Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2018 Aug;30(3):1063–1086. doi: 10.1017/S0954579418000500

Atypical fetal development: Fetal alcohol syndrome, nutritional deprivation, teratogens and risk for neurodevelopmental disorders and psychopathology

Michael K Georgieff 1, Phu V Tran 2, Erik S Carlson 3
PMCID: PMC6074054  NIHMSID: NIHMS958651  PMID: 30068419

Overview

Typical brain development follows an expectant rate during fetal and early neonatal life. The process of typical brain development has been well described at the neural circuit and gene expression levels (Allen-Institute, 2010; Thompson & Nelson, 2001). Development is largely driven by genes that are independent of environmental stimuli, allowing the brain to develop relatively normally in spite of a wide range of environmental conditions, a concept particularly relevant to the diathesis-stress model of psychopathologic etiology (Monroe & Simons, 1991). It is likely that this “clock-like” development assures a fundamentally similar outcome across a wide range of human experiences. Nevertheless, it is clear that experience plays a role in brain development and that the impact of experience is greater during periods of rapid growth and differentiation (Consortium, 2013). Indeed, the remarkable overlap among genes identified as risk factors for multiple disparate psychopathologies suggests that a common fundamental grounding leads through environmental factors to multi-finality of the behavioral phenotype in adulthood (Consortium, 2013).

The impact varies based on timing of the experience because the brain is not a homogenous organ. Rather, it is composed of different regions, cell types, and processes (eg, myelination, neurotransmission). Each has a different developmental trajectory and therefore peak period of rapid growth and differentiation. For example, the hippocampus’ peak period of development starts in the third trimester and proceeds through the first 18 months. Its peak is well before that of the prefrontal cortex that matures later postnatally.

This differential development timing becomes crucial when determining the effect of a fetal environmental event on a circuit that relies on the integrity of both structures, eg, the VTA loop. This neural circuit integrates multiple behaviors and dysfunction of it through imbalance of its tonic hippocampal input and phasic frontal lobe input has been implicated in the etiology of schizophrenia and drug addiction (Esmaeili & Grace, 2013). To illustrate the differential effect of timing of a fetal environmental event, we can consider what occurs to its construction and function in a preclinical rodent model when a nutritional substrate that is crucial for neuronal growth and differentiation (ie, iron) is deprived in the fetal and newborn period when hippocampal, but not frontal lobe development is particularly rapid. In this illustrative case, early-life iron deficiency results in an adult animal with significant hippocampal, but minimal frontal lobe impairment. The effect at the circuit level is complex and results in hippocampal disinhibition, which allows for greater dominance of frontal lobe inputs into the VTA loop (A. T. Schmidt, Alvarez, Grove, Rao, & Georgieff, 2012). Consequently, the adult shows greater cognitive flexibility on set-shifting tasks than always iron sufficient controls in spite of also demonstrating rather poor declarative memory. This example emphasizes the importance of both timing and duration of early life environmental events on subsequent adult functioning.

Fetal environmental effects can cause acute functional changes to the brain that return to normal once the environmental effector is removed. In the case of short-duration environmental effects, the long-term effects may be minimal if no critical periods of development were missed and if environmental homeostasis can be reestablished. However, all of the early life environmental effects that are discussed in this article have been shown in clinical studies and pre-clinical models to cause disruption of adult function, suggesting important long-term changes to brain anatomy and the molecular biology of synaptic plasticity gene regulation.

From a societal standpoint, the cost to society of early life adverse environmental events stems from the long-term effects, manifested as loss of education and job potential, medical costs for treatment of mental health issues and loss of opportunity to improve socioeconomic status. Indeed, policies and practices that do not ensure optimal early life environmental health represent a failure of a society to invest in its next generation. The adage “how are the children doing?” takes on full trans-generational biological meaning via mechanisms underlying the Developmental Origins of Adult (Mental) Health and Disease paradigm. In this article, we review three major categories of early life environmental disruptions that confer long-term risk to the mental health of humans: fetal alcohol exposure, teratogen exposure and nutritional deprivation.

It should be noted that differentiating fetal from early post-natal life effects is relatively arbitrary on the following counts. First, it may be difficult to separate exposures into such defined epochs. Nutritional deficits accrued by the fetus carry forward into postnatal life. For example, iron is accreted primarily during the last trimester and is utilized by the fetus to support regional neural differentiation, myelination and monoamine neurotransmitter production (Lozoff & Georgieff, 2006). Fetal iron deficiency results in acute recognition memory failure, slower speed of neural processing and a disengaged newborn infant (Amin et al., 2010; Siddappa et al., 2004; Wachs, Pollitt, Cueto, Jacoby, & Creed-Kanashiro, 2005); symptoms that are completely consistent with the known biology (Lozoff & Georgieff, 2006). However, low iron stores at birth due to reduced fetal accretion also results in reduced substrate for rapid postnatal brain growth and differentiation. The relatively slow transfer of postnatal dietary iron across the blood brain barrier results in an extended period of additional postnatal brain iron deficiency and thus confers a risk to more, postnatally-developing circuits. Similarly, teratogens can accumulate in the body during fetal life and thus affect fetal brain development acutely. Postnatally, those stored teratogens can continue to damage developing structures and alter gene expression. In contrast, fetal alcohol exposure clearly ends with delivery of the baby from the alcohol consuming mother, thus removing any risk of postnatal exposure.

Second, the DOHaD hypothesis, initially known as the Barker Hypothesis, initially focused on adult cardiovascular risk as a function of restricted fetal nutrition and growth (Barker, 1997). Barker’s conclusion that “fetal programming” accounted for the increased risk of hypertension, type II diabetes and cardiovascular in the elderly instigated the search for fetal factors that mediate the effect. Findings since the original reports indicate that the effect is not solely restricted to fetal life, but that the transition from fetal to neonatal life happened to provide a convenient “cut point” to study; ie, a time when a human goes from one nutritional environment (ie, intrauterine) to a potentially radically different one (ie, extrauterine). A major refinement of Barker’s “fetal programming” hypothesis came in the early 2000’s when Gluckman and Hanson demonstrated that the predictive value of fetal growth restriction for adult heart disease was much greater if one considered the growth rate of the individual postnatally during the first months of life. Rapid postnatal weight gain after intrauterine growth restriction exacerbated the risk in adulthood (Gluckman & Hanson, 2004) and it was postulated that any jarring change in environment early in life induces stress responses as indexed by altered hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis regulation and pro-inflammatory responses as indexed by elevated pro-inflammatory cytokines. Both responses are injurious to developing organs such as the heart and blood vessel systems. Indeed, if intrauterine growth restricted fetuses grew slowly postnatally their risk of cardiovascular diseases was mitigated. The findings raised the possibility that a developmental window exists when metabolic set points are set for the lifespan, likely through epigenetic mechanisms.

Subsequent studies in premature infants, who frequently undergo severe growth restriction postnatally, also demonstrate an increased risk of central adiposity, early-life hypertension and insulin resistance. These infants would have had their growth restriction followed by rapid catch-up growth in the same developmental window as Barker’s IUGR subjects (ie, 24–40 weeks postconceptional age), but all of the events would have occurred in the extrauterine environment. The postnatal duration of the sensitive period for setting metabolic set points is unknown. Data from internationally adopted children demonstrating increased central adiposity and relative stunting suggest that the period may extend through the first year of life. Although the birth history of most of these children is poorly documented, it is clear that they undergo a period of malnutrition, stress and growth restriction until placement (Johnson et al., 2010; Johnson et al., 1992). Multiple endocrine mediators of growth and development are disrupted (B. S. Miller et al., 2010). Adoption into a resource rich home typically stimulates rapid catch-up growth, consistent with the abrupt change in environmental conditions described by Gluckman and Hanson (2004), and accompanied by altered HPA axis regulation (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2008; Kertes, Gunnar, Madsen, & Long, 2008).

Besides the peripheral effects such as stimulation of central adiposity, the concern is that both cortisol and pro-inflammatory cytokines cross the blood-brain-barrier can affect early brain development. Thus, Insel and others have proposed that the DOHaD principle applies to mental as well as physical health (Bale et al., 2010). Thus, while “fetal programming” remains an attractive and relatively easily studied construct, the definition of early life environmental effects on long-term neurodevelopment must be broadened to account for carry over effects from fetal life and the possibility that postnatal environmental events particularly during the first-year affect lifespan neurodevelopment through similar mechanisms ascribed to “fetal programming.” Randomized clinical trials support this construct. Iron supplementation of pregnant women from early pregnancy to 12 weeks postpartum significantly increases performance on working memory and inhibitory control in their offspring at 7–9 years of age compared to children whose mothers were not supplemented (Christian et al., 2010). In contrast, iron supplementation of 12–36 month old children, whose mothers did not receive prenatal iron supplementation, had no effect on neurodevelopment at 7–9 years (Christian et al., 2011; Pongcharoen et al., 2011). Similarly, linear growth in the first 12 months postnatally strongly correlates with IQ at 9 years of age, but growth after 12 months had no impact (Pongcharoen et al., 2012). Clearly, the fetal period through 12-months postnatal age is of particular importance in maintaining fetal and early postnatal nutritional homeostasis in order to optimize long-term development.

The duration of a term pregnancy in the human is 9 months and is typically divided into trimesters. Neurodevelopment can be affected by events in any of the fetal trimesters. Typically, events that occur in the first trimesters have a larger impact on outcome than events in the third trimester because the neurologic processes that are disrupted are more fundamental in nature. These processes include structural embryogenesis in the first 8 weeks of pregnancy (when many mothers do not definitively know that they are pregnant), neurogenesis and cell migration. The respective clinical syndromes of neural tube defect (eg, spina bifida, anencephaly, holoprosencephaly), microcephaly, and migration defects (eg, lissencephaly) cause severe neurodevelopmental impairments. Events that occur in the third trimester typically cause less major disruptions but nevertheless can have significant impact on neural circuit construction. The latter is particularly important when relating fetal events to later risk of developmental psychopathology. All of the adverse fetal brain exposures discussed in this article follow these general rules of early vs late timing. For example, fetal alcohol exposure in the first trimester results in severe microcephaly and structural abnormalities while the same exposure in the third trimester often spares head growth, but results in more subtle deficits that may not become obvious for years (Bale et al., 2010).

A series of fundamental questions are thus raised when assessing environmental events in fetal life, brain development and subsequent risk of psychopathology. Does altered early brain development due to fetal environmental events result in altered child and adult brain function, as indexed by altered behavior? If so, how predictable is the linkage? What accounts for the variability in behavioral outcomes following fetal environmental events? What are the mechanisms by which early life events permanently alter neural circuitry such that the behavioral phenotype remains abnormal despite correction of the abnormal fetal environment? Do disparate events that occur during a specific epoch in pregnancy result in a similar behavioral phenotype (ie, unifinality) or does the nature of the fetal event matter in terms of structures that are affected?

To answer these questions, it is useful to consider the types of fetal environmental events that affect early brain development. Pre- and early postnatal events affect early brain development by two basic pathways: events of omission and events of commission. Events of omission occur when factors important for normal brain development are absent. For example, premature delivery shortchanges the fetus of expectant intrauterine nutrients that are critical for brain development and are transported from mother to fetus during the third trimester. Not only does the prematurely born fetus fail to accrete those nutrients, but most preterm infants fail to thrive for months postnatally (Ehrenkranz, 2000) because they are difficult to nourish and because their metabolic needs are quite different than the in utero fetus.

Events of commission are defined as adverse events that happen to the brain because substances that should not be in the young brain are present. Classic examples of events of commission include fetal alcohol or teratogen exposure because these disruptors alter the architecture and metabolism of the developing brain. Clinically, the outcomes of many populations are a function of a combination of omitted and committed events. Events of commission can lead to events of omission. For example, fetal alcohol exposure, a clear event of commission, also reduces iron accretion by the fetal brain leading to brain iron deficiency, a clear event of omission. Events of omission can also lead to secondary omission events. Intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR) due to maternal malnutrition or maternal hypertension shortchanges the developing fetal brain of multiple nutrients that are critical for growth and development including glucose, fatty acids, amino acids, iron, zinc, copper, folate, choline, and vitamin B12 (Rao & Georgieff, 2002). However, infants with IUGR are also more vulnerable to neonatal encephalopathy at birth because of the fragile nature of their physiology. Neonatal encephalopathy is characterized by a reduced oxygen delivery to the brain- clearly an event of omission. The compounding of adverse events is frequently a greater risk to the brain than the individual components added together.

Behavior is the efferent expression of the brain (Hebb, 1949). Since Hebb’s time, it has become apparent that while neuronal activity is the main driver of behavior, the glia play an important part in behavior as well. And, while neuronal anatomic complexity is closely correlated to functionality in certain brain structures (eg, hippocampus), non-anatomic functions such as neuronal metabolism and neurotransmitter function are drivers of the brain’s electrical potential. Environmental factors (eg, fetal nutrition) can affect all cellular aspects of brain function from anatomy to electrophysiology. For example, fetal nutrients such as protein, fats, iron, zinc and choline affect aspects of neuronal anatomy including cell number (cell division), size and complexity. These same nutrients affect anatomical characteristics of oligodendrocytes that generate myelin and improve speed of processing, astrocytes that deliver nutrients and carry out reparative processes and microglia that are involved in cell trafficking and inflammatory responses. Astrocytes also provide both the “gliogenic” and “neurogenic niches” which are likely important for stress responses in hippocampal neurogenesis, and cortical thickness (Horner & Palmer, 2003). The anatomic brain, however, is inert unless activated through its neurochemistry (eg, neurotransmitters, ion channels) and its ability to generate action potentials (eg, neurometabolism). The same nutrients responsible for anatomic integrity in the fetus also directly regulate neurotransmitter and neurotransmitter receptor expression as well as re-uptake mechanisms. The fetal brain is the most metabolically active organ during development, consuming 60% of the body’s total energy expenditure (Kuzawa, 1998). Much of that fetal energy metabolism is devoted to structural development including neurite extension and synaptogenesis during construction of neural circuits that underlie simple and complex behaviors and the energy required to support electrical activity of those circuits. Glucose, protein, iron, zinc and choline have profound effects on mitochondrial and glycolytic metabolism that provide ATP to fuel that fetal growth and brain activity.

As noted earlier, the cost to society of early life environmental disruptors is through their long-term effects. Two major theories that are not mutually exclusive can account for long-term loss of synaptic efficacy. The first revolves around the concept of critical and sensitive periods and the effects of environmental events that occur during a critical or sensitive period. In this conceptual framework, adverse events that occur throughout a critical period of development results in permanent structural damage to the nervous system (Hensch, 2004). The impact of an adverse early life event on the adult behavioral phenotype will vary since each brain region and process is on a unique developmental trajectory. The permanence of the negative effect appears to depend on the adverse event lasting throughout the critical period. For example, neuronal iron deficiency disrupts hippocampal structure if it occurs during the period of rapid growth and differentiation (postnatal day 10 to 28 in the mouse). Repletion of iron within the critical period (ie, day 21) restores structural integrity and preserves adult memory function. In contrast, repletion outside of the critical window (ie, day 42) results in permanent structural and behavioral abnormalities (Callahan, Thibert, Wobken, & Georgieff, 2013; Carlson et al., 2009; Fretham et al., 2012). In this conceptualization, the neurobehavioral deficits relate directly to the disordered neuronal structure and include poorer memory and stimulus gating (Fretham et al., 2012; Jorgenson, Sun, O’Connor, & Georgieff, 2005; Pisansky et al., 2013). The former is consistent with studies of early iron deficiency causing poorer neurocognitive function and an increased risk of schizophrenia in adulthood in humans (Insel, Schaefer, McKeague, Susser, & Brown, 2008; Lukowski et al., 2010).

The second conceptual framework revolves around the ability of early life events, particularly stress and specific nutritional deficiencies, to alter regulation of synaptic plasticity genes through epigenetic modifications (see below). Epigenetic modifications can involve methylation or hydroxyl-methylation of DNA and methylation, acetylation and other biochemical changes of histones. The sum positive or negative effect of epigenetic modification typically cannot be judged by the degree of modification, but instead through understanding whether the genes that are modified are repressed or activated and whether the function of the genes are positive or negative.

Epigenetic modification is the most investigated biological mechanism underlying fetal programming to date. Fetal programming refers to the mechanisms by which early-life environmental stimuli alter how genes are expressed throughout the lifetime. Epigenetics is the study of how supra-genomic changes established early in life can permanently alter gene expression and resultant phenotype, independent of alterations to the genome itself. Mechanisms of epigenetic modification include DNA methylation, histone modification, and modulation by small noncoding RNAs, all of which are capable of altering patterns of gene expression. DNA methylation is particularly well characterized, and includes two waves in which the genome is demethylated and then re-methylated de novo, allowing for a complete “reprogramming” of the methylome. The second wave of re-methylation establishes the methylation pattern that persists into mature somatic tissues, and takes place during early embryogenesis. At this stage, DNA methylation is particularly vulnerable to intrauterine environmental exposures such as stress, toxicants, and malnutrition. Altered methylation of genes or their regulatory domains during this period can lead to lasting changes in gene expression and respective adult phenotypes (Bale et al., 2010; Heijmans, Tobi, Lumey, & Slagboom, 2009; R. Simmons, 2005; Tobi et al., 2009).

Maternal/fetal stress clearly alters DNA methylation of Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF), an important growth and differentiation growth factor for the brain (Lubin, Roth, & Sweatt, 2008). Growth factors are important for brain growth because they promote the effect of nutrients through signaling systems such as the mammalian Target of Rapamycin (mTOR) pathway. Neuronal mTOR pathway activity is regulated by the presence of nutrients that are particularly important for growth and differentiation such as energy, protein, iron, oxygen and zinc. Growth factors regulate the utilization of these essential nutrients to regulate actin polymerization as well as DNA transcription and protein translation rates, all of which in turn determine cell size and complexity (Fretham, Carlson, & Georgieff, 2011; Wullschleger, Loewith, & Hall, 2006). Reduction of BDNF levels in the hippocampus results in reduced downstream expression of genes that are critical for neuronal function. Lower BDNF levels during development are associated with abnormal neuronal structure in adulthood in pre-clinical models (Brunette, Tran, Wobken, Carlson, & Georgieff, 2010a; Fretham et al., 2012; Jorgenson et al., 2005).

Several fetal/neonatal nutritional conditions alter brain epigenetic status in pre-clinical models. Intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR), a model of generalized fetal malnutrition, disrupts hippocampal histone methylation (Ke et al., 2014). It also activates alters BDNF DNA methylation (Ke et al., 2010), potentially through inducing activation of the stress axis hormones. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (eg, docosahexaenoic acid) found in fish oils also epigenetically modify BDNF through DNA methylation. Early life iron deficiency has little effect on BDNF methylation, but instead exerts is effects on the genome through a direct effect on a family of iron containing histone demethylases (JARIDs/KDMs) that regulate BDNF expression (Blegen et al., 2013; Tran, Kennedy, Lien, Simmons, & Georgieff, 2015). Finally, certain nutrients in the one-carbon family (eg, choline, folate, betaine) can act as methyl donors and affect both DNA and histone methylation status (Barua et al., 2014; Cho et al., 2013; Zeisel, 2017).

