Abstract
There is widespread interest in temperament and its impact upon cognitive and academic outcomes. Parents adjust their parenting according to their child’s temperament, however, few studies have accounted for parenting while estimating the association between temperament and academic outcomes. We examined the associations between temperament (2–3 years) and cognitive and academic outcomes (6–7 years) when mediation by parenting practices (4–5 years) was held constant, by estimating the controlled direct effect. Participants were from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (n = 5107). Cognitive abilities were measured by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (verbal) and the Matrix Reasoning test (non-verbal). Literacy and numeracy were reported by teachers using the Academic Rating Scale. Mothers reported children’s temperament using the Short Temperament Scale for Toddlers (subscales: reactivity, approach, and persistence). Parenting practices included items about engagement in activities with children. Marginal structural models with inverse probability of treatment weights were used to estimate the controlled direct effect of temperament, when setting parenting to the mean. All temperament subscales were associated with cognitive abilities, with persistence showing the largest associations with verbal (PPVT; β = 0.58; 95%CI 0.27, 0.89) and non-verbal (Matrix Reasoning: β = 0.19; 0.02, 0.34) abilities. Higher persistence was associated with better literacy (β = 0.08; 0.03, 0.13) and numeracy (β = 0.08; 0.03, 0.13), and higher reactivity with lower literacy (β = -0.08; -0.11, -0.05) and numeracy (β = -0.07; -0.10, -0.04). There was little evidence that temperamental approach influenced literacy or numeracy. Overall, temperament had small associations with cognitive and academic outcomes after accounting for parenting and confounders.
Introduction
There is widespread interest in whether children’s temperament influences their cognitive and academic outcomes [1, 2]. Temperament is one of many concepts captured under the phrase ‘non-cognitive’ skills, which are believed to improve human capital through improved cognitive, academic, social and health outcomes. Temperament is the individual characteristics in behavioral styles that are biologically-based, but also shaped by experiences and environment [3]. Three aspects of temperament thought to impact learning and cognition are reactivity, persistence and approach [4]. Reactivity encompasses a child’s emotional intensity and volatility [4]. For instance, a child who regularly shows emotional irritability is considered as having high emotional reactivity. Persistence reflects the ability to stay ‘on task’[5] despite distractions or difficulties. Temperamental approach is the degree of comfort experienced when encountering new situations or people [4]. For example, a child who withdrew or was wary to a novel environment is considered as having low temperamental approach. These three aspects of temperament have potential to influence children’s cognitive abilities and academic achievement. In our recent systematic review and meta-analysis of non-cognitive abilities, we reported that there were very few high quality studies relating various measures of temperament to either cognition or academic achievement [6].
With respect to cognition, the overall concept of temperament has been associated with cognitive outcomes in some [7, 8] but not all [9] studies. This suggests that certain components of temperament may have specificity for cognitive outcomes. Components of temperament such as higher persistence have been linked to better cognitive outcomes among American children [10]. There is also some indication that effects are consistent across cultural settings, where measures of persistence have been linked to better development among Japanese children [11].
For academic achievement, aspects of temperament such as high emotional reactivity may interfere with the child’s learning processes [1, 12]. For example, a highly reactive child may become easily frustrated and find it difficult to learn [13]. Whereas temperamental traits such as persistence are more likely to help a child stay on task and maintain their attention despite distractions [4], which has benefits for learning [14]. Low approach may present challenges for young children in the transition to school as they are faced with many new situations, teachers and peers [15].
Reactivity and persistence are two aspects of temperament that are linked to a child’s self-regulation skills. Self-regulation refers to the ability to regulate emotions, manage behaviors, focus attention in the face of distraction and to be able to persist at a task [13]. This definition highlights the interconnectedness of temperament, self-regulation, executive functioning (i.e. the cognitive control of one’s behaviors in order to achieve a goal) and attentional control. It is through these links that temperamental reactivity and persistence are likely to affect cognitive ability and academic achievement [16]. Previous research has shown that self-regulation skills in general and emotional reactivity and persistence specifically have been linked to cognition and academic achievement [13, 17, 18]. For instance, a review by Blair & Raver [17] showed that dimensions of self-regulation including emotional regulation and executive function are important skills needed for school readiness and early academic achievement. West, Denton, Germino-Hausken [18] found that teacher-rated children’s task persistence is related to reading, mathematics, and general knowledge in 22,000 American kindergarten students. While there may be a direct effect of temperament on child cognitive and academic outcomes, children’s temperament may also influence the types of parenting they received. For instance, a study of 35 pairs of mother-child showed that mothers displayed more negative and non-accepting behaviors to children with low attention span [19]. In addition, parents may also engage in fewer playing or reading activities with a child who shows emotional distress, and this could in turn influences the child’s cognitive and academic outcomes [20]. Hence the importance of considering parenting when evaluating whether aspects of temperament are associated with children’s outcomes.
Temperamental reactivity and persistence are modifiable [21, 22]. For instance, a cluster-randomized trial showed that an intervention to develop children’s persistence, attention, and impulse control resulted in improvements to academic outcomes [22]. Increasing children’s persistence and reducing their reactivity may be a mechanism to improve children’s cognitive and academic outcomes, provided that there are direct effects of temperament on these outcomes [21–23].
To estimate the direct effect of temperament, we need to account for the fact that children’s temperament may influence parenting [24, 25], which in turn, is known to influence children’s cognitive and academic outcomes [26]. Maccoby et al [25] showed that temperamentally difficult children (i.e. high emotional intensity, difficult to calm) received less teaching from parents at 18 months than temperamentally easy children. However, Dixon et al [24] found mothers engaged in more high quality play with temperamentally difficult than easy children. Parental engagement in play and cognitive stimulation activities has positive impacts on children’s outcomes [26], and differential parental engagement for temperamentally easy and difficult children might, in part, explain effects of temperament on cognitive and academic outcomes.
While some studies have examined the direct effect of temperament on cognitive and academic outcomes, most involve limited adjustment for confounding [2, 12, 27]. The few studies that accounted for parenting suggest an association between temperament and outcomes [20, 28, 29]. However, simple adjustment for parenting practices could introduce bias when parenting practices are affected by temperament (parenting is a mediator) and when there are confounders of parenting and outcomes (mediator-outcome confounding; S1 File) [30]. In the current study, we use traditional linear regression models to examine the total effect of temperament (reactivity, approach, persistence) at 2–3 years on cognitive and academic outcomes at 6–7 years, simply to compare results with past studies. In our main findings, we use marginal structural models (MSMs) to estimate what are called “controlled direct effects” (CDEs) [30] of temperament on cognitive and academic outcomes, while accounting for parenting practices at 4–5 years. Although these estimates are called controlled direct effects in the epidemiological literature, where interpretation is required, these estimates are referred to as associations. As the focus of our research is the association between temperament and cognitive and academic outcomes, rather than the mediated effect through parenting, the CDE is the most appropriate estimate to use. MSMs adjust for confounding using inverse probability of treatment weighting [31]. MSMs allow control of parenting by setting it to some uniform value, which in turn, enables the estimation of the ‘controlled’ direct effect of temperament on outcomes. Specifically, parenting is not the variable of interest, we need to account for it but it is not part of the effect we want to estimate. Another advantage of MSMs over traditional methods is weighting for mediator-outcome confounding, as this does not involve statistical adjustment for the intermediate variable, which could introduce bias (S1 File) [32].
Methods
Study design and sample
Data were from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC). LSAC is a population-based study that commenced in 2004. Participants were recruited using a two-stage clustered sampling process [33]. At commencement, 5107 infants (mean age 8.8 months) were recruited and followed-up at 2–3 (n = 4606), 4–5 (n = 4386), and 6–7 (n = 4242) years. LSAC is considered broadly representative of Australian children [33].
Ethical approval and consent
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. LSAC was approved by the Australian Institute of Family Studies ethics committee. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants’ caregivers.
Data
The authors do not have the authority to provide data to researchers. However, de-identified data used in the current study from LSAC is accessible to bona fide researchers by application. Further information on how to access the data is available at the following webpage (https://growingupinaustralia.gov.au/data-and-documentation/accessing-lsac-data).
Cognitive ability and academic achievement (Y)
Verbal ability (receptive vocabulary) was measured using an adapted Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT). The adapted PPVT-III [34] was administered by a trained interviewer to children aged 6–7 years during home interviews. The child pointed to the picture that best represented the meaning of a word spoken by the examiner [35]. The adapted PPVT-III was comparable to the full PPVT-III (correlations ranging 0.93–0.97) with high internal consistency (person-separation reliability 0.76) [34]. Scale scores were created using Rasch modelling (Mean = 64, standard deviation (SD) = 8) [34].
Non-verbal ability (fluid reasoning) was measured using the Matrix Reasoning test from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th edition [36]. The Matrix Reasoning test comprised 35 items. The child was presented with an incomplete set of diagrams and asked to select the picture that completes the set from 5 different options. Scores were reported as standard scores, from age-appropriate norms (mean = 10, SD = 3). High internal consistency of the Matrix Reasoning test has been established in normative samples of Australian children (Cronbach’s α = 0.88 for 6-year-olds, 0.91 for 7-year-olds) [36].
Academic achievement at 6–7 years was measured using the adapted Academic Rating Scale (ARS) [37], which has two subscales: literacy (10 items) and numeracy (9 items). Teachers rated the child’s skills and knowledge in relation to other children of the same age from ‘not yet demonstrated skill’ to ‘demonstrates skill competently and consistently’. Examples of literacy items included ‘reads books fluently’ and ‘writes sentences with more than one clause’. Numeracy items included ‘uses a variety of strategies to solve math problems’ and ‘makes reasonable estimates of quantities’. Total scores ranging from 1 to 5 were created using Rasch modelling. Higher scores indicate higher proficiency. The ARS has high internal reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.96 for literacy, 0.95 for numeracy) [38].
Temperament (X)
Temperament was measured at 2–3 years using the Short Temperament Scale for Toddlers (STST) [39]. The STST was adapted from the Toddler Temperament Scale [40]. The STST consists of 3 subscales (4 items each): reactivity, approach, and persistence, rated by the primary caregiver (98.2% mothers) from 1 (almost never) to 6 (almost always). Average scores were calculated for each subscale. Higher scores indicate higher reactivity (more negative emotion), higher approach (lower shyness), and higher persistence. All subscales had acceptable internal reliability in the current sample (α = 0.76 for approach, 0.68 for reactivity and 0.75 for persistence).
Parenting (M)
Parenting was assessed at 4–5 years using the home activities index which contains 7 items measuring how often mothers read to the child, tell stories, draw pictures, play indoor games, outdoor games, music, and involve the child in activities such as cooking or pet care. These items have been used as indicators of the quality of home environment, such as frequency of parent-child activities in UNICEF surveys [41]. Items were rated from 0 (none) to 3 (everyday), and a total score (0–21) derived by summing item scores (internal reliability α = 0.71). Higher scores indicates more positive parenting practices.
Confounders of the association between temperament and outcomes (C)
Factors that might confound the associations between temperament and cognitive and academic outcomes were decided a priori using a directed acyclic graph (S2 File). Confounders included indicators of socioeconomic position, intrauterine, child, maternal and family factors. These confounders were reported by mothers when children were 0–1 year. Details about how these confounders were measured are included in S2 File.
Confounders of the association between parenting practices and outcomes (L)
To estimate the CDEs, we need to account for confounding associated with parenting and cognitive or academic outcomes [30]. This set of confounders were reported by mothers at ages 4–5 years and included variables that were affected by temperament and in turn confound the parenting-outcome association (maternal psychological distress, number of siblings, maternal working status, household income, and financial hardship).
Analysis
MSMs are recommended for overcoming the limitations of standard regression models. They allow estimation of the direct effect in the presence of mediators and confounders of mediator-outcome associations, and allow interaction between the exposure and mediator[30]. The MSM differs from standard regression models because the MSM is a model for potential outcomes rather than observed outcomes [30, 42]. A potential outcome is the outcome that an individual would have had under some different level of the exposure. If the observed and potential outcomes differ, it is assumed that all other things being equal, the exposure has caused the difference in outcome. However, it is usually only possible to observe one outcome for an individual and therefore the potential outcome is not observed for that same individual. The potential outcome is also referred to as the counterfactual outcome when it is different from the observed value. This terminology is widely used in the field of causal inference, within the discipline of epidemiology.
The ‘marginal’ in the name of the marginal structural model refers to the fact that the estimated effects are marginal, not conditional as in standard regression. Theoretically, the marginal effect is the difference in outcome (Y) when the exposure X is set to a level x and a counterfactual level x* for each individual. In this analysis we used weighted regression to estimate the average of the differences in the potential outcomes to derive the direct effect. In the current study we are interested in the direct effect of temperament and not the mediated effect. A key reason for using the MSM in this instance is that it correctly accounts for mediation (by parenting) leading to less biased estimates of the association between temperament and cognitive and academic outcomes, than would be generated using standard regression analyses (S1 File).
We tested three models. Model 1 estimated the total effect of temperamental reactivity, approach or persistence (X) on cognitive and academic outcomes (Y) after adjusting for confounders (C) using standard linear regression. Model 2 included further adjustment for mediators (M) and confounders (C, L) in standard linear regression. Most studies have used standard regression models 1 and 2, and these are provided for comparison with previous work. However, our a priori primary analysis (model 3) involved MSM. Using MSMs, we estimated the CDEs of temperament (reactivity, approach, or persistence, X), on cognitive and academic outcomes (Y) after accounting for parenting practices (M), potential confounders of the association between temperament and cognitive and academic outcomes (C), and confounders of parenting practices and outcomes (L). As described above the standard regression estimates the conditional effect, whereas the MSM weighted regression estimates the marginal effect of temperament on cognitive and academic outcomes. The marginal effect is the difference in cognitive and academic outcomes under the observed (x) and counterfactual exposures (x*) of each individual. The mediator M (parenting) is set to a uniform level of m, such that the associations between temperament and outcomes are not mediated by parenting.[30] We used the log-likelihood ratio to test for interactions between temperament and parenting. As no interactions were found, the CDE generates the same result regardless of the level at which the mediator is set. Therefore, we set the mediator to its mean value (m). The CDEs were estimated from linear regression models of the form:
(1) |
Potential confounding was accounted by fitting the model above with stabilized inverse probability weights of the form , where
(2) |
and
(3) |
The weight accounted for the confounding of the association between temperament and cognitive and academic outcomes by conditioning on C. The weight for the mediator accounted for confounding of the association between parenting and cognitive and academic outcomes by conditioning on X, C, and L. Since the mediator was a continuous variable, probabilities were taken from the density functions. For computing the probabilities we assume that the mediator is normally distributed and hence used the normal density function. In the normal density function, parameters used to obtain probabilities were the observed mediator value, the predicted means and the root mean square error, estimated from linear regression [30]. Weights were truncated at the 1st and 99th percentile to deal with outliers (S1 File). MSMs were performed separately for temperament reactivity, approach, and persistence. Effect estimates were reported as unstandardized β coefficients and in SD units, which are calculated by dividing the unstandardized β by the SD of the imputed sample.
Associations from standard regressions and MSMs were estimated under the assumption that there was no unmeasured confounding between the outcome and exposure, or between the mediator and the outcome. However, as these assumptions are unverifiable we performed sensitivity analysis to determine the extent to which an unmeasured confounder U might affect the association between temperament and cognitive and academic outcomes. We estimated the bias for the CDEs under conditions varying in prevalence and effect size of U (S3 File) [43].
We tested sex by exposure interactions. As no significant interactions were found, analyses were conducted on the full sample.
Analyses were performed using STATA 13.0 (StataCorp, College Station, TX).
Multiple imputation
A non-monotonic pattern of missingness was observed. We generated twenty imputed datasets under the missing at random assumption [44]. The imputation model included temperament, parenting practices, cognitive and academic outcomes, confounding variables and auxiliary variables that predicted missingness (parenting self-efficacy, temperament sociability, persistence, and reactivity at 4–5 years). We also performed analyses on the sample with observed outcome and the results were similar to the imputed sample. Results from the imputed sample (n = 5107) are reported.
Results
Table 1 shows the characteristics of LSAC response, complete case (n = 1647) and imputed (n = 5107) samples. The highest proportion of missing data was teacher-reported outcomes of literacy and numeracy, although data from over 3300 children were available for these outcomes. Characteristics of the imputed sample were similar to the response sample.
Table 1. Characteristics of response, complete case, and imputed samplesa.
Response sampleb | Complete case samplec, n = 1647 | Imputed sample, n = 5107 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
N | M (SD) or % | M (SD) or % | M (SD) or % | |
Outcomes, Y (Cognitive ability and academic achievement) | ||||
PPVT | 4185 | 74.4 (5.2) | 75.2 (4.9) | 74.2 (5.2) |
Matrix reasoning | 4180 | 10.7 (3.0) | 11.0 (3.0) | 10.7 (3.0) |
ARS-Literacy | 3408 | 3.4 (0.8) | 3.5 (0.7) | 3.4 (0.8) |
ARS-Numeracy | 3357 | 3.3 (0.8) | 3.5 (0.8) | 3.3 (0.8) |
Exposures, X (Temperament) | ||||
Reactivity | 3530 | 3.0 (1.0) | 2.9 (0.9) | 3.0 (0.9) |
Approach | 3533 | 3.9 (1.0) | 4.0 (1.0) | 3.9 (1.0) |
Persistence | 3532 | 4.3 (0.7) | 4.3 (0.7) | 4.3 (0.7) |
Intermediate variable, M | ||||
Parenting practices | 4385 | 11.8 (3.9) | 12.0 (3.8) | 11.7 (3.9) |
Confounders of X-M or X-Y | ||||
Mother’s highest education, % | ||||
Tertiary | 1677 | 32.9 | 41.4 | 32.9 |
Diploma/certificate | 1766 | 34.6 | 34.2 | 34.6 |
Schooling only | 1656 | 32.5 | 24.4 | 32.5 |
Hardship score | 5089 | 0.9 (1.3) | 0.7 (1.1) | 0.9 (1.3) |
Housing tenure, rented or other, % | 5100 | 35.8 | 23.0 | 35.8 |
Aboriginal or Torres Straits Islander, % | 5107 | 4.5 | 2.1 | 4.5 |
Index of relative socio-economic disadvantage (IRSD) | 5107 | 1008.8 (60.2) | 1017.2 (57.2) | 1008.8 (60.2) |
Child is male, % | 5107 | 51.1 | 48.5 | 51.1 |
Birthweight for gestational age z-score | 4999 | 0.0 (1.1) | 0.1 (1.0) | 0.0 (1.1) |
Duration of breast feeding, % | ||||
Never breastfed | 420 | 9.2 | 5.8 | 9.2 |
<1 month | 518 | 11.4 | 8.4 | 11.4 |
<3 months | 473 | 10.4 | 8.7 | 10.3 |
<6 months | 692 | 15.2 | 14.0 | 15.2 |
6 months or more | 2461 | 53.9 | 63.2 | 54.0 |
Maternal age | 5106 | 31.0 (5.5) | 32.2 (4.6) | 31.0 (5.5) |
Mother is born in Australia, % | 4997 | 80.0 | 83.0 | 79.4 |
Mother’s psychological distress | 4308 | 4.4 (0.8) | 4.5 (0.5) | 4.4 (0.6) |
Mother and partner argumentative relationship | 3931 | 2.2 (0.6) | 2.1 (0.6) | 2.2 (0.6) |
Single-parent household, % | 5103 | 9.7 | 0.2 | 9.7 |
Mother had gestational hypertension, % | 4238 | 7.8 | 7.8 | 8.1 |
Mother had gestational diabetes, % | 4223 | 5.7 | 5.2 | 5.9 |
Any alcohol during pregnancy, % | 4227 | 38.9 | 44.1 | 37.3 |
Smoking during pregnancy, % | 4239 | 16.7 | 10.6 | 18.2 |
Confounders of M-Y | ||||
Mother’s psychological distress | 3818 | 4.5 (0.6) | 4.5 (0.5) | 4.5 (0.6) |
Number of siblings | 4386 | 1.5 (1.3) | 1.5 (0.9) | 1.5 (1.0) |
Mother’s working status, % | ||||
Working full-time | 957 | 21.8 | 19.8 | 21.1 |
Working part-time | 1804 | 41.2 | 46.6 | 40.7 |
Currently not working | 1622 | 37.0 | 33.6 | 38.2 |
Household income per week, $ | 3668 | 1977.8 (1343.4) | 2054.5 (1365.3) | 1893.6 (1297.7) |
Hardship score | 4365 | 0.3 (0.7) | 0.2 (0.6) | 0.3 (0.8) |
a Abbreviations: N, sample number; M(SD), mean (standard deviation); Y, outcomes; PPVT, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; ARS, Academic Rating Scale; X, exposures; M, mediators; X-M, exposure-mediator; X-Y, exposure-outcome; IRSD, index of relative socioeconomic disadvantage; M-Y, mediator-outcome
b Response sample is the number of participants who responded to specific assessment for each child exposure, outcome, or confounder,
cComplete case sample are participants who have data for every variable (i.e. no missing on any variable.
Table 2 displays associations between temperament subscales and child outcomes using regression models adjusted for C (Model 1), C, M, and L (Model 2) and the MSM (Model 3). The CDEs estimated from the MSM (Model 3) were closer to total effects (Model 1), while estimates from conventional regression (Model 2) were lower. The MSM showed higher reactivity had negative associations with all outcomes, particularly verbal ability (PPVT; β = -0.37 95% CI -0.59, -0.14). Higher approach had positive associations with verbal and non-verbal abilities but little or no association with literacy or numeracy. Higher persistence had positive associations with all outcomes. Among the four outcomes, the largest associations were for verbal ability. In the MSM for instance, 1-unit higher persistence (range 1–5) was associated with 0.58-unit (0.11 SD) higher verbal (PPVT) and 0.19-unit (0.06 SD) higher non-verbal ability (Matrix Reasoning).
Table 2. Effect estimates of temperament reactivity, approach, and persistence at ages 2 to 3 years on child outcomes at ages 6 to 7 years (n = 5107)a.
Model 1b, total effect | Model 2b, conventional regression | Model 3b, controlled direct effect | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
βc | (95% CI) | SD units | βc | (95% CI) | SD units | βc | (95% CI) | SD units | |
Reactivity | |||||||||
PPVT | -0.38 | (-0.59, -0.17) | -0.07 | -0.36 | (-0.56, -0.15) | -0.07 | -0.37 | (-0.59, -0.14) | -0.07 |
Matrix reasoning | -0.09 | (-0.18, 0.01) | -0.03 | -0.09 | (-0.19, 0.002) | -0.03 | -0.11 | (-0.21, -0.01) | -0.04 |
ARS-Literacy | -0.07 | (-0.10, -0.04) | -0.09 | -0.07 | (-0.10, -0.04) | -0.09 | -0.08 | (-0.11, -0.05) | -0.10 |
ARS-Numeracy | -0.06 | (-0.09, -0.03) | -0.08 | -0.06 | (-0.09, -0.03) | -0.08 | -0.07 | (-0.10, -0.04) | -0.09 |
Approach | |||||||||
PPVT | 0.46 | (0.28, 0.65) | 0.09 | 0.40 | (0.21, 0.59) | 0.08 | 0.45 | (0.22, 0.67) | 0.09 |
Matrix reasoning | 0.10 | (0.01, 0.19) | 0.03 | 0.08 | (-0.01, 0.18) | 0.03 | 0.11 | (0.01, 0.21) | 0.04 |
ARS-Literacy | 0.03 | (-0.01, 0.06) | 0.04 | 0.02 | (-0.01, 0.06) | 0.03 | 0.03 | (-0.01, 0.07) | 0.04 |
ARS-Numeracy | 0.02 | (-0.02, 0.05) | 0.03 | 0.01 | (-0.03, 0.05) | 0.01 | 0.01 | (-0.02, 0.05) | 0.01 |
Persistence | |||||||||
PPVT | 0.61 | (0.38, 0.84) | 0.12 | 0.55 | (0.32, 0.79) | 0.11 | 0.58 | (0.27, 0.89) | 0.11 |
Matrix reasoning | 0.16 | (0.04, 0.29) | 0.05 | 0.17 | (0.05, 0.30) | 0.06 | 0.19 | (0.02, 0.34) | 0.06 |
ARS-Literacy | 0.08 | (0.04, 0.12) | 0.10 | 0.08 | (0.04, 0.12) | 0.10 | 0.08 | (0.03, 0.13) | 0.10 |
ARS-Numeracy | 0.08 | (0.04, 0.12) | 0.10 | 0.09 | (0.05, 0.13) | 0.11 | 0.08 | (0.03, 0.13) | 0.10 |
a Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; SD, standard deviation units; PPVT, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; ARS, Academic Rating Scale
b Model 1 is adjusted for confounders, C. Model 2 is the conventional regression model including X, M, and all confounders C and L. Model 3 is a marginal structural model including M and weighted for all confounders C and L.
c β are unstandardized coefficients
The sensitivity analysis showed that the CDEs were generally robust in the presence of a binary unmeasured confounder (S3 File). The observed CDEs would be explained by an unmeasured confounder if its prevalence differed between the exposed (x) and counterfactual (x*) by ≥80% and the estimated mean of the outcome differed by ≥0.60 within the two levels of the unmeasured confounder.
Discussion
We found evidence that suggests an association between temperament at 2–3 years on children’s cognitive abilities and academic outcomes at ages 6–7 years. Of the three temperament dimensions, persistence had the largest association, where a 1-unit increase in persistence (5-point Likert scale) was associated with an increase of 0.11 SD for verbal ability, 0.10 SD for literacy and numeracy and 0.06 SD for non-verbal reasoning. These results are similar to studies that measured persistence using different questionnaires [1, 14], children of different ages and using different statistical approaches [1]. For instance, a cross-sectional study of effortful control measured using the Child Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ) at 3–5 years was associated with 0.29 SD higher letter knowledge and 0.17 SD math achievement [14]. Rudasill et al [27] reported attention at 4.5 years measured with the CBQ was associated with 0.18 SD higher reading scores and 0.14 SD mathematic scores in 8–10 year-olds.
Reactivity was negatively associated with cognitive and academic outcomes, with the largest association being 0.10 SD on literacy. This was similar to studies of preschoolers [1, 12] where CBQ temperament scores at 5.6 years from parents and teachers were averaged and standardized associations of ~0.18–0.28 SD on mathematics and reading were observed [12]. The similarity in findings reported in different studies from different countries, at different ages, as well as the use of different tools for measuring temperament adds strength to these findings.
Consistent with previous work involving the STST [15], higher scores on approach (lower shyness), were associated with higher verbal (0.09 SD) and non-verbal (0.04 SD) cognitive abilities. While we found little evidence of temperament approach on literacy and numeracy, others have reported that shy children were more likely to have poorer academic achievement [45]. Differences in these findings may be due to the small (n = 125) cross-sectional design or because shyness was measured at 9–13 years, when children were expected to have more developed sociability skills.
Few studies have accounted for parenting when investigating associations between temperament and cognitive and academic outcomes, where often only narrow aspects of parenting have been examined (e.g. involvement in schooling [28], joint attention [29]). We defined parenting as the frequency parents engaged in activities with their children because there is evidence this is important for children’s development [26]. This operationalization of parenting is limited because it only measures the frequency and not the quality of the parent-child interactions. The parenting measure does not include other actions by parents (such as harsh parenting, hitting) that might affect children’s development [46]. Furthermore, parenting activities were reported by parents themselves, which raises concerns about potential biases such as same-rater bias or social desirability bias. Same-rater bias may be attenuated by the 2-year period between parents reporting of temperament (age 2–3) and parenting (age 4–5). Although having the same rater might conflate the temperament-parenting (X-M) association, no interaction between temperament and parenting was detected. Possible social desirability bias might have been mitigated through the parent questionnaires being returned by post. Although we expect mothers to have excellent knowledge of their child’s temperament, it has been suggested that reporting of temperament might differ among mothers from lower socioeconomic position or suffering depression [47]. Broader definitions of parenting, temperament or use of different mediators might reveal different findings. Thus, although we accounted for parenting, there may be other pathways that temperament could influence cognitive and academic outcomes, such as peer and teacher relationships or child care. Given that the associations between temperament and cognitive and academic outcomes were small, future research could examine temperament within the context of parenting practices, for instance, the effect of parenting on cognitive and academic outcomes may be heightened in temperamentally difficult compared with easy children. Parenting interventions could specifically target children with difficult temperament if they were more susceptible to the impact of parenting on their cognitive and academic outcomes.
Strengths of this study include the use of a large nationally-representative sample, prospective follow-up, direct measures of cognitive outcomes and teacher-reports of academic outcomes. Previous studies have often been limited to small, non-representative samples, cross-sectional or short-term design (1–2 years) [6]. For instance, Valiente et al studied emotionality and academic abilities 6-months later in 291 children [12]. Furthermore, our methods account for parenting which is thought to influence children’s cognitive and academic outcomes, and adjust for a wide range of potential confounders. It has been proposed that teachers might grade children with more challenging temperamental traits less favorably [48–51]. Teacher-reported outcomes also reflect how children are perceived in “real world” settings and one reason why broader studies are needed on the potential impacts of temperament. Another strength of these findings is the consistency in the direction of associations for outcomes measured using direct assessments (verbal and non-verbal cognition) and outcomes reported by teachers (literacy and numeracy).
The temperament tool used here (the STST) was developed in an Australian sample and it was useable in a large population-based study. There are many different conceptualizations of temperament and its’ components, and it is possible that different temperament tools, or direct/laboratory based measures may result in different findings. Nevertheless, the results reported here are consistent with studies using different temperament tools.
To understand the practical implications of this work, we reported effect sizes in SD units (Table 2) to help compare the current study with other evidence. While we acknowledge the problems interpreting standardized effect sizes [52], the current study involves a nationally-representative sample. This means that the SDs on which the associations are standardized are likely to be relevant to population-based interventions, and are less likely to be exaggerated than studies from homogenous samples. So how do the effects reported here compare with interventions that aim to improve aspects of children’s regulation of temperament traits? Large, population-based interventions in school settings, such as PATHS and the Chicago School Readiness Program, report effect sizes in the order of 0.03–0.5SD for literacy outcomes [6, 22, 53, 54]. This is largely in line with what is reported here and with other observational studies [6]. Unfortunately, there are few rigorously tested interventions that can be scaled-up to the population-level that might improve academic outcomes, and this represents an area of future development.
In conclusion, the findings described in the present study suggest that temperamental traits such as persistence, approach and reactivity at 2–3 years of age may be linked to cognitive and academic outcomes at ages 6–7 years. As an indication of the magnitude of these associations, the largest association suggested that a 1-point increase in persistence was linked to a 0.11SD increase in verbal ability.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
We gratefully acknowledge the use of data from Growing Up in Australia, the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC). The study is conducted in partnership between the Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs (FaHCSIA), the Australian Institute of Family Studies (AIFS) and the Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS). The findings and views reported in this paper are the authors’ and should not be attributed to FaHCSIA, the AIFS, or the ABS.
All authors contributed to the idea for the study. SYC acquired data from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children to conduct the analyses, all authors had access to the data. Statistical advice was provided by MM, with input from all authors. All authors approved the final manuscript.
Data Availability
Data cannot be shared publicly by us because we are not the custodians of the dataset. Data are available from the Growing Up In Australia study for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data (contact via https://growingupinaustralia.gov.au/data-and-documentation/accessing-lsac-data).
Funding Statement
Dr Chong was funded by a University of Adelaide International Scholarship and a Fraser Mustard Centre PhD Scholarship. During this work, Professor Lynch was supported by an Australia Fellowship from the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (70120). Associate Professor Smithers and Dr Mittinty were also supported by funds from the Australia Fellowship awarded to Prof Lynch. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Matheny A. Temperament and cognition: Relations between temperament and mental test scores In: Kohnstamm G, Bates J, Rothbart M, editors. Temperament in childhood. West Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons; 1989. p. 263–282. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Martin RP, Holbrook J. Relationship of temperament characteristics to the academic achievement of first-grade children. J Psychoeduc Assess. 1985;3(2):131–140. 10.1177/073428298500300204 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Sanson A, Hemphill SA, Yagmurlu B, McClowry S. Temperament and social development In: Smith P, Hart S, editors. The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell; 2011. p. 227–245. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Vassallo S, Sanson A. The Australian Temperament Project: the first 30 years. Australia: Australian Institute of Family Studies; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 5.American Psychological Association (APA). Washington, DC: APA; 2009. APA Concise Dictionary of Psychology. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Smithers LG, Sawyer ACP, Chittleborough CR, Davies NM, Davey Smith G, Lynch JW. A systematic review and meta-analysis of effects of early life non-cognitive skills on academic, psychosocial, cognitive and health outcomes. Nat Hum Behav. 2018;2(11):867–80. 10.1038/s41562-018-0461-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Vaughn B, Joffe L, Bradley C, Seife R, Barglow P. Maternal characteristics measured prenatally are predictive of ratings of temperamental 'difficulty' on the Carey Infant Temperament Questionnaire. Dev Psychol. 1987;23(1):152–61. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wachs TD, Gandour MJ. Temperament, environment, and six-month cognitive-intellectual development: a test of the organismic specificity hypothesis. Int J Behav Dev. 1983;6(2):135–52. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Carey WB, Fox M, McDevitt SC. Temperament as a factor in early school adjustment. Pediatrics. 1977;60(4 Pt 2):621–4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Palisin H. Preschool temperament and performance on achievement tests. Dev Psychol. 1986;22(6):766–70. 10.1037/0012-1649.22.6.766 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Cheng S, Maeda T, Yamagata Z, Tomiwa K, Yamakawa N. Comparison of factors contributing to developmental attainment of children between 9 and 18 months. J Epidemiol. 2010;20(Suppl 2):S452–S8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Valiente C, Lemery-Chalfant K, Swanson J. Prediction of kindergartners' academic achievement from their effortful control and emotionality: Evidence for direct and moderated relations. J Educ Psychol. 2010;102(3):550–60. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Sawyer A, Chittleborough C, Mittinty M, Miller-Lewis L, Sawyer M, Sullivan T, et al. Are trajectories of self-regulation abilities from ages 2–3 to 6–7 associated with academic achievement in the early school years? Child Care Health Dev. 2014;41(5):744–54. 10.1111/cch.12208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. 2007;78(2):647–63. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Prior M, Bavin E, Cini E, Reilly S, Bretherton L, Wake M, et al. Influences on communicative development at 24 months of age: Child temperament, behaviour problems, and maternal factors. Infant Behav Dev. 2008;31:270–9. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.11.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Dixon WE, Salley BJ, Clement AD. Temperament, distraction, and learnig in toddlerhood. Infant Behav Dev. 2006;29(3):342–57. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.01.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Blair C, Raver CC. School readiness and self-regulation: a developmental psychobiological approach. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66:711–31. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.West J, Denton K, Germino-Hausken E. America's kindergartners. Washington, DC: US Department fo Education, National Center for Education Statistics, 2000. NCES 2000–070. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Webster-Stratton W, Eyberg S. Child temperament: Relationships with child behavior problems and parent-child interactions. J Clin Child Psychol. 1982;2:123–9. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Banerjee PN, Tamis-LeMonda CS. Infant's persistence and mother's teaching as predictors of toddlers' cognitive development. Infant Behav Dev. 2007;30(3):479–91. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.12.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Greenberg MT, Kusche C, Cook E, Quama J. Promoting emotional competence in school-aged children: The effects of the PATHS curriculum. Dev Psychopathol. 1995;7(1):117–36. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Raver CC, Jones SM, Li-Grining C, Zhai F, Bub K, Pressler E. CSRP's impact on low-income preschoolers' preacademic skills: Self-regulation as a mediating mechanism. Child Dev. 2011;82(1):362–78. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01561.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Heckman JJ, Rubinstein Y. The importance of noncognitive skills: Lessons from the GED testing program. Am Econ Rev. 2001;91(2):145–9. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Dixon WE, Smith P. Who's controlling whom? Infant contributors to maternal play behaviour. Infant Child Dev. 2003;12:177–95. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Maccoby EE, Snow ME, Jacklin CN. Children's dispositions and mother–child interaction at 12 and 18 months: A short-term longitudinal study. Dev Psychol. 1984;20(3):459–72. 10.1037/0012-1649.20.3.459 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Byford M, Kuh D, Richards M. Parenting practices and intergenerational associations in cognitive ability. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;41:263–72. 10.1093/ije/dyr188 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Rudasill KM, Gallagher KC, White JM. Temperamental attention and activity, classroom emotional support, and academic achievement in third grade. J School Psychol. 2010;48(2):113–34. 10.1016/j.jsp.2009.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rabiner D, Coie J. Early attention problems and children's reading achievement: A longitudinal investigation. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(7):859–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Salley B, Dixon W. Temperamental and joint attentional predictors of language development. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2007;53:131–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.VanderWeele TJ. Marginal structural models for the estimation of direct and indirect effects. Epidemiology. 2009;20(18–26). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hernan MA, Robins JM. Casual Inference. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Robins J, Greenland S. Identifiability and exchangeability for direct and indirect effects. Epidemiology. 1992;3(2):143–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Soloff C, Lawrence D, Johnstone R. LSAC Technical Paper No. 1: Sample Design. Melbourne: Australia Institute of Family Studies; 2005.
- 34.Rothman S. Data Issues Paper No 2. Report on the Adapted PPVT-III and the Who Am I? Melbourne, Australia: Australian Institute of Family Studies; 2005.
- 35.Dunn L, Dunn L. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 3rd ed: American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MN; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). San Antonio, TX, USA: Psychological Corporation; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 37.National Center for Education Statistics. User's Manual for the ECLS-K First Grade Public-Use Data Files and Electronic Code Book NCES 2002–135. Washington, DC2002.
- 38.Rock D, Pollack J. Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Kindergarten Class of 1998–99 (ECLS–K), Psychometric Report for Kindergarten Through First Grade. Washington, DC: US Dept of Education, National Center for Education Statistics; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Prior M, Sanson A, Oberklaid F, Northam E. Measurement of temperament in one to three year old children. Int J Behav Dev. 1987;10(1):121–32. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Fullard W, McDevitt S, Carey W. Assessing temperament in one- to three-year old children. J Pediatr Psychol. 1984;9(2):205–17. 10.1093/jpepsy/9.2.205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.United Nation's Children's Fund. Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey (MICS) [updated 23 October 2014May 27, 2015]. Available from: http://www.unicef.org/statistics/index_24302.html.
- 42.Hernan MA. A definition of causal effect for epidemiological research. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004;58:265–71. 10.1136/jech.2002.006361 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.VanderWeele TJ. Bias formula for sensitivity analysis for direct and indirect effects. Epidemiology 2010;21(4):540–51. 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181df191c [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Sterne J, White I, Carlin J, Spratt M, Royston P, Kenward M, et al. Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls. BMJ. 2009;338:b2393 10.1136/bmj.b2393 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hughes K, Coplan R. Exploring processes linking shyness and academic achievement in childhood. Sch Psychol Q. 2010;25(4):213–22. 10.1037/a0022070 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.MacKenzie MJ, Nicklas E, Waldfogel J, Brooks-Gunn J. Spanking and child development across the first decade of life. Pediatrics. 2013;132(5):e1118–25. 10.1542/peds.2013-1227 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Vaughn B, Joffe L, Bradley C, Seife R, Barglow P. Maternal characteristics measured prenatally are predictive of ratings of temperamenttal 'difficulty' on the Carey Infant Temperament Quesitonnaire. Dev Psychol. 1987;23:152–61. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Keogh BK. Children's temperament and teachers' decisions. Ciba Found Symp. 1982;89:269–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Lerner JV, Lerner RM, Zabski S. Temperament and elementary school children's actual and rated academic performance: a test of a 'goodness-of-fit' model. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1985;26(1):125–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Childs G, McKay M. Boys starting school disadvantaged: implications from teachers' ratings of behaviour and achievement in the first two years. Br J Educ Psychol. 2001;71(Pt 2):303–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Baker CN, Tichovolsky MH, Kupersmidt JB, Voegler-Lee ME, Arnold DH. Teacher (Mis)Perceptions of Preschoolers' Academic Skills: Predictors and Associations With Longitudinal Outcomes. J Educ Psychol. 2015;107(3):805–20. 10.1037/edu0000008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lipsey MW, Puzio K, Yun C, Herbert MA, Steinka-Fry K, Cole MW, et al. Translating the statistical representation of the effects of education interventions into more readily interpretable forms. Washington DC: US Department of Education, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bierman KL, Nix RL, Heinrichs BS, Domitrovich CE, Gest SD, Welsh JA, et al. Effects of Head Start REDI on children's outcomes 1 year later in different kindergarten contexts. Child Dev. 2014;85(1):140–59. 10.1111/cdev.12117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Nix RL, Bierman KL, Domitrovich CE, Gill S. Promoting children's social-emotional skills in preschool can enhance academic and behavioral functioning in kindergarten: Findings from Head Start REDI. Early Educ Dev. 2013;24(7):1000–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data cannot be shared publicly by us because we are not the custodians of the dataset. Data are available from the Growing Up In Australia study for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data (contact via https://growingupinaustralia.gov.au/data-and-documentation/accessing-lsac-data).