Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2019 Jan 31;14(1):e0210620. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210620

Sleeping through anything: The effects of unpredictable disruptions on mouse sleep, healing, and affect

Amy Robinson-Junker 1,¤,*, Bruce O’Hara 2,#, Abigail Durkes 1,#, Brianna Gaskill 3
Editor: Matthew Parker4
PMCID: PMC6354982  PMID: 30703113

Abstract

Many aspects of the laboratory environment are not tailored to the needs of rodents, which may cause stress. Unpredictable stressors can cause ulcers, prolonged pituitary-adrenal activation, and anhedonia. Similarly, pain has been demonstrated to slow wound healing, and mice experiencing pain exhibit altered behavior. However it is unknown how husbandry, which occurs when the mice are inactive, and lack of analgesia, specifically in a punch biopsy procedure, effects animal physiology, behavior, and welfare, particularly as it relates to sleep fragmentation. We hypothesized that sleep fragmentation, induced by unpredictable husbandry and lack of pain management will slow wound healing. Two main treatments were tested in a factorial design in C57BL/6 mice of both sexes (64 mice total); 1) analgesia (carprofen and saline) and 2) sleep disruptions (random and predictable). Mice were singly housed in a non-invasive sleep monitoring apparatus on arrival (Day -4). Disruption treatments were applied from Day -3 to 2. All mice received a punch biopsy surgery (Day 0) with topical lidocaine gel and their analgesic treatment prior to recovery, and on Days 1 and 2. Nesting behavior was assessed daily and a sugar cereal consumption test, as a measure of anhedonia, was conducted on Days -1 to 2. On Day 3, mice were euthanized and wound tissue and adrenal glands were collected. We found that the disruption predictability had no effect on mouse sleep, wound healing, or adrenal cortex:medulla ratio. It’s possible that the disruption period was not long enough to induce chronic stress. However, male mice who received analgesia slept more than their female counterparts; this may be related to sex differences in pain perception. Overall, it does not appear that the predictability of disturbance effects sleep fragmentation or stress responses, indicating that husbandry activities do not need to occur at set predictable times to improve welfare.

Introduction

As reproducibility and successful translation of research findings become more difficult to achieve, the scientific community has begun looking for explanations and solutions. One particular area of interest has been the effect of the laboratory environment, and the animals’ experience of it, on research models [16]. The laboratory environment has been tailored to human preferences as much, if not more, than rodent needs. In turn, those unmet needs may then induce physiological outcomes that disrupt research activities. Mice experience cold stress at normal laboratory temperatures, depleting their energetic resources for reproduction[79]. Routinely provided corncob bedding is aversive and decreases sleep[10, 11], and typical handling[12, 13] induces stress and alters performance in behavioral tests. Furthermore, laboratory mice are nocturnal, but live in a diurnal environment to accommodate human workers. This raises a real risk that human activity during mouse rest periods is interfering with their sleep quality, quantity, or both. That interference may then, in turn, be altering research findings and complicating our ability to translate them to humans.

We have previously investigated the effect of timing (day or night) of husbandry disturbances (day vs night) [14]. We found that mice slept the same amount of time (both percentage of time spent sleeping and mean bout length) whether they were disturbed at 10:00 AM or 10:00 PM. However, the timing of their sleep shifted in response to disturbance timing, though in a very sex and strain or stock specific way. However, limiting entrance to the room to 1 time, in a specific 1 hour period of the day is impractical in a working vivarium setting. This led us to wonder how we could apply these findings in a usable manner. We thought that the unpredictability of human activity in the vivarium could affect mouse sleep in a way that a brief, predictable disruption did not. Of particular concern was sleep fragmentation (the interruption of sleep either through waking or transitioning to a lighter sleep stage). It can induce physiologic, metabolic, and (if experienced during gestation) epigenetic effects, including slowed wound healing[1519]. However, there is a major gap in the literature regarding the impact of routine human activity on mouse sleep.

The ultimate function of sleep appears to be that of renewal. Anesthetized mice experience increased interstitial cerebrospinal fluid flow, which refreshes ADP into ATP and removes amyloid plaques [20]. Sleep is also a period of increased activity for pro-inflammatory cytokines, which assist the healing process [2123]. Sleep even improves the immune response to vaccines [24]. So it should come as no surprise that sleep disruption could have serious impacts on mouse welfare and research outcomes.

What we know of sleep fragmentation in mice has typically come from studies using mice as a proxy for humans with sleep apnea or periodic limb movements [25]. Mechanized disruptions are generally used to induce frequent arousals from sleep (every 1–2 minutes) [15, 16, 26], rather than trying to mimic vivarium situations and experiences that mice are exposed to. In other words, we know about mouse sleep disruption when treating them like humans, but we don’t know much about it when treating them like mice.

Unpredictability is stressful for animals; rats who receive unpredictable shocks develop ulcers[27] and anhedonia[28], and rats given a choice will choose a predictable shock over an unpredictable shock[29, 30]. Typical vivariums involve multiple unpredictable disruptions. Animals from several projects may be housed in the same room, meaning researcher activities may not be coordinated. Running water, cleaning equipment, and even caretakers can vary on a daily basis. Not only are mice experiencing unpredictability of disruption, but these disruptions are also occurring during the light phase, when mice would ordinarily be sleeping. This combination may be sufficient to induce sleep fragmentation and stress.

One method of assessing the physiological effects of stress is through measuring wound healing; increased stress leads to slow or imperfect healing[3138]. One stressor known to slow wound healing is pain [3944]. Pain slows the healing process in humans [4244] and alters general behavior; similarly, after experiencing a painful procedure, mice burrow less and build less complex nests[4548], and are slower to incorporate new nesting material into an existing nest[49]. Additionally, pain interferes with sleep [50, 51], and sleep deprivation can induce hyperalgesia in rats[52]. This suggests that a vicious cycle may exist between these factors and requires that the interaction between pain, sleep, and healing be considered.

The effects of sleep disruption are not solely physiological. Work in both humans [5356] and rodents [57] has shown cognitive changes after sleep deprivation and disruption, and sleep dysfunction is also associated with mood disorders in humans [5860]. These findings indicate that an investigation of the potential welfare implications of sleep disruption should also include assessment of changes in mental well-being.

Our hypotheses were that unpredictable disruptions are more disruptive to mouse sleep than predictable disruptions. We also hypothesized that pain, following from lack of post-operative analgesia would negatively affect nesting behavior and sleep patterning. We predicted that mice who experienced frequent, unpredictable disruptions during their normal rest period would sleep less and/or have more fragmented sleep during the day and have stronger indicators of stress (decreased sleep, wound healing, and sucrose consumption) than those whose disruptions occurred at predictable times. We also predicted that mice who received analgesia (rather than a control injection) would sleep more during their normal rest period and have weaker indicators of stress.

Materials and methods

Ethical statement

This study was approved by the Purdue Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol 1512001333), and conformed to all guidelines put forward by both the committee and the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals [61]. At the start of study, animals were free of a list of common mouse infectious agents; further details may be found at http://www.criver.com/files/pdfs/rms/hmsummary.aspx. All mice were monitored daily by trained members of the research team for food and water consumption and overall health status, with no adverse conditions or health outcomes noted.

Experimental design, animals, and housing

Two main treatments, in a factorial design, were assessed in naive C57BL/6NCrl mice of both sexes (6 weeks of age; Charles River, Kingston, NY); 1) sleep disruption (unpredictable or predictable) and 2) analgesia administration (analgesia and saline). Each factorial combination had four replicates for a total of 32 mice (Table 1). Mice were tested from May to June of 2016.

Table 1. Experimental factorial design.

Disruptions Predictable Unpredictable
Sex Male Female Male Female
Analgesia Y N Y N Y N Y N
Replicates 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4

Factorial design with number of replicates (mice) per combination of treatments. Mead’s rule was used to determine the number of mice needed based on our experimental design.

Mice were housed in one of the two sleep monitoring apparatuses (Fig 1). Each apparatus houses 4 mice, each in a separate chamber; this allowed us to test 8 mice simultaneously. The apparatus uses a piezoelectric mat underneath each cage to detect vibrational movement of the mouse and therefore mice must be housed singly. Customized software (MouseRec Data Toolbox, Signal Solutions, Lexington KY) uses an algorithm to process the signal and discern sleeping respiratory patterns from waking respiratory patterns; this algorithm has been validated using EEG, EMG, and visual evaluation [62]. A different algorithm also permits quantification of activity level, where higher numbers indicate greater intensity of activity; we used this measure in an effort to discern whether awake mice changed their activity levels.

Fig 1. Sleep apparatus viewed from above (A) and a close up side view of an individual mouse cage (B).

Fig 1

Sugary cereal used for the sucrose preference test can be seen in B.

Visual barriers were in place between cages, but audible and olfactory contact was still possible. Each cage included a built in food hopper and water bottle opening. Each cage (6.0 in x 6.1 in x 6.1 in) was bedded with 32g of laboratory grade aspen shavings (Harlan, Indianapolis IN) and 8g of nesting material (Enviro Dri, Shepherd Specialty Papers, Watertown, TN). Mice were provided with an 18% protein laboratory diet (Harlan 2018, Indianapolis IN) and reverse osmosis filtered water ad libitum. Lights were kept on a 12:12 light/dark cycle, with lights on at 05:00 and off at 17:00 hours. The room was maintained at 72± 2 F, and 36–64% humidity. Upon arrival (Day -4 –see Fig 2), mice were randomly assigned to an analgesia treatment using a random number generator (www.random.org). The experimenter was not blinded as to their assignment, because the experimenter also prepared and administered the medications. Mice were weighed and placed in their cage within the sleep apparatus no longer than 1 hour before lights out.

Fig 2. Experimental timeline.

Fig 2

Lists all measurements made on each day of experiment. Day -1 is considered baseline. Mice arrive on Day -4. Abbreviations: BWT—bodyweight; Sleep—sleep monitoring; TINT—Time to Integrate Nesting Material Test; Food—food consumption; Sucrose—sucrose consumption; Analgesia—analgesia treatment.

Disruption treatments

Sleep disruptions began immediately after arrival. Because all testing was conducted in a single room, all 8 mice in a test group were exposed to the same disruption treatment (unpredictable or predictable) simultaneously. Both treatments consisted of the same 8 disruptions—presence of a stranger, a recorded conversation playing in the room, a radio playing pop music, cage changing noises, presence of a t-shirt that was worn by a man, running water, a running cage changing station with ventilation hood, and floor disinfection (Table 2). These disruptions were chosen based upon activities that occur in a typical vivarium and factors that are known to alter mouse behavior (such as the presence of a male investigator, or a shirt worn by one [63]). The order and duration of the disruptions were initially scheduled in a random fashion, but the schedule itself was consistent across disruption replicates. For instance, all mice experienced the same disruptions on the same day during the experiment, for the same durations. The only difference was whether they were spread randomly throughout the day (unpredictable) or consolidated at the beginning and end of the day (predictable). No disruption was repeated in the same day, and there were a total of 4 disruptions per day. Potential durations of disruption were 15, 30, 45, or 60 minutes; floor disinfection and running water only lasted 15 minutes due to practical and environmental considerations. In the unpredictable disruption group, the interval between disruptions was also randomized, with intervals between them of either 45, 60, 90, or 120 minutes. For the predictable disruption group, disruptions occurred between 2.5–3.5 hours after lights on (7:30–8:30) or within an hour of lights off (16:00–17:00), with two disruptions scheduled for the morning period, and two for the evening period. The exception to this schedule was on the morning of the punch biopsy procedure; no disruptions were scheduled that morning.

Table 2. Sleep disruptions and durations.
Disruption Description Duration Number of Occurrences
Cage change Investigator removes mice from cage, supplies fresh bedding and nesting material, replaces mouse 30 min 1
Cage change noise Investigator rattles cages containing corncob bedding and lids 45 min, 60 min 3 (45 min x1, 60 min x2)
Conversation Smartphone used to play back each of two specific stand up comedy tracks (65–72 dB at cage level) 45 min 2
Exhaust fan Exhaust fan of cage changing station turned on (62 dB at cage level) 30 min, 60 min 3 (30 min x1, 60 min x2)
Floor cleaning Investigator uses power washer to distribute cleaning solution, scrubs floors with scrub brush, rinses with bucketed water, then squeegees floor dry 15 min 1
Male t-shirt Investigator places t-shirt that was worn the night before in the room near the cages 30 min, 60 min 3 (30 min x1, 60 min x2)
Music Antenna radio tuned to local rock music station 15 min, 30 min, 45 min, 60 min 4
Running water Water left running in stainless steel sink (58–62 dB at cage level) 15 min 3
Stranger Unfamiliar person sits or stands quietly in room without interacting with mice 15 min, 45 min 2
Unfamiliar smell Investigator sits quietly in room while wearing strongly scented lotion 30 min 1

Disruption descriptions, durations, and number of occurrences. When disruptions occurred more than once, different durations were possible; if that was the case, all duration times are listed. All mice experienced all disruptions.

