Abstract
In recent years, the emerging field of computational psychiatry has impelled the use of machine learning models as a means to further understand the pathogenesis of multiple clinical disorders. In this paper, we discuss how autism spectrum disorder (ASD) was and continues to be diagnosed in the context of its complex neurodevelopmental heterogeneity. We review machine learning approaches to streamline ASD’s diagnostic methods, to discern similarities and differences from comorbid diagnoses, and to follow developmentally variable outcomes. Both supervised machine learning models for classification outcome and unsupervised approaches to identify new dimensions and subgroups are discussed. We provide an illustrative example of how computational analytic methods and a longitudinal design can improve our inferential ability to detect early dysfunctional behaviors that may or may not reach threshold levels for formal diagnoses. Specifically, an unsupervised machine learning approach of anomaly detection is used to illustrate how community samples may be utilized to investigate early autism risk, multidimensional features, and outcome variables. Because ASD symptoms and challenges are not static within individuals across development, computational approaches present a promising method to elucidate subgroups of etiological contributions to phenotype, alternative developmental courses, interactions with biomedical comorbidities, and to predict potential responses to therapeutic interventions.
Introduction and autism’s history
In the 1940s, Kanner and Asperger separately published descriptions of patients who were aloof or withdrew from others and had socioemotional limitations in functioning1,2. Kanner highlighted that patients with autism insisted that things remained the same and were acutely upset when routines were changed. Although distinctions between Kanner’s early infantile autism and Asperger’s syndrome were made because of increased language, cognitive performance, and detailed knowledge in the Asperger’s group, each patient cohort highlights differences in intellectual abilities and their motivation for behavioral outcomes. In the early 20th century, dominantly held nurture-based theories blamed early maternal-child interactions and marginalized parents of affected children. Parent groups started to organize and advocate for children with autism by the 1960s3. Subsequently, autism research moved towards seeking biological etiologies and creating educational intervention strategies to increase functional capacities. Over the last forty to fifty years, the search for biological causes has largely focused on studying core elements of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). However, attempting to define a homogenous disorder group has been challenging.
ASD’s history captures many of the tensions between categorical and dimensional frameworks for psychiatric diagnoses. Traditional approaches rely on criteria lists that require clinicians to make dichotomous and categorical decisions, even though some individuals demonstrate significant symptoms that do not reach threshold for the disorder. Dimensional classifications and assessments conceptualize disorders as quantitatively rather than qualitatively different from a healthy state or a normative life course. There has been a historical sequence in autism classification, based on lumping or splitting its features based on clinical presentation, functional attributes, and genetic syndromes (e.g., Rett’s syndrome included in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, DSM IV)4. As early as the 1970s, genetic twin studies suggested strong heritability of the constellation of ASD symptoms5,6. However, specific genes were difficult to discover and clinical descriptions of “infantile autism” overlapped with childhood schizophrenia and psychosis in early versions of the DSM. More specific definitions of “autism” by the 1980s included impaired social communication and language, fear of change, and symptoms of odd interests manifesting before thirty months of age. Most ASD research to date has been designed with the categorical approaches defined by the DSM4,7 or the International Classification of Diseases8.
After the federal government made autism a special education category in 1990, they began collecting school data on identified children and quantifying access to special services. Children with high functioning ASD and Asperger’s often participated in typical classroom settings. Differences in defining Asperger’s syndrome continued even after it was added to the DSM in 19949. Identification based on DSM definitions became more rigorous, valid, and reliable as diagnosticians captured detailed early developmental history from caregivers10,11 and tested social engagement through direct observational tasks such as the Autism Diagnosis Observation Schedule (ADOS) developed in the 1990s. These assessments became available commercially and have been increasingly used since the early 2000s11,12. Data collected has resulted in reclassification of symptom features and ASD’s behavioral subdomains. For example, the three domains of language communication, social deficits, and restrictive/repetitive behaviors were subsumed into two functional sub-areas of social communication and restrictive/repetitive behaviors13,14. Whereas the development of standardized tools has made categorical distinctions more valid and reliable, etiological discovery and treatment advances have continued to lag.
In this paper, we will discuss how ASD is currently diagnosed and what contributes to its clinical and neurobiological heterogeneity. We will also review computational methods that have attempted to streamline ASD’s diagnostic methods, distinguish differences from comorbidities, and follow developmentally variable outcomes. For example, supervised machine learning models train each data input with a corresponding target or known classification outcome, such as an existing diagnosis for ASD. Supervised methods may link data back to our already-existing categorizations. In contrast, unsupervised methods focus on many data inputs in order to find the structural relationships that occur between different inputs (e.g., symptoms/features that cluster together). Unsupervised methods allow us to identify new dimensions and categories from methods such as clustering approaches, factor analyses, and independent components analysis. This paper will highlight machine learning approaches from the marriage of computer science and statistics for pattern recognition applications in ASD. In addition, we will present an example of how ASD risk and symptom-related data can be ascertained within a community sample and analyzed using an unsupervised machine learning approach such as anomaly detection15. Specifically, it demonstrates how initial unsupervised methods could eventually use longitudinal feedback data for supervised methods to improve detection of early ASD signs. In sum, we will explore how novel computational methods with large datasets are particularly useful for studying complex neurodevelopmental disorders with multidimensional features and outcomes.
Contributions to ASD heterogeneity
Determining how to effectively parse complex and overlapping features of a disorder with significant clinical variability has been an enduring challenge to the field of autism research. As a disorder, ASD exemplifies multidimensional processes because of its intra- and inter-subject clinical heterogeneity. Considered against other psychiatric disorders, ASD’s phenotypic variability is considerable16,17. Researchers are moving away from ASD as a unitary construct and viewing it as an umbrella term for multiple syndromes18–20, resulting from multiple varying etiological pathways21. There have been attempts to study subgroups of clinical subphenotypes (e.g., history of regression, presence of intellectual disability or limited language) in order to examine potential mechanisms21–23 and treatment targets. However, these approaches require increasingly large sample sizes in conjunction with refined and nuanced methods of subphenotyping24–26.