In summary, fetal environmental events can permanently alter the expression of genes that are critical to adult neural function. The effects can be circuit and region specific, thereby potentially altering the activity of circuits and leading to aberrant behavioral phenotypes in adulthood. The critical period and epigenetic conceptual frameworks are not mutually exclusive as altered structure will result in altered gene expression and vice versa.

For each of the fetal clinical conditions considered in the following sections, we explore the evidence at the epidemiologic, clinical, pre-clinical and molecular levels that supports a causative association between the fetal condition and later development of psychopathology. The range of conditions represents both events of commission (fetal alcohol syndrome; teratogen exposure) to events of commission (single or multiple nutrient deficits). While it is tempting to ascribe a direct cause and effect pathway for any of these conditions, it is likely that any of the psychopathologies that are proposed to have developmental origins (including schizophrenia, autism spectrum disorder, depression/anxiety, and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder) are polygenic, and potentially poly-environmental in their etiologies (Bale et al., 2010; Basavarajappa & Subbanna, 2016). Careful dissection of the relative contribution of each early-life factor may identify potential treatment and prevention targets in order to reduce the overall risk of disease appearance later in life.

Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder (FASD) and its relationship to developmental psychopathology

Epidemiological and clinical studies

Prenatal alcohol exposure is considered the most common cause of mental retardation (NIAAA, 1990). High rates of psychopathology are seen in children with FASD, including externalizing and internalizing behaviors, sleep disorders, abnormal habits and stereotypies, and behavioral management problems, even in the absence of intellectual disability (Steinhausen, Willms, & Spohr, 1993; Streissguth et al., 1991). The high rates of psychopathology persist in adulthood and include substance disorders, mood disorders, psychotic disorders, personality disorders, anxiety disorders and eating disorders (Famy, Streissguth, & Unis, 1998). Fetal alcohol damage is associated with later onset of schizophrenia (Slavney & Grau, 1978). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) was found to be the most common co-morbid disorder among children with FASD (52.9%), followed by ODD (12.9%), CD (7.0%), and ASD (2.6%) (Lange, Rehm, Anagnostou, & Popova, 2017). While FASD is associated with global executive impairments, executive function weaknesses are most commonly found and consist of impairments of planning, fluency, and set-shifting (Kingdon, Cardoso, & McGrath, 2016).

FASD results in changes to multiple neural circuits that are able to be imaged non-invasively in humans. High incidences of midline brain abnormalities of the corpus callosum such as hypoplasia or agenesis are seen after prenatal alcohol exposure (Bookstein et al., 2005; Bookstein et al., 2007; Bookstein, Sampson, Streissguth, & Connor, 2001; Riley et al., 1995; Sowell, Mattson, et al., 2001; Sowell, Thompson, et al., 2001; Swayze et al., 1997). More recent studies examining white matter microstructure with DTI have found global white matter changes that could account for dysfunction of multiple behavioral domains including working memory, mathematical ability, executive function, processing speed, intelligence and interhemispheric transfer (Donald, Eastman, et al., 2015; Donald, Roos, et al., 2015; Fan et al., 2016; Fryer et al., 2009; Lebel, Rasmussen, Wyper, Andrew, & Beaulieu, 2010; Lebel et al., 2008; Malisza et al., 2012; Spottiswoode et al., 2011; Taylor et al., 2015; Wozniak et al., 2006; Wozniak et al., 2009). FASD is also associated with changes in functional connectivity in cortical networks (medial prefrontal and posterior cingulate), default mode network, corticostriatal networks (affecting working memory), and whole-brain connectivity (associated with lower global cognitive functioning) (Roussotte et al., 2012; Santhanam et al., 2011; Wozniak et al., 2017; Wozniak et al., 2011). Hippocampal volume and declarative memory deficits are prominent long-term features of FASD in humans (Autti-Ramo et al., 2002; Willoughby, Sheard, Nash, & Rovet, 2008). The degree of hippocampal volume loss in humans is directly correlated with the amount of memory deficit (Autti-Ramo et al., 2002; Willoughby et al., 2008).

Preclinical models and molecular mechanisms

Preclinical models have investigated the effect of fetal alcohol exposure on the developing brain and the subsequent consequences to the adult brain. The behavioral findings in the preclinical model animals, including rats, mice and non-human primates, corroborate the findings in humans. Analysis of the brains from the models has provided biological plausibility at the regional anatomic and circuit level to support the hypothesis that fetal alcohol exposure results in psychopathology at an older age in humans. Molecular analyses of the brains from the models provide insights into the mechanisms of the long-term findings. In general, the long-term effects of fetal alcohol exposure follow the temporal and regionalization rules seen with nutrient deficiencies (Kretchmer, Beard, & Carlson, 1996; Maier, Miller, Blackwell, & West, 1999). Alcohol exposure during different trimesters in the rat resulted in regional brain area differences of cell loss and also contrasted with alcohol exposure throughout the entire pregnancy (Maier et al., 1999).

Preclinical models demonstrate that alcohol exposure results in both global and regional specific effects. Global effects are particularly prominent with early- to mid-gestation onset of exposure. Two doses of alcohol on day 9 of a 21-day gestation results in volume and significant misshaping of the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, striatum and cerebellum (Parnell et al., 2013). The regional effects of fetal alcohol exposure in the rodent demonstrate that the hippocampus is particularly vulnerable (Berman & Hannigan, 2000; Livy, Miller, Maier, & West, 2003; M. W. Miller, 1995), supporting similar findings in humans (Autti-Ramo et al., 2002; Willoughby et al., 2008). All subregions of the hippocampus are affected in rodent models. The dentate gyrus and CA1 have reduced neuronal number (Klintsova et al., 2007; M. W. Miller, 1995). Exposure restricted to the third trimester reduces the volume of CA1 due to lower cell number and reduced pyramidal cell density (Livy et al., 2003). Prenatal alcohol exposure also damages area CA3 (Ba, Seri, Aka, Glin, & Tako, 1999).

The loss of hippocampal anatomy is accompanied by loss of synaptic plasticity across the lifespan in rodent models (Hablitz, 1986; Krahl, Berman, & Hannigan, 1999; Swartzwelder, Farr, Wilson, & Savage, 1988; Tan, Berman, Abel, & Zajac, 1990). In vitro electrophysiological studies in hippocampal brain slices isolated from adult animals following prenatal alcohol-exposed animals demonstrate changes in synaptic activity (Berman & Hannigan, 2000). The loss of electrophysiologic potential may result from abnormalities in glutamatergic neurotransmission (Kimura, Reynolds, & Brien, 2000; Reynolds & Brien, 1995; Thomas, Fleming, Sl, & Riley, 2001). Interestingly, fetal alcohol exposure also results in brain iron deficiency (Helfrich, Saini, Kling, & Smith, 2017; Huebner, Blohowiak, Kling, & Smith, 2016; Rufer et al., 2012), which in turn compromises glutamatergic neurotransmission (Jorgenson et al., 2005; Pisansky et al., 2013; Rao, Tkac, Townsend, Gruetter, & Georgieff, 2003).

The translation of alcohol-induced hippocampal deficits from preclinical models to human long-term behavioral abnormalities extends beyond declarative memory deficits because the hippocampus is also central in circuits that support more complex behaviors. For example, tonic hippocampal input is critical for regulation of the VTA loop. Disruption of this input through compromise of hippocampal function with resulting imbalance between hippocampal and pre-frontal cortical inputs has been proposed as a mechanism in schizophrenia (Esmaeili & Grace, 2013; White, Joseph, Francis, & Liddle, 2010). Dysregulation of the VTA loop has also been implicated in drug addiction research and, as noted above, the rate of drug addiction is higher in adults with FASD (Macht, Kelly, & Gass, 2017). The hippocampus is central to the circuitry involved in sensory gating, a key behavior that is disordered in schizophrenia (Pisansky et al., 2013). Finally, the integrity of the hippocampus in early life is important in providing scaffolding for frontal lobe development later in childhood. While the prefrontal cortex may be directly injured by fetal alcohol exposure, it may also receive poor connections from an injured limbic system (Wachs, Georgieff, Cusick, & McEwen, 2014). Fetal insults that initially cause declarative learning and memory deficits in the newborn period can manifest as attentional issues at older ages (Deregnier, Nelson, Thomas, Wewerka, & Georgieff, 2000; Jabes, Thomas, Langworthy, Georgieff, & Nelson, 2015).

In humans, prenatal alcohol exposure modifies the somatic cell DNA methylation (Portales-Casamar et al., 2016), providing evidence for potential epigenetic mechanism underlying the long-term changes in gene expression. Certainly, more studies are needed to establish this relationship. In animal models, fetal alcohol exposure alters expression of neural gene critical for important cellular processes, including proliferation, fate specification, migration and differentiation (Kleiber, Mantha, Stringer, & Singh, 2013; Sanchez-Alvarez, Gayen, Vadigepalli, & Anni, 2013). These findings provide molecular and cellular basis to explicate the neurodevelopmental deficits associated with prenatal alcohol exposure. The relationship between prenatal alcohol exposure and epigenetic modifications is more evident in the animal models. In line with limited data from humans, prenatal alcohol modifies DNA methylation patterns in mouse embryos (Hicks, Middleton, & Miller, 2010; Y. Liu, Balaraman, Wang, Nephew, & Zhou, 2009), expression of non-coding RNAs implicated in epigenetic modifications (Laufer et al., 2013), and neural expression of DNA methylation modifiers such as DNMTs and MeCP2 (Gangisetty, Bekdash, Maglakelidze, & Sarkar, 2014; Kim et al., 2013; Liyanage, Zachariah, Davie, & Rastegar, 2015; Perkins, Lehmann, Lawrence, & Kelly, 2013). Emerging evidence has also implicated prenatal alcohol exposure in modifying histone acetylation and methylation as another epigenetic link to the long-term gene dysregulation (Chater-Diehl, Laufer, & Singh, 2017).

Preclinical models of teratogen exposure (bisphenol (BPA) and their relation to developmental psychopathology

Teratogens represent a major “commission” risk to the developing fetal brain. Teratogens typically fall into the categories of drug exposures and environmental toxicants. In fact, alcohol (discussed above) easily fits into the teratogen category as an abnormal drug exposure. In all cases they represent environmental molecules that are not needed for normal brain development. It is beyond the scope of this article to catalogue the suspected brain effects of all teratogens. The following discussion focuses on bisphenol-A (BPA) because of growing evidence that potential endocrine disruptors during pregnancy significantly affect brain development.

Epidemiological and clinical studies

Fetal-neonatal exposure to bisphenol A (BPA), a chemical used in the production of commonly used plastics, is associated with dysfunction in metabolism and reproductive development (Dolinoy, Huang, & Jirtle, 2007). BPA can traverse the placenta and is found in fetal serum and full-term amniotic fluid, with highest levels found between 15–18 weeks gestation (Ikezuki, Tsutsumi, Takai, Kamei, & Taketani, 2002). In humans, while the long-term effects of early-life BPA exposure on neurobehavioral development remains controversial (Casas et al., 2015), accumulating evidence suggest a positive association between BPA exposure and risk of ADHD in male (Philippat et al., 2017; Tewar et al., 2016), internalizing and externalizing behavior in male (Evans et al., 2014), and social communication deficit in female children (Lim et al., 2017; Miodovnik et al., 2011). Braun et al. found a relationship between prenatal BPA exposure and externalizing behavior in 2-year-old girls but not boys using the Behavioral Assessment System for Children (BASC) (Braun et al., 2009). Gestational urinary BPA concentrations were associated with some neurobehavioral measures at 3 years of age in this cohort (prospective birth cohort, 244 mothers). In particular, gestational BPA exposure was associated with higher scores for measures of anxiety, hyperactivity, emotional control, and behavioral inhibition (internalizing and externalizing behaviors) Results were more pronounced for externalizing behaviors: girls exhibited increases in hyperactivity, and boys exhibited decreases in hyperactivity (Braun & Hauser, 2011; Tewar et al., 2016). Conversely, another study found that among boys (but not girls), prenatal BPA exposure was significantly associated with emotionally reactive and aggressive behaviors and higher syndrome scores (internalizing and externalizing problems) between the ages of 3–5 (Perera et al., 2012). Another study found no correlation between maternal BPA and infant neurobehavioral abnormalities in 5-week-old infant neurobehaviors, however, the authors noted that BPA concentrations in the maternal samples may have been below the threshold of neurobehavioral effects (Yolton et al., 2011).

Preclinical models and molecular mechanisms

The findings in humans suggest a connection between BPA and brain circuits that mediate attention deficit disorder, schizophrenia, social communication deficits and internalizing/externalizing disorders. The biology underlying the behavioral deficits observed in human studies derives from the bodies of work in animal models and implicates abnormal structure in the hippocampus, caudate nucleus and prefrontal cortex as well as significant effects on at least two neurotransmitter systems. These cellular and structural changes likely underlie the behavioral effects of prenatal BPA exposure.

BPA, like nutrient deficiencies follows the rules of timing and regionalization. Rat dams were exposed to BPA in the last trimester of pregnancy and then randomized to additional exposure or placebo postnatally. Open field and elevated plus maze tests of anxiety-like behaviors administered during the juvenile and adult periods demonstrated that prenatal exposure reduced exploratory behavior and increased anxiety particularly in females (Gioiosa, Parmigiani, Vom Saal, & Palanza, 2013). The sexually dimorphic behavior may be due to inhibition of estrogen receptor-beta (Xu et al., 2010).

Fetal and neonatal BPA exposure in mice compromises recognition memory as evidenced by poorer performance on the novel object recognition test, a finding that suggests compromise of the developing hippocampus (Tian, Baek, Lee, & Jang, 2010). Adverse effects are seen at both the neuroanatomical and the neurotransmitter receptor level. Reduced recognition memory is accompanied by lower NMDA receptor, consistent with alterations to the glutamatergic neurotransmitter system that is dominant in that brain area. The NMDA receptor effect is dose-dependent. Low dose BPA exposure decreases NMDA concentration, whereas high doses more selectively suppress NR1 than NR2A or B. This is developmentally important because NR1 combines first with NR2A and then developmentally switches to NR2B in when long-term potentiation matures. BPA induced alteration of subunits expression would be expected to alter hippocampal electrophysiologic development and capacity (Jorgenson et al., 2005). The hippocampus of prenatally BPA exposed monkeys also contains fewer dopaminergic neurons (Elsworth et al., 2013), suggesting compromise of a second neurotransmitter system. While the hippocampus has relatively low amounts of dopamine, its presence is important for providing salience to fear memories. Dopamine blockade increases the rate of fear memory extinction, whereas dopamine agonists slow the extinction rate (Menezes et al., 2015; Ponnusamy, Nissim, & Barad, 2005). The finding of fewer dopaminergic neurons is consistent with the reduced anxiety and fear that BPA-exposed mice demonstrate on open field and elevated plus maze tests (Tian et al., 2010).

Prenatal BPA exposure disrupts at least three hormone systems that have a role in brain development. In addition to the effect on estrogen receptor beta (Xu et al., 2010), stress-axis hormones may also be involved as fetal/neonatal BPA exposure alters glucocorticoid receptors more in females than males accompanied by greater anxiety in female offspring in adulthood (Poimenova, Markaki, Rahiotis, & Kitraki, 2010). Furthermore, fetal/neonatal exposure acts as a thyroid hormone receptor antagonist in rats, potentially resetting neuronal metabolic set points and mitochondrial activity (Zoeller, Bansal, & Parris, 2005). Reduction of neuronal metabolic activity would result in simpler dendritic structure and consequent loss of function in the same manner as seen with nutrient deficiencies (Bastian, von Hohenberg, Mickelson, Lanier, & Georgieff, 2016).

The brain regions and neurotransmitter systems that are altered permanently by fetal/neonatal BPA exposure reside in neural circuits that mediate important complex behaviors including the VTA loop and mesolimbic-prefrontal connections. The involvement of these circuits’ anatomy and neurochemistry provides a plausible biological basis for the psychopathology reported in human juveniles and adults, including anxiety, internalizing/externalizing behaviors, and schizophrenia.

In mice, there are evidence that fetal-neonatal bisphenol-A (BP-A) exposure alters cortical development by accelerating neuronal differentiation and migration during the early embryonic stage, resulting in a mis-timing of critical development that had lasting effects into adulthood. The alterations were accompanied by changes to promoter-associated CpG islands suggesting that the long-term effects were epigenetic in nature (Itoh, Yaoi, & Fushiki, 2012) Indeed, whole genome DNA methylation is altered in a dose dependent manner by BP-A administered in physiologically relevant concentrations during pregnancy to the agouti mouse (Anderson et al., 2012). Provision of a diet enhanced with methyl donors (eg, folate) that are known to modify CpG island and histone methylation status counteracts the long-term epigenetic effects of fetal/neonatal PBA exposure (Dolinoy et al., 2007). Similarly to the epigenetic findings with early life BP-A exposure, three recent studies showed that developmental exposure to Atrazine, a commonly used agricultural herbicide, also induces changes in histone methylation and DNA methylation patterns in the rodent germ cells across multiple generations, thereby linking Atrazine exposure to stable and transmissible epigenetic modifications (Gely-Pernot et al., 2015; Hao et al., 2016; McBirney et al., 2017). The effects on brain development remain to be determined.

Preclinical models of select nutrient deficiencies and their relation to developmental psychopathology

There is a substantial body of literature on the effect of fetal nutrition on adult mental health. All nutrients are important for brain development during the last trimester, but certain nutrients have a greater acute and lasting impact on the brain and behavior. These include macronutrients such as glucose, protein, and fats (especially long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids; LC-PUFAs) and micronutrients such as iron, zinc, copper, iodine, folate, choline and vitamin B12. These higher-impact nutrients demonstrate critical or sensitive periods for neurodevelopment where early life deficiency results in long-term dysfunction. Several, including LC-PUFAs, iron, folate and choline as well as generalized fetal malnutrition confer these long-term risks through epigenetic mechanisms (Georgieff, Brunette, & Tran, 2015; Grissom & Reyes, 2013; Ke et al., 2014; Tran et al., 2015; Tyagi, Zhuang, Agrawal, Ying, & Gomez-Pinilla, 2015; Zeisel, 2017). A comprehensive review of each nutrient is beyond the scope of this article, but the principles are evident from well-studied nutrients such as iron deficiency and generalized fetal malnutrition, also referred to as intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR). Both have been associated with an increased incidence of later onset of significant developmental psychopathologies such as schizophrenia (Eide et al., 2013; Insel et al., 2008), autism (R. J. Schmidt, Tancredi, Krakowiak, Hansen, & Ozonoff, 2014) and depression/anxiety (Lukowski et al., 2010) and thus merit in depth analyses across all levels of investigation from epidemiology to molecular biology. They stand as valuable paradigms for the biological plausibility of early nutrition effects on adult mental health.