Analgesia treatment

Mice assigned to the analgesia treatment group received 10mg/kg carprofen subcutaneously on Day 0 (after wounding), Day 1, and Day 2. Mice in the analgesia control group received an equal volume of saline subcutaneously on the same days as the analgesia mice. On Day 1 and 2, a dorsal access mouse restrainer (Braintree Scientific, Braintree MA) was used to hold the mice while an investigator administered a subcutaneous injections in the caudal region, avoiding manipulation of the surgical area and the potential risk of medication leaking from the surgical site.

Punch biopsy procedure

After 4 days of disruptions (Day 0), all mice were anesthetized with isoflurane in an induction chamber and maintained on isoflurane administered via nose cone. We clipped and sterilized the cervical area of each mouse, placed them in lateral recumbency, and pulled the dorsal skin away from the animal, as if scruffing them. We then utilized a 3mm biopsy punch (Sklar Surgical Instruments, West Chester, PA) to push through both layers of skin, creating 2 symmetrical 3mm full-thickness wounds. The wounds were not sutured, stapled, or glued. Surgical order was balanced to account for order effects. During this procedure, we used a chemical hand-warmer (HotHands, Kobayashi Americas, Dalton GA) to provide thermal support to the mice. All mice, regardless of analgesia group, received 0.05 mL of 2% lidocaine gel topically applied to each wound for short-term local analgesia. Mice then received their assigned analgesic treatment. Mice were then moved to heated recovery cages until they were ambulating normally. Once recovered, they were returned to their home cage in the sleep apparatus. Two post-operative health checks were performed two hours apart. Sleep disruptions resumed as scheduled that afternoon.

Behavioral testing

Sleep and activity data were collected continuously via the sleep apparatus. We began data collection once the final mouse was housed on Day -4 (prior to 17:00) and ended by 9:00 the morning of Day 3, prior to euthanasia.

Mice were TINT tested[49] to assess pain and general welfare. In brief, in the TINT we provide a small amount of new nesting material to mice 2–3 hours after lights on and gave them 10 minutes to integrate this new material into their existing nest. A positive TINT score means the material has been incorporated, and suggests positive welfare. A negative TINT score suggests that the mice in that cage may be experiencing poor welfare, and personnel should investigate further. For this project, an investigator would enter the room, cut a Nestlet (Ancare, Bellmore, NY) into 4 equal squares, deliver one piece to each cage, and leave the room for 10 minutes. Upon returning to the room, the investigator assessed whether or not the material had been integrated into the nest. In this case, ‘integrated’ means ‘had been transported to the main body of the nest’. TINT testing occurred daily at 8:00 AM. This time corresponds with peak nest-building behavior [48]. The scores on Days -3 to -1 were considered ‘practice’, as mice have been shown to shorten their latency to incorporate material with repeated exposures [49, 64], so data presented from Day -1 is used as their baseline TINT.

Sucrose preference testing was used to assess anhedonia[65]. We did this by providing mice with 5g of sugary cereal (Froot Loops, Kelloggs, Battle Creek MI; a fruit-flavored breakfast cereal with approximately 12 g of sugar per 29 g of cereal) between 16:00 and 17:00 (prior to lights out), and then weighing the remainder between 7:30 and 8:00 the next morning. This allowed us to calculate the amount of cereal consumed each night; a decrease in consumption is indicative of anhedonia. We conducted these tests on Day -1, Day 1, and Day 2.

Sample collection

On Day 3, mice were euthanized via carbon dioxide. Immediately after euthanasia, mice were weighed and the punch biopsy area was excised, as well as surrounding tissues. Adrenal glands were also collected, in order to assess HPA axis activation[66, 67]. All tissues were fixed in 10% neutral buffered formalin and embedded in paraffin. Sections 5 μm thick were stained with haematoxylin and eosin according to standard methods. Microscopic examination was performed by a board-certified veterinary pathologist and the interpretation was based on standard histopathological morphology. The pathologist (AD) was blinded to the treatment groups. Wound width and re-epithelialization were quantified for all mice. Wound width was defined as the distance between wound margins in which the original epidermis was intact. Re-epithelialization was calculated as amount of newly formed epidermis as a percentage of the wound margin. Newly formed epidermis was defined as less than 3 cell layers thick of squamous epithelium devoid of stratum corneum.

Adrenal glands were sectioned en toto and representative sections were cut 50 micrometers deep. One adrenal gland per mouse was used to calculate an average cortex to medulla length ratio. Three cortical lengths and 3 cross-sectional medulla lengths were averaged and a ratio was calculated for each mouse.

Data analysis

The experimental unit in all analyses was the individual mouse, and main treatments were disruption and analgesia. All data, with the exception of TINT success/failure, were analyzed using up to 3rd degree factorial General Linear Model (GLM) in JMP (version 11, SAS Institute Inc) of the following factors: sex, disruption treatment, analgesia treatment, experiment day and (for sleep and activity data) lights on or off. To calculate food consumption, regular diet and sucrose cereal consumption were combined to calculate the total intake, where applicable. Individual mouse was the experimental unit and was used as a random factor, with sex, disruption treatment, and analgesia treatment nested within it. Cage location and sleep apparatus were used as blocking factors. Bodyweight was included as a covariate with food consumption, sucrose consumption, and adrenal cortex:medulla ratio. We used square root transformation for sleep bout length and activity level data, and log transformation for adrenal cortex:medulla ratio, in order to meet the assumptions of GLM. The assumptions of GLM (normality of error, homogeneity of variance, and linearity) were confirmed post-hoc[68]. Significant effects were then analyzed using post-hoc Tukey tests. All values are given as least squares means ± standard error (LSM ± SE).

TINT success/failure was analyzed using up to 3rd degree factorial Generalized Linear Model (GLIM) for binomial logistic regression, with Firth-adjusted bias, for the following factors: sex, disruption treatment, analgesia treatment, and day of experiment. Cage was used as a fixed factor, with sex, disruption treatment, and analgesia treatment nested within it. Cage location and apparatus number were used as blocking factors. Non-significant 3rd degree interactions were removed from the model, which produced a lower AICc number, denoting an improved model fit[69]. The full model had an AICc number of 220.972, while the reduced model’s number was 215.399. Pairwise planned contrasts were subsequently conducted on levels of significant factors to assess where differences arose, and were Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons.

For sleep measures, we excluded data from Day 0 because mice underwent surgery that day and that would alter sleep in a manner unrelated to our main treatments.

Results

Sleep measures

Proportion of time spent sleeping

We found multiple effects on proportion of time spent sleeping. A sex by analgesia treatment interaction (GLM, F(1, 21) = 6.38, P = 0.0196) indicated that males who received analgesia slept more than females with analgesia. However, no other differences between control animals or within the sex were observed. An interaction of sex and whether the lights were on or off was also significant (GLM, F(1, 184) = 5.34, P = 0.0219), with males sleeping more during lights off than females. Further a light phase by day in experiment interaction (GLM, F(3, 184) = 26.99, P < 0.0001; Fig 3) showed that animals slept less when the lights were on during Day 1 than Day -1, 2, or 3. Additionally, animals slept less during lights off during Day -1 than they did on Days 2 and 3. And mice slept more during lights on than lights off during Days 1, 2, and 3. Finally, a 3 way interaction between disturbance treatment, analgesia treatment, and light phase was significant (GLM, F(1, 184) = 14.32, P = 0.0002). However, this effect was solely due to light phase, with mice sleeping more when lights were on.

Fig 3. Average percentage of time spent sleeping by light phase and day of experiment.

Fig 3

Different letters indicate significant (Tukey, P < 0.05) differences within categories. Data presented are LSM and SE.

Sleep bout length

Mean sleep bout length had multiple significant interactions. Light phase by the day of experiment (GLM, F(3, 184) = 18.42, P < 0.0001; Fig 4) showed that, during lights on, mice had the shortest bout lengths on Day 1; during lights out, their bout lengths were shortest on Day 2. There was also a significant interaction between sex, analgesia treatment, and lights on/off (GLM, F(1, 184) = 4.48, P = 0.0356). However, post-hoc Tukey analysis showed no differences between groups. Sex by analgesia treatment (GLM, F(1,78.49) = 5.59, P = 0.0205) showed that female mice who received analgesia had shorter sleep bouts than those in the control group; there was no difference in the male mice, or within treatments.

Fig 4. Average sleep bout length by lights on/off and day of experiment.

Fig 4

Different letters indicate significant (Tukey, P < 0.05) differences within categories, bars indicate differences between categories. Data presented are LSM and SE.

Activity levels

Mean activity level analysis showed several significant factors. Light phase by day of experiment (GLM, F(3, 184) = 8.41, P < 0.0001; Fig 5) indicated a decrease in activity during lights off for Days 1 and 3. During lights on, mice were more active on Day 1 than on Days 2 or 3. Additionally, sex by analgesia treatment by day of experiment (GLM, F(3,184) = 3.64, P = 0.0139; Fig 6) demonstrated that female mice in the analgesia control group were less active on Day 1 than they were at baseline, and males in the analgesia treatment group were less active on Day 3 than at baseline. Finally, disruption treatment by analgesia treatment by light phase (GLM, F(1, 184) = 5.85, P = 0.0166) showed only one difference—that mice in the unpredictable disruption plus analgesia control group were more active during lights off than lights on; there were no other differences between lights on/off or treatment groups.

Fig 5. Mean activity level by light phase and day of experiment.

Fig 5

Different letters indicate significant differences within categories, bars with asterisks indicate differences between categories (Tukey, P < 0.05). Data were square root transformed for analysis; y-axis is back-transformed. Activity level is a linear measurement from 0 to 3; higher values indicate higher levels of activity. Data presented are LSM and SE.

Fig 6. Activity level by day of experiment, sex, and analgesia treatment.

Fig 6

Bars with asterisks indicate differences between categories (Tukey <0.05). Data were square root transformed for analysis; y-axis is back-transformed. Activity level is a linear measurement from 0 to 3; higher values indicate higher levels of activity. Data presented are LSM and SE.

Sucrose consumption

Sucrose consumption, used as a measure of anhedonia or decreased affect, was affected by the main effect of sex (GLM, F(1,24) = 5.49, P = 0.0277). Females were found to consume more sugared cereal than males. The day of the experiment was also significant (GLM, F(2,50) = 10.78, P = 0.0001). Mice consumed more sucrose on Days 1 and 2 than at baseline. Bodyweight was included as a covariate, but was not significant.

TINT

TINT success (a measure of general welfare) analysis had multiple significant effects. Mice at baseline were more likely to pass the TINT than on Days 1, 2, or 3 (GLIM, Χ2(3) = 25.17, P < 0.0001). The interaction of sex by disruption treatment was significant (GLIM, Χ2(1) = 6.82, P = 0.0090) but Bonferroni-corrected contrasts did not reveal any significant post-hoc comparisons. Sex by analgesia treatment interaction was also significant (GLIM, Χ2(1) = 11.98, P = 0.0005). Males given analgesia were more likely to succeed than controls. Additionally, females in the control group were more likely to succeed than their male counterparts. Finally, disruption treatment by analgesia treatment (GLIM, Χ2(1) = 7.84, P = 0.0051) was significant, but Bonferroni-corrected contrasts did not provide any significant comparisons.

Food consumption

Total food consumption had two significant main effects, sex (GLM, F(1, 20.91) = 4.99, P = 0.0366) and day of experiment (GLM, F(3, 77.35) = 40.90, P < 0.0001). Female mice consumed more than males, and mice at baseline and Day 1 consumed more than those on Day 2 and Day 3. Bodyweight was a significant covariate (GLM, F(1, 21.76) = 8.86, P = 0.007); as bodyweight increased, so did food consumption.

Bodyweight

Bodyweight was affected by 3 main effects. Sex (GLM, F(1, 27.71) = 53.26, P < 0.0001) showed that males were heavier than females. Day of experiment (GLM, F(3,75) = 32.21, P < 0.0001) indicated that mice weighed more at baseline than Day 1, but less than on Days 2 and 3. Finally, disruption treatment (GLM, F(1, 27.71) = 7.81, P = 0.0093) was significant, with mice in the unpredictable disruption group weighing more than those in the predictable group.

Histopathology

There were no significant factors in either percent re-epithelialization or adrenal cortex:medulla ratio.

Discussion

Few of our hypotheses (decreased proportion of time spent sleeping, shorter sleep bouts, decreased wound healing, decreased sucrose consumption, and increased adrenal cortex:medulla ratio) were supported by our results (Table 3). Proportion of time spent sleeping and sleep bout length were unaffected by predictability in disruption treatments, which was where we had expected to see the strongest results. This may be an example of anthropomorphism, where we as humans assumed that what we find unpleasant would also be aversive to the mice. While the investigator (ARJ) found stopping work constantly to conduct disruptions very frustrating and distracting, the mice did not seem to have been affected in the same way. However, it’s not clear whether the difference in perception by the mice was a matter of intensity of disruption, valence of disruption, or both. In fact, when compared to the percentage of time spent sleeping for C57BL/6 mice in our previous study with this sleep apparatus [14], it would appear that neither sleep disruption case had a strong effect.

Table 3. Measures, indication, hypotheses, and observations.

Measure What it indicates Hypothesized change Observed change
Percentage of time sleeping Level of sleep disruption ↓ percentage for unpredictable disruptions No change, except for decrease on Day 1 post-op
Sleep bout length Level of sleep fragmentation ↓ for unpredictable disruptions, ↓ for analgesia controls ↓ for female mice with analgesia
Activity level Behavioral patterning changes ↓ post-op ↓ in unpredictable disruption x analgesia control group
TINT Presence of pain or ↓ general welfare ↓ for controls and unpredictable disruptions post-op ↓ for analgesia controls
Sucrose preference Anhedonia—negative effective state ↓ consumption for unpredictable disruptions and post-op ↑ consumption post-op
Re-epithelialization of wounds Wound healing quality ↓ re-epithelialization for unpredictable disruptions and no analgesia No difference
Adrenal cortex:medulla ratio Chronic HPA axis activation ↑ ratio for unpredictable disruptions No difference

Measures, what they indicate, our hypothesized changes, and observed changes.

Punch biopsies are used for wound healing studies[31, 34, 35, 7074], and also for identification[61]. However, there is no consensus on analgesia protocols for mice who have had this procedure[31, 75, 76]. This seems to be a concern, since male mice who received analgesia spent more time sleeping than their female counterparts. This may be related to sex differences in pain perception. Females, in both humans and rodents, have been reported to perceive pain more intensely than males [7780]. So while male mice might have experienced sufficient pain relief from the carprofen dosage, the females may not. This doesn’t explain why female mice were more likely to succeed in the TINT, which is an indicator of pain. It’s possible that female mice were, due to their lower bodyweight and increased heat loss to the environment, more motivated to nest build for thermoregulation in spite of their discomfort. In the future, either higher doses of analgesia or perhaps a combination of non-steroidal and opioid medication could achieve effective relief, particularly for female mice, without altering behavior.

Additionally, females who got analgesia had shorter sleep bout lengths than controls. This implies fragmentation of sleep for treated mice, which is unexpected, as decreased pain perception would be expected to improve sleep quality rather than decrease it. However, some early research into NSAIDs indicated that their administration may affect sleep quality, through moderation of prostaglandin production, inhibition of melatonin synthesis, and increased body temperature during sleep phases [81, 82]. This work has not been done in rodents, but perhaps a similar phenomenon occurred with our female mice.

Histopathology measures were unaffected by any of our treatments. The sleep disruptions, and subsequent stress that these treatments were meant to induce, may not have been sufficiently intense and/or prolonged enough to induce adrenal morphology changes, and were more acute than chronic. In studies where adrenal changes have been noted, durations of stressors have been at least 2 weeks[8386], and when a stressor only lasted for one week, changes were not observed[87]. As far as the wound re-epithelialization, in rodents, wounds contract quickly due to their panniculus carnosus[35]. This is a layer of muscle that permits their skin to contract for healing. A wound splint process may have been helpful to prolong the healing process and more accurately assess re-epithelialization (more similarly to humans) [35]. However, it’s also possible that we didn’t sufficiently disrupt sleep in the mice, and therefore wound healing was not impaired.

Sucrose consumption results were also unexpected. We predicted that mice would have higher baseline consumption than any post-operative time point, regardless of treatments. Instead, we found exactly the opposite. Perhaps these mice required repeated exposures to overcome any food neophobia[88], needing time to learn that the cereal was highly palatable. Alternatively, this sucrose consumption pattern may be a reflection of how long mice actually need to acclimate to a new environment after transport. Baseline sucrose testing began for our mice approximately 3 days after arrival, with disruptions already occurring. These mice may not have been disturbed enough to change their sleeping patterns, but a decrease in general affect may have caused them to consume less sucrose.

Similarly our results for TINT success rate were lower than expected. The validation work on TINT demonstrated that mice at baseline were consistently successful after a few training tests [49, 64]; this was not the case for our mice. However, the mice in the referenced work had been present at the study facility for much longer than ours had (personal communication from BNG) and were almost certainly more acclimated to their environment.

One thing that was not surprising was the decrease in activity levels on Days 2 and 3. While perhaps counterintuitive, because presumably the mice were healing and should have been experiencing less pain, those days corresponded with the first restraint and injections the mice received. Mice responded negatively to these events, urinating, defecating, and vocalizing. This was the only time vocalizations were observed during the project. This suggests that the mice found the restraint extremely aversive, and their subsequent activity levels may be a reflection of that. We know that mice react differently to different types of handling[89], and that nest scores can be reduced after being handled by a novel individual[12]; this drop in activity may be a manifestation of their apparent aversion to unconditioned handling.

While our results didn’t support our hypotheses, they do raise some interesting questions regarding acclimation periods, sex differences in pain response, and just how disruptive human activity actually is to mice (particularly in regards to sleep). This project would suggest that direct interaction and restraint with the mice is more stressful than mere investigator presence or noise. However, this was only conducted with one strain of mice, over a relatively short time period. It is possible that mice in longer term projects may experience those events differently. At this time, we can’t make many recommendations, other than considering longer acclimation periods prior to commencing research, and investigating the longer term effects of carprofen use at higher doses for effective analgesia, particularly for female mice.

Supporting information

S1 File. Data set with SAS code for analyses performed.

(DOCX)

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its supporting information files.

Funding Statement

This project was funded by a 3500 pound small project grant from the Universities Federation for Animal Welfare (https://www.ufaw.org.uk/small-project-and-travel-awards/small-project-and-travel-awards ARJ and BG). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Mogil JS. Laboratory environmental factors and pain behavior: the relevance of unknown unknowns to reproducibility and translation. Lab Animal. 2017;46(4):136–41. 10.1038/laban.1223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gaskill BN, Garner JP. Stressed out: providing laboratory animals with behavioral control to reduce the physiological effects of stress. Lab Animal. 2017;46(4):142–5. 10.1038/laban.1218 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Prescott MJ, Lidster K. Improving quality of science through better animal welfare: the NC3Rs strategy. Lab Animal. 2017;46(4):152–6. 10.1038/laban.1217 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Jirkof P. Side effects of pain and analgesia in animal experimentation. Lab Animal. 2017;46(4):123–8. 10.1038/laban.1216 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kokolus KM, Capitano ML, Lee CT, Eng JW, Waight JD, Hylander BL, et al. Baseline tumor growth and immune control in laboratory mice are significantly influenced by subthermoneutral housing temperature. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(50):20176–81. 10.1073/pnas.1304291110 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Eng JW, Reed CB, Kokolus KM, Pitoniak R, Utley A, Bucsek MJ, et al. Housing temperature-induced stress drives therapeutic resistance in murine tumour models through beta2-adrenergic receptor activation. Nature communications. 2015;6:6426 Epub 2015/03/11. 10.1038/ncomms7426 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Gaskill BN, Gordon CJ, Pajor EA, Lucas JR, Davis JK, Garner JP. Heat or insulation: behavioral titration of mouse preference for warmth or access to a nest. PLoS One. 2012;7(3):e32799 10.1371/journal.pone.0032799 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gaskill BN, Gordon CJ, Pajor EA, Lucas JR, Davis JK, Garner JP. Impact of nesting material on mouse body temperature and physiology. Physiol Behav. 2013;110–111:87–95. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.12.018 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gaskill BN, Pritchett-Corning KR, Gordon CJ, Pajor EA, Lucas JR, Davis JK, et al. Energy reallocation to breeding performance through improved nest building in laboratory mice. PLoS One. 2013;8(9):e74153 10.1371/journal.pone.0074153 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Krohn TC, Hansen AK. Evaluation of Corncob as Bedding for Rodents. Scand J Lab Anim Sci. 2008;35(4):231–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Leys LJ, McGaraughty S, Radek RJ. Rats Housed on Corncob Bedding Show Less Slow-Wave Sleep. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 2012;51(6):764–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Gaskill BN, Pritchett-Corning KR. Nest building as an indicator of illness in laboratory mice. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 2016;180:140–6. 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.04.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Chesler EJ, Wilson SG, Lariviere WR, Rodriguez-Zas SL, Mogil JS. Identification and ranking of genetic and laboratory environment factors influencing a behavioral trait, thermal nociception, via computational analysis of a large data archive. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002;26(8):907–23. 10.1016/s0149-7634(02)00103-3 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Robinson-Junker AL, O'Hara BF, Gaskill BN. Out Like a Light? The Effects of a Diurnal Husbandry Schedule on Mouse Sleep and Behavior. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci. 2018;57(2):124–33. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Carreras A, Zhang SX, Peris E, Qiao Z, Gileles-Hillel A, Li RC, et al. Chronic Sleep Fragmentation Induces Endothelial Dysfunction and Structural Vascular Changes in Mice. Sleep. 2014;37(11):1817–24. 10.5665/sleep.4178 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hakim F, Wang Y, Carreras A, Hirotsu C, Zhang J, Peris E, et al. Chronic Sleep Fragmentation During the Sleep Period Induces Hypothalamic Endoplasmic Reticulum Stress and PTP1b-Mediated Leptin Resistance in Male Mice. Sleep. 2015;38(1):31–U367. 10.5665/sleep.4320 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Khalyfa A, Carreras A, Almendros I, Hakim F, Gozal D. Sex Dimorphism in Late Gestational Sleep Fragmentation and Metabolic Dysfunction in Offspring Mice. Sleep. 2015;38(4):545–57. 10.5665/sleep.4568 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Short M, Banks S. The Functional Impact of Sleep Deprivation, Sleep Restriction, and Sleep Fragmentation In: Bianchi MT, editor. Sleep Deprivation and Disease: Springer; New York; 2014. p. 13–26. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Trammell RA, Toth LA. Effects of Sleep Fragmentation and Chronic Latent Viral Infection on Behavior and Inflammation in Mice. Comparative medicine. 2015;65(3):173–85. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Xie L, Kang H, Xu Q, Chen MJ, Liao Y, Thiyagarajan M, et al. Sleep Drives Metabolite Clearance from the Adult Brain. Science. 2013;342(6156):373–7. 10.1126/science.1241224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Krueger JM, Obál F, Fang J, Kubota T, Taishi P. The Role of Cytokines in Physiological Sleep Regulation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2001;933(1):211–21. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05826.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Opp MR. Cytokines and sleep. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2005;9(5):355–64. 10.1016/j.smrv.2005.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wu X, Lu Y, Dong Y, Zhang G, Zhang Y, Xu Z, et al. The inhalation anesthetic isoflurane increases levels of proinflammatory cytokine TNF-α, IL-6 and IL-1β. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33(7):1364–78. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.11.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Prather AA, Hall M, Fury JM, Ross DC, Muldoon MF, Cohen S, et al. Sleep and Antibody Response to Hepatitis B Vaccination. Sleep. 2012;35(8):1063–9. 10.5665/sleep.1990 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Toth LA, Bhargava P. Animal Models of Sleep Disorders. Comparative medicine. 2013;63(2):91–104. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Baud MO, Magistretti PJ, Petit J-M. Sustained Sleep Fragmentation Induces Sleep Homeostasis in Mice. Sleep. 2015;38(4):567–U108. 10.5665/sleep.4572 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Abbott BB, Schoen LS, Badia P. Predictable and unpredictable shock: behavioral measures of aversion and physiological measures of stress. Psychol Bull. 1984;96(1):45–71. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Willner P, Towell A, Sampson D, Sophokleous S, Muscat R. REDUCTION OF SUCROSE PREFERENCE BY CHRONIC UNPREDICTABLE MILD STRESS, AND ITS RESTORATION BY A TRICYCLIC ANTIDEPRESSANT. Psychopharmacology. 1987;93(3):358–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Freeman J, Badia P. Do Rats Prefer Information About Shock Intensity. B Psychonomic Soc. 1975;6(1):75–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Badia P, Coker C, Harsh J. Choice of Higher Density Signaled Shock over Lower Density Unsignaled Shock. J Exp Anal Behav. 1973;20(1):47–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Padgett DA, Marucha PT, Sheridan JF. Restraint stress slows cutaneous wound healing in mice. Brain Behavior and Immunity. 1998;12(1):64–73. 10.1006/brbi.1997.0512 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Sheridan JF, Padgett DA, Avitsur R, Marucha PT. Experimental models of stress and wound healing. World Journal of Surgery. 2004;28(3):327–30. 10.1007/s00268-003-7404-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Broadbent E, Petrie KJ, Alley PG, Booth RJ. Psychological stress impairs early wound repair following surgery. Psychosomatic medicine. 2003;65(5):865–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Davidson JM. Experimental animal wound models. Wounds. 2001;13(1):9–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Dunn L, Prosser HC, Tan JT, Vanags LZ, Ng MK, Bursill CA. Murine model of wound healing. J Vis Exp. 2013;(75):e50265 10.3791/50265 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Ernst K, Tuchscherer M, Kanitz E, Puppe B, Manteuffel G. Effects of attention and rewarded activity on immune parameters and wound healing in pigs. Physiology & Behavior. 2006;89(3):448–56. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.House SL. Psychological distress and its impact on wound healing: an integrative review. J Wound Ostomy Continence Nurs. 2015;42(1):38–41. 10.1097/WON.0000000000000080 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Landis CA, Whitney JD. Effects of 72 hours sleep deprivation on wound healing in the rat. Research in Nursing & Health. 1997;20(3):259–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Albert N, da Silva C, Diez-Noguera A, Cambras T. Different adaptation of the motor activity rhythm to chronic phase shifts between adolescent and adult rats. Behav Brain Res. 2013;252:347–55. 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.06.025 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Graham JE, Robles TF, Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Malarkey WB, Bissell MG, Glaser R. Hostility and pain are related to inflammation in older adults. Brain Behav Immun. 2006;20(4):389–400. Epub 2005/12/27. 10.1016/j.bbi.2005.11.002 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Matsuzaki K, Upton D. Wound treatment and pain management: a stressful time. International Wound Journal. 2013;10(6):638–44. 10.1111/j.1742-481X.2012.01038.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.McGuire L, Heffner K, Glaser R, Needleman B, Malarkey W, Dickinson S, et al. Pain and wound healing in surgical patients. Ann Behav Med. 2006;31(2):165–72. 10.1207/s15324796abm3102_8 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Soon K, Acton C. Pain-induced stress: a barrier to wound healing. WOUNDS UK. 2006;2(4):92. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Woo KY. Exploring the effects of pain and stress on wound healing. Advances in skin & wound care. 2012;25(1):38–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Jirkof P. Burrowing and nest building behavior as indicators of well-being in mice. J Neurosci Methods. 2014;234:139–46. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.02.001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Jirkof P, Cesarovic N, Rettich A, Arras M. Housing of female mice in a new environment and its influence on post-surgical behaviour and recovery. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 2013;148(3–4):209–17. 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.08.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Jirkof P, Cesarovic N, Rettich A, Nicholls F, Seifert B, Arras M. Burrowing behavior as an indicator of post-laparotomy pain in mice. Front Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:165 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00165 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Jirkof P, Fleischmann T, Cesarovic N, Rettich A, Vogel J, Arras M. Assessment of postsurgical distress and pain in laboratory mice by nest complexity scoring. Lab Anim. 2013;47(3):153–61. 10.1177/0023677213475603 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Rock ML, Karas AZ, Rodriguez KBG, Gallo MS, Pritchett-Corning K, Karas RH, et al. The Time-to-Integrate-to-Nest Test as an Indicator of Wellbeing in Laboratory Mice. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 2014;53(1):24–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Lautenbacher S, Kundermann B, Krieg JC. Sleep deprivation and pain perception. Sleep Med Rev. 2006;10(5):357–69. Epub 2006/01/03. 10.1016/j.smrv.2005.08.001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Patel M, Chipman J, Carlin BW, Shade D. Sleep in the intensive care unit setting. Critical care nursing quarterly. 2008;31(4):309–18; quiz 19–20. 10.1097/01.CNQ.0000336816.89300.41 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ibironke GF, Ajonijebu CO. Sleep Deprivation-Induced Hyperalgesia in Rodents: Some Neurochemical Mechanisms. Neurophysiology. 2015;46(5):411–4. 10.1007/s11062-015-9466-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Dinges DF, Orne MT, Whitehouse WG, Orne EC. Temporal placement of a nap for alertness: contributions of circadian phase and prior wakefulness. Sleep. 1987;10(4):313–29. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Harding K, Feldman M. Sleep disorders and sleep deprivation: An unmet public health problem. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;47(4):473–4. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Maquet P. The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science. 2001;294(5544):1048–52. Epub 2001/11/03. 10.1126/science.1062856 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.McCoy JG, Strecker RE. The cognitive cost of sleep lost. Neurobiology of learning and memory. 2011;96(4):564–82. 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.07.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Colavito V, Fabene PF, Grassi-Zucconi G, Pifferi F, Lamberty Y, Bentivoglio M, et al. Experimental sleep deprivation as a tool to test memory deficits in rodents. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2013;7:106 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Asarnow LD, Soehner AM, Harvey AG. Circadian rhythms and psychiatric illness. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2013;26(6):566–71. 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328365a2fa [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Foster RG, Peirson SN, Wulff K, Winnebeck E, Vetter C, Roenneberg T. Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in social jetlag and mental illness. Prog Mol Biol Transl Sci. 2013;119:325–46. 10.1016/B978-0-12-396971-2.00011-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Jagannath A, Peirson SN, Foster RG. Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in neuropsychiatric illness. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23(5):888–94. 