Abstract
Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST) proposes a widely used taxonomy of human personality linked to individual differences at both behavioral and neuropsychological levels that describe a predisposition to psychopathology. However, the body of RST research was based on animal findings, and little is known about their anatomical correspondence in humans. Here we set out to investigate MRI structural correlates (i.e. voxel-based morphometry) of the main personality dimensions proposed by the RST in a group of 400 healthy young adults who completed the Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ). Sensitivity to punishment scores correlated positively with the gray matter volume in the amygdala, whereas sensitivity to reward scores correlated negatively with the volume in the left lateral and medial prefrontal cortex. Moreover, a negative relationship was found between the striatal volume and the reward sensitivity trait, but only for male participants. The present results support the neuropsychological basis of the RST by linking punishment and reward sensitivity to anatomical differences in limbic and frontostriatal regions, respectively. These results are interpreted based on previous literature related to externalizing and internalizing disorders, and they highlight the possible role of SPSRQ as a measure of proneness to these disorders.
Keywords: personality, voxel-based morphometry, limbic system, frontostriatal circuit, psychopathological predisposition
Introduction
The role of specific brain systems in personality development has long been hypothesized (i.e. Pavlov, 1941; Eysenck, 1960). One of the most influential models proposed in the past 50 years is Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST; Gray, 1970; Gray, 1982; Gray and McNaughton, 2000; Pickering and Gray, 2001; Corr, 2008; McNaughton and Corr, 2008; Corr and McNaughton, 2012), which proposes a detailed neuropsychological description of the neural circuits underlying personality and psychopathological predisposition. The current version of the RST proposes three different systems responsible for taking control of behavior in the presence of emotional stimuli. The fight-flight-freeze system (FFFS) is neurally represented by an interconnected circuit comprising the amygdala (fear perception) and the medial hypothalamus and periaqueductal gray matter (behavioral response), and it mediates reactions to aversive stimuli, promoting defensive avoidance behavior. The behavioral inhibition system (BIS) has traditionally been related to the septo-hippocampal system, although in the last revision of the theory (Gray and McNaughton, 2000; McNaughton and Corr, 2004), the amygdala was also included, and it responds specifically to goal conflict (i.e. approach–approach, approach–avoidance or avoidance–avoidance) to initiate defensive approach behavior. Finally, the behavioral approach system (BAS) is dependent on the frontostriatal system, mainly the ventral [nucleus accumbens (NAcc)] and dorsal (caudate nucleus) striatum and its dopaminergic connections to the prefrontal lobe, and it mediates reactions to appetitive stimuli, promoting approach.
Although the description of the three neural systems should involve three different personality dimensions, the currently most accepted RST framework solution shows the existence of two general traits (Perkins et al., 2007). The first is punishment sensitivity, which depends on the combination of the FFFS and BIS systems in a global fear/anxiety dimension, and the second is reward sensitivity, which is associated with the BAS. These two dimensions could be assessed with specific measures such as the BIS/BAS scales, the SPSRQ or the combination of the extraversion and neuroticism dimensions of the EPQ (Torrubia et al., 2008). Overall, the different punishment sensitivity measures typically show stronger correlations with each other (and with anxiety and fear measures), whereas the reward sensitivity measures only have moderate relationships with each other (Caseras et al., 2003).
Predictions of RST are very precise in the field of learning and conditioning. The theory postulates that individual differences in the activity of the FFFS/BIS and the BAS would be associated with (i) a different capacity to detect aversive and appetitive stimuli, respectively and (ii) a different probability and intensity of emitting defensive and approach responses in the presence of these stimuli (see Ávila and Torrubia, 2008; Corr, 2008, for reviews). Different studies have confirmed that individuals with higher scores on measures of FFFS/BIS activity have better learning in the presence of aversive conditioned stimuli and a greater probability of behavioral inhibition and better learning of the contingencies in aversive contexts. Similarly, individuals with higher scores on BAS measures have better learning and make more intense and probable approach responses in appetitive contexts.
RST has acquired great relevance in relating individual differences in the functioning of the FFFS, BIS, and BAS to the vulnerability to different psychopathologies. The RST formulation has related high FFFS activity to panic and phobic disorders (Barlow, 1988) and high BIS activity to generalized anxiety disorders (Maack et al., 2012). At the other extreme, evidence exists relating low BIS activity to psychopathy in adults (Fowles, 1980; Newman et al., 2005) and to the presence of callous/unemotional traits in children (Quay, 1988; Blair, 2003). In addition, some proposals relate high BAS activity to disinhibitory disorders such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Newman and Wallace, 1993; Nigg, 2001; Mitchell and Nelson-Gray, 2006) or drug addiction (Sher and Trull, 1994; Franken et al., 2006). At the opposite extreme, some reports have related depressive disorders to lower BAS activity (Pinto-Meza et al., 2006; Kimbrel et al., 2007). Overall, all these data link punishment sensitivity to the presence of internalizing disorders and reward sensitivity to externalizing disorders (Bijttebier et al., 2009; Slobodskaya, 2016).
Although the detailed description of the brain circuits involved in these systems is one of the strongest points of the theory, this description is based on animal studies, and less research has been dedicated to verifying it in humans. The first attempt to test the neuropsychological basis of RST in the human brain was carried out by Barrós-Loscertales et al. (2006b), who reported a negative association between BAS activity and gray matter (GM) volume in the striatum and prefrontal cortex. Since then, only one study conducted in a large sample has provided new evidence relating BAS measures and brain anatomy (Holmes et al., 2016). In this study, a composite measure of novelty seeking was associated with reduced cortical thickness in brain areas related to cognitive control, such as the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, the lateral prefrontal cortex and the supramarginal gyrus. Consistent with these anatomical studies, a DTI study that used the TCI of Cloninger showed that the two scales related to reward sensitivity were associated with different connectivity measures of the striatum (Lei et al., 2014). Specifically, the novelty-seeking scale was related to stronger connectivity between the striatum and the hippocampus/amygdala, whereas the reward dependence scale was related to stronger connectivity between the striatum and different medial and lateral areas of the prefrontal cortex. On the other hand, Barrós-Loscertales et al. (2006a) first linked BIS activity to higher GM volume in the amygdala and the hippocampus. However, these results have only been partially replicated. Previous studies only found associations with BIS measures in the amygdala (Iidaka et al., 2006) or the hippocampus (Cherbuin et al., 2008; Levita et al., 2014), whereas other studies found a relationship with both structures (Holmes et al., 2012). Thus, these previous studies have produced different results, and so further research is required to address this controversy.
Thus, the aim of the current study was to investigate the brain regions associated with the personality dimensions depicted in the RST. Specifically, our objective was to extend our previous reports (Barrós-Loscertales et al., 2006a; Barrós-Loscertales et al., 2006b) in a larger sample consisting of males and females. Additionally, we also conducted further analyses in order to investigate possible differences by sex. Based on previous studies, we hypothesized that an overactive BAS would be associated with lower GM volume in prefrontal and striatal areas, whereas an overactive FFFS/BIS would be linked to greater GM volume in the amygdala and hippocampus. Because personality traits in their extreme forms are considered vulnerability factors for personality and mood disorders (Gray and McNaughton, 2000; Bijttebier et al., 2009; Corr and McNaughton, 2015), greater knowledge about the underlying neural correlates of personality should also contribute to better understand these clinical conditions in unmedicated participants. Therefore, our reports would add new evidence to the RST model as a valid neuropsychological framework to study the proneness to psychopathological disorders in humans.
Materials and methods
Participants
Four hundred participants (233 males, 167 females; mean age 23.08, s.d. 5.32; range 18–44 years; years of education 14.46, s.d. 2.22) were studied after giving their prior informed consent. All the participants were recruited from a community sample through local advertisements and word of mouth. Most of them were undergraduate students (94.75%), given that our research group is integrated in a university campus. Participants had no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, major medical illnesses or traumatic brain injury with loss of consciousness. Additionally, all participants were right-handed, according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971; Bryden, 1977). The experiment was approved by the Ethical Committee of the University Jaume I (Spain).
Personality measurement
The Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ; Torrubia et al., 2001) was used to assess individual differences in personality. The SPSRQ provides two subscales; sensitivity to reward (SR) and sensitivity to punishment (SP), in order to evaluate the activity of the BAS and BIS/FFFS subsystems, respectively.
MRI acquisition and voxel-based morphometry
Images were all acquired with the same 1.5-T Siemens Avanto scanner (Erlangen, Germany). A high-resolution structural T1-weighted MPRAGE sequence was acquired (TE, 3.8 ms; TR, 2200 ms; flip angle, 15°; matrix, 256 × 256 × 160 mm; voxel size, 1 mm3). Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) was performed with the VBM8 toolbox (version r445; http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm/) for the SPM8 package (version 6313; Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). We performed the standard pre-processing procedure suggested in the VBM8 manual, which included (i) segmentation of the images into GM, white matter and cerebrospinal fluid, (ii) registration to a standard template provided by the International Consortium of Brain Mapping, (iii) DARTEL normalization of the GM segments to the MNI template and (iv) modulation by non-linear components derived from spatial normalization. After the pre-processing, a data quality check was carried out by analyzing the sample homogeneity using covariance. Ten participants showed a covariance of at least two standard deviations below the mean; however, only two participants were identified as outliers (poor data quality) and excluded from the final sample. Finally, images were spatially smoothed using an 8 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel.
Statistical analysis
Voxel-wise regression analyses were performed by taking GM volume as the dependent variable and scores on the SR and SP scales as covariates of interest within the framework of the general linear model in SPM8. We also added age, sex and years of education as nuisance covariates in order to control possible effects on brain volume. In addition, the absolute threshold masking was set at 0.20 to ensure that we only selected GM voxels. Finally, our results at the cluster level were also corrected for non-stationary smoothing.
The statistical significance threshold for the whole-brain analysis was determined following recent recommendations by Chen et al. (2019). Therefore, we established a voxel-level primary threshold of P < 0.0005 uncorrected, whereas we thresholded the obtained results at P < 0.025 FWE corrected at the cluster level. In addition, we also investigated focal VBM differences in small a priori regions of interest (ROIs). Based on the key regions involved in the BAS, BIS and FFFS subsystems, these ROIs were located in reward-related areas (NAcc and caudate) for the SR analyses and in punishment-related areas (amygdala and hippocampus) for the SP analyses. All the ROIs were defined for each hemisphere using the atlas provided by Neuromorphometrics, Inc. (http://Neuromorphometrics.com/) under academic subscription. Furthermore, as our previous study found differences in the anterior hippocampus (Barrós-Loscertales et al., 2006a), and this region has previously been related to anxiety-related behaviors (Bannerman et al., 2004; Fanselow and Dong, 2010), we decided to include this area as a ROI for the SP analysis. For this purpose, the left and right hippocampus were traced manually on contiguous coronal slices in an MNI template following the guidelines of Watson et al. (1992) and Hasboun et al. (1996). This segmentation was carried out by an expert tracer using the MRIcron software. Moreover, because the amygdala and the anterior segment of the hippocampus are adjacent structures, we applied an exclusive mask (the amygdala ROI) to the anterior hippocampal ROI in order to remove all the possible voxels contained in the anterior hippocampus that could be overlapping voxels in the amygdala. All the ROIs included in the SR and SP analyses are shown in Figure 1. The modulated GM volumes (without smoothing) were obtained for each ROI via a MATLAB script (http://www0.cs.ucl.ac.uk/staff/g.ridgway/vbm/get_totals.m), also applying an absolute threshold masking of 0.20. After that, partial correlations (one tailed based on a priori hypotheses) were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 25 (Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.), including GM volumes and scores on the SR and SP scales as variables, controlling for age, sex and years of education. Because we used four ROIs for the SR (left and right NAcc and caudate) and SP (left and right amygdala and anterior hippocampus) analyses, the statistical threshold for multiple comparisons (P < 0.05 FWE) was set at P < 0.0125. Finally, because in our previous studies (Barrós-Loscertales et al., 2006a; Barrós-Loscertales et al., 2006b) the sample consisted only of male participants, we decided to conduct a new set of analyses in order to investigate possible sex differences. Thus, we carried out an omnibus model in SPM8 by calculating an interaction term with sex (including age and years of education as nuisance covariates). At the ROI level, as the analyses were performed in SPSS, sex differences were computed by analyzing the difference between the correlation coefficients using Fisher’s z test.
Fig. 1.
ROIs included in the SR and SP analyses. The anterior hippocampus corresponds to a previous manual segmentation. Red: amygdala; green: anterior hippocampus; blue: caudate; violet: nucleus accumbens.
Results
Personality scores
Participants’ mean score on the SR scale was 10.54 (s.d. 4.68), and the internal consistency was good (Cronbach’s α = 0.81). The mean score on the SP scale was 9.67 (s.d. 5.24), and its internal consistency was also high (Cronbach’s α = 0.85). Furthermore, SR and SP did not show any relationship with each other (r = −0.05; P = 0.371). Personality, demographic and raw volume data for males and females are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1.
Personality, demographic and volumetric data by sex
Whole sample (N = 400) | Males (N = 233) | Females (N = 167) | t-value Males vs Females | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 23.08 (5.32)18–44 | 24.54 (6.07)18–44 | 21.05 (3.04)18–42 | 7.56 | 0.000** |
Years of education | 14.46 (2.22)8–20 | 14.30 (2.41)8–20 | 14.69 (1.91)8–19 | −1.85 | 0.066 |
SR | 10.54 (4.68)1–23 | 11.73 (4.80)3–23 | 8.88 (3.98)1–20 | 6.49 | 0.000** |
SP | 9.67 (5.24)0–23 | 8.70 (5.01)0–22 | 11.02 (5.27)1–23 | −4.46 | 0.000** |
Left NAcc | 0.51 (0.06)0.28–0.71 | 0.50 (0.06)0.28–0.67 | 0.53 (0.06)0.39–0.71 | −5.39 | 0.000** |
Right NAcc | 0.48 (0.06)0.26–0.64 | 0.46 (0.05)0.26–0.58 | 0.50 (0.05)0.38–0.64 | −7.07 | 0.000** |
Left caudate | 3.20 (0.39)1.86–4.27 | 3.09 (0.37)1.86–4.21 | 3.36 (0.36)2.43–4.27 | −7.51 | 0.000** |
Right caudate | 3.04 (0.40)1.50–4.13 | 2.93 (0.38)1.50–3.91 | 3.20 (0.36)2.40–4.13 | −7.33 | 0.000** |
Left amygdala | 1.04 (0.09)0.79–1.33 | 1.03 (0.08)0.79–1.25 | 1.05 (0.10)0.81–1.33 | −2.01 | 0.046* |
Right amygdala | 1.00 (0.09)0.78–1.27 | 0.99 (0.08)0.78–1.27 | 1.02 (0.09)0.81–1.23 | −3.56 | 0.000** |
Left anterior hippocampus | 1.13 (0.10)0.83–1.45 | 1.12 (0.09)0.85–1.42 | 1.14 (0.10)0.83–1.45 | −1.65 | 0.099 |
Right anterior hippocampus | 1.29 (0.11)1.00–1.64 | 1.28 (0.11)1.00–1.64 | 1.31 (0.11)1.06–1.59 | −2.67 | 0.008** |
** P < 0.01, *P < 0.05 (two-tailed t-tests). Volumetric data are reported in milliliters. The three first columns show mean, s.d. (in parentheses) and range (in italics) of each variable. NAcc: nucleus accumbens.
Sensitivity to reward
The voxel-wise GM analysis for the whole sample showed a negative correlation between SR scores and GM volume in frontal and temporal areas (see Table 2 for details). The most relevant reductions were located in the medial prefrontal cortex (from dorsomedial to ventromedial regions and also including the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex) and the left lateral prefrontal cortex, with a cluster extending from the middle to the inferior frontal cortex (Figure 2A and Figure 3). No positive correlations were found between SR scores and GM volume. Moreover, the omnibus model did not reveal any significant differences by sex.
Table 2.
Brain regions showing a negative correlation between SR scores and the voxel-wise GM volume
Region | Hemisphere | Coordinates MNI x, y, z | t-scores | k-voxels | P-value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Inferior frontal (pars opercularis) | L | −56, 8, 21 | 4.87 | 1731 | 0.0004 |
Middle frontal | L | −30, 45, 28 | 4.84 | ||
Inferior frontal (pars triangularis) | L | −45, 44, 13 | 4.43 | ||
Precentral | L | −54, −3, 30 | 4.23 | ||
Inferior frontal (pars orbitalis) | L | −47, 20, −6 | 3.86 | ||
Anterior cingulate | L | −2, 50, 1 | 4.37 | 1489 | 0.0008 |
Medial superior frontal | L | −6, 62, 15 | 4.17 | ||
Medial superior frontal | R | 3, 57, 15 | 3.95 | ||
Middle temporal | L | −51, −30, 4 | 4.45 | 851 | 0.008 |
Superior temporal | L | −59, −15, 6 | 4.15 | ||
Insula | L | −38, −16, −2 | 3.65 |
R: right; L: left; P < 0.05 FWE cluster-level corrected (two-tailed tests).
Fig. 2.
A) Correlation between SR scores and GM volume in the left lateral and medial prefrontal clusters derived from the voxel-wise multiple regression analysis in the whole sample (controlling for age, sex and years of education). B) Partial correlations for males and females showing a GM reduction in the left NAcc in males with high SR in comparison with females (after regressing out age and years of education). C) Partial correlation of the left amygdala GM volume with SP scores in the whole sample (after regressing out age, sex and years of education). NAcc, nucleus accumbens; PFC, prefrontal cortex.
Fig. 3.
Negative correlation between scores on the SR scale and GM volume in the left lateral and medial prefrontal cortex and superior temporal regions (P < 0.05 FWE corrected, two-tailed). Bar color represents t-values. L, left; R, right.
The partial correlations between SR scores and striatal ROI volumes appear on Table 3. Contrary to our hypothesis, the expected negative relationship between SR scores and striatum volume was only significant in males for the left NAcc and caudate volumes. Results of Fisher’s z tests comparing the correlation coefficients between males and females reached significance only for the left NAcc (z = −1.83; P = 0.034; Figure 2B).
Table 3.
Partial correlations (r) between a priori ROIs and scores on the SR scale (controlling for age, sex and years of education)
Whole sample (N = 400) | Males (N = 233) | Females (N = 167) | |
---|---|---|---|
Left NAcc | −0.09 | −0.17* | 0.02 |
Right NAcc | −0.03 | −0.06 | 0.01 |
Left caudate | −0.09 | −0.15* | −0.00 |
Right caudate | −0.09 | −0.13 | −0.04 |
One-tailed t-tests; *P < 0.05 FWE corrected.
Sensitivity to punishment
The voxel-wise GM analyses for the whole sample did not show any significant negative or positive correlations with the SP scale. Similarly, the omnibus model did not reveal any significant differences by sex.
At the ROI level, we observed a significant positive correlation between the scores on the SP scale and the GM volume in the left amygdala (r = 0.15; P = 0.002, FWE corrected; Figure 2C). Moreover, when analyzing the correlation coefficients in males and females, only the left amygdala showed a significant, positive association in males (Table 4). However, Fisher’s z test did not show any significant differences between males and females.
Table 4.
Partial correlations (r) between a priori ROIs and scores on the SP scale (controlling for age, sex and years of education)
Whole sample (N = 400) | Males (N = 233) | Females (N = 167) | |
---|---|---|---|
Left amygdala | 0.15* | 0.15* | 0.13 |
Right amygdala | 0.08 | 0.07 | 0.09 |
Left anterior hippocampus | 0.09 | 0.11 | 0.05 |
Right anterior hippocampus | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.07 |
One-tailed t-tests; *P < 0.05 FWE corrected.
Discussion
In the present study, we aimed to reveal the neuroanatomical anatomical base underlying individual differences in the main personality dimensions depicted by the RST in a sample of 400 healthy participants studied with the same MRI scanner. Consistent with previous research, our results showed that scores on the SP scale were positively associated with the GM volume in the left amygdala. Regarding SR, we found a direct negative relationship between reward sensitivity and the volume of the left lateral and medial prefrontal cortex. Furthermore, we have replicated our previous results relating this dimension to the striatum volume (Barrós-Loscertales et al., 2006b), although this result was not found in females. Thus, the main personality dimensions derived from the RST that measure vulnerability to the main psychopathological entities have been associated with differences in GM volume in target emotional brain areas. These results reinforce the usefulness of the SPSRQ in measuring individual differences in the neuropsychological systems depicted by the RST.
Our results are consistent with the RST and previous reports relating a greater GM volume in the amygdala to the scores on the SP scale from the SPSRQ (Barrós-Loscertales et al., 2006a), on the Harm Avoidance scale (Iidaka et al., 2006) and on Neuroticism (Koelsch et al., 2013). Furthermore, in a study conducted in a large sample (Holmes et al., 2012), the authors reported a GM increase in the amygdala associated with scores on a customized scale based on behavioral inhibition and trait negative affect measures. Studies in healthy children have also demonstrated that inhibited or anxious personalities are linked to greater amygdala volume (Clauss et al., 2014; Qin et al., 2014). When studying pathological populations, enlargement of the amygdala has been reported in adults, children and adolescents with Generalized Anxiety Disorder (De Bellis et al., 2000; Etkin et al., 2009; Schienle et al., 2011) and in adults with depression (Bremner et al., 2000; Tebartz van Elst et al., 2000; Lange and Irle, 2004; van Eijndhoven et al., 2009; Vassilopoulou et al., 2013), although results with depressive patients can vary depending on medication effects (Hamilton et al., 2008). Moreover, the amygdala structure has also been associated with certain genetic alleles in panic disorder (Smoller et al., 2014). At the opposite pole, the presence of psychopathic or callous/unemotional traits in children and adult populations has been consistently associated with lower amygdala volume (Yang et al., 2009; Pardini et al., 2014; Vieira et al., 2015; Aghajani et al., 2016; Coccaro et al., 2016; Cohn et al., 2016), which has also been associated with conduct problems in youths (Rogers and De Brito, 2016). Among the different factors that contribute to modulating the amygdala volume, previous studies have reported the influence of genetic factors (Smoller et al., 2014) and early experiences with adverse situations (Howell et al., 2014). Regarding the hippocampal volume, contrary to our expectations, the expected positive association between SP scores and GM volume in the anterior hippocampal ROI did not reach significance. Although previous studies have found a significant relationship between the GM volume in the hippocampus and scores on BIS measures (Barrós-Loscertales et al., 2006a; Cherbuin et al., 2008; Holmes et al., 2012; Levita et al., 2014), some methodological issues could account for these differences. One of them could be associated with the sample, as these previous studies were carried out with reduced sample sizes (Barrós-Loscertales et al., 2006a; Levita et al., 2014) or focused on a specific age range (middle-aged adults, who differ significantly from our sample; Cherbuin et al., 2008). Another methodological issue would be associated with the segmentation methods because, in contrast to our study, manual (Cherbuin et al., 2008) and automated (via FreeSurfer, Holmes et al., 2012; Levita et al., 2014) segmentations were applied to delimitate the amygdala and the hippocampus. Additionally, the study by Holmes et al. (2012) was carried out by taking a composite score of negative affect, which impedes the comparison with a direct single-scale score (i.e. SP). In addition, in the study by DeYoung et al. (2010), the authors did not find a positive association between the hippocampus and Neuroticism. In fact, they reported an inverse correlation between Neuroticism scores and hippocampal GM volume. Thus, further studies are needed to address the discrepancies about the involvement of the hippocampus and its anatomical correlates in behavioral inhibition. All in all, our results are in line with the last update of the RST and previous studies on the involvement of the amygdala in both fear and anxiety (McNaughton and Corr, 2004) and internalizing disorders.
The results have also shown a link between SR scores and reduced cortical volume in the lateral prefrontal cortex. The observed relationship was consistent with a recent study conducted in a large sample using impulsivity and sensation-seeking measures (Holmes et al., 2016) and with other studies that used the same self-reported measure of impulsivity (i.e. the Barratt Impulsivity Scale; Matsuo et al., 2009; Schilling et al., 2012; Cho et al., 2013). In addition, some studies have found that participants with lower GM volume in this region show a preference for immediate rewards on a delay-discounting measure (Bjork et al., 2009; Mohammadi et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2017). Therefore, less GM volume and cortical thickness in the lateral prefrontal area may predispose these people to worse cognitive control, which may determine a different probability of involvement in risky, goal-directed activities such as substance use (Holmes et al., 2016). Consistent with previous studies on impulsivity (Cho et al., 2013; Korponay et al., 2017), SR also correlated negatively with the volume in the medial prefrontal cortex, a brain area crucial for evaluating rewarding stimuli (Hayes et al., 2014; Hiser and Koenigs, 2018), especially in subjective value-based decision-making (Bartra et al., 2013; Clithero and Rangel, 2014; Acikalin et al., 2017). Moreover, this region has traditionally been established as a key region for emotion regulation [via top-down regulatory control of limbic areas (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004; Etkin et al., 2011; Kim et al., 2011)]. Hence, the pattern of GM associated with the reward sensitivity dimension found in our study suggests that individuals with an overactive BAS have worse emotional control of reward experiences, facilitating goal-directed behavior in risky situations or even engagement in more violent behaviors (Coccaro et al., 2018). Furthermore, these reductions in prefrontal areas have also been related to externalizing disorders, principally substance use disorders (see Yang et al., 2016, for a meta-analysis on alcohol; Kaag et al., 2018) or even behavioral addictions, such as Internet gambling disorder (Yao et al., 2017).
Contrary to our expectations, the association between striatal volume and SR scores was modulated by sex. The negative correlation between the volume of the striatum and SR scores was observed in males but not in females. This result verified our previous results in males (Barrós-Loscertales et al., 2006b) and clarified that this relationship does not extend to females. Interestingly, this differential pattern in males vs females in striatal areas was discussed in a recent paper (Caravaggio et al., 2017) in which impulsivity scores, measured by the TCI of Cloninger, correlated negatively with the striatal volume in males but not in females. Furthermore, a recent prospective study reported a correlation between striatal volume and Facebook use but noting that effect sizes were clearly higher in males than females (Montag et al., 2017). Regarding its clinical impact, smaller striatal volumes have been reported in a number of externalizing disorders associated with an overactive BAS, with a higher prevalence in males. Some of these disorders included cocaine addiction (Barrós-Loscertales et al., 2011; Moreno-López et al., 2012), alcoholism (Makris et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2016), nicotine addiction (Das et al., 2012), pornography consumption (Kühn and Gallinat, 2014) and ADHD (see Hoogman et al., 2017, for a mega-analysis).
The explanation of this sex effect is less clear. Animal research has linked differences in the dopaminergic function to sex-specific transcriptome profiles within the NAcc (Hodes et al., 2015), with females being more susceptible to stress cues (i.e. less prone to reward-related behaviors, in comparison with males). However, these different transcriptome profiles could be associated with complex genetic and/or hormonal interactions (Becker and Chartoff, 2019). In humans, the sex effect on the striatal volume has also been associated with different functioning of the striatal dopaminergic system (i.e. dopamine receptor availability; Caravaggio et al., 2017). Moreover, the volume of the NAcc has been associated with changes in testosterone levels during adolescence in males (Wierenga et al., 2018), coinciding in time with the maximum activity during processing of reward stimuli (Braams et al., 2015). Thus, although more research is needed, genetic, hormonal and developmental factors may contribute to sex effects on the relationship between striatum volume and reward sensitivity. In addition, we should also take into account that these differences could be influenced by sex-specific behaviors in variables related to health and lifestyle; thus, future studies should also address the neuropsychological impact of these variables. In sum, we found an association between SR scores and the GM volume in frontostriatal areas, pointing to the link between individual differences in personality traits and brain structures found to be relevant in the development of externalizing disorders.
Since 1970, RST has developed into a sophisticated model of emotion, motivation, personality, psychopathology and neuroscience. Overall, our study adds new evidence to previous reports linking brain structure and personality traits, revealing that the amygdala, the striatum (especially in males) and the prefrontal cortex are key regions associated with personality development. These results strengthen the role of the SPSRQ in measuring the main dimensions of the RST and the usefulness of this model as a good, validated framework with which to investigate the predisposition to psychopathology.
Finally, our study has some limitations. Although we reported significant brain correlates of RST in a large sample, surface-based metrics could account for more specific effects than VBM. Indeed, VBM is a product of cortical thickness and surface area; however, the two measures have been shown to be genetically dissociated (Panizzon et al., 2009). Thus, future research should address this issue in order to better comprehend the biological underpinnings of personality traits. Another limitation would be related to the SPSRQ because this questionnaire provides a unique, combined measure (the SP scale) of the BIS and FFFS. Although both systems are strongly associated with the punishment sensitivity dimension and share neuroanatomical structures (i.e. the amygdala), they are described as separate neuropsychological systems. Nonetheless, no agreement exists about how the BIS and FFFS can be psychometrically isolated. Hence, future studies are required to address this question because the factorization of the SP scale would provide more accurate measures of the BIS and FFFS.
Funding
This work was supported by grants from Generalitat Valenciana (PROMETEO/2017/109) and Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (PSI2016–78805-R) to C.A. Also, this work was supported by a pre-doctoral graduate program grant (National FPU to J.A-V.) and a post-doctoral graduate program grant (Juan de la Cierva to V.C.).
References
- Acikalin M.Y., Gorgolewski K.J., Poldrack R.A. (2017). A coordinate-based meta-analysis of overlaps in regional specialization and functional connectivity across subjective value and default mode networks. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 11, 1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aghajani M., Klapwijk E.T., Wee N.J., et al. (2016). Disorganized amygdala networks in conduct-disordered juvenile offenders with callous-unemotional traits. Biological Psychiatry, 82, 283–93 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ávila C., Torrubia R. (2008). Performance and conditioning studies In: Corr P.J., editor. The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality, New York: Cambridge University Press, 228–260. [Google Scholar]
- Bannerman D.M., Rawlins J.N.P., McHugh S.B., et al. (2004). Regional dissociations within the hippocampus—memory and anxiety. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 273–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barlow D.H. (1988). Anxiety and its Disorders: The Nature and Treatment of Anxiety Disorder, New York: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Barrós-Loscertales A., Garavan H., Bustamante J.C., et al. (2011). Reduced striatal volume in cocaine-dependent patients. NeuroImage, 56, 1021–1026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrós-Loscertales A., Meseguer V., Sanjuán A., et al. (2006a). Behavioral Inhibition System activity is associated with increased amygdala and hippocampal gray matter volume: a voxel-based morphometry study. NeuroImage, 33, 1011–1015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrós-Loscertales A., Meseguer V., Sanjuán A., et al. (2006b). Striatum gray matter reduction in males with an overactive behavioral activation system. European Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 2071–2074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartra O., McGuire J.T., Kable J.W. (2013). The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. NeuroImage, 76, 412–427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker J.B., Chartoff E. (2019). Sex differences in neural mechanisms mediating reward and addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44, 166–183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bijttebier P., Beck I., Claes L., et al. (2009). Gray’s Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory as a framework for research on personality–psychopathology associations. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 421–430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bjork J.M., Momenan R., Hommer D.W. (2009). Delay discounting correlates with proportional lateral frontal cortex volumes. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 710–713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair C. (2003). Behavioral inhibition and behavioral activation in young children: relations with self-regulation and adaptation to preschool in children attending Head Start. Developmental Psychobiology, 42, 301–311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braams B.R., Duijvenvoorde A.C.K., Peper J.S., et al. (2015). Longitudinal changes in adolescent risk-taking: a comprehensive study of neural responses to rewards, pubertal development, and risk-taking behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 7226–38 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bremner J.D., Narayan M., Anderson E.R., et al. (2000). Hippocampal volume reduction in major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 115–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryden M.P. (1977). Measuring handedness with questionnaires. Neuropsychologia, 15, 617–624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caravaggio F., Plitman E., Chung J.K., et al. (2017). Trait impulsiveness is related to smaller post-commissural putamen volumes in males but not females. European Journal of Neuroscience, 46, 2253–2264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caseras X., Àvila C., Torrubia R. (2003). The measurement of individual differences in Behavioural Inhibition and Behavioural Activation Systems: a comparison of personality scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 999–1013. [Google Scholar]
- Chen G., Cox R.W., Glen D.R., et al. (2019). A tail of two sides: artificially doubled false positive rates in neuroimaging due to the sidedness choice with t-tests. Human Brain Mapping, 40, 1037–1043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cherbuin N., Windsor T.D., Anstey K.J., et al. (2008). Hippocampal volume is positively associated with behavioural inhibition (BIS) in a large community-based sample of mid-life adults: the PATH through life study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3, 262–269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho S.S., Pellecchia G., Aminian K., et al. (2013). Morphometric correlation of impulsivity in medial prefrontal cortex. Brain Topography, 26, 479–487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clauss J.A., Seay A.L., VanDerKlok R.M., et al. (2014). Structural and functional bases of inhibited temperament. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 2049–2058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clithero J.A., Rangel A. (2014). Informatic parcellation of the network involved in the computation of subjective value. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 1289–1302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coccaro E.F., Cremers H., Fanning J., et al. (2018). Reduced frontal grey matter, life history of aggression, and underlying genetic influence. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 271, 126–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coccaro E.F., Fitzgerald D.A., Lee R., et al. (2016). Frontolimbic morphometric abnormalities in intermittent explosive disorder and aggression. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 1, 32–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohn M.D., Viding E., McCrory E., et al. (2016). Regional grey matter volume and concentration in at-risk adolescents: untangling associations with callous-unemotional traits and conduct disorder symptoms. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 254, 180–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corr P.J. (2008). Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST): introduction In: Corr P.J., editor. The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1–43. [Google Scholar]
- Corr P.J., McNaughton N. (2015). Neural mechanisms of low trait anxiety and risk for externalizing behavior In: Beauchaine P.J., Hinshaw S.P., editors. The Oxford Handbook of Externalizing Spectrum Disorders, New York: Oxford University Press, 220–238. [Google Scholar]
- Corr P.J., McNaughton N. (2012). Neuroscience and approach/avoidance personality traits: a two stage (valuation–motivation) approach. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 2339–2354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Bellis M.D., Casey B.J., Dahl R.E., et al. (2000). A pilot study of amygdala volumes in pediatric generalized anxiety disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 51–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Das D., Cherbuin N., Anstey K.J., et al. (2012). Lifetime cigarette smoking is associated with striatal volume measures. Addiction Biology, 17, 817–825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeYoung C.G., Hirsh J.B., Shane M.S., et al. (2010). Testing predictions from personality neuroscience. Brain structure and the big five. Psychological Science, 21, 820–828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etkin A., Egner T., Kalisch R. (2011). Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 85–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etkin A., Prater K.E., Schatzberg A.F., et al. (2009). Disrupted amygdalar subregion functional connectivity and evidence of a compensatory network in generalized anxiety disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 1361–1372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck H.J. (1960). Experiments in Personality, London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. [Google Scholar]
- Fanselow M.S., Dong H.-W. (2010). Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron, 65, 7–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fowles D.C. (1980). The three arousal model: implications of Gray’s two-factor learning theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity, and psychopathy. Psychophysiology, 17, 87–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franken I.H.A., Muris P., Georgieva I. (2006). Gray’s model of personality and addiction. Addictive Behaviors, 31, 399–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray J.A. (1982). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry Into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System, Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gray J.A. (1970). The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 8, 249–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray J.A., McNaughton N.J. (2000). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Inquiry Into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System, 2nd edn, Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton J.P., Siemer M., Gotlib I.H. (2008). Amygdala volume in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies. Molecular Psychiatry, 13, 993–1000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasboun D., Chantôme M., Zouaoui A., et al. (1996). MR determination of hippocampal volume: comparison of three methods. AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 17, 1091–1098. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes D.J., Duncan N.W., Xu J., et al. (2014). A comparison of neural responses to appetitive and aversive stimuli in humans and other mammals. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 45, 350–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hiser J., Koenigs M. (2018). The multifaceted role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion, decision making, social cognition, and psychopathology. Biological Psychiatry, 83, 638–647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodes G.E., Pfau M.L., Purushothaman I., et al. (2015). Sex differences in nucleus accumbens transcriptome profiles associated with susceptibility versus resilience to subchronic variable stress. Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 16362–16376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes A.J., Hollinshead M.O., Roffman J.L., et al. (2016). Individual differences in cognitive control circuit anatomy link sensation seeking, impulsivity, and substance use. Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 4038–4049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes A.J., Lee P.H., Hollinshead M.O., et al. (2012). Individual differences in amygdala-medial prefrontal anatomy link negative affect, impaired social functioning, and polygenic depression risk. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 18087–18100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoogman M., Bralten J., Hibar D.P., et al. (2017). Subcortical brain volume differences in participants with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adults: a cross-sectional mega-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry, 4, 310–319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howell B.R., Grand A.P., McCormack K.M., et al. (2014). Early adverse experience increases emotional reactivity in juvenile rhesus macaques: relation to amygdala volume. Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 1735–1746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iidaka T., Matsumoto A., Ozaki N., et al. (2006). Volume of left amygdala subregion predicted temperamental trait of harm avoidance in female young subjects. A voxel-based morphometry study. Brain Research, 1125, 85–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaag A.M., Schulte M.H.J., Jansen J.M., et al. (2018). The relation between gray matter volume and the use of alcohol, tobacco, cocaine and cannabis in male polysubstance users. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 187, 186–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim M.J., Loucks R.A., Palmer A.L., et al. (2011). The structural and functional connectivity of the amygdala: from normal emotion to pathological anxiety. Behavioural Brain Research, 223, 403–410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimbrel N.A., Nelson-Gray R.O., Mitchell J.T. (2007). Reinforcement sensitivity and maternal style as predictors of psychopathology. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1139–1149. [Google Scholar]
- Koelsch S., Skouras S., Jentschke S., et al. (2013). Neural correlates of emotional personality: a structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. PLoS One, 8, e77196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korponay C., Dentico D., Kral T., et al. (2017). Neurobiological correlates of impulsivity in healthy adults: lower prefrontal gray matter volume and spontaneous eye-blink rate but greater resting-state functional connectivity in basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical circuitry. NeuroImage, 157, 288–296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kühn S., Gallinat J. (2014). Brain structure and functional connectivity associated with pornography consumption. JAMA Psychiatry, 71, 827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lange C., Irle E. (2004). Enlarged amygdala volume and reduced hippocampal volume in young women with major depression. Psychological Medicine, 34, 1059–1064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lei X., Chen C., Xue F., et al. (2014). Fiber connectivity between the striatum and cortical and subcortical regions is associated with temperaments in Chinese males. NeuroImage, 89, 226–234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levita L., Bois C., Healey A., et al. (2014). The Behavioural Inhibition System, anxiety and hippocampal volume in a non-clinical population. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 4, 4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maack D.J., Tull M.T., Gratz K.L. (2012). Examining the incremental contribution of behavioral inhibition to generalized anxiety disorder relative to other Axis I disorders and cognitive-emotional vulnerabilities. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26, 689–695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makris N., Oscar-Berman M., Jaffin S.K., et al. (2008). Decreased volume of the brain reward system in alcoholism. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 192–202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsuo K., Nicoletti M., Nemoto K., et al. (2009). A voxel-based morphometry study of frontal gray matter correlates of impulsivity. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 1188–1195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNaughton N., Corr P. (2008). The neuropsychology of fear and anxiety: a foundation for Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory In: Corr P.J., editor. The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 44–94. [Google Scholar]
- McNaughton N., Corr P.J. (2004). A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 285–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell J.T., Nelson-Gray R.O. (2006). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder symptoms in adults: relationship to Gray’s Behavioral Approach System. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 749–760. [Google Scholar]
- Mohammadi B., Hammer A., Miedl S.F., et al. (2016). Intertemporal choice behavior is constrained by brain structure in healthy participants and pathological gamblers. Brain Structure and Function, 221, 3157–3170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montag C., Markowetz A., Blaszkiewicz K., et al. (2017). Facebook usage on smartphones and gray matter volume of the nucleus accumbens. Behavioural Brain Research, 329, 221–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreno-López L., Catena A., Fernández-Serrano M.J., et al. (2012). Trait impulsivity and prefrontal gray matter reductions in cocaine dependent individuals. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 125, 208–214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman J.P., MacCoon D.G., Vaughn L.J., et al. (2005). Validating a distinction between primary and secondary psychopathy with measures of Gray’s BIS and BAS constructs. Journal of abnormal psychology, 114, 319–323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman J.P., Wallace J.F. (1993). Diverse pathways to deficient self-regulation: implications for disinhibitory psychopathology in children. Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 699–720. [Google Scholar]
- Nigg J.T. (2001). Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychological bulletin, 127, 571–598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oldfield R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panizzon M.S., Fennema-Notestine C., Eyler L.T., et al. (2009). Distinct genetic influences on cortical surface area and cortical thickness. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2728–2735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pardini D.A., Raine A., Erickson K., et al. (2014). Lower amygdala volume in men is associated with childhood aggression, early psychopathic traits, and future violence. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 73–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlov I.P. (1941). Lectures on Conditioned Reflexes. Volume 2: Conditioned Reflexes and Psychiatry, London: Lawrence & Wishart. [Google Scholar]
- Perkins A.M., Kemp S.E., Corr P.J. (2007). Fear and anxiety as separable emotions: an investigation of the revised reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. Emotion, 7, 252–261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickering A.D., Gray J.A. (2001). Dopamine, appetitive reinforcement, and the neuropsychology of human learning: an individual differences approach In: Elias A., Angleitner A., editors. Advances in Individual Differences Research, Lengerich: PABST Science Publishers, 113–149. [Google Scholar]
- Pinto-Meza A., Caseras X., Soler J., et al. (2006). Behavioural Inhibition and Behavioural Activation Systems in current and recovered major depression participants. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 215–226. [Google Scholar]
- Qin S., Young C.B., Duan X., et al. (2014). Amygdala subregional structure and intrinsic functional connectivity predicts individual differences in anxiety during early childhood. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 892–900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quay H.C. (1988). The behavioral reward and inhibition systems in child behavior disorder In: Bloomingdale L.M., editor. Attention Deficit Disorder. Volume 3: New Research in Attention, Treatment, and Psychopharmacology, New York: Pergamon, 176–186. [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof K.R., Ullsperger M., Crone E.A., et al. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers J.C., De Brito S.A. (2016). Cortical and subcortical gray matter volume in youths with conduct problems. JAMA Psychiatry, 73, 64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schienle A., Ebner F., Schäfer A. (2011). Localized gray matter volume abnormalities in generalized anxiety disorder. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 261, 303–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schilling C., Kühn S., Romanowski A., et al. (2012). Cortical thickness correlates with impulsiveness in healthy adults. NeuroImage, 59, 824–830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sher K.J., Trull T.J. (1994). Personality and disinhibitory psychopathology: alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 92–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slobodskaya H.R. (2016). The contribution of reinforcement sensitivity to the personality-psychopathology hierarchical structure in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 1067–1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smoller J.W., Gallagher P.J., Duncan L.E., et al. (2014). The human ortholog of acid-sensing ion channel gene ASIC1a is associated with panic disorder and amygdala structure and function. Biological Psychiatry, 76, 902–910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tebartz van Elst L., Woermann F., Lemieux L., et al. (2000). Increased amygdala volumes in female and depressed humans. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroscience Letters, 281, 103–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torrubia R., Avila C., Caseras X. (2008). Reinforcement sensitivity scales In: Corr P.J., editor. The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality, New York: Cambridge University Press, 188–226. [Google Scholar]
- Torrubia R., Ávila C., Moltó J., et al. (2001). The Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray’s anxiety and impulsivity dimensions. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 837–862. [Google Scholar]
- Eijndhoven P., Wingen G., Oijen K., et al. (2009). Amygdala volume marks the acute state in the early course of depression. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 812–18 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vassilopoulou K., Papathanasiou M., Michopoulos I., et al. (2013). A magnetic resonance imaging study of hippocampal, amygdala and subgenual prefrontal cortex volumes in major depression subtypes: melancholic versus psychotic depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 146, 197–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vieira J.B., Ferreira-Santos F., Almeida P.R., et al. (2015). Psychopathic traits are associated with cortical and subcortical volume alterations in healthy individuals. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10, 1693–1704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Q., Chen C., Cai Y., et al. (2016). Dissociated neural substrates underlying impulsive choice and impulsive action. NeuroImage, 134, 540–549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang S., Kong F., Zhou M., et al. (2017). Brain structure linking delay discounting and academic performance. Human Brain Mapping, 38, 3917–3926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson C., Andermann F., Gloor P., et al. (1992). Anatomic basis of amygdaloid and hippocampal volume measurement by magnetic resonance imaging. Neurology, 42, 1743–1750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wierenga L.M., Bos M.G.N., Schreuders E., et al. (2018). Unraveling age, puberty and testosterone effects on subcortical brain development across adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 91, 105–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang X., Tian F., Zhang H., et al. (2016). Cortical and subcortical gray matter shrinkage in alcohol-use disorders: a voxel-based meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 66, 92–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang Y., Raine A., Narr K.L., et al. (2009). Localization of deformations within the amygdala in individuals with psychopathy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 986–994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yao Y.-W., Liu L., Ma S.-S., et al. (2017). Functional and structural neural alterations in Internet gaming disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 83, 313–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]