This 30-year follow-up study examines the association between kindergarten teacher-rated behavioral assessments with subsequent adulthood employment earnings at age 35 to 36 years.
Key Points
Question
Which disruptive behaviors in kindergarten are associated with employment earnings in adulthood for boys from low socioeconomic backgrounds?
Findings
This 30-year follow-up study of 920 boys found that kindergarten teachers’ ratings of inattention were associated with lower earnings at age 35 to 36 years and prosocial behavior with higher earnings after adjustment for child IQ and family adversity. Hyperactivity, opposition, and aggression were not associated with earnings.
Meaning
Preventive interventions targeting children’s inattention and/or limited prosocial behavior could have positive impacts on future employment earnings.
Abstract
Importance
Identifying early childhood behavioral problems associated with economic success/failure is essential for the development of targeted interventions that enhance economic prosperity through improved educational attainment and social integration.
Objective
To test the association between kindergarten teacher–rated assessments of inattention, hyperactivity, opposition, aggression, and prosociality in boys with their employment earnings at age 35 to 36 years as measured by government tax return data.
Design, Setting, and Participants
A 30-year prospective follow-up study analyzing low socioeconomic neighborhoods in Montreal, Quebec, Canada. Boys aged 5 to 6 years attending kindergarten in low socioeconomic neighborhoods were recruited. Teacher-rated behavioral assessments were obtained for 1040 boys. Data were collected from April 1984 to December 2015. Analysis began January 2017.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Mixed-effects linear regression models were used to examine the association between teacher ratings of inattention, hyperactivity, opposition, aggression, and prosociality at age 6 years and individual earnings obtained from government tax returns at age 35 to 36 years. The IQ of the child and family adversity were adjusted for in the analysis.
Results
Complete data were available for 920 study participants (mean age at follow-up was 36.3 years). Mean (SD) personal earnings at follow-up were $28 865.53 ($24 103.45) (range, $0-$142 267.84). A 1-unit increase in inattention (mean [SD], 2.66 [2.34]; range, 0-8) at age 6 years was associated with decrease in earnings at age 35 to 36 years of $1295.13 (95% CI, −$2051.65 to −$538.62), while a unit increase in prosociality (mean [SD], 8.0 [4.96]; range, 0-20) was associated with an increase in earnings of $406.15 (95% CI, $172.54-$639.77). Hyperactivity, opposition, and aggression were not significantly associated with earnings. Child IQ was associated with higher earnings and family adversity with lower earnings in all models. A 1-SD reduction in inattention at age 6 years was associated with a theoretical increase in annual earnings of $3040.41, a similar magnitude to an equivalent increase in IQ.
Conclusions and Relevance
Teacher ratings of inattention and prosociality in kindergarten boys from low socioeconomic neighborhoods are associated with earnings in adulthood after adjustment for hyperactivity, aggression, and opposition, which were not associated with earnings. Interventions beginning in kindergarten that target boys’ inattention and enhance prosociality could positively impact workforce integration and earnings.
Introduction
Childhood disruptive behaviors are among the most prevalent and costly mental health problems in industrialized countries and are associated with significant negative long-term outcomes for individuals and society.1,2,3,4 Recent evidence suggests that the presence of disruptive behavioral problems in the first years of life is an important early predictor of lower employment earnings in adulthood.5,6,7,8 Low earnings can harm individual and family well-being for many years and are associated with increased risk of financial dependence, stress, psychopathology, and early mortality.9,10,11,12,13 It is therefore essential to determine which individual behaviors contribute specifically to low earnings so that policy and preventive interventions can be used to target at-risk children with interventions and support.14,15,16
The longitudinal association between childhood traits and employment earnings is well documented,5,6,7,8,17,18,19 and both cognitive and noncognitive traits are believed to contribute to future earnings. The most frequently examined cognitive trait is intelligence, usually measured in terms of academic performance or IQ, which is strongly associated with occupational attainment, performance, and earnings.17,20,21,22,23 However, so-called noncognitive traits, such as self-control, self-esteem, and personality, are also known to be important.24 Interest in the role of noncognitive behavioral determinants of later earnings has grown recently on the back of evidence demonstrating that they are modifiable, perhaps more so than cognitive abilities, and therefore important targets for intervention.24,25,26
The longitudinal association between childhood disruptive behaviors and earnings in early adulthood is documented in a small but growing literature.5,6,7,8 Most studies have focused on the traits of aggression, opposition, hyperactivity, and inattention, as well as the related concept of self-control. Their findings are relatively consistent: higher levels of disruptive behaviors and lower levels of self-control, assessed when children are aged between 3 and 11 years, are associated with lower earnings and less wealth in early adulthood (age 26 to 36 years) after adjustment for the child’s intelligence and family socioeconomic status (SES).5,6,7,8,18,27 These disruptive behavioral traits are highly relevant from a research and policy point of view because they underpin some of the most prevalent1,2 and costly3,4 childhood psychiatric disorders, including conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Early interventions targeting clinical or subclinical disruptive behavioral problems therefore have the potential to yield wide-ranging social and economic benefits for individuals and society.26
The body of research documenting longitudinal associations between childhood disruptive behaviors and later earnings has important limitations. First, previous studies have used self-reported income. This methodology is subject to limitations including selective dropout (eg, individuals from low SES backgrounds), refusal to report income, social desirability bias (eg, overreporting or underreporting value of earnings), nondeliberate missreporting (eg, lack of knowledge about income or the value of welfare/insurance/childcare entitlements), and deliberate falsification. The use of government tax records is therefore preferable: they account for all sources of income (eg, salaries/wages, tax reductions, welfare credits), provide almost complete population coverage, and are usually reported by impartial third parties (eg, employers) and are consequently more accurate than self-reports, particularly for male individuals.28 Second, previous studies of childhood self-control18,27,29 and antisocial behavior5,6,7 have typically combined multiple behavioral dimensions (eg, inattention/hyperactivity, aggression/opposition) to create composite indices. The use of composite measures does not allow quantification of the cumulative or interactive contributions of behaviors, making it difficult to identify behaviors that can be targeted with preventive interventions. Identifying specific behaviors is important because it helps to specify causal pathways (ie, mediators) that can be used in targeted intervention programs, which are more effective and efficient than nontargeted generalized interventions.14 Third, several studies have averaged behavioral assessment across multiple years (eg, age 3 to 11 years),18,27 used assessments made in middle childhood (eg, age 10 years),5,6,8 or used parents’ rather than teachers’ ratings. Ideally, from the point of view of the development of preventive interventions, assessments should be made by teachers (they have a sense of normative behavior that parents often lack) at a single time point and as early as possible when children are most likely to benefit (ie, in kindergarten).30,31 These early assessments also provide the first opportunity for populationwide assessment that include children from low SES backgrounds who are less likely to attend daycare. Fourth, few previous studies have considered the effects of positive traits, such as prosociality, which could counteract the negative effects of disruptive behaviors and enhance earnings. Finally, previous studies have not examined effects in samples of male individuals from low SES neighborhoods. This group is at risk of both early behavioral problems and lower lifetime earnings, so early identification and intervention should yield high returns for individuals and society.
The aim of this study was to examine the association between 5 prevalent behaviors assessed in kindergarten—inattention, hyperactivity, aggression, opposition, and prosociality—and earnings averaged across age 35 and 36 years in a sample of boys from low SES neighborhoods. Behavioral assessments were based on teacher ratings at age 5 to 6 years, and earnings data were obtained from government tax return records. To test the possibility that some behaviors operate interactively rather than additively, 2-way interactions between all behaviors were examined.
Methods
Participants and Behavior Assessment
Teacher-rated behavioral assessments were obtained for boys aged 6 years (n = 1040) attending 53 schools in the poorest neighborhoods of Montreal, Quebec, Canada, using data from 1984.32 The study was approved by the University of Montreal ethics board. Informed written consent was obtained from children’s parents prior to participation. Data were collected from April 1984 to December 2015 and analyzed from January 2017 to May 2018.
Behavioral ratings were made by the child’s teacher using the well-validated Social Behavior Questionnaire.33,34,35 Inattention was assessed with 4 items: poor concentration, distracted, head in the clouds, and lack of persistence. Hyperactivity was assessed with 2 items: agitated/fidgety and moves constantly. Opposition was assessed with 5 items: disobeys, does not share materials, blames others, inconsiderate, and irritable. Physical aggression was assessed with 3 items: fights with other children, bullies/intimidates other children, and kicks/bites. Prosociality, defined as behaviors intended to benefit others or society including helping, sharing, and cooperating,36 was assessed with 10 items; examples of prosociality are tries to stop quarrels or disputes, will invite bystanders to join in a game, and will try to help someone who has been hurt. Items were rated on a 3-point scale with 0 indicating never/not true; 1, sometimes/somewhat true; and 2, often/very true. These scores were summed for each behavior. α Scores for inattention, hyperactivity, opposition, aggression, and prosociality were .81, .89, .84, .87, and .92, respectively.
Outcomes and Control Variables
Outcome data were obtained from government tax return records (Statistics Canada) and linked to participants’ study data (a description of the methods of linking administrative tax information with child-focused survey data has been previously published37). The successful linkage rate for participants with at least 1 tax return for 2014 or 2015 was 87.53%; there were no significant differences in baseline characteristics for cases with and without successful linkages (eTable 1 in the Supplement). Tax return data for each year included personal pretax earnings, marital status, and the number of children in the household. Earnings were defined as all pretax wages, salaries, and commissions, not including income from capital gains, and averaged for the 2 most recent years (2014 and 2015; r = 0.93). Currency data were collected as Canadian dollars but are reported as 2019 US dollars (CaD $1 = US $0.75) throughout this article.
Child IQ was assessed at age 13 years using the Sentence Completion Task.38 IQ is generally stable between childhood and adolescence39 so the assessment at age 13 years provides a good estimate of the child’s cognitive abilities at age 6 years. The correlation between the Sentence Completion Task at age 13 years and the vocabulary and block design subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Revised at age 10 years was 0.67 when conducted on a subsample of 80.
Family adversity was calculated by combining the following variables assessed at age 6 years: parents’ educational level, family structure (intact vs nonintact), parents’ age at birth of the first child, and parents’ occupational status based on the criteria described by Blishen et al.40 Families at or below the 30th percentile for each of these indices (or a nonintact family) were coded as having 1 adversity point. Scores were standardized on a scale of 0 to 1.
Statistical Analyses
After confirming the missing at random assumption, missing data were managed using multiple imputations by chained equations. Models were estimated across 50 data sets, and the results were pooled. Mixed-effects linear regression models were used to control for clustering effects in predictors at the school (n = 53) and classroom (n = 152) levels with fixed-effects estimates reported. Five multivariable partially adjusted models were estimated (1 for each behavior) controlling for child IQ and family adversity in each model. Next, a fully adjusted model including all behaviors, IQ, and adversity was estimated. Robust SEs were used to account for heteroskedasticity. Tests for 2-way continuous interactions between the 5 childhood behaviors were conducted in a single model. Behaviors and their interaction terms were centered prior to analysis. Effect sizes were calculated as the ratio of the coefficient estimate to the variable’s SD. Standardized effect sizes were calculated for the overall models from R2 values41 and interpreted using Cohen ƒ2, where ƒ2 ≥ 0.02, ƒ2 ≥ 0.15, and ƒ2 ≥ 0.35 represent small, medium, and large effect sizes, respectively.42 Finally, to estimate lost earnings over the course of a career, a financial effect was calculated:
in which βj is the estimate effect of variable j (eg, inattention) on earnings and stdj is the SD of variable j. The financial effect measures the present value over a 40-year work career of a 1-SD improvement in variable j assuming the commonly used discount rate of 3%. Thus, for inattention, financial effect measures the earnings gain from a 1-SD reduction, whereas for prosociality, it measures the earnings gain from a 1-SD increase.
Sensitivity Analyses
First, a variance inflation factor test of multicollinearity revealed that aggression and opposition were highly correlated; they approached but did not exceed the recommended threshold (variance inflation factor scores >5). As a precaution, these variables were standardized then summed to create a single item that was entered into the fully adjusted model. Second, a number of participants reported 0 earnings for the 2014 to 2015 tax years. To confirm that the results were not influenced by the presence of these zeros, a tobit regression with left censoring was used. Tobit regression simultaneously considers the likelihood of having data (any earnings greater than 0) and the value score for cases that have earnings greater than 0. Third, to test the robustness of associations between childhood behavior and earnings, we repeated the analysis using data from 2011 to 2012. Analyses were conducted using Stata version 14 (StataCorp). Significance was set at .05, and tests were 2 tailed.
Results
Participant Characteristics
Participants with complete outcome data were retained for analysis (N = 920). Child and family characteristics at age 6 years are shown in Table 1. There were no significant differences between participants with and without outcome data (eTable 1 in the Supplement).
Table 1. Personal and Family Characteristics of the Overall Sample vs Study Sample at Age 6 Years.
Characteristica | Mean (SD) [Range] |
---|---|
Child IQ | 9.0 (2.13) [0-13] |
Education, y | |
Mother | 10.5 (2.79) [2-24] |
Father | 10.6 (2.31) [1-24] |
Occupational prestige | |
Mother | 38.6 (12.10) [19.92-85.75] |
Father | 39.4 (12.86) [21.24-78.34] |
Age at birth of first child, y | |
Mother | 25.3 (4.66) [11.81-40.65] |
Father | 28.4 (5.58) [13.75-50.95] |
Intact family, No. (%) | 550 (80.0) |
Overall family adversity index | 0.3 (0.25) [0-1] |
Siblings, No. (%) | |
0 | 240 (26.8) |
1-2 | 600 (67.4) |
≥3 | 50 (5.8) |
Behavior at age 6 y | |
Inattention | 2.66 (2.34) [0-8] |
Hyperactivity | 1.36 (1.46) [0-4] |
Opposition | 2.47 (2.58) [0-10] |
Aggression | 1.29 (1.74) [0-6] |
Prosociality | 8.0 (4.96) [0-20] |
Up to 13.2% missing data, except for intact family, which had 26.0% missing data. In accordance with Statistics Canada data protection requirements, percentages are rounded to 1 decimal point, and counts are rounded to base 10.
Participant earnings and family circumstances at follow-up are presented in Table 2. The mean (SD) earnings at age 35 to 36 years was $28 865.53 ($24 103.45) (range, $0-$142 129.75). Earnings for each behavior, split by quartile, are shown in the Figure.
Table 2. Earnings and Family Characteristics at Follow-up.
Characteristica | Data |
---|---|
Age, y, mean (SD) | 36.29 (0.45) |
Employment earnings, $ | |
Mean (SD) | 28 865.53 (24 103.45) |
Median (IQR) | 27 180.48 (6959.96-44 397.23) |
Range | 0-142 129.75 |
Relationship status, No. (%) | |
Married/cohabiting | 520 (57.7) |
Divorced/separated/single | 380 (42.3) |
No. of children in household, No. (%) | |
0 | 330 (36.1) |
1-2 | 430 (48.6) |
>3 | 140 (15.3) |
Abbreviation: IQR, interquartile range.
Up to 3% missing data. In accordance with Statistics Canada data protection requirements, displayed counts are rounded to base 10 and percentages to 1 decimal point; earnings are rounded to the nearest hundred, and ranges represent the mean of the 5 lowest and 5 highest scores, respectively, and therefore represent a conservative estimate of the upper limit.
In partially adjusted models, which controlled for child IQ and family adversity only, all behaviors at age 6 years were associated with earnings at age 35 to 36 years at the P < .001 level. (Table 3). The fully adjusted model is shown in Table 4. A 1-unit increase in inattention at age 6 years was associated with decrease in earnings of $1295.13 (95% CI, −$2051.65 to −$538.62), while a unit increase in prosociality was associated with an increase in earnings of $406.15 (95% CI, $172.54-$639.77), holding all other variables constant. Hyperactivity did not pass the significance threshold, although it had an effect size equivalent to that of prosociality. In all models, IQ was associated with higher earnings and family adversity with lower earnings. No 2-way interactions were found. Bivariate correlations are presented in eTable 2 in the Supplement.
Table 3. Partially Adjusted Mixed-Effects Linear Regression Models of Association Between Behaviors at Age 6 Years and Earnings at Age 35 to 36 Years.
Variable | β (Robust SE), $a | 95% CI, $ | P Value | Effect Size | Cohen ƒ2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Inattention | −1838.31 (331.53) | −2488.11 to −1188.53 | .001 | 5718 | 0.105 |
IQ | 1474.37 (369.09) | 750.65 to 2198.09 | .001 | 4175 | |
Family adversity | −17 720.00 (3673.98) | −24 921.03 to −10 518.96 | .001 | 5889 | |
Hyperactivity | −2367.20 (509.45) | −3365.72 to −1368.68 | .001 | 4594 | 0.095 |
IQ | 1857.27 (335.38) | 1199.64 to 2514.91 | .001 | 5259 | |
Family adversity | −17 181.61 (3520.16) | −24 075.28 to −10 287.94 | .001 | 5710 | |
Opposition | −1097.64 (249.97) | −1587.56 to −607.70 | .001 | 3764 | 0.086 |
IQ | 1964.46 (334.64) | 1308.35 to 2620.70 | .001 | 5562 | |
Family adversity | −18 333.19 (3615.26) | −25 419.16 to −11 253.98 | .001 | 6093 | |
Aggression | −1255.27 (361.73 | −1964.25 to −546.29 | .001 | 2903 | 0.078 |
IQ | 1967.90 (330.40) | 1319.97 to 3292.89 | .001 | 5572 | |
Family adversity | −18 291.22 (3735.79) | −25 613.41 to −10 969.05 | .001 | 6079 | |
Prosociality | 560.02 (126.00) | 313.05 to 806.98 | .001 | 3692 | 0.091 |
IQ | 1848.26 (337.75) | 1185.96 to 2510.56 | .001 | 5233 | |
Family adversity | −18 181.67 (3645.25) | −25 326.50 to −11 036.83 | .001 | 6042 |
Unstandardized β coefficient for mixed-effects linear regression model, where a 1-unit change in the predictor (eg, inattention) is associated with a corresponding change in β, holding all other variables constant. Mean, SD, and range of behavioral and family characteristics at baseline are presented in Table 1.
Table 4. Mixed-Effects Linear Regression Models of Association Between Behaviors at Age 6 Years and Employment Earnings at Age 35 to 36 Years.
Variable | β (Robust SE), $a | 95% CI, $ | P Value | Effect Size |
---|---|---|---|---|
Inattention | −1295.13 (385.98) | −2051.65 to −538.62 | .001 | 4029 |
Hyperactivity | −1437.80 (764.52) | −2936.24 to 60.63 | .06 | 2790 |
Opposition | −204.35 (560.02) | −1301.96 to 893.26 | .72 | 701 |
Aggression | 408.36 (761.80) | −1084.74 to 1901.48 | .59 | 945 |
Prosociality | 406.15 (119.19) | 172.54 to 639.77 | .001 | 2678 |
Child IQ | 1360.67 (371.28) | 632.54 to 2088.79 | .001 | 3853 |
Family adversity | −15 687.57 (3742.60) | −23 023.04 to −8352.10 | .001 | 5213 |
Unstandardized β coefficient for mixed effects linear regression model, where a 1-unit change in the predictor (eg, inattention) is associated with a corresponding change in β, holding all other variables constant. Mean, SD, and range of behavioral and family characteristics at baseline are presented in Table 1. Effect size for full model: ƒ2 = 0.124.
Standardized effect sizes were largest for inattention followed by hyperactivity and prosociality. Viewed in monetary terms, a 1-SD reduction in inattention (SD, 2.34) was associated with an increase in annual earnings of $3040.41. Calculation of the financial effect for inattention over a 40-year career amounts to $70 532.97.
In the first sensitivity analysis, the merged opposition-aggression variable was nonsignificant (β = −162.17; P = .69; 95% CI, −633.96 to 958.31) and did not alter the overall model. Participants reporting no earnings in 2014 and 2015 (140 [15.2%]) had lower IQs, higher family adversity, and more behavioral problems (eTable 3 in the Supplement). Results from the tobit sensitivity analysis, which censored cases with no earnings, confirmed the results of the main analysis (eTable 4 in the Supplement). Replication of the analysis using 2011 to 2012 income data supported the robustness of the findings (eTable 5 in the Supplement).
Discussion
This study found that in a sample of boys living in low socioeconomic neighborhoods of a large North American city, behavioral assessments made by kindergarten teachers were associated with employment earnings 30 years later, as measured by government tax returns. Inattention was associated with lower earnings, while prosociality was associated with higher earnings after adjustment for the child’s IQ and family background. Hyperactivity, opposition, and aggression were not independently associated with earnings.
Our results are consistent with several previous studies showing that lower earnings are longitudinally associated with ratings of childhood attention,5,8,43 hyperactivity,7 and prosocial traits.19 Previous studies of the association between childhood opposition/aggression and earnings have produced mixed results. For instance, childhood aggression has been associated with lower SES in early adulthood,7 while antisocial behavior has been linked with lower earnings5,6 due largely to reduced rates of labor force participation. When only economically active men have been considered, however, 1 study6 found that childhood antisocial behavior/conduct problems were not associated with earnings, while another found that these men actually earned more.5 Our results help to clarify the contribution of distinct disruptive behaviors in early childhood by showing that while hyperactivity, opposition, and aggression are associated with later earnings on their own, these associations disappear once inattention and prosociality are adjusted for. They also highlight, for the first time to our knowledge, the positive association between childhood prosocial behaviors and later earnings. The failure to find significant interactions between behaviors is consistent with at least 1 previous study.5
Standardized effect sizes for individual behaviors and the overall model were in the small range. This was expected given that behavioral ratings were obtained from a single assessment rather than multiple assessments over multiple years, they were highly specific rather than composite scores of multiple behavioral dimensions, and the long duration of follow-up (30 years). The observed associations are therefore likely to represent an underestimation of the effect. Nevertheless, the effect size of individual behaviors (eg, inattention) on future earnings was of similar magnitude to that of IQ.
In monetary terms, the loss of employment earnings was nontrivial. A 1-SD reduction in inattention would be associated with an increase in annual earnings of $2963.18. Over a 40-year career, this would amount to $70 532.97. Since prosociality was associated with earnings after adjustment for inattention, a change in this behavior could further affect earnings. It is also highly likely that the harmful effects of childhood disruptive behaviors on earnings increase over time. This could occur through the accumulation of negative life events (eg, school failure, criminal convictions) that compound lost earnings, as well as through sectorial effects whereby people with low education and skills become trapped in job sectors with little or no wage growth.
Several plausible mechanisms may account for the associations observed in this study. There is already a well-established literature documenting the association between childhood disruptive behaviors and poor academic and educational attainment,44,45,46 and both are likely to mediate the association with employment earnings. Peer rejection, which is strongly associated with childhood symptoms of inattention-hyperactivity and low prosociality,47,48 could also function as an important mediator by lowering academic achievement and opportunities to learn and practice social skills.49 Other consequences of childhood disruptive behaviors, such as comorbid psychiatric problems in adulthood (eg, personality disorder, depression) or executive function deficits, which frequently cooccur with disruptive behaviors,50,51 could disrupt the ability to find and retain paid work and compromise workplace functioning, further diminishing earnings. Lower earnings could also be influenced by higher rates of substance use in adolescence and criminal convictions in early adulthood.52
Childhood inattention (which frequent cooccurs with hyperactivity) has been repeatedly linked with a wide range of negative long-term outcomes including higher rates of substance misuse, criminal conviction, educational failure, and unemployment,53 so targeting these behaviors could yield large returns across multiple life domains, including earnings. There are numerous interventions designed to target inattention-hyperactivity and low prosociality in children aged 5 to 8 years. The majority have been evaluated in terms of their impact on putative intermediary mechanisms, for example, education attainment, peer relationships, and executive function, described above, rather than on employment outcomes per se. Beyond the standard interventions, targeting inattention and hyperactivity symptoms (eg, pharmacologic and nonpharmacologic)54,55,56,57,58 and prosocial behaviors,59,60 educational interventions that improve academic outcomes should also be considered.
Strengths and Limitations
The strengths of this study were its long duration of follow-up (30 years), its use of objective measures of earnings (ie, government tax returns), its focus on a wider range of disruptive behaviors than previous studies, the use of teacher-rated behavioral assessments, the early age at which assessments were obtained (school entry), and the focus on male individuals from low SES backgrounds. However, this was an observational association study, and causal mechanisms underpinning the associations were not assessed. Future studies should investigate the causal pathways through which these associations occur to identify milestones that are important in the process linking early behavior problems to later earnings, such as educational attainment and social integration. This study examined earning as measured by government tax returns; it did not account for earnings obtained through the informal economy. Finally, this study focused on male individuals recruited from low-income neighborhoods of Montreal, which limits its generalizability. Future studies should examine these associations using low- and high-income neighborhood schools in other cultures and in larger population samples of female and male individuals.
Conclusions
Kindergarten teachers can identify boys from low socioeconomic backgrounds at risk of lower employment earnings 3 decades later. Monitoring of inattention and low prosocial behavior should begin in kindergarten so that at-risk boys can be identified and targeted with early intervention and support.
References
- 1.Christenson JD, Crane DR, Malloy J, Parker S. The cost of oppositional defiant disorder and disruptive behavior: a review of the literature. J Child Fam Stud. 2016;25(9):2649-2658. doi: 10.1007/s10826-016-0430-9 23021476 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(6):942-948. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Doshi JA, Hodgkins P, Kahle J, et al. . Economic impact of childhood and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the United States. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(10):990-1002. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Costello EJ, Mustillo S, Erkanli A, Keeler G, Angold A. Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(8):837-844. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Knapp M, King D, Healey A, Thomas C. Economic outcomes in adulthood and their associations with antisocial conduct, attention deficit and anxiety problems in childhood. J Ment Health Policy Econ. 2011;14(3):137-147. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Healey A, Knapp M, Farrington DP. Adult labour market implications of antisocial behaviour in childhood and adolescence: findings from a UK longitudinal study. Appl Econ. 2004;36(2):93-105. doi: 10.1080/0003684042000174001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Alatupa S, Pulkki-Råback L, Hintsanen M, Elovainio M, Mullola S, Keltikangas-Järvinen L. Disruptive behavior in childhood and socioeconomic position in adulthood: a prospective study over 27 years. Int J Public Health. 2013;58(2):247-256. doi: 10.1007/s00038-012-0408-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Feinstein L. The Relative Economic Importance of Academic, Psychological and Behavioural Attributes Developed on Childhood. London, UK: Centre for Economic Performance, London School of Economics and Political Science; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Paul KI, Moser K. Unemployment impairs mental health: meta-analyses. J Vocat Behav. 2009;74(3):264-282. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2009.01.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jin RL, Shah CP, Svoboda TJ. The impact of unemployment on health: a review of the evidence. CMAJ. 1995;153(5):529-540. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Roelfs DJ, Shor E, Davidson KW, Schwartz JE. Losing life and livelihood: a systematic review and meta-analysis of unemployment and all-cause mortality. Soc Sci Med. 2011;72(6):840-854. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.01.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Chetty R, Stepner M, Abraham S, et al. . The association between income and life expectancy in the United States, 2001-2014. JAMA. 2016;315(16):1750-1766. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.4226 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Santiago CD, Wadsworth ME, Stump J. Socioeconomic status, neighborhood disadvantage, and poverty-related stress: prospective effects on psychological syndromes among diverse low-income families. J Econ Psychol. 2011;32(2):218-230. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2009.10.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Heckman JJ. Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science. 2006;312(5782):1900-1902. doi: 10.1126/science.1128898 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kautz T, Heckman JJ, Diris R, ter Weel B, Borghans L. Fostering and Measuring Skills: Improving Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Skills to Promote Lifetime Success. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2014. doi: 10.3386/w20749 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Campbell F, Conti G, Heckman JJ, et al. . Early childhood investments substantially boost adult health. Science. 2014;343(6178):1478-1485. doi: 10.1126/science.1248429 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Ridder EM. Show me the child at seven II: childhood intelligence and later outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(8):850-858. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01472.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, et al. . A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(7):2693-2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Jones DE, Greenberg M, Crowley M. Early social-emotional functioning and public health: the relationship between kindergarten social competence and future wellness. Am J Public Health. 2015;105(11):2283-2290. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2015.302630 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Furnham A, Cheng H. Factors influencing adult earnings: findings from a nationally representative sample. J Socio-Econ. 2013;44(suppl C):120-125. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2013.02.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hanushek EA, Woessmann L. The role of cognitive skills in economic development. J Econ Lit. 2008;46(3):607-668. doi: 10.1257/jel.46.3.607 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Schmidt FL, Hunter J. General mental ability in the world of work: occupational attainment and job performance. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;86(1):162-173. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Schmidt FL. Select on Intelligence In: Locke EA, ed. Handbook of Principles of Organizational Behavior. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 2012:1-17. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Heckman JJ, Stixrud J, Urzua S. The Effects of cognitive and noncognitive abilities on labor market outcomes and social behavior. J Labor Econ. 2006;24(3):411-482. doi: 10.1086/504455 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Almlund M, Duckworth AL, Heckman JJ, Kautz TD. Personality Psychology and Economics. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2011. doi: 10.3386/w16822 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Heckman J, Pinto R, Savelyev P. Understanding the mechanisms through which an influential early childhood program boosted adult outcomes. Am Econ Rev. 2013;103(6):2052-2086. doi: 10.1257/aer.103.6.2052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fergusson DM, Boden JM, Horwood LJ. Childhood self-control and adult outcomes: results from a 30-year longitudinal study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(7):709-7171. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bound J, Krueger AB. The extent of measurement error in longitudinal earnings data: do two wrongs make a right? J Labor Econ. 1991;9(1):1-24. doi: 10.1086/298256 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Converse PD, Piccone KA, Tocci MC. Childhood self-control, adolescent behavior, and career success. Pers Individ Dif. 2014;59(suppl C):65-70. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Boisjoli R, Vitaro F, Lacourse E, Barker ED, Tremblay RE. Impact and clinical significance of a preventive intervention for disruptive boys: 15-year follow-up. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;191:415-419. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.030007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Castellanos-Ryan N, Brière FN, O’Leary-Barrett M, et al. ; IMAGEN Consortium . The structure of psychopathology in adolescence and its common personality and cognitive correlates. J Abnorm Psychol. 2016;125(8):1039-1052. doi: 10.1037/abn0000193 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Nagin D, Tremblay RE. Trajectories of boys’ physical aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physically violent and nonviolent juvenile delinquency. Child Dev. 1999;70(5):1181-1196. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Tremblay RE, Loeber R, Gagnon C, Charlebois P, Larivée S, LeBlanc M. Disruptive boys with stable and unstable high fighting behavior patterns during junior elementary school. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1991;19(3):285-300. doi: 10.1007/BF00911232 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Swanson JM, Arnold LE, Molina BSG, et al. ; MTA Cooperative Group . Young adult outcomes in the follow-up of the multimodal treatment study of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: symptom persistence, source discrepancy, and height suppression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(6):663-678. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12684 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Murray AL, Obsuth I, Eisner M, Ribeaud D. Evaluating longitudinal invariance in dimensions of mental health across adolescence: an analysis of the social behavior questionnaire. Assessment. 2017;1073191117721741. doi: 10.1177/1073191117721741 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Weir K, Stevenson J, Graham P. Behavioral deviance and teacher ratings of prosocial behavior: preliminary findings. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1980;19(1):68-77. doi: 10.1016/S0002-7138(09)60653-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Findlay LC, Beasley E, Park J, et al. . Longitudinal child data: what can be gained by linking administrative data and cohort data? Int J Popul Data Sci. 2018;3(21). doi: 10.23889/ijpds.v3i1.451 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Lorge I, Thorndike R. The Lorge-Thorndike Intelligence Test. New York, NY: Houghton; 1950. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Mackintosh NJ. IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Blishen BR, Carroll WK, Moore C. The 1981 socioeconomic index for occupations in Canada. Can Rev Sociol. 1987;24(4):465-488. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-618X.1987.tb00639.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Selya AS, Rose JS, Dierker LC, Hedeker D, Mermelstein RJ. A practical guide to calculating Cohen’s f2, a measure of local effect size, from PROC MIXED. Front Psychol. 2012;3:111. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for Behavioral Science. 2nd ed Hilsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Fletcher J. The Effects of Childhood ADHD on Adult Labor Market Outcomes. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2013. doi: 10.3386/w18689 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Loe IM, Feldman HM. Academic and educational outcomes of children with ADHD. Ambul Pediatr. 2007;7(1)(suppl):82-90. doi: 10.1016/j.ambp.2006.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Pingault J-B, Tremblay RE, Vitaro F, et al. . Childhood trajectories of inattention and hyperactivity and prediction of educational attainment in early adulthood: a 16-year longitudinal population-based study. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(11):1164-1170. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10121732 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Burke JD, Rowe R, Boylan K. Functional outcomes of child and adolescent oppositional defiant disorder symptoms in young adult men. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(3):264-272. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Mrug S, Molina BSG, Hoza B, et al. . Peer rejection and friendships in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: contributions to long-term outcomes. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2012;40(6):1013-1026. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9610-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Asher S, Coie J.. Peer Rejection in Childhood. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bierman K. Peer Rejection: Developmental Processes and Intervention Strategies. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Barkley RA, Murphy KR. Impairment in occupational functioning and adult ADHD: the predictive utility of executive function (EF) ratings versus EF tests. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2010;25(3):157-173. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acq014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Clarke TL. Executive Functions and Overt/Covert Patterns of Conduct Disorder Symptoms in Children With ADHD [dissertation]. College Park, Maryland: University of Maryland; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Fletcher J, Wolfe B. Long-term consequences of childhood ADHD on criminal activities. J Ment Health Policy Econ. 2009;12(3):119-138. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Shaw M, Hodgkins P, Caci H, et al. . A systematic review and analysis of long-term outcomes in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: effects of treatment and non-treatment. BMC Med. 2012;10:99. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-10-99 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Molina BSG, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, et al. ; MTA Cooperative Group . The MTA at 8 years: prospective follow-up of children treated for combined-type ADHD in a multisite study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(5):484-500. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819c23d0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.The MTA Cooperative Group. Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(12):1073-1086. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333(6045):959-964. doi: 10.1126/science.1204529 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Wyman PA, Cross W, Hendricks Brown C, Yu Q, Tu X, Eberly S. Intervention to strengthen emotional self-regulation in children with emerging mental health problems: proximal impact on school behavior. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2010;38(5):707-720. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9398-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Parker J, Wales G, Chalhoub N, Harpin V. The long-term outcomes of interventions for the management of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2013;6:87-99. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S49114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Durlak JA, Weissberg RP, Dymnicki AB, Taylor RD, Schellinger KB. The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: a meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Dev. 2011;82(1):405-432. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Durlak JA, Weissberg RP, Pachan M. A meta-analysis of after-school programs that seek to promote personal and social skills in children and adolescents. Am J Community Psychol. 2010;45(3-4):294-309. doi: 10.1007/s10464-010-9300-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.