Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2018 Jul 23;13(7):e0200882. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200882

Salivary testosterone in relation to social cognition and social anxiety in children and adolescents with 47,XXY (Klinefelter syndrome)

Sophie van Rijn 1,2,*
Editor: Andrew Wolfe3
PMCID: PMC6056033  PMID: 30036387

Abstract

Background

Approximately 1 in 650 boys are born with an extra X chromosome. Boys and men with 47,XXY (Klinefelter syndrome) are at risk for neurodevelopmental disorders and specific cognitive impairments. This study was focused on social anxiety and social cognition. The aim was to assess if these aspects of the phenotype are related to testosterone deficiency, which is typically seen in 47,XXY from puberty onwards.

Methods

In the study 20 boys with 47,XXY and 25 non-clinical controls between 8 and 19 years participated. None had ever used testosterone supplements. Cognitive tests measuring the labeling of facial expressions and perspective taking (Theory of Mind) were administered. Self-report questionnaires were used to assess social anxiety. Testosterone was measured in saliva.

Results

Within the 47,XXY group lower levels of salivary testosterone were significantly associated with higher levels of social anxiety. The correlation was strong, andindependent of age and pubertal development. However, salivary levels of testosterone were uncorrelated to social cognitive skills.

Discussion

These findings point out that lower testosterone levels might contribute to high social anxiety in 47,XXY, suggesting that anxiety should be monitored in pubertal boys with XXY presenting with testosterone deficiency. This should be done in addition to exploring cognitive behavioral therapy or psychopharmacologic treatments targeting anxiety, which are more evidence based. In contrast, testosterone levels were not associated with social cognitive functioning, suggesting that other mechanisms are driving vulnerabilities in this domain.

Introduction

There is increasing interest in the phenotype of individuals with sex chromosome aneuploidies, not only because this is crucial for optimizing clinical care, but also because by we may learn about factors that drive risk for psychopathology. Among these aneuploidies is the 47,XXY (Klinefelter syndrome) variant, which is present in approximately 1 in 650 boys and men [1]. Because the X chromosome harbors a large number of genes that code for brain development, any of these genes that escape inactivation (and have a homologue on the Y chromosome in males), may be overexpressed in individuals with these genetic variants, putting individuals at risk for neurodevelopmental disorders [2].

Neurodevelopmental vulnerability in 47,XXY is for example expressed in increased risk for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), depressive and anxiety disorders, psychotic disorders and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [3]. However, the clinical phenotype is variable and only a proportion of children and adults meet such diagnostic criteria. In contrast, social adaptive dysfunction is often seen in boys and men with 47,XXY [4, 5]. This includes social withdrawal, social anxiety, depressed social skills, and in a proportion (an estimated 20–25%) of the individuals high levels of autism traits or symptoms [6, 7]. Many cognitive dysfunctions may contribute to such social problems including language deficits, executive dysfunction or social cognitive impairments [8], cognitive domains that are considered vulnerable in individuals with an extra X chromosome [912]. In the domain of social cognition, difficulties in labeling of facial expressions of emotion and difficulties in Theory of Mind, i.e. taking perspective to understand others’ emotions and thoughts, have been found in individuals with 47,XXY [12, 13]. Psychophysiological studies using skin conductance measures have revealed that 47,XXY adults respond with increased arousal towards socio-emotional scenes and faces [14], which fits with reports of increased experiences of stress and anxiety during social interactions, both in adults and children with 47,XXY [5, 6].

Some of these studies included boys as well as girls with an extra X chromosome, which is interesting in considering genetic versus hormonal effects. Direct comparisons of scores on social behavioral and social cognitive measures across boys and girls showed no significant differences. Although these results require replication in more and larger studies, such findings tentatively suggest that social developmental risks might be related to what these boys and girls have in common, i.e. the extra X chromosome, rather than what distinguishes the boys and girls, such as hormonal profile. However, there is a body of evidence showing that 47,XXY is associated with testosterone deficiency, which calls for studies assessing the potential consequences on cognitive and behavioral development, which might be alleviated with testosterone supplementation.

Over the last few years a series of reviews have been devoted to the presence and course of testosterone deficiency in 47,XXY [1518]. Overall, these reviews show that evidence for prenatal testosterone abnormalities is unclear because of the limited number of studies. There is one study showing adequate testosterone levels based on amniotic fluid samples. Evidence for testosterone deficiency in the first few months of life (i.e. ‘mini-puberty’) is mixed, with some suggesting signs of low testosterone concentrations and some suggesting high-normal or low-normal concentrations of testosterone. In childhood, most studies point towards adequate levels of testosterone in boys with 47,XXY. However, from puberty onset, testosterone deficiency becomes increasingly more prominent with lower levels in the majority of adolescents and adults with 47,XXY, who are then typically treated with testosterone supplementation.

It is important to investigate if lower testosterone levels are associated with more compromised social functioning in boys with 47,XXY, because testosterone is thought to play a role in the regulation of social behavior. To illustrate, in typically developing populations higher levels of testosterone are associated with social approach behavior, social dominance and reduced social anxiety [19]. As these effects are observed in response to administration of a single dosage of testosterone, these social behaviors are likely co-directed by acute, activational effects of testosterone. It is hypothesized (i.e. the challenge hypothesis) that circulating testosterone in typically developing males helps in mobilizing males to respond adaptively to challenging social interactions [19, 20]. Thus, in understanding factors that drive social dysfunction in individuals with 47,XXY, it is relevant to assess if lower levels of testosterone are related to problems in social adaptation and underlying social cognitive skills.

In studying the relationship between testosterone and cognition or behavior, there are several factors that contribute to complexity in design and interpretation. First, the biological effects of testosterone on brain development in 47,XXY are likely mediated by its binding to the androgen receptor, which can vary across individuals [15]. Second, there are both activational effects that depend on levels of circulating levels testosterone and organizational effects on brain development that start already prenatally. Thus, studying circulating levels may reveal activational and thus more immediate effects on functional neural circuits, impacting information processing and related behavior [21], but may not reveal the influence of testosterone on brain maturation over the course of development. Third, the majority of testosterone is bound to plasma proteins and only 1–2% of testosterone is free and bioavailable [22]. Only in its free form testosterone can cross the blood-brain barrier, where it can bind to androgen receptors in the brain [23]. Free, bioavailable testosterone in serum is significantly correlated with salivary levels of testosterone [2427], based on which salivary testosterone may reveal androgen dysfunctions that can impact brain development and brain functioning.

The aim of this study was to assess if salivary levels of testosterone in boys and adolescents with 47,XXY are correlated with social anxiety and social cognition, areas that have previously been identified as vulnerable in this sample [5, 12]. Studies linking testosterone deficiency to difficulties in social cognition and social behavior in individuals with 47,XXY are needed, and this preliminary study was done to explore such associations. This knowledge is important because of the need to understand the contribution of puberty onset testosterone deficiency to cognitive and behavioral problems, and related opportunities to positively influence social development in children with 47,XXY.

Materials and methods

Participants

Only boys participated in this study, with age ranging from 8 to 19 years. A group of 20 boys and adolescents with 47,XXY were compared to 25 typically developing boys and adolescents. Average age in the 47,XXY group was not significantly different from the control group, F(1,43) = 0.15, p = .69, Cohen’s d = 0.1. See Table 1.

Table 1. Characteristics of the control group and XXY group.

Control group XXY group
Sample size n = 25 n = 20
Age 13.1 (SD 2.9) 12.8 (SD 3.1)
prenatal diagnosis 12.4 (SD 2.6) (n = 10)
postnatal diagnosis 13.2 (SD 3.6) (n = 10)
IQ 100.9 (SD 13.1) 83.7 (SD 15.4)
prenatal diagnosis 80.3 (SD 16.3) (n = 10)
postnatal diagnosis 87.1 (SD 14.5) (n = 10)
Pubertal (Tanner) stage Stage 1: n = 7 Stage 1: n = 9
Stage 2: n = 8 Stage 2: n = 4
Stage 3: n = 6 Stage 3: n = 6
Stage 4: n = 3 Stage 4: n = 1
Stage 5: n = 0 Stage 5: n = 0
Salivary testosterone (pg/ml) 102.4 (SE 29.2) 40.0 (SE 11.9)
boys < 12 years 22.1 (SE 6.3) (n = 10) 39.9 (SE 16.3) (n = 9)
boys > 12 years 155.9 (SE 43.6) (n = 15) 40.2 (SE 17.8) (n = 11)

The group of children with an extra X chromosome was recruited through active follow up of families after prenatal diagnosis with help of clinical genetics departments (50% of the group), and through support groups, calls for participants in the media or referrals from pediatricians or psychologists (50% of the group). The prenatal and postnatal group did not significantly differ in age (p = .59) or IQ (p = .33), see Table 1. Previous studies from our group have shown that the prenatal and postnatal groups did not significantly differ in severity of social behavioral problems, social anxiety, or social cognitive abilities, see Van Rijn et al. [5] and Van Rijn et al. [12].

Boys or adolescents who were currently taking or who had ever received testosterone supplementation were excluded from this study, because treatment with testosterone supplements influences salivary levels of testosterone, and therefore complicates interpretation of the data. Thus, none of the participants were using or had ever used testosterone supplements. Controls from the general population were recruited from regular schools. None of the children in this group scored in the clinical range (≥70) on the Total score, Internalizing score or Externalizing score of the Childhood Behavior Checklist (CBCL) [28].

The study was approved by the Medical Research and Ethics Committee of Leiden University Medical Center, and written informed consent was obtained from all parents/guardians, as well as participants older than 12 years. Data were collected (and hormone levels were analyzed) between 2009 and 2011.

Salivary testosterone

Testosterone was measured in saliva, which has been shown to correlate highly with serum testosterone in adult men (r = .94) [24], and in hypogonadal men (r = .63) [25]. Salivary levels of testosterone have also shown to correlate significantly with serum levels in prepubertal and pubertal boys, who have lower levels of testosterone as compared to adult men. A study with a large sample size of 218 typically developing boys between 11 and 23 years, showed a significant correlation of r = .83 between salivary testosterone and serum testosterone [26]. Because levels of testosterone may be even lower in boys with 47,XXY, it is also important to assess validity of salivary measures in the XXY pediatric population. Butler et al. [27] compared salivary testosterone to serum testosterone in a group of 84 boys aged 7 to 16 years, of which 18 boys had XXY: in this XXY subgroup there was a significant correlation between serum testosterone and salivary testosterone; r = .88.

Saliva samples were collected and individually averaged across two daily time points; one morning sample at 08:00 and one afternoon sample at 16:00, to control for diurnal variation [29]. None of the measurements were missing, and collection times and number of samples were identical for each individual. On the saliva collecting day, participants were asked not to eat, brush their teeth or drink for one hour prior to collection in order to prevent sample dilution or contamination. Then saliva was collected by unstimulated passive drool. Samples were stored at -20 °C within 2 hours. Testosterone in saliva was measured in duplo using the EiAsy ELISA kit from Diagnostic Biochemicals Canada (CAN-TE-300) using 2 x 100 ul saliva. All essays were analyzed in one run and had acceptable intra-assay variation (Coefficient of Variability of max 10%). The lower limit of detection of the DBC Testosterone Saliva kit is 1.0 pg/ml.

Tanner pubertal stage

Stage of pubertal development was assessed using the Peterson Puberty Development Scale (PDS) [30], which is based on self-ratings of schematic drawings of secondary sex characteristics associated with the five standard Tanner stages of pubertal development [31], with acceptable psychometric properties and validity. Self-reports of schematic drawings have demonstrated acceptable validity [32]. PDS scores were based on body hair, skin change, growth spurt, facial hair and voice change.

Intellectual functioning

The subtests Vocabulary (V) and Block design (BD) of the Dutch adaptations of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children-III [33] were used to asses intellectual functioning. The V-BD short form of the Wechsler Scales has shown to be a valid proxy of full scale IQ (FSIQ) in clinical and non-clinical populations [34, 35].

Social cognition

Facial affect identification was assessed using the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces (KDEF) set [36], which contains 4900 pictures (562 * 762 pixels) of facial expressions of male and female amateur actors, aged between 20 and 30 years of age. No beards, mustaches, earrings or eyeglasses, or make-up are visible. We used 144 face forward pictures displaying either angry, afraid, disgusted, or sad expressions, digitally presented using Eprime software. There were four subtests of 36 trials each; 18 pictures of the target emotion and 18 pictures of other emotions. In each trial, one picture was presented. Participants were asked to identify whether the target emotion was present, using the mouse buttons to respond with ‘yes’ or ‘no’. Participants were asked to work as fast and as accurate as possible. The task was self-paced, with an intertrial interval of 1000 ms. In this study, overall accuracy (% correct) and reaction time (RT) were used as the outcome parameters.

Theory of Mind (ToM) was measured using the Social Cognitive Skills Test (SCST) [37, 38]. Psychometric properties of the SCST have been rated by the COTAN (Dutch Committee on Tests and Testing) as satisfactory with regard to reliability and validity, the internal consistency is α = .96 and test-retest reliability is r = .82. The SCST consists of seven cartoon stories, which are visually presented together with a verbal description of what can be seen in the cartoon. For each story eight different questions are formulated that increase in complexity. In this study, total sumscore was used as the outcome parameter.

Social anxiety

The Social Anxiety Scale (SAS) is a Dutch questionnaire to assess social anxiety in the context of social skills, intellectual skills, physical skills, and appearance. It is designed for children aged 8 and older. The reliability of the SAS is high (internal consistency = 0.90) [39], and its validity is satisfactory [40]. The SAS consists of 36 items, which are each followed by two options: one option indicating social anxiety and the other indicating no social anxiety. For example: ‘If someone in the group looks at me when I am doing something (1) I do not become nervous, (2) I become nervous’. Higher scores indicate more social anxiety. For this study, a subset of subscales was selected in order to minimize number of variables in statistical analyses: these were ‘social skills’ and ‘physical skills’, in order to be able to contrast anxiety in contexts with and without social interactions.