10.1016/j.conb.2013.03.008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.National Research Council. Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals: Eighth Edition Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2011. 248 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Donohue KD, Medonza DC, Crane ER, O'Hara BF. Assessment of a non-invasive high-throughput classifier for behaviours associated with sleep and wake in mice. Biomed Eng Online. 2008;7:14 10.1186/1475-925X-7-14 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Sorge RE, Martin LJ, Isbester KA, Sotocinal SG, Rosen S, Tuttle AH, et al. Olfactory exposure to males, including men, causes stress and related analgesia in rodents. Nat Meth. 2014;11(6):629–32. http://www.nature.com/nmeth/journal/v11/n6/abs/nmeth.2935.html#supplementary-information. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Rock ML, Karas AZ, Gallo MS, Pritchett-Corning K, Gaskill BN. Housing condition and nesting experience do not affect the Time to Integrate to Nest Test (TINT). Animal Welfare. 2014;23(4):381–5. 10.7120/09627286.23.4.381 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Papp M, Willner P, Muscat R. AN ANIMAL-MODEL OF ANHEDONIA—ATTENUATION OF SUCROSE CONSUMPTION AND PLACE PREFERENCE CONDITIONING BY CHRONIC UNPREDICTABLE MILD STRESS. Psychopharmacology. 1991;104(2):255–9. 10.1007/bf02244188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Heat and Burns Physiology and Behaviour of Animal Suffering: Blackwell Publishing; 2008. p. 72–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Miyamoto H, Mitani F, Mukai K, Suematsu M, Ishimura Y. Studies on cytogenesis in adult rat adrenal cortex: circadian and zonal variations and their modulation by adrenocorticotropic hormone. Journal of biochemistry. 1999;126(6):1175–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Grafen A, Hails R. Modern statistics for the life sciences: learn how to analyse your experiments: OUP; Oxford; 2002. i–xv, 1–351 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Akaike H. New Look at Statistical-Model Identification. Ieee T Automat Contr. 1974;Ac19(6):716–23. 10.1109/Tac.1974.1100705 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Ansell DM, Campbell L, Thomason HA, Brass A, Hardman MJ. A statistical analysis of murine incisional and excisional acute wound models. Wound Repair and Regeneration. 2014;22(2):281–7. 10.1111/wrr.12148 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Padgett DA, Marucha PT, Sheridan JF. Stress and Wound Healing: Animal Models Psychoneuroimmunology, Vols I and Ii, 4th Edition 2007:837–50. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Pyter LM, Yang L, McKenzie C, da Rocha JM, Carter CS, Cheng B, et al. Contrasting mechanisms by which social isolation and restraint impair healing in male mice. Stress-the International Journal on the Biology of Stress. 2014;17(3):256–65. 10.3109/10253890.2014.910761 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Sheridan FJ, Padgett AD, Avitsur R, Marucha TP. Experimental Models of Stress and Wound Healing. World Journal of Surgery. 2004;28(3):327–30. 10.1007/s00268-003-7404-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Wang X, Ge J, Tredget EE, Wu Y. The mouse excisional wound splinting model, including applications for stem cell transplantation. Nat Protocols. 2013;8(2):302–9. http://www.nature.com/nprot/journal/v8/n2/abs/nprot.2013.002.html#supplementary-information. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.University of Pennsylvania Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Mouse Anesthesia and Analgesia Recommendations. 2014.
  • 76.University of Illinois at Chicago Office of Animal Care and Institutional Biosafety. Guidelines—Rodent Surgical Classifications and Analgesic Guidelines.
  • 77.Cairns BE, Gazerani P. Sex-related differences in pain. Maturitas. 2009;63(4):292–6. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2009.06.004 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Craft RM. Sex differences in drug- and non-drug-induced analgesia. Life Sci. 2003;72(24):2675–88. 10.1016/S0024-3205(03)00178-4. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Mapplebeck JC, Beggs S, Salter MW. Sex differences in pain: a tale of two immune cells. Pain. 2016;157 Suppl 1(2):S2–6. 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000389 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Mogil JS, Bailey AL. Sex and gender differences in pain and analgesia In: Savic I, editor. Sex Differences in the Human Brain, Their Underpinnings and Implications. Progress in Brain Research. 186 2010. p. 141–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Murphy PJ, Badia P, Myers BL, Boecker MR, Wright KP Jr. Nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs affect normal sleep patterns in humans. Physiol Behav. 1994;55(6):1063–6. Epub 1994/06/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Murphy PJ, Myers BL, Badia P. Nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs alter body temperature and suppress melatonin in humans. Physiol Behav. 1996;59(1):133–9. Epub 1996/01/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Gamallo A, Villanua A, Trancho G, Fraile A. Stress adaptation and adrenal activity in isolated and crowded rats. Physiol Behav. 1986;36(2):217–21. 10.1016/0031-9384(86)90006-5 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Trentani A, Kuipers SD, Meerman GJT, Beekman J, ter Horst GJ, den Boer JA. Immunohistochemical changes induced by repeated footshock stress: revelations of gender-based differences. Neurobiology of Disease. 2003;14(3):602–18. 10.1016/j.nbd.2003.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Marti O, Gavalda A, Jolin T, Armario A. Effect of regularity of exposure to chronic immobilization stress on the circadian pattern of pituitary adrenal hormones, growth hormone, and thyroid stimulating hormone in the adult male rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1993;18(1):67–77. Epub 1993/01/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Moraska A, Deak T, Spencer RL, Roth D, Fleshner M. Treadmill running produces both positive and negative physiological adaptations in Sprague-Dawley rats. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2000;279(4):R1321–9. Epub 2000/09/27. 10.1152/ajpregu.2000.279.4.R1321 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Zelena D, Mergl Z, Földes A, Kovács KJ, Tóth Z, Makara GB. Role of hypothalamic inputs in maintaining pituitary-adrenal responsiveness in repeated restraint. American Journal of Physiology—Endocrinology And Metabolism. 2003;285(5):E1110–E7. 10.1152/ajpendo.00219.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Kronenberger JP, Medioni J. Food neophobia in wild and laboratory mice (Mus musculus domesticus). Behav Processes. 1985;11(1):53–9. 10.1016/0376-6357(85)90102-0 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Hurst JL, West RS. Taming anxiety in laboratory mice. Nat Methods. 2010;7(10):825–6. 10.1038/nmeth.1500 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 File. Data set with SAS code for analyses performed.

(DOCX)

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are within the paper and its supporting information files.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES