Abstract
Background:
The Body Position Spatial Task (BPST) is a novel measure of whole-body spatial cognition involving multidirectional steps and turns. Individuals with Parkinson’s disease (PD) are affected by motor and cognitive impairments, particularly in spatial function, which is important for mental imagery and navigation. Performance on the BPST may inform understanding of motor-cognitive and spatial cognitive function of individuals with PD.
Objectives:
We conducted this study to determine feasibility and validity of the BPST with standard, validated and reliable measures of spatial cognition and motor-cognitive integration and compare BPST performance in adults with and without PD.
Methods:
Ninety-one individuals with mild-moderate PD and 112 neurotypical (NT) adults of similar age were recruited for the study to complete the BPST and other measures of mobility and cognition. Correlations were used to determine construct and concurrent validity of BPST with valid measures of spatial cognition and motor-cognitive integration. Performance was compared between PD and NT adults using independent t-tests.
Results:
BPST was feasible to administer. Analyses show evidence of construct validity for spatial cognition and for motor-cognitive integration. Concurrent validity was demonstrated with other tests of mobility and cognition. Relationships were stronger and more significant for individuals with PD than for NT individuals. BPST performance was not significantly different between groups.
Conclusion:
Tests that integrate cognitive challenge in mobility contexts are necessary to assess the health of spatial cognitive and motor-cognitive integration. The BPST is a feasible and valid test of whole body spatial cognition and motor-cognitive integration in individuals with PD.
Introduction
Individuals with Parkinson’s disease (PD) experience motor and cognitive impairments, impeding activities of daily living (ADL), increasing fall risk, and decreasing quality of life (QOL). Sixty-three percent of people with PD report disabling sensory/perceptual deficits [1–3], which can result in impaired proprioception and kinesthesia [4–7]. Thus, patients experience impaired motion sensitivity, joint position sense, haptic acuity, altered attention to action, and inaccurate center of gravity [7, 8]. Proprioceptive and kinesthetic deficits are closely linked to and underlie both motor deficits in PD [4, 9, 10] and spatial cognitive deficits [7, 8]. Cognitive impairments, including in spatial domains, manifest early on in many people with PD [11, 12]. Cognitive domains impacted include those important for mobility: executive function, working memory/attention [13], and spatial cognition [14, 15]. Spatial cognition is concerned with the acquisition, organization, utilization, and revision of knowledge about spatial environments. PD patients are frequently impaired in tasks requiring spatial attention and spatial-location memory, perception of optic flow, and line orientation [16], as well as visuospatial working memory and planning [15, 17] and mental rotation [18]. All of these functions have significant implications for navigating environments. As such, spatial-cognitive deficits result in orientation disability and interact adversely with mobility in individuals with PD [14], resulting in impaired motorcognitive integration (i.e., cognition is embodied in action, i.e., the motor system participates in what is effectively mental processing during functional activities). Such deficits have major adverse effects on QOL because of functional activity impairments.
Processes underlying spatial cognition.
Individuals use visual information and perception to navigate [19], and perform successful action in the environment [20, 21]. Being able to orient oneself in space is important for navigation and environmental recognition, and accomplished via self-motion cues (physical location) and external cues (landmarks) [22]. Accordingly, the brain represents visuospatial information in egocentric and allocentric coordinates. Egocentric reference frames integrate vision and distance in the dorsal caudate nucleus, posterior parietal cortex, precuneus, and lateral frontal cortex. These frames are self-referent and updated as one moves through the environment [23]. Egocentric frames can be provided by static visual cues (e.g., size, texture), dynamic cues generated by the observer’s self-motion and the motion of objects around them, as well as muscle and joint proprioceptive cues, motor and vestibular information [19]. Allocentric reference frames integrate vision and distance in the medial temporal lobe (hippocampus), and ventral stream structures. These frames describe how object locations are processed in reference to each other and help develop cognitive maps [23].
Spatial Issues in PD.
Spatial issues in PD are thought to result from dysfunction from dopaminergic receptors disrupting cortical functioning [22], as well as impaired parietal-mediated perception of bilateral visual space [24]. Proper completion of internally or self-guided movement relies on efficient function of subcortical loops involving the basal ganglia [25]. Due to dysfunction of the striatothalamo-cortical (STC) circuit, people with PD have particular difficulty with self-initiated tasks [26]. Internally guided movements require timing characteristics to drive motor output. These timing characteristics are largely driven by caudate and other subcortical interactions with motor planning and execution regions. Given disrupted dopaminergic communication within the basal ganglia, PD patients are challenged in maintaining internal timing patterns that are reliant on basal ganglia function. As a result, PD patients are more likely to engage externally guided circuits to assist in task execution. Indeed, over time, the cerebellar-thalamo-cortical (CTC) circuit is increasingly recruited to perform self-initiated tasks [27]. In healthy individuals, internally guided behaviors benefit from automaticity of movement. But even in early PD, lost dopamine in the dorsal putamen leads to diminished automaticity and requires continually increased cognitive control of motor function [28]. As such, conceiving egocentric reference frames may be difficult for people with PD, which impacts visuospatial function. Rodent studies have shown a role in egocentric navigation for the caudate and in allocentric navigation for the hippocampus. People with Huntington’s disease, another basal ganglia disorder, are more impaired on egocentric working memory [29]. Based on PD pathological impact on caudate nucleus functioning, PD may have a greater impact on egocentric processing of spatial information than on allocentric aspects [23].
Given strong overlap between cognition and mobility, referred to as motor-cognitive integration, mobility deficits are common in PD. Mobility in particular, requires the intersection of spatial function and motor-cognitive integration. Adding a cognitive task to a motor task, thus creating a dual-task paradigm, can worsen both motor and cognitive performance in patients with PD [30, 31]. A cogent example of the need for motor-cognitive integration is found in spatial learning, both passive (visual information, available path of motion) and active (egocentric cues, proprioception and kinesthesia; cognitive processing of spatial information). Spatial learning is a key component of how individuals define their location, i.e., orient themselves in space and is required for mental imagery, identifying objects, and especially navigation [32] [33]. As such, motor cognitive-integration is necessary for successful completion of ADLs and simply traveling from one place to another.
However, current standard, valid spatial cognition assessments (e.g. Reverse Corsi Blocks (Corsi)), do not require whole-body movement from one location to another in a learned sequence [14]. A clinically-evaluated task requiring whole-body movement assaying spatialcognitive functions for navigation, mental imagery, short-term memory, movement planning, initiation, and coordination may provide information on the level of motor-cognitive integration and spatial cognitive function, which is requisite for basic movement in PD patients. We created the Body Position Spatial Task (BPST) to respond to the lack of a whole-body movement evaluation instrument that engages spatial-cognitive function, motor planning, and movement initiation. We developed the BPST because 1. it requires spatial cognition, which is early and often impaired in people with PD [12]; and 2. spatial cognitive deficits intersect with motor performance resulting in degraded motor-cognitive integration and impaired performance on the task.
The BPST is modeled after Corsi Blocks [34], a well-validated, widely used test of visuospatial cognition. Corsi does not measure whole body spatial cognition. BPST requires participants to observe and learn a series of multidirectional steps and turns in lengthening sequences, and then execute the movement series. We studied BPST in older adults with and without mild-moderate PD to 1) establish BPST feasibility for these populations; 2) examine BPST’s construct validity as a test of spatial cognition and motor-cognitive integration; 3) examine concurrent validity with mobility, executive function, and attention tests; 4) evaluate BPST’s discriminant validity (i.e., compare PD performance on BPST to that of age-matched neurotypical (NT) adults); and 5) explore relationships between disease state, cognition, and BPST performance.
Successful execution of BPST requires spatial cognition and navigation of a predetermined path, short-term memory, motor planning, and internal guidance of movement. BPST was hypothesized to have construct validity with spatial cognition and motor-cognitive integration, and concurrent validity with tests of attention, executive function, and mobility. Due to impaired spatial cognition and motor planning [35], and deficient execution of complex, goaldirected behavior [36] in PD patients, as well as impacted egocentric spatial reference frames [23] and impaired internal generation of movement, we hypothesized that individuals with PD would perform worse on BPST, as measured by span length (length of sequence of moves performed correctly) and number of trials successfully completed compared to similarly aged NT adults.
Materials and Methods
Participants
PD participants were recruited through flyers, referral, PD newsletters, support groups, and websites. NT adults without a history of progressive neurological disorders were recruited at diverse, senior-independent living communities, at research social events, and through word of mouth. Eligible PD and NT participants were at least 40 years of age. Potential participants with early-onset PD (i.e., PD that manifested before the age of 40) or history of other neurological insult were excluded [37]. Ninety-one individuals with PD (38 women/ 42%) and 112 NT adults (82 women/ 73%) were eligible and enrolled. Although the gender percentages differ, 40% is the prevalence of women in PD [38] and among older adults 65 years and older, the prevalence of women is nearly 60%, a percentage that increases with age (US Census Bureau Brief, the Older Population 2010).
Procedure
The Institutional Review Board at Emory University School of Medicine approved this work. Prior to testing, all participants provided written informed consent. All measures were administered during one assessment session in invariant order (to hold constant across participants any effects of testing, such as assessment experience, order, or fatigue.) and demographics, medical history, fall history, general health, ability to perform ADLs, education, and QOL were obtained. A fall was defined as “unintentionally coming to rest on the ground or lower level, not as a result of a major intrinsic event (e.g., myocardial infarction, stroke or seizure) or an overwhelming external hazard (e.g., hit by a vehicle)” [39, 40].
Participants were given breaks ad libitum and offered snacks and water during testing sessions. Testing required approximately 2 –2.5 hours depending on the participant. PD participants were tested while “ON” medications at a self-determined optimal time. Patients were instructed to come to the assessment having taken medications exactly as prescribed from their provider, and when they felt their medications were likely to be working effectively.
Measures
All tests of construct validity are valid, reliable and widely used in middle-aged adults, older adults and/or those with PD.
The Body Position Spatial Task [41] is modeled after Corsi [34], which assesses visuospatial working memory. In BPST, examiners visually and verbally demonstrate a pattern of side and forward steps, and turns in place, which the participant then repeats. After a practice trial of two moves, the examiner begins with two moves and progresses to a maximum of nine moves. The number of moves increases by one at each subsequent level containing two trials. Participants advance to the next level if they correctly completed at least one of the trials in a level. Once a participant missed both trials in a level, the task ends. Span (length of sequence of moves that were correctly performed) and number of trials performed correctly are used for analyses. Participants are allowed to use habitual assistive devices. The scoring of BPST is exactly as in validated and standardized Corsi blocks. Participants need to perform the trial perfectly to get a point/correct trial. If they perform all the moves but in an incorrect order, the trial is considered incorrect.
Measure of Parkinson Disease Severity
Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale Motor Subscale III (UPDRS-III) [42–45] and Hoehn & Yahr (H&Y) Stage of Disease Severity [46] were administered to PD patients and scored by qualified and trained raters to measure disease severity. UPDRS-III scores range from 0 (asymptomatic) to 108. H&Y stages range from 0 (asymptomatic) to 5 (wheelchair bound/bedridden).
Measures of Construct Validity for Visuospatial Cognition and Motor-Cognitive Integration
Primary Measure for Construct Validity for Spatial Cognition:
Reverse Corsi Blocks Visuospatial Task is valid for middle-aged and older adults, and those with PD. [34, 47–49] Corsi blocks requires participants to watch the examiner point to a series of blocks in a certain order, and then repeat the pattern backwards. The examiner began with two moves and progressed to a maximum of nine moves, with two trials per level of equal move length. Participants were given one practice trial of two moves. At each subsequent level, the number of required moves increased by one. A participant would advance to the next level if they successfully completed at least one trial in a level. The task was concluded after the participant failed, i.e., received a zero, for both trails in a level. The span (the longest sequence a participant can correctly repeat) and number of trials completed correctly were considered for analyses. Participants receive 1 point for a correctly completed trial and zero points for an unsuccessful trial.
Primary Measure of Construct Validity for Motor-Cognitive Integration:
Four Square Step Test (FSST) [50, 51] measures movement planning, initiation, movement speed, and coordination. Time to complete FSST has been associated with tests of executive function, as well as cognitive-motor dual task performance [52]. Participants are required to step clockwise then counterclockwise into four squares created by rods arranged on the ground in a cross. The task is timed and three successful trials recorded. If a participant steps on or touches a rod, the trial ends and a new attempt starts. The fastest time is selected for group analyses. This test is valid in middle-aged and older adults [50], and people with PD [52].
Measures of Concurrent Validity:
Motor:
All of the following measures are standard, valid and reliable for older adults with and without PD.
Timed Up and Go Test (TUG) [53] is a valid measure of functional mobility for PD patients, middle-aged adults [54], and older adults [55, 56] and was administered as published. Individuals are asked to rise from a chair, walk three meters to cross a line, turn around, and return to the chair as quickly and safely as possible. Dual Timed Up and Go Test [53, 57] is a valid measure of functional dual-task abilities for mobility and cognition for middle-aged and older adults, and for PD patients. Participants are timed while they perform both single TUG (as described above) and a concurrent cognitive component (TUG-Cognitive; counting backwards by 3s (“serial 3s”) from a random number between 20 and 100) or manual component (TUG-Manual; carrying a full glass of water). Before completing TUG-Cognitive, the participant is given three, 15-second trials of serial 3s while seated to be comfortable with the counting task while also performing the motor task.
Six Minute Walk Test (6MWT) [58, 59] assesses community ambulation and endurance. Participants are instructed to walk as far as they could in six minutes, without running. 6MWT is a valid measure for middle-aged and older adults, and for PD patients [60].
Thirty Second Chair Stand [61, 62] assesses lower body strength. With their arms crossed over their chest, participants are asked to rise from a chair to full standing as many times as possible in 30 seconds. The 30 second chair stand is a valid measure for middle-aged and older adults, and for PD patients [63].
Gait Speed [59, 64] assesses multi-directional preferred and fast-as-possible (fast) walking speeds. Participants perform three trials each of walking forward at a preferred pace, backward at a preferred pace, and forward as fast as possible over 20 feet. For the latter, participants are instructed to walk quickly, but safely. Participants start walking 1 m and finish walking 1 m beyond the start and finish lines. Qualified raters time participants with a stopwatch as the first heel passed the starting line, and stop when the second heel passed the end line. The three trials from each condition are averaged and speed (m/s) derived. Gait speed is a valid measure for middle-aged and older adults, and for PD patients [65].
Fullerton Advanced Balance Scale (FAB) [66, 67] predicts fall risk in older adults and people with PD with a series of items that challenge static and dynamic balance. FAB was only administered to those with PD. The use of assistive devices is prohibited during this test.
Qualified raters judged performance on 10 individual items: 1) standing with feet together, eyes closed; 2) reaching forward to retrieve an object; 3) turning 360 degrees in place (variables: number of steps taken during the turn, and greatest amount of time needed to turn 360 degrees); 4) stepping up and over an obstacle; 5) tandem walking; 6) standing on one leg; 7) standing on foam with eyes closed; 8) jumping for distance; 9) walking with head turns; and 10) recovering from an unexpected loss of balance. Each item is rated on a 5-point ordinal scale (0–4), with a maximum of 40 possible points.
Cognition:
The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) is a 30-point test providing a measure of global status of cognitive impairment through the assessment of a range of executive functions including orientation, memory recall, visuospatial function, attention/ concentration, and language. MoCA achieves high sensitivity and specificity for detecting mild cognitive dysfunction in middle aged and older adults [68] and is valid and reliable in people with PD [69]. If an individual had fewer than 12 years of education, they receive an additional point. A score of 26 or greater is considered a normal screen for cognition [70].
Trails Making Tests A and B [71–73], a neuropsychological task, is valid and reliable in PD, and middle-aged and older adults [74]. It tests visual attention, processing speed, and executive function. In Trails A, a test of visual motor speed and numeric sequencing, the participant connects numbers scattered on the page in ascending order. In Trails B, a test of global frontal lobe dysfunction and executive function, the participant connects numbers and letters on a page in alternating, ascending order (i.e. 1-A-2-B-3-C, etc.). Participants are required to connect the letters and/or numbers as quickly as possible, without lifting the writing utensil from the paper. They receive a practice attempt for both Trails A and B. Errors made while completing the task are pointed out immediately, so the participant can correct them. Time to complete each trial (up to 300 seconds) is recorded. The time to complete Trails A, Trails B, and the difference between Trails A and B (Trails difference= Trails B – Trails A) are considered for analyses.
Questionnaires
The participants completed the following questionnaires within one week prior to their testing session. These questionnaires are valid, standard and reliable in both older adults and people with PD.
Activities-specific Balance Confidence Scale (ABC) [59, 75] asks about an individual’s confidence in “not losing his/her balance” in everyday situations. Participants rated their confidence for each situation on a scale of 0 to 100% confidence. Scores are averaged and overall percent confidence was used for analysis. ABC is a valid measure for all middle-aged and older adults [76].
Composite Physical Function Scale (CPF) [77, 78] asks about an individual’s functional ability as related to basic ADLs, intermediate ADLs, and advanced activities. Participants were asked to rate activities as “can do,” “can do with difficulty or assistance,” or “cannot do.” This 24-point scale can provide estimates of risk for loss of function.
Data Analysis
Data were entered and cross-verified by research assistants. F-tests determined equality of variance between performance on outcome measures of individuals with PD and NT older adults. Independent t-tests or Chi squares as appropriate were run to determine differences in performance between PD and NT scores. The association between BPST and measures to provide evidence of construct validity and measures to determine concurrent validity and selfreported data (demographics, fall incidence, psychosocial measures) were explored with Spearman’s correlation coefficient. Both concurrent and construct validity may be assessed with a correlation coefficient and are not mutually exclusive conditions to the extent that the assessment of one validity precludes the assessment of all others with the same statistic. While concurrent validity may be assessed directly with a correlation coefficient, this methodology is not sufficient for assessment of construct validity. We can use the correlation to provide evidence of construct validity. Therefore, Corsi was examined for its correlation to BPST to provide evidence of construct validity for visuospatial cognition, because Corsi is a validated test of visuospatial cognition. Furthermore, task structure, measures, and scoring of BPST were modeled directly from Corsi. BPST span and trials successfully completed were analyzed against FSST to provide evidence of the construct validity of the BPST for motor-cognitive function. FSST was used as a comparable measure that integrates motor and cognitive function because both BPST and FSST involve navigation, and forward and side steps in multiple directions in a remembered pattern. Both interval and ordinal scale variables were gathered in this study. Spearman’s rho can be used to identify a correlational relationship between two variables, even if the relationship is nonlinear but still monotonic. We used a random procedure to introduce continuity to the ordinal measures, e.g. BPST, in the data because of the limited number of possible values of the BPST trials and span variables. By allowing the observations to vary a little within each measurement value group, we introduced additional variability to model our data, which allows us to better estimate the linear relationship and quantify the validity of our measures. By randomly generating a small uniform deviation of up to ±0.25, we maintained the relative value of the data but removed the potential for ties (i.e., equal magnitude) between observations in the same category [79]. To account for this additional level of uncertainty, we replicated the random unrounding procedure 1000 times and built 95% bootstrap confidence intervals for our correlation estimates [80]. Bootstrapping provides empirical intervals in cases where the data may not be sufficient for methods based on the assumption of normality. The underlying data of the study meet this insufficiency criteria because of the discrete nature of the scales and the random unrounding we applied to the data make the measures satisfactory for assessing the correlations. To further explore the relationship between disease stage, cognitive status (as measured by MoCA) and performance on BPST (as measured by BPST span), the PD group was subdivided into three cohorts of H&Y stages 1 and 1.5, 2 and 2.5, and 3. Correlations were examined and the data were plotted on a figure to visualize relationships. Alpha level was set at 0.05. We used Cohen conventions (.1 =small, .3= moderate, .5 = large) to interpret the strength of each correlation. Some participants’ data were not available for some assessments. To not be coercive, research assistants did not probe for reasons when patients declined to perform a task. We used all available data for each analysis to avoid introducing bias by only using patients with complete information. By using all the data, we increase the variability in effective sample size, but ensure that the results are as generalizable and replicable as possible.
Results
Ninety-one individuals with mild-moderate PD and 112 NT adults were enrolled and assessed in this study. PD participants had more prescription medications, less balance confidence, and were more likely to need assistance with daily activities. On cognitive measures, the two groups performed similarly. With respect to motor tasks, PD participants were slower in completing TUG, preferred, backward, and fast gait speed, completed fewer chair stands, and required more time and steps to turn 360 degrees in place (Table 1).
Table 1.
Baseline participant demographics for the enrolled sample and performance on motor, cognitive, and psychosocial measures
N | PD n=91* Mean (SD) [range] |
N | NT n=112 Mean (SD) [range] |
p valuesa | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | 91 | 38F; 53M | 112 | 82F; 30M | <.001 |
Age (years) | 91 | 69.0 (8.0) [44, 85] | 112 | 71.2 (9.2) [49, 86] |
0.075 |
Education (years) | 91 | 16.6 (2.2) [12, 20] | 112 | 15.9 (2.1) [12,20] |
0.016 |
Years with PD | 91 | 6.26 (5.2) [1,33] | NA | NA | NA |
Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (motor score) |
91 | 24.9 (9.0) [9, 58] | NA | NA | NA |
Hoehn & Yahr (median, interquartile range) |
91 | 2.0 (1.0) [1.0, 4.0] | NA | NA | NA |
Comorbidities (#) | 91 | 3.3 (1.7) [1, 8] | 112 | 2.9 (2.0) [0, 22] | 0.498 |
Prescription Medications (#) |
90 | 5.8 (4.4) [0, 21] | 89 | 3.5 (3.5) [0, 19] | <0.001 |
Assistive Device use (y/n) | 91 | 29y/62n | 112 | 26y/86n | .168b |
Falls in last year (#) | 88 | 8.5 (40.5) [0, 365] | 109 | 1.2 (8.6) [0,90] | 0.100 |
Assistance with daily activities |
91 | 14y/77n | 112 | 6y/106n | .017b |
Fear of Falling§ | 91 | 3.3 (1.5) [1, 7] | 112 | 2.3 (1.4) [1, 7] | <0.001 |
Composite Physical Function (/24) |
89 | 19.7 (4.8) [6, 24] | 112 | 20.5 (4.6) [7, 24] |
0.211 |
BPST span | 89 | 3.4 (1.3) [0, 6] | 112 | 3.5 (0.8) [2, 6] | 0.626 |
BPST correct trials | 89 | 4.0 (1.9) [0, 9] | 112 | 3.9 (1.3) [2, 8] | 0.750 |
Corsi span | 91 | 4.2 (1.1) [2, 8] | 112 | 4.0 (1.1) [2, 7] | 0.339 |
Corsi correct trials | 91 | 5.6 (2.0) [1, 12] | 112 | 5.1 (1.7) [0, 10] | 0.078 |
Four Square Step Test | 77 | 12.5 (5.8) [7.2, 48.9] | 109 | 11.5 (4.9) [5.9, 42.7] |
0.205 |
Forward Walk Gait Speed (m/s) |
90 | 1.0 (0.3) [0.4, 1.6] | 111 | 1.1 (0.3) [0.5, 1.7] |
0.039 |
Backward Walk Gait Speed (m/s) |
89 | 0.6 (0.3) [0.1, 1.3] | 111 | 0.7 (0.3) [0.3, 1.9] |
0.011 |
Fast Walk Gait Speed (m/s) |
90 | 1.4 (0.3) [0.4, 2.0] | 111 | 1.5 (0.4) [0.7, 2.6] |
0.035 |
30s Chair Stand (w/o hands) |
90 | 10.8 (4.6) [0, 21] | 112 | 12.5 (6.0) [0, 31] |
0.026 |
6 Min Walk Test | 87 | 371.2 (101.8) [0, 631.2] |
111 | 395.6 (109.3) [0, 637.0] |
0.110 |
Time to turn 360° (s) | 87 | 5.4 (5.6) [2.0, 45.6] | 28 | 4.0 (1.3) [1.8, 7.5] |
0.025 |
Steps to turn 360° | 87 | 10.5 (5.4) [4, 36] | 28 | 8.0 (2.0) [5, 13] | <0.001 |
Timed Up and Go (s) | 88 | 11.2(6.8) [5.5, 50.0] | 111 | 9.3(2.8) [5.0, 21.9] |
.014^ |
Timed Up and Go- Cognitive (s) |
88 | 16.7(18.3) [167.1, 6.2] |
111 | 15.1 (12.9) [177.0, 5.4] |
0.491 |
Timed Up and Go- Manual (s) |
88 | 15.0(14.4) [133.1, 7.2] |
110 | 12.9 (12.3) [142.4, 6.0] |
0.257 |
Fullerton Advanced | 88 | 27.5 (7.5) [0, 38] | NA | N/A | N/A |
Montreal Cognitive Assessment (/30) |
81 | 25.1 (3.7) [10, 30] | 24.7 (3.4) [17, 30] |
0.455 |
|
Trails A (s) (cutoff at 300s)^ |
82 | 45.9 (38.2) [15.1, 252.3] |
110 | 39.0 (17.1) [16.7, 122] |
0.133 |
Trails B (s) (cutoff at 300s)^ |
82 | 113.0 (86.7) [25.7, 300.0] |
110 | 94.6 (50.8) [26.1, 288.7] |
0.089 |
Trails difference [B- A](s)^ |
82 | 67.2 (66.3) [4.1, 244.1] |
110 | 55.6 (45.8) [21.7, 239.9] |
0.178 |
Activities-Specific Balance Confidence Scale (/100) |
87 | 76.3 (19.0) [19.4, 98.8] |
112 | 86.7 (14.4) [26.3, 100] |
<.001 |
Abbreviations: PD, Parkinson’s disease; NT, neurotypical; SD, Standard Deviation.
91 participants are included in demographics because they were enrolled in the trial; however, 2 individuals with PD did not complete the BPST.
Independent t-tests compared groups (PD & NT).
Chi square statistic
Rating worry about falling from 1 (low) to 7 (high)
Bolded p-values indicate significant difference between PD and NT at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
Equal variances not assumed
Feasibility of BPST for clinical use for participants with PD and NT participants
A small space, i.e. a clinical office, was needed for the BPST. No adverse events (falls or injuries) occurred during testing. Only two PD individuals refused to perform the task for unknown reasons. All NT participants completed the task. Instruction and practice trial time took approximately two minutes. Altogether, BPST took five to 10 minutes to complete depending on how many levels the participant successfully completed. Tables 2 (PD, n=89) and 3 (NT agematched adults) list Spearman’s correlation means and 95% bootstrap confidence intervals (CI) between BPST span and BPST correctly completed trials, and concurrent measures of mobility, cognitive and psychosocial function, disease severity, and demographics among participants.
Table 2.
Spearman's correlation means and 95% confidence intervals (CI) between BPST span and trials performance and Cognition, Mobility and Dual Tasking, Psychosocial, Disease Severity/Demographics for individuals with PD (n=89)
N | BPST span | BPST trials | |
---|---|---|---|
Primary Measure of Construct Validity for Spatial Cognition | |||
Reverse Corsi Blocks Spatial Task (span) | 89 | 0.437 [0.359, 0.510]** |
0.473 [0.398, 0.542]** |
Reverse Corsi Blocks Spatial Task (trials) | 89 | 0.414 [0.350, 0.479]** |
0.456 [0.407, 0.507]** |
Primary Measure of Construct Validity for Motor-Cognitive Integration | |||
Four Square Step Test (s) | 77 | −0.369 [−0.433, −0.308]** |
−0.369 [−0.376, −0.283]** |
Measures of Concurrent Validity Cognition | |||
Trails difference (s) | 82 | −0.488 [−0.536, −0.441]** |
−0.454 [−0.489, −0.422]** |
Trails A | 82 | −0.381 [−0.433, −0.328]** |
−0.381 [−0.417, −0.347]** |
Trails B | 82 | −0.514 [−0.563, −0.463]** |
−0.482 [−0.515, −0.449]** |
Montreal Cognitive Assessment (/30) | 81 | 0.393 [0.334, 0.450]** |
0.37 [0.330, 0.408]** |
Measures of Motor-Cognition and
Mobility | |||
Timed Up and Go (s) | 88 | −0.344 [−0.406, − 0.287]** |
−0.301 [−0.346, − 0.258]** |
Timed Up and Go-Cognitive (s) | 88 | −0.36 [−0.419, ‒0.351]** |
−0.302 [−0.343, −0.262]** |
Timed Up and Go-Manual (s) | 88 | −0.284 [−0.346, −0.229]* |
−0.237 [−0.282, −0.192]* |
Preferred (Forward) Gait Speed (m/s) | 90 | 0.446 [0.391, 0.486]** |
0.396 [0.357, 0.432]** |
Backward Preferred Gait Speed (m/s) | 89 | 0.418 [0.362, 0.476]** |
0.371 [0.328, 0.413]** |
Fast (Forward) Gait Speed (m/s) | 90 | 0.392 [0.338, 0.450]** |
0.365 [0.324, 0.403]** |
6 Minute Walk Test (m) | 87 | 0.317 [0.257, 0.379]** |
0.322 [0.284, 0.361]** |
Fullerton Advanced Balanced Scale (/40) | 88 | 0.445 [0.388, 0.502]** |
0.367 [0.325, 0.407]** |
30 Second Chair Stand (s) | 90 | 0.177 [0.120, 0.235] |
0.111 [0.067, 0.155] |
360 degree Turn Time (s) | 87 | −0.204 [−0.268, −0.145]* |
−0.150 [−0.194, −0.106]** |
Steps to Turn 360 degrees | 87 | −0.224 [−0.285, −0.167]* |
−0.130 [−0.170, −0.090]* |
Psychosocial | |||
Activities Specific Balance Confidence Scale (/100%) |
87 | 0.405 [0.347, 0.466]** |
0.407 [0.367, 0.445]** |
Composite Physical Function (/24) | 89 | 0.286 [0.225, 0.344]** |
0.323 [0.284, 0.363]** |
Fear of Falling | 89 | −0.383 [−0.452, −0.320]** |
−0.43 [−0.479, −0.383]** |
Assistance with daily activities | 89 | −0.143 [−0.197, −0.089]* |
−0.174 [−0.211, −0.138]* |
Disease Severity / Demographics | |||
Hoehn & Yahr | 89 | −0.268 [−0.358, −0.180]* |
−0.254 [−0.339, −0.174]* |
PD duration (years) | 0.041 [−0.025, 0.102] |
0.036 [−0.006, 0.078] |
|
Comorbidities | 89 | −0.229 [−0.286, −0.171] |
−0.269 [−0.311, −0.227]* |
Prescription Medications | 89 | −0.098 [−0.159, −0.040] |
−0.177 [−0.215, −0.136] |
Age | 89 | −0.186 [−0.241, −0.129] |
−0.157 [−0.200, −0.117] |
Gender | 89 | 0.157 [0.101, 0.214]* |
0.107 [0.069, 0.147]* |
Education (years) | 89 | 0.003 [−0.055, 0.059] |
0.091 [0.049, 0.130] |
Spearman’s correlation coefficients presented with 95% uncertainty intervals.
significant correlation, p<.05
p<.01
Validity for individuals with PD (Table 2)
Although correlations were similar between BPST span and trials, correlations were greater for BPST span than trials for all measures, except for Corsi blocks span and trials variables.
Construct validity of spatial cognition:
Large, significant correlations between BPST span and Corsi span provide evidence of construct validity of BPST for visuospatial cognition in people with PD.
Construct validity of motor-cognitive integration:
Moderate, significant correlations between BPST span and FSST, and between BPST trials and FSST provide evidence of construct validity of BPST for motor-cognitive integration in people with PD.
Concurrent validity:
Large, significant correlations between Trails difference, Trails A and Trails B, and BPST span and trials demonstrate concurrent validity of BPST with tests of attention, inhibition, and set switching. Significant, moderate correlations between BPST and MoCA demonstrate concurrent validity with global cognition. Significant, moderate correlations between BPST and TUG, TUG-M, TUG-C, preferred forward, backward, and fast gait speed, 6MWT, and FAB, demonstrate concurrent validity with additional dual motor-cognitive measures, mobility, and balance measures. [Table 2]
Validity for NT adults (Table 3)
Table 3.
Spearman's correlation means and 95% confidence intervals between BPST measures and Cognition, Mobility and Dual Tasking, Psychosocial, Disease Severity/Demographics for NT individuals (n=112)
N | BPST span | BPST trials | |
---|---|---|---|
Primary Measure of Construct Validityfor Spatial Cognition | |||
Reverse Corsi Blocks Spatial Task (span) | 0.188 [0.105, 0.276]* |
0.230 [0.163, 0.296]* |
|
Reverse Corsi Blocks Spatial Task (trials) | 0.182 [0.104, 0.261]* |
0.242 [0.186, 0.297]** |
|
Primary Measure of Construct Validityfor Motor- Cognitive Integration | |||
Four Square Step Test (s) | 109 | −0.123 [−0.190, 0.051] |
−0.228 [−0.274, 0.181]* |
Measures of Concurrent Validity Cognition | |||
Trails difference (s) | 110 | −0.039 [−0.112, 0.031] |
−0.055 [−0.101, 0.006] |
Trails A | 110 | −0.232 [−0.302, 0.163]* |
−0.302 [−0.344, 0.259]* |
Trails B | 110 | −0.125 [−0.194, 0.055] |
−0.184 [−0.230, 0.139]* |
Montreal Cognitive Assessment (/30) | 111 | 0.199 [0.129, 0.276]* |
0.295 [0.251, 0.334]** |
Motor-Cognition and Mobility | |||
Timed Up and Go (s) | 111 | −0.183 [−0.253, − 0.117]* |
−0.229 [−0.275, − 0.183]* |
Timed Up and Go-Cognitive (s) | 111 | −0.109 [−0.181, −0.036 |
−0.217 [−0.262, 0.169]* |
Timed Up and Go-Manual (s) | 110 | −0.170 [−0.244, −0.106] |
−0.254 [−0.301, 0.209]* |
Preferred (Forward) Gait Speed (m/s) | 111 | 0.120 [0.049, 0.198]* |
0.185 [0.140, 0.233]* |
Backward Preferred Gait Speed (m/s) | 111 | 0.075 [0.005, 0.148]* |
0.185 [0.139, 0.227]* |
Fast (Forward) Gait Speed (m/s) | 111 | 0.151 [0.080, 0.224]* |
0.203 [0.159, 0.248]* |
6 Minute Walk Test (m) | 111 | 0.115 [0.046, 0.183 |
0.161 [0.111, 0.211] |
30 Second Chair Stand (s) | 112 | 0.049 [−0.023, 0.121] |
0.100 [0.054, 0.144] |
360 degree Turn Time (s) | 28 | 0.391 [−0.535, 0.248]** |
−0.378 [−0.496, 0.257]** |
Steps to Turn 360 degrees | 28 | 0.040 [−0.195, 0.116 |
−0.081 [−0.208, 0.057] |
Psychosocial | |||
Activities Specific Balance Confidence Scale (/100%) | 112 | 0.145 [0.070, 0.217] |
0.203 [0.156, 0.253]* |
Composite Physical Function (/24) | 112 | 0.108 [0.034, 0.180] |
0.169 [0.123, 0.214] |
Fear of Falling | 112 | −0.019 [−0.108, 0.069] |
−0.091 [−0.160, 0.019]* |
Assistance with daily activities | 112 | −0.088 [−0.157, 0.020]* |
−0.209 [−0.265, 0.152]* |
Disease Severity / Demographics | |||
Comorbidities | 112 | −0.099 [−0.169, 0.026]* |
−0.193 [−0.237, 0.148]* |
Prescription Medications | 89 | −0.082 [−0.169, 0.009] |
−0.209 [−0.265, 0.152]* |
Age | 112 | −0.041 [−0.110, 0.037] |
−0.172 [−0.219, 0.129] |
Gender | 112 | 0.013 [−0.057, 0.082] |
0.062 [0.016, 0.110]* |
Education (years) | 112 | −0.051 [−0.123, 0.020 |
−0.047 [−0.088, 0.002] |
Spearman’s correlation coefficients presented with 95% uncertainty intervals.
significant correlation p<.05
p<.01
Correlations were generally stronger for BPST trials than span. Overall, correlations were less strong for NT adults, compared to the PD sample.
Construct validity of spatial cognition:
Small-to-moderate, but significant correlations between better performance on Corsi span and trials, and better BPST span and trials provide minimal evidence of construct validity for spatial cognition.
Construct validity of motor-cognitive integration:
Small, but significant correlations between better scores on FSST, and BPST span and trials provide minimal evidence of construct validity for motor-cognitive integration.
Concurrent validity:
Significant, moderate correlations between better performance on Trails A, Trails B and MoCA, and better BPST scores suggest concurrent validity with tests of global cognition and executive function. Significant, moderate correlations between better BPST span and trials, and less 360 degree turn time suggest concurrent validity with a mobility measure. Small, but significant correlations between better BPST and better TUG-C, TUG-M, preferred forward, backward, and fast gait speed, and 6MWT scores suggest concurrent validity with measures of mobility. All other correlations were small.
BPST relationship with psychosocial function
PD participants: Greater BPST span and trials were moderate-strongly correlated with greater balance confidence and fear of falling. Greater BPST span and trials were significantly and moderately correlated with CPF (Table 2).
NT adults: A small, significant correlation between BPST trials and balance confidence was noted (Table 3).
BPST relationship with demographics and metrics of health status
In people with PD, both greater H&Y staging and having more co-morbidities were significantly and moderately correlated to lesser performance on BPST span and trials. In NT adults, having more co-morbidities and prescription medications were significantly but minimally correlated with lesser performance on BPST span, and moderately correlated with lesser performance on BPST trials (Table 3).
Performance in Individuals with PD versus NT Adults
The groups did not significantly differ in BPST span and trials performance, or in time needed to complete the FSST (Table 1).
Post-hoc analyses exploring the relationship between stage of Parkinson’s disease, executive function, cognitive status, and performance on BPST
The relationship depicted in Figure 1 shows that worse disease staging led to lesser performance on the BPST span. Higher MoCA scores were related to greater performance on BPST span.
Figure 1.
The relationship between cognitive status and performance on BPST with respect to stage of disease. The PD group was subdivided into H&Y stage 1–1.5 (diamonds), 2–2.5 (triangles), and 3 (circles). Cognitive status is more strongly related to BPST performance than is disease stage.
Discussion
BPST is feasible, and these correlations provide evidence of construct validity, i.e., that BPST may be a valid measure of spatial cognition and motor-cognitive function for adults with mild-moderate PD, and possibly for NT adults of similar age. Concurrent validity for other tests of cognition and mobility was demonstrated. We believed performance on BPST would reveal deficits related to neuromotor processes that are necessary for dividing attention between mental processing and whole-body movement, i.e., that the test would reveal discriminant validity. This was not the case. Performance on BPST was not significantly different between individuals with PD and NT adults. Furthermore, we observed that the relationship between cognitive status and BPST performance was stronger than that between BPST performance and disease stage in those with PD. Neither BPST span, nor trials were significantly correlated with measures of demographics, suggesting broad validity of the task for individuals beyond the populations examined with these samples.
Differences between men and women in spatial cognition and how these differences may have impacted results.
Previous research has shown sex differences in seated (i.e., non-whole-body movement) tasks of spatial cognition usually for mental rotation [81]. Differences may arise if men and women do indeed differ in spatial processing style, e.g., allocentric (cardinal direction based) versus egocentric (personal directions, e.g., left and right) [82]. Some have reported that men take a more allocentric approach and women an egocentric; [83] therefore, instructions requiring allocentric perspectives (an Euclidean approach) favor men and instructions necessitating egocentric perspective (personal directions) lead to women performing better [84]. Other work has shown that these differences are mediated by and can be eliminated by manipulations in confidence [81], and negative performance expectations and that spatial differences previously noted do not show an actual lack of ability [85]. Our sample included more males with PD than women. However, the epidemiological incidence of PD is 60% male [38], which our study closely approximates in distribution. Future study should investigate the differences in wholebody spatial cognition performance between men and women with and without PD to help understanding of spatial cognitive processing with respect to mobility. Since older women in particular are at greater fall risk, further study is warranted.
Clinical Use.
BPST is feasible for clinical use because the test took under ten minutes to administer in a small space for both groups and could be used for individuals using assistive devices. BPST performance was associated with performance on FSST and Corsi for people with PD and to a lesser degree, NT adults. Association with these valid motor-cognitive integration/spatial cognitive tasks provides evidence of construct validity for BPST. Corsi is an accepted instrument for assessing spatial cognition and requires that examinees mentally orient in space [34, 86, 87]. However, physically orienting oneself in a space, as measured by BPST, has more relevance for mobility contexts and for individuals with greater fall risk. Previous research shows cognitive ability and training is important for functional mobility [86, 88, 89], further cementing the importance of jointly measuring spatial cognition and mobility, which BPST does. From correlations with valid spatial-cognitive and mobility tasks, BPST span and trials may accurately measure the interaction of an individual’s motor and spatial-cognitive function, especially for individual diagnosed with idiopathic PD.
Concurrent Validity of BPST with mobility, cognitive and pschosocial measures
A faster preferred walking speed and greater confidence in one’s ability to maintain balance were associated with better BPST performance in PD individuals. Faster walking speeds and greater balance confidence may have facilitated the task demands by decreasing the necessity to focus on balance maintenance (with or without an assistive device), while mentally retaining and executing a series of steps. Faster completion of one dual-tasking measure, TUG-Motor, was associated with better BPST performance among individuals with PD. TUG-Motor requires participants to remember multiple motor movements in a sequence, a common feature to both BPST and TUG-Motor. Physical function and fall risk are also important variables to understand and measure when considering individuals with PD. For PD participants, BPST performance was significantly correlated with ability to perform ADLs, balance confidence, and fear of falling as rated by the participant. Additionally, the FAB Scale, a challenging multi-item balance assessment not administered to NT adults, was strongly correlated with BPST. All of these correlations suggest that better performance on BPST may indicate better physical function, balance confidence, and gait. Therefore, administering the BPST may open a window into an individual with PD’s fall risk and physical ability in his or her daily life.
The Neurotypical Age Matched Adults
For the NT group, correlations between BPST performance, psychosocial measures, and gait speed were weaker, indicated that the BPST has less potential to reveal information related to these constructs in these age-matched individuals without PD. The lack of significant and strong associations between BPST performance and mobility, dual-tasking, and cognitive measures among NT older adults may be due to their ability to undertake coordinated, planned activity without the deficit in internal generation of movement that people with PD often have. This deficit and general slowness in PD should have increased the time needed to enact a movement and possibly led to increased working memory demands. Individual differences across testing measures likely made performance less associated with other tests and more associated with the innate motor and cognitive abilities of NT middle-aged and older adults.
Although the NT group performed significantly better on standard tests of motor function (e.g., chair stand and gait speed) and had greater balance confidence than those with PD, the groups performed similarly on the BPST. The NT group also performed similarly to the PD group on Corsi and the FSST, which agrees with research demonstrating that individuals with mild-moderate PD do not differ significantly from healthy older adults in FSST performance
[52]. While other studies have shown spatial deficits in Corsi between those with and without PD [90], their samples were considerably smaller than in this study. BPST was strongly correlated with both FSST and Corsi in the PD group, suggesting that these two tests make an ideal combination to measure individual advantages: a valid measure of spatial cognition in the former and a test of multidirectional mobility in the latter.
Cognition in PD.
Although this PD group was not considered cognitively impaired, several individuals had moderate PD with considerable physical impairment. Among those with PD, cognitive status was more associated with BPST performance than was disease stage. Without differences in cognition between groups, yet differences in certain motor tasks, the similarities in the BPST scores may be more indicative of cognitive outcomes than motor. Interestingly, a test of executive function, Trails B, was most strongly correlated with BPST performance in the PD group, indicating that the BPST may have broader application to cognitive assessment, which is important considering the strong import that executive function has for mobility and gait in PD [13]. The large and significant correlation of BPST with "Trails Difference" may indicate that executive function, specifically inhibition and set switching, has construct validity with BPST in a PD population. This finding is reasonable, given the strong interaction between executive function and mobility [13].
Egocentric versus allocentric visuospatial abilities in PD and impact on task performance and associations.
Because of striatal dysfunction, PD may have a greater impact on egocentric processing, than on allocentric aspects [23]. We believe that the BPST uses both egocentric and allocentric spatial abilities. The participant is required to observe and listen to directions given by the rater and then perform the task, which is modeled off of many clinical tests that involve simultaneous visual and verbal demonstration. Likely, participants use multiple strategies to complete a BPST trial successfully. Egocentric spatial abilities make use of proprioception and kinesthesia, which are vital for movement. To move one has to use egocentric abilities, but in addition, the verbal directions given during BPST are not to particular locations in the room, or towards particular walls or any other room-specific landmarks, e.g., “the window.” The verbal directions given are from an egocentric (personal) perspective, e.g., turn left, turn right, step left, step right. However, allocentric properties are also used because one observes a rater travel from one place to another, and to or away from a given wall. Thus, if more comfortable using an allocentric perspective, the participant may note in their mind different landmarks (walls of the clinic room) to which the person traveled to or away from to eventually arrive in the same places, when the participant performs the task themselves. People with PD may use an allocentric strategy to address loss of automaticity and internally guided movement ability. Therefore, people with PD might be particularly reliant on observing the spatial endpoints of each move, and the relationship to global landmarks, rather than self-referents given by the verbal directions. People with PD may be able to make use of relatively intact (at milder stages of PD) allocentric spatial processing. But, concurrently, these participants with PD required more cognitive resources, e.g., executive function to accomplish the task, than NT adults, which was reflected in the enhanced correlations (and particularly executive function) for those with PD, observed to a lesser extent in the NT adults. The data show that those with PD who scored well on executive function also scored well on the BPST.
Limitations
The study has several limitations. One limitation of this study is that more women were in the NT group than in the PD group, which may have impacted performance on BPST and/or other measures in unknown ways. Additionally, the PD group had slightly but significantly more years of education than the NT group; however, the two groups did not differ on MoCA and education was not correlated to performance on the BPST in PD. Impairments beyond visuospatial short-term memory span, for example motor learning deficits, may adversely affect performance on this test. Future tests of BPST should develop age, sex, and education norms to minimize these variables as possible sources of data distortion. Further, additional tests of shortterm memory and executive function (see correlation with Trails B) may have added dimensions to these findings. The small correlations noted in the NT group suggest additional study in a more heterogeneous group of older adults is warranted. Future research should use other tests, like the Digit Span WAIS-III that was previously used to validate Corsi blocks, to validate the BPST. It must be noted that the BPST could have correlated to Corsi for reasons other than visuospatial cognition. When examining left versus right onset PD [22], using the Clock drawing test to measure visuospatial function, researchers found that people with left-side onset performed worse on the test than healthy controls and right onset patients. As such, examining differences in BPST performance between those with left and right onset PD may be important in the future. The BPST probably measures neither allocentric nor egocentric spatial abilities purely. To our knowledge, there are no clinically available tests that do so. [23] As such, future experiments should isolate aspects of BPST or another whole-body spatial cognition task in order to test the influence of allocentric versus egocentric strategies and abilities on task performance.
Conclusions
The Body Position Spatial Task, a task requiring whole-body movement, is feasible and provides a valid measure of spatial cognitive function and motor-cognitive interaction for people with PD. Several significant relationships were demonstrated between BPST and other functional abilities, which indicate the validity of the test, both as a soundly constructed measure of wholebody movement necessitating spatial cognition, and as a discriminant between groups on performance of physical function and fall risk. BPST performance was significantly correlated with motor skills and cognitive ability; these abilities are integral to functioning independently and fully performing ADLs. Additionally, relationships with BPST performance and other functional abilities were strong in the PD group, indicating that BPST may be valid for examining association between motor and cognitive ability in PD individuals. Similarly, BPST was shown to be associated with measures of daily physical function, balance, and fall risk, but only for PD participants. Among PD participants, BPST performance and disease stage (as measured by H&Y Stage) are only weakly correlated, indicating that BPST may reveal motorcognitive interactions independent of disease severity. BPST should be included in clinical assessment tools utilized to measure impairments in PD individuals that may not be observed in assessments purely measuring mobility, cognition, or disease severity. Because BPST closely mirrors the mobility, motor-cognitive interaction, and spatial-cognitive demands of daily life, understanding how individuals with and without PD perform on the task is important. In so doing, performance on BPST can elucidate clinical signs and identify the impacts of the disease in people with PD.
Acknowledgements.
The Emory Center for Injury Control, the Dan and Merrie Boone Foundation, and the Emory Center for Health in Aging supported the study. This study was also supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number UL1TR000454. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The Department of VA R&D Service Career Development Awards (E7108M and N0870W) supported ME Hackney. We acknowledge the participants’ time. We acknowledge Dr. Kathleen E. McKee, M.D., for recruitment and assessment assistance.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest: There are no apparent conflicts of interest in terms of honoraria received, speaker forums, consultant roles, stocks, royalties, expert testimony, board memberships, patents, or personal relationships of any of the authors.
Sponsor’s Role: The study sponsors played no design in the writing of the manuscript, the final conclusions drawn or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
References
- 1.Koller WC, Sensory symptoms in Parkinson's disease. Neurology, 1984. 34(7): p. 957–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Shulman LM, et al. , Internal tremor in patients with Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord, 1996. 11(1): p. 3–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Witjas T, et al. , Nonmotor fluctuations in Parkinson's disease: frequent and disabling. Neurology, 2002. 59(3): p. 408–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Conte A, et al. , Pathophysiology of somatosensory abnormalities in Parkinson disease. Nat Rev Neurol, 2013. 9(12): p. 687–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hayley T, Elisabeth P, and Gordon W, Proprioception of the Ankle is Impaired in People with Parkinson's Disease. Movement Disorders Clinical Practice, 2017. 4(4): p. 524–528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ribeiro L, et al. , Proprioceptive deficits in Parkinson's disease: from clinical data to animal experimentation. Psychology & Neuroscience, 2011. 4: p. 235–244. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Friedman JH, Misperceptions and Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Sci, 2017. 374: p. 4246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Zia S, Cody F, and O'Boyle D, Joint position sense is impaired by Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol, 2000. 47(2): p. 218–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Vaugoyeau M, et al. , Impaired vertical postural control and proprioceptive integration deficits in Parkinson's disease. Neuroscience, 2007. 146(2): p. 852–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Demirci M, et al. , A mismatch between kinesthetic and visual perception in Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol, 1997. 41(6): p. 781–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Litvan I, et al. , Diagnostic criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease: Movement Disorder Society Task Force guidelines. Mov Disord, 2012. 27(3): p. 349–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Broeders M, et al. , Evolution of mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease. Neurology, 2013. 81(4): p. 346–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hausdorff JM, et al. , A common cognitive profile in elderly fallers and in patients with Parkinson's disease: the prominence of impaired executive function and attention. Exp Aging Res, 2006. 32(4): p. 411–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Peterson DS, et al. , Cognitive Contributions to Freezing of Gait in Parkinson Disease: Implications for Physical Rehabilitation. Phys Ther, 2016. 96(5): p. 659–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Possin KL, et al. , Spatial and object working memory deficits in Parkinson's disease are due to impairment in different underlying processes. Neuropsychology, 2008. 22(5): p. 585–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Davidsdottir S, Cronin-Golomb A, and Lee A, Visual and spatial symptoms in Parkinson's disease. Vision Res, 2005. 45(10): p. 1285–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kemps E, et al. , Visuo-spatial processing in Parkinson's disease: evidence for diminished visuo-spatial sketch pad and central executive resources. Parkinsonism Relat Disord, 2005. 11(3): p. 181–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Amick MM, Grace J, and Chou KL, Body side of motor symptom onset in Parkinson's disease is associated with memory performance. J Int Neuropsychol Soc, 2006. 12(5): p. 736–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sun HJ, et al. , The contributions of static visual cues, nonvisual cues, and optic flow in distance estimation. Perception, 2004. 33(1): p. 49–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gibson EJ, Improvement in perceptual judgments as a function of controlled practice or training. Psychol Bull, 1953. 50(6): p. 401–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gibson JJ, Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. Br J Psychol, 1958. 49(3): p. 182–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Seichepine DR, et al. , Side and type of initial motor symptom influences visuospatial functioning in Parkinson's disease. J Parkinsons Dis, 2015. 5(1): p. 75–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Possin KL, Visual spatial cognition in neurodegenerative disease. Neurocase, 2010. 16(6): p. 466–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Davidsdottir S, et al. , Impact of optic flow perception and egocentric coordinates on veering in Parkinson's disease. Brain, 2008. 131(Pt 11): p. 2882–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Alexander GE, DeLong MR, and Strick PL, Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci, 1986. 9: p. 357–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wu T, et al. , Effective connectivity of brain networks during self-initiated movement in Parkinson's disease. Neuroimage, 2011. 55(1): p. 204–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Sen S, et al. , Dynamic changes in cerebello-thalamo-cortical motor circuitry during progression of Parkinson's disease. Neuroscience, 2010. 166(2): p. 712–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Petzinger GM, et al. , Exercise-enhanced neuroplasticity targeting motor and cognitive circuitry in Parkinson's disease. Lancet Neurol, 2013. 12(7): p. 716–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Possin KL, et al. , Egocentric and allocentric visuospatial working memory in premotor Huntington's disease: A double dissociation with caudate and hippocampal volumes. Neuropsychologia, 2017. 101: p. 57–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Leroi I, Collins D, and Marsh L, Non-dopaminergic treatment of cognitive impairment and dementia in Parkinson's disease: a review. J Neurol Sci, 2006. 248(1–2): p. 104–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Salazar RD, et al. , Dual tasking in Parkinson's disease: Cognitive consequences while walking. Neuropsychology, 2017. 31(6): p. 613–623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Klencklen G, Despres O, and Dufour A, What do we know about aging and spatial cognition? Reviews and perspectives. Ageing Res Rev, 2012. 11(1): p. 123–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Chrastil ER and Warren WH, Active and passive spatial learning in human navigation: acquisition of graph knowledge. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 2015. 41(4): p. 1162–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kessels RP, et al. , The backward span of the Corsi Block-Tapping Task and its association with the WAIS-III Digit Span. Assessment, 2008. 15(4): p. 426–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Low KA, Miller J, and Vierck E, Response slowing in Parkinson's disease: a psychophysiological analysis of premotor and motor processes. Brain, 2002. 125(Pt 9): p. 1980–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Plotnik M, et al. , Effects of cognitive function on gait and dual tasking abilities in patients with Parkinson's disease suffering from motor response fluctuations. Exp Brain Res, 2011. 208(2): p. 169–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Schrag A, et al. , Young-onset Parkinson's disease revisited--clinical features, natural history, and mortality. Mov Disord, 1998. 13(6): p. 885–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wooten GF, et al. , Are men at greater risk for Parkinson's disease than women? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 2004. 75(4): p. 637–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Leveille SG, et al. , The MOBILIZE Boston Study: design and methods of a prospective cohort study of novel risk factors for falls in an older population. BMC Geriatr, 2008. 8: p. 16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Escobar JI, et al. , Use of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in a community population of mixed ethnicity. Cultural and linguistic artifacts. J Nerv Ment Dis, 1986. 174(10): p. 607–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hackney ME, et al. , Dancing for balance: feasibility and efficacy in oldest-old adults with visual impairment. Nurs Res, 2013. 62(2): p. 138–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Recent developments in Parkinson's disease . Edited by Fahn S, Mardsen CD, Jenner P, and Teychenne P New York, Raven Press, 1986 375 pp, illustarted. Annals of Neurology, 1987. 22(5): p. 672–672. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Gallagher DA, et al. , Validation of the MDS-UPDRS Part I for nonmotor symptoms in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord, 2012. 27(1): p. 79–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Goetz CG, et al. , Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov Disord, 2008. 23(15): p. 2129–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Fahn S and Chouinard S, Experience with tranylcypromine in early Parkinson's disease. J Neural Transm Suppl, 1998. 52: p. 49–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Hoehn MM and Yahr MD, Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology, 1967. 17(5): p. 427–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Corsi PM, Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1972. 34(2). [Google Scholar]
- 48.Orsini A, et al. , Verbal and spatial memory span in patients with extrapyramidal diseases. Percept Mot Skills, 1987. 65(2): p. 555–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kessels RP, et al. , The Corsi Block-Tapping Task: standardization and normative data. Appl Neuropsychol, 2000. 7(4): p. 252–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Dite W and Temple VA, A clinical test of stepping and change of direction to identify multiple falling older adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, 2002. 83(11): p. 1566–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Whitney SL, et al. , The reliability and validity of the Four Square Step Test for people with balance deficits secondary to a vestibular disorder. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, 2007. 88(1): p. 99–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.McKee KE and Hackney ME, The Four Square Step Test in individuals with Parkinson's disease: association with executive function and comparison with older adults. NeuroRehabilitation, 2014. 35(2): p. 279–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Morris S, Morris ME, and Iansek R, Reliability of measurements obtained with the Timed "Up & Go" test in people with Parkinson disease. Phys Ther, 2001. 81(2): p. 810–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Khant N, et al. , Establishing the reference value for "timed up-and-go" test in healthy adults of Gujarat, India. J Educ Health Promot, 2018. 7: p. 62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Podsiadlo D and Richardson S, The timed "Up & Go": a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J Am Geriatr Soc, 1991. 39(2): p. 142–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Brooks D, Davis AM, and Naglie G, Validity of 3 physical performance measures in inpatient geriatric rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, 2006. 87(1): p. 105–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Maranhao-Filho PA, et al. , Rethinking the neurological examination II: dynamic balance assessment. Arq Neuropsiquiatr, 2011. 69(6): p. 959–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Guyatt GH, et al. , The 6-minute walk: a new measure of exercise capacity in patients with chronic heart failure. Can Med Assoc J, 1985. 132(8): p. 919–23. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Steffen T and Seney M, Test-retest reliability and minimal detectable change on balance and ambulation tests, the 36-item short-form health survey, and the unified Parkinson disease rating scale in people with parkinsonism. Phys Ther, 2008. 88(6): p. 733–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Enright PL and Sherrill DL, Reference equations for the six-minute walk in healthy adults. Am J Respir Crit Care Med, 1998. 158(5 Pt 1): p. 1384–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Rikli R and Jones C, Senior Fitness Test Manual 2001, Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Jones CJ, Rikli RE, and Beam WC, A 30-s chair-stand test as a measure of lower body strength in community-residing older adults. Res Q Exerc Sport, 1999. 70(2): p. 113–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Wright AA, et al. , A comparison of 3 methodological approaches to defining major clinically important improvement of 4 performance measures in patients with hip osteoarthritis. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther, 2011. 41(5): p. 319–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bohannon RW, Gait performance with wheeled and standard walkers. Percept Mot Skills, 1997. 85(3 Pt 2): p. 1185–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Middleton A, Fritz SL, and Lusardi M, Walking speed: the functional vital sign. J Aging Phys Act, 2015. 23(2): p. 314–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Hernandez D and Rose DJ, Predicting which older adults will or will not fall using the Fullerton Advanced Balance scale. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, 2008. 89(12): p. 2309–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Klein PJ, Fiedler RC, and Rose DJ, Rasch Analysis of the Fullerton Advanced Balance (FAB) Scale. Physiother Can, 2011. 63(1): p. 115–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Nasreddine ZS, et al. , The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc, 2005. 53(4): p. 695–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Chou KL, et al. , A recommended scale for cognitive screening in clinical trials of Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord, 2010. 25(15): p. 2501–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Hoops S, et al. , Validity of the MoCA and MMSE in the detection of MCI and dementia in Parkinson disease. Neurology, 2009. 73(21): p. 1738–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Carone DA, Strauss E, Sherman EMS, & Spreen O, A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. Applied Neuropsychology, 2007. 14(1): p. 62–63. [Google Scholar]
- 72.Tombaugh TN, Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol, 2004. 19(2): p. 203–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Arbuthnott K and Frank J, Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol, 2000. 22(4): p. 51828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Salthouse TA, What cognitive abilities are involved in trail-making performance? Intelligence, 2011. 39(4): p. 222–232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Powell LE and Myers AM, The Activities-specific Balance Confidence (ABC) Scale. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci, 1995. 50A(1): p. M28–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Alghwiri AA, et al. , The activities-specific balance confidence scale and berg balance scale: Reliability and validity in Arabic-speaking vestibular patients. J Vestib Res, 2016. 25(5–6): p. 253–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Rikli RE, Reliability, validity, and methodological issues in assessing physical activity in older adults. Res Q Exerc Sport, 2000. 71(2 Suppl): p. S89–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Rikli RE and Jones CJ, The Reliability and Validity of a 6-Minute Walk Test as a Measure of Physical Endurance in Older Adults. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 1998. 6(4): p. 363–375. [Google Scholar]
- 79.Kendall MG, The treatment of ties in ranking problems. Biometrika, 1945. 33: p. 23951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Efron B and Tibshirani R, An introduction to the bootstrap. Monographs on statistics and applied probability 1993, New York: Chapman & Hall; xvi, 436 p. [Google Scholar]
- 81.Estes Z and Felker S, Confidence mediates the sex difference in mental rotation performance. Arch Sex Behav, 2012. 41(3): p. 557–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Pletzer B, Scheuringer A, and Scherndl T, Global-local processing relates to spatial and verbal processing: implications for sex differences in cognition. Sci Rep, 2017. 7(1): p. 10575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Andersen NE, et al. , Eye tracking, strategies, and sex differences in virtual navigation. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 2012. 97(1): p. 81–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Saucier DM, et al. , Are sex differences in navigation caused by sexually dimorphic strategies or by differences in the ability to use the strategies? Behav Neurosci, 2002. 116(3): p. 403–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Tarampi MR, Heydari N, and Hegarty M, A Tale of Two Types of Perspective Taking: Sex Differences in Spatial Ability. Psychol Sci, 2016. 27(11): p. 1507–1516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Qu X, Age-related cognitive task effects on gait characteristics: do different working memory components make a difference? J Neuroeng Rehabil, 2014. 11: p. 149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Sanchez-Cubillo I, et al. , Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: role of taskswitching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J Int Neuropsychol Soc, 2009. 15(3): p. 438–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Blackwood J, et al. , The Impact of a Home-Based Computerized Cognitive Training Intervention on Fall Risk Measure Performance in Community Dwelling Older Adults, a Pilot Study. J Nutr Health Aging, 2016. 20(2): p. 138–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Mirelman A, et al. , Association between performance on Timed Up and Go subtasks and mild cognitive impairment: further insights into the links between cognitive and motor function. J Am Geriatr Soc, 2014. 62(4): p. 673–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Stoffers D, et al. , Deficits on Corsi's block-tapping task in early stage Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord, 2003. 10(2): p. 107–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]