Statistical analyses

Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 23 was used for statistical analyses. Group comparisons of age and IQ were performed using ANOVA. For Pubertal (Tanner) stage, a Chi-Square test was used to analyze group differences. Group differences in testosterone levels were examined using MANOVA, which allows for analysis of main effects as well as interaction effects of the factors group (XXY, control) and age (boys <12 years, boys > 12 years). For correlation analyses partial correlations were used when controlling for age and pubertal (Tanner) stage, and Spearman correlations were added for data that we not normally distributed. Level of significance was set at p = .05.

Results

Group characteristics

One boy in the control group did not complete the Pubertal Development Scale. Pubertal development (Tanner stage) was not significantly different in the XXY group as compared to the control group, χ (3) = 2.2, p = .52, Cohen’s d = 0.2. However, mean level of intellectual functioning was significantly lower in the XXY group in comparison to the control group, F(1,43) = 16.1, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 1.2. See Table 1.

Salivary testosterone

As additional validation of the salivary testosterone measurements, we tested the hypotheses that in the control group levels of testosterone correlated with age and/or measures of pubertal development. Indeed, level of testosterone was significantly and highly correlated with age (r = .77, p < .001) and pubertal development (r = .75, p < .001). As expected (considering that testosterone deficiency may present at different ages in different boys with XXY), in the group of XXY boys, there were no significant correlations between testosterone levels and age (p = .31) or pubertal development (p = .94).

Group differences in testosterone

Although the aim of the study was not to assess if boys with XXY have deficient testosterone levels, average levels of testosterone were explored and compared across the groups. Because deviations in testosterone in the XXY group were expected to be more prominent in older children, levels of testosterone were compared for separate age groups. Using a cutoff point of 12.0 years, age groups of younger (8–12 years) and older (12–19 years) boys were identified. Within the younger and older groups, there were no significant differences between the XXY group and control group with respect to age and pubertal stage.

Group differences in testosterone were examined using MANOVA with testosterone level as the dependent variable and group (control, XXY) and age (younger, older) as independent variables. Average testosterone levels in each group are presented in Table 1. There was no significant effect of group (F(1,41) = 2.3, p = .13), a significant effect of age (F(1,41) = 4.4, p = .04) and a significant group by age interaction (F(1,41) = 4.3, p = .04). Post hoc group comparisons (control, XXY) were done to evaluate testosterone levels for younger and older boys separately. This showed no significant difference between boys with XXY versus controls in the younger age group, F(1,17) = 1.1, p = .30. In contrast, boys with XXY showed significantly lower levels of testosterone than controls in the older age group, F(1,24) = 4.7, p = .04. In other words, level of testosterone was lower than controls in older, but not younger, boys with XXY. See Fig 1 for the group by age interaction for average levels of testosterone.

Fig 1. Levels of salivary testosterone (mean, SE) in the XXY group versus controls, stratified for different age groups, i.e. younger boys (8–12 years) and older boys (12–19 years).

Fig 1

* Significant at p < .05.

Correlations between testosterone and social cognition and social anxiety

Key to our aim of understanding the relationship between testosterone levels and social cognition and social anxiety in boys with XXY, within-group correlational analyses were performed. Group differences in social anxiety and social cognition in the cohort the current samples of participants were drawn from, have been published elsewhere [5, 12]. Of one XXY boy, the SAS questionnaire was missing. All correlations were corrected for age and pubertal development to make sure that these factors did not explain any of the relationships.

Within the group of 20 boys with XXY, there were no significant correlations between testosterone levels and social cognitive measures, i.e. facial affect recognition and Theory of Mind (see Table 1 and Fig 2). In contrast, testosterone levels did significantly correlate with social anxiety. Although there were no significant correlations with the SAS subscale ‘physical skills’, there was a significant correlation with the SAS subscale ‘social skills’ (see Table 2 and Fig 2). The relationship was thus specific for anxiety during social interactions, and not anxiety in general. Higher testosterone level was associated with lower social anxiety, irrespective of age and pubertal development. The strength of the correlation was high (r = —.64), with an explained variance of 41%.

Fig 2. Scatterplots of the relationship between salivary testosterone level and parameters of social anxiety and social cognition (theory of mind and facial affect recognition) in the 47,XXY group.

Fig 2

Table 2. Correlations between levels of salivary testosterone and social cognition and social anxiety within the XXY group (n = 20).

Correlation with testosterone level, controlled for age and pubertal stage Nonparametric correlation with testosterone level
Facial expressions: % correct r = -.41, p = .10 r = .13, p = .58
Facial expressions: RT r = -.11, p = .66 r = -.20, p = .38
Theory of Mind r = .13, p = .62 r = .05, p = .84
Social anxiety: social skills r = -.64, p = .004* r = -.47, p = .03*
Social anxiety: physical skills r = -.17, p = .49 r = -.01, p = .96

*significant at p < .05

To make sure that the correlations between testosterone and social anxiety or social cognition were not impacted by the somewhat skewed distribution of the testosterone data, we also performed nonparametric correlations (Spearman rank), see Table 2. This also showed a significant nonparametric correlation between testosterone and social anxiety. Similarly, the nonparametric correlations between testosterone and facial affect recognition as well as theory of mind remained non-significant.

Discussion

This study showed that within the 47,XXY group lower levels of salivary testosterone were associated with higher levels of social anxiety in the context of social skills. This association was strong, and independent of age and pubertal development. However, salivary levels of testosterone were uncorrelated to social cognitive skills, i.e. facial affect recognition and Theory of Mind. As expected, we found that only older boys but not younger boys with 47,XXY had significantly lower levels of salivary testosterone.

These findings are in line with studies in other populations pointing towards lower testosterone levels being a risk factor for anxiety. For example, hypogonadal men from the general population have higher levels of anxiety, which can be alleviated through testosterone supplementation [41]. Also, in typically developing adolescent males a decline in salivary testosterone over the course of the day (due to circadian fluctuation) is shown to be directly correlated with increased anxiety levels [42].

Testosterone may influence social anxiety by impacting subcortical brain regions that have high concentrations of androgen receptors, such as the hypothalamus and amygdala, thereby influencing perception, emotion, cognition and behavior [41]. Interestingly, in non-clinical groups testosterone administration has shown to enhance neural responsiveness of subcortical brain areas that play a role in social approach towards social stimuli [43] [44]. These findings suggest that levels of testosterone are related to social anxiety and social approach behavior, driven by subcortical brain areas [19]. Our finding of a specific correlation between testosterone and social anxiety in relation to social interactions, and not in relation to social anxiety about physical skills, fits with this notion of testosterone having a regulatory role in social interactions.

Interestingly, the lack of association with social cognitive measures may suggest that lower testosterone levels might contribute to high social anxiety, without influencing social cognitive functioning. Although speculatively, there may be other pathways through which lower testosterone increases social anxiety, affecting arousal levels more directly rather than through cognitive mechanisms. Interestingly, a range of studies have pointed to the inhibitory nature by which testosterone regulates the stress-related HPA (hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal) axis [45], which is implicated in social anxiety. Animal studies have shown testosterone, controlled by the HPG (hypothalamic–pituitary–gonadal) axis, can act and interact on different aspects of stress and basal HPA function [45]. To illustrate, gonadectomy (and related lack of testosterone) in male rats increases the cortisol response to stressors, an effect that is reversed by testosterone supplementation [46, 47]. Although speculative, interactions between the HPG and HPA axes on the physiological level might also help explain our findings.

The current study had several limitations which should be addressed. First, the sample size was rather small and may have resulted in limited statistical power to detect effects. However, the correlational analyses were core to the study, and showed significant results and high levels of explained variance, even with the current sample size. Second, the social anxiety measure was based on self-report. Although subjective experiences are core to the concept of anxiety, the use of psychophysiological arousal measures may have improved the study. Also, a broader range of social cognition or social behavioral measurements would have be interesting to investigate. Third, testosterone was measured in saliva rather than blood plasma, which provides more accurate measures. This also made it difficult to compare levels in this study to other, more clinically oriented studies. Fourth, pubertal (Tanner) stage was evaluated using self-report rather than by assessment of an expert clinical professional. Finally, the study was limited in that it was focused on associations between testosterone and social cognitive/behavioral functioning, and did not involve evaluating the impact of testosterone supplementation.

Unfortunately, double-blind, randomized controlled trials evaluating effect of testosterone supplementation in individuals with 47,XXY are very scarce. The finding that levels of salivary testosterone are related to social anxiety in 47,XXY may have relevance to studies assessing the impact of testosterone supplementation. Interestingly, in a recent double-blind, randomized trial from Ross et al. [48], the effect of two-year androgen supplementation on social behavior and anxiety (as well as other measures) was measured in 84 boys with XXY. This study showed significant improvements in social behavior and a reduction in anxiety, which is in line with our findings of higher testosterone levels associated with less social anxiety. Taken together, social functioning may be a potential parameter of interest in testosterone supplementation studies in XXY. However, based on the current findings we expect that effects of testosterone supplementation, increasing circulating levels of testosterone, may not necessarily improve social cognition. Although our findings require replication, this may fit with observations that many of the cognitive features in 47,XXY already present before puberty, when testosterone deficiency is considered not yet prominent, and therefore less under influence of the activational effects of testosterone. This hypothesis is also supported by the finding that androgen supplementation did not affect cognitive functioning in boys with XXY the double-blind, randomized trial from Ross et al. [48].

Also, it would be relevant to learn more about early organizational effects of testosterone in 47,XXY, which start already prenatally. Especially since studies with non-clinical groups have shown that high levels (rather than low levels) of prenatal testosterone are predictive of more compromised social behavior and social cognition in 6 to 8 year olds from the general population [49, 50]. However, short course treatment with testosterone during the ‘mini-puberty’ in young infants with 47,XXY may have positive effects on neurodevelopment [51]. Such findings call for replication in prospective, double-blind, randomized controlled trials (RCT) in these very early stages of development of boys with XXY.

Identifying aspects of behavioral and cognitive functioning that are under influence of testosterone in boys with 47,XXY is important, because this may help in identifying factors that might be influenced to improve outcome. If testosterone levels impact social anxiety levels in the absence of influencing social cognition, this may prove to be a promising avenue of further research. However, in addition to assessing potential positive effects of hormonal intervention, we also need to address efficacy of other types of interventions such as cognitive behavioral therapies (CBT), as such psychosocial interventions have shown positive effects in terms of reducing (social) anxiety in other vulnerable populations [52, 53]. As interventions such as CBT or psychopharmacologic treatments targeting anxiety are much more evidence based, these should still be considered the primary treatment for anxiety in XXY.

Acknowledgments

We thank dr. Sabine Hannema, paediatric endocrinologist at Leiden University Medical Center, for consulting in writing of the manuscript.

Data Availability

Data cannot be made publicly available for ethical reasons, because public availability would compromise participant confidentiality. Data are digitally stored at the secure servers of the Faculty of Social and Behavioral Science of Leiden University (The Netherlands), according to the guidelines of the Medical Research and Ethics Committee of Leiden University Medical Center. Please contact d.j.g.arnoldus@fsw.leidenuniv.nl (secretary of the Ethics Committee at Leiden University) for accessing the data.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by a VENI grant (grant number 016.095.060 to SvR) and VIDI grant (grant number 016.165.397 to SvR) from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (www.nwo.nl).

References

  • 1.Bojesen A, Juul S, Gravholt CH. Prenatal and postnatal prevalence of Klinefelter syndrome: a national registry study. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 2003;88(2):622–6. 10.1210/jc.2002-021491 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lee NR, Lopez KC, Adeyemi EI, Giedd JN. Sex chromosome aneuploidies: A window for examining the effects of the x and y chromosomes on speech, language, and social development. In: Fidler DJ, editor. International Review of Research in Developmental Disabilities: Early Development in Neurogenetic Disorders, Vol 40. International Review of Research in Developmental Disabilities. 40 2011. p. 139–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Cederlof M, Gotby AO, Larsson H, Serlachius E, Boman M, Langstrom N, et al. Klinefelter syndrome and risk of psychosis, autism and ADHD. J Psychiatr Res. 2014;48(1):128–30. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Visootsak J, Graham JM. Social function in multiple X and Y chromosome disorders: XXY, XYY, XXYY, XXXY. Dev Dis Res Rev. 2009;15(4):328–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.van Rijn S, Stockmann L, Borghgraef M, Bruining H, van Ravenswaaij-Arts C, Govaerts L, et al. The social behavioral phenotype in boys and girls with an extra X chromosome (Klinefelter syndrome and Trisomy X): a comparison with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(2):310–20. Epub 2013/07/05. 10.1007/s10803-013-1860-5 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.van Rijn S, Swaab H, Aleman A, Kahn RS. Social behavior and autism traits in a sex chromosomal disorder: Klinefelter (47XXY) syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(9):1634–41. Epub 2008/03/08. 10.1007/s10803-008-0542-1 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Cordeiro L, Tartaglia N, Roeltgen D, Ross J. Social deficits in male children and adolescents with sex chromosome aneuploidy: A comparison of XXY, XYY, and XXYY syndromes. Res Dev Disabil. 2012;33(4):1254–63. 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.02.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Beauchamp MH, Anderson V. SOCIAL: An Integrative Framework for the Development of Social Skills. Psychol Bull. 2010;136(1):39–64. 10.1037/a0017768 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.van Rijn S, Swaab H. Executive dysfunction and the relation with behavioral problems in children with 47,XXY and 47,XXX. Genes Brain and Behavior. 2015;14(2):200–8. 10.1111/gbb.12203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lee NR, Wallace GL, Clasen LS, Lenroot RK, Blumenthal JD, White SL, et al. Executive Function in Young Males with Klinefelter (XXY) Syndrome with and without Comorbid Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17(3):522–30. 10.1017/S1355617711000312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Boada R, Janusz J, Hutaff-Lee C, Tartaglia N. The cognitive phenotype in Klinefelter syndrome: A review of the literature including genetic and hormonal factors. Dev Dis Res Rev. 2009;15(4):284–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.van Rijn S, Stockmann L, van Buggenhout G, van Ravenswaaij-Arts C, Swaab H. Social cognition and underlying cognitive mechanisms in children with an extra X chromosome: a comparison with autism spectrum disorder. Genes Brain and Behavior. 2014;13(5):459–67. 10.1111/gbb.12134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.van Rijn S, Swaab H, Aleman A, Kahn RS. X Chromosomal effects on social cognitive processing and emotion regulation: A study with Klinefelter men (47,XXY). Schizophr Res. 2006;84(2–3):194–203. Epub 2006/04/11. 10.1016/j.schres.2006.02.020 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.van Rijn S, Barendse M, van Goozen S, Swaab H. Social attention, affective arousal and empathy in men with Klinefelter syndrome (47,XXY): evidence from eyetracking and skin conductance. Plos One. 2014;9(1):e84721 Epub 2014/01/15. 10.1371/journal.pone.0084721 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Aksglaede L, Juul A. Testicular function and fertility in men with Klinefelter syndrome: a review. European Journal of Endocrinology. 2013;168(4):R67–R76. 10.1530/EJE-12-0934 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Host C, Skakkebk A, Groth KA, Bojesen A. The role of hypogonadism in Klinefelter Syndrome. Asian Journal of Andrology. 2014;16(2):185–91. 10.4103/1008-682X.122201 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Wosnitzer MS, Paduch DA. Endocrinological issues and hormonal manipulation in children and men with Klinefelter syndrome. Am J Med Genet Part C Sem in Med Gen. 2013;163C(1):16–26. 10.1002/ajmg.c.31350 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Oates RD. The natural history of endocrine function and spermatogenesis in Klinefelter syndrome: what the data show. Fertil Steril. 2012;98(2):266–73. 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2012.06.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bos PA, Panksepp J, Bluthe R-M, van Honk J. Acute effects of steroid hormones and neuropeptides on human social-emotional behavior: A review of single administration studies. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2012;33(1):17–35. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2011.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Archer J. Testosterone and human aggression: an evaluation of the challenge hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(3):319–45. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Sisk CL, Zehr JL. Pubertal hormones organize the adolescent brain and behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2005;26(3–4):163–74. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2005.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Dunn JF, Nisula BC, Rodbard D. Transport of steroid-hormones—Binding of 21 endogenous steroids to both testosterone-binding globulin and corticosteroid-binding globulin in human-plasma. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1981;53(1):58–68. 10.1210/jcem-53-1-58 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bialek M, Zaremba P, Borowicz KK, Czuczwar SJ. Neuroprotective role of testosterone in the nervous system. Pol J Pharmacol. 2004;56(5):509–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wang C, Plymate S, Nieschlag E, Paulsen CA. Salivary testosterone in men—Further evidence of a direct correlation with free serum testosterone. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1981;53(5):1021–4. 10.1210/jcem-53-5-1021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Morley JE, Perry HM III, Patrick P, Dollbaum CM, Kells JM. Validation of salivary testosterone as a screening test for male hypogonadism. Aging Male. 2006;9(3):165–9. 10.1080/13685530600907993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Rilling JK, Worthman CM, Campbell BC, Stallings JF, Mbizva M. Ratios of plasma and salivary testosterone throughout puberty: production versus bioavailability. Steroids. 1996;61(6):374–8. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Butler GE, Walker RF, Walker RV, Teague P, Riad-Fahmy D, Ratcliffe SG. Salivary testosterone levels and the progress of puberty in the normal boy. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf). 1989;30(5):587–96. Epub 1989/05/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behaviour Checklist / 4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington VT University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991.
  • 29.Brambilla DJ, Matsumoto AM, Araujo AB, McKinlay JB. The Effect of Diurnal Variation on Clinical Measurement of Serum Testosterone and Other Sex Hormone Levels in Men. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2009;94(3):907–13. 10.1210/jc.2008-1902 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Carskadon MA, Acebo C. A SELF-ADMINISTERED RATING-SCALE FOR PUBERTAL DEVELOPMENT. J Adolesc Health. 1993;14(3):190–5. 10.1016/1054-139x(93)90004-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Tanner JM. Growth at Adolescence. 2nd ed ed. Oxford, England: Blackwell Scientific Publications; 1962. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Coleman L, Coleman J. The measurement of puberty: a review. J Adolesc. 2002;25(5):535–50. 10.1006/jado.2002.0494 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wechsler D. WAIS-III NL Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale WAIS-III. Third edition, Dutch version. Manual. Amsterdam: Harcourt Test Publishers; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Campbell JM. Internal and external validity of seven Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition short forms in a sample of psychiatric inpatients. Psychol Assess. 1998;10(4):431–4. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Crawford JR, Allan KM, Jack AM. Short-forms of the UK WAIS-r: Regression equations and their predictive-validity in a general-population sample. Br J Clin Psychol. 1992;31:191–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Öhman A. The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces—KDEF [CD, ISBN 91-630-7164-9]. Stockholm, Sweden: Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet; 1998.
  • 37.Coleman N, Hare DJ, Farrell P, Van Manen T. The use of the Social Cognitive Skills Test with children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of intellectual disabilities: JOID. 2008;12(1):49–57. Epub 2008/03/14. 10.1177/1744629507086608 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Van Manen TG, Prins PJM, Emmelkamp PMG. Manual for the Social Cognitive Skills Test. Houten, The Netherlands: Bohn Stafleu van Loghum; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Dekking YM. [Handleiding sociale angst schaal voor kinderen] Manual social anxiety scale for children. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Evers A, Van Vliet-Mulder JC, Ter Laak J. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Van Gorcum; 1992.
  • 41.McHenry J, Carrier N, Hull E, Kabbaj M. Sex differences in anxiety and depression: Role of testosterone. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2014;35(1):42–57. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Granger DA, Shirtcliff EA, Zahn-Waxler C, Usher B, Klimes-Dougan B, Hastings P. Salivary testosterone diurnal variation and psychopathology in adolescent males and females: Individual differences and developmental effects. Dev Psychopathol. 2003;15(2):431–49. 10.1017/s0954579403000233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hermans EJ, Ramsey NF, van Honk J. Exogenous Testosterone Enhances Responsiveness to Social Threat in the Neural Circuitry of Social Aggression in Humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(3):263–70. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.van Wingen G, Mattern C, Verkes RJ, Buitelaar J, Fernández G. Testosterone reduces amygdala & orbitofrontal cortex coupling. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(1):105–13. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Viau V. Functional cross-talk between the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal and -adrenal axes. J Neuroendocrinol. 2002;14(6):506–13. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Viau V, Meaney MJ. The inhibitory effect of testosterone on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress is mediated by the medial preoptic area. J Neurosci. 1996;16(5):1866–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Handa RJ, Nunley KM, Lorens SA, Louie JP, McGivern RF, Bollnow MR. Androgen regulation of adrenocorticotropin and corticosterone secretion in the male-rat following novelty and foot shock stressors. Physiol Behav. 1994;55(1):117–24. 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90018-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Ross JL, Kushner H, Kowal K, Bardsley M, Davis S, Reiss AL, et al. Androgen Treatment Effects on Motor Function, Cognition, and Behavior in Boys with Klinefelter Syndrome. J Pediatr. 2017;185:193–9.e4. Epub 2017/03/14. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.02.036 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Chapman E, Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G. Fetal testosterone and empathy: Evidence from the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and the "Reading the Mind in the Eyes" Test. Soc Neurosci. 2006;1(2):135–48. 10.1080/17470910600992239 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Taylor K, Hackett G. Fetal testosterone and empathy. Horm Behav. 2006;49(3):282–92. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.08.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Samango-Sprouse CA, Sadeghin T, Mitchell FL, Dixon T, Stapleton E, Kingery M, et al. Positive effects of short course androgen therapy on the neurodevelopmental outcome in boys with 47,XXY syndrome at 36 and 72 months of age. Am J Med Genet A. 2013;161A(3):501–8. 10.1002/ajmg.a.35769 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Mohatt J, Bennett SM, Walkup JT. Treatment of Separation, Generalized, and Social Anxiety Disorders in Youths. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171(7):741–8. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13101337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ung D, Selles R, Small BJ, Storch EA. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Anxiety in Youth with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2015;46(4):533–47. 10.1007/s10578-014-0494-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Data cannot be made publicly available for ethical reasons, because public availability would compromise participant confidentiality. Data are digitally stored at the secure servers of the Faculty of Social and Behavioral Science of Leiden University (The Netherlands), according to the guidelines of the Medical Research and Ethics Committee of Leiden University Medical Center. Please contact d.j.g.arnoldus@fsw.leidenuniv.nl (secretary of the Ethics Committee at Leiden University) for accessing the data.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES