Abstract
Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (UWS) patients may retain intact portions of the thalamocortical system that are spontaneously active and reactive to sensory stimuli but fail to engage in complex causal interactions, resulting in loss of consciousness. Here, we show that loss of brain complexity after severe injuries is due to a pathological tendency of cortical circuits to fall into silence (OFF-period) upon receiving an input, a behavior typically observed during sleep. Spectral and phase domain analysis of EEG responses to transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals the occurrence of OFF-periods in the cortex of UWS patients (N = 16); these events never occur in healthy awake individuals (N = 20) but are similar to those detected in healthy sleeping subjects (N = 8). Crucially, OFF-periods impair local causal interactions, and prevent the build-up of global complexity in UWS. Our findings link potentially reversible local events to global brain dynamics that are relevant for pathological loss and recovery of consciousness.
Many brain-injured patients retain large cortical islands that are intact, active and reactive but blocked in a state of low complexity, leading to unconsciousness. Here, the authors show that this loss of complexity is due to the pathological engagement of sleep-like neuronal mechanisms.
Introduction
Patients diagnosed with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (UWS), previously known as vegetative state1, can open their eyes, recover sleep–wake cycles, but do not show behavioral signs of consciousness2. Despite behavioral unresponsiveness, many of these patients retain large parts of the thalamocortical system that are structurally intact, spontaneously active3,4 as well as reactive to sensory stimuli, though cortical responses tend not to propagate beyond primary areas3,5,6. Preserved cortical reactivity in UWS patients can be directly demonstrated by measuring the electroencephalographic response to transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS/EEG); apart from severe post-anoxic patients, TMS always elicits significant cortical responses in UWS patients. In a minority of such patients, EEG responses to TMS are similar to those observed in conscious subjects, suggesting that they may retain a covert capacity for consciousness. However, in most cases, the EEG response to TMS is simple and stereotypical, as assessed by the perturbational complexity index (PCI): in these patients, identified as “low-complexity” UWS, TMS elicits a strong initial activation, which fails to evolve into complex patterns of interactions7. In summary, in many UWS patients cortical circuits seem to be active, reactive but blocked in a pathological low-complexity state.
Non-Rapid Eye Movement (NREM) sleep is a physiological condition in which thalamocortical circuits are structurally intact, functionally active and reactive, yet unable to engage in long-range, complex responses8,9. Recent studies employing intracortical stimulation and simultaneous local field potential recordings in humans suggest that the mechanism responsible for this impairment in NREM sleep is the tendency of cortical neurons to fall into a period of suppressed firing (OFF-period) after a transient increase in activity10,11. This intrinsic propensity of cortical neurons to fall into OFF-periods has been thoroughly studied in the realm of sleep physiology across species and models and is often referred to as cortical bistability12,13. In silico, in vitro as well as in vivo animal models suggests that cortical bistability is due to adaptation mechanisms, such as activity-dependent K+ currents14,15 as well as active inhibition16,17. Crucially, intracranial measurements in sleeping humans show that, due to cortical bistability, neurons react briefly to incoming signals and then fall into an OFF-period, which rapidly disrupts the cause-effects chain triggered by the initial input. Thus, in physiological sleep a simple mechanism leads to a breakdown of deterministic responses and prevents the emergence of sustained, complex patterns of interaction, despite preserved activity and reactivity.
Can a pathological form of bistability play a role also in the residual cortex of low-complexity UWS patients? Asking this question is relevant for at least two reasons. First, cortical bistability and OFF-periods represent a basic default mode of cortical activity18, which can be engendered by physiological changes as well as by pathological alterations, such as shifts of the inhibition/excitation balance19 or white matter lesions20. Second, OFF-periods can disrupt complex cortico-cortical interactions, but are in principle reversible.
Here, we specifically test the following hypotheses: (i) pathological sleep-like OFF-periods occur in the cortex of awake UWS patients and (ii) this mechanism is responsible for the collapse of causality and overall brain complexity associated with loss of consciousness following brain injury. To do so, we analyzed TMS-evoked EEG potentials recorded in low-complexity UWS patients with the same analysis previously used on intracranially-evoked local field potentials during sleep10. First, we show that in UWS patients with their eyes open, the EEG response to TMS in anatomically preserved cortical areas matches the electrophysiological criteria for the detection of an OFF-period, as assessed during NREM sleep, i.e. the presence of a simple positive-negative wave, associated with a suppression of high-frequency activity. Next, we demonstrate that OFF-periods rapidly disrupt the local causal effects of TMS (as indexed by phase-locking measures) and in turn, the emergence of global complex cortico-cortical interactions (as indexed by PCI).
Results
Measurements in UWS, sleep and wakefulness
We analyzed 72 TMS/EEG measurements performed in 16 awake UWS patients, as assessed by the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R21), and 20 healthy subjects during wakefulness and NREM sleep, while stimulating both frontal and parietal cortex. Specifically, we assessed (1) the occurrence of TMS-evoked slow waves (<4 Hz) associated with the presence of cortical OFF-periods, i.e. significant high frequency (>20 Hz) suppression of EEG power compared to baseline22–24, (2) the impact of the OFF-periods on local causal interactions quantified by means of broadband (>8 Hz) phase-locking factor (PLF), (3) the consequences of the OFF-period on the build-up of complex global interactions as indexed by the time course of PCI. For a detailed description of the experimental and analytical procedures, see the Methods section and Supplementary Fig. 1.
TMS reveals sleep-like cortical OFF-periods in UWS patients
TMS-evoked EEG potentials recorded in UWS patients consisted of a slow wave, which was associated with an initial activation rapidly followed by a significant high frequency (>20 Hz) suppression of EEG power (HFp) starting at around 103 ± 9 ms (mean ± SEM; Fig. 1b and Supplementary Fig. 2B). This pattern of local reactivity, matching the criteria for an OFF-period22–24, was observed in all stimulated areas (both frontal and parietal bilaterally; Fig. 2a) in each of the 16 UWS patients. As shown in Fig. 2b TMS-evoked slow waves and OFF-periods could be detected irrespective of the presence/absence of spontaneous slow waves in the ongoing pre-stimulus activity (Fig. 2b–d).
