Abstract
High levels of trait impulsivity are considered a risk factor for substance abuse and drug addiction. We recently found that non-planning trait impulsivity was negatively correlated with post-commissural putamen volumes in men, but not women, using the Karolinska Scales of Personality (KSP). Here, we attempted to replicate this finding in an independent sample using an updated version of the KSP: the Swedish Universities Scales of Personality (SSP). Data from 88 healthy male participants (Mean Age: 28.16±3.34), who provided structural T1-weighted magnetic resonance images (MRIs) and self-reported SSP impulsivity scores, were analyzed. Striatal sub-region volumes were acquired using the Multiple Automatically Generated Templates (MAGeT-Brain) algorithm. Contrary to our previous findings trait impulsivity measured using SSP was not a significant predictor of post-commissural putamen volumes (β = .14, df = 84, p = .94). A replication Bayes Factors analysis strongly supported this null result. Consistent with our previous findings, secondary exploratory analyses found no relationship between ventral striatum volumes and SSP trait impulsivity (β = -.05, df = 84, p = .28). An exploratory analysis of the other striatal compartments showed that there were no significant associations with trait impulsivity. While we could not replicate our previous findings in the current sample, we believe this work will aide future studies aimed at establishing meaningful brain biomarkers for addiction vulnerability in healthy humans.
Introduction
Heightened impulsivity is considered both a risk factor for, and a consequence of, chronic substance abuse and drug addiction [1–3]. Impulsivity is a multidimensional construct, encompassing impulsive choice, impulsive action, and self-reported impulsive personality traits [4]. While studies have not observed strong relationships between measures of impulsive choice and impulsive action, both are weakly correlated with measures of trait impulsivity [4–6]. Thus, elucidating the neural correlates of trait impulsivity may help inform models of drug addiction vulnerability [7, 8].
Several in vivo positron emission tomography (PET) studies have examined the neurochemical correlates of trait impulsivity in humans [9], with an emphasis on the striatal dopamine (DA) system [10]. Lower DA D2/3 receptor (D2/3R) availability, particularly in the ventral striatum (VS), has been associated with both drug-use and impulsivity in animals [11–15] and in people with drug addiction [16–18]. However, whether higher trait impulsivity is related to lower D2/3R availability in the VS of healthy humans remains unclear. While studies have differed in terms of the radiotracers and personality scales employed, there have been reports of no correlations [19–21], positive correlations [22, 23], as well as negative correlations [24, 25] between striatal D2/3R availability and impulsivity. These mixed findings do not lend strong support to the hypothesis that VS D2/3R availability may be a link between impulsivity and drug addiction [10].
We recently observed a negative correlation between D2/3R availability in the VS and trait impulsivity, measured using the Karolinska Scales of Personality (KSP), in healthy people [24]. Since lower D2/3R availability in the VS has been related to smaller VS volumes in rodents [26] and humans [27, 28], we explored whether trait impulsivity measured with the KSP was related to VS volumes in healthy humans [29]. While trait impulsivity was not significantly associated with VS volumes, in a post-hoc analysis we observed a significant sex-interaction in the post-commissural putamen. Specifically, greater impulsivity was associated with smaller post-commissural putamen volumes in males, but not in females [29].
Overall, studies examining the relationship between impulsivity and striatal volumes in healthy persons have provided mixed results [30–33]. These studies have employed various measures, sample sizes, and age ranges. Regarding impulsive choice (i.e. delay discounting), negative correlations with post-commissural putamen volumes (n = 34) [31], positive correlations with caudate volumes (n = 70) [32], and positive correlations with VS volumes in adolescents (n = 1830) [33] have been observed. Also, trait negative urgency (the tendency to act rashly under extreme negative emotions) has been related to smaller left ventral striatum volumes after controlling for other potentially confounding traits: neuroticism (negative emotionality), sensation seeking, and lack of planning and perseverance (n = 152) [30]. While our observed sex-interaction between post-commissural putamen volumes and trait impulsivity was unexpected [29], this work warranted replication for several reasons. First, if the effect is robust, it could potentially reconcile previously reported mixed-findings in the literature between trait impulsivity and striatal volume. Second, it could further inform biological factors underlying sex-differences in substance abuse vulnerability.
In the current investigation, we attempted to replicate our previous findings between trait impulsivity and post-commissural putamen volumes in a larger sample of healthy Swedish men, using the Swedish Universities Scale of Personality (SSP) [34], an updated version of the KSP. Specifically, our a priori hypothesis was that trait impulsivity would be negatively correlated with post-commissural putamen volumes in healthy men. To help us further interpret our a priori findings, secondary exploratory analyses were conducted examining potential laterality effects as well as potential relationships between trait impulsivity and VS volumes. Collectively, this work will help to further clarify how striatal morphology may relate to trait impulsivity in humans.
Materials and methods
Participants
Data from 88 healthy male participants was included in this replication study (Table 1). Specifically, structural T1-weighted images and self-report measures of trait impulsivity data were pooled from various PET studies [35–38]. All studies, and their design, were approved by the Regional Ethics Committee in Stockholm and the Karolinska University Hospital Radiation Safety Committee. All subjects gave written informed consent prior to participating according to the Helsinki declaration.
Table 1. Participant demographics (n = 88, male).
Age | Trait Impulsivity– SSP |
Total Brain Volume |
Post-Commissural Putamen Volume | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean ±S.D. | 28.16 ±3.34 |
50.84 ±9.29 |
1584685.86 ±101878.52 |
3781.23 ±372.50 |
Range: | 21.32–34.96 | 29.48–78.85 | 1369636–1872065 | 2957–4751 |
Structural data for 76 subjects was acquired on a 1.5 T GE Signa system (Milwaukee, WI) (hereafter termed Scanner 1) and for 12 subjects on a 1.5T Siemens Magnetom Avanto system (Erlangen, Germany) (hereafter termed Scanner 2). Exclusion criteria for all subjects included historical or present episode of psychiatric illness, alcohol or drug abuse, major somatic illness, or habitual use of nicotine as determined by a physical and psychiatric examination by a physician.
Swedish Universities Scales of Personality (SSP)
All participants completed either the KSP196 questionnaire (N = 54) or the Swedish Universities Scales of Personality (SSP) (N = 34) [34]. KSP196 and SSP are updated versions of the original KSP [39] employed in our previous studies [24, 29]. The SSP shows improved psychometric properties, as well as updated normative data for healthy samples [34]. The SSP impulsivity scale was derived from the KSP196 impulsivity scale. This measure of trait impulsivity denotes the degree to which subjects’ report that they act on the spur of the moment (non-planning impulsivity). Responses are made on a four-point Likert scale (“does not apply” to “applies completely”), and includes statements such as, “I have a tendency to act on the spur of the moment without really thinking ahead.” Notably, the impulsivity subscale on the KSP and KSP196 includes 10-items (Cronbach's α = .68), while the SSP includes 7 items showing slightly higher internal reliability (Cronbach's α = .73) [34]. In the creation of the SSP impulsivity scale, three of the original items were removed: KSP-20, KSP-62 and KSP-113. The phrasing of two other items was also slightly changed (KSP-8/SSP-5 and KSP-68/SSP-44). In general, the reasons for the rephrasing was to enhance clarity and replace outdated words/phrasing. The impulsivity scales of the KSP and SSP are highly correlated with one another (r = 0.89, p < 1 x 10−15), based on data from [40].
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)
The parameters for scanner 1 were as follows: T1-weighted imaging, TE = 5 ms, TR = 20 ms, 3D, 256 x 256, voxel-size 1 mm isotropic. The parameters for scanner 2 were as follows: T1-weighted imaging, TE = 3.53 ms, TR = 1.79 seconds, 3D, 256 x 256, voxel-size 1 mm isotropic.
Subcortical volume analyses
The Multiple Automatically Generated Templates (MAGeT-Brain) algorithm [41, 42] was employed to provide fully-automated segmentation of striatal subdivisions (Fig 1) [43]. The delineation of the striatal subdivisions based on serial histological data, and the Collin27 Brain atlas (http://www.bic.mni.mcgill.ca/ServicesAtlases/Colin27), has been described in detail elsewhere (https://github.com/CobraLab/atlases) [43]. These include the pre-commissural caudate, post-commissural caudate, pre-commissural putamen, post-commissural putamen, and the VS. Briefly, the putamen (and caudate) can be divided based on the position of the anterior commissure as seen on the coronal plane. The portion of the putamen located anteriorly to the coronal location of the anterior commissure is demarcated as pre-commissural. The remaining portions of the putamen posterior to the anterior commissure, is considered post-commissural [44, 45]. Several studies have been conducted to validate the reliability of MAGeT-Brain against “gold-standard” manual segmentation–the correlation between methods in the striatum have been reported to be around r = .92 (p = .0001) with a DiceKappa of 0.861 [41, 42, 46–48]. Typically, in a multi-atlas segmentation approach, manually drawn labels from atlases are warped (or propagated) into native subject space by applying transformations estimated from non-linear image registration. Candidate labels from all atlas images are fused (via probabilistic segmentation techniques) to create a final segmentation. The goal of the MAGeT-Brain algorithm is to mitigate sources of error from regular multi-atlas segmentation approaches, including: 1) spurious non-linear registration or resampling errors (including partial volume effects in label resampling), and 2) irreconcilable differences in neuroanatomy between the atlas and target images. The MAGeT-Brain algorithm is a modified multi-atlas segmentation technique, which employs a limited number of high-quality manually segmented atlases as an input to reduce bias and enhance segmentation accuracy. MAGeT-Brain propagates atlas segmentations to a template library, formed from a subset of target images, via transformations estimated by nonlinear image registration. The resulting segmentations are then propagated to each target image and fused using a label fusion method.
Templates images for Scanner 1 (n = 21) and Scanner 2 (n = 11) were chosen as random sub-samples of the full-samples. These randomly selected cases were used as a template library through which the final segmentation was bootstrapped. Each subject in the template library was segmented through non-linear atlas-to-template registration followed by label propagation, yielding a unique definition of the subdivisions for each of the templates. The bootstrapping of the final segmentations through the template library produces candidate labels for each subject, and the labels are then fused using a majority vote to complete the segmentation process. Since this is a majority vote process, to avoid potential “ties” an odd number of template images were employed. Non-linear registration was performed using a version of the Automatic Normalization Tools (ANTS) registration technique [49] that is compatible with the minc toolkit (https://github.com/vfonov/mincANTS).
The effects of using multiple input atlases, varying the size of the template library constructed, has been rigorously examined for MAGeT-Brain previously [42]. Given the high computational demands of multi-atlas techniques, the MAGeT-Brain method is predicated on the finding that a useful template library can be generated from a small set of labelled atlas images [42]. While increasing the number of templates used improves the MAGeT-Brain segmentation, using even a smaller number of atlases (as low as n = 21 for samples ≥22) greatly improves the overlap between manually generated “gold standard” segmentations and automatically generated segmentations [46, 50]. Thus, while we have used smaller template libraries than the full sample, our segmentations are improved compared to other automated segmentation approaches [46].
Volumes (mm3) from ROIs were averaged across hemispheres. Compared to other automated techniques such as FreeSurfer and FSL, MAGeT-Brain demonstrates the highest correlation with gold-standard manual segmentation techniques, while FreeSurfer and FSL significantly overestimate subcortical volumes compared to MAGeT-Brain [46]. Quality control by visual inspection was carried out by authors FC and EP to ensure that, 1) there were no major artifacts in the original T1 images, and, 2) no anomalies in the labelling of the subcortical structures by examining for each subject the original subject image with the resulting labelled image.
Total brain volume analysis
Total brain volume (TBV) was obtained using the Brain Extraction based on non-local Segmentation Technique (BEaST) method [51]. This method is based on non-local segmentation in a multi-resolution framework. Each voxel is labeled based on the similarity of its neighborhood of voxels to all the neighborhoods in a library of pre-defined priors, and a non-local means estimator is used to estimate the label at the voxel. Inputs are down-sampled to a lower resolution, segmentation is performed, and results are propagated up to higher resolutions [51]. BEaST is designed to include CSF (in the ventricles, cerebellar cistern, deep sulci, along surface of brain, and brainstem), the brainstem, and cerebellar white matter (WM) and gray matter (GM) in the brain mask, while excluding the skull, skin, fat, muscles, dura, eyes, bone, exterior blood vessels, and exterior nerves.
Statistical analysis
We conducted a priori as well as complementary secondary exploratory analyses. For our a priori analysis, the hypotheses tested were:
H1: A negative relationship between post-commissural putamen volume and SSP impulsivity.
H0: No relationship between post-commissural putamen volume and SSP impulsivity.
We employed a linear-mixed-effects (LME) model, taking the hierarchical structure (subjects belonging to two different scanner groups) into account. The post-commissural putamen volumes were specified as the dependent variable, SSP impulsivity as the independent variable, and TBV and age as co-variates. Scanner condition (‘Scanner 1’ or ‘Scanner 2’) was specified as a random effect, allowing the intercepts to vary. All continuous variables of interest were standardized (z-scored) before being entered into the statistical model. We examined that the assumptions of linear regression were not violated our analyses. Alpha for this a priori test was set to 0.05 (one-sided expecting a negative relationship).
A p-value in-and-of itself is often a poor metric for assessing the success of a replication attempt, since the difference between a “significant” and a “non-significant” p-value is not necessarily meaningful [52]. For this reason, a statistical procedure known as the replication Bayes Factor (BF) [53] was also employed. A BF quantifies the relative strength of evidence in favor of two hypotheses by computing the predicative adequacy of H1 over H0 relative to one another. For the correlation replication BF specifically, H1 is defined as the posterior distribution of the correlation coefficient from the original study, assuming that a uniform prior was employed, and H0 is defined as a point null hypothesis of no effect [54]. Correlation coefficients were obtained by converting the test-statistics of the original finding (KSP Impulsivity, r = -.62) and the test-statistics from the abovementioned LME model to correlation coefficients [55]. The BF was calculated using the Savage-Dickey ratio [56]. A BF above 3 for H1 (BF10 > 3 or BF01 < 1/3) is commonly interpreted as providing moderate evidence for a successful replication, and a BF above 3 for H0 (BF01 > 3 or BF10 < 1/3) as moderate evidence for a failed replication. A BF above 10 indicates strong evidence in favor of one hypothesis (H1 or H0), over the other.
We also conducted secondary post hoc exploratory analyses to help inform our a priori findings. Specifically, we explored whether there were relationships between the left versus the right post-commissural putamen volumes and SSP impulsivity. Finally, in accordance with the previously reviewed literature, we examined whether VS volumes were related to SSP impulsivity (average ROI, as well as left and right ROIs separately). All statistical modelling was carried out using R (v.3.3.2).
Results
A priori analysis
Contrary to our previous findings [29], trait impulsivity measured by the SSP was not a significant predictor of post-commissural putamen volumes (β = .14, df = 84, p = .94) (Table 2 & Fig 2). Table 3 outlines the beta-weights compared between the previous publication [29], the current findings, as well as the replication BF. The replication BF in favor of H0 (a failed replication) was 607.8 and inversely, the replication BF in favor of H1 (a successful replication) was 0.0016. Hence, replication BFs showed that the data was over 607 times more likely to have occurred under H0, compared to under that of the posterior distribution of the original study, i.e. a strong negative relationship between trait impulsivity and post-commissural putamen volume (Fig 3).
Table 2. Simple liner-mixed effects (LME) model predicting post-commissural putamen volumes.
Variables | Estimate (β) | t-value | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Independent: | ||||
Trait Impulsivity–SSP | .14 | 1.60 | .94 | |
Covariates: | ||||
Age | -.33 | -3.91 | .0002 | |
Total Brain Volume | .49 | 5.65 | .0000002 |
All variables of interest were standardized (z-scored) before entering the statistical model.
Table 3. Data used for calculating replication Bayes function.
Study | Beta (β) | Degrees of Freedom | p-value† | BF10 | BF01 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Original | -.62 | 27 | .0003 | - | - |
Current | .14 | 84 | .94 | .0016 | 607.8 |
†The original analysis was two-tailed. The current analysis was a one-tailed test in the direction of the original study.
Post hoc exploratory analyses
First, excluding subjects imaged with Scanner 2 (n = 12) did not significantly change our a priori null findings (β = .16, df = 72, p = .96). Second, trait impulsivity measured by the SSP was not a significant predictor of either left (β = .12, df = 84, p = .92) or right (β = .15, df = 84, p = .96) post-commissural putamen volumes (Table 4). Further exploratory analyses demonstrated that trait impulsivity measured by the SSP was also not a significant predictor of VS volumes (Table 5). A further exploration of all the other striatal subdivisions is presented in Table 6 (two-tailed tests). Notably, pre-commissural caudate volumes were negatively associated with trait impulsivity measured with the SSP (β = -.20, t = -2.15, p = .03). However, this relationship did not survive Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (corrected p-threshold = .006).
Table 4. Results of secondary post hoc analyses exploring potential effects of laterality.
Region of Interest | Variables | Estimate (β) | t-value | p-value† | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Left Post-Commissural Putamen | |||||
Independent: | |||||
Trait Impulsivity—SSP | .12 | 1.43 | .92 | ||
Covariates: | |||||
Age | -.33 | -3.91 | p < .0001 | ||
Total Brain Volume | .49 | 5.68 | p < .0001 | ||
Right Post-Commissural Putamen | |||||
Independent: | |||||
Trait Impulsivity—SSP | .15 | 1.71 | .96 | ||
Covariates: | |||||
Age | -.33 | -3.73 | p < .0001 | ||
Total Brain Volume | .47 | 5.35 | p < .0001 |
†Results are reported as one-tailed tests.
Table 5. Results of secondary post hoc analyses exploring potential relationships between trait impulsivity and ventral striatum volumes.
Region of Interest | Variables | Estimate (β) | t-value | p-value† | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ventral Striatum | |||||
Independent: | |||||
Trait Impulsivity—SSP | -.05 | -.58 | .28 | ||
Covariates: | |||||
Age | -.21 | -2.34 | .02 | ||
Total Brain Volume | .48 | 5.21 | .000001 | ||
Left Ventral Striatum | |||||
Independent: | |||||
Trait Impulsivity—SSP | -.04 | -.41 | .34 | ||
Covariates: | |||||
Age | -.25 | -2.77 | .01 | ||
Total Brain Volume | .48 | 5.24 | .001 | ||
Right Ventral Striatum | |||||
Independent: | |||||
Trait Impulsivity—SSP | -.05 | -.55 | .29 | ||
Covariates: | |||||
Age | -.19 | -1.91 | .06 | ||
Total Brain Volume | .45 | 4.71 | .001 |
†Results are reported as one-tailed tests.
Table 6. Results of secondary post hoc analyses exploring potential relationships between trait impulsivity and all the remaining striatal subdivisions.
Region of Interest | Variables | Estimate (β) | t-value | p-value† | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pre-Commissural Putamen | |||||
Independent: | |||||
Trait Impulsivity—SSP | -0.05 | -0.58 | .56 | ||
Covariates: | |||||
Age | -0.21 | -2.34 | .02 | ||
Total Brain Volume | 0.48 | 5.21 | >.0001 | ||
Pre-Commissural Caudate | |||||
Independent: | |||||
Trait Impulsivity—SSP | -0.20 | -2.15 | .03 | ||
Covariates: | |||||
Age | -0.34 | -3.66 | .0004 | ||
Total Brain Volume | 0.33 | 3.60 | .0005 | ||
Post-Commissural Caudate | |||||
Independent: | |||||
Trait Impulsivity—SSP | -0.10 | -1.18 | 0.24 | ||
Covariates: | |||||
Age | -0.49 | -5.82 | >.0001 | ||
Total Brain Volume | 0.34 | 3.95 | .0002 |
†Results are reported as two-tailed tests.
Discussion
Determining the neural correlates of impulsivity in healthy persons may help inform biological markers of drug addiction vulnerability. Research on how trait impulsivity may be related to striatal neurochemistry and morphology have yielded mixed results [30–33]. In the current investigation, we attempted to replicate our previous observation that higher trait impulsivity was related to smaller post-commissural putamen volumes in healthy men [29]. Contrary to our previous findings, we observed no significant relationship between trait impulsivity and post-commissural putamen volumes in a larger, independent sample of healthy males. Rather, we found strong evidence in favor of a failed replication: the data were over 607 times more likely to have occurred under the null hypothesis of no effect than they were under the outcome of the original study. Furthermore, in line with our initial study, our exploratory analyses did not show a significant relationship between trait impulsivity and VS volumes in healthy males.
It is difficult to definitively interpret the results of a failed replication, and there were several differences between the studies which could be speculated to have led to the differing results. First, the original investigation employed a statistically significantly older sample than the current investigation (Mean Age: 32.13±9.13 versus 28.16±3.34; t = 3.49, df = 117, p = .0007). Thus, it is possible that trait impulsivity may be negatively correlated with post-commissural putamen volumes in older (≥34 years of age) but not younger subjects. However, we are currently unaware of any evidence a priori to support this potential interpretation. Thus, we do not suspect that this age difference significantly accounts for the observed discrepancy between studies. Second, the current investigation employed an updated scale measuring trait impulsivity compared to the original investigation. However, the KSP and SSP Impulsivity scales are in theory meant to measure the same construct [34]. In a separate sample of 304 individuals who filled out both the KSP and SSP [40], the correlation between the two measures was found to be very high. It is therefore highly unlikely that the removal of three out of ten items, which was done to improve the reliability, could explain the different results. Moreover, it cannot be fully excluded that differences in genetic, cultural, and socioeconomic measures between the Canadian and Swedish samples could have lead to discrepancies between studies, and these factors should be taken into consideration in future investigations. Finally, different sequence parameters for the T1-image acquisition were employed between the previous study and the current investigation. Since these parameters (TR, TE, flip angle) influence image contrast, it is possible that there were slight differences in tissue classification and segmentation between studies [57], which in turn may have contributed to our differing results. While this potential effect can not be directly examined by our current data, it is important to note that the MAGeT-brain method shows both a high degree of test-retest reliability and congruence with “gold-standard” manual segmentation techniques [46].
There are several strengths and weaknesses associated with the current investigation. First, this study employed a substantially larger sample of healthy males compared to our initial study (n = 88 versus n = 31). Second, we employed an improved measure of trait impulsivity compared to the original investigation. However, like our original investigation, this study was retrospective. Therefore, the influence of other important demographic and psychological measures relevant to trait impulsivity and striatal morphology could not be investigated. For example, several lines of evidence suggest that motivational deficits may be related to both striatal dopaminergic functioning and striatal morphology in healthy persons [58–61] and persons with neuropsychiatric diseases [61–63]. Therefore, future studies should examine the potential interactions between trait impulsivity and motivational functioning on striatal morphology; for instance, using behavioural measures specifically designed to dissociate these related traits and behaviours [64, 65]. Moreover, there may be other participant differences–such as lifestyle factors and other personality traits (e.g. social desirability)–which may have lead to the differing results between the Toronto and Karolinska samples. Unfortunately, such influences cannot be readily determined from the data collected from both samples. Finally, our investigation only examined male participants. Future studies should examine the relationship between trait impulsivity and striatal morphology in larger samples of both healthy men and women.
We attempted to replicate a previously observed negative association between trait impulsivity and post-commissural putamen volumes in healthy males, finding strong evidence in favor of a failed replication. Especially in neuroscience, there is a growing need for more replication attempts in larger independent samples before strong research claims can be made [66, 67]. Neuroscience as a field particularly suffers from a lack of replicability for several reasons. First, surveys suggest neuroscience studies in general are underpowered [67]. Low statistical power overestimates the true effect sizes of observed findings, resulting in these findings being less likely to be reproduced. While unexpected findings from smaller samples–like in our original study (n = 31)–can point to true, strong effects [68], replication in multiple, larger samples is the only means of validating these findings [69]. Large variations in neuroimaging methods further decrease replicability [70], with the majority of surveyed studies reporting associations between structural brain morphology and behaviour failing to replicate [71]. Collectively, this crisis in replicability severely impairs the progress of neuroscience as a field [72]. We believe our replication attempt represents an important step towards such goals, and we hope that future studies building upon this line of research will help establish new meaningful brain biomarkers for addiction vulnerability in healthy humans.
Data Availability
These data are pseudonymised according to national (Swedish) and EU legislation, and can’t be anonymised and published in an open repository. Metadata will be openly published, and the underlying data can instead be made available upon request on a case by case basis as allowed by the legislation and ethical permits. Requests for access can be made to the Karolinska Institutet’s Research Data Office at rdo@ki.se.
Funding Statement
Author SC is supported by the Swedish Research Council (523-2014-3467) (https://www.vr.se/english.html). Author FC is supported by a post-doctoral fellowship from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (http://www.cihr-irsc.gc.ca/e/193.html). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Jentsch JD, Ashenhurst JR, Cervantes MC, Groman SM, James AS, Pennington ZT. Dissecting impulsivity and its relationships to drug addictions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2014;1327:1–26. Epub 2014/03/25. 10.1111/nyas.12388 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Grant JE, Chamberlain SR. Impulsive action and impulsive choice across substance and behavioral addictions: cause or consequence? Addictive behaviors. 2014;39(11):1632–9. Epub 2014/05/28. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.04.022 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.de Wit H. Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes. Addiction biology. 2009;14(1):22–31. Epub 2008/10/16. 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00129.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.MacKillop J, Weafer J, Gray J, Oshri A, Palmer A, de Wit H. The Latent Structure of Impulsivity: Impulsive Choice, Impulsive Action, and Impulsive Personality Traits. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233(18):3361–70. 10.1007/s00213-016-4372-0 PMC5204128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Cyders MA, Coskunpinar A. Measurement of constructs using self-report and behavioral lab tasks: is there overlap in nomothetic span and construct representation for impulsivity? Clinical psychology review. 2011;31(6):965–82. Epub 2011/07/08. 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.06.001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Broos N, Schmaal L, Wiskerke J, Kostelijk L, Lam T, Stoop N, et al. The relationship between impulsive choice and impulsive action: a cross-species translational study. PloS one. 2012;7(5):e36781 Epub 2012/05/11. 10.1371/journal.pone.0036781 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Fractionating impulsivity: neuropsychiatric implications. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2017;18(3):158–71. Epub 2017/02/18. 10.1038/nrn.2017.8 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Mitchell MR, Potenza MN. Addictions and Personality Traits: Impulsivity and Related Constructs. Current behavioral neuroscience reports. 2014;1(1):1–12. 10.1007/s40473-013-0001-y PMC3996683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Farde L, Plaven-Sigray P, Borg J, Cervenka S. Brain neuroreceptor density and personality traits: towards dimensional biomarkers for psychiatric disorders. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological sciences. 2018;373(1744). Epub 2018/02/28. 10.1098/rstb.2017.0156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Trifilieff P, Ducrocq F, van der Veldt S, Martinez D. Blunted Dopamine Transmission in Addiction: Potential Mechanisms and Implications for Behavior. Seminars in nuclear medicine. 2017;47(1):64–74. Epub 2016/12/19. 10.1053/j.semnuclmed.2016.09.003 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Barlow RL, Gorges M, Wearn A, Niessen HG, Kassubek J, Dalley JW, et al. Ventral striatal D2/3 receptor availability is associated with impulsive choice behavior as well aslimbic corticostriatal connectivity. The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology. 2018. Epub 2018/03/20. 10.1093/ijnp/pyy030 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Caprioli D, Jupp B, Hong YT, Sawiak SJ, Ferrari V, Wharton L, et al. Dissociable rate-dependent effects of oral methylphenidate on impulsivity and D2/3 receptor availability in the striatum. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2015;35(9):3747–55. Epub 2015/03/06. 10.1523/jneurosci.3890-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Caprioli D, Hong YT, Sawiak SJ, Ferrari V, Williamson DJ, Jupp B, et al. Baseline-dependent effects of cocaine pre-exposure on impulsivity and D2/3 receptor availability in the rat striatum: possible relevance to the attention-deficit hyperactivity syndrome. Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38(8):1460–71. Epub 2013/02/13. 10.1038/npp.2013.44 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Dalley JW, Fryer TD, Brichard L, Robinson ES, Theobald DE, Laane K, et al. Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science (New York, NY). 2007;315(5816):1267–70. Epub 2007/03/03. 10.1126/science.1137073 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Nader MA, Czoty PW. PET imaging of dopamine D2 receptors in monkey models of cocaine abuse: genetic predisposition versus environmental modulation. The American journal of psychiatry. 2005;162(8):1473–82. Epub 2005/08/02. 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1473 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lee B, London ED, Poldrack RA, Farahi J, Nacca A, Monterosso JR, et al. Striatal dopamine d2/d3 receptor availability is reduced in methamphetamine dependence and is linked to impulsivity. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2009;29(47):14734–40. Epub 2009/11/27. 10.1523/jneurosci.3765-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ballard ME, Mandelkern MA, Monterosso JR, Hsu E, Robertson CL, Ishibashi K, et al. Low Dopamine D2/D3 Receptor Availability is Associated with Steep Discounting of Delayed Rewards in Methamphetamine Dependence. The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;18(7):pyu119 Epub 2015/01/22. 10.1093/ijnp/pyu119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ashok AH, Mizuno Y, Volkow ND, Howes OD. Association of Stimulant Use With Dopaminergic Alterations in Users of Cocaine, Amphetamine, or Methamphetamine: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA psychiatry. 2017;74(5):511–9. Epub 2017/03/16. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.0135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Oswald LM, Wong DF, Zhou Y, Kumar A, Brasic J, Alexander M, et al. Impulsivity and chronic stress are associated with amphetamine-induced striatal dopamine release. Neuroimage. 2007;36(1):153–66. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Weiland BJ, Heitzeg MM, Zald D, Cummiford C, Love T, Zucker RA, et al. Relationship between impulsivity, prefrontal anticipatory activation, and striatal dopamine release during rewarded task performance. Psychiatry Res. 2014;223(3):244–52. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.05.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Buckholtz JW, Treadway MT, Cowan RL, Woodward ND, Li R, Ansari MS, et al. Dopaminergic Network Differences in Human Impulsivity. Science (New York, NY). 2010;329(5991):532–. 10.1126/science.1185778 PMC3161413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kim JH, Son YD, Kim HK, Lee SY, Kim YB, Cho ZH. Dopamine D2/3 receptor availability and human cognitive impulsivity: a high-resolution positron emission tomography imaging study with [11C]raclopride. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 2014;26(1):35–42. 10.1017/neu.2013.29 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Reeves SJ, Polling C, Stokes PR, Lappin JM, Shotbolt PP, Mehta MA, et al. Limbic striatal dopamine D2/3 receptor availability is associated with non-planning impulsivity in healthy adults after exclusion of potential dissimulators. Psychiatry Res. 2012;202(1):60–4. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Caravaggio F, Fervaha G, Chung JK, Gerretsen P, Nakajima S, Plitman E, et al. Exploring personality traits related to dopamine D2/3 receptor availability in striatal subregions of humans. European neuropsychopharmacology: the journal of the European College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;26(4):644–52. Epub 2016/03/06. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.02.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Anderson BA, Kuwabara H, Wong DF, Courtney SM. Density of available striatal dopamine receptors predicts trait impulsiveness during performance of an attention-demanding task. Journal of neurophysiology. 2017;118(1):64–8. Epub 2017/03/31. 10.1152/jn.00125.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Caprioli D, Sawiak SJ, Merlo E, Theobald DE, Spoelder M, Jupp B, et al. Gamma aminobutyric acidergic and neuronal structural markers in the nucleus accumbens core underlie trait-like impulsive behavior. Biological psychiatry. 2014;75(2):115–23. Epub 2013/08/27. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.07.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Caravaggio F, Ku Chung J, Plitman E, Boileau I, Gerretsen P, Kim J, et al. The relationship between subcortical brain volume and striatal dopamine D2/3 receptor availability in healthy humans assessed with [(11) C]-raclopride and [(11) C]-(+)-PHNO PET. Human brain mapping. 2017;38(11):5519–34. Epub 2017/07/29. 10.1002/hbm.23744 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Woodward ND, Zald DH, Ding Z, Riccardi P, Ansari MS, Baldwin RM, et al. Cerebral morphology and dopamine D(2)/D(3)receptor distribution in humans: A combined [(18)F]fallypride and voxel-based morphometry study. NeuroImage. 2009;46(1):31–8. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.049 PMC2804466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Caravaggio F, Plitman E, Chung JK, Gerretsen P, Kim J, Iwata Y, et al. Trait impulsiveness is related to smaller post-commissural putamen volumes in males but not females. The European journal of neuroscience. 2017;46(7):2253–64. Epub 2017/08/24. 10.1111/ejn.13661 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Muhlert N, Lawrence AD. Brain structure correlates of emotion-based rash impulsivity. Neuroimage. 2015;115:138–46. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.061 PMC4463859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Cho SS, Pellecchia G, Aminian K, Ray N, Segura B, Obeso I, et al. Morphometric Correlation of Impulsivity in Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Brain topography. 2013;26(3):479–87. 10.1007/s10548-012-0270-x PMC4452220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Tschernegg M, Pletzer B, Schwartenbeck P, Ludersdorfer P, Hoffmann U, Kronbichler M. Impulsivity relates to striatal gray matter volumes in humans: evidence from a delay discounting paradigm. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2015;9:384 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00384 PMC4488624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Mackey S, Chaarani B, Kan KJ, Spechler PA, Orr C, Banaschewski T, et al. Brain Regions Related to Impulsivity Mediate the Effects of Early Adversity on Antisocial Behavior. Biological psychiatry. 2017;82(4):275–82. Epub 2016/03/14. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.027 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gustavsson JP, Bergman H, Edman G, Ekselius L, von Knorring L, Linder J. Swedish universities Scales of Personality (SSP): construction, internal consistency and normative data. Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2000;102(3):217–25. Epub 2000/09/29. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Borg J, Cervenka S, Kuja-Halkola R, Matheson GJ, Jonsson EG, Lichtenstein P, et al. Contribution of non-genetic factors to dopamine and serotonin receptor availability in the adult human brain. Molecular psychiatry. 2016;21(8):1077–84. Epub 2016/01/30. 10.1038/mp.2015.147 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Plavén-Sigray P, Gustavsson P, Farde L, Borg J, Stenkrona P, Nyberg L, et al. Dopamine D1 receptor availability is related to social behavior: A positron emission tomography study. NeuroImage. 2014;102:590–5. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Plaven-Sigray P, Matheson GJ, Gustavsson P, Stenkrona P, Halldin C, Farde L, et al. Is dopamine D1 receptor availability related to social behavior? A positron emission tomography replication study. PloS one. 2018;13(3):e0193770 Epub 2018/03/16. 10.1371/journal.pone.0193770 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Schain M, Tóth M, Cselényi Z, Stenkrona P, Halldin C, Farde L, et al. Quantification of serotonin transporter availability with [11C]MADAM—A comparison between the ECAT HRRT and HR systems. NeuroImage. 2012;60(1):800–7. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Schalling D, Edman G. The Karolinska Scales of Personality (KSP) An inventory for assessing temperament dimensions associated with vulnerability for psychosocial deviance. Stockholm: Department of Psychiatry, Karolinska Institute; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Jonsson EG, Nothen MM, Gustavsson JP, Berggard C, Bunzel R, Forslund K, et al. No association between serotonin 2A receptor gene variants and personality traits. Psychiatric genetics. 2001;11(1):11–7. Epub 2001/06/21. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Chakravarty MM, Steadman P, van Eede MC, Calcott RD, Gu V, Shaw P, et al. Performing label-fusion-based segmentation using multiple automatically generated templates. Human brain mapping. 2013;34(10):2635–54. Epub 2012/05/23. 10.1002/hbm.22092 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPmc4896505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Pipitone J, Park MT, Winterburn J, Lett TA, Lerch JP, Pruessner JC, et al. Multi-atlas segmentation of the whole hippocampus and subfields using multiple automatically generated templates. Neuroimage. 2014;101:494–512. Epub 2014/05/03. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.054 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Chakravarty MM, Bertrand G, Hodge CP, Sadikot AF, Collins DL. The creation of a brain atlas for image guided neurosurgery using serial histological data. Neuroimage. 2006;30(2):359–76. Epub 2006/01/13. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.041 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Al-Hakim R, Nain D, Levitt J, Shenton M, Tannenbaum A. Semi-Automatic Parcellation of the Corpus Striatum. Proceedings of SPIE. 2007;6512:651236 10.1117/12.708950 PMC3916827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Tziortzi AC, Searle GE, Tzimopoulou S, Salinas C, Beaver JD, Jenkinson M, et al. Imaging dopamine receptors in humans with [11C]-(+)-PHNO: dissection of D3 signal and anatomy. Neuroimage. 2011;54(1):264–77. Epub 2010/07/06. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.044 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Makowski C, Beland S, Kostopoulos P, Bhagwat N, Devenyi GA, Malla AK, et al. Evaluating accuracy of striatal, pallidal, and thalamic segmentation methods: Comparing automated approaches to manual delineation. Neuroimage. 2017. Epub 2017/03/06. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.069 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Béland S, Makowski C, Konsztowicz S, Buchy L, Chakravarty MM, Lepage M. Clarifying associations between cortical thickness, subcortical structures, and a comprehensive assessment of clinical insight in enduring schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2018. 10.1016/j.schres.2018.08.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Chakravarty MM, Rapoport JL, Giedd JN, Raznahan A, Shaw P, Collins DL, et al. Striatal shape abnormalities as novel neurodevelopmental endophenotypes in schizophrenia: a longitudinal study. Human brain mapping. 2015;36(4):1458–69. Epub 2014/12/17. 10.1002/hbm.22715 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Avants BB, Epstein CL, Grossman M, Gee JC. Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Medical image analysis. 2008;12(1):26–41. Epub 2007/07/31. 10.1016/j.media.2007.06.004 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPmc2276735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Guo T, Winterburn JL, Pipitone J, Duerden EG, Park MT, Chau V, et al. Automatic segmentation of the hippocampus for preterm neonates from early-in-life to term-equivalent age. NeuroImage Clinical. 2015;9:176–93. Epub 2016/01/08. 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.07.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Eskildsen SF, Coupe P, Fonov V, Manjon JV, Leung KK, Guizard N, et al. BEaST: brain extraction based on nonlocal segmentation technique. Neuroimage. 2012;59(3):2362–73. Epub 2011/09/29. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.012 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Gelman A, Stern H. The Difference Between “Significant” and “Not Significant” is not Itself Statistically Significant. The American Statistician. 2006;60(4):328–31. 10.1198/000313006X152649 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Verhagen J, Wagenmakers EJ. Bayesian tests to quantify the result of a replication attempt. Journal of experimental psychology General. 2014;143(4):1457–75. Epub 2014/05/29. 10.1037/a0036731 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Wagenmakers E-J, Verhagen J, Ly A. How to quantify the evidence for the absence of a correlation. Behavior Research Methods. 2016;48(2):413–26. 10.3758/s13428-015-0593-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Rosenthal R, DiMatteo MR. Meta-analysis: recent developments in quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual review of psychology. 2001;52:59–82. Epub 2001/01/10. 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.59 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Wagenmakers E-J, Lodewyckx T, Kuriyal H, Grasman R. Bayesian hypothesis testing for psychologists: A tutorial on the Savage–Dickey method. Cognitive Psychology. 2010;60(3):158–89. 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Wonderlick JS, Ziegler DA, Hosseini-Varnamkhasti P, Locascio JJ, Bakkour A, van der Kouwe A, et al. Reliability of MRI-derived cortical and subcortical morphometric measures: effects of pulse sequence, voxel geometry, and parallel imaging. Neuroimage. 2009;44(4):1324–33. Epub 2008/11/29. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Treadway MT, Buckholtz JW, Cowan RL, Woodward ND, Li R, Ansari MS, et al. Dopaminergic mechanisms of individual differences in human effort-based decision-making. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2012;32(18):6170–6. Epub 2012/05/04. 10.1523/jneurosci.6459-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Wardle MC, Treadway MT, Mayo LM, Zald DH, de Wit H. Amping up effort: effects of d-amphetamine on human effort-based decision-making. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2011;31(46):16597–602. Epub 2011/11/18. 10.1523/jneurosci.4387-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Caravaggio F, Fervaha G, Browne CJ, Gerretsen P, Remington G, Graff-Guerrero A. Reward motivation in humans and its relationship to dopamine D2/3 receptor availability: A pilot study with dual [(11)C]-raclopride and [(11)C]-(+)-PHNO imaging. Journal of psychopharmacology (Oxford, England). 2018;32(3):357–66. Epub 2018/02/15. 10.1177/0269881118756059 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Le Heron C, Apps MAJ, Husain M. The anatomy of apathy: A neurocognitive framework for amotivated behaviour. Neuropsychologia. 2017. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Caravaggio F, Fervaha G, Iwata Y, Plitman E, Chung JK, Nakajima S, et al. Amotivation is associated with smaller ventral striatum volumes in older patients with schizophrenia. International journal of geriatric psychiatry. 2018;33(3):523–30. Epub 2017/11/08. 10.1002/gps.4818 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Passamonti L, Lansdall CJ, Rowe JB. The neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of apathy and impulsivity in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2018;22:14–20. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Le Bouc R, Rigoux L, Schmidt L, Degos B, Welter M-L, Vidailhet M, et al. Computational Dissection of Dopamine Motor and Motivational Functions in Humans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36(25):6623–33. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3078-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Kohli A, Blitzer DN, Lefco RW, Barter JW, Haynes MR, Colalillo SA, et al. Using Expectancy Theory to quantitatively dissociate the neural representation of motivation from its influential factors in the human brain: An fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2018. Epub 2018/05/12. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.021 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Maxwell SE, Lau MY, Howard GS. Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does “failure to replicate” really mean? American Psychologist. 2015;70(6):487 10.1037/a0039400 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Button KS, Ioannidis JP, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ES, et al. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2013;14(5):365 10.1038/nrn3475 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Friston K. Ten ironic rules for non-statistical reviewers. Neuroimage. 2012;61(4):1300–10. Epub 2012/04/24. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.018 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Reality check on reproducibility. Nature. 2016;533(7604):437 Epub 2016/05/27. 10.1038/533437a . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Poldrack RA, Baker CI, Durnez J, Gorgolewski KJ, Matthews PM, Munafo MR, et al. Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2017;18(2):115–26. Epub 2017/01/06. 10.1038/nrn.2016.167 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Boekel W, Wagenmakers EJ, Belay L, Verhagen J, Brown S, Forstmann BU. A purely confirmatory replication study of structural brain-behavior correlations. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior. 2015;66:115–33. Epub 2015/02/17. 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.11.019 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Yeung AWK. Do Neuroscience Journals Accept Replications? A Survey of Literature. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2017;11:468 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00468 PMC5611708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
These data are pseudonymised according to national (Swedish) and EU legislation, and can’t be anonymised and published in an open repository. Metadata will be openly published, and the underlying data can instead be made available upon request on a case by case basis as allowed by the legislation and ethical permits. Requests for access can be made to the Karolinska Institutet’s Research Data Office at rdo@ki.se.