetized flies, 24 h before testing.
2.4.2. Increasing male number (1–10)
The effect of the presence of 1 to 10 virgin male(s) of the same strain on the copulation success with single homo- or heterotypic virgin females was measured under red light. In this case, males were introduced in the observation chamber 10 min after the female.
2.4.3. Effect of the social context without physical contact
To test the role of olfactory and auditory stimuli produced by flies physically separated from the focal pair, we used a copulation chamber made of two parts separated by a fine nylon mesh (electronic supplementary material, figure S1; see above). In the upper part, a Z6 female was paired with an intact male (Z6 or AL), while five Z6 males, or five AL males, or 2 AL × AL pairs were placed in the lower part of the chamber. In all these experiments, the overall copulation frequency and latency of each focal pair or genotype were recorded over a 60 min period.
2.4.4. Longer periods of contact between intact pairs
To evaluate whether sexual isolation can be affected by longer periods of contact, homo- and heterotypic pairs of male and female flies were kept together in fresh food vials for either 24 or 48 h. Males were then discarded and the number of vials containing at least one adult progeny was noted 2 weeks later. This allowed us to estimate the frequency of fertile pairs.
2.5. Statistics
Courtship and copulation frequencies were recorded during the first 10 min or 60 min and compared using Wilks G2 likelihood ratio tests completed with a computation of significance by cell (Fisher's test). Proportions were compared using a Marascuilo procedure at p = 0.05. Courting and copulation latencies, courtship and locomotor indices and video data were compared using Kruskall–Wallis tests completed by a Conover–Iman multiple pairwise comparison (at level p = 0.05), after excluding extreme outliers using Tukey's method ([54]; maximum = 6% of the sample). Statistical analyses were conducted with XLSTAT 2012 [55]. For each test, the sample size is indicated in the figure legend.
3. Results
3.1. Precise characterization of courtship behaviour
To decipher the behavioural events potentially involved in sexual isolation between Z-type females and M-type males, we precisely characterized the locomotor activity and the sexual interaction of Z6 females paired either with AL males (Z6 × AL pairs), or with homotypic males (Z6 × Z6 pairs; figure 1).
3.1.1. Locomotor activity
In each pair, the locomotor activity of the two flies over the time course of their courtship was highly positively correlated (r2 range: 0.69–0.978; p range: < 10−4−0.049; see electronic supplementary material, figure S3A). The locomotor activity of each pair was measured during eight periods of equal duration (1/8 to 8/8; the duration of these periods was calculated relative to copulation onset and therefore varied between pairs; figure 1a). A significant difference between copulating and non-copulating pairs was observed very rapidly after courtship initiation (1/8 and 2/8 periods in Z6 × AL and Z6 × Z6 pairs, respectively; Kruskal–Wallis test: range with K3df = 14.4–19.6, p = < 10−4–0.002). Immediately after these periods, the locomotor activity of pairs that eventually copulated decreased and remained lower than that observed in pairs that did not copulate. This difference was maintained until copulation. In single flies observed alone, locomotor activity was generally lower in Z6 flies compared with AL flies of the same sex (electronic supplementary material, figure S2). This difference was observed under both white (W) and red light (R; K3df = 23.6, p < 10−4 and K3df = 39.3, p < 10−4 for females and males, respectively; n = 29–30).
3.1.2. Behavioural elements
To detect the behavioural elements potentially involved in the copulation success of Z6 × AL and Z6 × Z6 pairs, we used video recording to measure the total duration of several precise behavioural elements shown by Z6 females. Some of these behaviours, which are assumed to predict the acceptance or rejection of the male partner, significantly differed according to whether the pair eventually copulated or not (figure 1b,c). In Z6 × AL pairs, Z6 females that copulated spent more time preening (K3df = 8.2, p = 0.042) and kicking (K3df = 10.8, p = 0.013), but less time walking (K3df = 14.3, p = 0.002) and completely extruding their ovipositor (K3df = 9.8, p = 0.020), compared to Z6 females that did not copulate. A closer examination of the body parts involved in preening revealed several differences according to whether the female eventually copulated or not (electronic supplementary material, figure S3B). In both kinds of pairing, females that eventually mated showed a higher relative duration of head preening; the only effect seen for wing preening was a decrease observed in Z6 × AL pairs that did not copulate. The two kinds of pair showed different effects for leg preening: Z6 females that eventually copulated showed increased leg 1 (prothoracic) preening and decreased leg 2 (mesothoracic) preening when paired with AL males, whereas they decreased leg 1 and increased leg 3 preening when paired with Z6 males.
In Z6 × Z6 pairs, Z6 females that eventually mated spent a longer period immobile (not moving and not showing any other visible behaviour; K3df = 12.0, p = 0.007) and they walked less than females that did not copulate (K3df = 14.3, p = 0.002). In summary, the comparison of the two pairs suggests that copulation in Z6 × AL pairs occurred more frequently when Z6 females showed more preening and kicking but less complete ovipositor extrusion.
3.2. Role of visual signals, locomotion and female receptivity
To investigate the sensory basis of the strong relationship between a decline in locomotor behaviour and successful copulation (figure 1a), we suppressed their visual perception of motion-related signals. By definition, female locomotion was suppressed in decapitated females, although mating could not occur in these flies. The behaviour of all four female × male pairs (AL × AL, AL × Z6, Z6 × Z6 and Z6 × AL) was observed under white light and red light. Male courtship indices were also compared between intact (figure 2a,c,e) and decapitated females (figure 2b,d).
Under white light (W), Z6 males showed a reduced courtship frequency with all females except their own intact females (figure 2a; Wilks G2 likelihood ratio test: , p < 10−4). In particular, they showed a similarly decreased courtship frequency with both types of decapitated female (figure 2b; , p < 10−4). Compared with other pairs, Z6 males showed less frequent courtship with both intact and decapitated AL females (figure 2a,b; Wilks G2 likelihood ratio test: , p < 10−4). By contrast, AL males courted intact Z6 females slightly more often under red light (R) and decapitated Z6 females under W, compared with other pairs (, p < 10−4). The courtship frequency with intact females generally decreased under red light; no such effect occurred with decapitated females. The courtship index with decapitated females showed no or only a slight reduction under red light, but strongly decreased in the case of intact females (K7df = 124.4, p < 10−4; electronic supplementary material, figure S4).
During the 60 min observation period, very few Z6 × AL pairs copulated under either R or W conditions (figure 2e). Copulation latency was delayed in Z6 × Z6 pairs compared with AL × AL pairs, independent of light condition (electronic supplementary material, figure S4C). Under R, copulation was delayed in all pairs and AL × Z6 pairs mated less frequently than under W (figure 2e and electronic supplementary material, figures S4C; , p < 10−4 and K6df = 48.3, p < 10−4, respectively; n = 32–125). We also measured the fertility (ability to leave at least one progeny) of pairs kept in contact for either 24 or 48 h (figure 2f). The fertility frequency of AL × AL and AL × Z6 pairs significantly increased compared to the copulation frequency observed during 1 h (, p < 10−4, n = 37–63). This indicates that the great majority of pairs that did not copulate during the first hour (figure 2e) mated during the next 23 h. Similarly, the fertility of Z6 × AL pairs kept in contact for 24 and 48 h was much higher compared to their copulation frequency during the first hour of contact (we used the Marascuilo procedure, with significance set at p < 0.05, to compare the two independent datasets). The frequency of fertile Z6 × Z6 pairs was similar to their 60 min copulation frequency (figure 2e,f). To summarize, while impairment of visual cues did not increase copulation frequency in Z6 × AL pairs as measured over 60 min, mating frequency did significantly increase over the subsequent 23 h period.
3.3. Manipulating sensory cue production and perception
To evaluate the role of sensory cues exchanged during courtship, including those potentially involved in the fine behavioural differences noted above (figure 1), we manipulated some of the tissues and appendages potentially involved in the production and/or perception of these cues.
3.3.1. Female wings and legs
Given the role of female wings and metathoracic legs (legs 3) in copulation success [28], we ablated these appendages in Z6 females to evaluate their role in the copulation of Z6 × AL pairs (figure 3). While copulation frequency significantly increased in Z6 females deprived of either wings or legs 3, this frequency slightly increased in females deprived of both wings and metathoracic legs (figure 3a; , p = 0.011, n = 38–40). These manipulations did not affect copulation latency (figure 3b).
3.3.2. Chemosensory and/or auditory appendages
As the cuticular pheromones that vary between Z6 and AL flies are involved in sexual isolation, we ablated the chemosensory appendages potentially involved in their detection. In females, the maxillary palps or the funiculi—both involved in olfactory perception—were bilaterally ablated (figure 4a; note that funiculi, which carry the aristae, feather-like organs involved in auditory perception, have been studied in females [28]). In males, both funiculi, or only aristae, were ablated (figure 4b). In both sexes, ablation of the funiculi strongly decreased copulation frequency ( ranging from 28.2 to 96.8, p < 10−4, n = 20–96). The two other types of ablation had much less or no effect on copulation frequency: females without maxillary palps were not affected, while males without aristae showed only a moderate decrease in mating with homotypic females (in AL × AL and Z6 × Z6 pairs). Note that the manipulation of Z6 males induced a significant effect on their copulation frequency with homotypic but not heterotypic females.
3.4. Manipulating sensory cues and social interaction
Given the importance of both the quality and quantity of sensory cues exchanged during courtship, we performed five experiments to evaluate the influence of the interaction between sensory cues and social context, with a special focus on Z6 × AL pairs.
3.4.1. With two intact males
Significant copulation differences were noted when two intact males (Z6 and AL) were associated with each type of female either under W or R light (figure 5a; , p < 10−4, n = 59–131). When paired with AL females, both types of males showed a similar copulation success under both light conditions. Very few AL males copulated with Z6 females under W, while no AL male and few Z6 males copulated under R.
3.4.2. With one intact male and one genitalia-altered male
To change male sensory stimuli without creating male–male competition for copulation, one intact male was associated with a second male (of either strain) that had glued genitalia and was therefore unable to copulate (figure 5b). All tests performed with Z6 females showed that the type of genitalia-impaired male did not affect the courtship success of the intact male (, p < 10−4, n = 89–206).
3.4.3. With one intact AL male and one wing-manipulated Z6 male
Z6 females were associated with one intact AL male and one Z6 male with various degrees of wing ablation (figure 5c). The copulation success of Z6 males was significantly affected in two cases: they showed a drastic reduction after complete wing ablation and a slight increase with distal ablation (, p = 0.005, n = 31–131). No such effects were seen for AL males. The copulation frequency of AL males, which was always low, was similar to that of Z6 males deprived of their posterior wing margin.
3.4.4. With an increasing number of males
To increase the quantity of male sensory cues during male–male competition, a single female was associated with 1 to 10 males of the same strain (1, 5, 10 with AL female; 1, 2, 5, 10 with Z6 female). Female copulation was differentially affected depending both on (i) female and male strain and (ii) number of males. While AL female copulation frequency was not affected by male number, their copulation latency was delayed when placed with 10 Z6 males when compared with 10 AL males or with 1 Z6 male (figure 6a; K5df = 14.3, p = 0.014 254, n = 11–25). By contrast, Z6 females copulated more frequently but also more slowly with 5 AL males compared with lower or higher numbers of AL males (figure 6b; , p < 10−4, n = 17–46). Z6 females copulated less frequently with 5 or 10 Z6 males than with 1 or 2 Z6 males, but these matings were faster with 1 Z6 male than with 2 Z6 males (K7df = 35.8, p < 10–4, n = 6–27).
3.4.5. Providing more fly stimuli without physical contact
Using divided mating chambers, we measured the behaviour of an intact focal pair of flies (either Z6 × Z6 or Z6 × AL) in the presence, or not, of various combinations of live intact flies placed in the lower part of the chamber (electronic supplementary material, figure S1). The copulation frequency of Z6 × Z6 pairs (i) decreased with five AL males, (ii) remained unchanged with five Z6 males and (iii) increased with two AL × AL pairs (figure 6c; , p = 0.003, n = 28–186). By contrast, no copulation occurred in Z6 × AL pairs whatever combination of flies was placed in the lower part of the observation chamber (figure 6d).
4. Discussion
Populations of the cosmopolitan D. melanogaster species were initially thought to share a similar SMRS [56,57]. Following the discovery of several exceptions to this panmictic rule [58–60], a marked case of sexual isolation was detected between Zimbabwe females (Z) and males from all other strains (M [31,61]). Previous explorations of the sensory cues involved in this strong asymmetric sexual isolation have revealed that strain-specific cuticular pheromones and, to a lesser extent, acoustic signals produced by male wings, are involved [30]. Here, we used complementary approaches to further unravel the behavioural and sensory bases of this example of incipient speciation.
We sought to precisely characterize the behavioural elements of the courtship of M males and Z females in order to detect subtle behavioural differences between copulating and non-copulating pairs. The comparison of these data with those shown by copulating and non-copulating Z × Z homotypic pairs allowed us to describe several behavioural elements that diverged in Z × M pairs, depending on whether they mated or not. These findings enabled us to map and manipulate some of the sensory tissue(s) or appendage(s) apparently involved. Another experiment involved manipulating choosy Z females either by changing (i) the number, genotype and/or sensory or behavioural phenotype of male partners (figures 4, 5 and 6a,b), or (ii) the quantity and quality of the sensory signals emitted in the presence of a single male (figure 6c,d).
4.1. Zimbabwe females’ choosiness: role of locomotion and visual cues
The asymmetric sexual isolation observed here clearly depends on the low sexual receptivity of Z6 females when presented with AL males, which nevertheless persisted in courting these females with a sustained intensity beyond the 1 h observation period (as indicated by their fertility frequency when kept in contact during 24 h; figure 2f; see below). The simultaneous decrease of locomotor activity shown by the two partners [28] was a good proxy for copulation success (figure 1a, electronic supplementary material, figure S3A). Indeed, when locomotor activity markedly decreased during the very early phases of courtship, this decrease lasted until copulation began. The locomotion of copulating Z6 × AL pairs diverged from that of non-copulating Z6 × Z6 pairs only during the last two phases of courtship (7/8 and 8/8).
The higher locomotor activity shown by single AL flies (electronic supplementary material, figure S2) may be partly involved in the sexual isolation: most AL males may have been unable to sufficiently reduce their locomotor activity to that of the Z6 female during the first two phases of courtship [28]. However, the fertility frequency observed in these pairs (and in all pairs involving AL females) kept together for 24 h indicates that an increased proportion of AL males copulated with Z6 females during the subsequent hours of contact (figure 2f). This could be the product of repeated copulation attempts by AL males [62,63]. This fits with the fact that single Z6 females copulated more frequently with five AL males than with a single AL male (figure 6b). The delayed copulation latency observed with five AL males may result of an increased male–male competition.
By contrast, more Z6 females copulated faster with a single Z6 male than with two Z6 males (figure 6b). The similarity between the 1 h copulation frequency and fertility (during 24 h or 48 h; figure 2f) of Z6 × Z6 pairs indicates that no or few Z6 males copulated with Z6 females after one hour, in marked difference to the results for AL males. More copulations yielding less progeny occurred in Z6 × Z6 pairs during the 24 h period, suggesting that some of these flies showed gametic incompatibility as previously reported [64].
Although AL females were less choosy than Z6 females under white light, they discriminated between AL and Z6 males under red light (figure 2e). This indicates that the visual signals had a differential impact on the courtship of AL and Z6 males paired with AL females and that, in the absence of light, AL females were using other cues to choose males. Further studies involving similar Z6 female manipulations are needed to determine which cues are used by AL females to discriminate between male types.
The visual cues perceived during courtship, including the anatomical characteristics of the other fly and its movement, are crucial for mating. The drastic decrease of both courtship frequency and intensity under red light with intact females clearly supports the role of visual cues in sexual interactions; this is further shown by the fact that no such effect was observed with motionless decapitated females (figure 2a,b; electronic supplementary material, figure S2A). Vision may allow the two partners to precisely synchronize their locomotor activity during courtship [32]. Optomotor-blind white mutant males cannot track the moving female and show delayed copulation latency [65–67]. Deprivation of visual cues did not affect the two male types in the same way: under red light, copulation frequency was more affected in Z6 than in AL males (figure 2e). This may explain why copulation- and strain-dependent variations of locomotor activity constitute a critical signal in D. melanogaster mate recognition and copulation success.
4.2. Appendages involved in sexual isolation
4.2.1. Legs and wings
The comparison of behavioural elements that showed differences between copulating and non-copulating Z6 × AL pairs provides a hint about the body parts responsible for the asymmetric sexual isolation. Although few Z6 × AL pairs mated, we were able to take this small sample size into account through our statistical tests. First, Z6 × AL copulating pairs showed an increase in the total duration of female preening. This could conceivably be an acceptance behaviour and indicates the role of female legs in mating (figure 1b; [28,68,69]). This is consistent with our other findings: Z6 females without their rear legs (figure 3) showed an increased copulation frequency with AL males. In Z6 females, leg preening was also linked with mating with both kinds of male (electronic supplementary material, figure S3B). Two other behavioural elements, which again could conceivably be associated with female rejection, also varied with mating in Z6 × AL pairs (figure 1c). First, Z6 females that mated directed less complete ovipositor extrusion to AL males, as previously reported [28,70]. However, these females kicked AL males more often with their rear legs 3 when compared with non-mating Z6 females. The unexpected stimulatory effect of this behaviour may be due to a strain-specific effect, stimulating AL males and inhibiting Z6 males.
Wings of both sexes are also involved in sexual isolation (figures 3 and 5c). When females accept males, they open their wings to permit copulation [71]. This may explain why AL males copulated more often with wing-deprived Z6 females than with intact ones. Wing preening in Z6 females also varied with mating of Z6 × AL pairs and its relative increase seems to stimulate male copulation (electronic supplementary material, figure S3b). We do not know why.
Male wings produce acoustic signals that stimulate mating, and all wing parts are not equivalent with regard to copulation success [16,47,50]. Complete bilateral wing ablation in Z6 males drastically reduced their mating frequency with Z6 females (a similar effect was found in AL × AL pairs: figure 3b in [30]). Z6 males missing the posterior wing region showed a low mating success, similar to that of intact AL males in a competition experiment. Surprisingly, the bilateral ablation of the distal wing part increased copulation frequency in Z6 males competing with intact AL males for Z6 females. Given that the anterior wing margin is involved in the chemosensory perception of odorant and tastant molecules [48,49], this suggests that wings play a more complex role than hitherto suspected in the exchange of sensory cues during courtship. Males with just one ablated wing need to be compared with intact males to determine whether or not male behaviour is altered because their perception of female cues is involved.
4.2.2. Olfactory and/or auditory cues
Extra-additional sensory stimuli provided by physically separated males affected the copulation frequency of focal Z6 × Z6 pairs: it was decreased with five AL males, but increased with two AL × AL pairs (figure 6c). This indicates that some olfactory and auditory signals produced by M flies can stimulate mating in Z flies at a short distance and without contact. Groups of 5 AL males may have sent inhibitory stimuli, including the sex pheromone cis-Vaccenyl acetate (cVA; [72]) and/or repulsive vibrations [73], in order to deter male–male courtship, potentially affecting copulation in Z6 × Z6 focal pairs. Conversely, the stimulatory effect induced in Z6 × Z6 focal pairs by the two extra pairs of AL flies could be due to one or several stimulatory pheromone(s) emitted by flies of either sex and perceived at a short distance [74–76]. The mating frequency of Z6 females did not change with or without physical contact with a larger number of Z6 males (figure 6b,c). Together with the fact that copulation in Z6 × AL focal pairs was not affected by any of these stimuli produced by live flies (figure 6d), this reinforces the idea that Z6 females need a very precise sensory picture of their potential mating partner for copulation to occur.
The stimuli perceived in the two-floor mating chambers may mostly act through olfactory perception, and much less by vibration, given that funiculi ablation strongly decreased copulation in both sexes (figure 4b; fig. 3c in [30]); arista ablation had a smaller or non-existent effect. The effect induced by funiculi ablation was larger in females than in males. The third antennal segment (funiculus) carries many olfactory sensilla, some of which may be involved in the perception of male stimulatory pheromones [75,77]. However, the fact that maxillary palp ablation in females had no effect was surprising, given that these structures carry olfactory receptor neurons responding to cVA [78], a pheromone thought to increase female sexual receptivity [79]. Taken together, these data suggest that some of the sensory cues known to affect the sexual behaviour of M-type flies induce a different effect in Z-type flies, or at least in Z6 × AL pairs.
5. Conclusion
While none of our experimental manipulations of appendages, sensory cues and physical contact allowed us to fully rescue the marked sexual isolation seen between Z6 females and AL males, our study adds several pieces to the puzzle of this intensely studied example of incipient speciation. The cosmopolitan distribution of current M strains, as opposed to the geographically restricted area of Z strains, suggests that the survival of contemporary Z strains, considered to be derived from the ancestral form of the D. melanogaster species [80], depends on its tight adaptation to a very specific environment. Our next challenge will be to explore whether or not the survival of Z strains among a world of M strains is related to the high choosiness of Z females, thereby preventing the introgression of M paternal genes into the population.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Pierre-Alain Monfrond, Jérôme Cortot, Isabelle Chauvel, Serge Loquin and José Solonot for their technical help, Prof. Marie Herberstein, Dr. Jennifer Gleason, Prof. Matthew Cobb and five anonymous reviewers for their very constructive comments on the previous versions of the manuscript.
Data accessibility
All experimental data can be found in the electronic supplementary material (XLSX files) and at the Dryad Digital Repository (http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.sg095) [81].
Authors' contributions
M.G. designed the study, carried out the behavioural tests and revised the manuscript; J.-F.F. conceived the study, designed the study, coordinated the study, interpreted data and drafted the manuscript; C.E. conceived of the study, designed the study, carried out the statistical analyses, interpreted data, drafted the manuscript and produced the figures. All authors read and approved the manuscript before submission.
Competing interests
We have no competing interests.
Funding
This work was funded, in part, by the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (INSB), the Agence Nationale pour la Recherche (ANR-Gustaile) and the French Ministry of Research and Higher Education, the Conseil Régional Bourgogne, Franche-Comté (PARI 2012; PARI2014) and the FEDER (European Funding for Regional Economical Development).
References
- 1.Rick IP, Mehlis M, Bakker TC. 2011. Male red ornamentation is associated with female red sensitivity in sticklebacks. PLoS ONE 6, e25554 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025554) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Singh ND, Shaw KL. 2012. On the scent of pleiotropy. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 5–6. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1118531109) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wiley C, Shaw KL. 2010. Multiple genetic linkages between female preference and male signal in rapidly speciating Hawaiian crickets. Evolution 64, 2238–2245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Wyatt TD. 2014. Pheromones and animal behavior: chemical signals and signatures, 2nd edn, 420 p Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lassance JM, Liénard MA, Antony B, Qian S, Fujii T, Tabata J, Ishikawa Y, Lofstedt C. 2013. Functional consequences of sequence variation in the pheromone biosynthetic gene pgFAR for Ostrinia moths. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci USA 110, 3967–3972. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1208706110) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Roelofs WL, Liu W, Hao G, Jiao H, Rooney AP, Linn CE Jr. 2002. Evolution of moth sex pheromones via ancestral genes. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 13 621–13 626. (doi:10.1073/pnas.152445399) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Mayr E. 1963. Animal species and evolution, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Forscher EC, Li W. 2012. Hemispheric asymmetry and visuo-olfactory integration in perceiving subthreshold (micro) fearful expressions. J. Neurosci. 32, 2159–2165. (doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5094-11.2012) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Guo Z, Holy TE. 2007. Sex selectivity of mouse ultrasonic songs. Chem. Senses 32, 463–473. (doi:10.1093/chemse/bjm015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mongeau JM, Demir A, Lee J, Cowan NJ, Full RJ. 2013. Locomotion- and mechanics-mediated tactile sensing: antenna reconfiguration simplifies control during high-speed navigation in cockroaches. J. Exp. Biol. 216: 4530–4541. (doi:10.1242/jeb.083477) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Paterson HEH. 1985. The recognition concept of species In Species and speciation (ed. E Vrba), pp. 21–29. Transvaal museum monograph. Pretoria, South Africa: Transvaal museum. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Endler JA, Houde AE. 1995. Geographic-variation in female preferences for male traits in Poecilia reticulata. Evolution 49, 456–468. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.1995.tb02278.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ejima A, Griffith LC. 2008. Courtship initiation is stimulated by acoustic signals in Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS ONE 3, e3246 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003246) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Duménil C, Woud D, Pinto F, Alkema JT, Jansen I, Van Der Geest AM, Roessingh S, Billeter J-C. 2016. Pheromonal cues deposited by mated females convey social information about egg-laying sites in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Chem. Ecol. 42, 259–269. (doi:10.1007/s10886-016-0681-3) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Greenspan RJ, Ferveur JF. 2000. Courtship in Drosophila. Annu. Rev. Genet. 34, 205–232. (doi:10.1146/annurev.genet.34.1.205) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Krstic D, Boll W, Noll M. 2009. Sensory integration regulating male courtship behavior in Drosophila. PLoS ONE 4, e4457 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0004457) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Markow TA. 1987. Behavioral and sensory basis of courtship success in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 84, 6200–6204. (doi:10.1073/pnas.84.17.6200) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Billeter JC, Levine JD. 2013. Who is he and what is he to you? Recognition in Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 17–23. (doi:10.1016/j.conb.2012.08.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Yukilevich R, Harvey T, Nguyen S, Kehlbeck J, Park A. 2016. The search for causal traits of speciation: divergent female mate preferences target male courtship song, not pheromones, in Drosophila athabasca species complex. Evolution 70, 526–542. (doi:10.1111/evo.12870) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gleason JM, Pierce AA, Vezeau AL, Goodman SF. 2012. Different sensory modalities are required for successful courtship in two species of the Drosophila willistoni group. Anim. Behav. 83, 217–227. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.029) [Google Scholar]
- 21.Giglio EM, Dyer KA. 2013. Divergence of premating behaviors in the closely related species Drosophila subquinaria and D. recens. Ecol. Evol. 3, 365–374. (doi:10.1002/ece3.477) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Colyott K, Odu C, Gleason JM. 2016. Dissection of signalling modalities and courtship timing reveals a novel signal in Drosophila saltans courtship. Anim. Behav. 120, 93–101. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.07.015) [Google Scholar]
- 23.Sturtevant AH. 1915. Experiments on sex recognition and the problems of sexual selection in Drosophila. Anim. Behav. 5, 351–366. (doi:10.1037/h0074109) [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bastock M, Manning A. 1955. The courtship of Drosophila melanogaster. Behaviour 8, 85–111. (doi:10.1163/156853955X00184) [Google Scholar]
- 25.Demir E, Dickson BJ. 2005. fruitless splicing specifies male courtship behavior in Drosophila. Cell 121, 785–794. (doi:10.1016/j.cell.2005.04.027) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Billeter JC, Levine JD. 2014. Neurogenetics: sex and the female brain. Curr. Biol. 24, R812–R814. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.07.052) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ferveur JF. 2010. Drosophila female courtship and mating behaviors: sensory signals, genes, neural structures and evolution. Curr. Opin Neurobiol. 20, 764–769. (doi:10.1016/j.conb.2010.09.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lasbleiz C, Ferveur JF, Everaerts C. 2006. Courtship behaviour of Drosophila melanogaster revisited. Anim. Behav. 72, 1001–1012. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.01.027) [Google Scholar]
- 29.Coyne JA, Orr HA. 2004. Speciation. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates Inc. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Grillet M, Everaerts C, Houot B, Ritchie MG, Cobb M, Ferveur JF. 2012. Incipient speciation in Drosophila melanogaster involves chemical signals. Sci. Rep. 2, 224 (doi:10.1038/srep00224) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wu CI, Hollocher H, Begun DJ, Aquadro CF, Xu YJ, Wu ML. 1995. Sexual isolation in Drosophila melanogaster: a possible case of incipient speciation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 92, 2519–2523. (doi:10.1073/pnas.92.7.2519) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ting CT, Takahashi A, Wu CI. 2001. Incipient speciation by sexual isolation in Drosophila: concurrent evolution at multiple loci. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 6709–6713. (doi:10.1073/pnas.121418898) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Dallerac R, Labeur C, Jallon JM, Knippie DC, Roelofs WL, Wicker-Thomas C. 2000. A Delta 9 desaturase gene with a different substrate specificity is responsible for the cuticular diene hydrocarbon polymorphism in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 9449–9454. (doi:10.1073/pnas.150243997) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Fang S, Takahashi A, Wu CI. 2002. A mutation in the promoter of desaturase 2 is correlated with sexual isolation between Drosophila behavioral races. Genetics 162, 781–784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bousquet F, Nojima T, Houot B, Chauvel I, Chaudy S, Dupas S, Yamamoto D, Ferveur J-F. 2012. Expression of a desaturase gene, desat1, in neural and nonneural tissues separately affects perception and emission of sex pheromones in Drosophila. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 249–254. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1109166108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Marcillac F, Grosjean Y, Ferveur JF. 2005. A single mutation alters production and discrimination of Drosophila sex pheromones. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 272, 303–309. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2971) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Houot B, Bousquet F, Ferveur JF. 2010. The consequences of regulation of desat1 expression for pheromone emission and detection in Drosophila melanogaster . Genetics 185, 1297–1309. (doi:10.1534/genetics.110.117226) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Houot B, Fraichard S, Greenspan RJ, Ferveur JF. 2012. Genes involved in sex pheromone discrimination in Drosophila melanogaster and their background-dependent effect. PLoS ONE 7, e30799 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030799) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ferveur JF. 2005. Cuticular hydrocarbons: their evolution and roles in Drosophila pheromonal communication. Behav. Genet. 35, 279–295. (doi:10.1007/s10519-005-3220-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Colegrave N, Hollocher H, Hinton K, Ritchie MG. 2000. The courtship song of African Drosophila melanogaster. J. Evol. Biol. 13, 143–150. (doi:10.1046/j.1420-9101.2000.00148.x) [Google Scholar]
- 41.Takahashi A, Tsaur SC, Coyne JA, Wu CI. 2001. The nucleotide changes governing cuticular hydrocarbon variation and their evolution in Drosophila melanogaster . Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 3920–3925. (doi:10.1073/pnas.061465098) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bousquet F, Chauvel I, Flaven-Pouchon J, Farine JP, Ferveur JF. 2016. Dietary rescue of altered metabolism gene reveals unexpected Drosophila mating cues. J. Lipid Res. 57, 1–11. (doi:10.1194/jlr.M064683) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Eastman Kodak Company. 1999. Technical information data sheet.
- 44.Noldus LP, Trienes RJ, Hendriksen AH, Jansen H, Jansen RG. 2000. The Observer Video-Pro: new software for the collection, management, and presentation of time-structured data from videotapes and digital media files. Behav. Res. Methods, Instrum. Comput. 32, 197–206. (doi:10.3758/BF03200802) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Grossfield J. 1971. Geographic distribution and light-dependent behavior in Drosophila. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 68, 2669–2673. (doi:10.1073/pnas.68.11.2669) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Cobb M, Ferveur JF. 1996. Evolution and genetic control of mate recognition and stimulation in Drosophila. Behav. Process. 35, 35–54. (doi:10.1016/0376-6357(95)00052-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kyriacou CP, Hall JC. 1982. The function of courtship song rhythms in Drosophila. Anim. Behav. 30, 794–801. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(82)80152-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Raad H, Ferveur JF, Ledger N, Capovilla M, Robichon A. 2016. Functional gustatory role of chemoreceptors in Drosophila wings. Cell Rep. 15, 1442–1454. (doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2016.04.040) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Houot B, Gigot V, Robichon A, Ferveur JF. 2017. Free-flight odor tracking in Drosophila: effect of wing chemosensors, sex, and pheromonal gene regulation. Sci. Rep. 7, 40221 (doi:10.1038/srep40221) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Ewing AW. 1964. The influence of wing area on the courtship behaviour of Drosophila melanogaster. Anim. Behav. 12, 316–320. (doi:10.1016/0003-3472(64)90018-1) [Google Scholar]
- 51.Menezes BF, Vigoder FM, Peixoto AA, Varaldi J, Bitner-Mathé BC. 2013. The influence of male wing shape on mating success in Drosophila melanogaster. Anim. Behav. 85, 1217–1223. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.03.008) [Google Scholar]
- 52.Dodd DMB. 1989. Reproductive Isolation as a consequence of adaptive divergence in Drosophila pseudoobscura . Evolution 43, 1308–1311. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.1989.tb02577.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Nanda P, Singh BN. 2008. No effect of marking procedures and choice-situations on the pattern of matings in Drosophila ananassae . Dros. Inf. Serv. 91, 10–13. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Tukey JW. 1977. Exploratory data analysis. Addison-Wesley series in behavioral science, 688 p Reading, Mass: Addison-Wesley Pub. Co. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Addinsoft. 2012. XLSTAT 2012, data analysis and statistics with Microsoft Excel. Paris, France: Addinsoft. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Henderson NR, Lambert DM. 1982. No significant deviation from random mating of worldwide populations of Drosophila melanogaster. Nature 300, 437–440. (doi:10.1038/300437a0) [Google Scholar]
- 57.Marin I. 1994. Sexual isolation in Drosophila. II. Intraspecific variation in mate recognition systems. J. Evol. Biol. 7, 303–314. (doi:10.1046/j.1420-9101.1994.7030303.x) [Google Scholar]
- 58.Haerty W, Jallon JM, Rouault J, Bazin C, Capy P. 2002. Reproductive isolation in natural populations of Drosophila melanogaster from Brazzaville (Congo). Genetica 116, 215–224. (doi:10.1023/A:1021288527291) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Korol A, Rashkovetsky E, Iliadi K, Michalak P, Ronin Y, Nevo E. 2000. Nonrandom mating in Drosophila melanogaster laboratory populations derived from closely adjacent ecologically contrasting slopes at ‘Evolution Canyon’. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 12 637–12 642. (doi:10.1073/pnas.220041397) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Yukilevich R, Lachance J, Aoki F, True JR. 2008. Long-term adaptation of epistatic genetic networks. Evolution 62, 2215–2235. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00445.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Begun DJ, Aquadro CF. 1993. African and North American populations of Drosophila melanogaster are very different at the DNA level. Nature 365, 548–550. (doi:10.1038/365548a0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Gay L, Eady PE, Vasudev RAM, Hosken DJ, Tregenza TOM. 2009. Costly sexual harassment in a beetle. Physiol. Entomol. 34, 86–92. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-3032.2008.00656.x) [Google Scholar]
- 63.Maklakov AA, Immler S, Lovlie H, Flis I, Friberg U. 2013. The effect of sexual harassment on lethal mutation rate in female Drosophila melanogaster . Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 20121874 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.1874) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Alipaz JA, Wu C, Karr TL. 2001. Gametic incompatibilities between races of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. R. Soc. B 268, 789–795. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1420) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Fischbach KF, Heisenberg M. 1981. Structural brain mutant of Drosophila melanogaster with reduced cell number in the medulla cortex and with normal optomotor yaw response. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 78, 1105–1109. (doi:10.1073/pnas.78.2.1105) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Rister J, et al. 2007. Dissection of the peripheral motion channel in the visual system of Drosophila melanogaster. Neuron 56, 155–170. (doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Yin Hing AL, Carlson JR. 1996. Male-male courtship behavior induced by ectopic expression of the Drosophila white gene: Role of sensory function and age. J. Neurobiol. 30, 454–464. (doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-4695(199608)30:4<454::AID-NEU2>3.0.CO;2-2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Connolly K. 1968. The social facilitation of preening behaviour in Drosophila melanogaster. Anim. Behav. 16, 385–391. (doi:10.1016/0003-3472(68)90023-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Thistle R, Cameron P, Ghorayshi A, Dennison L, Scott K. 2012. Contact chemoreceptors mediate male-male repulsion and male-female attraction during Drosophila courtship. Cell 149, 1140–1151. (doi:10.1016/j.cell.2012.03.045) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Connolly K, Cook R. 1973. Rejection responses by female Drosophila melanogaster: their ontogeny, causality and effects upon the bebaviour of the courting male. Behaviour 44, 142–166. (doi:10.1163/156853973X00364) [Google Scholar]
- 71.Spieth H. 1974. Courtship behavior in Drosophila . Annu. Rev. Entomol. 19, 385–405. (doi:10.1146/annurev.en.19.010174.002125) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Laturney M, Billeter JC. 2016. Drosophila melanogaster females restore their attractiveness after mating by removing male anti-aphrodisiac pheromones. Nat. Commun. 7, 12322 (doi:10.1038/ncomms12322) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Paillette M, Ikeda H, Jallon JM. 1991. A new acoustic message in Drosophila: the rejection signal (R. S.) of Drosophila melanogaster and Drosophila simulans. Bioacoustics 3, 247–252. (doi:10.1080/09524622.1991.9753188) [Google Scholar]
- 74.Farine JP, Ferveur JF, Everaerts C. 2012. Volatile Drosophila cuticular pheromones are affected by social but not sexual experience. PLoS ONE 7, e40396 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0040396) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Grillet M, Dartevelle L, Ferveur JF. 2006. A Drosophila male pheromone affects female sexual receptivity. Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 315–323. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3332) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Lebreton S. et al. 2017. A Drosophila female pheromone elicits species-specific long-range attraction via an olfactory channel with dual specificity for sex and food. BMC Biol. 15, 888 (doi:10.1186/s12915-017-0427-x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Stocker RF. 1994. The organization of the chemosensory system in Drosophila melanogaster—a review. Cell Tissue Res. 275, 3–26. (doi:10.1007/BF00305372) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.de Bruyne M, Clyne PJ, Carlson JR. 1999. Odor coding in a model olfactory organ: the Drosophila maxillary palp. J. Neurosci. 19, 4520–4532. (doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04520.1999) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Kurtovic A, Widmer A, Dickson BJ. 2007. A single class of olfactory neurons mediates behavioural responses to a Drosophila sex pheromone. Nature 446, 542–546. (doi:10.1038/nature05672) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Arguello JR, Cardoso-Moreira M, Grenier JK, Gottipati S, Clark AG, Benton R. 2016. Extensive local adaptation within the chemosensory system following Drosophila melanogaster’s global expansion. Nat. Commun. 7, 11855 (doi:10.1038/ncomms11855) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Grillet M., Ferveur JF, Everaerts C. 2017. Data from: Behavioural elements and sensory cues involved in sexual isolation between Drosophila melanogaster strains. Dryad Digital Repository. (doi:10.5061/dryad.sg095) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Citations
- Grillet M., Ferveur JF, Everaerts C. 2017. Data from: Behavioural elements and sensory cues involved in sexual isolation between Drosophila melanogaster strains. Dryad Digital Repository. (doi:10.5061/dryad.sg095) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All experimental data can be found in the electronic supplementary material (XLSX files) and at the Dryad Digital Repository (http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.sg095) [81].