What is the neural basis of the DSM-5-defined pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)? The DSM-5 (1) defines PTSD as a maladaptive response to at least one severe, threatening event. The symptoms that arise ostensibly involve neural systems underlying threat learning, the stress response, and emotion regulation. In this issue, Heyn and colleagues (2) demonstrate a disciplined approach to identifying major neural systems associated with pediatric PTSD. Their approach has three major aspects: a well-defined PTSD phenotype, an exploratory, whole brain analysis, and a longitudinal design. The importance of this work is in its empirical implication of prefrontal and affective regions supporting emerging theories of affective dysfunction in pediatric PTSD.
With regards to phenotyping, the authors use a combination of the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia, Present and Lifetime Version (KSADS-PL; 3) supplemented with Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (4). Common to both, a severe traumatic event is established, and symptoms are linked to that event. Thus, all in the PTSD group experienced at least one, severe traumatic event and that PTSD-associated symptoms are linked to it. Such clarity in clinical phenotyping improves reproducibility and comparisons with other studies, such as the effect of increasing exposure to KSADS-PL-style traumatic events to social and executive cognitive dysfunction (5).
Moreover, defining PTSD by a reaction to a traumatic event allows Heyn and colleagues (2) to invoke a promising theory based on a substantial basic literature on the neural basis of threat-learning to explain their results (6). In this formulation, PTSD represents a set of threat associations that produce a maladaptive response. The response is a defensive state induced by associations, e.g. “triggers,” that do not represent a presently valid threat. Of course, a first step is to establish that this system is related to pediatric PTSD. The significance of doing so has direct implications for treatment development. Widely used exposure-based treatments target this system to reduce maladaptive responses to threat-associations. And so, it may be that exposure-based therapies for PTSD may be improved when considering this theory (7).
While this phenotype does establish a threatening event and associated symptoms, it does not address the degree of sustained stress. Often a point of confusion, it cannot be assumed that the pathophysiology of PTSD described here is same for maladaptive behavior and distress following chronic deprivation or chronic stress (e.g., toxic stress). Keeping focused on a clearly defined clinical phenotype may be the way out of the confusion among clinicians and investigators as to what comprises PTSD. The current Research Domain Criteria divides “acute threat” and “sustained threat” as separable systems that differentiate the two for future research.
With regards to an exploratory approach, the study maps brain differences between youth with PTSD and typically developing youth with no psychopathology (TD). It uses established, exploratory techniques surveying PTSD-TD differences in structure via voxel-based morphometry and function via resting state functional connectivity. Both techniques are ideal for in vivo exploration, demonstrating the utility of magnetic resonance imaging to implicate regions in the pathophysiology of mental illness. The reliance on exploratory work is critical when we know little of the system, here the neural basis of PTSD symptoms, in which any future theory will operate.
The authors have a two-staged approach, using volume changes found by morphometry as a localizer for functional connectivity. Averaged across time, PTSD-TD volume differences were found in four prefrontal regions, the precentral gyrus, and the posterior cingulate cortex. Over the course of a year, decreases in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volumes found in the TD were not observed in the PTSD group. In the second stage, the authors lean on theory a bit to constrain their analyses to search for PTSD-TD differences in functional connectivity between the prefrontal regions found in morphometry study and the bilateral amygdala and hippocampus. These regions are critical mediators of associative learning for threat (described above) as well as a complementary theory in affective neuroscience also relevant for PTSD. This theory describes prefrontal-subcortical circuitry involvement in context-sensitive regulation of emotions (8). Here the PTSD-TD differences in connectivity involving three prefrontal regions were largely driven by changes in prefrontal-amygadala/hippocampal connectivity across time.
Overall these techniques can only implicate these regions in the pathophysiology of PTSD or something related to having PTSD, e.g. a general mental illness factor, treatment effects, symptoms changes, etc. The addition of a functional connectivity arm provides some external validation for the morphometry results. It provides evidence that this relationship has something to do with prefrontal-amygdala/hippocampal connectivity predicted by major theories in affective neuroscience. As the authors are careful to note, the functional significance of these findings cannot be known from their data. Regardless, empirical evidence implicating these regions with PTSD and their striking correspondence with emerging theory is an advance.
Another important aspect of this study is the longitudinal approach. The adolescent brain changes. Failing to capture this change would have clearly cost the authors important findings in connectivity. It also makes the study more demanding from a technical standpoint. In clinical research, establishing safety and following individuals’ treatments substantially increases the effort to conduct the study. As part of the nature of clinical studies, no interference in standard treatment is acceptable. While it is encouraging to see symptom improvement and treatment, this complicates interpretation of the results. Indeed, the authors discuss the implications of medication use on type II error. However, it is critical to appreciate that mechanisms of treatment and recovery are of as much clinical interest as pathophysiology. The authors have a limited sample but do attempt preliminary analyses that only a longitudinal design would allow.
In sum, longitudinal pediatric neuroimaging studies involving careful clinical phenotyping are of high value. Heyn and colleagues (2) advance our knowledge of pediatric PTSD by linking it with neural systems underlying threat learning and context sensitive emotion regulation.
Acknowledgements:
This work was supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Mental Health, K23MH113731 and the Pediatric Mental Health Institute at Children’s Hospital Colorado and the Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado School of Medicine.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Disclosures: The author declares no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
References
- 1.American Psychiatric Association (2013): Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Heyn SA, Keding TJ, Ross MC, Cisler JM, Mumford JA, Herringa RJ. (2019). Abnormal prefrontal development in pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder: A longitudinal structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Bio Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. XX: XXX–XXX [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, et al. (1997). Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 36: 980–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Weathers FW, Keane TM, Davidson JR. (2001). Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale: A review of the first ten years of research. Depress Anxiety. 13: 132–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Barzilay R, Calkins ME, Moore TM, Wolf DH, Satterthwaite TD, Scott JC, et al. (2018). Association between traumatic stress load, psychopathology, and cognition in the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort. Psychol Med. 1–10. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718000880 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ledoux JE. (2014). Coming to terms with fear. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 111: 2871–2878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Craske MG, Treanor M, Conway CC, Zbozinek T, Vervliet B. (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy: An inhibitory learning approach. Behav Res Ther. 58: 10–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, et al. (2014). Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: A meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex. 24: 2981–2990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]