The responses found in UWS patients differed markedly from awake healthy subjects stimulated over the same areas (Fig. 1a and Supplementary Fig. 2A); in this latter case, evoked slow waves were absent, low-frequency (<4 Hz) EEG amplitude (max SWa, see Methods section) was significantly lower (Wilcoxon ranksum test, P = 0.014 and P = 0.010 for parietal and frontal stimulation, respectively; Fig. 1d and Supplementary Fig. 2C, top panel; Table 1 and Supplementary Table 1) and the suppression of high-frequency power was never observed (Fig. 1d and Supplementary Fig. 2C, middle panel). Conversely, the UWS response was similar to the one found in healthy subjects during NREM (Fig. 1c), where TMS evoked a slow wave with a comparable level of low-frequency EEG amplitude (Wilcoxon ranksum test, P = 0.357; Fig. 1d, top panel; Table 1) associated with a significant high-frequency suppression (Wilcoxon ranksum test, P = 0.999; Fig. 1d, middle panel; Table 1) starting at around 127 ± 11 ms.
Table 1.
Wilcoxon ranksum test (P) | Wilcoxon ranksum test (P) | Wilcoxon signrank test (P) | |
---|---|---|---|
HW (N = 20) vs. UWS (N = 14) | UWS (N = 14) vs. HS (N = 8) | HW (N = 8) vs. HS (N = 8) | |
max SWa | 0.014 | 0.357 | 0.008 |
HFp | 1.053*10–6 | 0.999 | 0.008 |
max PLFt | 0.003 | 0.785 | 0.016 |
Statistical comparison regarding boxplots of slow wave amplitude (max SWa), High-Frequency power (HFp), and duration of PLF (max PLFt) presented in Fig. 1d. Details regarding the applied tests, the sample size and the significance values for each comparison between conditions (HW, UWS, HS) are reported
Cortical OFF-periods disrupt local causality in UWS patients
The duration of the causal effects of TMS on local cortical activity, as assessed by the PLF, was short-lived in UWS patients. Indeed, the latest significant PLF value (max PLFt, see Methods section) occurred at 167 ± 21 ms (mean ± SEM) when stimulating parietal cortex and at 188 ± 18 ms when stimulating frontal cortex (Fig. 1d, Supplementary Fig. 2C). These values roughly corresponded to the timing of the maximum of high frequency (>20 Hz) suppression (max SHFt) and were similar to the max PLFt of healthy controls during NREM sleep (168 ± 9 ms—Fig. 1d). On the contrary, in healthy awake controls, PLF persisted until 248 ± 12 ms when stimulating parietal cortex and 248 ± 15 ms when stimulating frontal cortex (Fig. 1d and Supplementary Fig. 2C). These results were statistically significant at the group level, whereby max PLFt was significantly shorter in UWS patients (Wilcoxon ranksum test, P = 0.003 and P = 0.031 for parietal and frontal stimulation, respectively; Fig. 1d and Supplementary Fig. 2C, bottom panel; Table 1 and Supplementary Table 1) and healthy subjects during NREM sleep in comparison to healthy awake subjects (Wilcoxon signrank test, P = 0.016; Fig. 1d, bottom panel; Table 1).
Next, we asked whether the three distinctive features of the cortical response found in UWS (i.e. the presence of a slow wave-like response, high-frequency (>20 Hz) suppression and shorter PLF duration) were related. These variables are thought to reflect neurophysiological events (such as the level of neuron membrane polarization, the degree of neuronal silencing and its impact on deterministic responses) that are causally linked and showed significant correlation in previous intracranial10 and extracranial25 studies. In order to demonstrate this relationship, we computed linear correlations between max SWa and the maximum level of high-frequency (>20 Hz) suppression (max SHFp) and between the timing of max SHFp (max SHFt) and max PLFt, respectively. Interestingly, max SWa was significantly correlated with max SHFp (R2 = 0.4, P = 9.927*10–4; Fig. 3 left). Also, max SHFt was significantly correlated with max PLFt (R2 = 0.34, P = 4.765*10–4; Fig. 3 right), showing that (i) larger evoked slow waves corresponded to more pronounced OFF-periods and (ii) earlier OFF-periods corresponded to an earlier dampening of the causal effects induced by the initial activation.
OFF-periods reduce global complexity in UWS patients
We finally asked whether cortical OFF-periods and their aftermath on local causality might be responsible for the loss of global brain complexity. All UWS patients included in the present study were characterized by levels of brain complexity (PCI range: 0.13–0.30) invariably lower than the ones measured in healthy awake subjects (PCI range: 0.32–0.64; Wilcoxon ranksum test, P = 2.911*10–11). Notably, these lower PCI values could be explained by a difference in the time-course of the build-up of brain complexity after TMS (PCI(t), see Methods section). While in awake healthy subjects PCI(t) kept growing up to about 300 ms (272 ± 4.6 ms, mean ± SEM, Fig. 4a, right plot), in low complexity UWS patients PCI(t) grew initially but reached a plateau at an earlier time point (197 ± 12 ms) resulting in a significantly shorter build-up (Wilcoxon ranksum test, P = 1.400*10–6). Most relevant, the timing at which global complexity stopped growing (max PCIt) showed a significant positive correlation with the timing of the OFF-period (max SHFt; R2 = 0.46, P = 3.034*10–4; Fig. 4c upper plot) as well as with the timing at which local causality broke-off (max PLFt; R2 = 0.56, P = 2.974*10–5; Fig. 4c lower plot). This result is highlighted in Fig. 4b for a representative UWS patient (Patient 12), where the time courses of high-frequency EEG power modulation, broadband PLF and PCI are depicted. To further strengthen the link between OFF-periods, loss of local causality and global complexity, we observed that recovery of consciousness (as assessed by the CRS-R) in a longitudinally recorded patient (Patient 16) was paralleled by a progressive reduction of high-frequency (>20 Hz) suppression, a concurrent prolongation of PLF and an increase of PCI up to values found in conscious awake subjects (Fig. 5).
Discussion
Previous studies employing TMS/EEG have shown that most UWS patients retain portions of the cerebral cortex that are active and reactive26–28 but blocked in a state of low complexity7,9. Here we investigated the electrophysiological mechanisms underlying this condition; we show that in these low-complexity patients the cortical response to TMS is underpinned by an OFF-period. Further, we demonstrate that the occurrence of this OFF-period rapidly disrupts the build-up of causal effects of the initial activation thus preventing the emergence of large-scale complex interactions. Similar electrophysiological events were detected in sleeping healthy controls but were never found in healthy awake subjects, suggesting that a pathological form of sleep-like OFF-periods may occur in UWS patients. Therefore, the present findings link cortical bistability—a phenomenon with a well-characterized neuronal mechanism that is known to play a role in physiological NREM sleep—to the pathophysiology of the UWS.
At the cortical level, the key feature of NREM sleep is the occurrence of OFF-periods, reflecting a profound hyperpolarization in the membrane of cortical neurons. This phenomenon, often referred to as cortical bistability, is caused by the enhancement of adaptation (or activity-dependent) K+ currents, brought about by decreased levels of neuromodulation from brainstem activating systems29–32 and/or by increased inhibition16,17. Due to these mechanisms, cortical neurons tend to plunge into a silent, hyperpolarized state, lasting few hundreds of milliseconds, after an initial activation13,33. In the sleeping brain, the occurrence of synchronous membrane hyperpolarization in cortical neurons is reflected at the extracellular level in large slow waves associated with transient suppressions of high-frequency (>20 Hz) activity that may be detectable in spontaneous activity both in the local field potential22,23 and in the EEG24,25. However, due to its activity-dependent nature, bistability and the associated OFF-periods can be better revealed using a perturbational approach, whereby the impulse-response properties of cortical neurons is probed by means of direct activations. Hence, intracortical stimulations have been employed to investigate cortical bistability in humans and its effect on the propagation of cortico-cortical evoked potentials during wakefulness and sleep10,11.
A key finding of the present work is the demonstration of a pathological form of sleep-like OFF-periods in the brain of UWS patients (Fig. 1b and Supplementary Fig. 2B). Specifically, targeting neuronavigated TMS to intact portions of both their frontal and parietal cortices invariably elicited a stereotypical slow wave associated with a high-frequency (>20 Hz) suppression activity matching that of healthy sleeping subjects (Fig. 1d and Supplementary Fig. 2C). Notably, these sleep-like OFF-periods were never found when the same cortical areas were stimulated in awake healthy subjects (Fig. 1a and Supplementary Fig. 2A).
Why do awake brain-injured patients show cortical responses that are typical of the sleeping brain? A possibility is that structural lesions may lead to functional changes that enhance adaptation mechanisms and hence the tendency of intact portions of the thalamocortical system to transiently fall into a quiescent OFF-period34,35. For example, this might happen when subcortical lesions, such as diffuse axonal injury, interrupt a critical amount of fibers of the ascending activating systems36. In an extreme case, the thalamocortical system may be largely intact but functionally constrained to a pathological tendency towards OFF-periods due a predominance of adaptation currents31,37.
Multifocal white matter lesions and diffuse axonal injury may also induce bistability by engendering a state of cortico-cortical disfacilitation, that is by reducing recurrent excitation20. Indeed, intracellular recordings have shown that, following a surgical white matter undercut (cortical slab), pyramidal neurons can switch their discharge patterns from tonic firing to an intrinsically bursting regime promoting the alternation between periods of intense firing and silence35,38–40. Crucially, a critical reduction of cortico-cortical connectivity may shift the excitation/inhibition balance, leading to cortical OFF-periods by excessive inhibition16,19. This is known to occur locally after a stroke41, but may involve the whole remaining cortex following severe, multifocal injury42.
Finally, multifocal brain injury can also induce critical functional shifts by altering the balance within the cortico-striatal mesocircuits43,44. This mechanism is particularly relevant because it may lead to both cortical disfacilitation and thalamic hyperpolarization. Importantly, if the latter exceeds a given threshold, thalamic neurons may switch their firing pattern from tonic to bursting mode45, thus further enhancing cortical bistability46.
Overall, different mechanisms, alone or in combination, may engender a tendency towards cortical bistability in brain-injured patients, as also reflected by the presence of slow waves in their spontaneous waking EEG (Supplementary Fig. 3)47–49. While their relative contribution is difficult to disentangle, it is worth noting that all the above mechanisms can be effectively engaged by a cortical perturbation. For example, a direct cortical hit with TMS may (i) trigger activity-dependent K+ currents and an OFF-period, if K+ channels are de-inactivated; (ii) massively recruit local inhibitory circuits leading to an OFF-period, if the excitation–inhibition balance is biased towards the latter; (iii) force hyperpolarized thalamocortical neurons to fire bursts of action potentials back to the cortex and then fall into a prolonged silence, when these cells are in a bursting mode. In fact, TMS perturbations could reveal the presence of adaptation mechanisms and of the ensuing OFF-periods in all patients, regardless of their background EEG pattern (Supplementary Table 2), of the prevalence of spontaneously occurring slow waves (Supplementary Fig. 3), and of pre-stimulus ongoing activity (Fig. 2b–d).
Intriguingly, the strength of adaptation mechanisms has been considered as a key factor in shaping the behavior of dynamical systems that are employed to model sleep-like activity15,50,51. In future studies, it would be crucial to explore the formal relationships between cortical bistability as experimentally observed here and the notion of bistability as a system-wide phenomenon defined in the framework of dynamical systems52,53.
Besides the specific mechanisms engendering cortical OFF-periods, it is relevant to consider their large scale consequences in brain-injured patients. In UWS patients OFF-periods were ubiquitously observed for TMS applied over parietal and frontal cortices (Figs. 1 and 2 and Supplementary Fig. 2). In this way, bistability obliterated the physiological differentiation of the impulse response across cortical areas (i.e. the natural frequency54) that is normally observed in awake, conscious subjects. At the same time, OFF-periods curtailed the duration of deterministic responses, as revealed by the correlation between the timing of their occurrence (SHFt) and the abrupt termination of phase-locked oscillations (PLFt). This finding is in line with the results of in vitro14 and in vivo20 observations suggesting that the resumption of cortical activity following an OFF-period is a stochastic process. To the extent that recurrent interactions rely on the amplification of coherent activity across distributed sets of neurons, the scrambling of phases operated by the OFF-periods at each node may critically impair the emergence of large-scale cortical integration55.
In view of the above, we assessed the relationships between the occurrence of OFF-periods, the duration of phase-locking and the temporal evolution of PCI, an index that is explicitly designed to quantify the joint presence of differentiation and integration in cortical networks9, a crucial requirement for consciousness according to theoretical neuroscience56. In UWS patients the build-up of complexity (max PCIt) was shorter and never reached the levels attained in awake, conscious subjects; crucially, the time at which PCI stopped growing correlated significantly with both the occurrence of the OFF-period (maxSHFt) and the termination of phase-locked activity (PLFt) (Fig. 3). These results corroborate the hypothesis that bistability and OFF periods may be in a key position to impair overall brain complexity. Most important, these significant correlations draw a first link between neuronal events and global brain dynamics relevant for pathological loss and recovery of consciousness.
Previous studies have shown that loss of consciousness in UWS patients is associated with a variable degree of brain damage and physical disconnection of neural linkages57,58. In a minority of brain-injured patients plastic structural changes, including axonal regrowth, may directly support behavioral recovery59,60; in others cases, functional adjustments may play a major role, while the amount of structural brain damage remains substantially equal61–63. In this respect, to the extent that pathological sleep-like bistability represents a common functional endpoint disrupting large-scale interactions across structurally intact portions of the cortex, its reversal may potentially be relevant for clinical recovery.
The course of events illustrated in Fig. 5 is compatible with this hypothesis. This figure illustrates the results of longitudinal TMS/EEG measurements performed in one patient evolving from UWS to minimally conscious state (MCS), and eventually regaining consciousness. In this patient, behavioral recovery occurred in the space of two weeks and was associated with a progressive decrease of bistability and a concurrent recovery of causality and complexity.
As a proof of principle, a recent microscale study employing electrical stimulation and recordings in isolated cortical slices showed that phase-locking and complex causal interactions, as assessed by an adapted version of PCI, could be effectively restored by pharmacological interventions that reduce bistability and increase cortico-cortical excitability64. This microscale finding further suggests a causal link between cortical bistability and complexity and may have translational implications since brain slices can be considered a simplified model of the electrophysiological state of the cerebral cortex under conditions of severe deafferentation.
While elucidating the mechanisms of recovery is clearly beyond the scope of this work, the present observations in UWS patients raise important questions. Can neuromodulation or pharmacological manipulation push neurons beyond the threshold for bistable dynamics thus promoting recovery of complexity? Are some patients just below this critical threshold? Different interventions, such as zolpidem or amantadine administration65,66, thalamic stimulation with deep brain stimulation62 or low-intensity focused ultrasound pulsation63, transcranial direct current stimulation67, and vagal nerve stimulation68 have demonstrated significant behavioral improvements in individual patients but a reliable read-out and interpretation of their end-point effects at the level of cortical circuits is still lacking. In the view of the present results, detecting the presence of cortical sleep-like bistability and tracking its evolution over time, may offer an objective reference to devise, guide, and titrate therapeutic strategies aimed at restoring consciousness. In this respect, it will be crucial to further elucidate the relationships between cortical bistability, neuronal OFF-periods and overall network complexity through extensive experiments across scales, species, and models, spanning from ionic channel modeling to whole-brain simulations and macroscale measurements at the patient’s bedside.
Methods
Study design
Here, we tested the hypothesis that sleep-like cortical OFF-periods characterizes the cortical response to TMS in UWS patients by analyzing 72 TMS/EEG measurements in 36 subjects.
According to an open-label design, we first compared at the group level TMS-evoked EEG potentials recorded in 16 (N = 16) awake severely brain-injured patients diagnosed with a UWS (Supplementary Table 2) with PCI < 0.31 and 20 healthy volunteers (N = 20) in wakefulness and NREM sleep (Supplementary Table 3). Specifically, from TMS-evoked EEG potentials we derived the EEG power <4 Hz (SWa) and >20 Hz (HFp) to detect the presence of cortical OFF-periods, and compared these two indices between awake healthy subjects, awake UWS patients, and healthy subjects during NREM sleep. Then, we calculated the duration of the broadband PLF (>8 Hz), which accounts for the impact over time of the TMS on the phase of the EEG response. Finally, we compared the time course of PCI in an open-label design between healthy awake subjects and awake UWS patients. For details about the recruitment criteria of study participants and the analysis of TMS-evoked EEG potentials see “Protocols and procedures” and “Data Analysis” sections, respectively.
Protocols and procedures
Patients underwent multiple behavioral assessments by means of the CRS-R; for a period of one week (four times, every other day), and one session of electrophysiological recording (TMS/EEG and spontaneous EEG at rest) within the same evaluation week. One additional patient (Patient 16 in Supplementary Table 2) was clinically monitored for a period of 1 month and underwent three neurophysiological assessments: the first while in a UWS condition, then after clinical evolution to a MCS, and eventually upon emergence from the minimally conscious state (EMCS) as assessed by the CRS-R. All UWS patients were included in the low-complexity UWS subgroup described in a recent study7. A single recording session was performed in each patient, except for the longitudinal assessment performed in Patient 16. During the recording, UWS patients were lying in their beds with eyes open, and vigilance was continuously monitored. In case signs of drowsiness appeared, recordings were temporarily interrupted and patients were stimulated using the CRS-R arousal facilitation protocols21. TMS targets were selected bilaterally within the frontal and the parietal cortices (Brodmann area—BA6 and BA7, respectively) based on the individual anatomical MRI7 and the precision and reproducibility of stimulation were controlled using a Navigated Brain Stimulation system (Nexstim Ltd., Finland). The need to avoid direct stimulation of cortical lesions guided the specific selection of TMS targets27 in UWS patients. Depending on the spatial extent and location of lesions in each individual patient, in the present study we considered 24 TMS/EEG measurements, from 16 UWS patients, obtained by stimulating either one cortical site (BA6 or BA7) or both (see Supplementary Table 2).
Healthy volunteers (see Supplementary Table 3) underwent a general medical and neurological examination in order to prevent potential adverse effects of TMS, and exclude major medical and/or neurological diseases as well as substance abuse. All healthy subjects were recorded during wakefulness with eyes open while lying on a reclining chair with a headrest to ensure a stable head position. In 20 healthy subjects, both BA6 and BA7 were targeted with TMS either on the left or on the right side, counterbalanced across individuals. In a subgroup of eight healthy subjects we performed TMS/EEG measurements by targeting BA6 and BA7 during wakefulness before lights off, while as soon as the participant reached a stable N3 sleep stage69, only BA7 was stimulated (see Supplementary Table 3). This choice was dictated by the previous observation that during NREM sleep TMS evokes larger EEG responses in parietal areas as compared to frontal sites70.
The intensity of the TMS-induced electric field was always set above 120 V/m based on the neuronavigation system. The intensity of 120 V/m has been shown to generate significant and reproducible TMS-evoked EEG potentials71. Overall, the TMS-induced electric field was comparable between UWS patients (124.8 ± 9.8 and 138.3 ± 12.9 V/m, mean ± SEM, for parietal and frontal stimulation, respectively) and awake healthy subjects (132.9 ± 4.8 and 133.9 ± 5.5 V/m, Wilcoxon ranksum test, P = 0.452 and P = 0.813, for parietal and frontal stimulation, respectively). In healthy subjects who underwent TMS/EEG measurements both during wakefulness and sleep, the same stimulation parameters were applied by means of the Navigated Brain Stimulation system. For all the TMS/EEG measurements, the location of the maximum electric field induced by TMS on the cortical surface (hotspot) was always kept on the convexity of the targeted cortical gyrus with the induced current perpendicular to its main axis. In each TMS/EEG measurement, at least 200 stimulation pulses were delivered with an inter-stimulus interval randomly jittering between 2000 and 2300 ms (0.4–0.5 Hz). For a detailed description of the TMS and EEG equipment see Supplementary Methods.
All the experimental procedures were approved by the following ethical committees: Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico Fondazione Don Gnocchi Onlus, Milan, Italy; Comitato Etico Milano Area 1, Milan, Italy; Comitato Etico Milano Area 3, Milan, Italy; Medicine Faculty of the University of Liège, Liège, Belgium. All healthy participants gave written informed consent, while for non-communicating UWS patients the informed consent was obtained by a legal surrogate.
Spontaneous EEG classification in UWS patients
Rest EEG recordings collected in UWS patients were evaluated according to a clinical classification recently proposed72 after bandpass filtering between 1 and 70 Hz, downsampling to 725 Hz, and re-referencing to the standard longitudinal montage. The EEG category of each patient is reported in Supplementary Table 2.
Data analysis
Data analysis was performed using Matlab R2012a (The MathWorks Inc.). TMS/EEG recordings were visually inspected to reject trials and channels containing noise or muscle activity7,27. Then, EEG data were bandpass filtered (1–45 Hz, Butterworth, third order), down-sampled to 725 Hz and segmented in a time window of ±600 ms around the stimulus. Bad channels were interpolated using the spherical function of EEGLAB73. Recording sessions with either more than 10 bad channels or less than 100 artifact-free trials were excluded from further analysis. Then, trials were re-referenced to the average reference and baseline corrected. Finally, independent component analysis (ICA) was applied in order to remove residual eye blinks/movements, TMS-evoked and spontaneous scalp muscle activations.
In order to characterize TMS-evoked EEG potentials we (1) measured the amplitude of a TMS-evoked slow wave (<4 Hz) and then (2) detected the occurrence of a cortical OFF-period by quantifying the amount of significant suppression of high-frequency (>20 Hz) EEG power compared to pre-stimulus10,22–24. Operationally, for each EEG channel i (1–60), we followed the stepwise procedure presented in Supplementary Fig. 1 and described below:
(1) To assess the amplitude of TMS-evoked slow waves, single trials were low-pass filtered below 4 Hz (third-order Chebyshev filtering as in ref. 10), re-referenced to the mathematically linked mastoids, averaged and eventually rectified. For each channel i, the maximum Slow Wave amplitude (max SWa(i)) was computed as the maximum amplitude of the rectified signal within the 8–350 ms time window (Supplementary Fig. 1A).
(2) To assess the suppression of high-frequency (>20 Hz) EEG power, we applied the event-related spectral perturbation (ERSP) routine implemented in EEGLAB73. Specifically, single trials were time–frequency decomposed between 8 and 45 Hz using Wavelet transform (Morlet, 3.5 cycles; as in ref. 54) and then normalized with the full-epoch length (here ranging from −350 to 350 ms) single-trial correction74. The resulting ERSPs were averaged across trials and baseline corrected (from −350 to −100 ms). Furthermore, power values that were not significantly different from the baseline were set to zero. To detect statistically significant activation in the time–frequency domain we applied a bootstrap statistics (α < 0.05), with a number of permutations = 500. Finally, the time course of the significant high-frequency EEG power was obtained by averaging over frequency the ERSP values above 20 Hz22. Then, from the time course of significant high-frequency EEG power of each channel i, we extracted three parameters: the integral between 100 and 350 ms of the high-frequency (>20 Hz) power (HFp(i)), the maximum value of high-frequency power suppression (max SHFp(i)) and the timing of the maximum high frequency (>20 Hz) power suppression (max SHFt(i)).
All the measures described above (max SWa(i), HFp(i), max SHFp(i) max SHFt(i)) and calculated at the single channel level were averaged over the four channels closer to the stimulation site (Supplementary Fig. 1D) and the resulting averages were labeled: max slow wave amplitude (SWa), high-frequency power (HFp), max SHFp (maximum value of Suppression of High-Frequency power) and max SHFt (timing of the maximum value of Suppression of High Frequency).
The impact of the OFF-periods on local causal interactions was assessed by means of broadband (>8 Hz) PLF75. PLF can be calculated for every single electrode as an adimensional index (range 0–1) defined as the absolute value of the average of the Hilbert Transform across trials. To the extent that instantaneous PLF (i.e. the time-course of PLF) measures the coherence of the response to a perturbation across trials in a specific time-window, it can be used to quantify the duration of the deterministic effect of a given input10,75. Here, for each EEG channel i (1–60), single trials were high-pass filtered above 8 Hz (third-order Butterworth filter) and PLF was computed as the absolute value of the average of the Hilbert Transform of all single trials. Assuming a Rayleigh distribution of the baseline values from −500 to −100 ms, PLF time points that were not significantly different from baseline (α < 0.01) were set to zero. For each channel i, the latest significant PLF time point was identified and labeled as max PLFt(i). Finally, max PLFt (timing of the last significant time point of phase-locking) was calculated as the average of max PLFt(i) over the four channels closer to the stimulation site (Supplementary Fig. 1D).
Finally, in order to assess the effects of bistable dynamics on the complexity of global causal interactions, we first half-sampled the data and then we calculated PCI by applying a fully automatic procedure7,9. Specifically, after source modeling (three spheres BERG method as conductive head volume, weighted minimum norm constraint applied to an “empirical” Bayesian approach), non-parametric bootstrap-based statistical analysis was performed to extract the significant spatiotemporal pattern of the TMS-evoked responses. Then, PCI was obtained as the Lempel–Ziv complexity of the matrix of significant cortical source activity and normalized by source entropy, resulting in a positive real number between 0 and 1 (minimally and maximally complex patterns, respectively). To further study the relationships between bistable dynamics and the emergence of complex interactions, we used the temporal evolution of PCI, i.e PCI(t), which describes the buildup of complexity of the deterministic brain responses to TMS over time. Specifically, we rounded PCI(t) to the second decimal place and we measured the first time point in which PCI(t) reached its maximum (max PCIt).
Statistical analysis
Group analyses were performed in Matlab R2012a by using Wilcoxon ranksum test and Wilcoxon signrank test where appropriate (see Table 1 and Supplementary Table 1 for details).
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
This research has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Framework Program for Research and Innovation under the Specific Grant Agreement No. 720270 (Human Brain Project SGA1) (to M.M., M.R., and S.L.) and No. 785907 (Human Brain Project SGA2) (to M.M., M.R., and S.L.) and by the EU grant H2020, FETOPEN 2014-2015-RIA no. 686764 “Luminous” (to M.M. and S.L.). The study has been also funded by the grant “Sinergia” CRSII3_160803/1 of the Swiss National Science Foundation (to M.M.), by the James S. McDonnell Foundation Scholar Award 2013 (to M.M.), and by the Tiny Blue Dot Foundation (to M.M.). Finally, this study has been partially funded by the grant “Giovani Ricercatori” GR-2011-02352031 of the Italian Ministry of Health (to M.R. and S.S.), by the Belgian Funds for Scientific Research (FRS) (to S.L.), by the “Fondazione Europea di Ricerca Biomedica” (to S.L.), and by the grant #2016/08263-9 of the São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP) (to A.G.C.). We are grateful to Chiara Cirelli and Giulio Tononi for their comments and suggestions. We thank Anna Cattani, Thierry Nieus, Maurizio Mattia, Ezequiel Mikulan, and Giovanni Casazza for their help and comments. We also thank Simone Russo, Sasha D’ambrosio, Chiara-Camilla Derchi, and Alice Mazza for critically reading the manuscript.
Author contributions
Conceptualization: M.R., M.F. and M.M.; designed and performed experiments in patients: M.R., M.F., S.C., A.C., O.G., O.B. and S.L.; clinical evaluation of patients: G.D., A.C., G.C., M.B. and O.G.; designed and performed experiments in healthy subjects during wakefulness and sleep: M.F., A.P., S.S., A.C. and M.R.; data analysis: M.F., A.G.C., A.P., F.S. and S.C.; wrote the manuscript: M.R,, M.F., S.S. and M.M.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the authors on reasonable request; see author contributions for specific data sets.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors equally contributed: Rosanova M., Fecchio M.
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41467-018-06871-1.
References
- 1.Laureys S, et al. Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med. 2010;8:68. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-8-68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Laureys S, Owen AM, Schiff ND. Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3:537–546. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00852-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Schiff ND, et al. Residual cerebral activity and behavioural fragments can remain in the persistently vegetative brain. Brain. 2002;125:1210–1234. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Stender J, et al. Diagnostic precision of PET imaging and functional MRI in disorders of consciousness: a clinical validation study. Lancet. 2014;384:514–522. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60042-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Boly M, et al. Cerebral processing of auditory and noxious stimuli in severely brain injured patients: differences between VS and MCS. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2005;15:283–289. doi: 10.1080/09602010443000371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Fischer C, Luaute J, Morlet D. Event-related potentials (MMN and novelty P3) in permanent vegetative or minimally conscious states. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2010;121:1032–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Casarotto S, et al. Stratification of unresponsive patients by an independently validated index of brain complexity. Ann. Neurol. 2016;80:718–729. doi: 10.1002/ana.24779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Massimini M, et al. Breakdown of cortical effective connectivity during sleep. Science. 2005;309:2228–2232. doi: 10.1126/science.1117256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Casali AG, et al. A theoretically based index of consciousness independent of sensory processing and behavior. Sci. Transl. Med. 2013;5:198ra105–198ra105. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3006294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Pigorini A, et al. Bistability breaks-off deterministic responses to intracortical stimulation during non-REM sleep. Neuroimage. 2015;112:105–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Usami K, et al. Sleep modulates cortical connectivity and excitability in humans: direct evidence from neural activity induced by single-pulse electrical stimulation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015;36:4714–4729. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Steriade M, Nuñez A, Amzica F. A novel slow (<1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J. Neurosci. 1993;13:3252–3265. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-08-03252.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Timofeev I, Grenier F, Steriade M. Disfacilitation and active inhibition in the neocortex during the natural sleep-wake cycle: an intracellular study. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2001;98:1924–1929. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.4.1924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sanchez-Vives MV, McCormick DA. Cellular and network mechanisms of rhythmic recurrent activity in neocortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2000;3:1027–1034. doi: 10.1038/79848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Compte A, Sanchez-Vives MV, McCormick DA, Wang XJ. Cellular and network mechanisms of slow oscillatory activity (<1 Hz) and wave propagations in a cortical network model. J. Neurophysiol. 2003;89:2707–2725. doi: 10.1152/jn.00845.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Funk CM, et al. Role of somatostatin-positive cortical interneurons in the generation of sleep slow waves. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:9132–9148. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1303-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Zucca S, et al. An inhibitory gate for state transition in cortex. eLife. 2017;6:e26177. doi: 10.7554/eLife.26177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Sanchez-Vives MV, Massimini M, Mattia M. Shaping the default activity pattern of the cortical network. Neuron. 2017;94:993–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mann EO, Kohl MM, Paulsen O. Distinct roles of GABA(A) and GABA(B) receptors in balancing and terminating persistent cortical activity. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:7513–7518. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6162-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Timofeev I, Grenier F, Bazhenov M, Sejnowski TJ, Steriade M. Origin of slow cortical oscillations in deafferented cortical slabs. Cereb. Cortex. 2000;10:1185–1199. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.12.1185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK Coma Recovery Scale-Revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2004;85:2020–2029. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.02.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mukovski M, Chauvette S, Timofeev I, Volgushev M. Detection of active and silent states in neocortical neurons from the field potential signal during slow-wave sleep. Cereb. Cortex. 2007;17:400–414. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cash SS, et al. The human K-complex represents an isolated cortical down-state. Science. 2009;324:1084–1087. doi: 10.1126/science.1169626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Piantoni G, et al. Modulation of γ and spindle-range power by slow oscillations in scalp sleep EEG of children. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013;89:252–258. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Menicucci D, et al. Fragments of wake-like activity frame down-states of sleep slow oscillations in humans: new vistas for studying homeostatic processes during sleep. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013;89:151–157. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rosanova M, et al. Recovery of cortical effective connectivity and recovery of consciousness in vegetative patients. Brain. 2012;135:1308–1320. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Gosseries O, et al. On the cerebral origin of EEG responses to TMS: insights from severe cortical lesions. Brain Stimul. 2015;8:142–149. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bodart O, et al. Measures of metabolism and complexity in the brain of patients with disorders of consciousness. Neuroimage Clin. 2017;14:354–362. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Moruzzi G, Magoun HW. Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1949;1:455–473. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(49)90219-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Steriade M, Datta S, Paré D, Oakson G, Curró Dossi RC. Neuronal activities in brain-stem cholinergic nuclei related to tonic activation processes in thalamocortical systems. J. Neurosci. 1990;10:2541–2559. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-08-02541.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.McCormick DA, Williamson A. Convergence and divergence of neurotransmitter action in human cerebral cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1989;86:8098–8102. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.20.8098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Edlow BL, et al. Neuroanatomic connectivity of the human ascending arousal system critical to consciousness and its disorders. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 2012;71:531–546. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e3182588293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hill S, Tononi G. Modeling sleep and wakefulness in the thalamocortical system. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;93:1671–1698. doi: 10.1152/jn.00915.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gloor P, Ball G, Schaul N. Brain lesions that produce delta waves in the EEG. Neurology. 1977;27:326–333. doi: 10.1212/WNL.27.4.326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ball GJ, Gloor P, Schaul N. The cortical electromicrophysiology of pathological delta waves in the electroencephalogram of cats. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1977;43:346–361. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(77)90258-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Meythaler JM, Peduzzi JD, Eleftheriou E, Novack TA. Current concepts: diffuse axonal injury-associated traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2001;82:1461–1471. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2001.25137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Schwindt PC, Spain WJ, Crill WE. Long-lasting reduction of excitability by a sodium-dependent potassium current in cat neocortical neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 1989;61:233–244. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.2.233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Nita DA, Cissé Y, Timofeev I, Steriade M. Waking-sleep modulation of paroxysmal activities induced by partial cortical deafferentation. Cereb. Cortex. 2007;17:272–283. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Lemieux M, Chen JY, Lonjers P, Bazhenov M, Timofeev I. The impact of cortical deafferentation on the neocortical slow oscillation. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:5689–5703. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1156-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Lőrincz ML, et al. A distinct class of slow (~0.2-2 Hz) intrinsically bursting layer 5 pyramidal neurons determines UP/DOWN state dynamics in the neocortex. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:5442–5458. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3603-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Fanciullacci C, et al. Delta power is higher and more symmetrical in ischemic stroke patients with cortical involvement. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017;11:385. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Schaul N. The fundamental neural mechanisms of electroencephalography. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1998;106:101–107. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(97)00111-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Schiff ND. Recovery of consciousness after brain injury: a mesocircuit hypothesis. Trends Neurosci. 2010;33:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Fridman EA, Beattie BJ, Broft A, Laureys S, Schiff ND. Regional cerebral metabolic patterns demonstrate the role of anterior forebrain mesocircuit dysfunction in the severely injured brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111:6473–6478. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1320969111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.McCormick DA, Bal T. Sleep and arousal: thalamocortical mechanisms. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1997;20:185–215. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.20.1.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Crunelli V, Hughes SW. The slow (<1 Hz) rhythm of non-REM sleep: a dialogue between three cardinal oscillators. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:9–17. doi: 10.1038/nn.2445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Leon-Carrion J, Martin-Rodriguez JF, Damas-Lopez J, Barroso y Martin JM, Dominguez-Morales MR. Brain function in the minimally conscious state: a quantitative neurophysiological study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2008;119:1506–1514. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.03.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Lehembre R, et al. Resting-state EEG study of comatose patients: a connectivity and frequency analysis to find differences between vegetative and minimally conscious states. Funct. Neurol. 2012;27:41–47. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Sitt JD, et al. Large scale screening of neural signatures of consciousness in patients in a vegetative or minimally conscious state. Brain. 2014;137:2258–2270. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Mattia M, Sanchez-Vives MV. Exploring the spectrum of dynamical regimes and timescales in spontaneous cortical activity. Cogn. Neurodyn. 2012;6:239–250. doi: 10.1007/s11571-011-9179-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Tartaglia EM, Brunel N. Bistability and up/down state alternations in inhibition-dominated randomly connected networks of LIF neurons. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:11916. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12033-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Freyer F, Aquino K, Robinson PA, Ritter P, Breakspear M. Bistability and non-Gaussian fluctuations in spontaneous cortical activity. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:8512–8524. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0754-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Jercog D, et al. UP-DOWN cortical dynamics reflect state transitions in a bistable network. eLife. 2017;6:e22425. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Rosanova M, et al. Natural frequencies of human corticothalamic circuits. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:7679–7685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0445-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lumer ED, Edelman GM, Tononi G. Neural dynamics in a model of the thalamocortical system. II. The role of neural synchrony tested through perturbations of spike timing. Cereb. Cortex. 1997;7:228–236. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.3.228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Tononi G, Boly M, Massimini M, Koch C. Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;17:450–461. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Fernández-Espejo D, et al. A role for the default mode network in the bases of disorders of consciousness. Ann. Neurol. 2012;72:335–343. doi: 10.1002/ana.23635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Lutkenhoff ES, et al. Thalamic and extrathalamic mechanisms of consciousness after severe brain injury. Ann. Neurol. 2015;78:68–76. doi: 10.1002/ana.24423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Voss HU, et al. Possible axonal regrowth in late recovery from the minimally conscious state. J. Clin. Invest. 2006;116:2005–2011. doi: 10.1172/JCI27021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Sidaros A, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging during recovery from severe traumatic brain injury and relation to clinical outcome: a longitudinal study. Brain. 2008;131:559–572. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Laureys S, et al. Restoration of thalamocortical connectivity after recovery from persistent vegetative state. Lancet. 2000;355:1790–1791. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02271-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Schiff ND, et al. Behavioural improvements with thalamic stimulation after severe traumatic brain injury. Nature. 2007;448:600–603. doi: 10.1038/nature06041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Monti MM, Schnakers C, Korb AS, Bystritsky A, Vespa PM. Non-invasive ultrasonic thalamic stimulation in disorders of consciousness after severe brain injury: a first-in-man report. Brain Stimul. 2016;9:940–941. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.D’Andola M, et al. Bistability, causality, and complexity in cortical networks: an in vitro perturbational study. Cereb. Cortex. 2018;28:2233–2242. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Giacino JT, et al. Placebo-controlled trial of amantadine for severe traumatic brain injury. N. Engl. J. Med. 2012;366:819–826. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1102609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Sutton JA, Clauss RP. A review of the evidence of zolpidem efficacy in neurological disability after brain damage due to stroke, trauma and hypoxia: a justification of further clinical trials. Brain Inj. 2017;31:1019–1027. doi: 10.1080/02699052.2017.1300836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Thibaut A, Bruno MA, Ledoux D, Demertzi A, Laureys S. tDCS in patients with disorders of consciousness: sham-controlled randomized double-blind study. Neurology. 2014;82:1112–1118. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Corazzol M, et al. Restoring consciousness with vagus nerve stimulation. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:R994–R996. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.07.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Silber MH, et al. The visual scoring of sleep in adults. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2007;3:121–131. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Massimini M, et al. Triggering sleep slow waves by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:8496–8501. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702495104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Casarotto S, et al. EEG responses to TMS are sensitive to changes in the perturbation parameters and repeatable over time. PLoS One. 2010;5:e10281. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Forgacs PB, et al. Preservation of electroencephalographic organization in patients with impaired consciousness and imaging-based evidence of command-following. Ann. Neurol. 2014;76:869–879. doi: 10.1002/ana.24283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2004;134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Grandchamp R, Delorme A. Single-trial normalization for event-related spectral decomposition reduces sensitivity to noisy trials. Front. Psychol. 2011;2:236. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Palva S, Linkenkaer-Hansen K, Näätänen R, Palva JM. Early neural correlates of conscious somatosensory perception. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:5248–5258. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0141-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the authors on reasonable request; see author contributions for specific data sets.