In addition, co-morbid clinical features add to the complexity of ASD characterization and presentation over developmental periods when other clinical populations or larger control groups are compared. In child psychiatry, co-morbidity or convergently arising diagnoses are common. Youth rarely have one disorder consistent with adult-defined phenomenological categories. Over a third of individuals with ASD meet criteria for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)27, obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD)28,29, disruptive behavior disorders (that includes oppositional defiant disorder)30,31, anxiety and mood disorders31,32, intellectual disability33, or epilepsy34,35. Other commonly reported co-morbidities include specific language disorder, constipation, and other known genetic and medical disorders36,37. Diagnostic trends have switched from viewing cognitive, language, compulsive, attentional, behavioral, mood and anxiety symptoms as part of the disorder to being named independently when they are severe enough to warrant specific treatments27,38. Child psychiatrists have long known that certain disorders frequently emerge and present together. For example, ADHD, OCD, and Tourette’s or tic disorders39 as well as oppositional defiant disorder, ADHD, and minor depression/dysthymia40 are common triads. These often present by early school age and with varying severity of symptom clusters in boys versus girls. Some disorders may have earlier signs, but diagnoses are made when children struggle to reach or maintain expected milestones at school or home. Various diagnostic combinations occur with ASD and specific convergent diagnoses are increasingly being identified. Treatments often require modification based on cognitive features of ASD. Research studies that exclude other psychiatric disorders have limited application in the community because of the pathophysiological overlap between ASD and many comorbid disorders.
Computational psychiatry and new approaches to studying ASD
Computational psychiatry is of growing interest because it uses mathematical approaches to quantitatively investigate interacting variables across biobehavioral system levels within and between psychiatric disorders. As a newly emerging conceptual approach, it covers a range of strategies to characterize and investigate complex and interacting phenomena that contribute to outputs such as clinical presentation of neurobehavioral disorders. Computational methods can be applied at multiple levels in psychiatry by improving behavioral and biological diagnostic approaches (e.g., diagnostic or treatment-related biomarkers) and to subcategorize brain and behavioral dysfunction through the use of large datasets. For example, methods may be used to model neural circuits by accounting for multifactorial contributions (e.g., genetic and environmental factors) as explicit mathematical terms in order to test hypotheses about how multiple variables affect circuit function.
Time and progression of a disorder are important because psychiatric disorders present differentially across the lifespan and are nonlinearly influenced by biological processes related to growth, reproduction, or degeneration. Computational models in psychiatry have the potential to test how circuit or biological dysfunction at an initial time interval could create progressive disruptions through alterations in neural development and plasticity. These approaches have the potential to characterize individual differences required to ascertain “what is different about” how this specific child at this time “processes information about the world”; this is required to tailor biobehavioral interventions for subgroups41. As our technological ability to capture and share data increases, neural and other biological variables collected over time may be used to sequentially predict and discern behavioral outcomes at the level of the individual. Ultimately, computational and machine learning approaches will help subgroup multifactorial inputs and outputs in order to create specific treatment plans for individual children with ASD and other developmental disorders.
Machine learning approaches used to identify key diagnostic features of ASD
Highly standardized ASD assessments require more evaluation time than most psychiatric disorders and a high level of clinical training with ongoing reliability confirmation. As the need for assessments increase, care providers seek to decrease redundant measures and minimize the time to complete separate instruments. Given the range of signs and symptoms listed in the DSM, questions arise about whether some features are more important and central to the diagnostic category. Researchers are now utilizing large datasets required for genetic studies and analyses to address such concerns.
Several studies have evaluated machine learning as a means to shorten the clinician-expert administrated ADOS assessment, to test the accuracy of an observation-based classifier for rapid detection of autism risk, or to detect a minimal set of behaviors through feature selection-based algorithms42–44. An early machine learning classifier of scored behaviors reported 99.7% sensitivity and 94% specificity using 8 of the 29 items contained in ADOS Module 144. Although limiting items reduces testing time, this approach fails to consider that these expert testers were already drawing from broader information and their high level of training in diagnosis. For example, clinical evaluators integrate multifaceted information from their full encounters and do not assess subtest sections in isolation. Later, the authors retested the 8 items in subsequently larger datasets (autism = 1884, broader ASD = 449, and 283 non-ASD diagnoses) and reported sensitivity of 97.1% and specificity of 83.3%44. They attributed the lower specificity to the small number of controls used in the earlier study. The 8 items do not robustly produce optimal performance across each dataset previously combined to create the large sample45, suggesting that information from some of the remaining 21 items were also valuable. Subsequently, this group examined modules 2 and 3 of the ADOS, which are appropriate for individuals with higher language and cognitive abilities43. They reported between 98.3 and 97.7% accuracy using 9 of the 28 items from module 2 and 12 of the 28 items from module 3 to be sufficient to detect ASD risk, respectively.
Across all three ADOS focused studies described above, atypical eye contact, facial expressions (e.g., social smile in Module 1), interaction enjoyment, and joint attention were key features of ASD. In the modules requiring higher language and cognitive functioning, use of gestures, social communication or conversation, quality of social overtures, amount of reciprocal interactions, atypical motor mannerisms, and restricted/repetitive interests were also important features43. These studies suggest that cognitive level and daily functional abilities influence how many and what symptoms inform a diagnosis. This work also highlights that developmental level differentially influences the contribution of individual items. Future studies will be needed to account for chronological or adaptive age in streamlined diagnostic algorithms.
Data from detailed early developmental parent interviews obtained from the revised Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADI-R) were also investigated using machine learning methods. Wall and colleagues46 tested the accuracy of a 7-question classifier (reduced from 93 items of clinician-expert interview scores) in research datasets with the full standardized parent interview. Bone et al.45 were not able generate comparable findings when they used a larger dataset with more controls and severely affected ASD participants. In a follow up study, Bone and colleagues47 used machine learning to generate screening questions using the ADI-R and the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS)48. Both sensitivity and specificity were differentially weighted to achieve near-peak performance with five or fewer codes using Machine Learning-based fusion of ADI-R and SRS items. A screener algorithm for under versus over 10 years of age reached 89.2% (>10 years, 86.7%) sensitivity and 59.0% (>10 years, 53.4%) specificity for five behavioral codes. Note that demarcating age is important here and that items vary in importance over the developmental time course. The most frequently coded ADI-R items that overlap across papers include reciprocal conversation, direct gaze, and group play with peers. Authors highlight that it is possible to create robust, customizable screening or diagnostic instrument algorithms47. However, outcomes are different when controls with other difficulties or co-morbidities are included and age cutoffs are varied. Future testing with screening items alone in a community-based population versus a research clinic sample will be required to confirm the effectiveness of prioritizing specific or temporal features of ASD.
In addition to this diagnostic testing literature, there are prospective studies on neuroimaging of infants at high familial risk for ASD. For example, Emerson and colleagues49 utilized resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and a cross-validated machine learning algorithm applied to the imaging data collected at age 6 months to predict diagnostic outcomes at age 2 years. They reported a positive predictive value of 100% and negative predictive value of 96% and functional connections with social communication and repetitive behavior at age 2 years. See reviews of the literature on imaging and early identification of ASD50, as well as limitations of use of machine learning approaches with limited sample sizes in many current neuroimaging studies51.
Machine learning approaches may be used to compare frequent comorbidities and convergencies, such as ASD and ADHD
It is estimated that between 30–80% of individuals with ASD meet ADHD criteria52. The diagnostic time frame may overlap but tends to be later for identifying ADHD, which is more often noted with increased attentional demands required for abstract and analytical thinking in elementary education. DSM-5 modified symptoms being detected for ADHD prior to twelve years of age versus the earlier seven years that was required for DSM-IV-TR. ADHD also has subdomains of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. Some research has attempted to clarify overlapping and unique patterns of cognitive impairment for children with ASD versus ADHD53.
Research on ADHD alone has attempted to integrate behavioral and/or phenotypic information with brain functional and structural MRI. Anderson and colleagues54 used four Non-Negative Matrix Factorization algorithms to find the best fit for subnetworks that clustered with the ADHD-Inattentive diagnosis. Brain areas highlighted were the posterior cingulate, precuneus, and parahippocampal regions. Authors concluded that multimodal data in ADHD (N = 730) can be interpreted by latent dimensions and unsupervised computational approaches, adding to a growing number of studies using supervised computational approaches54.
Few studies have attempted cross-diagnostic classification across ASD and ADHD. One study, conducted by Lim and colleagues, reported high accuracy when discriminating ADHD from controls versus ASD (accuracy 85.2 vs. 79.3%) when applying a Gaussian process classification to gray matter volumetric data55. Another study considered both ASD and ADHD, but only compare each classification against controls and not with each other56. They used automated classification based on histograms of oriented gradients features extracted from MR brain images. Authors reported hold-out diagnostic accuracy ranged from 65.0–69.6% (over baseline 51.6–55.0%) in ASD and ADHD, respectively.
Using behavioral rating data, another study57 attempted to distinguish between ASD and ADHD by using different machine learning classification scores from the 65-item SRS. They tested six machine learning models on ASD (N = 2775) or ADHD (N = 150) individuals, reporting that five of the 65 behaviors measured were sufficient to distinguish ASD from ADHD (area under the curve = 0.965). Challenges with these studies occur because of the difficulty in subcategorizing the >20% number of children with ASD who also have significant ADHD.
In a recent review by Uddin et al.58, they summarize machine learning neuroimaging approaches from both populations. For ASD they reviewed 29 neuroimaging-based classification studies, and report how functional connectivity, gray matter volume, and default mode network approaches are being used to discriminate ASD from typical development. For ADHD alone, they reviewed nineteen studies showing that areas are more widespread but frontal and cerebellar regions appear to be important for classification compared to typical development. Obstacles for reliability and reproducibility include challenges of clinical heterogeneity in populations and standardization of data acquisition methods across sites. Addressing such heterogeneity is consistent with new research initiatives that are motivated to find biologically homogenous profiles of impairment. Identifying the structural and functional network signatures of multi-dimensionally-defined developmental profiles using computational psychiatry has the potential to move us toward a more biologically informed nosology, consistent with current research initiatives41,59–61.
Computational approaches used to study longitudinal changes in ASD
With increasing follow-up and standardized longitudinal data on individuals with ASD, computational methods are helpful for predicting outcomes by characterizing developmental trajectories. In computational models of neurodevelopmental disorders, time is an important variable because it captures sensitive and critical periods of growth that influence functional outcomes.
Lord and colleagues62 have published a series of papers examining trajectories of change in symptoms over the developmental course of ASD. In a clinic-referred population, latent class growth curve models assessed longitudinal data from 2 to 15 year olds (N = 345). The best-fit model identified a large subgroup (80%) with the stable high or stable moderate severity of ASD symptoms and two smaller groups with increases (9%) or decreases (7%) in severity over time. Although age, gender, race, and nonverbal IQ did not predict group membership, verbal IQ was maintained or increased over time in all groups and adaptive behavior worsened in all groups (except the small improving group). More recently, Lord and colleagues completed a growth curve analyses with a very detailed longitudinal follow-up (N = 85) of developmental trajectories from age 2 to 19 grouped by outcome62. Although the sample size was small, groupings were based on 19-year-old outcomes of verbal IQ, nonverbal IQ, social adaptive skills, and parent-reported social-communication. Differences in childhood trajectories for more or less cognitively able children were plotted over time beginning at age 2. Linear (Nonverbal IQ, ADI-R Repetitive Sensory Motor and Insistence on Sameness) and quadratic (Verbal-IQ, Vineland Social Adaptation, ADI-R Social Deficits) growth curves were shown. Differences in independent functioning and lack of comorbidity were associated with preschool through adolescent trajectories in social adaptation, social deficits, and insistence on sameness. Of note, change in social adaptation and decreased insistence on sameness distinguished ASD with higher cognitive abilities by adulthood from those with lower IQ outcomes. A small group of young adults who had childhood diagnoses of ASD (N = 8) with IQs in the average range were functioning socially and adaptively at age-appropriate levels.
Another study followed a cohort of children with ASD (N = 152) at three discrete time points and a subset of outcome measures over a ten year period63. Two distinct but parallel trajectories were identified for adaptive behavior and daily living skills. For social and communication, one trajectory showed increased growth while a flatter trajectory for adolescent outcomes was observed when participants started with lower cognitive and language skills, early epilepsy, and more severe ASD symptoms around age five.
More recently, another study reported longitudinal data (N = 105 children with ASD) using growth mixture model analysis with four assessments between the ages of 3–8 years64. Best-fit models produced one decreasing trajectory (73% of sample) and another moderate and stable class (27%) using a standardized adaptive functioning measure (Vineland-II)65. Focusing only within the preschool years, a multisite Canadian study used a semiparametric group-based approach to identify distinct mixtures of trajectories of ASD children (N = 421) over four time points (baseline, at 6 months and 12 months after baseline, and at age 6 years)66. Best-fit models showed an improvement in adaptive functioning in approximately 20% of the sample. In contrast, ASD symptom severity was more stable and only 11% of the sample showed a decrease in symptom severity. Taken together, these findings confirm that we have limited data over extended developmental periods, that outcome measures are inconsistent across studies, and that sample sizes need to be larger to better characterize heterogeneous trajectories of development with ASD.
Machine learning approaches to study ASD utilizing large or biologically defined datasets
In contrast to the highly specialized and intensive resources required to follow a clinical sample over time, another computational approach is to use large existing datasets that store health information as it was accessed. For example, electronic medical record time series analyses (6 month windows from birth to 15 years old) were used to examine comorbidities with ASD67. Hierarchical clustering methods were used to identify four groups (defined by salient features that included seizure, psychiatric, auditory, gastrointestinal) that were distinct from the larger sample and that could not be attributed to another medical cause. Three patterns of medical trajectories were identified using an unsupervised approach. Only the gastrointestinal and seizures disorder groups had between group correlations with both symptoms, and this finding was replicated in another sample population. Future research may use these methods of subgrouping to examine etiological risk factors related to ASD, including genetically-linked subgrouping based on specific comorbidities68.
Scientists often seek “causal” determinants that make disorders easier to classify in a binary categorical manner and to find targeted treatments or cures. For example, the gene required for Rett syndrome was identified when comparisons were made to the broader category of “idiopathic autism”69,70. Other studies have examined ASD phenotypic presentation and overlap in fragile X and Prader-Willi syndrome (PWS). A recent paper71 reported that 51% of males versus 18% of females with fragile X syndrome (FXS) have co-morbid ASD as assessed by the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). This comorbid subgroup had a higher prevalence of seizures, more sleep and behavior problems, and similar side effect profiles with some medications. They were underserved with behavioral treatments offered to children with “idiopathic” ASD. In contrast, 12.3% of children with PWS had ASD according to the ADOS-2 versus the 29–49% that screened positive for ASD with the SCQ72. Communication problems were observed in positive screens that did not make clinical diagnostic cut-off criteria. Genetic specificity was observed because the majority with ADOS-2 confirmed ASD also had maternal uniparental disomy PWS genetic subtype. This approach requires very large datasets in order to chip away at discovering small subgroups of the broader phenotype that can be attributed to specific genetic causes.
Although refined subgroup characterization promises to identify genetic or other etiological subtypes, the wide net of broader autism phenotype captures features of the many neurodevelopmental genetic syndromes altogether. Studies focusing on subgroups may be useful in understanding brain development across a broader population. For example, large clinical samples required to do genetic studies of ASD have recently been used to also study spatiotemporal development in the brain73. Authors calculated expression signatures specific to spatiotemporal windows (16 brain regions and 13 developmental stages). In order to identify when and where predicted ASD genes are specifically active, their analytic approach required carefully controlled permutation tests. There are large gaps of knowledge between causal and contributing genes and common neural networks that need to be targeted for educational and clinical interventions across overlapping phenotypic clusters.
Advances in precision or personalized medicine will require an ability to disentangle the complexity of overlapping symptoms in order to identify neurobehavioral pathways that specifically impact functional outcomes. For example, ASD researchers have struggled to identify circuit pathways or molecular mechanisms that lead to specific treatment targets because subgroups are influenced by additional dimensions of variability (e.g., attention deficits, intellectual disability, or severity of insistence on sameness). Moreover, sampling approaches intending to create “clean” study samples by excluding participants with other disorders have produced mixed outcomes. Clinicians already know that research methods optimized to increase homogeneity and reduce heterogeneity have limited utility for translating research outcomes to real-world communities. Long-term approaches will benefit from increased sample sizes of typical and atypical data from children to model developmental trajectories of ASD. This would increase our ability to find the weak links or pathways that lead to systemic and specific functional impairments41 as children face incremental challenges with age.
Given that ASD is known to emerge during the first years of life, understanding variability in early typical versus atypical development is likely to yield particularly important insights regarding heterogeneity. The data presented below uses a community sample that can be followed over time and a novel computational approach to examine risk for developing ASD-related symptoms. Focusing on early development is an essential step for creating and selecting treatments that target plastic neural systems and compensatory processes unique to this period.
Proof of principle: anomaly detection as a computational example for detecting ASD risk and variance in a community sample to be followed longitudinally
Anomaly detection focuses on identifying data that markedly deviates from the normal patterns that are observed within datasets. Although statistical approaches have detected outliers or anomalies since the 19th century74, current methods have advanced by integrating machine learning, data mining, information theory, and spectral theory in order to tackle specific data questions15. Atypical observations sometimes group together in clusters, but those clusters are often relatively small and less cohesive, and thus specialized techniques for finding anomalies have been developed in a wide variety of disciplines15,75. Multiple anomaly detection techniques are available, including techniques such as local outlier factor (LOF)76, one class support vector machines77, and autoencoder neural networks78.
Here, we demonstrate how unsupervised anomaly detection is used to identify early risk features that may predict autism or related disorders in early development. The data is from a community sample of data gathered from 1570 children between 17–25 months of age. Methods used have been previously described79,80 for online acquisition81 of the Video-Referenced Rating of Reciprocal Social Behavior82, the Repetitive Behavior Scale for Early Childhood80, and the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories83, along with demographic information. Over time, subsamples of these toddlers will be recruited to complete an independent follow-up evaluations with researchers blind to the online assessment data from the first time point. This longitudinal data will be used to give feedback information and to improve working models of early developmental heterogeneity related to autism versus other neurodevelopmental outcomes.
In this example, anomaly detection is initially being used to calculate how each parent rating about their child deviates across normal dimensional patterns of behavior. This approach produced anomaly scores that fell into a relatively small range around a central peak, with “true” anomalies extending into a rightward tail. Routines were considered robust after a process of checking the data. When anomalous cases were eliminated from the full dataset of 1570 children, few to no new anomalies were detected when the routines were rerun. To illustrate this process, we computed LOF anomaly scores76. Note that the scores incorporate information across many items and dimensions of behaviors. As represented in Fig. 1a, we identified 80 out of 1570 toddlers (or 5%) with an LOF score greater than 1.32, which we chose (for this example) as the threshold for being anomalous (95th percentile of LOF scores).
How accurate or stable is this computational model in identifying anomalies with these behavioral variables?
Figure 1 shows a histogram of LOF scores. Although it looks 2-dimensional in this plot, the LOF scores are calculated from many variables of each participant, which indeed represents multiple dimensions of behavior. For example, the distance between two points in 3-dimensional space is a 1- dimensional number. The accuracy of LOF threshold score can thus be tested. If 80 anomalous toddlers are omitted and LOF scores are recomputed, only 6 subjects obtain LOF scores greater than 1.32. Note these 6 had initial LOF scores near the 1.32 threshold. This suggests that this threshold is fairly accurate when all these behavioral variables are considered together (see Fig. 1d for listing of variables). For this dataset, 5% was chosen as the LOF threshold because it corresponded to a point at which the LOF distribution changed from a “bump,” i.e., a somewhat normal looking distribution to a more uniform distribution. Such a region represents a different and lower density area of the data space where anomalies are expected to be found. For other datasets, the appropriate LOF threshold might correspond to a different percentage of the data, e.g., 1 or 10%. Using this approach, we are able to test if an appropriate LOF threshold is identified for a particular population based on how the distribution is affected when anomalies are removed.
How accurate or stable is this computation with this sample size?
Many anomaly detection routines are also relatively robust to sample size. To test this in our data, we randomly sampled the data set to obtain 1000 half size samples. The overlap in anomalies found in the samples with the full data set is shown in the Fig. 1b. On average, 74% of the anomalies (LOF > 1.32) found in the half size samples occurred in the full data set. Furthermore, the correlation between the LOF scores in the full sample and the half sized sample is about 91%. On average 94% of the anomalies (LOF > 1.32) found in the half size samples have an LOF score of 1.22 or more in the full data set. These are signs that LOF scores are relatively stable for this current sample under consideration. Moreover, it suggests that the threshold determined as anomalous in a large sample could also be used for a smaller sample. Given our focus on heterogeneity, we are also able to evaluate how much the LOF score varied by comparing the LOF scores of a subject in the half size samples to the LOF score in the full sample.
How does this sample relate to previously collected population data?
With the initial developmental data, we were able to determine that 80 children had LOF > 1.32 scores out of the overall sample of 1570 toddlers. This represents about 5% of the sample. This percent of outliers is between the prevalence data of 13% for developmental disabilities and the 1.5–2% estimates for prevalence data of ASD84,85. Longitudinal follow up will confirm if around 3% of this sample will have closely related developmental disorders affecting language or developmental delays that do not fulfill full criteria for autism. There are different ways to view and select outliers and anomalies from typical development. An alternative way to view this data is to look at the number and identity of individuals outside of two standard deviations across variables (see Fig. 1c). Another way to view relationships between variables is to plot correlations between the variables assessed (Fig. 1d).
What are way to improve predictive models with a longitudinal study design?
Next steps would be test how predictive and accurate this unsupervised approach to identify children at risk is for later diagnoses of autism or related developmental disorders (e.g., developmental delay, specific language disorder, etc.). As we follow these children longitudinally, we will use supervised computational models to augment what is initially learned from the initial anomaly detection analyses. Expert examination and tracking outcome diagnosis back to the anomaly detection results will help refine the model. Multiple waves of data collection for ages 18–24 months, would allow for testing and retesting of the reliability of this computational approach as the sample size increases. Different trajectories may be observed. Subgroupings of anomalous subjects with similar clinical features may be identified earlier and be linked to similar etiologic factors or genetic risk.
Supervised anomaly detection approaches use class labels, but have the same goal of distinguishing anomalous and normal points. More generally, some supervised anomaly detection approaches produce an anomaly score that, as with unsupervised anomaly detection, allow for the ranking of data points according to how anomalous each clinical feature is. Combined with an understanding of the anomaly detection algorithms, supervised feedback could be used to refine the algorithms and filter the results for those relevant to identifying subjects at high risk for ASD. Recent implementations of anomaly detection approaches have been able to detect patterns and outliers in sequence data86,87.
These advances will afford the opportunity to model complex, sequential data such as those observed in neurodevelopmental disorders. Other analytical methods can be tested and compared with the results of anomaly detection methods, including clustering techniques such as k-means, hierarchical, and shared nearest neighbor88. Alternative supervised approaches such as ensemble techniques89 may be employed to further incorporate the clinical symptom observations as feedback. The utility of such approaches for early identification of autism risk needs to be confirmed through future research. Community samples that include typically developing children as well as children at risk for a range of neurodevelopmental disorders may be helpful to develop population-based methods for early detection of disorder risk.
Next steps and conclusions
The complex, heterogeneous nature of ASD has impeded our efforts to understand etiology and to predict which treatments will be effective. Big data and machine learning approaches may not only serve to parse subgroups within a large heterogeneous clinical category, but may also be used to examine common treatment targets across distinct neurodevelopmental trajectories. In such computational studies, samples need to be large enough for training and retesting computational models as they are optimized. The goal is to capture as much of the variation of the disorder as possible and conduct analyses to delineate biologically and clinically meaningful subgroups. An advantage of computationally driven research is the ability to compare multiple analytic methods to hone our ability to predict outcomes and quantify risk in the midst of heterogeneity.
Although large data-driven approaches require multidisciplinary collaboration and investment, they are increasingly important given the complexity and heterogeneity associated with developmental disorders such as ASD. We have learned through over 70 years of research that ASD defies simple categorical classification. This necessitates new approaches that leverage larger samples to build reliable models that accurately reflect the complexity inherent to autism. This “complexity” refers not solely to inter-subject variability, but also to intra-subject phenotypic variability as a function of development. Because ASD symptoms and challenges are not static within individuals across development, computational methods may contribute to better understanding of growth and time-related courses, subgroups of etiological contributions to phenotype, and interactions with medical-psychiatric comorbidities.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by R01 MH104324, NIMH K01 MH 101653, Minnesota Clinical & Translational Research funding. We are grateful to Mengdie Wang, Angela Tseng, and Dalia Istephanous for assistance with data and editing. We are thankful to David Redish for his comments on an early version of this manuscript.
Conflict of interest
S.J. is part of Roche multisite ASD treatment trials and has been a site-investigator for an investigator initiated, federally funded clinical trial in ASD. The remaining authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Asperger H. The “autistic psychopathy” in childhood. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr. 1944;117:76–136. doi: 10.1007/BF01837709. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv. Child. 1943;2:217–250. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rimland, B. Infantile autism: The syndrome and its implications for a neural theory of behavior. (East Norwalk, CT, US: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1964).
- 4.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic And Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV. (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, 1994).
- 5.Folstein S, Rutter M. Infantile autism: a genetic study of 21 twin pairs. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 1977;18:297–321. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1977.tb00443.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Damasio AR, Maurer RG. A neurological model for childhood autism. Arch. Neurol. 1978;35:777–786. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1978.00500360001001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic And Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, 2013).
- 8.World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification Of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. (Geneva: World Health Organization, 1992).
- 9.Barahona-Correa JB, Filipe CN. A concise history of asperger syndrome: the short reign of a troublesome diagnosis. Front. Psychol. 2015;6:2024. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lecouteur A, et al. Autism diagnostic interview—a standardized investigator-based instrumeNT. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1989;19:363–387. doi: 10.1007/BF02212936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1994;24:659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Lord, C., Rutter, M., Dilavore, P. C. & Risi, S. ADOS: Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. (Boston, MA: Hogrefe, 2008).
- 13.Frazier TW, et al. Validation of proposed DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. 2012;51:28–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.09.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gotham K, Risi S, Pickles A, Lord C. The autism diagnostic observation schedule: Revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2007;37:613–627. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0280-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Chandola V, Banerjee A, Kumar VV. Anomaly detection: A survey. ACM Comput. Surv. 2009;41:1–58. doi: 10.1145/1541880.1541882. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Boucher J. Research review: structural language in autistic spectrum disorder—characteristics and causes. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2012;53:219–233. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02508.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Waterhouse, L. Rethinking Autism: Variation and Complexity. (London: Elsevier Inc, 2013).
- 18.Amaral DG, Schumann CM, Nordahl CW. Neuroanatomy of autism. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31:137–145. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Geschwind DH, Levitt P. Autism spectrum disorders: developmental disconnection syndromes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2007;17:103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Insel T, et al. Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiat. 2010;167:748–751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Whitehouse AJO, Stanley FJ. Is autism one or multiple disorders? Med. J. Aust. 2013;198:302–303. doi: 10.5694/mja12.11667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Burmeister M, McInnis MG, Zollner S. Psychiatric genetics: progress amid controversy. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2008;9:527–540. doi: 10.1038/nrg2381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Magnusson C, et al. Migration and autism spectrum disorder: population-based study. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2012;201:109–115. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.095125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Beauchaine TP, Cicchetti D. A new generation of comorbidity research in the era of neuroscience and research domain criteria. Dev. Psychopathol. 2016;28:891–894. doi: 10.1017/S0954579416000602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Beauchaine, T. P. & Constantino, J. N. Redefining the endophenotype concept to accommodate transdiagnostic vulnerabilities and etiological complexity. Biomark. Med.10.2217/bmm-2017-0002 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 26.Esler AN, Stronach ST, Jacob S. Insistence on sameness and broader autism phenotype in simplex families with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2018;11:1253–1263. doi: 10.1002/aur.1975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Gargaro BA, Rinehart NJ, Bradshaw JL, Tonge BJ, Sheppard DM. Autism and ADHD: how fare have we come in the comorbidity debate? Neurosci. Behav. Rev. 2011;35:1081–1088. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Geller DA, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder: Fact or artifact? J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. 2002;41:52–58. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200201000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Geller DA, Biederman J, Griffin S, Jones J, Lefkowitz TR. Comorbidity of juvenile obsessive-compulsive disorder with disruptive behavior disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. 1996;35:1637–1646. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199612000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gjevik E, Eldevik S, Fjaeran-Granum T, Sponheim E. Kiddie-SADS reveals high rates of DSM-IV disorders in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2011;41:761–769. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1095-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Joshi G, et al. The heavy burden of psychiatric comorbidity in youth with autism spectrum disorders: a large comparative study of a psychiatrically referred population. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2010;40:1361–1370. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0996-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.MacNeil BM, Lopes VA, Minnes PM. Anxiety in children and adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Res. Autism Spectr. Disco. 2009;3:1–21. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2008.06.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Matson JL, Shoemaker M. Intellectual disability and its relationship to autism spectrum disorders. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2009;30:1107–1114. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2009.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Tuchman R, Rapin I. Epilepsy in autism. Lancet Neurol. 2002;1:352–358. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(02)00160-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Viscidi, E. W. et al. Clinical Characteristics of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Co-Occurring Epilepsy. PLoS ONE.10.1371/journal.pone.0067797 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 36.Hsiao EY. Gastrointestinal issues in autism spectrum disorder. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry. 2014;22:104–111. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Jeste SS, Geschwind DH. Disentangling the heterogeneity of autism spectrum disorder through genetic findings. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2014;10:74–81. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2013.278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Davis NO, Kollins SH. Treatment for co-occurring attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9:518–530. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0126-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Rice T, Coffey B. Pharmacotherapeutic challenges in treatment of a child with “the triad” of obsessive compulsive disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and Tourette’s disorder. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 2015;25:176–179. doi: 10.1089/cap.2015.2522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Elia J, Ambrosini P, Berrettini W. ADHD characteristics: I. Concurrent co-morbidity patterns in children & adolescents. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health. 2008;2:15. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-2-15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Redish, D. & Gordon, J. Computational Psychiatry: New Perspectives on Mental Illness (Strungmann Forum Reports). (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2016).
- 42.Duda, M., Kosmicki, J. A. & Wall, D. P. Testing the accuracy of an observation-based classifier for rapid detection of autism risk (vol 4, pg e424, 2014). Transl. Psychiatry.10.1038/tp.2015.51 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 43.Kosmicki, J. A., Sochat, V., Duda, M. & Wall, D. P. Searching for a minimal set of behaviors for autism detection through feature selection-based machine learning. Transl. Psychiatry.10.1038/tp.2015.7 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 44.Wall, D. P., Kosmicki, J., DeLuca, T. F., Harstad, E. & Fusaro, V. A. Use of machine learning to shorten observation-based screening and diagnosis of autism. Transl. Psychiatry.10.1038/tp.2012.10 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 45.Bone D, et al. Applying machine learning to facilitate autism diagnostics: pitfalls and promises. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015;45:1121–1136. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2268-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Wall, D. P., Dally, R., Luyster, R., Jung, J. Y. & DeLuca, T. F. Use of artificial intelligence to shorten the behavioral diagnosis of autism. PLoS ONE. 10.1371/journal.pone.0043855 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 47.Bone D, et al. Use of machine learning to improve autism screening and diagnostic instruments: effectiveness, efficiency, and multi-instrument fusion. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2016;57:927–937. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Constantino, J. & Gruber, C. The Social Responsiveness Scale Manual, Second Edition (SRS-2). (Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services, 2012).
- 49.Emerson, R. W. et al. Functional neuroimaging of high-risk 6-month-old infants predicts a diagnosis of autism at 24 months of age. Sci. Transl. Med.10.1126/scitranslmed.aag2882 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 50.Wolff JJ, Jacob S, Elison JT. The journey to autism: Insights from neuroimaging studies of infants and toddlers. Dev. Psychopathol. 2018;30:479–495. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kassraian-Fard P, Matthis C, Balsters JH, Maathuis MH, Wenderoth N. Promises, pitfalls, and basic guidelines for applying machine learning classifiers to psychiatric imaging data, with autism as an example. Front. Psychiatry. 2016;7:177. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Rommelse N, Franke B, Geurts H, Hartman C, Buitelaar J. Shared heritability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2010;19:281–295. doi: 10.1007/s00787-010-0092-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Karalunas SL, et al. Overlapping and distinct cognitive impairments in attention-deficit/hyperactivity and autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2018;46:1705–1716. doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0394-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Anderson A, et al. Non-negative matrix factorization of multimodal MRI, fMRI and phenotypic data reveals differential changes in default mode subnetworks in ADHD. Neuroimage. 2014;102(Pt 1):207–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lim L, et al. Disorder-specific predictive classification of adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) relative to autism using structural magnetic resonance imaging. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:10. doi: 10.1371/annotation/fb854e6a-cc9e-4446-b50a-5318cffb68c5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Ghiassian, S., Greiner, R., Jin, P. & Brown, M. R. G. Using functional or structural magnetic resonance images and personal characteristic data to identify ADHD and autism. PLoS ONE.10.1371/journal.pone.0166934 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 57.van der Meer JM, et al. Are autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder different manifestations of one overarching disorder? Cognitive and symptom evidence from a clinical and population-based sample. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2012;51:1160–1172.e1163. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Uddin, L. Q., Dajani, D. R., Voorhies, W., Bednarz, H. & Kana, R. K. Progress and roadblocks in the search for brain-based biomarkers of autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Transl. Psychiatry.10.1038/tp.2017.164 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 59.Wang XJ, Krystal JH. Computational psychiatry. Neuron. 2014;84:638–654. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Huys QJ, Maia TV, Frank MJ. Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:404–413. doi: 10.1038/nn.4238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Totah, N. et al. in Computational Psychiatry: New Perspectives on Mental Illness (eds. Redish, A.D. & Gordon, J.A.) 33–59 (Cambridge, MA: Strüngmann Forum Reports, 2016).
