Abstract
Translating noisy sensory signals to perceptual decisions is critical for successful interactions in complex environments. Learning is known to improve perceptual judgments by filtering external noise and task-irrelevant information. Yet, little is known about the brain mechanisms that mediate learning-dependent suppression. Here, we employ ultra-high field magnetic resonance spectroscopy of GABA to test whether suppressive processing in decision-related and visual areas facilitates perceptual judgments during training. We demonstrate that parietal GABA relates to suppression of task-irrelevant information, while learning-dependent changes in visual GABA relate to enhanced performance in target detection and feature discrimination tasks. Combining GABA measurements with functional brain connectivity demonstrates that training on a target detection task involves local connectivity and disinhibition of visual cortex, while training on a feature discrimination task involves inter-cortical interactions that relate to suppressive visual processing. Our findings provide evidence that learning optimizes perceptual decisions through suppressive interactions in decision-related networks.
Learning improves perceptual decisions by enhancing the brain's ability to filter noise and irrelevant information. Here, the authors show that GABAergic inhibition in decision-making circuits supports our ability to optimize perceptual judgments through learning and experience.
Introduction
Making successful decisions entails extracting meaningful information from multiple sources in the environment that are inherently noisy and ambiguous. Experience and training have been shown to play a key role in optimizing perceptual decisions1–3 by filtering external noise (e.g., when detecting targets in cluttered scenes) and retuning task-relevant feature templates (e.g., when discriminating highly similar objects)4–6. Previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have demonstrated learning-dependent changes in functional brain activity due to training on perceptual tasks that involve detecting targets in clutter or discriminating fine feature differences (for reviews7,8). However, fMRI does not allow us to distinguish excitatory from inhibitory mechanisms of experience-dependent plasticity, as BOLD reflects aggregate activity from both excitatory and inhibitory signals across large neural populations9. Thus, the inhibitory brain plasticity mechanisms that support our ability to improve our perceptual decisions by learning to suppress noisy and task-irrelevant information through training remain largely unknown.
To investigate inhibitory mechanisms of learning-dependent plasticity, we employed magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) that has only recently made it possible to measure γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA), the primary inhibitory neurotransmitter in the brain. Previous animal studies have linked decreased GABAergic activity to learning and synaptic plasticity in primary motor cortex10,11. In accordance with these findings, human MRS studies have shown that GABA levels in the primary motor cortex decrease following interventions that facilitate cortical reorganisation12 and motor training13,14. In the visual cortex, human MRS studies have shown that GABA levels relate positively to performance in perceptual tasks15–17. Further, decrease in visual GABA has been shown to relate to homeostatic plasticity18. Here, we took advantage of the high spectral resolution afforded by ultra-high field (7T) MRS to reliably resolve GABA19,20 and take fast and reliable repeated measurements of functional GABA during training. This allowed us to test changes in GABA during training (i.e., while the participants were trained on a task), extending beyond standard correlational approaches that relate single measurements of GABA at baseline (i.e., when participants are at rest) to behavior. Further, we tested whether changes in GABAergic inhibition during task-specific training relate to improvement in perceptual decisions.
To probe the brain mechanisms that support learning by suppressing noisy and irrelevant signals, we employed two learning tasks that have been shown to rely either on noise filtering or feature template retuning: (1) a signal-in-noise task that involves extracting a target masked by noise, (2) a feature differences task that involves judging fine differences21. Recent computational investigations22,23 and animal studies propose dissociable roles for inhibition in learning to interpret noisy sensory signals vs. tuning fine feature processing. Based on this work, we hypothesized that distinct GABAergic inhibition mechanisms are involved in task-dependent learning and plasticity. Specifically, we reasoned that decreased GABAergic inhibition during training would relate to improved ability to detect targets in clutter, as changes in GABAergic inhibition have been linked to neural gain, (i.e., changes in information transmission between neurons24 or the slope of the neural input-output relationship25). In contrast, we reasoned that increased GABAergic inhibition would relate to improved performance in fine feature discrimination, as increased GABAergic inhibition has been linked to enhanced orientation selectivity in visual cortex16,26–28.
Further, previous neuroimaging and neurophysiology studies have implicated distinct functional roles for the visual and posterior parietal cortex (PPC) in sensory processing vs. perceptual decision making, respectively29,30. To test the role of inhibitory processing in learning for both visual and parietal cortex, we implemented an imaging protocol that measured GABA in two voxels (one in occipito-temporal (OCT), one in PPC) in alternating order and allowed us to track changes in GABA in both areas during training. Interestingly, previous studies have proposed that perceptual learning is implemented by top-down influences from decision-related areas that re-weight processing in sensory areas30,31. To test whether learning involves local processing within visual cortex or suppressive interactions between decision-related and sensory areas, we combined GABA measurements in occipito-temporal and posterior parietal cortex with functional brain connectivity, as measured by resting-state fMRI. In particular, we tested the hypothesis that learning is implemented by local inhibitory processing in visual cortex that is gated by functional interactions between sensory and decision-related areas. Specifically, we tested whether learning-dependent changes in visual cortex GABA relate to functional connectivity between visual and posterior parietal cortex.
Our results reveal distinct GABAergic inhibition mechanisms in a cortical network that is known to be involved in perceptual decisions. In particular, increased parietal GABA with training suggests suppression of task-irrelevant information. In contrast, changes in occipito-temporal GABA with training relate to enhanced target detection and discriminability, suggesting learning-dependent changes in the processing of task-relevant features. Further, analysis of functional brain connectivity at rest reveals interactions within this network that relate to GABA changes and behavioral improvement during training. Learning to detect targets from clutter is implemented by local connectivity and disinhibition of the visual cortex. In contrast, learning feature differences is implemented by interactions between parietal and visual areas that relate to increased GABAergic inhibition in visual cortex. Our results provide evidence that learning improves perceptual decisions through suppressive interactions within decision-related circuits in the human brain.
