Introduction
There is no doubt that economic inequality in the US has increased over the last several decades (Piketty, Saez, and Zucm 2016; Congressional Budget Office 2013). Diminished labor market opportunities and the ensuing decline in (inflation-adjusted) economic fortunes for the least educated Americans have been blamed for initiating a cascade of consequences culminating in rising mortality related to drugs, alcohol, and suicide (Case and Deaton 2017; 2015)—collectively referred to as “deaths of despair” (Khazan 2015; Case 2015; Monnat 2016). The health effects are evident in overall mortality as well: socioeconomic disparities in life expectancy have widened dramatically over this period (Chetty et al. 2016b; Bosworth, Burtless, and Zhang 2016), particularly among non-Latino whites (Olshansky et al. 2012; Sasson 2016). Beyond its effects on health, inequality1 can have far-reaching consequences for society as a whole, for example, by compromising social trust and cohesion and jeopardizing the effectiveness of social institutions (Kawachi and Berkman 2000; Kawachi et al. 1997). Indeed, arguments related to growing inequality have been invoked to explain many of the worrisome trends not only in mortality, but in a broader range of health outcomes, as well as social and political phenomena.
Socioeconomic disparities are typically measured in terms of objective criteria such as education, income, wealth, and unemployment. Although there is a large literature on subjective social status (the “social ladder”) and its effects on health (Ostrove et al. 2000; Singh-Manoux et al. 2003; Adler et al. 2000), few studies have incorporated subjective measures of economic distress. Both constructs are subjective: perceived economic distress is based on the respondents’ evaluations of their financial and employment circumstances; the “social ladder” asks respondents how they would rank themselves relative to others. In this analysis, we focus only on perceived economic distress. To quantify socioeconomic disparities in our measures of perceived economic distress, we construct another measure that forms an essential part of our analysis. For this measure, which we refer to as relative socioeconomic status (relative SES), we use objective criteria (i.e., education, income, assets, and occupation) to assign a percentile rank denoting the respondent’s position within the overall distribution. It is distinct from the “social ladder” in that the relative ranking is derived completely from objective criteria rather than from respondents’ own evaluations of their social positions.
Although we would expect disparities in perceived economic distress to widen alongside objective measures, people’s perceptions are influenced by a broad set of factors. In particular, these perceptions may vary across birth cohorts because the conditions experienced by a cohort during its formative years can influence whether individuals view their financial means to be sufficient. Case and Deaton (2017) argue that the labor market opportunities available to members of a cohort as they enter the work force may affect not only their career trajectories, but also have ramifications for marital prospects and family formation (Case and Deaton 2017). Collectively, those cohort histories may shape a person’s view of his/her economic well-being even much later in life.
More importantly, the social and health consequences of inequality may depend on perceptions at least as much, if not more, than they do on objective criteria. Indeed, as Thomas and Thomas (1928: 572) put it, “If men define situations as real, they are real in their consequences.” Previous studies have demonstrated that subjective measures of poor financial well-being predict mortality (Szanton et al. 2008), decline in mental health (Wilkinson 2016), and worse self-assessed health status (Arber et al. 2014; Shippee et al. 2012), even after controlling for objective economic factors. Therefore, efforts to identify the underlying causes of recent demographic trends are likely to benefit from considering subjective assessments in addition to objective measures of economic distress.
In this paper, we begin by quantifying the extent to which the socioeconomic disparities in perceived economic distress widened since the mid-1990s. Specifically, we investigate the degree to which subjective measures of financial strain and employment uncertainty differ between those in the top and bottom percentiles of relative SES and whether that SES disparity widened over time. We subsequently address a more interesting question: did the SES-associated disparities in perceptions widen more than we would expect based on changes in objective measures? Thus, we determine the extent to which the growing disparities (i.e., differences between those with low versus high relative SES) in subjective measures can be explained by changes in objective measures of economic and employment conditions. As discussed in the next section, reference group theory would predict that more advantaged subgroups would perceive the same economic and employment conditions more negatively. Therefore, we also test whether there is a racial/ethnic differential in perceptions of economic distress net of objective circumstances.
Background
Voydanoff (1990) conceptualizes economic distress as having both objective and subjective dimensions. The objective component includes economic deprivation (e.g., real income, assets) as well as employment instability (e.g., patterns of employment), whereas the subjective dimension encompasses financial strain (e.g., perceived financial inadequacy) and employment uncertainty (e.g., assessments of current work situation and prospects for the future). Conger (1990) argues that the effects of objective economic conditions on health and behavior are mediated by subjective financial strain. Leininger and Kalil (2014) suggest that perceived financial strain during the Great Recession (December 2007 to June 2009 in the US) might be less connected with objective economic factors and relate more to perceived uncertainty about the future. That suggestion raises an empirical question: are subjective evaluations of financial strain and employment uncertainty simply a function of objective circumstances? Or, do other factors play a role in shaping an individual’s perception of his/her economic and employment situation?
By most objective measures, the Great Recession had a greater financial impact on blacks and Latinos than on whites (Kochhar et al. 2011). US blacks continue to have far higher overall mortality than US whites (National Center for Health Statistics 2017, Table 15), but recent attention has highlighted the slowing of mortality decline and the increasing death rates related to drugs, alcohol, and suicide that appear to have affected non-Latino whites more than blacks or Latinos (Kochanek et al. 2016; Squires and Blumenthal 2016; Case and Deaton 2017; Case and Deaton 2015). Although there has been a surge in overdose mortality among blacks and Latinos since 2010, rates of overdose mortality remain much higher among non-Latino whites than their non-Latino black and Latino counterparts (Hedegaard et al. 2017).
Reference group theory (Hyman 1942; Merton and Rossi 1950; Merton 1957), which posits that individuals compare themselves to the social group to which they aspire, provides one possible explanation for the racial difference. Relatedly, the theory of relative deprivation (Davis 1959; Runciman 1966; Merton and Rossi 1950) refers to the notion that comparing oneself with others who are more advantaged can create a feeling of deprivation. Because people generally focus on upward comparisons (Runciman 1966; Evans et al. 2004), growing levels of income inequality are likely to affect perceptions adversely for most, if not all, of the population, but those with the lowest income may experience the strongest feelings of being worse off (Hastings 2017). Thus, we would predict that as inequality increases, individuals with low socioeconomic status would perceive themselves to be worse off than their actual economic circumstances would suggest. That is, we expect their sense of relative deprivation to grow over time because they are comparing their condition with those at the top who have enjoyed greater prosperity.
Yet people compare themselves not only with positive reference groups (i.e., those they admire), but also negative reference groups (i.e., those to whom they feel superior and from whom they differentiate themselves). Runciman (1966) distinguishes between comparisons within one’s own subgroup (i.e., “egoist comparison”) versus comparisons of one’s own group relative to other subgroups (i.e., “fraternal comparison”). In the case of fraternal (out-group) comparisons, Davis (1959) uses the term “relative subordination” to refer to the resulting attitude when someone who is worse off relative to the population as a whole compares her/himself to a better off member of an out-group, whereas he denotes the attitude as “relative superiority” when a better off individual compares her/himself to a worse off member of an out-group. Cherlin (2016; 2014) points out that reference group theory might explain why people who have more might feel like they have less; it all depends on the reference group to which they compare themselves. Within the US, non-Latino whites have historically enjoyed greater advantages than minorities. Reference group theory would suggest that subgroups that have been more advantaged would have higher expectations. Thus, worse off members of a historically advantaged subgroup may be more prone to feelings of relative subordination.
Status anxiety may further explain an apparent discordance between objective and subjective measures. Lipset (1955) used the term “status anxiety” to explain how concerns about one’s relative social position can arise during periods of apparent prosperity or at least economic recovery. Unlike the financial deprivation that results from economic recession and widespread unemployment, status anxiety is more subjective in nature. As Wilkinson (2016) notes, the link between objective circumstances and subjective evaluations may seem counterintuitive: during periods of economic decline, a person may downplay his/her own financial troubles. But when times are more prosperous, people who are not doing as well might feel left behind, thereby generating status anxiety. Previous authors have argued that increased income inequality can heighten status anxiety, which can, in turn, affect an individual’s health via emotional and stress responses and also adversely affect social relationships and social organization (Layte and Whelan 2013).
Hypotheses
As a result of increasing inequality and weakening employment prospects for less educated Americans, we hypothesize that,
H1 : Perceived financial strain and employment uncertainty increased more over time for individuals at the lower end of the SES distribution than for those at the upper end of the SES continuum.
Widening SES disparities in these perceived measures, implied by H1, may be simply a function of changes in objective measures of economic and employment conditions: people perceive themselves as worse off because they are worse off. Yet growing inequality may exacerbate one’s sense of relative deprivation. Based on reference group and relative deprivation theories, we would predict that people’s expectations rise as they try to “keep up with the Joneses.” Consequently, we suspect that the SES disparity in perceptions widened even more than observed economic circumstances would suggest.
H2 : The growing disparity in perceived economic distress was greater than would be expected based on changes in objective, inflation-adjusted economic and employment conditions.
Within US society, non-Latino whites have historically enjoyed socioeconomic advantages relative to minorities. Consequently, they are likely to have higher expectations and be more susceptible to status anxiety. We anticipate that,
H3 : Given similar economic and employment circumstances, non-Latino whites will have perceived themselves as worse off than groups who have been more disadvantaged historically.
We also expect that, after controlling for differences in economic and employment conditions, non-Latino whites will experience a greater rise than minorities in perceived economic distress since the mid-1990s. Our rationale is the observation that minorities may perceive themselves as financially better off than their parents were at the same age. In contrast, non-Latino whites, particularly those with low levels of education, fared rather well during the post-World War II period. Yet in recent years, the effects of globalization and the decline of well-paid manufacturing jobs for high school educated individuals (the so-called “blue collar aristocracy”) have made it increasingly difficult for this group to match the improvements in the standard of living enjoyed by their parents. Although the number of minorities in the sample utilized does not provide sufficient statistical power to adequately test differences in the period effects by race/ethnicity, in the Discussion we briefly review some exploratory analyses along these lines.
Methods
Data
We use data from two cross-sectional waves of the Midlife Development in the US study (MIDUS). In 1995–1996 (Wave M1), MIDUS conducted phone interviews (N = 3,487, 70 percent response rate) with a national sample of non-institutionalized, English-speaking adults aged 25–74 in the coterminous United States, selected by random digit dialing with over-sampling of older people and men (Brim et al. 2016). Among those who completed the phone interview, 3,034 (87 percent) also completed mail-in self-administered questionnaires (SAQ). In 2011–2014 (Wave R1), a new refresher cohort with the same age range was sampled from the national population (Ryff et al. 2016). Among those who completed the phone interview (N = 3,577, 59 percent response rate), 2,598 (73 percent) also completed the SAQ.
We restrict our analyses to respondents who completed the SAQ. Thus, our pooled analysis sample comprises 5,632 respondents.
Measures
Subjective measures of economic distress.
The subjective outcomes include two measures of financial strain and two measures related to employment uncertainty. Our first measure is an index of current financial strain based on the following five questions from the SAQ:
-
(1)
“Using a scale from 0 to 10 where 0 means ‘the worst possible financial situation’ and 10 means ‘the best possible financial situation,’ how would you rate your financial situation these days?”
-
(2)
“Looking ahead ten years into the future, what do you expect your financial situation will be like at that time?” [using the same 0–10 scale]
-
(3)
“Using a 0 to 10 scale where 0 means ‘no control at all’ and 10 means ‘very much control,’ how would you rate the amount of control you have over your financial situation these days?”
-
(4)
“In general, would you say you (and your family living with you) have more money than you need, just enough for your needs, or not enough to meet your needs?”
-
(5)
“How difficult is it for you (and your family) to pay your monthly bills?” [response categories: very difficult, somewhat difficult, not very difficult, not at all difficult]
Each item was standardized based on the distribution of the pooled sample and coded so that higher values indicate more financial strain. Then, we computed the mean across the five items (Cronbach’s α = 0.84).
The second measure of financial strain relates to intergenerational financial disadvantage and is based on the following question: “When your parents were the age you are now, were they better off or worse off financially than you are now?” Responses were coded on a seven-point scale from “a lot better off” to “a lot worse off”; higher values indicate that the respondent perceived him/herself as worse off than his/her parents (i.e., parents were better off than the respondent).
We measure employment uncertainty using the respondent’s ratings of his/her current work situation (“Please think of the work situation you are in now, whether part-time or full-time, paid or unpaid, at home or at a job. Using a scale from 0 to 10 where 0 means ‘the worst possible work situation’ and 10 means ‘the best possible work situation,’ how would you rate your work situation these days?”) and his/her expected work situation 10 years in the future (“Looking ahead ten years into the future, what do you expect your work situation will be like at that time?” using the same scale). These two questions were asked of all respondents regardless of whether they were employed at the time of the survey. We reverse-coded these measures so that higher values indicate worse evaluations (i.e., more uncertainty).
Objective measures of economic and employment circumstances.
Socioeconomic status (SES) is typically measured based on education, occupation, income, and/or wealth and can be specified in either absolute or relative terms. We create a measure of relative SES (percentile rank within each survey wave) that includes measures of economic deprivation as well as education and occupation (see Supplementary Material for details). Much of the literature on widening social disparities in health and mortality focuses on educational differentials, but trends in health disparities by education are compromised by the problem of lagged selection bias (Dowd and Hamoudi 2014). The proportion of the US population that completed high school increased dramatically during the twentieth century and thus, high school dropouts have become an increasingly select group (Dowd and Hamoudi 2014). Indeed, Hendi (2015) finds that half of the decline in life expectancy among the least-educated white women and much of widening of the education gap in survival results from shifts in the educational distribution. Our composite measure of relative SES is less prone to the problem of lagged selection bias because it allows us to evaluate changes over time in the outcome measures for fixed quantiles of the population.
In the models that control for objective economic and employment circumstances, we include each of the individual components used to construct relative SES (see Table 1 for the full list),2 a dichotomous variable indicating whether the respondent was covered by health insurance at the time of the survey, and two measures of employment instability: current work status (categorized as employed, retired, or not employed) and the life-time maximum period of unemployment (when not a student). We recode the maximum unemployment spell into the following categories: never, less than six months, six months to less than two years, and two or more years.
TABLE 1.
Descriptive statistics for analysis variables, weighted
Pooled Waves M1 & R1 (N = 5632) |
Wave M1 1995–1996 (N = 3034) |
Wave R1 2011–2014 (N = 2598) |
|
---|---|---|---|
Demographic characteristics | |||
Male, % | 47.8 | 47.7 | 48.0 |
Age (20-76), mean (SD) | 47.1 (13.6) | 45.5 (13.5) | 49.0 (13.6) |
Cohort group | |||
Born before 1943,a % | 19.9 | 31.2 | 6.7 |
Early Baby Boomers (1943-53), % | 23.3 | 26.1 | 20.0 |
Late Baby Boomers (1954-64), % | 29.5 | 30.2 | 28.7 |
Born after 1964,b % | 27.3 | 12.5 | 44.7 |
Non-Latino white, % | 81.2 | 81.6 | 80.7 |
Married or living with a partner, % | 70.8 | 72.2 | 69.1 |
Objective economic/employment measures | |||
Educational degree (1-12), mean (SD) | 6.6 (2.5) | 6.3 (2.4) | 7.0 (2.5) |
Current/previous occupation | |||
Never employed,c % | 0.8 | 0.9 | 0.7 |
Farming/labor/military, % | 23.9 | 27.1 | 20.2 |
Service/sales/administrative, % | 37.3 | 38.0 | 36.5 |
Management/business/financial, % | 16.2 | 15.9 | 16.7 |
Professional, % | 21.7 | 18.2 | 25.8 |
Household income (0-833),d mean (SD) | 62.0 (65.8) | 69.5 (73.9) | 53.3 (53.6) |
Wage/salary income (0-698),d mean(SD) | 41.1 (46.2) | 41.3 (41.1) | 40.9 (51.5) |
Net assets (0-1820),d mean (SD) | 121.3 (281.6) | 105.8 (275.3) | 139.4 (287.8) |
No assets or a deficit, % | 40.3 | 36.1 | 45.2 |
Spouses’ educational degree (1-12),e mean (SD) | 6.9 (2.5) | 6.5 (2.4) | 7.4 (2.5) |
Spouses’ current/previous occupatione | |||
Never employed,f % | 0.9 | 1.1 | 0.5 |
Farming/labor/military, % | 24.5 | 27.1 | 21.3 |
Service/sales/administrative, % | 35.9 | 38.5 | 32.7 |
Management/business/financial, % | 17.4 | 14.9 | 20.3 |
Professional/, % | 21.4 | 18.3 | 25.2 |
Covered by health insurance, % | 86.8 | 87.5 | 86.0 |
Current employment status | |||
Working, % | 68.3 | 71.3 | 64.8 |
Neither working nor retired, % | 17.5 | 15.9 | 19.4 |
Retired, % | 14.2 | 12.8 | 15.9 |
Maximum unemployment spell | |||
Never unemployed,c % | 44.4 | 39.0 | 50.8 |
<6 months, % | 16.9 | 22.9 | 10.0 |
6 months to <2 years, % | 16.4 | 16.8 | 15.9 |
2+ years, % | 23.3 | 21.4 | 23.4 |
Spouses’ current employment statuse | |||
Working, % | 69.0 | 68.7 | 69.5 |
Neither working nor retired, % | 16.4 | 18.4 | 14.0 |
Retired, % | 14.5 | 12.9 | 16.6 |
Perceived financial strain | |||
Index of current financial strain (−1.98 to 2.9), mean (SD) | 0.0 (1.0) | −0.1 (0.9) | 0.1 (1.1) |
Current financial situation (0-10, where 10 = worst), mean (SD) | 4.2 (2.4) | 4.1 (2.2) | 4.3 (2.5) |
Future financial situation (0-10, 10 = worst), mean (SD) | 2.9 (2.2) | 2.6 (2.0) | 3.2 (2.4) |
Control over financial situation (0-10, 10 = no control), mean (SD) | 3.7 (2.6) | 3.4 (2.5) | 4.0 (2.7) |
Money to meet needs (0-2, 2 = not enough), mean (SD) | 1.2 (0.7) | 1.2 (0.6) | 1.2 (0.7) |
Difficulty paying monthly bills (0-3, 3 = very difficult), mean (SD) | 1.3 (0.9) | 1.3 (0.9) | 1.3 (1.0) |
Intergenerational financial disadvantage (0-6, 6 = a lot worse off), mean (SD) | 2.8 (1.9) | 2.6 (1.8) | 3.0 (1.9) |
Perceived employment uncertainty | |||
Current work situation (0-10, 10 = worst), mean (SD) | 3.0 (2.6) | 2.8 (2.4) | 3.3 (2.8) |
Expected future work situation (0-10, 10 = worst), mean (SD) | 2.6 (2.6) | 2.4 (2.5) | 2.9 (2.7) |
Includes the Silent Generation (1925–1942, although only those born in 1938 or later were observed at both waves) and, in the M1 wave, the late GI Generation (i.e., those born in 1920–1924, who represent the tail end of the GI Generation born in 1901–1924).
Includes Gen X (1965–1979, although only those born in 1970 or earlier were observed at both waves) and in R1 wave, some of the early Millennial cohorts (i.e., those born in 1980–1989).
A small number of respondents (N = 22 at M1, N = 11 at R1) had never been employed. For the purposes of modeling, these respondents are coded to the reference group for occupation (farming/labor). The questions about unemployment spells were skipped if the respondent had never been employed for six or more months (N = 137 at M1, N = 54 at R1); we coded those respondents as never having been unemployed.
Expressed in thousands of 1995 dollars.
Among those who were married or living with a partner (N = 2087 at M1, N = 1812 at R1). For the purposes of modeling, spouse’s education is coded as high school graduate and spouse’s occupation is coded to the reference group (farming/labor) for those who were not married/partnered (N = 947 at M1, N = 786 at R1).
If the respondent’s spouse/partner had never been employed (N = 23 at M1, N = 8 at R1), spouses’ occupation was coded to the reference group (farming/labor) for modeling purposes.
Demographic factors.
We include the following factors as controls: sex, survey wave, age, cohort, race/ethnicity, and marital status. A dummy variable indicating survey wave (2011–2014 vs. 1995–1996) represents the period effect, while age is specified as linear. We also control for cohort because, as noted in the introduction, economic conditions experienced by a cohort during early life may influence its perceptions of economic distress throughout life. We classified respondents into four cohorts: (1) those born before 1943; (2) Early Baby Boomers (born in 1943–1953); (3) Late Baby Boomers (born in 1954–1964); and (4) those born after 1964.3 Because there are few racial/ethnic minorities in MIDUS, we included a dummy variable that distinguishes non-Latino whites from all other groups. Marital status was also coded as dichotomous, indicating those who were married or living with a partner.
Analytical strategy
In order to include as much data as possible in the analysis, we followed standard practices of multiple imputation to handle missing data (Schafer 1999; Rubin 1996).4 All analyses were weighted using post-stratification weights. We standardized all outcome measures based on the pooled distribution so that values are measured on the same scale and effect size can be compared across time and across outcomes.
We began with bivariate analyses, using local mean smoothing to plot each of the outcome variables by SES percentile for the two survey waves.5 In supplementary analyses, we further examined smoothed bivariate plots of the outcome variables by single-year birth cohort.
We then fitted a series of linear regression models for each out-come. The baseline model controlled for period, age, cohort group, sex, race/ethnicity, and marital status. This model also included an interaction between period and cohort group to allow for the fact that some cohorts may have been more affected by changes over time in economic and employment conditions (e.g., cohorts entering the labor force during a period of high unemployment may have struggled harder to establish their careers). In Model 2, we added relative SES (i.e., percentile rank) interacted with survey wave to test H1 : Did perceived economic distress increase more between 1995–1996 to 2011–2014 for those at the bottom than for those at the top of the SES spectrum? In Model 3, we added controls for the objective measures of economic and employment circumstances to test H2 : Did the SES disparity in perceptions widen more than expected based on changes in objective measures? Using the results from this same model, we consider the coefficient for race/ethnicity to evaluate H3 : Given similar objective economic and employment circumstances, did non-Latino whites perceive their situation more negatively than minorities?
Results
Descriptive statistics for all analysis variables are presented in Table 1 by survey wave. Rising levels of educational attainment over time are apparent. In 1995–1996, the average educational level for the respondent (6.3) and his or her spouse/partner (6.5) corresponds to one or two years of college without a degree, while the mean level in 2011–2014 (7.0 for respondent, 7.4 for spouse/partner) equates to three or more years of college without a degree. The occupational structure also appears to have shifted over time as the fraction working in the lower two categories declined while the percentage employed in the upper two categories increased. For example, respondents employed in farming, construction, maintenance, production, transportation, or the military declined from 27 percent in 1995–1996 to 20 percent in 2011–2014. Over that same period, the percentage of respondents in professional occupations increased from 18 to 26. Adjusted for inflation, average household income declined,6 although wage/salary income for the respondent and spouse combined was more stable over time. The mean level of assets (in real dollars) increased over time, but the average is deceptive because the distribution is extremely skewed. The percentage with no net assets or a deficit increased from 36 in 1995–1996 to 45 in 2011–2014. At the same time, the percentage with very high assets (i.e., $500,000 or more in 1995 dollars) increased from 4.3 to 7.4 (not shown).
In order to situate changes in the mean levels of subjective economic distress in context, it is useful to consider historical economic conditions during this period. The monthly unemployment rate for adults aged 16 and older hovered between 5.1 and 5.8 during the baseline wave of MIDUS (January 1995-September 1996); those rates were an improvement over the higher levels of unemployment in the early 1990s that peaked at 7.8 in June 1992 (Bureau of Labor Statistics 2017). Compared with the baseline wave, unemployment rates were higher during the refresher wave of MIDUS (November 2011 to May 2014), ranging from a high of 8.6 (November 2011) to a low of 6.2 (April 2014). Unemployment was trending downward during this later period but followed on the heels of even higher levels of unemployment spurred by the Great Recession. The unemployment rate peaked at 10.0 in October 2009 before beginning a slow decline. Thus, one would expect to find substantially higher levels of economic distress at the later wave than the earlier wave of MIDUS.
We do see a slight increase between the two waves in employment uncertainty as evidenced by respondents’ ratings of their current work situation and their expected work situation ten years in the future, both of which worsened over time. There was also some deterioration in respondents’ perceptions of their current financial situation, future financial situation, control over their financial situation, and perceived intergenerational financial disadvantage. Yet, we see no change over time in the means for self-reported ability to meet financial needs and difficulty paying monthly bills. As we demonstrate in the next section, the mean levels of these subjective outcomes obscure the fact that changes over time were very different for those at the bottom of the SES distribution from those at the top. The most socioeconomically disadvantaged segment of the population was likely to suffer the brunt of the recession and resultant increases in unemployment.
Growing disparity in perceived economic distress over recent decades
Figure 1 shows the bivariate association between the subjective outcomes and relative SES at the two waves (1995–1996 and 2011–2014). Across all four outcomes, levels of perceived economic distress generally increased among people below the 40th percentile of the SES distribution. At the top of the SES spectrum, the trends varied across outcomes: perceived current financial strain declined slightly, but there was virtually no period change in perceptions of intergenerational disadvantage or the measures of employment uncertainty. Overall, the SES disparities in perceived financial strain and employment uncertainty widened over the last two decades.
FIGURE 1. Smoothed bivariate plots of perceived economic distress by relative SES at MIDUS waves M1 (1995–1996) and R1 (2011–2014).
NOTES: These plots are produced using local mean smoothing (see endnote 5 for details). Variables on the y-axis are scaled in terms of standard deviation units, where higher values indicate more strain/uncertainty. For example, a value of zero on the outcome variable indicates that the smoothed mean for individuals at the specified percentile rank of SES is equal to the overall mean for the pooled sample (both waves combined), whereas a value of 0.5 would indicate a level half a standard deviation higher than the overall mean.
Supplementary Figure S1 shows additional bivariate analysis of the subjective measures by birth cohort.7 These graphs suggest that the late Baby Boomers (i.e., those born in 1954–1964)8 were hit hardest (see Supplementary Figure S1). That is, the biggest increase in perceived financial strain and employment uncertainty appeared among the cohorts who were in midlife in 2011–2014. Among the older cohorts,9 there was little change in perceived current financial strain or perceived intergenerational disadvantage; a decline in uncertainty regarding current work—perhaps because these older cohorts would have reached retirement age by 2011–2014; and a smaller increase in uncertainty about future work. Even the younger cohorts did not exhibit as much change over time in these subjective measures as the late Baby Boomers, but they are still fairly early in their working careers.10
This pattern apparent at the bivariate level persists when we simultaneously control for other demographic factors (i.e., sex, age, race/ethnicity, marital status) as well as cohort group in our baseline regression models (Model 1, Tables S1 & S2). The estimated change in these outcomes for each cohort group adjusted for demographic factors is shown in Figure 2. Here, the results show even more strikingly that the late Baby Boomers exhibited a larger increase in perceived economic distress (0.18 to 0.39 in SD units) than the other cohort groups.
FIGURE 2. Estimated change between 1995–1996 and 2011–2014 in perceived economic distress by cohort group.
NOTES: The estimates are based on results from Model 1 (Tables S1 & S2), which adjusts for sex, age, cohort group, race/ethnicity, marital status, survey wave, and wave*cohort group. Estimated change for each cohort group is computed by summing the relevant coefficients (e.g., the estimated change for late Baby Boomers is the sum of the main effect for survey wave and the interaction between wave and the late Baby Boomer cohort group). Outcome measures are coded so that higher values indicate more strain/uncertainty. An estimated change of 0.2 indicates that level of economic distress increased by onefifth of a SD between 1995–1996 and 2011–2014. Error bars indicate the 95% confidence interval for each estimate.
When we add relative SES to the model, we find that trends in perceived outcomes differed greatly by SES for three of the four outcomes (Model 2, Tables S1 & S2). By summing the relevant coefficients, we show the estimated change in each outcome for someone in the first or in the 99th percentiles of SES by cohort group in Figure 3.11 As hypothesized in H1, perceived financial strain and employment uncertainty increased much more for those at the bottom of the SES spectrum. For example, among the late Baby Boomers, current financial strain grew by 0.63 SD between 1995–1996 and 2011–2014 for those in the bottom percentile, while there was virtually no change for their counterparts in the top percentile.12
FIGURE 3. Estimated change between 1995–1996 and 2011–2014 in perceived economic distress by cohort group for a person in 1st and 99th percentile of the SES distribution.
NOTES: The estimates are based on results from Model 2 (Tables S1 & S2), which adjusts for sex, age, cohort group, race/ethnicity, marital status, survey wave, wave*cohort group, relative SES, and wave*SES. Estimated change for each subgroup is computed by summing the relevant coefficients (e.g., the estimated change for late Baby Boomers in the 99th percentile is the sum of the main effect for survey wave, the interaction between wave and the late Baby Boomer cohort group, and the interaction between wave and relative SES). Error bars indicate the 95% confidence interval for each estimate.
The results for perceived intergenerational disadvantage tell a different story: the change over time in these perceptions did not differ significantly by SES. The deterioration in this outcome was nearly as large for those at the top of the SES spectrum as it was for those at the bottom. Yet, evidence of cohort differences remains: respondents’ perceptions of their own financial situations relative to those of their parents at the same age deteriorated more for the late Baby Boomers than for other cohorts (Figure 3).
Subjectively-measured disparities grew more than expected based on objective criteria
The difference between the first and 99th percentiles of relative SES (seen in Figure 3) represents the SES disparity, which we show explicitly in Figure 4. As a result of increased economic distress at the bottom of the SES continuum, coupled with some improvements at the top, the disparities widened. In Figure 4, the first bar for each outcome show the estimated widening of the SES gap adjusted for demographic factors (Model 2): the SES gap increased substantially for current financial strain (by 0.65 SD), current work uncertainty (by 0.56 SD), and future work uncertainty (by 0.51 SD), although there was little change in the SES disparity in perceived intergenerational disadvantage because perceptions deteriorated at all levels of SES.
FIGURE 4. Estimated increase between 1995–1996 and 2011–2014 in the SES disparity (1st–99th percentile) in perceived economic distress before and after adjustment for objectively-measured economic and employment circumstances.
NOTES: Based on the coefficient for the interaction between survey wave and relative SES from Model 2, which adjusts for demographic factors, and Model 3, which adjusts for objective measures of economic and employment circumstances (Tables S1 & S2). Error bars indicate the 95% confidence interval for each estimate.
In Model 3 (Tables S1 and S2), we added controls for objective measures of economic and employment circumstances. Not surprisingly, these objective measures were important predictors of subjective evaluations. For example, perceived current financial well-being was 0.35 SD lower for those who were neither working nor retired than for those who were currently employed; the corresponding difference in ratings of current work situation was 0.63 SD (Table S2, Model 3). As expected, income and assets were inversely associated with perceived financial strain, as was health insurance: perceived financial well-being and intergenerational advantage were about one-quarter SD higher for those covered by health insurance than for those without coverage.
Model 3, which includes both relative and absolute measures of SES, poses a hypothetical scenario: what might the SES disparity in perceptions look like if individuals at the top and bottom shared similar levels of education, income, assets, employment status, etc.? If perceptions were simply a function of observed financial and employment circumstances, then we would expect the SES gap (and widening of that gap over time) to disappear in the fully adjusted model (Model 3).
Although the increases over time in the SES gap in current financial well-being and ratings of current work situation are reduced somewhat after adjustment for objective measures (see fully-adjusted bars in Figure 4), our model accounts for only a small fraction of the increased SES disparity in these perceived measures. As hypothesized in H2, these results suggest that the disparity in perceived financial well-being and employment uncertainty increased even more than we would have expected based on changes in the objective factors measured here. We tested the sensitivity of this result to various alternative specifications for income and assets and to substituting wage/salary income for the respondent and spouse combined in place of household income (not shown). Regardless of how income and assets are specified, controlling for objective measures of economic conditions accounts for only a modest fraction of the widening of the SES differential in perceived current financial strain and work uncertainty.
Non-Latino whites perceive similar economic conditions more negatively than minorities
Based on reference group theory, we hypothesized (H3 ) that non-Latino whites would perceive the same circumstances more negatively than minorities. After controlling for objective measures, we found that non-Latino whites reported higher levels of financial strain than minorities, but there was no significant difference in work uncertainty (Figure 5). The racial/ethnic differential was largest for perceived intergenerational disadvantage: given similar economic and employment conditions, non-Latino whites reported levels of intergenerational disadvantage that were 0.15 SD higher than those of minorities. That result may reflect the reality that economic conditions relative to the previous generation improved more for minorities than for non-Latino whites. Even though non-Latino whites remain financially advantaged relative to minorities, the gap may have narrowed since their parents’ generation.
FIGURE 5. Estimated racial/ethnic differential (non-Latino whites relative to others) in perceived economic distress before and after adjustment for objectively-measured economic and employment circumstances.
NOTES: Based on the coefficient for race/ethnicity from Model 1, which adjusts for demographic factors, and Model 3, which adjusts for objective measures of economic and employment circumstances (Tables S1 & S2). A positive value means that non-Latino whites report higher levels of economic distress than minorities. Error bars indicate the 95% confidence interval for each estimate.
Discussion
Given the well-established increase in economic inequality over the study period, it is not surprising to find that perceived financial strain and employment uncertainty increased more for those at the lower end of the socioeconomic spectrum than for those at the upper end (i.e., the socioeconomic disparity in perceived economic distress widened considerably since the mid-1990s). This finding is consistent with prior studies based on objective measures. For example, Chetty et al. (2016) demonstrated that the fraction of people earning more at age 30 than their parents did at the same age declined sharply from 92 percent for those born in 1940 to 50 percent for those born in 1984. People perceive themselves as worse off because they are worse off. Our own analyses of the objective measures of economic distress bear that out: between the mid-1990s and the early 2010s, income and wealth deteriorated at the bottom of the distribution, although wealth improved for those at the top.
More interesting is our finding that, for those with low SES, perceptions have deteriorated even more than we would expect based on changes in objective measures of economic and employment circumstances. The “have nots” feel worse off than standard economic indicators suggest. Thus, either our measures of the relevant objective circumstances are inadequate (e.g., measurement error or omitted variables such as characteristics of jobs) or individuals’ perceptions of their financial and employment situations are influenced by factors other than standard economic indicators. Such factors could include characteristics of individuals and their social context that influence their reference level for comparison.
Perceived intergenerational advantage shows a different pattern from the other subjective outcomes in the sense that deterioration occurred at most levels of SES, although it was concentrated among the late Baby Boomer cohort. Among the late Baby Boomers, bivariate analyses (not shown) show that the percentage reporting they were “somewhat” or “a lot” better off than their parents were at the same age declined between 1995–1996 and 2011–2014, and that decline was as big for those in the highest SES quintile as it was in the lowest quintile. This perceptual deterioration persisted even after adjusting for objective measures.13 One possible explanation is that the sample was more negatively selected in the 2011– 2014 wave (i.e., more representative of the less fortunate than the earlier wave), but we find little evidence of that. Among late Baby Boomers, the percentage reporting that they did not complete high school was 8 at M1 versus 7 at R1, while the percentage of college graduates increased from 25 at M1 to 31 at R1. Similarly, the distributions for reported educational attainment of respondents’ mothers and fathers among the late Baby Boomer cohort were similar at both waves.
Paradox: Why would whites be more likely than minorities to succumb to despair?
If economic woes are the source of the “despair” that has captured so much recent attention, in particular attention on the plight of working class whites, it seems paradoxical that the deceleration of mortality decline and increasing deaths of despair appear to have affected non-Latino whites more than blacks or Latinos (Kochanek, Arias, and Bastian 2016; Squires and Blumenthal 2016; Case and Deaton 2017). Despite notable narrowing of the race gap in death rates, US blacks continue to experience much higher mortality than US whites (National Center for Health Statistics 2017). Moreover, the Great Recession hit minorities harder than whites and black-white differentials in wages and wealth have persisted unabated (Kochhar and Fry 2014; Wilson and Rodgers 2016). To explain the paradox, we have examined the possibility that a person’s interpretation of his/her current financial condition may not be purely a function of objective circumstances such as income and wealth as viewed by an external observer.
After we adjust for racial/ethnic differences in objective measures of economic and employment conditions, we find that non-Latino whites express higher levels of perceived financial strain than minorities. This racial/ethnic gap in perceptions is widest for intergenerational advantage, which lends credence to Cherlin’s hypothesis that differences in the reference group are critical for understanding despair. Cherlin (2016: A19) notes that, “many non-college-educated whites are comparing themselves to a generation that had more opportunities than they have, whereas many blacks and Hispanics are comparing themselves to a generation that had fewer opportunities.” Among respondents aged 25 to 54 in the General Social Survey, he found that in 2000, whites were more likely than blacks to perceive their standard of living as better than that of their parents, but by 2014 whites were less positive than blacks and Latinos. Case and Deaton (2015: 15081) hinted at a similar idea: “After the productivity slowdown in the early 1970s, and with widening income inequality, many of the babyboom generation are the first to find, in midlife, that they will not be better off than were their parents." In later work, Case and Deaton (2017) point out that whites were likely to be more affected than blacks by the declining rates of absolute mobility noted by Chetty et al. (2016). Although we do not have information about the income level of the respondents’ childhood families, the percentage of respondents who reported that their mothers did not complete high school was much higher for minorities than it was for non-Latino whites (53 percent vs. 34 percent, respectively, in 1995–1996), as was the percentage reporting that they had ever been on welfare during childhood (13 percent vs. 6 percent respectively, in 1995–1996). The racial gap in childhood disadvantage appears to be wider for the Baby Boomer cohorts compared with earlier generations: among those born between 1920 and 1942, 55 percent of non-Latino whites vs. 68 percent of minorities reported that their mother did not complete high school, but the corresponding figures were 31 percent vs. 55 percent for early Baby Boomers, and 22 percent vs. 43 percent for late Baby Boomers. That gap appears to have narrowed for Gen X (born 1965–1979): 14 percent of non-Latino whites vs. 19 percent of minorities had a mother who did not complete high school.
Thus, we might expect non-Latino whites to be more sensitive to status anxiety than minorities, particularly during this period of rising inequality. In auxiliary analyses, we added a two-way interaction between race/ethnicity and period to Model 3 to test whether non-Latino whites exhibited a larger increase in perceived economic distress over this period than minorities. Although the interaction was not statistically significant, the direction of the coefficients is consistent with the notion that the increase in financial strain and future work uncertainty was larger for non-Latino whites compared with minorities. Unfortunately, with fewer than 500 minority respondents at each wave, we have limited statistical power to test this interaction.
The downfall of the “Blue Collar Aristocracy”
Wages (in real terms) among working-class whites peaked for the cohorts that completed high school in the early 1970s; since then, job prospects have steadily deteriorated for successive cohorts with low education (Cherlin 2014; Cherlin 2009; Case and Deaton 2017). Based on qualitative interviews with conservative populist “Tea Party” supporters in southwestern Louisiana, Hochschild (2016a) suggests that many older, native-born working- and middle-class whites in America are feeling left behind. Hochschild (2016b, p. 141) points to 1950 as the turning point “when the Dream stopped working for the [bottom] 90 percent” of white American men: “If you were born before 1950, on average, the older you got, the more your income rose. If you were born after 1950, it did not.” The downfall of the “blue collar aristocracy” has implications not only for employment, but also for traditional social structure (Case and Deaton 2017). One manifestation of this breakdown is the widening of the education gap in rates of marriage and non-marital childbearing (Cherlin 2014; Case and Deaton 2017). Among those without a four-year college degree, successive birth cohorts (since 1940) of non-Latino whites have been less likely to marry, whereas there has been little cohort change in marriage among college graduates (Case and Deaton 2017, Figure 3.2).
We also see evidence of cohort effects: the late Baby Boomers—born between 1954 and 1964—exhibit the largest increases in perceived financial strain and employment uncertainty, whereas those born in 1950 and earlier show little or no increase in perceived economic distress. Similarly, changes over time in perceptions are modest for cohorts born after 1970. What was unique about the experience of the late Baby Boomers? These cohorts were among the hardest hit by declining rates of absolute income mobility, dubbed by Chetty (2016) as “the fading American dream.” The early Baby Boomers also experienced declining rates of mobility, but started from a higher point such that the percentage earning more than their parents ranged from nearly 90 percent of the 1943 cohort to more than 70 percent of the 1953 cohort; in contrast, only 57–70 percent of the late Baby Boomers were better off than their parents (Chetty et al. 2016, Figure 1B). By Gen X (1965–1979), rates of mobility remained low (≈55–60 percent), but the steep decline seemed to be leveling off. Our results are consistent with the economics literature documenting the conditions experienced by the Baby Boomers. As Macunovich (1998) explains, cohorts born during the first half of the boom fared much better than the late Baby Boomers because the older cohorts benefited from an expanding economy fueled in part by the expenditures of the Baby Boomers’ parents and later, by the boomers themselves. In contrast, she notes that those born as the boom ebbed were hit hardest because they still represented relatively large cohorts who had to compete in the labor market with early boomers, and, by the time they entered the labor force, economic growth was slowing as a result of their own declining numbers.
Case and Deaton (2017) argue that economic conditions at the time that cohorts enter the labor force are critically important. The late Baby Boomers would have completed high school between 1972 and 1982, which means they experienced economic recessions (1973–1975 and 1980–1982) characterized by high unemployment during their early working life. In contrast, the early Baby Boomers completed high school during a period of generally low unemployment, while Gen X entered the labor force during a time when economic conditions were improving.
The late Baby Boomers faced the Great Recession during their midlife, when they were likely to own a home with a large mortgage; after the housing market crashed, their debt may have become greater than the market value of the property, making them vulnerable to foreclosure. During this period, the early Baby Boomers were nearing retirement and Gen X was still relatively young (aged 28–42 in 2007). In contrast, as older workers who were not yet old enough to retire, the late Baby Boomers may have had more difficulty finding a new job in the face of unemployment (Coile et al. 2014).
Feeling like others are “Coming from Behind and Cutting in Line”?
The preceding sections focus on how reference levels may differ by race/ethnicity and by cohort. Here we suggest that reference levels within a given subgroup may also change over time depending on social context. As Wilkinson (2016) points out, the psychological effects of economic challenges may be easier to bear in the context of widespread trouble shared by everyone. By contrast, people may have more difficulty accepting their situation when they think others are making better progress. As Hochschild (2016a) describes it, working class whites feel like other people are “coming from behind and cutting in line” (686), thus generating feelings of anxiety, suspicion, and anger.
Decades of increased socioeconomic inequality juxtaposed with social movements (e.g., civil rights, women’s rights) that have reduced some of the disparities between demographic subgroups may have generated conditions that are ripe for status anxiety among Americans who have historically enjoyed greater advantage. As Cherlin (2016) notes, the fact that the median weekly earnings of white men aged 25–54 remains well above the earnings of corresponding black or Latino men may be beside the point. If whites perceive themselves as falling behind, then perception has its own reality. Recent survey data suggest that whites are more likely than blacks to agree that “conditions for black people have improved” (Cherlin 2016) and to say that “blacks and whites are treated about equally in the workplace” (Pew Research Center 2016).
Do perceptions really matter?
An important but unanswered question pertains to the relative impact of subjective versus objective measures of economic distress on social and health outcomes. Objective economic deprivation and employment instability have obvious consequences for those who suffer from them and for the productive potential of society. Yet, perceived economic distress could have more far-reaching social consequences for the population as a whole, potentially contributing to civil unrest, distrust of established institutions, cultural schisms in the fabric of society, and the breakdown of democracy. There is also reason to believe that subjective factors can affect health above and beyond the effects of objective measures. In terms of those health consequences, the salience of perception is perhaps most obvious for mental health outcomes. As the literature on resilience demonstrates, the same life challenge may be internalized very differently depending on the characteristics of the individual and the social context in which s/he is embedded (Pearlin et al. 2005; Bonanno 2012; Pearlin et al. 1981; Pearlin 1989). How a person perceives their reality is intertwined with depression and other forms of mental distress, but perception can also have important consequences for physical health outcomes. Kahn & Pearlin (2006) find that perceived financial strain has long-lasting effects on a variety of health outcomes—including physical impairment, serious chronic conditions, and depressive symptoms—even after controlling for current income.
Limitations and future directions
The limitations of this study include issues related to measurement, modeling, and research design. Even the “objective” measures are self-reported and thus subject to problems of measurement error. There are also extensive missing data, particularly with respect to income and assets. Our measures of objective circumstances are not exhaustive. For example, we have limited information about the nature of the individual’s job, his/her employer, workplace, and employee benefits that might influence ratings of his/her work situation. Furthermore, our model assumes that the effects of losses and gains in income or assets are symmetric, but the concept of loss aversion (Kahneman and Tversky 1984; Kahneman and Tversky 1992) suggests that the psychological cost of an economic loss is greater in magnitude than the perceived benefit of an equivalent gain (i.e., the direction of change matters). In terms of research design, our analysis is based on repeated cross-sectional surveys rather than longitudinal data that would permit us to evaluate within-individual changes over time in economic distress. With two survey waves nearly 20 years apart, a limited age range (25–74) at each wave, and only selected cohorts (1938–1970) observed at both waves, it is difficult to separate the effects of age, period, and cohort. The oldest and youngest cohorts were observed at only one survey wave.
Much of the recent attention to the problem of “despair” centers on its consequences for population health and survival. Thus, it is worth exploring whether perceived measures of economic distress (financial strain and employment uncertainty) might illuminate the roots of this despair. For example, does perceived economic distress predict mortality better than objective measures alone? Perhaps the role of economic distress has been under-estimated because standard economic indicators fail to capture the subjective component. Leininger and Kalil (2014) have suggested that subjective dimensions of economic distress have become more important since the Great Recession. Thus, we might ask whether the predictive abilities of perceived and objective economic distress have changed over time. Invoking the theory of stress proliferation, Kahn and Pearlin (2006) argue that the cumulative effects of financial strain may stem not only from their direct effects, but also from the fact that economic distress perpetuates additional stressors and disruptions in other domains of life. Therefore, it may be fruitful to investigate the links between changes in economic, psychological, and social distress, as well as the strain generated by the interaction between work and family demands.
One way to build on the cross-sectional analyses presented here would be to make use of the longitudinal data in MIDUS: three waves conducted approximately ten years apart between the mid-1990s and the mid-2010s. In future work, we plan to investigate whether similar period and cohort effects for perceived economic distress are evident among the original MIDUS cohort as it ages. The longitudinal data will also allow us to examine life course patterns of economic distress based on a retrospective question about financial difficulties during childhood combined with the questions about current financial strain and employment uncertainty that were repeated at each of the three waves. This longitudinal analysis could be further enriched by the availability of questions in Wave 3 of MIDUS regarding exposure to the Great Recession. Thus, we could explore the effects of foreclosure, selling one’s home at a loss, having difficulty finding a new job, or taking a job for which one is overqualified. Coile et al. (2014) found that exposure to a recession (i.e., high levels of unemployment) has the counter-intuitive positive effect on short-term survival that has been extensively documented in previous literature. In the longer-term, their results demonstrate a negative effect on survival for workers who are nearing retirement (in their late 50s). This finding may be particularly salient for the late Baby Boomers who appear hardest hit in our analysis: they were reaching ages 45–55 in 2009, when unemployment surpassed 9 percent and remained above 7 percent until late 2013 (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics 2017).
Many scholars have pointed to income stagnation and decline as a major explanation for recent health crises, but as Case and Deaton (2017) acknowledged, the income-based explanation by itself does not appear adequate to account for the observed trends. They suggested that we need to look at cumulative lifetime disadvantage across multiple domains (i.e., work, marriage and family, social networks). Link and Phelan (1995) argue that the power of socioeconomic status in affecting health derives from its being a “fundamental cause” that is linked with a broad cluster of interrelated factors, which may not be easily captured by objective measures. The subjective nature of self-reports may represent an advantage in the sense that these responses integrate myriad unobserved factors experienced over a lifetime. Our findings suggest that if we want to understand widening socioeconomic disparities in health and survival, we should look beyond objective measures to consider the role of perceptions. If we want to know how people are doing, perhaps we should ask them.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging [grant numbers P01 AG020166, U19AG051426]; the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [grant number P2CHD047879]; and the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, Georgetown University. We are grateful to Samuel H. Preston, Peter Muennig, Carol D. Ryff, and Barry Radler for their helpful comments and advice regarding this analysis. We also appreciate the useful comments and suggestions from anonymous reviewers.
Footnotes
We use the terms “inequality” and “socioeconomic disparity” interchangeably to refer broadly to the unequal distribution of social and economic resources across the population. Such inequality can take many forms: the unequal distribution of income, wealth, education, employment prospects, job stability, insurance coverage, etc.
Given the high proportion of respondents reporting no net assets or a deficit, we also include in the regression models a dichotomous variable indicating no assets/deficit.
Respondents born before 1943 comprise the Silent Generation (born in 1925–1942) as well as the late GI Generation (i.e., the 1995–1996 wave of MIDUS included those born in 1920–1924, which represents the tail end of the GI cohort born in 1901–1924). Those born after 1964 include Gen X (born in 1965–1979) and some early Millennials (i.e., the 2011–2014 wave included those born in 1980–1989).
Among the pooled analysis sample (N=5,632), 39 percent were missing data for one or more analysis variables. The variables with the highest percentage of missing data were household income (18 percent), assets (13 percent), maximum unemployment spell (7 percent), rating of future work situation (6 percent), and rating of current work situation (5 percent).
We used the “lpoly” command in Stata 12.1 (StataCorp 2011) to perform local mean smoothing—also known as the Nadaraya-Watson estimator (Nadaraya 1964; Watson 1964)—in which a locally weighted average is computed for each point in the smoothing grid (in this case, each age) using a kernel (in this case, Epanechnikov) as the weighting function.
Median household income also declined from $47,500 to $43,065 in 1995 dollars (not shown).
At Wave M1 (1995–1996), MIDUS surveyed individuals representing the cohorts born in 1920 through 1974 (age 23–76 at the end of 1996). When the same age range was targeted at Wave R1 (2011–2014), the sample included cohorts born in 1937 through 1989 (age 25–77 at the end of 2014).
The late Baby Boomer cohort would have completed high school in 1971–1982 and would have reached age 40 between 1994 and 2004. By the R1 Wave (2011–2014), they would have been age 45–65.
The older cohorts observed at both waves included the early Baby Boomers (born 1943–1953) and the late Silent Generation (those born in 1938–1942).
The youngest cohorts observed at both waves comprised those born in 1965–1970 (i.e., early Gen X), who would have been age 45 and younger in 2011.
We show the first and 99th percentiles for simplicity: the main effect for survey wave represents the change over time for those in the first percentile (among cohorts born before 1943), while the sum of the main effect and the interaction between survey wave and relative SES denotes the change among their counterparts above the 99th percentile.
Despite limited statistical power, we tested a three-way interaction between cohort, period, and SES, but none of the interaction terms were significant nor did they jointly improve model fit. Thus, we found no evidence that the widening of the SES differential varied by cohort.
References
- Adler Nancy E., Epel Elissa S., Castellazzo Grace and Ickovics Jeanette R. 2000. “Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women,” Health Psychol 19(6): 586–592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arber Sara, Fenn Kristy and Meadows Robert. 2014. “Subjective financial well-being, income and health inequalities in mid and later life in Britain,” Social Science & Medicine (1982) 100: 12–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonanno George A. 2012. “Uses and abuses of the resilience construct: loss, trauma, and health-related adversities,” Social Science & Medicine (1982) 74(5): 753–756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bosworth Barry, Burtless Gary and Zhang. K 2016. “Later retirement, inequality in old age, and the growing gap in longevity between rich and poor .” Washington, DC: Brookings Institute. [Google Scholar]
- Brim Orville G., Baltes Paul B., Bumpass Larry L., Cleary Paul D., Featherman David L., Hazzard William R., Kessler Ronald C., Lachman Margie E., Markus Hazel R., Marmot Michael G., Rossi Alice S., Ryff Carol D., and Shweder Richard A. 2016. National Survey of Midlife Development in the United States (MIDUS), 1995–1996. ICPSR02760-v11. 10.3886/ICPSR02760.v11, Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research, Ann Arbor, MI. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bureau of Labor Statistics. 2017. “Labor Force Statistics from the Current Population Survey, seasonally adjusted unemployment rate, age 16 years and older.” https://data.bls.gov/pdq/SurveyOutputServlet. Accessed 17 December 2017.
- Case Anne. 2015. “‘Deaths of despair’ are killing America’s white working class.” https://qz.com/583595/deaths-of-despair-are-killing-americas-white-working-class/. Accessed 11 December 2017.
- Case Anne and Deaton Angus. 2015. “Rising morbidity and mortality in midlife among white non-Hispanic Americans in the 21st century,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112(49): 15078–15083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- ———. 2017, “Mortality and morbidity in the 21st century,” eds. Eberly J & Stock J, Brookings Institution, Washington, DC, p. 397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cherlin Andrew J. 2016, “Why are white death rates rising? ” http://www.nytimes.com/2016/02/22/opinion/why-are-white-death-rates-rising.html?emc=eta1&_r=0edn.
- Cherlin Andrew J. 2009. The Marriage-Go-Round: The State of Marriage and the Family in America to-Day. New York: Vintage. [Google Scholar]
- ———. 2014. Labor’s Love Lost: The Rise and Fall of the Working Class Family in America. New York: Russell Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Chetty Raj, Grusky David, Hell Maximilian, Hendren Nathaniel, Manduca Robert. et al. 2016a. “The fading American dream: Trends in absolute income mobility since 1940 .” Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chetty Raj, Stepner Michael, Abraham Sarah, Lin Shelby, Scuderi Benjamin. et al. 2016b. “The association between income and life expectancy in the United States, 2001–2014,” Jama 315(16): 1750–1766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coile Courtney C., Levine Phillip B. and McKnight Robin. 2014. “Recessions, Older Workers, and Longevity: How Long Are Recessions Good for Your Health?” American Economic Journal: Economic Policy 6(3): 92–119. [Google Scholar]
- Conger Rand D., Elder Glen H., Lorenz Frederick O., Conger Katherine J., Simons Ronald L. et al. 1990. “Linking economic hardship to marital quality and instability,” Journal of Marriage and Family 52(3): 643–656. [Google Scholar]
- Congressional Budget Office. 2013. “The Distribution of Household Income and Federal Taxes, 2013 .” Washington, DC: Congress of the United States, Congressional Budget Office. [Google Scholar]
- Davis James. 1959. “A formal interpretation of the theory of relative deprivation,” Sociometry 22(4): 280–296. [Google Scholar]
- Dowd Jennifer B. and Hamoudi Amar. 2014. “Is life expectancy really falling for groups of low socio-economic status? Lagged selection bias and artefactual trends in mortality,” International Journal of Epidemiology 43(4): 983–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans William, Hout Michael and Mayer Susan. 2004. “Assessing the effect of economic inequality,” in Neckerman Kathryn (ed.), Social Inequality. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, pp. 933–968. [Google Scholar]
- Hastings Orestes P. 2017. Inequality and Society: Mechanisms and Methods for Understanding the Consequences of Rising Income Inequality, University of California, Berkeley. [Google Scholar]
- Hedegaard Holly, Warner Margaret and Miniño Arialdi M. 2017. “Drug overdose deaths in the United States, 1999–2015 .” Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics. [Google Scholar]
- Hendi Arun S. 2015. “Trends in U.S. life expectancy gradients: the role of changing educational composition,” International Journal of Epidemiology 44(3): 946–955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hochschild Arlie R. 2016a. “The Ecstatic Edge of Politics: Sociology and Donald Trump,” Contemporary Sociology 45(6): 683–689. [Google Scholar]
- ———. 2016b. Strangers in their Own Land: Anger and Mourning on the American Right. New York, NY: The New Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hyman Herbert H. 1942. “The psychology of status,” Archives of Psychology 269: 94. [Google Scholar]
- Kahn Joan R. and Pearlin Leonard I. 2006. “Financial strain over the life course and health among older adults,” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 47(1): 17–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman Daniel and Tversky Amos. 1984. “Choices, Values, and Frames,” American Psychologist 39(4): 341–350. [Google Scholar]
- ———. 1992. “Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 5(4): 297–323. [Google Scholar]
- Kawachi Ichiro and Berkman Lisa F. 2000. “Social cohesion, social capital, and health,” in Berkman LF and Kawachi I (eds.), Social Epidemiology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 174–190 [Google Scholar]
- Kawachi Ichiro, Kennedy Bruce P., Lochner Kimberley and Prothrow-Stith. Deborah 1997. “Social capital, income inequality, and mortality,” American Journal of Public Health 87(9): 1491–1498 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khazan Olga. 2015. “Middle-Aged White Americans Are Dying of Despair.” https://www.theatlantic.com/health/archive/2015/11/boomers-deaths-pnas/413971/. Accessed 11 December 2017.
- Kochanek Kenneth D., Arias Elizabeth, and Bastian Brigham A. 2016. “The Effect of Changes in Selected Age-specific Causes of Death on Non-Hispanic White Life Expectancy Between 2000 and 2014 .” Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistic. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kochhar Rakesh and Fry Richard. 2014. “Wealth inequality has widened along racial, ethnic lines since end of Great Recession.” http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2014/12/12/racial-wealth-gaps-great-recession/ Accessed 5 December 2017.
- Kochhar Rakesh, Fry Richard and Taylor Paul. 2011. “Wealth gaps rise to record highs between whites, blacks, Hispanics.” http://www.pewsocialtrends.org/2011/07/26/wealth-gaps-rise-to-record-highs-between-whites-blacks-hispanics/. Accessed 26 September 2017.
- Layte Richard and Whelan Christopher T.. 2013. “Who feels inferior? A test of the status anxiety hypothesis of social inequalities in health .” Dublin: AIAS. [Google Scholar]
- Leininger Lindsey J. and Kalil Ariel. 2014. “Economic strain and children’s behavior in the after-math of the great recession,” Journal of Marriage and Family 76: 998–1010. [Google Scholar]
- Link Bruce G. and Phelan Jo. 1995. “Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease,” J Health Soc Behav Spec No: 80–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipset Seymour M. 1955. “The Sources of the Radical Right,” in Bell Daniel(ed.), The New American Right. New York: Criterion Books, pp. 166–233. [Google Scholar]
- Macunovich Diane J. 1998. “Relative cohort size and inequality in the United States,” American Economic Review 88(2): 259–264. [Google Scholar]
- Merton Robert K. and Rossi Alice K.. 1950. “Contributions to the theory of reference group behavior,” in Merton Robert K. and Lazarsfeld Paul F. (eds.), Continuities in Social Research: Studies in the Scope and Method of “the American Soldier. Glencoe, IL: Free Press, 40–105. [Google Scholar]
- Merton Robert K. 1957. “Continuities in the theory of reference groups and social structure,” in Social Theory and Social Structure. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. [Google Scholar]
- Monnat Shannon M. 2016. “Deaths of Despair and Support for Trump in the 2016 Presidential Election, The Pennsylvania State University Department of Agricultural Economics, Sociology, and Education Research Brief .” State College, PA: Pennsylvania State University. [Google Scholar]
- Nadaraya Elizbar A. 1964. “On estimate regression,” Theory of Probability and its Application 9: 141–142. [Google Scholar]
- National Center for Health Statistics. 2017. “Health, United States, 2016: With Chartbook on Long-term Trends in Health .” Hyattsville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics. [Google Scholar]
- Olshansky SJ, Antonucci Toni, Berkman Lisa, Binstock Robert H., Boersch-Supan Axel et al. 2012. “Differences in life expectancy due to race and educational differences are widening, and many may not catch up,” Health Affairs (Project Hope) 31(8): 1803–1813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostrove Joan M., Adler Nancy E., Kuppermann Miriam and Eugene Washington A.. 2000. “Objective and Subjective Assessments of Socioeconomic Status and Their Relationship to Self-Rated Health in an Ethnically Diverse Sample of Pregnant Women,” Health Psychology 19(6): 613–618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearlin Leonard I., Menaghan Elizabeth G., Lieberman Morton A. and Mullan Joseph T.. 1981. “The stress process,” Journal of Health and Social Behavior; Journal of Health and Social Behavior 22(4): 37–356. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearlin Leonard I., Schieman Scott, Fazio Elena M. and Meersman Stephen C.. 2005. “Stress, health, and the life course: some conceptual perspectives,” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 46(2): 205–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearlin Leonard I. 1989. “The sociological study of stress,” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 30(3): 241–256. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pew Research Center. 2016. “How blacks and whites view the state of race in America.” http://www.pewsocialtrends.org/interactives/state-of-race-in-america. Accessed 5 December 2017.
- Piketty Thomas, Saez Emmanuel and Zucm Gabriel. 2016. “Distributional national accounts: methods and estimates for the United States, NBER Working Paper Series .” Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research. [Google Scholar]
- Rubin Donald B. 1996. “Multiple imputation after 18+ years (with discussion),” Journal of the American Statistical Association 91: 473–489. [Google Scholar]
- Runciman Walter G. 1966. Relative Deprivation and Social Justice. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. [Google Scholar]
- Ryff Carol, Almeida David, Ayanian John, Binkley Neil, Carr Deborah, Coe Christopher, Davidson Richard, Grzywacz Joseph, Karlamangla Arun, Krueger Robert, Lachman Margie, Love Gayle, Mailick Marsha, Mroczek Daniel, Radler Barry, Seeman Teresa, Sloan Richard, Thomas Duncan, Weinstein Maxine, and Williams David. 2016. Midlife in the United States (MIDUS Refresher), 2011–2014. ICPSR36532-v2 10.3886/ICPSR36532.v2, Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor], Ann Arbor, MI. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sasson I 2016. “Diverging Trends in Cause-Specific Mortality and Life Years Lost by Educational Attainment: Evidence from United States Vital Statistics Data, 1990–2010,” PloS One 11(10): e0163412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafer Joseph L. 1999. “Multiple imputation: a primer,” Statistical Methods in Medical Research 8(1): 3–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shippee Tetyana P., Wilkinson Lindsay R. and Ferraro Kenneth F.. 2012. “Accumulated financial strain and women’s health over three decades,” The Journals of Gerontology.Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 67(5): 585–594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh-Manoux Archana, Adler Nancy E. and Marmot Michael G.. 2003. “Subjective social status: its determinants and its association with measures of ill-health in the Whitehall II study,” Soc Sci Med 56(6): 1321–1333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squires David and Blumenthal David. 2016. “Mortality Trends Among Working-Age Whites: The Untold Story. 2016. (January):11” Commonwealth Fund Issue Brief 3(1861): 1–11. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp. 2011. Stata: Release 12. Statistical Software. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP. [Google Scholar]
- Szanton Sarah L., Allen Jerilyn K., Thorpe Roland J. Jr., Seeman Teresa, Bandeen-Roche Karen. et al. 2008. “Effect of financial strain on mortality in community-dwelling older women,” The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 63(6): S369–S374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. 2017. “Labor Force Statistics from the Current Population Survey.” https://data.bls.gov/pdq/SurveyOutputServlet. Accessed 13 December 2017.
- Voydanoff Patricia. 1990. “Economic distress and family relations: a review of the eighties,” Journal of Marriage and Family 52(4): 1099–1115. [Google Scholar]
- Watson Geoffrey. 1964. “Smooth regression analysis,” Sankhya Series A 26: 359–372. [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson Lindsay R. 2016. “Financial Strain and Mental Health Among Older Adults During the Great Recession,” The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 71(4): 745–754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson Valerie and Rodgers William M.. 2016. “Black-white wage gaps expand with rising wage inequality .” Washington, D.C.: Economic Policy Institute. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.