- 62.Lord C, Bishop S, Anderson D. Developmental trajectories as autism phenotypes. Am. J. Med. Genet. C. 2015;169:198–208. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.c.31440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Baghdadli A, et al. Developmental trajectories of adaptive behaviors from early childhood to adolescence in a cohort of 152 children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012;42:1314–1325. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1357-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Farmer C, Swineford L, Swedo SE, Thurm A. Classifying and characterizing the development of adaptive behavior in a naturalistic longitudinal study of young children with autism. J. Neurodev. Disord. 2018;10:1. doi: 10.1186/s11689-017-9222-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D. & Balla, D. Vineland-II Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition, Survey Forms Manual (Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing, 2005).
- 66.Szatmari P, et al. Developmental trajectories of symptom severity and adaptive functioning in an inception cohort of preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. Jama Psychiatry. 2015;72:276–283. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Doshi-Velez F, Ge YR, Kohane I. Comorbidity clusters in autism spectrum disorders: an electronic health record time-series analysis. Pediatrics. 2014;133:E54–E63. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-0819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Ceroni F, et al. A deletion involving CD38 and BST1 results in a fusion transcript in a patient with autism and asthma. Autism Res. 2014;7:254–263. doi: 10.1002/aur.1365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Amir RE, et al. Rett syndrome is caused by mutations in X-linked MECP2, encoding methyl-CpG-binding protein 2. Nat. Genet. 1999;23:185–188. doi: 10.1038/13810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Cuddapah VA, et al. Methyl-CpG-binding protein 2 (MECP2) mutation type is associated with disease severity in Rett syndrome. J. Med. Genet. 2014;51:152–158. doi: 10.1136/jmedgenet-2013-102113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Kaufmann WE, et al. Autism spectrum disorder in fragile X syndrome: cooccurring conditions and current treatment. Pediatrics. 2017;139:S194–S206. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1159F. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Dykens, E. M. et al. Diagnoses and characteristics of autism spectrum disorders in children with Prader-Willi syndrome. J. Neurodev. Disord.10.1186/s11689-017-9200-2 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 73.Krishnan A, et al. Genome-wide prediction and functional characterization of the genetic basis of autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:1454–1462. doi: 10.1038/nn.4353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Edgeworth FY. On discordant observations. Philos. Mag. 1887;23:364–375. doi: 10.1080/14786448708628471. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Aggarwal, C. C. Data Mining (Switzerland: Springer International Publishing, 2015).
- 76.Breunig, M. M., Kriegel, H. P., Ng, R. T. & Sander, J. Vol. 29, 582–588 (New York, NY: ACM Sigmod Record, 2000).
- 77.Scholkopf, B., Burges, C. J. C. & Smola, A. J. Advances in kernel methods—support vector learning—Introduction. Adv. Kernel Method. 1-15 (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999)
- 78.Hawkins, S., He, H., Williams, G. & Baxter, R. International Conference on Data Warehousing and Knowledge Discovery. 170–180 (Berlin Heidelberg: Springer, 2002).
- 79.Sifre R, et al. Restricted, repetitive, and reciprocal social behavior in toddlers born small for gestation duration. J. Pediatr. 2018;200:118–124 e119. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2018.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Wolff, J. J., Boyd, B. A. & Elison, J. T. J.Neurodev.Disord.8, 27 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 81.Weigold A, Weigold IK, Russell EJ. Examination of the equivalence of self-report survey-based paper-and-pencil and internet data collection methods. Psychol. Methods. 2013;18:53–70. doi: 10.1037/a0031607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Marrus N, et al. Rapid video-referenced ratings of reciprocal social behavior in toddlers: a twin study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2015;56:1338–1346. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Fenson, L. et al. MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories (2nd ed.). (Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes, 2007).
- 84.Rosenberg SA, Zhang D, Robinson CC. Prevalence of developmental delays and participation in early intervention services for young children. Pediatrics. 2008;121:e1503–e1509. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-1680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Rosenberg SA, Ellison MC, Fast B, Robinson CC, Lazar R. Computing theoretical rates of part C eligibility based on developmental delays. Matern. Child Health J. 2013;17:384–390. doi: 10.1007/s10995-012-0982-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Bouadjenek MR, Verspoor K, Zobel J. Automated detection of records in biological sequence databases that are inconsistent with the literature. J. Biomed. Inform. 2017;71:229–240. doi: 10.1016/j.jbi.2017.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Lu W, et al. Unsupervised sequential outlier detection with deep architectures. IEEE Trans. Image Process. 2017;26:4321–4330. doi: 10.1109/TIP.2017.2713048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Tan, P., Steinbach, M. & Kumar, V. Introduction to Data Mining. (Boston: Pearson Addison-Wesley, 2006).
- 89.Lazarevic, A. & Kumar, V. Feature bagging for outlier detection. Proc. 11th ACM SIGKDD International Conference on Knowledge Discovery in Data Mining. 157–166 (Chicago, Illinois: 2005).