Results
Training improves behavioral performance
We tested two groups of participants on (a) a signal-in-noise (SN) task that involves extracting shapes (radial vs. concentric Glass patterns) masked by noise or (b) a feature differences (FD) task that involves judging fine differences induced by morphing between two stimulus classes (Fig. 1a). Participants were asked to judge the identity of the stimulus presented per trial (i.e., radial vs. concentric). Our results showed that participants improved in their judgments within a single training session during scanning (Fig. 1b), consistent with previous reports showing fast behavioral improvement early in the training (for a review, see ref. 32). A linear mixed effects (LME) model with Task and Training Block (6 blocks, 200 trials per block) as fixed effects showed significantly improved performance during training across tasks (main effect of Block: F(1, 249) = 9.35, p = 0.002). No significant interaction between Task × Block (F(1, 249) = 0.10, p = 0.75) was observed, suggesting similar improvement across tasks (Fig. 1b).
To quantify behavioral improvement due to training in individual participants, we compared performance at the beginning of training (i.e., first training block) to the maximum performance achieved (i.e., when participants achieved highest accuracy) by each participant during training (Fig. 1b) (see Supplementary Methods). We chose this measure to capture individual variability across participants that may be more pronounced in our data, as participants were trained only for a single training session in contrast to our previous studies that have shown that performance saturates after multiple training sessions on similar perceptual tasks33,34. A repeated measures two-way analysis of variance (Task × Block) showed significantly improved performance during training across tasks (main effect of block: F(1, 45) = 59.88, p < 0.0001) but no significant interaction between Task × Block (F(1, 45) = 1.07, p = 0.31).
Learning-dependent changes in GABA
To test whether GABAergic inhibition in the visual or parietal cortex changes with training, we measured GABA (Fig. 2a) before (baseline block) and during training. During the baseline block, participants were presented with random dot stimuli, while during the training blocks participants performed either the SN or the FD task on Glass pattern stimuli. We tested two MRS voxels—one centered on the occipito-temporal cortex (OCT voxel) and the other on the PPC (PPC voxel) (Fig. 2b)—following previous studies showing that these areas are involved in learning using the same tasks and stimuli as in our study33,34. Each block comprised one MRS acquisition per voxel and the order of the voxels within each block was counterbalanced across participants.
Figure 3 shows that OCT GABA/tCr changed in the two tasks during training in opposite directions. In contrast, PPC GABA/tCr changed in the same direction (i.e., increased during training) for both tasks. These effects were supported by an LME analysis (Task, Voxel, and MRS Blocks as fixed effects) that modeled GABA before (baseline block) and during (three training blocks) training in both OCT and PPC. This analysis showed that learning-dependent changes in GABA levels differed between tasks and regions (Task × Voxel × Block: F(1, 264) = 6.24, p = 0.01). In particular, we found that changes in OCT GABA (OCT GABA) levels during training differed between tasks (LME model for OCT GABA with Task and MRS Block as fixed effects; Task × Block: F(1, 119) = 10.77, p = 0.001) (Fig. 3a). In contrast, GABA changes in the PPC (PPC GABA) during training did not differ significantly between tasks (LME model for PPC GABA with Task and MRS Block as fixed effects; Task × Block: F(1, 145) = 0.18, p = 0.68) (Fig. 3a). That is, PPC GABA increased during training independent of Task (LME model for PPC GABA with MRS Block as fixed effect; main effect of Block: F(1, 147) = 6.27, t = 2.50, p = 0.01).
Specifically, for the FD task, GABA changes during training did not differ significantly between OCT and PPC (LME model for FD task GABA with Voxel and MRS Block as fixed effects; Voxel × Block: F(1, 143) = 0.10, p = 0.74). That is, GABA increased significantly during training independent of Voxel (LME model for FD GABA with MRS Block as fixed effect; main effect of MRS Block: F(1, 145) = 6.13, t = 2.48, p = 0.01). In contrast, for the SN task GABA changes during training differed in the two regions (LME model for SN task with Voxel and MRS Block as fixed effects; Voxel × Block: F(1, 121) = 13.06, p = 0.0004). That is, we found a significant decrease for OCT GABA (LME model for SN task with MRS Block as fixed effect; main effect of Block: F(1,58) = 16.65, t = −4.08, p = 0.0001), but a nonsignificant increasing trend for PPC GABA (LME model for SN task with MRS Block as fixed effect; main effect of Block: F(1,63) = 3.26, t = 1.80, p = 0.08).
We conducted the following control analyses that corroborated our results (see Supplementary Methods for more details). First, we demonstrated that the learning-dependent changes we observed in GABA levels: (a) were not simply driven by GABA measurements at baseline, (b) could not be simply due to the order with which the MRS voxels were acquired during training. Second, we tested whether the learning-dependent changes we observed in GABA, were simply due to differences in data quality across multiple MRS measurements. In particular, we showed that there were no significant differences in the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of MRS measurements over time, nor BOLD effects on MRS spectra as represented by narrowing of the linewidth. In addition, we conducted a no-training control experiment on an independent group of participants (n = 8). We measured OCT GABA before and after exposing the participants to Glass pattern stimuli for a duration comparable to the training in our main study. During this time participants performed a fixation task rather than the SN or FD tasks. We did not observe any significant differences (t(7) = 1.56, p = 0.16) between two measurements of OCT GABA, suggesting that the GABA changes we observed in our main study were specific to task-training. Third, we showed that the learning-dependent changes we observed in GABA/tCr (a) could not be explained simply by changes in tCr concentration over time (Supplementary Figure 5), (b) remained significant when we referenced GABA to water rather than tCr concentration (Supplementary Figure 4), (c) were specific to GABA and did not generalize to Glutamate (Fig. 3b).
Learning-dependent changes in GABA relate to behavior
We next tested whether the learning-dependent changes we observed in occipito-temporal and parietal GABA related to behavioral improvement. Correlating change in OCT GABA (see Supplementary Methods) to behavioral improvement showed differences between tasks. Specifically, we observed a significant negative correlation of OCT GABA change with behavioral improvement (r = −0.43, CI = [−0.75, −0.02]) for the SN task, whereas a significant positive correlation (r = 0.55, CI = [0.10, 0.78]) for the FD task (Fig. 4a). A linear regression analysis confirmed this dissociation (OCT GABA change × Task interaction: F(1, 29) = 9.03, p = 0.005), suggesting that lower vs. higher occipito-temporal GABA relates to improved performance when learning to detect targets vs. discriminate feature differences, respectively. This dissociable result between tasks cannot be simply explained by differences in task difficulty, as participants showed similar behavioral improvement across tasks (Fig. 1b).
In contrast to these correlations of OCT GABA change with behavioral improvement, we did not observe any significant correlations between PPC GABA change and behavioral improvement for either task (SN: r = −0.23, CI = [−0.61, 0.19]; FD: r = 0.05, CI = [−0.37, 0.43]) nor a significant PPC GABA change × Task interaction (F(1, 34) = 0.57, p = 0.45) (Fig. 4b). Following previous work on the role of the PPC early rather than later in training35, we next tested the link between PPC GABA and performance early in training for the two tasks. A linear regression model with baseline PPC GABA and Task as predictors of behavioral performance (first training block) showed a significant effect of PPC GABA (F(1, 37) = 4.69, p = 0.04), but no interaction between PPC GABA and Task (F(1, 37) = 0.03, p = 0.85). This result was confirmed by a significant positive correlation between baseline PPC GABA and performance in the first training block (r = 0.34, CI = [0.06, 0.59]), suggesting GABAergic inhibition in the parietal cortex before training relates to suppression of task-irrelevant information early in the training across tasks. We did not find a significant effect of OCT GABA (F(1, 33) = 2.14, p = 0.15) nor an interaction between OCT GABA and Task (F(1, 33) = 2.33, p = 0.14), suggesting that this result linking GABA to performance early in training was specific to parietal cortex.
Functional connectivity at rest relates to behavior and GABA
Previous studies have shown that functional connectivity in motor14 and visual36 networks relates to behavioral improvement and learning-dependent plasticity. Further, GABAergic inhibition has been suggested to shape network connectivity37. In particular, extra-synaptic GABA has been shown to relate to local oscillatory activity in the high gamma frequency range38 and to inter-regional functional connectivity39. In the human brain, MRS-assessed GABA has been linked to functional connectivity as measured by resting-state fMRI37,40,41, suggesting that GABAergic inhibition may relate to local neural dynamics42. Here we test whether functional connectivity between the OCT and PPC relates to behavioral improvement and GABA changes during training to detect targets in clutter vs. discriminate fine features.
First, we tested whether functional connectivity between these regions relates to behavioral improvement in each of the two learning tasks. We extracted the resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI) time course only from the gray matter voxels within the OCT MRS voxel and the PPC MRS voxel (see Supplementary Methods). We measured functional connectivity by correlating the rs-fMRI time courses at rest between these two regions (OCT-PPC connectivity) (see Supplementary Methods). We observed a significant positive correlation between OCT-PPC connectivity and behavioral improvement for the FD task (r = 0.37, CI = [0.03, 0.69]), whereas a significant negative correlation for the SN task (r = −0.72, CI = [−0.90, −0.29]) (Fig. 5). This dissociation in the relationship of OCT-PPC connectivity and behavioral improvement between tasks was confirmed by a linear regression showing a significant interaction between OCT-PPC connectivity and Task (F(1, 39) = 10.72, p = 0.002), suggesting that higher connectivity between parietal and visual cortex facilitates learning of fine feature differences. We interpret these results according to previous studies that show both positive and negative values for resting-state connectivity (e.g.,36). Positive connectivity values (i.e., correlated signals) are typically interpreted to indicate integration or cooperation within or between brain regions. In contrast, negative connectivity values (i.e., anti-correlated signals) are typically interpreted to indicate segregation of processing or competition between networks. Thus, the correlations we observed between resting-state connectivity and behavioral improvement suggest higher cooperation between OCT and PPC for learning fine features differences (FD task) than learning to detect targets from noise (SN task). Further, to test whether this link between functional connectivity and behavioral improvement is specific to interactions between parietal and visual areas, we extracted rs-fMRI for two additional control regions: early visual cortex and motor cortex. We did not observe any significant results for correlations of behavioral improvement and rs-fMRI connectivity between OCT and (a) early visual cortex (SN: r = 0.37, CI = [–0.17, 0.70]; FD: r = –0.22, CI = [–0.56, 0.15]) or (b) motor cortex (SN: r = –0.26, CI = [–0.61, 0.12]; FD: r = –0.06, CI = [–0.39, 0.42]) (see Supplementary Methods).
Second, we tested whether OCT-PPC functional connectivity relates to changes in visual cortex GABA during training. Our results showed a significant positive correlation between functional connectivity and OCT GABA change for the FD task (r = 0.55, CI = [0.13, 0.87]), but not the SN task (r = 0.27, CI = [–0.08, 0.60]) (Fig. 6a). These correlations were specific to GABA changes in OCT. That is, there were no significant correlations between OCT-PPC connectivity and PPC GABA change for the FD task (r = 0.29, CI = [–0.37, 0.69]) nor the SN task (r = 0.20, CI = [–0.31, 0.60]). A multivariate linear regression showed that OCT-PPC connectivity had a significant effect on GABA change in OCT (F(1, 14) = 20.74, p = 0.0005), but not PPC (F(1, 14) = 0.46, p = 0.51) for the FD task. The correlation between functional connectivity and OCT GABA change for the FD task remained significant (r = 0.49, CI = [0.10, 0.82]) when we tested for percentage GABA change (GABA change/baseline GABA), suggesting that our results could not be due to differences in baseline GABA. These results show that parietal-visual cortex connectivity relates to GABAergic inhibition in visual cortex, suggesting top-down influences to suppressive processing in visual cortex for learning fine feature differences.
Interestingly, for the SN task we observed a significantly negative correlation between connectivity within the OCT (i.e., OCT connectivity: connectivity measured as rs-fMRI correlations across voxels within the OCT MRS voxel, see Supplementary Methods) and OCT GABA change (r = –0.55, CI = [–0.86, –0.01]) (Fig. 6b). That is, higher OCT connectivity related to decreased OCT GABA with training, suggesting that learning to detect targets in clutter is supported by local connectivity and decreased GABAergic inhibition within visual cortex. Correlations of functional connectivity and GABA change in the OCT were not significant for the FD task (r = 0.12, CI = [–0.29, 0.51]) and were significantly different from correlations for the SN task (Z = 2.33, p = 0.02). The correlation between OCT connectivity and GABA change for the SN task remained significant (r = –0.55, CI = [–0.88, –0.003]), when we used percentage GABA change (GABA change/baseline GABA), suggesting that our results could not be due to differences in baseline GABA. Correlating OCT connectivity with PPC GABA change did not show any significant results (r = 0.18, CI = [–0.39, 0.60]), suggesting that local connectivity relates specifically to GABA changes in visual cortex.
Taken together, our results suggest that learning fine discriminations involves interactions between decision-related (posterior parietal) and sensory (visual) areas that may facilitate retuning of task-relevant features in visual cortex. In contrast, learning to detect targets from clutter involves local processing and decreased inhibition in visual cortex. To further test this proposal, we performed moderation analyses43 that allowed us to test whether the influence that an independent variable (i.e., GABA change) has on the outcome (i.e., behavioral improvement) is moderated by a moderator variable (i.e., connectivity). Our results showed that OCT-PPC connectivity moderates the relationship between OCT GABA and behavior for the FD task (F(1, 15) = 6.19, p = 0.03) but not the SN task (F(1, 12) = 0.74, p = 0.41). In contrast, local connectivity within OCT moderates the relationship between GABA change and behavior for the SN task (F(1, 12) = 7.65, p = 0.02) but not the FD task (F(1, 15) = 2.80, p = 0.11). These moderation analyses suggest that the relationship between learning-dependent changes in GABA and behavior is moderated by functional connectivity; that is, interactions between parietal and visual areas for the FD task, whereas local connectivity within visual cortex for the SN task.
Discussion
Here, we provide evidence that learning improves perceptual decisions (i.e., learning to detect targets in clutter or discriminate highly similar features) by suppressive interactions in the human brain. First, we demonstrate dissociable GABAergic inhibition mechanisms for learning in a posterior cortical network (i.e., occipito-temporal and posterior-parietal cortex) known to be involved in perceptual decisions. In particular, increased GABAergic inhibition in the PPC with training suggests suppressive processing of task-irrelevant information. In contrast, changes in occipito-temporal GABA with training relate to enhanced target detection and discriminability, suggesting learning-dependent changes in the processing of behaviorally relevant features. Second, we provide evidence that interactions within this network, as measured by functional brain connectivity at rest, gate suppressive processing of sensory signals. Learning to detect targets from clutter involves local processing and disinhibition in visual cortex, while learning feature differences involves suppressive interactions between decision-related (parietal) and visual areas.
Previous studies have used MRS to measure GABA in the context of visual15, sensory-motor44–46, and learning tasks47,48. Here, we exploited the high spectral resolution afforded by ultra-high field (7T) MRS to reliably resolve GABA from other metabolites19,20 and measure GABA changes over time at faster time scales than typically recorded at lower field strength. However, as the temporal resolution of MRS-assessed GABA is in the order of minutes, it is not possible to separate GABAergic inhibition related to different processes within a trial (i.e., stimulus processing, perceptual judgment, response, feedback); thus, it is likely that the observed GABA changes relate to aggregate signals across all processes involved in the tasks performed by the participants. Despite this, our methodology provides two main advantages over previous work. First, moving beyond the standard correlational approach that relates single measurements of GABA at baseline to behavior, we implemented an interventional approach to test whether GABAergic inhibition changes due to task-specific training. In particular, we captured learning-dependent changes in functional GABA during training. Two previous studies reporting measurements of GABA during training have focused on motor cortex GABAergic inhibition during training on a motor learning task13,49. Second, we implemented an MRS protocol that allows us to track longitudinal changes in GABA during training from both OCT and PPC that are thought to play distinct functional roles in perceptual learning29,30. Using this protocol, in combination with functional connectivity as measured by rs-fMRI, we tested for the role of suppressive interactions between sensory and decision-related areas in perceptual learning.
Our findings provide evidence for distinct GABAergic inhibition mechanisms in PPC vs. OCT for learning. In particular, we demonstrate a common mechanism across tasks in the PPC. That is, GABA increases during training may facilitate suppression of task-irrelevant information (i.e., background clutter when learning to detect targets, task-irrelevant features when learning fine differences). It is unlikely that changes in GABA levels with training reflect reduced attention to the task, as the task remained sufficiently demanding (i.e., mean maximum performance was 72%) during the single training session employed in our study. Our findings are consistent with the known role of parietal cortex in perceptual decision-making and attentional selection. In particular, the PPC has been implicated in detecting low-saliency targets by suppressing distractors50, providing a salient representation of the environment and top-down attentional feedback51, accumulating sensory information52 and directing attention to task-relevant features29,51. Interestingly, we show that parietal cortex GABA before training relates to performance early in training across tasks. This is in contrast with visual cortex changes in GABA that relate to improvement in behavioral performance during training. This finding suggests that suppression of task-irrelevant information in the PPC may precede suppression in the visual cortex related to the processing of task-relevant features. This is consistent with previous studies showing that transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in the parietal cortex disrupts performance in visual discrimination tasks early in the training compared with TMS in the visual cortex that disrupts performance after training35.
In contrast to this common GABAergic inhibition mechanism across tasks in the PPC, our results in the visual cortex suggest distinct GABAergic mechanisms between tasks that relate to improved behavioral performance. In particular, decreased occipito-temporal GABA related to improved performance when training to detect targets in clutter, while increased occipito-temporal GABA related to improved performance when training to discriminate fine feature differences. These results suggest that learning to detect targets in clutter is implemented by decreased local GABAergic inhibition that may facilitate noise filtering and feature detection. Disinhibition of the visual cortex may facilitate cortical recruitment that enhances probability summation and SNR for target detection53. This mechanism is consistent with previous animal studies linking GABAergic inhibition to neural gain25 and interventional studies showing that blocking GABAergic inhibition increases neural gain24. In contrast, discriminating highly similar targets is implemented by increased GABAergic inhibition in the visual cortex that may facilitate learning through feature template retuning. This mechanism is consistent with neurophysiological studies linking GABAergic inhibition to cortical tuning26 and pharmacological interventions showing that GABA agonists enhance orientation selectivity in visual cortex27,28, whereas blocking GABAergic inhibition results in broader neural tuning27. Interestingly, recent work has implicated different populations of inhibitory interneurons in neural gain vs. tuning54 that may contribute differentially to learning by noise filtering vs. feature template retuning. In particular somatostatin (SOM)-positive interneurons have been implicated in spatial summation55 and have been shown to gate plasticity by providing contextual information56. In contrast, parvalbumin (PV)-positive interneurons have been implicated in selective inhibition16 that sharpens feature representations after training57. It is therefore possible that SOM interneurons may support learning to detect targets from clutter (SN task) through noise filtering, whereas PV interneurons may support learning fine differences (FD task) through feature retuning. Thus, our findings propose testable hypotheses for further animal studies on the micro-circuits that mediate adaptive behavior and underlie the macroscopic learning-dependent plasticity as measured by human brain imaging.
We interpret these links between MRS-assessed GABA, behavior and possible neural mechanisms with caution, as the precise mechanisms that underlie changes in GABA as measured by MRS remain under investigation. In particular, it is unclear whether MRS-assessed GABA represents the entire pool of GABA available in a voxel. It is possible that the individual GABA pools are not equally visible using MRS13,58. Another possibility is that MRS-assessed GABA reflects the exchange between the intra-cellular and synaptic pools of GABA59. A number of studies using paired-pulse TMS (e.g., 60,61) have shown that MRS-assessed GABA does not relate to GABAA or GABAB activity and therefore it is unlikely to reflect synaptic transmission of GABA. It is more likely that MRS-assessed GABA reflects ambient extracellular GABA that contributes to tonic GABAergic activity (for reviews e.g., 62,63). This is consistent with animal studies showing that GABA synthesis64 is associated with Glutamic Acid Decarboxylase (GAD)67 that is predominantly found throughout the cell, rather than GAD65 that is found in axon terminals65. Finally, we did not observe changes in glutamate simultaneously with the change in GABA observed during training. Thus, it is unlikely that the changes in GABA reflect overall changes in the metabolite cycling. GABA undergoes rapid turnover in the mammalian cortex, and GAD activity has been shown to be rapidly modulated in a variety of physiological processes66 in both human67 and animal68 studies. Future studies combining invasive investigations in animals (e.g., two photon imaging of interneurons) with non-invasive MRS are necessary to shed more light on the basis MRS-assessed GABA.
Finally, investigating functional connectivity within this posterior cortical network revealed suppressive interactions that relate to our ability to improve perceptual judgments through training. We demonstrate that higher connectivity between parietal and visual cortex at rest relates to increased GABA levels in visual cortex during training and behavioral improvement in fine feature discrimination. This finding suggests top-down influences from the parietal cortex on suppressive processing in the visual cortex for retuning feature templates. Previous theoretical investigations have proposed that sensory selectivity is enhanced by suppressive feedback mechanisms that change recurrent processing in visual cortex23 and relate to attention-guided selection of behaviorally relevant information69. Our findings suggest that suppression of task-irrelevant information in the parietal cortex enhances tuning of task-relevant features in visual cortex through top-down feedback, consistent with previous proposals that perceptual learning re-weights sensory processing30,31. In contrast, we show that higher connectivity within the visual cortex (rather than connectivity between visual and parietal cortex) relates to decreased GABA in visual cortex and behavioral improvement in detecting targets from clutter. Lateral interactions within the early visual cortex are shown to support contextual processing and shape integration53,70. Further, recurrent processing has been implicated in robust representations of ambiguous stimuli in higher visual cortex71, suggesting that local connectivity in occipito-temporal cortex facilitates visual processing under uncertainty.
In sum, our findings provide evidence for distinct GABAergic inhibition mechanisms that support our ability to optimize perceptual decisions through training. We propose that local suppressive processing within visual cortex enhances target detection, whereas top-down suppression from decision-related areas (i.e., posterior parietal cortex) enhances retuning of task-relevant features for fine discrimination in the visual cortex. Decision-making models have proposed suppressive mechanisms that resolve competition between neuronal ensembles that represent behavioral choices72. Our findings provide novel insights in understanding how these suppressive mechanisms are implemented in decision-related and sensory areas in the human brain and optimize our ability for perceptual decisions through training.
Methods
Participants
Fifty participants (32 females; mean age 25.6 ± 3.3 years) took part in this study. Data from three participants were not useable due to technical problems. All participants were right handed, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and gave written informed consent. The study was approved the University of Oxford Central University Research Ethics committee (MSD-IDREC-C1-2014-081).
Stimuli
We presented participants with Glass patterns generated using previously described methods33,34. In particular, stimuli were defined by white dot pairs (dipoles) displayed within a square aperture on a black background. Experiment stimuli (size = 7.9o × 7.9o) were presented in the center of the screen. The dot density was 3%, and the Glass shift (i.e., the distance between two dots in a dipole) was 16.2 arc min. The size of each dot was 2.3 × 2.3 arc min2. For each dot dipole, the spiral angle was defined as the angle between the dot dipole orientation and the radius from the center of the dipole to the center of the stimulus aperture. Each stimulus comprised dot dipoles that were aligned according to the specified spiral angle (signal dipoles) for a given stimulus and noise dipoles for which the spiral angle was randomly selected. The proportion of signal dipoles defined the stimulus signal level.
We generated radial (0o spiral angle) and concentric (90o spiral angle) Glass patterns by placing dipoles orthogonally (radial stimuli) or tangentially (concentric stimuli) to the circumference of a circle centered on the fixation dot. Further, we generated intermediate patterns between these two Glass pattern types by parametrically varying the spiral angle of the pattern from 0° (radial pattern) to 90° (concentric pattern). We randomized the presentation of clockwise (0° to 90° spiral angle) and counterclockwise patterns (0° to –90° spiral angle) across participants. A new pattern was generated for each stimulus presented in a trial, resulting in stimuli that were locally jittered in their position.
For the SN task, radial and concentric stimuli (spiral angle: 0° ± 90°) were presented at 24% ± 1% signal level; that is, 76% of the dipoles were presented at random position and orientation. For the FD task, stimuli were presented at 100% signal and spiral angle of ± 38o (radial) or ± 52o (concentric) (Fig. 1a). To control for stimulus-specific training effects, we presented each participant with a newly generated set of stimuli. To control for potential local adaptation due to stimulus repetition, we jittered ( ± 1–3°) the spiral angle across stimuli. These procedures ensured that learning related to global shape rather than local stimulus features.
Experiment design
All participants took part in a single brain imaging session (Fig. 2a) during which they were randomly assigned and trained on either the SN or the FD task. We recorded whole-brain rs-fMRI data before training while participants fixated on a cross at the center for the screen.
Following the rs-fMRI scan, we recorded MRS GABA before and during training (Fig. 2a). We collected MRS GABA from one baseline block before training and three blocks during task training. Each block comprised two MRS acquisitions: one from OCT and one from PPC. The order of the voxels within each block was counterbalanced across participants. During each training block, participants were presented with stimuli for 400 trials (200 trials per MRS voxel acquisition). During the baseline MRS block (400 trials) participants engaged in a task with similar stimuli as those presented during the training; that is, participants viewed random dot patterns (0% signal dipoles) and were asked to respond (button press) as soon as a pattern appeared. This ensured that differences in GABA between blocks could not be simply attributed to differences in overall alertness. During the MRS training blocks (3 MRS blocks, 400 trials each), participants were presented with Glass patterns and were asked to judge and indicate by button press whether the presented stimulus in each trial was radial or concentric. Two stimulus conditions (radial vs. concentric Glass patterns; 200 trials per condition), were presented for each training block. For each trial, a stimulus was presented for 300 ms and was followed by fixation (i.e., blank screen with a central fixation dot) while waiting for the participant’s response (self-paced training paradigm). Trial-by-trial feedback was provided by means of a visual cue (green tick for correct, red ‘x’ for incorrect) followed by a fixation dot for 500 ms before the onset of the next trial. Average time to complete a trial ranged between 1.4 s and 1.9s across participants (1.66 ± 0.16 s for SN; 1.69 ± 0.16 s for FD). Mean trial duration across participants decreased across MRS blocks for both tasks (LME model for Trial duration with Task and MRS Block as fixed effects; main effect of Block: F(1, 113) = 4.68, p = 0.03) but did not differ between tasks (main effect of Task: F(1, 113) = 0.12, p = 0.73; Task × Block: F(1, 113) = 0.002, p = 0.97), suggesting that participant became faster in their judgments with training for both tasks. Each MRS acquisition lasted for 5 min and 56 s, and each MRS training block (400 trials) for 11 min and 11 s ± 63 s. Thus, in most cases training was completed within the duration of the training block (i.e., 2 MRS acquisitions × 5 min 56 s). In the event that the training took longer than the MRS block, the next MRS acquisition was delayed until completion of the previous training block.
Data acquisition
Experiments were conducted at the Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, using a Siemens 7T Magnetom (Siemens, Erlangen) with a 32-channel head coil.
We acquired structural data (MPRAGE; TR 2200 ms; TE 2.82 ms; slice thickness 1.0 mm; in-plane resolution 1.0 × 1.0 mm2; GRAPPA factor = 4) and echo planar imaging data (gradient echo-pulse sequences) from 40 slices (TR 2250 ms; TE 28 ms; slice thickness 3.0 mm; in-plane resolution 3.0 × 3.0 mm2; GRAPPA factor = 2, 140 volumes).
We acquired MRS data using a semi-localization by adiabatic selective refocusing (semi-LASER) sequence73 (64 averages, TR 5010 ms, TE 36 ms). We measured two MRS voxels (2 × 2 × 2 cm3 isotropic), one in the left OCT (OCT voxel) and one in the left PPC (PPC voxel), avoiding contact with the dura to minimize macromolecule contamination. We focused on the left hemisphere as previous fMRI50 and TMS35 studies have shown that the left PPC is involved in suppressing distracting signals. To cover both areas with the same dielectric pad, we placed both MRS voxels on the left hemisphere.
Statistical analyses
To compare changes in behavioral performance and neurotransmitter concentrations across blocks, tasks, and MRS voxels, we used a LME approach. LME models allow modeling of longitudinal data (i.e., multiple measurements over time) and can account for missing values across participants. In each of the models, we tested both for random (Participants) and fixed (MRS Block, Voxel, and Task) effects. To relate behavioral improvement to GABA changes and rs-fMRI connectivity, we computed Pearson skipped correlations using the Robust Correlation Toolbox74. This method accounts for potential bivariate outliers and determines statistical significance using bootstrapped confidence intervals (CI) for 1000 permutations. Note that bivariate outliers are not shown in the data figures. To directly compare the relationship of GABA change and rs-fMRI connectivity with behavioral improvement between the two tasks, we used linear regression models with interaction terms. Data distribution assumptions of normality and heteroscedasticity of variance were verified using Shapiro–Wilk and Levene’s tests, respectively. All statistical tests are two sided.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Adam Berrington and Clark Lemke for help with the data collection, Joseph Giorgio and Ke Jia for help on preliminary data analysis, Rogier Kievit, Delia Fuhrmann, and Mark Haggard for advice on statistical analyses, Adrian Garcia for preliminary work, Jasper Poort for helpful comments. This work was supported by funding to Z.K. from the Alan Turing Institute, the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (grants: H012508, P021255), the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme (grant FP7/2007–2013 under agreement PITN-GA 2011–290011), and the Wellcome Trust (grant 205067). C.J.S. holds a Sir Henry Dale Fellowship, funded by the Wellcome Trust and the Royal Society (102584/Z/13/Z). E.L.H. is supported by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre. The Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust (203139/Z/16/Z).
Author contributions
P.F. conceived the study, collected the data, performed the analysis and wrote the manuscript. U.E.E. provided acquisition and analysis tools for MR Spectroscopy. V.K. performed the analysis of the rs-fMRI data. C.N., E.L.H. and S.L. collected the data. H.B. and C.J.S guided the study design and analysis. Z.K. conceived the study, guided the analysis, and wrote the manuscript.
Data availability
Data files have been uploaded on the Cambridge Data Repository: 10.17863/CAM.33382.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Journal peer review information: Nature Communications thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41467-019-08313-y.
References
- 1.Fine I, Jacobs RA. Comparing perceptual learning tasks: a review. J. Vis. 2002;2:190–203. doi: 10.1167/2.2.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gilbert CD, Sigman M, Crist RR. The neural basis of perceptual learning. Neuron. 2001;31:681–697. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00424-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Goldstone RL. Perceptual learning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1998;49:585–612. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Dosher BA, Jeter P, Liu J, Lu ZL. An integrated reweighting theory of perceptual learning. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:13678–13683. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312552110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dosher BA, Lu ZL. Perceptual learning reflects external noise filtering and internal noise reduction through channel reweighting. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1998;95:13988–13993. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.23.13988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Li RW, Levi DM, Klein SA. Perceptual learning improves efficiency by re-tuning the decision ‘template’ for position discrimination. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:178–183. doi: 10.1038/nn1183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kourtzi Z. Visual learning for perceptual and categorical decisions in the human brain. Vision. Res. 2010;50:433–440. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.09.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Welchman, A. E. & Kourtzi, Z. Linking brain imaging signals to visual perception. Vis. Neurosci. 30, 229–241 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 9.Logothetis NK. What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature. 2008;453:869–878. doi: 10.1038/nature06976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Castro-Alamancos Ma, Donoghue JP, Connors BW. Different forms of synaptic plasticity in somatosensory and motor areas of the neocortex. J. Neurosci. 1995;15:5324–5333. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-07-05324.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Trepel C, Racine RJ. GABAergic modulation of neocortical long-term potentiation in the freely moving rat. Synapse. 2000;35:120–128. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(200002)35:2<120::AID-SYN4>3.0.CO;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Levy LM, Ziemann U, Chen R, Cohen LG. Rapid modulation of GABA in sensorimotor cortex induced by acute deafferentation. Ann. Neurol. 2002;52:755–761. doi: 10.1002/ana.10372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Floyer-Lea A, et al. Rapid modulation of GABA concentration in human sensorimotor cortex during motor learning. J. Neurophysiol. 2006;95:1639–1644. doi: 10.1152/jn.00346.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sampaio-Baptista C, et al. Changes in functional connectivity and GABA levels with long-term motor learning. Neuroimage. 2015;106:15–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Edden RAE, Muthukumaraswamy SD, Freeman TCA, Singh KD. Orientation discrimination performance is predicted by GABA concentration and gamma oscillation frequency in human primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:15721–15726. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4426-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Rokem A, et al. Broader visual orientation tuning in patients with schizophrenia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2011;5:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Song C, Sandberg K, Andersen LM, Blicher JU, Rees G. Human occipital and parietal GABA selectively influence visual perception of orientation and size. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:8929–8937. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3945-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Lunghi C, Emir UE, Morrone MC, Bridge H. Short-term monocular deprivation alters GABA in the adult human visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:1496–1501. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Puts NAJJ, Edden RAEE. In vivo magnetic resonance spectroscopy of GABA: a methodological review. Prog. Nucl. Magn. Reson. Spectrosc. 2012;60:29–41. doi: 10.1016/j.pnmrs.2011.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Tkác I, Oz G, Adriany G, Uğurbil K, Gruetter R. In vivo 1H NMR spectroscopy of the human brain at high magnetic fields: metabolite quantification at 4T vs. 7T. Magn. Reson. Med. 2009;62:868–879. doi: 10.1002/mrm.22086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Chang DHF, Kourtzi Z, Welchman AE. Mechanisms for extracting a signal from noise as revealed through the specificity and generality of task training. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:10962–10971. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0101-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Chalk M, Masset P, Gutkin B, Deneve S. Sensory noise predicts divisive reshaping of receptive fields. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2017;13:1–26. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Moldakarimov, S., Bazhenov, M. & Sejnowski, T. J. Top-down inputs enhance orientation selectivity in neurons of the primary visual cortex during perceptual learning. PLoS Comput. Biol. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003770 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 24.Hamann M, Rossi DJ, Attwell D. Tonic and spillover inhibition of granule cells control information flow through cerebellar cortex. Neuron. 2002;33:625–633. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00593-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mitchell SJ, Silver RA. Shunting inhibition modulates neuronal gain during synaptic excitation. Neuron. 2003;38:433–445. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00200-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wehr M, Zador AM. Balanced inhibition underlies tuning and sharpens spike timing in auditory cortex. Nature. 2003;426:442–446. doi: 10.1038/nature02116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Leventhal, A. G., Wang, Y., Pu, M., Zhou, Y. & Ma, Y. GABA and its agonists improved visual cortical function in senescent monkeys. Science300, 812–815 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 28.Li G, et al. GABA-mediated inhibition correlates with orientation selectivity in primary visual cortex of cat. Neuroscience. 2008;155:914–922. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.06.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Freedman DJ, Ibos G. An integrative framework for sensory, motor, and cognitive functions of the posterior parietal cortex. Neuron. 2018;97:1219–1234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Law CT, Gold JI. Shared mechanisms of perceptual learning and decision making. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2010;2:226–238. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01044.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ahissar M, Hochstein S. The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004;8:457–464. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Li S, et al. Learning shapes the representation of behavioral choice in the human brain. Neuron. 2009;62:441–452. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Li S, et al. Learning shapes the representation of behavioral choice in the human brain. Neuron. 2009;62:441–452. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Mayhew SD, Li S, Kourtzi Z. Learning acts on distinct processes for visual form perception in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:775–786. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2033-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Chang DHF, Mevorach C, Kourtzi Z, Welchman AE. Training transfers the limits on perception from parietal to ventral cortex. Curr. Biol. 2014;24:2445–2450. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.08.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Baldassarre A, et al. Individual variability in functional connectivity predicts performance of a perceptual task. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;113:E6723–E6723. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113148109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Stagg C, et al. Local GABA concentration is related to network-level resting functional connectivity. eLife. 2014;2014:1–9. doi: 10.7554/eLife.01465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Towers SK, et al. α5 subunit-containing GABAA receptors affect the dynamic range of mouse hippocampal kainate-induced gamma frequency oscillations in vitro. J. Physiol. 2004;559:721–728. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.071191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Shmuel A, Leopold DA. Neuronal correlates of spontaneous fluctuations in fMRI signals in monkey visual cortex: implications for functional connectivity at rest. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 2008;29:751–761. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bachtiar V, Near J, Johansen-Berg H, Stagg C. Modulation of GABA and resting state functional connectivity by transcranial direct current stimulation. eLife. 2015;4:1–9. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kapogiannis D, Reiter DA, Willette AA, Mattson MP. Posteromedial cortex glutamate and GABA predict intrinsic functional connectivity of the default mode network. Neuroimage. 2013;64:112–119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Cabral J, Hugues E, Sporns O, Deco G. Role of local network oscillations in resting-state functional connectivity. Neuroimage. 2011;57:130–139. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hayes, A. F. PROCESS: A Versatile Computational Tool for Observed Variable Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Modeling (Guildford Press, 2012). https://doi.org/978-1-60918-230-4
- 44.Kolasinski, J. et al. A mechanistic link from GABA to cortical architecture and perception. Curr. Biol. 27, 1685–1691 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 45.Heba S, et al. Local GABA concentration predicts perceptual improvements after repetitive sensory stimulation in humans. Cereb. Cortex. 2016;26:1295–1301. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Stagg C, Bachtiar V, Johansen-Berg H. The role of GABA in human motor learning. Curr. Biol. 2011;21:480–484. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Scholl J, et al. Excitation and inhibition in anterior cingulate predict use of past experiences. eLife. 2017;6:e20365. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Shibata, K. et al. Overlearning hyperstabilizes a skill by rapidly making neurochemical processing inhibitory-dominant. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 470–475 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 49.Kolasinski, J. et al. The dynamics of cortical GABA in human motor learning. J. Physiol. 597, 271–282 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 50.Mevorach C, Humphreys GW, Shalev L. Reflexive and preparatory selection and suppression of salient information in the right and left posterior parietal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2009;21:1204–1214. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Gottlieb J. From thought to action: the parietal cortex as a bridge between perception, action, and cognition. Neuron. 2007;53:9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mazurek ME, Roitman JD, Ditterich J, Shadlen MN. A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. Cereb. Cortex. 2003;13:1257–1269. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Gilbert CD, Li W. Adult visual cortical plasticity. Neuron. 2012;75:250–264. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Phillips EA, Hasenstaub AR. Asymmetric effects of activating and inactivating cortical interneurons. eLife. 2016;5:e18383. doi: 10.7554/eLife.18383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Adesnik H, Bruns W, Taniguchi H, Huang ZJ, Scanziani M. A neural circuit for spatial summation in visual cortex. Nature. 2012;490:226–231. doi: 10.1038/nature11526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.van Versendaal, D. & Levelt, C. N. Inhibitory interneurons in visual cortical plasticity. Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 73, 3677–3691 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 57.Khan AG, et al. Distinct learning-induced changes in stimulus selectivity and interactions of GABAergic interneuron classes in visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:851–859. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0143-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Stagg C, et al. Neurochemical effects of theta burst stimulation as assessed by magnetic resonance spectroscopy. J. Neurophysiol. 2009;101:2872–2877. doi: 10.1152/jn.91060.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Ashton AC, Ushkaryov YA. Properties of synaptic vesicle pools in mature central nerve terminals. J. Biol. Chem. 2005;280:37278–37288. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M504137200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Stagg C, et al. Relationship between physiological measures of excitability and levels of glutamate and GABA in the human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 2011;589:5845–5855. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.216978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Tremblay, S. et al. Relationship between transcranial magnetic stimulation measures of intracortical inhibition and spectroscopy measures of GABA and glutamate+glutamine. J. Neurophysiol. 109, 1343–1349 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 62.Johnstone, A., Levenstein, J. M., Hinson, E. L. & Stagg, C. Neurochemical changes underpinning the development of adjunct therapies in recovery after stroke: a role for GABA? J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 38, 1564–1583 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 63.Stagg C. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy as a tool to study the role of GABA in motor-cortical plasticity. Neuroimage. 2014;86:19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Mason GF, et al. Decrease in GABA synthesis rate in rat cortex following GABA-transaminase inhibition correlates with the decrease in GAD67 protein. Brain Res. 2001;914:81–91. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02778-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Kaufman DL, Houser CR, Tobin AJ. Two forms of the γ‐aminobutyric acid synthetic enzyme glutamate decarboxylase have distinct intraneuronal distributions and cofactor interactions. J. Neurochem. 1991;56:720–723. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1991.tb08211.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Garraghty PE, Lachica EA, Kaas JH. Injury-induced reorganization of somatosensory cortex is accompanied by reductions in GABA staining. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 1991;8:347–354. doi: 10.3109/08990229109144757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Shen J, et al. Determination of the rate of the glutamate/glutamine cycle in the human brain by in vivo 13C NMR. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1999;96:8235–8240. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.14.8235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Manor D, et al. The rate of turnover of cortical GABA from [1-13C]glucose is reduced in rats treated with the GABA -transaminase inhibitor vigatrin (g-vinyl GABA) Neurochem. Res. 1996;21:1031–1041. doi: 10.1007/BF02532413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Roelfsema PR, Ooyen A. Attention-gated reinforcement learning of internal representations for classification. Neural Comput. 2005;17:2176–2214. doi: 10.1162/0899766054615699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Stettler DD, Das A, Bennett J, Gilbert CD. Lateral connectivity and contextual interactions in macaque primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2002;36:739–750. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01029-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.O’Reilly RC, Wyatte D, Herd S, Mingus B, Jilk DJ. Recurrent processing during object recognition. Front. Psychol. 2013;4:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Bogacz R, Brown E, Moehlis J, Holmes P, Cohen JD. The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. Psychol. Rev. 2006;113:700–765. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.4.700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Scheenen, T., Klomp, D., van de Moortele, P., Adriany, G. & Heerschap, A. Semi-LASER 1H-MR spectroscopic imaging of the human brain at 7T. Proc. 16th Sci. Meet. Int. Soc. Magn. Reson. Med. 59, 1–6 (2008).
- 74.Pernet, C. R., Wilcox, R., Rousselet, G. A., Finch, H. & Budden, J. S. Robust correlation analyses: false positive and power validation using a new open source Matlab toolbox. Front Psychol.10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00606 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 75.Stagg C, et al. Polarity-sensitive modulation of cortical neurotransmitters by transcranial stimulation. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:5202–5206. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4432-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data files have been uploaded on the Cambridge Data Repository: 10.17863/CAM.33382.