Epidemiological and clinical studies of iron deficiency

Iron-deficiency (ID) is the most common nutrient deficiency in the world (Walker et al., 2007). Fetal ID that affects brain development occurs in the context of maternal diabetes mellitus during pregnancy, maternal hypertension during pregnancy, chronic severe iron deficiency, or maternal anemia due to iron deficiency (J. L. Beard, 2001; Lozoff et al., 2006; Siddappa, Rao, Long, Widness, & Georgieff, 2007). Multiple studies have demonstrated the association between gestational diabetes in the mother and subsequent risk of developmental psychopathologies, particularly autism spectrum disorders and schizophrenia, in the offspring. Gestational diabetes mellitus is more common among mothers who have children with ASD and is associated with greater deficits in expressive language among children with ASD (Krakowiak et al., 2012). The risk for ASD is increased for children of women with diabetes (Leonard, de Klerk, Bourke, & Bower, 2006). Preexisting Type 2 diabetes mellitus and exposure to gestational diabetes diagnosed before (but not after) 26 weeks’ gestation is associated with an increased risk of ASD in offspring (Xiang et al., 2015). In a meta-analysis, diabetes during pregnancy was significantly associated with development of schizophrenia (Cannon, Jones, & Murray, 2002). Uncontrolled maternal diabetes also resulted in poorer performance on measures of intelligence, verbal ability, acquired knowledge, spatial and sequence abilities (Rizzo, Metzger, Dooley, & Cho, 1997). Gestational diabetes reduced expressive language performance in infants of diabetic mothers at 18, 30, and 72/84 months (Dionne, Boivin, Seguin, Perusse, & Tremblay, 2008). An increased risk for mild to moderate intellectual disability was found in children of mothers with diabetes (Leonard et al., 2006). The role of iron deficiency in linking maternal diabetes to poorer brain development was demonstrated in infants of diabetic mothers born with low cord ferritin concentrations indicating fetal iron deficiency. These infants have behavioral and electrophysiological measures of poorer recognition memory throughout the first 12 months of life in spite of resolution of their neonatal iron deficiency (DeBoer, Wewerka, Bauer, Georgieff, & Nelson, 2005; Nelson, Wewerka, Borscheid, Deregnier, & Georgieff, 2003; Nelson et al., 2000; Siddappa et al., 2004).

The role of early life iron deficiency and anemia (most commonly due to iron deficiency) in psychopathology has been assessed independent of maternal diabetes during pregnancy. For mothers with anemia, there is increased risk of severe intellectual disability in their offspring (Hablitz, 1986). Fetal/neonatal ID results in behavioral and electrophysiological measure of poor recognition memory of mother’s voice at 2 months of age (Geng et al., 2015). Early postnatal iron deficiency in Chilean infants is associated with attentional control deficits at age 10 years and heightened risk taking, excessive alcohol use and risky sexual behavior in adolescence (East et al., 2017). Iron deficiency anemia (IDA) during early infancy is also associated with dull affect and social reticence at age 5, leading to functional isolation later in childhood (East et al., 2017). IDA results in long-lasting effects such as slower reaction time and poorer inhibitory control (with correlated EEG findings) on a Go/No-Go task in children 8 to 9 years after diagnosis of iron deficiency and treatment with iron supplementation (Algarin et al., 2013). Infants with ID have adversely affected social-emotional behaviors such as increased shyness and latency to interact with an examiner, decreased positive affect and soothability (Lozoff et al., 2008), as well as poor object permanence and memory encoding and retrieval (Carter et al., 2010). The effects of ID are long lasting, with decreased performance on measures of executive functioning and recognition memory, at 19 years of age (Lukowski et al., 2010). This same group has now been followed out to 25 years, with formerly ID subjects having less completion of secondary school and fewer long-term relationships, and more negative emotions and feelings of detachment (Lozoff et al., 2013). Case control studies have shown that infants with ID have lower scores on measures of mental and motor development, alterations in visual recognition memory, and improvement in these measures with iron supplementation (Friel et al., 2003; Lozoff et al., 2003; Moffatt, Longstaffe, Besant, & Dureski, 1994; Shafir, Angulo-Barroso, Calatroni, Jimenez, & Lozoff, 2006; Sherriff, Emond, Bell, & Golding, 2001). Children with ID in infancy have poorer performance on tests of some specific cognitive functions such as executive function requiring inhibition and planning at 5 and 10 years; and spatial memory, and selective attention in 11–14 year olds (Congdon et al., 2012; Lozoff, Jimenez, Hagen, Mollen, & Wolf, 2000).

Recent neuroimaging studies are beginning to provide anatomic evidence of altered neonatal brain structure due to fetal iron deficiency. A diffusion tensor imaging study showed that low maternal prenatal iron status alters markers of normal development of axonal pathways (such as corpus callosum, internal capsule, and the longitudinal fasciculus) and frontal cortical gray matter (Monk et al., 2016). In a resting-state MRI study, former IDA subjects showed alterations in default mode and dorsal attention networks (Algarin et al., 2017).

Preclinical models and molecular mechanisms of fetal/neonatal iron deficiency

In animal models, fetal-neonatal iron deficiency showed similar neurological and behavioral deficits to the human cohorts (Lozoff & Georgieff, 2006). Fetal-neonatal iron deficiency during critical periods of cortical development increases the risk of autism, ADHD, and schizophrenia pathogenesis (Erikson, Jones, & Beard, 2000; Gambling, Kennedy, & McArdle, 2011; Schubert, Martens, & Kolk, 2015). Pre-clinical models provide evidence for acute and long-term abnormalities in neuronal structure, connectivity, metabolism, and plasticity that are accompanied by changes in gene expression (Tran et al., 2016a).

The neurobiological importance of iron in optimal fetal/neonatal brain development is well established (Lozoff & Georgieff, 2006; Rao, Tkac, Schmidt, & Georgieff, 2011). Iron is a critical nutrient for the enzymatic activity of proteins involved in myelination (J. L. Beard, Wiesinger, & Connor, 2003; Connor & Menzies, 1996), energy metabolism (Goodman, Warshaw, & Dallman, 1970; Willis, Brooks, Henderson, & Dallman, 1987), and monoamine neurotransmission (Goodman et al., 1970; Willis et al., 1987). The proteins include hydroxylases (eg, tyrosine and tryptophan hydroxylase for dopamine and serotonin, respectively) and cytochromes that modulate electron transport in mitochondria.

The effects of fetal/neonatal iron deficiency follow the rules of timing, dose and duration (Kretchmer et al., 1996). Timing appears to be particularly important in determining the long-term behavioral phenotype following early-life deficiency. The prevalence of iron deficiency varies with age in the fetal and pediatric population. Three time periods are characterized by the highest prevalences: late fetal life, toddlerhood and adolescence, particularly in menstruating females. Iron deficiency in each of these epochs causes acute neurocognitive and motor effects (Geng et al., 2015; Murray-Kolb & Beard, 2007; Siddappa et al., 2004). However, only the two early time periods are associated with long-term effects in spite of iron repletion, suggesting that critical periods for brain structures exist in those early time periods. Consideration of which brain structures are rapidly developing when iron deficiency is present has informed our understanding of later childhood and adult behavioral phenotypes. For example, the late fetal period is characterized by rapid hippocampal development, onset of myelination and sculpting of the dopamine neurotransmitter system. Fetal/neonatal iron deficiency compromises each of these and behavioral phenotypes in humans are characterized by poorer declarative memory (Geng et al., 2015; Siddappa et al., 2004); slower speed of processing (Amin et al., 2010; Roncagliolo, Garrido, Walter, Peirano, & Lozoff, 1998) and alterations in mood and affect (Wachs et al., 2005). However, dopaminergic frontal lobe findings that are highly characteristic of iron deficiency in toddlerhood are not a prominent feature of fetal/neonatal iron deficiency most likely because of the relatively rudimentary nature of the prefrontal cortex.

Preclinical models confirm the regional brain vulnerability to fetal/neonatal iron deficiency. Two types of models are utilized to understand the neurobiology. The classic dietary maternal iron deficiency model in which the dam is started on an iron deficiency diet early in gestation with continuation until either postnatal 10 (term human brain equivalent) or postnatal day 21 (weaning) has been the mainstay model (Felt et al., 2006; Rao et al., 2003). More recently, genetically modified mice have been utilized to understand the role of iron in hippocampal neuronal development without the confounding factor of anemia (Carlson et al., 2009; Fretham et al., 2012).

The bulk of the pre-clinical evidence in the literature is based on the dietary rat model. Gestational and gestational/lactational iron deficiency in rats alters brain-wide fatty acid composition of myelin acutely during the period of deficiency (Connor & Menzies, 1996) and causes long term suppression of myelin basic protein gene expression in adulthood after iron repletion (Clardy et al., 2006). It alters dopamine and serotonin neurotransmission acutely in the neonatal period particularly in the nucleus accumbens, striatum, substantia nigra and ventral midbrain. Meso-cortico-limbic and nigro-striatal pathways are particularly vulnerable (J. Beard, Erikson, & Jones, 2003; Pinero, Jones, & Beard, 2001; Youdim & Green, 1978). In adulthood, the cerebellum, frontal cortex, striatum, hippocampus and midbrain show variations in markers of dopamine and serotonin metabolism, accompanied by alterations in behaviors that rely on these structures including negative geotaxis, forelimb placement and novel object recognition (Unger et al., 2012).

Fetal/neonatal dietary iron deficiency anemia in the rat or non-anemic neuronal specific iron deficiency in the mouse alters adult hippocampal metabolism and neurotrophic factor gene expression (Carlson et al., 2009; Tran, Fretham, Carlson, & Georgieff, 2009), dendrite morphology (Brunette et al., 2010a; Carlson et al., 2009; Fretham et al., 2012; Jorgenson, Wobken, & Georgieff, 2003), electrophysiology (Jorgenson et al., 2005; Pisansky et al., 2013) and learning and memory behavior (Carlson et al., 2009; Felt & Lozoff, 1996; Fretham et al., 2012; A. T. Schmidt, Waldow, Grove, Salinas, & Georgieff, 2007). Persistence of these effects into adulthood in the dietary rat model occurred in spite of initiation of iron treatment during the lactation phase (Unger et al., 2012). In the specifically neuronal iron deficiency model, adult structural and behavioral changes were prevented when iron was repleted in the neurons during lactation (Fretham et al., 2012). The likely cause of the discrepancy between the dietary rat and genomic mouse models is that the rat is anemic and total body iron deficient and thus the repletion period is much longer compared to the genomic mouse model. The long-term findings in both models suggest a critical period for iron during development, most likely during the rapid brain growth phase immediately postnatally.

The adult behavioral phenotypes seen in both rodent species models are instructive with respect to developmental psychopathology. An important finding from the non-anemic hippocampal neuronal iron deficiency mouse models was that extra-hippocampal abnormalities were also noted in striatum in spite of normal iron content (Carlson et al., 2010) in addition to the spatial learning and memory deficits that would be expected from a hippocampal iron deficit (Carlson et al., 2009; Fretham et al., 2012). Specifically, striatal metabolism is compromised by hippocampal iron deficiency along with procedural memory behaviors that depend on an intact striatum (Carlson et al., 2010). The findings emphasize the interconnectedness of these brain areas as they work together in integrated, complex neural circuits. The striatal findings may well be driven by compromised hippocampal-derived glutamatergic neurons (Jorgenson et al., 2005) failing to normally potentiate dopaminergic neurons in the VTA loop. Similar circuit effects were demonstrated by Pisansky et al (2013) in the non-anemic hippocampal neuron specific and the dietary iron deficiency anemia models (Pisansky et al., 2013). Paired-pulse inhibition (PPI), a measure of sensory gating often utilized as a model of schizophrenia, is dependent on normal hippocampal function in the complex circuitry that underlies this behavior. PPI is highly abnormal in the adult mouse and rat following fetal/neonatal iron deficiency, suggesting that hippocampal damage sustained in the neonatal period continues to have ramifications at the circuit level. The finding also suggests a plausible biology for the epidemiologic finding that fetal iron deficiency increases the risk of schizophrenia in adulthood in humans (Insel et al., 2008).

Schmidt et al., also found evidence of circuit level dysfunction in adulthood following fetal/neonatal dietary iron deficiency in the rat (A. T. Schmidt et al., 2012; A. T. Schmidt et al., 2007). Formerly iron deficient adult rats showed expected impairments in spatial memory (A. T. Schmidt et al., 2007), but superior performance on set-shifting paradigms (A. T. Schmidt et al., 2012). The spatial memory abnormalities are consistent with the anatomic and electrophysiologic studies in the model that showed permanent hippocampal damage (Brunette et al., 2010a; Jorgenson et al., 2005). However, the increased cognitive flexibility seen in the set-shifting paradigm implies intact or enhanced frontal lobe function. The lower amount of hippocampal inhibition allowed the rats to utilize frontal strategies to successfully complete the set-shifting paradigm. This finding illustrates an important circuitry principle because hippocampus and frontal cortex coordinately regulate VTA loop activity. Disruption of the balance of these inputs is thought to underlie the disordered decision making observed in schizophrenia (Belujon & Grace, 2008; Lisman et al., 2008). Selective lesioning of either hippocampal or frontal inputs results in a schizophrenia phenotype in the rodent (Esmaeili & Grace, 2013).

Non-human primates have been used in a small number of studies to demonstrate the importance of timing of iron deficiency in a more human-like model. Prenatal iron deficiency results in an impulsive behavioral phenotype in the juvenile animal, while postnatal iron deficiency results a hesitant, wary phenotype (Golub, 2010). The findings are consistent with those in humans. Felt et al., have described a hesitant/wary behavioral phenotype in postnatally iron deficient animals and have postulated that the effects are due to compromise of dopaminergic projections to the frontal lobe from the striatum (Felt et al., 2006). In contrast, the attention deficit/hyperactivity phenotype seen with prenatal iron deficiency may be due to a secondary compromise of hippocampal-derived projections to the PFC.

As noted previously, two potential principles can explicate these long-term negative effects. First, in terms of structure-function relationship, poorly formed structure can result in poor neural plasticity and gene regulation. Classical neurotransmitter such as dopamine, whose synthesis is dependent on iron-dependent tyrosine hydroxylase, can compromise neuron number in the developing cortex (Kolk et al., 2009; Popolo, McCarthy, & Bhide, 2004). Second, emerging evidence suggests fetal-neonatal iron deficiency also alters the function of iron-dependent epigenetic modifiers, leading to stable changes in chromatin remodeling, providing an alternative mechanism underlying the long-term gene dysregulation in neurons (Tran et al., 2015).

In preclinical models, early-life ID anemia permanently alters the regulation of genes that are important for adult function, including neurological networks underlying synaptic plasticity, major depression, anxiety, autism, and schizophrenia (Brunette, Tran, Wobken, Carlson, & Georgieff, 2010b; Carlson, Stead, Neal, Petryk, & Georgieff, 2007; Patton, Coe, Lubach, & Connor, 2012; Tran, Dakoji, Reise, Storey, & Georgieff, 2013; Tran et al., 2016b). Epigenetic modifications of these gene networks by iron are highly plausible mechanisms to explain these long-term findings because two of the known epigenetic mechanisms (i.e., methyl-cytosine hydroxylation and histone methylation) are iron-dependent and are mediated by two families of iron-containing proteins: The Ten-Eleven-Translocation methyl-cytosine hydroxylases (TETs) and the methyl-lysine demethylases (KDMs/JARIDs). Both protein families require iron for their catalytic activity (Kouzarides, 2007; Pedersen & Helin, 2010; Ponnaluri, Maciejewski, & Mukherji, 2013). KDMs interact with histone deacetylases (HDACs) (Barrett et al., 2007; Chang, Chen, Zhao, & Bruick, 2007; Chicas et al., 2012; Ramadoss, Chen, & Wang, 2012; Suh, Aoyama, Matsumori, Liu, & Swanson, 2005) further supporting a mechanistic link between neuronal iron status and epigenetic modifications. The findings of fetal-neonatal iron deficiency effects on TET activity remain unknown. In contrast, the persistently dysregulated KDMs accompanied by substantial changes in histone methylation at the Bdnf locus (Blegen et al., 2013; Tran et al., 2015), suggesting KDM dysfunction in adult hippocampus following early-life ID, provides a strong rationale to for KDM’s role in reprogramming gene regulation. In addition to the intracellular iron content, KDMs may be further regulated by iron-dependent proline hydroxylases (PHDs), thereby potentiating the effect of early-life ID. Iron availability is essential for PHD to hydroxylate proline residues of HIF1a (Kaelin & Ratcliffe, 2008; Nandal et al., 2011), which in turn directly regulates the expression of KDMs (Beyer, Kristensen, Jensen, Johansen, & Staller, 2008; Lee, Choi, Oh, Park, & Park, 2014; Mudie et al., 2014; Niu et al., 2012; Pollard et al., 2008). PHD1 expression is upregulated acutely in iron-deficient hippocampus (Carlson et al., 2007) and is normalized following iron treatment in the formerly ID hippocampus (Tran et al., 2016b). The finding that KDMs remain dysregulated while PHD effect is resolved in adulthood points to a role of PHD in reprogramming of KDMs during the neonatal period. Elucidating the iron-dependent epigenetic mechanisms and their roles in reprogramming neural genes remains an emerging field and an active area of research.

Epidemiological and clinical studies of IUGR

IUGR occurs in 10% of pregnancies. Worldwide, the most common etiology of IUGR is maternal undernutrition while in the United States the most common cause is nutrient restriction due to placental insufficiency from maternal hypertension. Maternal infections particularly early in the pregnancy also causes IUGR. Each would be expected to have differential effects on the developing brain and animal models can provide such insight. All etiologies result in restriction of nutrient flow from mother to fetus and result in a fetus that is smaller than its genetic potential. Both maternal malnutrition and IUGR have been epidemiologically associated with increased risks of developmental psychopathologies including schizophrenia, autism, depression and neurocognitive delay.

The relationship of IUGR to schizophrenia has been documented in several studies (E. Susser et al., 1996; E. S. Susser & Lin, 1992). A meta-analysis revealed that birth weight < 2000g and <2500g is significantly associated with development of schizophrenia (Cannon et al., 2002). Not only is a birth weight of less than 2500g at term significantly associated with increased risk of schizophrenia, but a significant linear trend of increasing odds ratios with decreasing birth weight has been observed for infants with birthweights greater than 2500g (Abel et al., 2010). Notably, the same risk pattern has been shown for other psychiatric categories, including ‘any psychiatric diagnosis,’ ‘alcohol and drug disorders,’ affective disorders, and ‘neurotic, stress-related and somatoform disorders’ (Abel et al., 2010).

The risk of autism is associated with daily smoking during pregnancy (a risk factor for IUGR), and with being low weight for gestational age (Hultman, Sparen, & Cnattingius, 2002). Depression in adulthood has also been associated with having had IUGR as a fetus. Young adults, who were born very-low birth weight (vlbw) and small for gestational age reported significantly higher use of antidepressants, higher scores on the Beck Depression Inventory and more likely to report a depression diagnosis than control subjects and VLBW infants born appropriate for gestational age (Raikkonen et al., 2008). A prospective study of IUGR infants born preterm compared with age-match preterm appropriate for gestational age (AGA) and term AGA controls found decreased verbal and full-scale IQ scores; IUGR and preterm AGA infants had lower performance IQ than term control subjects (Morsing, Asard, Ley, Stjernqvist, & Marsal, 2011). Small body size at birth significantly correlates with increased symptoms of ADHD in children ages 5 to 6 years old (Lahti et al., 2006). Being small for gestational age and born preterm is significantly correlated with risk of psychiatric hospitalization for drug and alcohol dependency in adulthood (Nosarti et al., 2012). Placental pathology consistent with under-perfusion, a hallmark of IUGR, is associated with poorer cognitive, language and motor performance at 2 years of age in near-term small for gestational age infants (Parra-Saavedra et al., 2014).

Neuroimaging studies confirm the global and regional effects of IUGR on neuroanatomy seen in preclinical models. Globally, IUGR infants have reductions in absolute cortical gray matter volume and overall brain tissue volume, and lack of postnatal catch-up brain growth. The anatomic findings correlate with reductions in attention and interaction capacity at term, and less mature neurobehavioral development on the Assessment of Preterm Infant Behavior (Tolsa et al., 2004).

Several studies have found changes in regional brain size of IUGR infants. IUGR infants have reductions in frontal lobe and cerebellar volumes (Caetano et al., 2015) as well as reduction in hippocampal volume on MRI (Lodygensky et al., 2008). Fetuses with late-onset IUGR had deeper measurements of left and right insula and cingulate fissure as well as smaller intracranial, total brain, left opercula, and right opercula volumes (Egana-Ugrinovic, Sanz-Cortes, Figueras, Bargallo, & Gratacos, 2013). In another study, IUGR fetuses had reductions in brain volume, but no demonstrable delay in cortical maturation based on grading of sulci and gyri; instead they showed evidence of accelerated cortical development on these measures (Businelli, de Wit, Visser, & Pistorius, 2014). IUGR infants had texture changes in several brain regions including frontal cortex, cingulum, basal ganglia, midbrain and cerebellum, indicative of microstructural differences and possibly brain reorganization compared to appropriate for gestational age infants (Sanz-Cortes et al., 2013). IUGR altered markers of maturation in frontal white matter, thalami, centrum semiovale and pons associated with cognitive disorders in 36% of children at follow up (Arthurs et al., 2017). A tractography, a 3-D modeling of neural track using diffusion tensor imaging data, study showed that IUGR is associated with altered connectivity in motor and cortico-striatal-thalamic networks (Eixarch, Munoz-Moreno, Bargallo, Batalle, & Gratacos, 2016). A stereological study of post-mortem brain tissue of IUGR infants found severe reduction of total cell numbers in cortex, but not other developmental zones (Samuelsen et al., 2007). A brain network topology study showed evidence of brain reorganization in IUGR infants at one year of age (Batalle et al., 2012). IUGR infants had decreases in measures of global and local connectivity, which were associated with abnormal performance in later cognitive, language and motor outcomes at two years of age (Batalle et al., 2012). Structural MRI showed that 12 month old IUGR infants had gray matter reductions in temporal, parietal, frontal, and insular regions, increased white matter in temporal regions compared to the AGA group and in frontal, parietal, occipital, and insular regions compared to term group, and decreased white matter in the cerebellum compared to term infants (Padilla et al., 2011). Not surprisingly, functional consequences at the electrophysiologic and behavioral levels occur as a function of degree of IUGR. IUGR results in markers of delayed maturation of EEG, and associated delay in neuromotor development (Yerushalmy-Feler et al., 2014). Poorer intellectual performance at 7 years of age is associated when poor weight gain occurs postnatally in infants with IUGR (Pylipow et al., 2009), indicating that sensitive periods for brain growth and connectivity to nutrient availability extend beyond the fetal period.

Preclinical models and molecular mechanisms of IUGR

IUGR is not a single nutrient disorder; rather it represents the anthropometric end result of global fetal malnutrition. Placental transfer of nutrients is the final common pathway from the mother to the developing fetus and disruption of that process by any number of conditions results in the “unifinality” of retarded or restricted intrauterine growth. The failure to grow reflects primarily macronutrient (ie, protein, calorie) malnutrition although micronutrient deficits (eg, iron deficiency) are also prevalent and can restrict somatic and brain growth (Chockalingam, Murphy, Ophoven, Weisdorf, & Georgieff, 1987; Georgieff, MM, Gordon, & Wobken, 1995). The traditional assessment of growth restriction through anthropometric analysis of weight, length and head circumference for estimated gestational age is a blunt tool that does not have the precision to detect differential effects of the various etiologies of growth restriction on the developing brain.

Multiple preclinical models of IUGR have been developed because of the intense interest in Barker’s observation of “fetal programming” of adult cardiovascular disease (Barker, 1997; Gluckman & Hanson, 2004). Typically, rodent models have predominated because their short life-spans allow for assessment of adult outcomes following fetal nutrient restriction events. The two most commonly used models are maternal dietary restriction and uterine artery ligation. Maternal dietary restriction is usually accomplished through reducing total food consumption during the entire pregnancy, thus shortchanging the mother of macro- and micronutrients. This approach models severe malnutrition often presents in the “majority world.” Individual nutrients such as protein or energy (calories) are restricted in some experiments while maintain the rest of the diet in order to isolate the specific role of the nutrient in question. Uterine artery ligation in late gestation accomplishes a similar fetal nutrient restriction as the dietary models but does so without compromising maternal health status. It models the IUGR that occurs as a consequence of gestational hypertension in an otherwise nutritionally sufficient mother, seen primarily in “westernized” countries.

Pre-clinical models other than rodents, eg, sheep, have been used to assess the effects of IUGR predominantly on fetal and neonatal metabolism in order to provide information about insulin resistance and cardiovascular risk (Hay, Brown, Rozance, Wesolowski, & Limesand, 2016). However, sheep are limited in the information they can provide about brain development and risk of later psychopathology. Rodents are the preferred model for studying IUGR on brain development because of their robust behavioral repertoire and the analogies that can be drawn between their abnormalities and those of humans (Table 2).

Table 2.

Integrated approaches between pre-clinical models and humans that provide biological plausibility in assessment of non-fatal developmental disorders.

Brain Assessment Human Preclinical Model
Behavior Yes Yes (Fit to human)
Neuroanatomy Neuroimaging
Autopsy (rare)
Neuroimaging
Microscopy
Electrophysiology EEG, ERP invasive in vivo and ex vivo recording
Metabolism Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy (MRS) MRS
Direct biochemical assay, including metabolomics of tissue/CSF
Regulation Autopsy (rare)
CSF analysis (unusual)
Tissue/CSF gene expression and proteomics

All nutrients are needed for normal brain development. Since IUGR preclinical models typically restrict global nutrient delivery from mother to fetus, global effects are observed in the brain. Differential regional effects, where one brain area may be more affected than another, are largely related to relative metabolic rates (and thus nutrient demands) of the affected regions with those regions with higher metabolic demands at the time of the nutrient restriction being most affected. Regional metabolic demands are driven by growth rates, which in turn are driven by the age of the fetus. In humans, malnutrition early in pregnancy (or throughout pregnancy) results in global loss of brain volume and microcephaly (Winick, 1971). The microcephaly in humans is due to less cells and lower amounts of DNA and RNA (Rosso, Hormazabal, & Winick, 1970; Winick, 1971; Winick, Rosso, & Waterlow, 1970). First trimester viral infectious agents such as cytomegalovirus also restrict neurogenesis and result in microcephaly (Kagan & Hamprecht, 2017; Swanson & Schleiss, 2013). Migration defects are more prominent with earlier nutritional abnormalities.

In contrast, nutrient deficiencies that occur later in gestation have less global effects on neurogenesis, but more intense effects on neural differentiation in regions that are developing just prior to birth (eg, hippocampus). Selective disruption of regional development can contribute to unbalanced neural circuit function later in life.

An extensive meta-analysis of the literature on adult neurodevelopmental outcomes following uterine artery ligation in rats, mice and guinea pigs was recently reviewed by Basilious, Yager and Fehlings (Basilious, Yager, & Fehlings, 2015). Global effects were noted in all of the models specifically related to the degree of somatic growth restriction. Ventricular enlargement, indicative of reduced brain volume, was common across species (E. C. Mallard, Rehn, Rees, Tolcos, & Copolov, 1999; Rehn et al., 2004; Ruff et al., 2017). The loss of brain volume is due to compromise of both white and gray matter. Oligodendrocyte precursors are reduced throughout the brain resulting in a delay in onset of myelination (Olivier, Baud, Evrard, Gressens, & Verney, 2005; Reid et al., 2012) and an overall loss of myelination in adulthood (Ruff et al., 2017). Major white matter tracts are reduced in volume and density (Delcour, Olivier, et al., 2012; Delcour, Russier, et al., 2012; Ruff et al., 2017). The loss of white matter can be wide-spread including within the corpus callosum, brainstem, hippocampus and somatosensory cortex (Delcour, Olivier, et al., 2012). The profound effects on white matter are not surprising because IUGR compromises maternal-fetal oxygen transport and oligodendrocytes are highly sensitive to hypoxia.

Generalized gray matter volume loss in IUGR models occurs predominantly from global and regional loss of neurons through neuronal cell death and/or apoptosis (Delcour, Olivier, et al., 2012; C. Mallard, Loeliger, Copolov, & Rees, 2000). Beyond reduced neuronal numbers, dendrite branching patterns are less complex and contribute to the volume loss particularly in the hippocampus (Dieni & Rees, 2003). Neuronal function is clearly related to dendrite branching complexity; thus, the loss of complexity due to the substrate restriction of IUGR would be expected to compromise behaviors that are dependent on the affected structures. Neuronal loss and loss of morphological integrity is found in all hippocampal areas (C. Mallard et al., 2000; Ruff et al., 2017), the caudate nucleus (Rehn et al., 2004; Ruff et al., 2017), the cerebral cortex (E. C. Mallard et al., 1999), the medial and entorhinal cortex (Basilious et al., 2015) and the cerebellum (C. Mallard et al., 2000).

Beyond the structural changes, IUGR due to UA ligation also compromises signaling pathways that are critical for establishment and maintenance of synaptic plasticity. BDNF expression is reduced throughout the brain (Nishigori, Mazzuca, Nygard, Han, & Richardson, 2008) and specifically in the hippocampus (Dieni & Rees, 2005). The hippocampus is highly dependent on glutamatergic signaling, which is disrupted significantly by IUGR. NMDA receptor subunit expression is reduced overall and a delay in the maturational switch of subunit NR2a to NR2b occurs (Catteau et al., 2011; Schober et al., 2009). Neuroprotective pathways in the cortex and hippocampus are compromised as evidenced by an increase in the pro-apoptotic p53 protein and a reduction in the anti-apoptotic Bcl-2 protein (Ke et al., 2005; Lane et al., 2001).

Overall, preclinical models of late fetal intrauterine growth restriction due to placental insufficiency indicate widespread acute abnormalities, many of which carry forward into adulthood. The changes are found at the anatomic and signaling level and are particularly prominent in regions that are rapidly developing and demonstrate critical periods of development in the late fetal/early neonatal stages. UA ligation results in adult behavioral abnormalities that rely on the regions that were affected in the neonatal period. Specifically, cerebellar, hippocampal and cortical injury due to IUGR results in abnormalities of gait, memory, object recognition, and spatial processing in adulthood (Basilious et al., 2015).

In considering the epidemiologic data in humans linking IUGR to developmental psychopathologies that emerge later in life, it is important to note that all of the fetally affected regions are destined to be interconnected postnatally to establish functional circuits underlying far more complex behaviors. Thus, a multiplicative or ripple effect throughout the nervous system likely occurs through complex circuits (eg, the VTA loop or mesolimbic frontal connections), thus providing the anatomic framework for abnormalities in attentional regulation, ordered thinking, cognitive flexibility, sensory gating and set-shifting behaviors that have been noted in adult survivors of IUGR. For example, rats with UA-ligation induced IUGR have damage to hippocampal area CA1 and prefrontal cortex and demonstrate hyperactivity (Delcour, Olivier, et al., 2012; Delcour, Russier, et al., 2012) similar to the ADHD phenotype described in humans. Pre-pulse inhibition abnormalities have been noted in the guinea pig model of IUGR (Rehn et al., 2004). Impaired PPI has been utilized as a marker of the sensory gating abnormalities in schizophrenia (Rehn et al., 2004), the incidence of which is increased in adults following IUGR. The behavioral studies in the preclinical models provide biological plausibility for both the primary (eg, learning and memory deficits) as well as downstream complex behavioral abnormalities (ie, sensory gating) abnormalities noted in humans following IUGR.

Similar to the effects of iron deficiency, the role of timing in driving the acute and long-term abnormalities should be appreciated with IUGR. Late onset IUGR, as seen with the late gestation UA ligation model, induces severe, but relatively short-lived reductions in critical nutritional/metabolic substrates. This “one-hit” model produces a large “area under the curve” effect more from the large dose of the insult, as opposed to its duration (Kretchmer et al., 1996). An encephalopathic insult relegated to a brief epoch significantly damages only those regions with the greatest nutrient demand. Regions that have a more protracted critical period of development (and that show large amounts of plasticity) theoretically have an opportunity to recover, while those with relatively brief critical periods (and lesser plasticity) likely end up with permanent effects. Studying the effect of early versus late onset fetal UA ligation is technically challenging in the rodent due to excessive fetal demise in the early treatment group. However, the question has been studied in the fetal sheep and supports the principle that timing matters. Second trimester UA ligation in the sheep results in more widespread white matter injury and neuroinflammation in the cortex when compared to third trimester UA ligation (Alves de Alencar Rocha et al., 2017). The greater induction of inflammation during a period of extensive neuronal migration is particularly concerning, especially in light of recent theories that autism spectrum disorders may have a neuroinflammatory etiology.

In contrast to late-gestation UA ligation experiments, IUGR due to chronic maternal malnutrition throughout pregnancy results in an “area under the curve” characterized by a low dose insult over a long duration. While the magnitude of the dose x duration area under the curve may be similar to UA ligation, the shape is decidedly different, with many more periods of critical regional development at risk. Not surprisingly, the effects of IUGR due to maternal malnutrition are more global than UA ligation.

Macronutrient IUGR models restrict protein or protein + energy intake of the mother beginning early in the first trimester. The nutrient restriction is present throughout pregnancy and is frequently severe with the dams receiving as little as 50% of their normal intake. The effect of protein restriction at various stages of pregnancy has been described in rodent models since 1975 (Koshy, Sara, King, & Lazarus, 1975).

The global findings in these preclinical models corroborate the findings from autopsy studies in human infants including a reduction in brain size due primarily to loss of neuronal number (Rosso et al., 1970; Winick, 1971). The models suggest that maternal malnutrition affects the rate of brain development by causing premature cessation of neurogenesis and an earlier switch from proliferation to cell differentiation (Koshy et al., 1975). IUGR induced by a low protein diet given to the rat dam starting on day one of pregnancy reduces overall myelination and connectivity in the offspring. Significant deregulation of genes controlling neuroinflammation occurs, allowing for a more pro-inflammatory state in the developing brain (Rideau Batista Novais et al., 2016). The authors suggest that neuroinflammation due to IUGR may provide a biological explanation for poor cognitive outcome in IUGR humans (Rideau Batista Novais et al., 2016).

As with UA ligation, regional brain effects are also seen with protein and protein-energy restriction. The hypothalamus is reprogrammed with respect to satiety and hunger cues, which may relate to appetite regulation issues and metabolic syndrome seen in adulthood following IUGR (Pedroso et al., 2017). IUGR also lowers cerebral weight, results in fewer neurons through increased apoptosis accompanied by reduction in PKA-CREB signaling pathway activity (J. Liu, Liu, & Chen, 2011; J. Liu, Liu, Wang, Chen, & Yang, 2013). Synaptic counts are reduced and glial cell proliferation is suppressed in the cortex (J. Liu et al., 2011). IUGR increases oxidative stress and results in lipid peroxidation in the cerebellum and the cerebral cortex (Tatli et al., 2007).

Behaviorally, the long-term effects are particularly magnified if malnutrition continues postnatally after IUGR. Rat pups born to dams on a low protein diet and maintained on that diet to adulthood have poor spatial learning and memory. Male offspring have more hyperactivity than females (Naik, Patro, & Patro, 2015). IUGR followed by postnatal growth restriction increases anxiety in males and females in young adulthood, but without social or cognitive impairment. Nevertheless, both males and females exhibit increased biochemical evidence of Alzheimer’s Disease at 15–17 months with more astrogliosis and a tenfold increase in amyloid-beta42 protein expression (Tomi, Zhao, Thamotharan, Shin, & Devaskar, 2013).

Studies of both IUGR human cohorts and animal models have provided evidence for epigenetic programming of the epigenome (Heijmans et al., 2009; Joss-Moore, Albertine, & Lane, 2011; R. A. Simmons, 2007; Tobi et al., 2009). DNA methylation was examined at the regulatory regions of 15 genes associated with cardiovascular and metabolic disease in adults who were exposed to famine (members of the Dutch famine cohort) during gestation. Gestational famine significantly altered the DNA methylation at 6 of these 15 gene regulatory loci, indicating that gene expression might be altered in these individuals, even in adulthood, long after the period of gestational famine (Heijmans et al., 2009; Tobi et al., 2009). Both modifications of DNA and histones have been demonstrated in IUGR animal models, particularly in genes that have important roles in brain development and brain health across the lifespan (Joss-Moore et al., 2011; R. A. Simmons, 2007).

Concluding remarks

Clinically, psychopathologies are thought of as discrete entities (as found in the DSM-5 or ICD-10) with little overlap, and their diagnosis points towards specific treatment protocols (Caspi et al., 2014). Practically, there can be difficulty even with experienced clinicians in distinguishing between presentations of different psychiatric illnesses, as diagnosis often entirely relies on presenting signs and symptoms, without regard to the underlying neurobiology (Cuthbert & Insel, 2013). This is especially true in children, where comorbidity of different psychopathologies is more common than in adults (Angold, Costello, & Erkanli, 1999). This is especially notable in that genes identified as risk factors (via GWAS in this reference) in multiple psychopathologies have remarkable overlap (Consortium, 2013) and meta analyses of imaging studies have found neural substrates common for both individual psychiatric entities (an affective subdivision of the corticostriatal-pallidal-thalamic circuit in depression (Hamilton et al., 2012)), and common to multiple psychiatric diagnoses, i.e., transdiagnostically across different psychopathologies (gray matter loss in the dorsal anterior cingulate, right insula, and left insula (Goodkind et al., 2015)). These imaging studies further point towards neural substrates supporting specific behaviors (increased salience of negative information in major depressive disorder; a transdiagnostic association with poorer executive functioning). A fundamental question with respect to psychopathology resulting from events of commission or omission in the intrauterine environment is whether these are purely environmental influences or if they represent an interaction between gene and environment, which could be mistaken for either genetic traits or environmental insults. For example, evidence from twin studies demonstrate a large correlation between internalizing and externalizing factors, with a strong genetic influence (Cosgrove et al., 2011; Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman, & Rathouz, 2011). Internalizing behaviors are considered “overcontrolled” behaviors characterized by anxiety and depression, whereas externalizing behaviors are considered “undercontrolled” and characterized by aggression and hyperactivity (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1984). In this same study, psychiatric disorder-specific causes were mostly attributed to nonshared environmental influences. These results are consistent with a “generalist gene, specialist environments model” of psychopathology (Eley, 1997). Internalizing and externalizing behaviors, while distinguishable, may reflect a common neurobiological substrate that results in negative emotionality, a transdiagnostic construct that may explain genetic or gene X environment influences on behavior (Rhee, Lahey, & Waldman, 2015). Many of the conditions we reviewed here result in changes in internalizing and externalizing behaviors, or in the case of prenatal alcohol exposure, Iron deficiency and IUGR, executive functioning. Executive functions include updating/working memory, inhibition and shifting/selective attention and may encompass internalizing and externalizing behaviors (Alvarez & Emory, 2006; Miyake & Friedman, 2012). Executive functioning impairments are associated with most forms of psychopathology, and may be a common underlying factor accounting for lifespan functional impairment, family history of mental illness, prospective psychopathology, worse developmental histories, and more compromised early-life brain function (Caspi et al., 2014; McTeague, Goodkind, & Etkin, 2016; Snyder, Miyake, & Hankin, 2015). Notably, a recent study showed that in depression, antidepressant treatment did not result in improvements in executive functioning, suggesting that common treatments are ineffective in affecting this domain (Shilyansky et al., 2016). The conditions we have reviewed here may provide insight into neurobiological substrates that underlie transdiagnostic entities such as negative emotionality or executive functioning, which in turn, may point towards discovery of “generalist” genes (affected by epigenetic mechanisms) and brain structures for targeted therapeutics.

In this article, we have presented information that provides biological plausibility to support the hypothesis that common fetal events affect brain development and function across the lifespan through a variety of mechanisms. While the literature has moved over the decades from describing the global effects of these events to documenting more regionally specific effects, it still tends to focus on “one event-one brain region” types of analyses. This approach stems from classic neuroscience research thinking in relatively static adult brains where accidents of nature in humans are used to decipher what a given brain area “does” (eg, a stroke in the motor cortex leads to hemiplegia). The same rules do not appear to apply to the developing fetal and neonatal brain because of the incompleteness of its circuitry at the time of the environmental event and the enormous plasticity that the young brain exhibits. Thus, development itself becomes a mediating factor in the ultimate behavioral outcome.

A greater understanding of how affected (and non-affected) regions interact at the circuit level is critical to truly understand the effect of prenatal events on complex behaviors. Given the commonality of circuits affected (by global metabolic deficits) we no longer expect to find a “signature” effect on the developing brain based on etiology, but rather based on timing, caused by unbalanced effects on circuit inputs. Thus, different events that have a common timing (eg, last trimester) might result in unifinality, whereas a single type of environmental disruptor can lead to multiple outcomes (multi-finality) based on the timing of that single disruptor (eg, last trimester vs toddlerhood for iron).

Table 1.

Importance of timing of nutrient deficiency on long-term brain developmental outcomes.

Factor Age Risk for Deficiency Critical Period Affected brain Regions and (neural processes) Long-term Impact after Resolution of Nutrient Deficit
Nutrient Protein Fetus Maternal malnutrition; Maternal hypertension 3rd trimester Cortex, Eye, Hippocampus (Proliferation, differentiation, synaptogenesis, growth factors expression, myelination) Lower IQ at 7 years of age (Pylipow et al., 2009)
Child Variable based on growth rates 6 months –10 years Impaired cognition (Pollitt, Gorman, Engle, Martorell, & Rivera, 1993)
Iron Fetus Maternal anemia; Maternal diabetes; Smoking during pregnancy 3rd trimester White matter, Striatal-Frontal connectivity, Hippocampal-Frontal connectivity (Myelination, dopamine synthesis, neurometabolism, migration, differentiation) Lower recognition and working memory (Riggins, Miller, Bauer, Georgieff, & Nelson, 2009)
Infant & Toddler Rapid growth, poor dietary sources, intestinal blood loss; intestinal parasites 6 months –24 months Increased hesitancy and wariness; increased depression; motor abnormalities; slower processing (Lukowski et al., 2010; Shafir et al., 2008)
Zinc Fetus & Toddler Maternal deficiency Pregnancy –24 months Decreased novelty preference as toddler (Adamo & Oteiza, 2010), decrease working memory function (Fuglestad, Kroupina, Johnson, & Georgieff, 2016).
Teratogens Alcohol Fetus Alcohol consumption during pregnancy Pregnancy –12 months

Acknowledgments

Supported in part by grants from the National Institutes of Health to MKG (HD029421; AA024123), PVT (NS099178) and ESC (K08-MH104281)

Contributor Information

Michael K. Georgieff, Professor of Pediatrics and Child Psychology, University of Minnesota School of Medicine and College of Education and Human Development, Minneapolis, MN.

Phu V. Tran, Assistant Professor of Pediatrics, University of Minnesota School of Medicine, Minneapolis, MN.

Erik S. Carlson, Assistant Professor of Psychiatry, University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle, WA.

References

  1. Abel KM, Wicks S, Susser ES, Dalman C, Pedersen MG, Mortensen PB, Webb RT. Birth weight, schizophrenia, and adult mental disorder: is risk confined to the smallest babies? Archives of general psychiatry. 2010;67(9):923–930. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS. Psychopathology of childhood. Annual review of psychology. 1984;35:227–256. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.35.020184.001303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Adamo AM, Oteiza PI. Zinc deficiency and neurodevelopment: the case of neurons. BioFactors (Oxford, England) 2010;36(2):117–124. doi: 10.1002/biof.91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Algarin C, Karunakaran KD, Reyes S, Morales C, Lozoff B, Peirano P, Biswal B. Differences on Brain Connectivity in Adulthood Are Present in Subjects with Iron Deficiency Anemia in Infancy. Frontiers in aging neuroscience. 2017;9:54. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Algarin C, Nelson CA, Peirano P, Westerlund A, Reyes S, Lozoff B. Iron-deficiency anemia in infancy and poorer cognitive inhibitory control at age 10 years. Developmental medicine and child neurology. 2013;55(5):453–458. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Allen-Institute. Allen Human Brain Atlas. 2010 human.brain-map.org.
  7. Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology review. 2006;16(1):17–42. doi: 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Alves de Alencar Rocha AK, Allison BJ, Yawno T, Polglase GR, Sutherland AE, Malhotra A, … Miller SL. Early- versus Late-Onset Fetal Growth Restriction Differentially Affects the Development of the Fetal Sheep Brain. Developmental neuroscience. 2017;39(1–4):141–155. doi: 10.1159/000456542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Amin SB, Orlando M, Eddins A, MacDonald M, Monczynski C, Wang H. In utero iron status and auditory neural maturation in premature infants as evaluated by auditory brainstem response. The Journal of pediatrics. 2010;156(3):377–381. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.09.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Anderson OS, Nahar MS, Faulk C, Jones TR, Liao C, Kannan K, … Dolinoy DC. Epigenetic responses following maternal dietary exposure to physiologically relevant levels of bisphenol A. Environmental and molecular mutagenesis. 2012;53(5):334–342. doi: 10.1002/em.21692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Angold A, Costello EJ, Erkanli A. Comorbidity. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 1999;40(1):57–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Arthurs OJ, Rega A, Guimiot F, Belarbi N, Rosenblatt J, Biran V, … Alison M. Diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging of the fetal brain in intrauterine growth restriction. Ultrasound in obstetrics & gynecology: the official journal of the International Society of Ultrasound in Obstetrics and Gynecology. 2017;50(1):79–87. doi: 10.1002/uog.17318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Autti-Ramo I, Autti T, Korkman M, Kettunen S, Salonen O, Valanne L. MRI findings in children with school problems who had been exposed prenatally to alcohol. Developmental medicine and child neurology. 2002;44(2):98–106. doi: 10.1017/s0012162201001748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Ba A, Seri BV, Aka KJ, Glin L, Tako A. Comparative effects of developmental thiamine deficiencies and ethanol exposure on the morphometry of the CA3 pyramidal cells. Neurotoxicology and teratology. 1999;21(5):579–586. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(99)00014-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bale TL, Baram TZ, Brown AS, Goldstein JM, Insel TR, McCarthy MM, … Nestler EJ. Early life programming and neurodevelopmental disorders. Biological psychiatry. 2010;68(4):314–319. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.05.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Barker DJ. Fetal nutrition and cardiovascular disease in later life. British medical bulletin. 1997;53(1):96–108. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a011609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Barrett A, Santangelo S, Tan K, Catchpole S, Roberts K, Spencer-Dene B, … Taylor-Papadimitriou J. Breast cancer associated transcriptional repressor PLU-1/JARID1B interacts directly with histone deacetylases. International journal of cancer. 2007;121(2):265–275. doi: 10.1002/ijc.22673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Barua S, Kuizon S, Chadman KK, Flory MJ, Brown WT, Junaid MA. Single-base resolution of mouse offspring brain methylome reveals epigenome modifications caused by gestational folic acid. Epigenetics & chromatin. 2014;7(1):3. doi: 10.1186/1756-8935-7-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Basavarajappa BS, Subbanna S. Epigenetic Mechanisms in Developmental Alcohol-Induced Neurobehavioral Deficits. Brain sciences. 2016;6(2) doi: 10.3390/brainsci6020012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Basilious A, Yager J, Fehlings MG. Neurological outcomes of animal models of uterine artery ligation and relevance to human intrauterine growth restriction: a systematic review. Developmental medicine and child neurology. 2015;57(5):420–430. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bastian TW, von Hohenberg WC, Mickelson DJ, Lanier LM, Georgieff MK. Iron Deficiency Impairs Developing Hippocampal Neuron Gene Expression, Energy Metabolism, and Dendrite Complexity. Developmental neuroscience. 2016;38(4):264–276. doi: 10.1159/000448514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Batalle D, Eixarch E, Figueras F, Munoz-Moreno E, Bargallo N, Illa M, … Gratacos E. Altered small-world topology of structural brain networks in infants with intrauterine growth restriction and its association with later neurodevelopmental outcome. NeuroImage. 2012;60(2):1352–1366. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Beard J, Erikson KM, Jones BC. Neonatal iron deficiency results in irreversible changes in dopamine function in rats. The Journal of nutrition. 2003;133(4):1174–1179. doi: 10.1093/jn/133.4.1174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Beard JL. Iron biology in immune function, muscle metabolism and neuronal functioning. The Journal of nutrition. 2001;131(2s-2):568S–579S. doi: 10.1093/jn/131.2.568S. discussion 580S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Beard JL, Wiesinger JA, Connor JR. Pre- and postweaning iron deficiency alters myelination in Sprague-Dawley rats. Developmental neuroscience. 2003;25(5):308–315. doi: 10.1159/000073507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Belujon P, Grace AA. Critical role of the prefrontal cortex in the regulation of hippocampus-accumbens information flow. The Journal of neuroscience. 2008;28(39):9797–9805. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2200-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Berman RF, Hannigan JH. Effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on the hippocampus: spatial behavior, electrophysiology, and neuroanatomy. Hippocampus. 2000;10(1):94–110. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-1063(2000)10:1&#x0003c;94::aid-hipo11&#x0003e;3.0.co;2-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Beyer S, Kristensen MM, Jensen KS, Johansen JV, Staller P. The histone demethylases JMJD1A and JMJD2B are transcriptional targets of hypoxia-inducible factor HIF. The Journal of biological chemistry. 2008;283(52):36542–36552. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M804578200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Blegen MB, Kennedy BC, Thibert KA, Gewirtz JC, Tran PV, Georgieff MK. Multigenerational effects of fetal-neonatal iron deficiency on hippocampal BDNF signaling. Physiological reports. 2013;1(5):e00096. doi: 10.1002/phy2.96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Bookstein FL, Connor PD, Covell KD, Barr HM, Gleason CA, Sze RW, … Streissguth AP. Preliminary evidence that prenatal alcohol damage may be visible in averaged ultrasound images of the neonatal human corpus callosum. Alcohol (Fayetteville, NY) 2005;36(3):151–160. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2005.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Bookstein FL, Connor PD, Huggins JE, Barr HM, Pimentel KD, Streissguth AP. Many infants prenatally exposed to high levels of alcohol show one particular anomaly of the corpus callosum. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2007;31(5):868–879. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00367.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Bookstein FL, Sampson PD, Streissguth AP, Connor PD. Geometric morphometrics of corpus callosum and subcortical structures in the fetal-alcohol-affected brain. Teratology. 2001;64(1):4–32. doi: 10.1002/tera.1044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Braun JM, Hauser R. Bisphenol A and children’s health. Current opinion in pediatrics. 2011;23(2):233–239. doi: 10.1097/MOP.0b013e3283445675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Braun JM, Yolton K, Dietrich KN, Hornung R, Ye X, Calafat AM, Lanphear BP. Prenatal bisphenol A exposure and early childhood behavior. Environmental health perspectives. 2009;117(12):1945–1952. doi: 10.1289/ehp.0900979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Brunette KE, Tran PV, Wobken JD, Carlson ES, Georgieff MK. Gestational and neonatal iron deficiency alters apical dendrite structure of CA1 pyramidal neurons in adult rat hippocampus. Developmental neuroscience. 2010a;32(3):238–248. doi: 10.1159/000314341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Brunette KE, Tran PV, Wobken JD, Carlson ES, Georgieff MK. Gestational and neonatal iron deficiency alters apical dendrite structure of CA1 pyramidal neurons in adult rat hippocampus. Dev Neurosci. 2010b;32(3):238–248. doi: 10.1159/000314341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Businelli C, de Wit C, Visser GH, Pistorius LR. Ultrasound evaluation of cortical brain development in fetuses with intrauterine growth restriction. The journal of maternal-fetal & neonatal medicine. 2014:1–6. doi: 10.3109/14767058.2014.953474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Caetano AC, Zamarian AC, Araujo E, Junior, Cavalcante RO, Simioni C, Silva CP, … Nardozza LM. Assessment of Intracranial Structure Volumes in Fetuses With Growth Restriction by 3-Dimensional Sonography Using the Extended Imaging Virtual Organ Computer-Aided Analysis Method. Journal of ultrasound in medicine. 2015;34(8):1397–1405. doi: 10.7863/ultra.34.8.1397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Callahan LS, Thibert KA, Wobken JD, Georgieff MK. Early-life iron deficiency anemia alters the development and long-term expression of parvalbumin and perineuronal nets in the rat hippocampus. Developmental neuroscience. 2013;35(5):427–436. doi: 10.1159/000354178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Cannon M, Jones PB, Murray RM. Obstetric complications and schizophrenia: historical and meta-analytic review. The American journal of psychiatry. 2002;159(7):1080–1092. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Carlson ES, Fretham SJ, Unger E, O’Connor M, Petryk A, Schallert T, … Georgieff MK. Hippocampus specific iron deficiency alters competition and cooperation between developing memory systems. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders. 2010;2(3):133–143. doi: 10.1007/s11689-010-9049-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Carlson ES, Stead JD, Neal CR, Petryk A, Georgieff MK. Perinatal iron deficiency results in altered developmental expression of genes mediating energy metabolism and neuronal morphogenesis in hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2007;17(8):679–691. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Carlson ES, Tkac I, Magid R, O’Connor MB, Andrews NC, Schallert T, … Petryk A. Iron is essential for neuron development and memory function in mouse hippocampus. The Journal of nutrition. 2009;139(4):672–679. doi: 10.3945/jn.108.096354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Carter RC, Jacobson JL, Burden MJ, Armony-Sivan R, Dodge NC, Angelilli ML, … Jacobson SW. Iron deficiency anemia and cognitive function in infancy. Pediatrics. 2010;126(2):e427–434. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-2097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Casas M, Forns J, Martinez D, Avella-Garcia C, Valvi D, Ballesteros-Gomez A, … Vrijheid M. Exposure to bisphenol A during pregnancy and child neuropsychological development in the INMA-Sabadell cohort. Environmental research. 2015;142:671–679. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2015.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Goldman-Mellor SJ, Harrington H, Israel S, … Moffitt TE. The p Factor: One General Psychopathology Factor in the Structure of Psychiatric Disorders? Clinical psychological science: a journal of the Association for Psychological Science. 2014;2(2):119–137. doi: 10.1177/2167702613497473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Catteau J, Gernet JI, Marret S, Legros H, Gressens P, Leroux P, Laudenbach V. Effects of antenatal uteroplacental hypoperfusion on neonatal microvascularisation and excitotoxin sensitivity in mice. Pediatric research. 2011;70(3):229–235. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e318224285f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Chang B, Chen Y, Zhao Y, Bruick RK. JMJD6 is a histone arginine demethylase. Science (New York, NY) 2007;318(5849):444–447. doi: 10.1126/science.1145801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Chater-Diehl EJ, Laufer BI, Singh SM. Changes to histone modifications following prenatal alcohol exposure: An emerging picture. Alcohol (Fayetteville, NY) 2017;60:41–52. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2017.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Chicas A, Kapoor A, Wang X, Aksoy O, Evertts AG, Zhang MQ, … Lowe SW. H3K4 demethylation by Jarid1a and Jarid1b contributes to retinoblastoma-mediated gene silencing during cellular senescence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109(23):8971–8976. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1119836109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Cho CE, Sanchez-Hernandez D, Reza-Lopez SA, Huot PS, Kim YI, Anderson GH. High folate gestational and post-weaning diets alter hypothalamic feeding pathways by DNA methylation in Wistar rat offspring. Epigenetics. 2013;8(7):710–719. doi: 10.4161/epi.24948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Chockalingam UM, Murphy E, Ophoven JC, Weisdorf SA, Georgieff MK. Cord transferrin and ferritin values in newborn infants at risk for prenatal uteroplacental insufficiency and chronic hypoxia. The Journal of pediatrics. 1987;111(2):283–286. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(87)80088-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Christian P, Morgan ME, Murray-Kolb L, LeClerq SC, Khatry SK, Schaefer B, … Tielsch JM. Preschool iron-folic acid and zinc supplementation in children exposed to iron-folic acid in utero confers no added cognitive benefit in early school-age. The Journal of nutrition. 2011;141(11):2042–2048. doi: 10.3945/jn.111.146480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Christian P, Murray-Kolb LE, Khatry SK, Katz J, Schaefer BA, Cole PM, … Tielsch JM. Prenatal micronutrient supplementation and intellectual and motor function in early school-aged children in Nepal. Jama. 2010;304(24):2716–2723. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.1861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Clardy SL, Wang X, Zhao W, Liu W, Chase GA, Beard JL, … Connor JR. Acute and chronic effects of developmental iron deficiency on mRNA expression patterns in the brain. Journal of neural transmission. Supplementum. 2006;(71):173–196. doi: 10.1007/978-3-211-33328-0_19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Congdon EL, Westerlund A, Algarin CR, Peirano PD, Gregas M, Lozoff B, Nelson CA. Iron deficiency in infancy is associated with altered neural correlates of recognition memory at 10 years. The Journal of pediatrics. 2012;160(6):1027–1033. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2011.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Connor JR, Menzies SL. Relationship of iron to oligodendrocytes and myelination. Glia. 1996;17(2):83–93. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1136(199606)17:2&#x0003c;83::AID-GLIA1&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Consortium, Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics. Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet (London, England) 2013;381(9875):1371–1379. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(12)62129-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Cosgrove VE, Rhee SH, Gelhorn HL, Boeldt D, Corley RC, Ehringer MA, … Hewitt JK. Structure and etiology of co-occurring internalizing and externalizing disorders in adolescents. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 2011;39(1):109–123. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9444-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Cuthbert BN, Insel TR. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC medicine. 2013;11:126. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-11-126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. DeBoer T, Wewerka S, Bauer PJ, Georgieff MK, Nelson CA. Explicit memory performance in infants of diabetic mothers at 1 year of age. Developmental medicine and child neurology. 2005;47(8):525–531. doi: 10.1017/s0012162205001039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Delcour M, Olivier P, Chambon C, Pansiot J, Russier M, Liberge M, … Coq JO. Neuroanatomical, sensorimotor and cognitive deficits in adult rats with white matter injury following prenatal ischemia. Brain pathology (Zurich, Switzerland) 2012;22(1):1–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2011.00504.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Delcour M, Russier M, Amin M, Baud O, Paban V, Barbe MF, Coq JO. Impact of prenatal ischemia on behavior, cognitive abilities and neuroanatomy in adult rats with white matter damage. Behavioural brain research. 2012;232(1):233–244. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.03.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Deregnier RA, Nelson CA, Thomas KM, Wewerka S, Georgieff MK. Neurophysiologic evaluation of auditory recognition memory in healthy newborn infants and infants of diabetic mothers. The Journal of pediatrics. 2000;137(6):777–784. doi: 10.1067/mpd.2000.109149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Dieni S, Rees S. Dendritic morphology is altered in hippocampal neurons following prenatal compromise. Journal of neurobiology. 2003;55(1):41–52. doi: 10.1002/neu.10194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Dieni S, Rees S. BDNF and TrkB protein expression is altered in the fetal hippocampus but not cerebellum after chronic prenatal compromise. Experimental neurology. 2005;192(2):265–273. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2004.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Dionne G, Boivin M, Seguin JR, Perusse D, Tremblay RE. Gestational diabetes hinders language development in offspring. Pediatrics. 2008;122(5):e1073–1079. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-3028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Dolinoy DC, Huang D, Jirtle RL. Maternal nutrient supplementation counteracts bisphenol A-induced DNA hypomethylation in early development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007;104(32):13056–13061. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703739104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Donald KA, Eastman E, Howells FM, Adnams C, Riley EP, Woods RP, … Stein DJ. Neuroimaging effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on the developing human brain: a magnetic resonance imaging review. Acta neuropsychiatrica. 2015;27(5):251–269. doi: 10.1017/neu.2015.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Donald KA, Roos A, Fouche JP, Koen N, Howells FM, Woods RP, … Stein DJ. A study of the effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on white matter microstructural integrity at birth. Acta neuropsychiatrica. 2015;27(4):197–205. doi: 10.1017/neu.2015.35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. East P, Lozoff B, Blanco E, Delker E, Delva J, Encina P, Gahagan S. Infant iron deficiency, child affect, and maternal unresponsiveness: Testing the long-term effects of functional isolation. Developmental psychology. 2017;53(12):2233–2244. doi: 10.1037/dev0000385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Egana-Ugrinovic G, Sanz-Cortes M, Figueras F, Bargallo N, Gratacos E. Differences in cortical development assessed by fetal MRI in late-onset intrauterine growth restriction. American journal of obstetrics and gynecology. 2013;209(2):126e121–128. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2013.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Ehrenkranz RA. Growth outcomes of very low-birth weight infants in the newborn intensive care unit. Clinics in perinatology. 2000;27(2):325–345. doi: 10.1016/s0095-5108(05)70024-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Eide MG, Moster D, Irgens LM, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Stoltenberg C, Skjaerven R, … Abel K. Degree of fetal growth restriction associated with schizophrenia risk in a national cohort. Psychological medicine. 2013;43(10):2057–2066. doi: 10.1017/s003329171200267x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Eixarch E, Munoz-Moreno E, Bargallo N, Batalle D, Gratacos E. Motor and cortico-striatal-thalamic connectivity alterations in intrauterine growth restriction. American journal of obstetrics and gynecology. 2016;214(6):725e721–729. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2015.12.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Eley TC. General genes: a new theme in developmental psychopathology. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1997;6:90–95. [Google Scholar]
  77. Elsworth JD, Jentsch JD, Vandevoort CA, Roth RH, Redmond DE, Jr, Leranth C. Prenatal exposure to bisphenol A impacts midbrain dopamine neurons and hippocampal spine synapses in non-human primates. Neurotoxicology. 2013;35:113–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2013.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Erikson KM, Jones BC, Beard JL. Iron deficiency alters dopamine transporter functioning in rat striatum. The Journal of nutrition. 2000;130(11):2831–2837. doi: 10.1093/jn/130.11.2831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Esmaeili B, Grace AA. Afferent drive of medial prefrontal cortex by hippocampus and amygdala is altered in MAM-treated rats: evidence for interneuron dysfunction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38(10):1871–1880. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Evans SF, Kobrosly RW, Barrett ES, Thurston SW, Calafat AM, Weiss B, … Swan SH. Prenatal bisphenol A exposure and maternally reported behavior in boys and girls. Neurotoxicology. 2014;45:91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2014.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Famy C, Streissguth AP, Unis AS. Mental illness in adults with fetal alcohol syndrome or fetal alcohol effects. The American journal of psychiatry. 1998;155(4):552–554. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.4.552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Fan J, Jacobson SW, Taylor PA, Molteno CD, Dodge NC, Stanton ME, … Meintjes EM. White matter deficits mediate effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on cognitive development in childhood. Human brain mapping. 2016;37(8):2943–2958. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Felt BT, Beard JL, Schallert T, Shao J, Aldridge JW, Connor JR, … Lozoff B. Persistent neurochemical and behavioral abnormalities in adulthood despite early iron supplementation for perinatal iron deficiency anemia in rats. Behavioural brain research. 2006;171(2):261–270. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Felt BT, Lozoff B. Brain iron and behavior of rats are not normalized by treatment of iron deficiency anemia during early development. The Journal of nutrition. 1996;126(3):693–701. doi: 10.1093/jn/126.3.693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Fretham SJ, Carlson ES, Georgieff MK. The role of iron in learning and memory. Advances in nutrition (Bethesda, Md ) 2011;2(2):112–121. doi: 10.3945/an.110.000190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Fretham SJ, Carlson ES, Wobken J, Tran PV, Petryk A, Georgieff MK. Temporal manipulation of transferrin-receptor-1-dependent iron uptake identifies a sensitive period in mouse hippocampal neurodevelopment. Hippocampus. 2012;22(8):1691–1702. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Friel JK, Aziz K, Andrews WL, Harding SV, Courage ML, Adams RJ. A double-masked, randomized control trial of iron supplementation in early infancy in healthy term breast-fed infants. The Journal of pediatrics. 2003;143(5):582–586. doi: 10.1067/s0022-3476(03)00301-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Fryer SL, Schweinsburg BC, Bjorkquist OA, Frank LR, Mattson SN, Spadoni AD, Riley EP. Characterization of white matter microstructure in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2009;33(3):514–521. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00864.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Fuglestad AJ, Kroupina MG, Johnson DE, Georgieff MK. Micronutrient status and neurodevelopment in internationally adopted children. Acta paediatrica (Oslo, Norway: 1992) 2016;105(2):e67–76. doi: 10.1111/apa.13234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Gambling L, Kennedy C, McArdle HJ. Iron and copper in fetal development. Seminars in cell & developmental biology. 2011;22(6):637–644. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2011.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Gangisetty O, Bekdash R, Maglakelidze G, Sarkar DK. Fetal alcohol exposure alters proopiomelanocortin gene expression and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function via increasing MeCP2 expression in the hypothalamus. PloS one. 2014;9(11):e113228. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Gely-Pernot A, Hao C, Becker E, Stuparevic I, Kervarrec C, Chalmel F, … Smagulova F. The epigenetic processes of meiosis in male mice are broadly affected by the widely used herbicide atrazine. BMC genomics. 2015;16:885. doi: 10.1186/s12864-015-2095-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Geng F, Mai X, Zhan J, Xu L, Zhao Z, Georgieff M, … Lozoff B. Impact of Fetal-Neonatal Iron Deficiency on Recognition Memory at 2 Months of Age. The Journal of pediatrics. 2015;167(6):1226–1232. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.08.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Georgieff MK, Brunette KE, Tran PV. Early life nutrition and neural plasticity. Development and psychopathology. 2015;27(2):411–423. doi: 10.1017/s0954579415000061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Georgieff MK, Ills MMM, Gordon K, Wobken JD. Reduced neonatal liver iron concentrations after uteroplacental insufficiency. The Journal of pediatrics. 1995;127(2):308–304. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(95)70317-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Gioiosa L, Parmigiani S, Vom Saal FS, Palanza P. The effects of bisphenol A on emotional behavior depend upon the timing of exposure, age and gender in mice. Hormones and behavior. 2013;63(4):598–605. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Gluckman PD, Hanson MA. Living with the past: evolution, development, and patterns of disease. Science (New York, NY) 2004;305(5691):1733–1736. doi: 10.1126/science.1095292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Golub MS. Recent studies of iron deficiency during brain development in nonhuman primates. BioFactors (Oxford, England) 2010;36(2):111–116. doi: 10.1002/biof.86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Goodkind M, Eickhoff SB, Oathes DJ, Jiang Y, Chang A, Jones-Hagata LB, … Etkin A. Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. JAMA psychiatry. 2015;72(4):305–315. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Goodman JR, Warshaw JB, Dallman PR. Cardiac hypertrophy in rats with iron and copper deficiency: quantitative contribution of mitochondrial enlargement. Pediatric research. 1970;4(3):244–256. doi: 10.1203/00006450-197005000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Grissom NM, Reyes TM. Gestational overgrowth and undergrowth affect neurodevelopment: similarities and differences from behavior to epigenetics. International journal of developmental neuroscience. 2013;31(6):406–414. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2012.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Gunnar MR, Quevedo KM. Early care experiences and HPA axis regulation in children: a mechanism for later trauma vulnerability. Progress in brain research. 2008;167:137–149. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(07)67010-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Hablitz JJ. Prenatal exposure to alcohol alters short-term plasticity in hippocampus. Experimental neurology. 1986;93(2):423–427. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(86)90203-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Hamilton JP, Etkin A, Furman DJ, Lemus MG, Johnson RF, Gotlib IH. Functional neuroimaging of major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis and new integration of base line activation and neural response data. The American journal of psychiatry. 2012;169(7):693–703. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11071105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Hao C, Gely-Pernot A, Kervarrec C, Boudjema M, Becker E, Khil P, … Smagulova F. Exposure to the widely used herbicide atrazine results in deregulation of global tissue-specific RNA transcription in the third generation and is associated with a global decrease of histone trimethylation in mice. Nucleic acids research. 2016;44(20):9784–9802. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkw840. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Hay WW, Jr, Brown LD, Rozance PJ, Wesolowski SR, Limesand SW. Challenges in nourishing the intrauterine growth-restricted foetus - Lessons learned from studies in the intrauterine growth-restricted foetal sheep. Acta paediatrica (Oslo, Norway: 1992) 2016;105(8):881–889. doi: 10.1111/apa.13413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Hebb D. The organization of behavior. New York, NY: Wiley; 1949. [Google Scholar]
  108. Heijmans BT, Tobi EW, Lumey LH, Slagboom PE. The epigenome: archive of the prenatal environment. Epigenetics. 2009;4(8):526–531. doi: 10.4161/epi.4.8.10265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Helfrich KK, Saini N, Kling PJ, Smith SM. Maternal Iron Nutriture as a Critical Modulator of FASD Risk in Alcohol-Exposed Pregnancies. Biochemistry and cell biology. 2017 doi: 10.1139/bcb-2017-0206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Hensch TK. Critical period regulation. Annual review of neuroscience. 2004;27:549–579. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Hicks SD, Middleton FA, Miller MW. Ethanol-induced methylation of cell cycle genes in neural stem cells. Journal of neurochemistry. 2010;114(6):1767–1780. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06886.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Horner PJ, Palmer TD. New roles for astrocytes: the nightlife of an ‘astrocyte’. La vida loca! Trends in neurosciences. 2003;26(11):597–603. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2003.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Huebner SM, Blohowiak SE, Kling PJ, Smith SM. Prenatal Alcohol Exposure Alters Fetal Iron Distribution and Elevates Hepatic Hepcidin in a Rat Model of Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders. The Journal of nutrition. 2016;146(6):1180–1188. doi: 10.3945/jn.115.227983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Hultman CM, Sparen P, Cnattingius S. Perinatal risk factors for infantile autism. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass ) 2002;13(4):417–423. doi: 10.1097/00001648-200207000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Ikezuki Y, Tsutsumi O, Takai Y, Kamei Y, Taketani Y. Determination of bisphenol A concentrations in human biological fluids reveals significant early prenatal exposure. Human reproduction (Oxford, England) 2002;17(11):2839–2841. doi: 10.1093/humrep/17.11.2839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Insel BJ, Schaefer CA, McKeague IW, Susser ES, Brown AS. Maternal iron deficiency and the risk of schizophrenia in offspring. Archives of general psychiatry. 2008;65(10):1136–1144. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.10.1136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Itoh K, Yaoi T, Fushiki S. Bisphenol A, an endocrine-disrupting chemical, and brain development. Neuropathology. 2012;32(4):447–457. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1789.2011.01287.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Jabes A, Thomas KM, Langworthy S, Georgieff MK, Nelson CA. Functional and Anatomic Consequences of Diabetic Pregnancy on Memory in Ten-Year-Old Children. Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics: JDBP. 2015;36(7):529–535. doi: 10.1097/dbp.0000000000000203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Johnson DE, Guthrie D, Smyke AT, Koga SF, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA., 3rd Growth and associations between auxology, caregiving environment, and cognition in socially deprived Romanian children randomized to foster vs ongoing institutional care. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2010;164(6):507–516. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Johnson DE, Miller LC, Iverson S, Thomas W, Franchino B, Dole K, … Hostetter MK. The health of children adopted from Romania. Jama. 1992;268(24):3446–3451. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Jorgenson LA, Sun M, O’Connor M, Georgieff MK. Fetal iron deficiency disrupts the maturation of synaptic function and efficacy in area CA1 of the developing rat hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2005;15(8):1094–1102. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Jorgenson LA, Wobken JD, Georgieff MK. Perinatal iron deficiency alters apical dendritic growth in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Developmental neuroscience. 2003;25(6):412–420. doi: 10.1159/000075667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Joss-Moore LA, Albertine KH, Lane RH. Epigenetics and the developmental origins of lung disease. Molecular genetics and metabolism. 2011;104(1–2):61–66. doi: 10.1016/j.ymgme.2011.07.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Kaelin WG, Jr, Ratcliffe PJ. Oxygen sensing by metazoans: the central role of the HIF hydroxylase pathway. Molecular cell. 2008;30(4):393–402. doi: 10.1016/j.molcel.2008.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Kagan KO, Hamprecht K. Cytomegalovirus infection in pregnancy. Archives of gynecology and obstetrics. 2017;296(1):15–26. doi: 10.1007/s00404-017-4380-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Ke X, McKnight RA, Wang ZM, Yu X, Wang L, Callaway CW, … Lane RH. Nonresponsiveness of cerebral p53-MDM2 functional circuit in newborn rat pups rendered IUGR via uteroplacental insufficiency. American journal of physiology: Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology. 2005;288(4):R1038–1045. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00701.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Ke X, Schober ME, McKnight RA, O’Grady S, Caprau D, Yu X, … Lane RH. Intrauterine growth retardation affects expression and epigenetic characteristics of the rat hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor gene. Physiological genomics. 2010;42(2):177–189. doi: 10.1152/physiolgenomics.00201.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Ke X, Xing B, Yu B, Yu X, Majnik A, Cohen S, … Joss-Moore L. IUGR disrupts the PPARgamma-Setd8-H4K20me(1) and Wnt signaling pathways in the juvenile rat hippocampus. International journal of developmental neuroscience. 2014;38:59–67. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Kertes DA, Gunnar MR, Madsen NJ, Long JD. Early deprivation and home basal cortisol levels: a study of internationally adopted children. Development and psychopathology. 2008;20(2):473–491. doi: 10.1017/s0954579408000230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Kim P, Park JH, Choi CS, Choi I, Joo SH, Kim MK, … Shin CY. Effects of ethanol exposure during early pregnancy in hyperactive, inattentive and impulsive behaviors and MeCP2 expression in rodent offspring. Neurochemical research. 2013;38(3):620–631. doi: 10.1007/s11064-012-0960-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Kimura KA, Reynolds JN, Brien JF. Ethanol neurobehavioral teratogenesis and the role of the hippocampal glutamate-N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor-nitric oxide synthase system. Neurotoxicology and teratology. 2000;22(5):607–616. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(00)00089-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Kingdon D, Cardoso C, McGrath JJ. Research Review: Executive function deficits in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder - a meta-analysis. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2016;57(2):116–131. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Kleiber ML, Mantha K, Stringer RL, Singh SM. Neurodevelopmental alcohol exposure elicits long-term changes to gene expression that alter distinct molecular pathways dependent on timing of exposure. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders. 2013;5(1):6. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-5-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Klintsova AY, Helfer JL, Calizo LH, Dong WK, Goodlett CR, Greenough WT. Persistent impairment of hippocampal neurogenesis in young adult rats following early postnatal alcohol exposure. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2007;31(12):2073–2082. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00528.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Kolk SM, Gunput RA, Tran TS, van den Heuvel DM, Prasad AA, Hellemons AJ, … Pasterkamp RJ. Semaphorin 3F is a bifunctional guidance cue for dopaminergic axons and controls their fasciculation, channeling, rostral growth, and intracortical targeting. The Journal of neuroscience. 2009;29(40):12542–12557. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2521-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Koshy TS, Sara VR, King TL, Lazarus L. The influence of protein restriction imposed at various stages of pregnancy on fetal and placental development. Growth. 1975;39(4):497–506. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Kouzarides T. Chromatin modifications and their function. Cell. 2007;128(4):693–705. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.02.005. S0092-8674(07)00184-5 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Krahl SE, Berman RF, Hannigan JH. Electrophysiology of hippocampal CA1 neurons after prenatal ethanol exposure. Alcohol (Fayetteville, NY ) 1999;17(2):125–131. doi: 10.1016/s0741-8329(98)00043-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Krakowiak P, Walker CK, Bremer AA, Baker AS, Ozonoff S, Hansen RL, Hertz-Picciotto I. Maternal metabolic conditions and risk for autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders. Pediatrics. 2012;129(5):e1121–1128. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Kretchmer N, Beard JL, Carlson S. The role of nutrition in the development of normal cognition. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 1996;63(6):997s–1001s. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/63.6.997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Kuzawa CW. Adipose tissue in human infancy and childhood: an evolutionary perspective. American journal of physical anthropology, Suppl. 1998;27:177–209. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-8644(1998)107:27+<177::aid-ajpa7>3.0.co;2-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Lahey BB, Van Hulle CA, Singh AL, Waldman ID, Rathouz PJ. Higher-order genetic and environmental structure of prevalent forms of child and adolescent psychopathology. Archives of general psychiatry. 2011;68(2):181–189. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Lahti J, Raikkonen K, Kajantie E, Heinonen K, Pesonen AK, Jarvenpaa AL, Strandberg T. Small body size at birth and behavioural symptoms of ADHD in children aged five to six years. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2006;47(11):1167–1174. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01661.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Lane RH, Ramirez RJ, Tsirka AE, Kloesz JL, McLaughlin MK, Gruetzmacher EM, Devaskar SU. Uteroplacental insufficiency lowers the threshold towards hypoxia-induced cerebral apoptosis in growth-retarded fetal rats. Brain research. 2001;895(1–2):186–193. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02074-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Lange S, Rehm J, Anagnostou E, Popova S. Prevalence of Externalizing Disorders and Autism Spectrum Disorder among Children with Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder: Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Biochemistry and cell biology. 2017 doi: 10.1139/bcb-2017-0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Laufer BI, Mantha K, Kleiber ML, Diehl EJ, Addison SM, Singh SM. Long-lasting alterations to DNA methylation and ncRNAs could underlie the effects of fetal alcohol exposure in mice. Disease models & mechanisms. 2013;6(4):977–992. doi: 10.1242/dmm.010975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Lebel C, Rasmussen C, Wyper K, Andrew G, Beaulieu C. Brain microstructure is related to math ability in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2010;34(2):354–363. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01097.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Lebel C, Rasmussen C, Wyper K, Walker L, Andrew G, Yager J, Beaulieu C. Brain diffusion abnormalities in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2008;32(10):1732–1740. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00750.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Lee HY, Choi K, Oh H, Park YK, Park H. HIF-1-dependent induction of Jumonji domain-containing protein (JMJD) 3 under hypoxic conditions. Molecules and cells. 2014;37(1):43–50. doi: 10.14348/molcells.2014.2250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Leonard H, de Klerk N, Bourke J, Bower C. Maternal health in pregnancy and intellectual disability in the offspring: a population-based study. Annals of epidemiology. 2006;16(6):448–454. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2005.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Lim YH, Bae S, Kim BN, Shin CH, Lee YA, Kim JI, Hong YC. Prenatal and postnatal bisphenol A exposure and social impairment in 4-year-old children. Environmental health: a global access science source. 2017;16(1):79. doi: 10.1186/s12940-017-0289-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Lisman JE, Coyle JT, Green RW, Javitt DC, Benes FM, Heckers S, Grace AA. Circuit-based framework for understanding neurotransmitter and risk gene interactions in schizophrenia. Trends in neurosciences. 2008;31(5):234–242. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Liu J, Liu L, Chen H. Antenatal taurine supplementation for improving brain ultrastructure in fetal rats with intrauterine growth restriction. Neuroscience. 2011;181:265–270. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.02.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Liu J, Liu Y, Wang XF, Chen H, Yang N. Antenatal taurine supplementation improves cerebral neurogenesis in fetal rats with intrauterine growth restriction through the PKA-CREB signal pathway. Nutritional neuroscience. 2013;16(6):282–287. doi: 10.1179/1476830513y.0000000057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Liu Y, Balaraman Y, Wang G, Nephew KP, Zhou FC. Alcohol exposure alters DNA methylation profiles in mouse embryos at early neurulation. Epigenetics. 2009;4(7):500–511. doi: 10.4161/epi.4.7.9925. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Livy DJ, Miller EK, Maier SE, West JR. Fetal alcohol exposure and temporal vulnerability: effects of binge-like alcohol exposure on the developing rat hippocampus. Neurotoxicology and teratology. 2003;25(4):447–458. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(03)00030-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Liyanage VR, Zachariah RM, Davie JR, Rastegar M. Ethanol deregulates Mecp2/MeCP2 in differentiating neural stem cells via interplay between 5-methylcytosine and 5-hydroxymethylcytosine at the Mecp2 regulatory elements. Experimental neurology. 2015;265:102–117. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2015.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Lodygensky GA, Seghier ML, Warfield SK, Tolsa CB, Sizonenko S, Lazeyras F, Huppi PS. Intrauterine growth restriction affects the preterm infant’s hippocampus. Pediatric research. 2008;63(4):438–443. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e318165c005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Lozoff B, Beard J, Connor J, Barbara F, Georgieff M, Schallert T. Long-lasting neural and behavioral effects of iron deficiency in infancy. Nutrition reviews. 2006;64(5 Pt 2):S34–43. doi: 10.1301/nr.2006.may.S34-S43. discussion S72–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Lozoff B, Clark KM, Jing Y, Armony-Sivan R, Angelilli ML, Jacobson SW. Dose-response relationships between iron deficiency with or without anemia and infant social-emotional behavior. The Journal of pediatrics. 2008;152(5):696–702. 702.631–693. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.09.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Lozoff B, De Andraca I, Castillo M, Smith JB, Walter T, Pino P. Behavioral and developmental effects of preventing iron-deficiency anemia in healthy full-term infants. Pediatrics. 2003;112(4):846–854. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Lozoff B, Georgieff MK. Iron deficiency and brain development. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology. 2006;13(3):158–165. doi: 10.1016/j.spen.2006.08.004. S1071-9091(06)00103-3[pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Hagen J, Mollen E, Wolf AW. Poorer behavioral and developmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron deficiency in infancy. Pediatrics. 2000;105(4):E51. doi: 10.1542/peds.105.4.e51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Lozoff B, Smith JB, Kaciroti N, Clark KM, Guevara S, Jimenez E. Functional significance of early-life iron deficiency: outcomes at 25 years. The Journal of pediatrics. 2013;163(5):1260–1266. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.05.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Lubin FD, Roth TL, Sweatt JD. Epigenetic regulation of BDNF gene transcription in the consolidation of fear memory. The Journal of neuroscience. 2008;28(42):10576–10586. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1786-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Lukowski AF, Koss M, Burden MJ, Jonides J, Nelson CA, Kaciroti N, … Lozoff B. Iron deficiency in infancy and neurocognitive functioning at 19 years: evidence of long-term deficits in executive function and recognition memory. Nutritional neuroscience. 2010;13(2):54–70. doi: 10.1179/147683010x12611460763689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Macht VA, Kelly SJ, Gass JT. Sex-specific effects of developmental alcohol exposure on cocaine-induced place preference in adulthood. Behavioural brain research. 2017;332:259–268. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Maier SE, Miller JA, Blackwell JM, West JR. Fetal alcohol exposure and temporal vulnerability: regional differences in cell loss as a function of the timing of binge-like alcohol exposure during brain development. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 1999;23(4):726–734. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1999.tb04176.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Malisza KL, Buss JL, Bolster RB, de Gervai PD, Woods-Frohlich L, Summers R, … Longstaffe S. Comparison of spatial working memory in children with prenatal alcohol exposure and those diagnosed with ADHD; A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders. 2012;4(1):12. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-4-12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Mallard C, Loeliger M, Copolov D, Rees S. Reduced number of neurons in the hippocampus and the cerebellum in the postnatal guinea-pig following intrauterine growth-restriction. Neuroscience. 2000;100(2):327–333. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00271-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Mallard EC, Rehn A, Rees S, Tolcos M, Copolov D. Ventriculomegaly and reduced hippocampal volume following intrauterine growth-restriction: implications for the aetiology of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia research. 1999;40(1):11–21. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00041-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. McBirney M, King SE, Pappalardo M, Houser E, Unkefer M, Nilsson E, … Skinner MK. Atrazine induced epigenetic transgenerational inheritance of disease, lean phenotype and sperm epimutation pathology biomarkers. PloS one. 2017;12(9):e0184306. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0184306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. McTeague LM, Goodkind MS, Etkin A. Transdiagnostic impairment of cognitive control in mental illness. Journal of psychiatric research. 2016;83:37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Menezes J, Alves N, Borges S, Roehrs R, de Carvalho Myskiw J, Furini CR, … Mello-Carpes PB. Facilitation of fear extinction by novelty depends on dopamine acting on D1-subtype dopamine receptors in hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2015;112(13):E1652–1658. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1502295112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Miller BS, Kroupina MG, Mason P, Iverson SL, Narad C, Himes JH, … Petryk A. Determinants of catch-up growth in international adoptees from eastern europe. International journal of pediatric endocrinology. 2010;2010:107252. doi: 10.1155/2010/107252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Miller MW. Generation of neurons in the rat dentate gyrus and hippocampus: effects of prenatal and postnatal treatment with ethanol. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 1995;19(6):1500–1509. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01014.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Miodovnik A, Engel SM, Zhu C, Ye X, Soorya LV, Silva MJ, … Wolff MS. Endocrine disruptors and childhood social impairment. Neurotoxicology. 2011;32(2):261–267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2010.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Miyake A, Friedman NP. The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(1):8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Moffatt ME, Longstaffe S, Besant J, Dureski C. Prevention of iron deficiency and psychomotor decline in high-risk infants through use of iron-fortified infant formula: a randomized clinical trial. The Journal of pediatrics. 1994;125(4):527–534. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(94)70003-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Monk C, Georgieff MK, Xu D, Hao X, Bansal R, Gustafsson H, … Peterson BS. Maternal prenatal iron status and tissue organization in the neonatal brain. Pediatric research. 2016;79(3):482–488. doi: 10.1038/pr.2015.248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Monroe SM, Simons AD. Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: implications for the depressive disorders. Psychological bulletin. 1991;110(3):406–425. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Morsing E, Asard M, Ley D, Stjernqvist K, Marsal K. Cognitive function after intrauterine growth restriction and very preterm birth. Pediatrics. 2011;127(4):e874–882. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Mudie S, Bandarra D, Batie M, Biddlestone J, Moniz S, Ortmann B, … Rocha S. PITX1, a specificity determinant in the HIF-1alpha-mediated transcriptional response to hypoxia. Cell cycle (Georgetown, Tex ) 2014;13(24):3878–3891. doi: 10.4161/15384101.2014.972889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Murray-Kolb LE, Beard JL. Iron treatment normalizes cognitive functioning in young women. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 2007;85(3):778–787. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/85.3.778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Naik AA, Patro IK, Patro N. Slow Physical Growth, Delayed Reflex Ontogeny, and Permanent Behavioral as Well as Cognitive Impairments in Rats Following Intra-generational Protein Malnutrition. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2015;9:446. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Nandal A, Ruiz JC, Subramanian P, Ghimire-Rijal S, Sinnamon RA, Stemmler TL, … Philpott CC. Activation of the HIF prolyl hydroxylase by the iron chaperones PCBP1 and PCBP2. Cell metabolism. 2011;14(5):647–657. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2011.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Nelson CA, Wewerka SS, Borscheid AJ, Deregnier RA, Georgieff MK. Electrophysiologic evidence of impaired cross-modal recognition memory in 8-month-old infants of diabetic mothers. The Journal of pediatrics. 2003;142(5):575–582. doi: 10.1067/mpd.2003.210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Nelson CA, Wewerka S, Thomas KM, Tribby-Walbridge S, deRegnier R, Georgieff M. Neurocognitive sequelae of infants of diabetic mothers. Behavioral neuroscience. 2000;114(5):950–956. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. NIAAA. 7th Special report to the US Congress on alcohol and health: DHHS Publication ADM 90-1656. Washington DC: US Government; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  190. Nishigori H, Mazzuca DM, Nygard KL, Han VK, Richardson BS. BDNF and TrkB in the preterm and near-term ovine fetal brain and the effect of intermittent umbilical cord occlusion. Reproductive sciences (Thousand Oaks, Calif ) 2008;15(9):895–905. doi: 10.1177/1933719108324135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Niu X, Zhang T, Liao L, Zhou L, Lindner DJ, Zhou M, … Yang H. The von Hippel-Lindau tumor suppressor protein regulates gene expression and tumor growth through histone demethylase JARID1C. Oncogene. 2012;31(6):776–786. doi: 10.1038/onc.2011.266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Nosarti C, Reichenberg A, Murray RM, Cnattingius S, Lambe MP, Yin L, … Hultman CM. Preterm birth and psychiatric disorders in young adult life. Archives of general psychiatry. 2012;69(6):E1–8. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Olivier P, Baud O, Evrard P, Gressens P, Verney C. Prenatal ischemia and white matter damage in rats. Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology. 2005;64(11):998–1006. doi: 10.1097/01.jnen.0000187052.81889.57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Padilla N, Falcon C, Sanz-Cortes M, Figueras F, Bargallo N, Crispi F, … Gratacos E. Differential effects of intrauterine growth restriction on brain structure and development in preterm infants: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain research. 2011;1382:98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.01.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Parnell SE, Holloway HT, O’Leary-Moore SK, Dehart DB, Paniaqua B, Oguz I, … Sulik KK. Magnetic resonance microscopy-based analyses of the neuroanatomical effects of gestational day 9 ethanol exposure in mice. Neurotoxicology and teratology. 2013;39:77–83. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2013.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Parra-Saavedra M, Crovetto F, Triunfo S, Savchev S, Peguero A, Nadal A, … Figueras F. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of near-term small-for-gestational-age infants with and without signs of placental underperfusion. Placenta. 2014;35(4):269–274. doi: 10.1016/j.placenta.2014.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. Patton SM, Coe CL, Lubach GR, Connor JR. Quantitative proteomic analyses of cerebrospinal fluid using iTRAQ in a primate model of iron deficiency anemia. Developmental neuroscience. 2012;34(4):354–365. doi: 10.1159/000341919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Pedersen MT, Helin K. Histone demethylases in development and disease. Trends in cell biology. 2010;20(11):662–671. doi: 10.1016/j.tcb.2010.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. Pedroso AP, Souza AP, Dornellas AP, Oyama LM, Nascimento CM, Santos GM, … Ribeiro EB. Intrauterine Growth Restriction Programs the Hypothalamus of Adult Male Rats: Integrated Analysis of Proteomic and Metabolomic Data. Journal of proteome research. 2017;16(4):1515–1525. doi: 10.1021/acs.jproteome.6b00923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Perera F, Vishnevetsky J, Herbstman JB, Calafat AM, Xiong W, Rauh V, Wang S. Prenatal bisphenol a exposure and child behavior in an inner-city cohort. Environmental health perspectives. 2012;120(8):1190–1194. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1104492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. Perkins A, Lehmann C, Lawrence RC, Kelly SJ. Alcohol exposure during development: Impact on the epigenome. International journal of developmental neuroscience. 2013;31(6):391–397. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2013.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Philippat C, Nakiwala D, Calafat AM, Botton J, De Agostini M, Heude B, Slama R. Prenatal Exposure to Nonpersistent Endocrine Disruptors and Behavior in Boys at 3 and 5 Years. Environmental health perspectives. 2017;125(9):097014. doi: 10.1289/ehp1314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Pinero D, Jones B, Beard J. Variations in dietary iron alter behavior in developing rats. The Journal of nutrition. 2001;131(2):311–318. doi: 10.1093/jn/131.2.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  204. Pisansky MT, Wickham RJ, Su J, Fretham S, Yuan LL, Sun M, … Georgieff MK. Iron deficiency with or without anemia impairs prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex. Hippocampus. 2013;23(10):952–962. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  205. Poimenova A, Markaki E, Rahiotis C, Kitraki E. Corticosterone-regulated actions in the rat brain are affected by perinatal exposure to low dose of bisphenol A. Neuroscience. 2010;167(3):741–749. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.02.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  206. Pollard PJ, Loenarz C, Mole DR, McDonough MA, Gleadle JM, Schofield CJ, Ratcliffe PJ. Regulation of Jumonji-domain-containing histone demethylases by hypoxia-inducible factor (HIF)-1alpha. The Biochemical journal. 2008;416(3):387–394. doi: 10.1042/bj20081238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  207. Pollitt E, Gorman KS, Engle PL, Martorell R, Rivera J. Early supplementary feeding and cognition: effects over two decades. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1993;58(7):1–99. discussion 111–118. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  208. Pongcharoen T, DiGirolamo AM, Ramakrishnan U, Winichagoon P, Flores R, Martorell R. Long-term effects of iron and zinc supplementation during infancy on cognitive function at 9 y of age in northeast Thai children: a follow-up study. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 2011;93(3):636–643. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.002220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  209. Pongcharoen T, Ramakrishnan U, DiGirolamo AM, Winichagoon P, Flores R, Singkhornard J, Martorell R. Influence of prenatal and postnatal growth on intellectual functioning in school-aged children. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2012;166(5):411–416. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.1413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  210. Ponnaluri VK, Maciejewski JP, Mukherji M. A mechanistic overview of TET-mediated 5-methylcytosine oxidation. Biochemical and biophysical research communications. 2013;436(2):115–120. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2013.05.077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  211. Ponnusamy R, Nissim HA, Barad M. Systemic blockade of D2-like dopamine receptors facilitates extinction of conditioned fear in mice. Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, NY) 2005;12(4):399–406. doi: 10.1101/lm.96605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  212. Popolo M, McCarthy DM, Bhide PG. Influence of dopamine on precursor cell proliferation and differentiation in the embryonic mouse telencephalon. Developmental neuroscience. 2004;26(2–4):229–244. doi: 10.1159/000082140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  213. Portales-Casamar E, Lussier AA, Jones MJ, MacIsaac JL, Edgar RD, Mah SM, … Kobor MS. DNA methylation signature of human fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Epigenetics & chromatin. 2016;9:25. doi: 10.1186/s13072-016-0074-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  214. Pylipow M, Spector LG, Puumala SE, Boys C, Cohen J, Georgieff MK. Early postnatal weight gain, intellectual performance, and body mass index at 7 years of age in term infants with intrauterine growth restriction. The Journal of pediatrics. 2009;154(2):201–206. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  215. Raikkonen K, Pesonen AK, Heinonen K, Kajantie E, Hovi P, Jarvenpaa AL, … Andersson S. Depression in young adults with very low birth weight: the Helsinki study of very low-birth-weight adults. Archives of general psychiatry. 2008;65(3):290–296. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  216. Ramadoss S, Chen X, Wang CY. Histone demethylase KDM6B promotes epithelial-mesenchymal transition. The Journal of biological chemistry. 2012;287(53):44508–44517. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M112.424903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  217. Rao R, Georgieff MK. Perinatal aspects of iron metabolism. Acta paediatrica (Oslo, Norway: 1992) Supplement. 2002;91(438):124–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2002.tb02917.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  218. Rao R, Tkac I, Schmidt AT, Georgieff MK. Fetal and neonatal iron deficiency causes volume loss and alters the neurochemical profile of the adult rat hippocampus. Nutritional neuroscience. 2011;14(2):59–65. doi: 10.1179/1476830511y.0000000001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  219. Rao R, Tkac I, Townsend EL, Gruetter R, Georgieff MK. Perinatal iron deficiency alters the neurochemical profile of the developing rat hippocampus. The Journal of nutrition. 2003;133(10):3215–3221. doi: 10.1093/jn/133.10.3215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  220. Rehn AE, Van Den Buuse M, Copolov D, Briscoe T, Lambert G, Rees S. An animal model of chronic placental insufficiency: relevance to neurodevelopmental disorders including schizophrenia. Neuroscience. 2004;129(2):381–391. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.07.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  221. Reid MV, Murray KA, Marsh ED, Golden JA, Simmons RA, Grinspan JB. Delayed myelination in an intrauterine growth retardation model is mediated by oxidative stress upregulating bone morphogenetic protein 4. Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology. 2012;71(7):640–653. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e31825cfa81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  222. Reynolds JD, Brien JF. Ethanol neurobehavioural teratogenesis and the role of L- glutamate in the fetal hippocampus. Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology. 1995;73(9):1209–1223. doi: 10.1139/y95-173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  223. Rhee SH, Lahey BB, Waldman ID. Comorbidity Among Dimensions of Childhood Psychopathology: Converging Evidence from Behavior Genetics. Child development perspectives. 2015;9(1):26–31. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  224. Rideau Batista Novais A, Pham H, Van de Looij Y, Bernal M, Mairesse J, Zana-Taieb E, … Baud O. Transcriptomic regulations in oligodendroglial and microglial cells related to brain damage following fetal growth restriction. Glia. 2016;64(12):2306–2320. doi: 10.1002/glia.23079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  225. Riggins T, Miller NC, Bauer PJ, Georgieff MK, Nelson CA. Consequences of low neonatal iron status due to maternal diabetes mellitus on explicit memory performance in childhood. Developmental neuropsychology. 2009;34(6):762–779. doi: 10.1080/87565640903265145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  226. Riley EP, Mattson SN, Sowell ER, Jernigan TL, Sobel DF, Jones KL. Abnormalities of the corpus callosum in children prenatally exposed to alcohol. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 1995;19(5):1198–1202. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01600.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  227. Rizzo TA, Metzger BE, Dooley SL, Cho NH. Early malnutrition and child neurobehavioral development: insights from the study of children of diabetic mothers. Child development. 1997;68(1):26–38. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  228. Roncagliolo M, Garrido M, Walter T, Peirano P, Lozoff B. Evidence of altered central nervous system development in infants with iron deficiency anemia at 6 mo: delayed maturation of auditory brainstem responses. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 1998;68(3):683–690. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/68.3.683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  229. Rosso P, Hormazabal J, Winick M. Changes in brain weight, cholesterol, phospholipid, and DNA content in marasmic children. The American journal of clinical nutrition. 1970;23(10):1275–1279. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/23.10.1275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  230. Roussotte FF, Sulik KK, Mattson SN, Riley EP, Jones KL, Adnams CM, … Sowell ER. Regional brain volume reductions relate to facial dysmorphology and neurocognitive function in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Human brain mapping. 2012;33(4):920–937. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  231. Rufer ES, Tran TD, Attridge MM, Andrzejewski ME, Flentke GR, Smith SM. Adequacy of maternal iron status protects against behavioral, neuroanatomical, and growth deficits in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. PloS one. 2012;7(10):e47499. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  232. Ruff CA, Faulkner SD, Rumajogee P, Beldick S, Foltz W, Corrigan J, … Fehlings MG. The extent of intrauterine growth restriction determines the severity of cerebral injury and neurobehavioural deficits in rodents. PloS one. 2017;12(9):e0184653. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0184653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  233. Samuelsen GB, Pakkenberg B, Bogdanovic N, Gundersen HJ, Larsen JF, Graem N, Laursen H. Severe cell reduction in the future brain cortex in human growth-restricted fetuses and infants. American journal of obstetrics and gynecology. 2007;197(1):56e51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2007.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  234. Sanchez-Alvarez R, Gayen S, Vadigepalli R, Anni H. Ethanol diverts early neuronal differentiation trajectory of embryonic stem cells by disrupting the balance of lineage specifiers. PloS one. 2013;8(5):e63794. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  235. Santhanam P, Coles CD, Li Z, Li L, Lynch ME, Hu X. Default mode network dysfunction in adults with prenatal alcohol exposure. Psychiatry research. 2011;194(3):354–362. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  236. Sanz-Cortes M, Figueras F, Bonet-Carne E, Padilla N, Tenorio V, Bargallo N, … Gratacos E. Fetal brain MRI texture analysis identifies different microstructural patterns in adequate and small for gestational age fetuses at term. Fetal diagnosis and therapy. 2013;33(2):122–129. doi: 10.1159/000346566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  237. Schmidt AT, Alvarez GC, Grove WM, Rao R, Georgieff MK. Early iron deficiency enhances stimulus-response learning of adult rats in the context of competing spatial information. Developmental cognitive neuroscience. 2012;2(1):174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.07.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  238. Schmidt AT, Waldow KJ, Grove WM, Salinas JA, Georgieff MK. Dissociating the long-term effects of fetal/neonatal iron deficiency on three types of learning in the rat. Behavioral neuroscience. 2007;121(3):475–482. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.3.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  239. Schmidt RJ, Tancredi DJ, Krakowiak P, Hansen RL, Ozonoff S. Maternal intake of supplemental iron and risk of autism spectrum disorder. American journal of epidemiology. 2014;180(9):890–900. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  240. Schober ME, McKnight RA, Yu X, Callaway CW, Ke X, Lane RH. Intrauterine growth restriction due to uteroplacental insufficiency decreased white matter and altered NMDAR subunit composition in juvenile rat hippocampi. American journal of physiology: Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology. 2009;296(3):R681–692. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.90396.2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  241. Schubert D, Martens GJ, Kolk SM. Molecular underpinnings of prefrontal cortex development in rodents provide insights into the etiology of neurodevelopmental disorders. Molecular psychiatry. 2015;20(7):795–809. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  242. Shafir T, Angulo-Barroso R, Calatroni A, Jimenez E, Lozoff B. Effects of iron deficiency in infancy on patterns of motor development over time. Human movement science. 2006;25(6):821–838. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2006.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  243. Shafir T, Angulo-Barroso R, Jing Y, Angelilli ML, Jacobson SW, Lozoff B. Iron deficiency and infant motor development. Early human development. 2008;84(7):479–485. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  244. Sherriff A, Emond A, Bell JC, Golding J. Should infants be screened for anaemia? A prospective study investigating the relation between haemoglobin at 8, 12, and 18 months and development at 18 months. Archives of disease in childhood. 2001;84(6):480–485. doi: 10.1136/adc.84.6.480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  245. Shilyansky C, Williams LM, Gyurak A, Harris A, Usherwood T, Etkin A. Effect of antidepressant treatment on cognitive impairments associated with depression: a randomised longitudinal study. The lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(5):425–435. doi: 10.1016/s2215-0366(16)00012-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  246. Siddappa AM, Georgieff MK, Wewerka S, Worwa C, Nelson CA, Deregnier RA. Iron deficiency alters auditory recognition memory in newborn infants of diabetic mothers. Pediatric research. 2004;55(6):1034–1041. doi: 10.1203/01.pdr.0000127021.38207.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  247. Siddappa AM, Rao R, Long JD, Widness JA, Georgieff MK. The assessment of newborn iron stores at birth: a review of the literature and standards for ferritin concentrations. Neonatology. 2007;92(2):73–82. doi: 10.1159/000100805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  248. Simmons R. Developmental origins of adult metabolic disease: concepts and controversies. Trends in endocrinology and metabolism: TEM. 2005;16(8):390–394. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2005.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  249. Simmons RA. Developmental origins of diabetes: the role of epigenetic mechanisms. Current opinion in endocrinology, diabetes, and obesity. 2007;14(1):13–16. doi: 10.1097/MED.0b013e328013da5b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  250. Slavney PR, Grau JG. Fetal alcohol damage and schizophrenia. The Journal of clinical psychiatry. 1978;39(10):782–783. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  251. Snyder HR, Miyake A, Hankin BL. Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Frontiers in psychology. 2015;6:328. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  252. Sowell ER, Mattson SN, Thompson PM, Jernigan TL, Riley EP, Toga AW. Mapping callosal morphology and cognitive correlates: effects of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Neurology. 2001;57(2):235–244. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.2.235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  253. Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Mattson SN, Tessner KD, Jernigan TL, Riley EP, Toga AW. Voxel-based morphometric analyses of the brain in children and adolescents prenatally exposed to alcohol. Neuroreport. 2001;12(3):515–523. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200103050-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  254. Spottiswoode BS, Meintjes EM, Anderson AW, Molteno CD, Stanton ME, Dodge NC, … Jacobson SW. Diffusion tensor imaging of the cerebellum and eyeblink conditioning in fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2011;35(12):2174–2183. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01566.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  255. Steinhausen HC, Willms J, Spohr HL. Long-term psychopathological and cognitive outcome of children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1993;32(5):990–994. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199309000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  256. Streissguth AP, Aase JM, Clarren SK, Randels SP, LaDue RA, Smith DF. Fetal alcohol syndrome in adolescents and adults. Jama. 1991;265(15):1961–1967. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  257. Suh SW, Aoyama K, Matsumori Y, Liu J, Swanson RA. Pyruvate administered after severe hypoglycemia reduces neuronal death and cognitive impairment. Diabetes. 2005;54(5):1452–1458. doi: 10.2337/diabetes.54.5.1452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  258. Susser E, Neugebauer R, Hoek HW, Brown AS, Lin S, Labovitz D, Gorman JM. Schizophrenia after prenatal famine. Further evidence. Archives of general psychiatry. 1996;53(1):25–31. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830010027005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  259. Susser ES, Lin SP. Schizophrenia after prenatal exposure to the Dutch Hunger Winter of 1944–1945. Archives of general psychiatry. 1992;49(12):983–988. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820120071010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  260. Swanson EC, Schleiss MR. Congenital cytomegalovirus infection: new prospects for prevention and therapy. Pediatric clinics of North America. 2013;60(2):335–349. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2012.12.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  261. Swartzwelder HS, Farr KL, Wilson WA, Savage DD. Prenatal exposure to ethanol decreases physiological plasticity in the hippocampus of the adult rat. Alcohol (Fayetteville, NY ) 1988;5(2):121–124. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(88)90008-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  262. Swayze VW, 2nd, Johnson VP, Hanson JW, Piven J, Sato Y, Giedd JN, … Andreasen NC. Magnetic resonance imaging of brain anomalies in fetal alcohol syndrome. Pediatrics. 1997;99(2):232–240. doi: 10.1542/peds.99.2.232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  263. Tan SE, Berman RF, Abel EL, Zajac CS. Prenatal alcohol exposure alters hippocampal slice electrophysiology. Alcohol (Fayetteville, NY ) 1990;7(6):507–511. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(90)90040-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  264. Tatli M, Guzel A, Kizil G, Kavak V, Yavuz M, Kizil M. Comparison of the effects of maternal protein malnutrition and intrauterine growth restriction on redox state of central nervous system in offspring rats. Brain research. 2007;1156:21–30. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.04.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  265. Taylor PA, Jacobson SW, van der Kouwe A, Molteno CD, Chen G, Wintermark P, … Meintjes EM. A DTI-based tractography study of effects on brain structure associated with prenatal alcohol exposure in newborns. Human brain mapping. 2015;36(1):170–186. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  266. Tewar S, Auinger P, Braun JM, Lanphear B, Yolton K, Epstein JN, … Froehlich TE. Association of Bisphenol A exposure and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in a national sample of U.S. children. Environmental research. 2016;150:112–118. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2016.05.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  267. Thomas JD, Fleming Sl, Riley EP. MK-801 can exacerbate or attenuate behavioral alterations associated with neonatal alcohol exposure in the rat, depending on the timing of administration. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2001;25(5):764–773. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  268. Thompson RA, Nelson CA. Developmental science and the media. Early brain development. The American psychologist. 2001;56(1):5–15. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.56.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  269. Tian YH, Baek JH, Lee SY, Jang CG. Prenatal and postnatal exposure to bisphenol a induces anxiolytic behaviors and cognitive deficits in mice. Synapse (New York, NY) 2010;64(6):432–439. doi: 10.1002/syn.20746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  270. Tobi EW, Lumey LH, Talens RP, Kremer D, Putter H, Stein AD, … Heijmans BT. DNA methylation differences after exposure to prenatal famine are common and timing- and sex-specific. Human molecular genetics. 2009;18(21):4046–4053. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddp353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  271. Tolsa CB, Zimine S, Warfield SK, Freschi M, Sancho Rossignol A, Lazeyras F, … Huppi PS. Early alteration of structural and functional brain development in premature infants born with intrauterine growth restriction. Pediatric research. 2004;56(1):132–138. doi: 10.1203/01.pdr.0000128983.54614.7e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  272. Tomi M, Zhao Y, Thamotharan S, Shin BC, Devaskar SU. Early life nutrient restriction impairs blood-brain metabolic profile and neurobehavior predisposing to Alzheimer’s disease with aging. Brain research. 2013;1495:61–75. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.11.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  273. Tran PV, Dakoji S, Reise KH, Storey KK, Georgieff MK. Fetal iron deficiency alters the proteome of adult rat hippocampal synaptosomes. American journal of physiology Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology. 2013;305(11):R1297–1306. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00292.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  274. Tran PV, Fretham SJ, Carlson ES, Georgieff MK. Long-term reduction of hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor activity after fetal-neonatal iron deficiency in adult rats. Pediatric research. 2009;65(5):493–498. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819d90a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  275. Tran PV, Kennedy BC, Lien YC, Simmons RA, Georgieff MK. Fetal iron deficiency induces chromatin remodeling at the Bdnf locus in adult rat hippocampus. American journal of physiology: Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology. 2015;308(4):R276–282. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00429.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  276. Tran PV, Kennedy BC, Pisansky MT, Won KJ, Gewirtz JC, Simmons RA, Georgieff MK. Prenatal Choline Supplementation Diminishes Early-Life Iron Deficiency-Induced Reprogramming of Molecular Networks Associated with Behavioral Abnormalities in the Adult Rat Hippocampus. The Journal of nutrition. 2016a;146(3):484–493. doi: 10.3945/jn.115.227561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  277. Tran PV, Kennedy BC, Pisansky MT, Won KJ, Gewirtz JC, Simmons RA, Georgieff MK. Prenatal choline supplementation diminishes early-life iron deficiency induced reprogramming of molecular networks associated with behavioral abnormalities in the adult rat hippocampus. The Journal of nutrition. 2016b;146(3):484–493. doi: 10.3945/jn.115.227561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  278. Tyagi E, Zhuang Y, Agrawal R, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F. Interactive actions of Bdnf methylation and cell metabolism for building neural resilience under the influence of diet. Neurobiology of disease. 2015;73:307–318. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2014.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  279. Unger EL, Hurst AR, Georgieff MK, Schallert T, Rao R, Connor JR, … Felt B. Behavior and monoamine deficits in prenatal and perinatal iron deficiency are not corrected by early postnatal moderate-iron or high-iron diets in rats. The Journal of nutrition. 2012;142(11):2040–2049. doi: 10.3945/jn.112.162198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  280. Wachs TD, Georgieff M, Cusick S, McEwen BS. Issues in the timing of integrated early interventions: contributions from nutrition, neuroscience, and psychological research. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2014;1308:89–106. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  281. Wachs TD, Pollitt E, Cueto S, Jacoby E, Creed-Kanashiro H. Relation of neonatal iron status to individual variability in neonatal temperament. Developmental psychobiology. 2005;46(2):141–153. doi: 10.1002/dev.20049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  282. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, Lozoff B, Wasserman GA, Pollitt E, Carter JA. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet (London, England) 2007;369(9556):145–157. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(07)60076-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  283. White TP, Joseph V, Francis ST, Liddle PF. Aberrant salience network (bilateral insula and anterior cingulate cortex) connectivity during information processing in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia research. 2010;123(2–3):105–115. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.07.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  284. Willis WT, Brooks GA, Henderson SA, Dallman PR. Effects of iron deficiency and training on mitochondrial enzymes in skeletal muscle. Journal of applied physiology (Bethesda, Md : 1985) 1987;62(6):2442–2446. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1987.62.6.2442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  285. Willoughby KA, Sheard ED, Nash K, Rovet J. Effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on hippocampal volume, verbal learning, and verbal and spatial recall in late childhood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS. 2008;14(6):1022–1033. doi: 10.1017/s1355617708081368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  286. Winick M. Effects of perinatal nutrition on CNS development and function. California medicine. 1971;115(1):73. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  287. Winick M, Rosso P, Waterlow J. Cellular growth of cerebrum, cerebellum, and brain stem in normal and marasmic children. Experimental neurology. 1970;26(2):393–400. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(70)90134-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  288. Wozniak JR, Mueller BA, Chang PN, Muetzel RL, Caros L, Lim KO. Diffusion tensor imaging in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2006;30(10):1799–1806. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00213.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  289. Wozniak JR, Mueller BA, Mattson SN, Coles CD, Kable JA, Jones KL, … Sowell ER. Functional connectivity abnormalities and associated cognitive deficits in fetal alcohol Spectrum disorders (FASD) Brain imaging and behavior. 2017;11(5):1432–1445. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9624-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  290. Wozniak JR, Mueller BA, Muetzel RL, Bell CJ, Hoecker HL, Nelson ML, … Lim KO. Inter-hemispheric functional connectivity disruption in children with prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2011;35(5):849–861. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01415.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  291. Wozniak JR, Muetzel RL, Mueller BA, McGee CL, Freerks MA, Ward EE, … Lim KO. Microstructural corpus callosum anomalies in children with prenatal alcohol exposure: an extension of previous diffusion tensor imaging findings. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2009;33(10):1825–1835. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01021.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  292. Wullschleger S, Loewith R, Hall MN. TOR signaling in growth and metabolism. Cell. 2006;124(3):471–484. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  293. Xiang AH, Wang X, Martinez MP, Walthall JC, Curry ES, Page K, … Getahun D. Association of maternal diabetes with autism in offspring. Jama. 2015;313(14):1425–1434. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.2707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  294. Xu XH, Wang YM, Zhang J, Luo QQ, Ye YP, Ruan Q. Perinatal exposure to bisphenol-A changes N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor expression in the hippocampus of male rat offspring. Environmental toxicology and chemistry. 2010;29(1):176–181. doi: 10.1002/etc.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  295. Yerushalmy-Feler A, Marom R, Peylan T, Korn A, Haham A, Mandel D, … Bassan H. Electroencephalographic characteristics in preterm infants born with intrauterine growth restriction. The Journal of pediatrics. 2014;164(4):756–761. e751. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.12.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  296. Yolton K, Xu Y, Strauss D, Altaye M, Calafat AM, Khoury J. Prenatal exposure to bisphenol A and phthalates and infant neurobehavior. Neurotoxicology and teratology. 2011;33(5):558–566. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2011.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  297. Youdim MB, Green AR. Iron deficiency and neurotransmitter synthesis and function. The Proceedings of the Nutrition Society. 1978;37(2):173–179. doi: 10.1079/pns19780022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  298. Zeisel S. Choline, Other Methyl-Donors and Epigenetics. Nutrients. 2017;9(5) doi: 10.3390/nu9050445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  299. Zoeller RT, Bansal R, Parris C. Bisphenol-A, an environmental contaminant that acts as a thyroid hormone receptor antagonist in vitro, increases serum thyroxine, and alters RC3/neurogranin expression in the developing rat brain. Endocrinology. 2005;146(2):607–612. doi: 10.1210/en.2004-1018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES