Skip to main content
Bioscience logoLink to Bioscience
. 2018 Oct 10;68(12):990–995. doi: 10.1093/biosci/biy118

Collaboration Matters: Honey Bee Health as a Transdisciplinary Model for Understanding Real-World Complexity

Sainath Suryanarayanan 1,, Daniel Lee Kleinman 2, Claudio Gratton 3, Amy Toth 4, Christelle Guedot 3, Russell Groves 3, John Piechowski 5, Brad Moore 6, Deborah Hagedorn 7, Dayton Kauth 8, Heather Swan 9, Mary Celley 8
PMCID: PMC6278639  PMID: 30524133

Abstract

We develop a transdisciplinary deliberative model that moves beyond traditional scientific collaborations to include nonscientists in designing complexity-oriented research. We use the case of declining honey bee health as an exemplar of complex real-world problems requiring cross-disciplinary intervention. Honey bees are important pollinators of the fruits and vegetables we eat. In recent years, these insects have been dying at alarming rates. To prompt the reorientation of research toward the complex reality in which bees face multiple challenges, we came together as a group, including beekeepers, farmers, and scientists. Over a 2-year period, we deliberated about how to study the problem of honey bee deaths and conducted field experiments with bee colonies. We show trust and authority to be crucial factors shaping such collaborative research, and we offer a model for structuring collaboration that brings scientists and nonscientists together with the key objects and places of their shared concerns across time.

Keywords: complex systems, interdisciplinary science, policy/ethics, stakeholders, honey bee


Researchers increasingly recognize that developing truly effective solutions to real-world problems demands collaborative approaches that cut across the silos of traditional scientific disciplines (National Academy of Sciences 2005). Configuring research collaborations for grappling with complex phenomena requires us to consider whose voices could matter and in what ways. Using the case of declining honey bee health as an exemplar of a complex real-world problem requiring cross-disciplinary intervention, we developed a transdisciplinary deliberative model that moves beyond traditional scientific collaborations to include nonscientists in designing research. Although it is not uncommon for scientists to consider perspectives offered by nonscientist stakeholders on research-related matters (e.g., Goldstein et al. 2012, North-Central Region Sustainable Agriculture Research and Education Grant Program 2018), most scientist–nonscientist consultations are ancillary to the actual processes of experimental knowledge production (Carolan 2008). Our collaboration suggests that an iterative process of facilitated interactions between nonscientists and scientists in conjunction with the actual objects, sites and tools of research can enable nonscientists to offer key methodological and data-interpretive insights.

Burgeoning literature on “the science of team science” (SciTS) and on socioecological systems point to the importance of boundary-spanning collaborations involving multidisciplinary teams of scientists to address complex and urgent societal problems (Börner et al. 2010, Cundill et al. 2015). Much of the SciTS work focuses on collaborations between groups of certified scientists. When nonscientists are considered at all, they are either relegated to data gathering roles or consultative capacities that tend to be removed from key everyday choices and practices related to research questions, methods, and modes of analyses (Carolan 2008). Prevalent models of reconfiguring collaboration implicitly adhere to a deficit model of science literacy in which nonscientist members of the public are assumed to lack relevant knowledge (Sismondo 2010). In some cases, however, nonscientists have actively contributed to advancing understanding of real-world complex phenomena. For example, AIDS treatment activists helped advance a scientifically valid and ethical alternative to double-blind randomized clinical trials for therapeutic interventions (Epstein 1996). Indeed, in multiple instances spanning complex human diseases, livestock and crop management and environmental pollution, nonscientists have demonstrated that despite lacking scientific training or credentials, they can make valuable substantive contributions (Brown and Mikkelsen 1990, Wynne 1992, Epstein 1996). Although these contributions have typically come about in the context of ad hoc collaborations that occurred in the middle of social controversies in which nonscientists have significant stakes, the model we offer is based on an intentionally structured process. We developed a deliberative model that fosters sustained interactions over time between nonscientists and scientists in the context of their joint participation with the actual objects and sites of research concern. In the present article, we report on this process.

Complexity and collaboration in the case of honey bee health

Honey bees (Apis mellifera) are semidomesticated and managed social insects that are the single most important insect species for crop pollination and honey production in the United States (US) and are a key source of livelihood for beekeepers and farmers worldwide. Despite their social and economic importance, there have been reports of elevated colony loss in recent years and declines in the number of managed colonies in some countries, including the United States. A 2015–2016 survey found the honey bee colony loss rate in the United States was 44%, well above annual loss rates prior to 2005–2006 (Kulhanek et al. 2017). Scientists agree that above-average bee deaths are caused by a combination of factors, including pathogens, pesticides (beekeeper and farmer applied), parasitic mites, and poor nutrition, but how these factors interact to cause the “new normal” of honey bee deaths remain unresolved, uncertain and controversial (Grozinger and Evans 2015).

The ongoing phenomenon of elevated honey bee mortality in the United States and elsewhere is a complex real-world problem that cuts across the categories of “biological,” “social,” and “environmental.” Contemporary honey bees are embedded in intertwined networks of human and nonhuman systems interacting across multiple spatial and temporal scales. Indeed, honey bees are not “wild.” Over 90% of all honey bees in the United States are managed by beekeepers, mainly for commercial crop pollination and honey production (Mader et al. 2010). Around 1600 large-scale migratory beekeeping operations, which represent less than 10% of all beekeeping firms in the United States, circulate over 72% of all colonies for pollinating various industrial farming operations in the United States (Daberkow et al. 2009, Burgett et al. 2010).

Because honey bees rely on plant pollen and nectar for their nutrition, agribusinesses and farmers are also implicated in bee health. Apart from these overlapping anthropogenic networks, honey bees are also exposed to dynamic patchworks of landscapes, (agro)chemicals, and other-than-human biotic communities. Therefore, questions about honey bee deaths are not narrowly biological, but are also questions about the political economies and ecologies of beekeeping and agriculture. To understand the problem, we must grapple with the full array of factors and dimensions plausibly involved.

Prevalent research practices in entomological and ecological investigations of honey bee deaths emphasize the precise isolation of the direct effects of individual factors on honey bees over relatively short time frames (Suryanarayanan and Kleinman 2017). In this framework, to draw accurate conclusions about the causal effects of relationships between multiple interacting factors in a replicable manner and with sufficient statistical power would require a very large replicated experiment across broad temporal and spatial scales, and such an approach is practically unfeasible. This has left a crucial knowledge gap in our understanding of the ways in which multiple factors may be interacting across spatial and temporal scales to affect honey bee health (Kleinman and Suryanarayanan 2013). Practical constraints to the established experimental framework suggest the urgent need to develop alternative approaches.

To prompt the reorientation of research toward the complex reality in which honey bees (and other insect pollinators) face multiple challenges, we came together as a group, including beekeepers, farmers, university scientists with various specialties, and for part of our process—a land manager from a federal governmental agency and a nongovernmental conservation group representative joined us (table 1). With the twin aims of facilitating genuine collaboration and fostering alternative research methods to study the complexity of honey bee decline, we undertook four structured daylong deliberations between 2014 and 2016. These deliberations were interlaced with a pair of field experiments centering on honey bees, which served as conduits for developing shared methodologies that would draw on the varieties of expertise of the participants (figure 1).

Table 1.

Stakeholders enrolled in the study.

Beekeepers Pollinator-reliant growers University scientists Policymakers
Migratory pollination and honey production (3) Large-scale monocrop fruit (1) Honey bee and social insect biologist (1) US Fish and and Wildlife Services (1)
Stationary honey production (2) Midscale vegetable production (1) Landscape ecologist (1) Nongovernmental conservation group (1)
Vegetable extension entomologist (1)
Fruit extension entomologist (1)

Note: The numbers in brackets denote the number of individual participants representing the stakeholder group.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Timeline and stages of the process for structuring transdisciplinary collaboration. The graphical summary and timeline of the process undertaken to build transdisciplinary collaboration including goals and/or accomplishments in each stage of the process. Four structured daylong deliberations involving various scientific and nonscientific stakeholders were carried out between 2014 and 2016. These deliberations were interlaced with a pair of field experiments centering on honey bees in 2014 and 2015.

In the initial deliberation, scientists and nonscientists drew attention to two enduring issues: the lack of diverse landscapes for foraging honey bees in heavily monocropped agricultural areas and potentially problematic management practices such as patterns of pesticide usage among farmers and beekeepers. In our second discussion, we collectively designed a honey bee field study. From the summer to the fall of 2014, we collected an array of field data from sixteen honey bee colonies distributed evenly across four field sites in Central Wisconsin with relatively high and low agricultural intensities (see supplemental figure S1). The agricultural intensity of each site was categorized with the help of collaborating beekeepers’ and farmers’ knowledge of these locales and complemented with remote-satellite data of the proportion of cropland in the 0.5–3 miles radius—the maximum range of foraging honey bees (Mader et al. 2010)—surrounding each site. Given the exploratory nature of the first field experiment, we decided to gather multiple measures of honey bee health including estimates of adult population size, amounts of pupae and immature brood, levels of stored pollen and nectar, pathogen and parasite loads and pesticide residues in comb pollen across five time points.

On the basis of the third deliberation, in which we discussed the results of the first field study, we carried out a second field experiment in the summer of 2015, which continued the comparison between honey bee health in more and less agriculturally intensive sites, this time at eight field sites, and with key changes in study design and measures initiated by the participating nonscientists. The purpose of the comparison was to identify ways to understand the relationships between landscape features, agricultural practices and beekeeping practices rather than looking at each one of these factors in isolation. The group met one last time in 2016 to consider the second field study results and the merits of a place-based approach to developing research and policy on honey bee health.

Throughout the deliberative process, beekeepers and farmers demonstrated not only their capacity to grasp complex conceptual, methodological, data and statistical issues, but also the ability to problematize and contribute to scientists’ understandings and approaches. For example, nonscientists drew on their practical knowledge of cranberry pollination to explain sources of variability that were construed as “noise” by one of the scientists, who showed a graph depicting a linear statistical relationship between cranberry yield and number of honey bee colonies per acre. Beekeepers and growers pointed out agronomic characteristics of particular cranberry marshes such as the surrounding habitat and cranberry variety, as well as the proximity of honey bee colonies placed on neighboring cranberry marshes, as sources of variability in the relationship between number of honey bee colonies and cranberry yield.

However, in the first two deliberations, there was little substantive contribution from nonscientists due, we believe, to a lack of trust and authority differentials between the different stakeholder groups represented by the participants. Beekeepers and farmers, for example, were skeptical of each others’ patterns of pesticide use and the veracity of claims regarding harmful effects on honey bee health. Similarly, small-scale stationary beekeepers blamed migratory crop-pollinating beekeepers of becoming vectors of honey bee pathogens, whereas the latter criticized the former for not effectively managing for parasitic mites and allowing their colonies to become carriers facilitating the spread of mites. In the initial discussions around the appropriate design of the first field experiment, beekeepers and farmers largely deferred to the scientists, who promoted a traditional approach, where the effects of each factor could be examined in isolation and compared to the effects of each combination.

These social dynamics shifted in subtle yet significant ways after the first field study. In contrast to the deliberations before the first field study, it was the nonscientists who initiated key methodological innovations during the third deliberation, where we discussed the design of the second field study. The results of the first field experiment (see the supplemental material)—the most striking of which was that only two out of eight colonies survived in the highly agriculturally intensive sites compared to five out of eight colonies in the less agriculturally intensive sites (see supplemental table S1)—led the nonscientists, especially beekeepers, to express significant concern about the methods of data collection in the first field experiment. The scientists had pushed for more rigorous and extensive data sampling, which, in practice, meant that each colony was open for at least 25 minutes. For the beekeepers, this was inordinately long and overly invasive, with potentially negative ramifications for colony health. In deploying these critiques and in suggesting rapid, less-invasive data collection methods of gauging colony health, beekeepers drew on alternative modes of observation and measurement. These included attention to smell and sound (e.g., a high-pitched noise emanating from the colony suggesting that it is queenless) and visual measures (e.g., a colony's “brood pattern”). Using such beekeeper modes of observation, one of the beekeepers noted that colonies he was managing commercially (but not for experimental purposes), which were at the same four field sites of the first field study, performed much better than the experimental ones. On this basis, the nonscientists proposed a key shift in the experimental design for the second field experiment—to have a pair of colonies at each site, one that would be exposed to intensive commercial beekeeping management and the other not. The results of the second field experiment (see supplemental figure S6) while not meeting the widely used statistical p-value threshold of .05, suggested a trend: more intensive beekeeping management practices seemed to make a bigger impact on parasite and pathogen loads in places with higher intensity of agriculture than in places with lower intensity agriculture. Nonscientists also initiated discussions during the deliberation that drew on their deep knowledge of local features of the places where colonies were located to explain sources of variability in the data.

The study's breakthrough in the quality of participation by nonscientists was the result of shifts in the tenuous dynamics of authority and trust between the participants, which we believe were enabled by the participants’ sustained interactions over time with each other and with the honey bee colonies in the field experiments. Meetings were structured longitudinally over a span of 2 years and facilitated in ways that sought to encourage input from nonscientists. For example, some of the deliberations were held in a beekeeping operation and in a farming operation, enabling participating scientists to better appreciate nonscientists’ varieties of knowledge, practices, and constraints. Beyond the daylong meetings, the honey bee field experiments required participants to interact over time not only with each other in the processes of design, implementation and analysis, but also with honey bee colonies and field sites.

The process was iterative, allowing for adjustments to be made on the basis of the overlapping practical experiences of both scientists and nonscientists, with time to build relationships and trust. Participating over time in the field experiments allowed the nonscientists, especially beekeepers, to experience the choices, challenges and practices of experimental research and provided them with opportunities to share their own experientially based knowledge and data-related insights about honey bee colonies. The field experiments intertwined with the daylong meetings thus sensitized the nonscientists and scientists to each others’ understandings, enabling them to relate to the research meaningfully and gave the nonscientists a shared sense of creative ownership of the research.

A new model

Our venture to enhance collaborations between scientists, beekeepers and farmers offers a model for future field experiments in honey bee research that could arguably allow for better understandings of the complex biotic, abiotic and societal matrices shaping the health and decline of honey bees. In contrast to the originally conceptualized experiment, in such field experiments, what counts as signs of health and disease and how to count these would be assessed jointly through integrated approaches, observations, and interpretations of participating beekeepers, farmers and scientists. Although experimental designs would be structured to eliminate artifactual results, they would not narrowly adhere to arbitrary thresholds such as the statistical p-value of .05, which constrains efforts to capture subtle yet plausibly important interactive effects, and which is congruent with the emerging recognition of the widespread overreliance on p-values in experimental biology (Wasserstein and Lazar 2016).

Beyond honey bee health, the ultimate breakthrough of our study lies in its offering a mechanism for enhancing the social dynamics of trust and authority across scientist–nonscientist boundaries in contexts of making knowledge that are marked by high levels of uncertainty, complexity and public concern. Historically established dynamics of trust and authority between various nonscientist stakeholders and scientists do not change overnight, and the breakthrough we achieved required careful attention to the spatiotemporal structure and process of collaboration. Dynamics of trust, power and authority are absolutely crucial in shaping the success or failure of any knowledge production enterprise.

Although our initiative did not generate novel or actionable substantive biological findings, by demonstrating the social conditions necessary for carrying out genuinely collaborative research between scientists and nonscientists, our study offers a template (figure 1) for teams of scientists and nonscientist stakeholders to co-organize transdisciplinary experiments that might potentially lead to new insights into complex real-world problems. In our study of scientist–nonscientist collaboration in the case of honey bee health, participating scientists realized that they could not have gotten to the second field experiment without learning from beekeepers and farmers about how the latter see their particular situations. Participating nonscientists, on the other hand, increasingly felt as if they had something valuable to offer in terms of methodology and data analysis.

Our study demonstrates that nonscientists who stand to be primarily affected by the research can offer valuable methodological and other insights in investigations of complex real-world phenomena. Their involvement can complement and change the research questions, methods and interpretive frames used by scientists to answer them. Furthermore, our deliberative “experiment” provides a process-based model for enabling substantive contributions by nonscientists to knowledge making along real-world and complexity-oriented lines (figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Transdisciplinary deliberative model for collaborative research on complex phenomena. This process-based model for enabling substantive contributions by nonscientists entails not only sustained interactions over time between scientists and nonscientists, but also shared interactions with the actual research objects of concern.

Conclusions

An increasing number of governmental funding agencies and research institutions are recognizing the value of reorganizing collaborative research in ways that transcend disciplinary silos and individual-investigator-oriented reward structures (e.g., see National Institutes of Health Funding Opportunity 2018). Our study suggests the value of extending collaboration beyond scientists, at least in cases where primarily affected nonscientists possess expertise to contribute to understandings of complex real-world phenomena. Importantly, our proposal is not relegated to scientists consulting with nonscientist stakeholders. Rather, our model proposes structuring collaboration across time in a way that brings scientists and nonscientists together with the key objects and places of their shared concerns, thus setting the stage for creating new complexity-oriented knowledge.

Supplementary Material

Supplemental Files

Acknowledgments

We thank the anonymous reviewers for providing useful feedback. Emily Duerr extracted and compiled geospatial data on crops surrounding the field sites. Mark Evans and Kirsten King helped gather data and samples from honey bee colonies in 2014. Krysta Koralesky, Gbemisola Famule, Diane Nhieu, and Heide Knoppke-Wetzel analyzed stored honey bee bodies and comb samples for mites, Nosema, and pesticide levels. This research was supported by National Science Foundation (award no. 1257175). Suryanarayanan was also partially supported by National Institutes of Health Grants U54AI117924 to the University of Wisconsin Center for Predictive Computational Phenotyping and UL1TR000427 to the University of Wisconsin–Madison Institute for Clinical and Translational Research.

Author Biographical

Sainath Suryanarayanan (ssuryanaraya@wisc.edu) is affiliated with the Population Health Institute and the Nelson Institute for Environmental Studies in the University of Wisconsin–Madison. Daniel Lee Kleinman is with the Department of Sociology in Boston University. Claudio Gratton, Russ Groves and Christelle Guedot are in the Department of Entomology in the University of Wisconsin–Madison. Amy Toth is in the Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Organismal Biology and the Department of Entomology at Iowa State University in Ames, Iowa. John Piechowski is with Henry's Honey Farm in Redgranite, Wisconsin. Brad Moore is with B's Honey in Mazomanie, Wisconsin. Debbie Hagedorn works with Heath Farms in Coloma, Wisconsin. Dayton Kauth and Mary Celley are beekeepers. Heather Swan is in the Department of English at the University of Wisconsin–Madison.

References cited

  1. Börner K, Contractor N, Falk-Krzesinski HJ, Fiore SM, Hall KL, Keyton J, Spring B, Stokols D, Trochim W, Uzzi B. 2010. A multi-level systems perspective for the science of team science. Science Translational Medicine 2: 1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Brown P, Mikkelsen EJ. 1990. No Safe Place: Toxic Waste, Leukemia and Community Action University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  3. Burgett M, Daberkow S, Rucker R, Thurman W. 2010. US pollination markets: Recent changes and historical perspectives. American Bee Journal 150: 35–41. [Google Scholar]
  4. Carolan MS. 2008. Democratizing knowledge: Sustainable and conventional agricultural field days as divergent democratic forms. Science, Technology and Human Values 33: 508–528. [Google Scholar]
  5. Cundill G, Roux DJ, Parker JN. 2015. Nurturing communities of practice for transdisciplinary research. Ecology and Society 20: 22. [Google Scholar]
  6. Daberkow S, Korb P, Hoff F. 2009. The structure of the US beekeeping industry. Journal of Economic Entomology 102: 868–886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Epstein S. 1996. Impure Science: AIDS, Activism, and the Politics of Knowledge. University of California Press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Goldstein JH, Caldarone G, Duarte TK, Ennaanay D, Hannahs N, Mendoza G, Polasky S, Wolny S, Daily GC. 2012. Integrating ecosystem-service tradeoffs into land-use decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109: 7565–7570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Grozinger CM, Evans JD. 2015. Social insects: From the lab to the landscape - translational approaches to pollinator health. Current Opinion in Insect Science 10: vii–ix. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Kleinman DL, Suryanarayanan S. 2013. Dying bees and the social production of ignorance. Science, Technology and Human Values 38: 492–517. [Google Scholar]
  11. Kulhanek K, et al. 2017. A national survey of managed honey bee 2015–2016 annual colony losses in the USA. Journal of Apicultural Research 56: 328–340. [Google Scholar]
  12. Mader E, Spivak M, Evans E. 2010. Managing Alternative Pollinators: A Handbook For Growers, Beekeepers and Conservationists. Natural Resource, Agriculture and Engineering Service Press. [Google Scholar]
  13. National Academy of Sciences 2005. Facilitating Interdisciplinary Research. National Academies Press; www.nap.edu/catalog/11153/facilitating-interdisciplinary-research [Google Scholar]
  14. National Institutes of Health Funding Opportunity 2018. https://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/pa-files/PAR-13-305.html
  15. North-Central Region Sustainable Agriculture Research and Education Grant Program 2018. (16 September 2016; www.northcentralsare.org/Grants/Our-Grant-Programs/Research-and-Education)
  16. Wasserstein RL, Lazar NA. 2016. The ASA's statement on p-values: Context, process, and purpose. The American Statistician 70: 129–133. [Google Scholar]
  17. Sismondo S. 2010. The public understanding of science. 168–179 in Sismondo S. ed. An Introduction to Science and Technology Studies 2nd edition Blackwell Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  18. Suryanarayanan S, Kleinman DL. 2017. Vanishing Bees: Science, Politics and Honeybee Health. Rutgers University Press. [Google Scholar]
  19. Wynne B. 1992. Misunderstood misunderstandings: Social identities and public update of science. Public Understanding of Science 1: 281–304. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Files

Articles from Bioscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES