Abstract
The columnar organization is most apparent in the whisker barrel cortex but seems less apparent in the gustatory insular cortex. We addressed here whether there are any differences between the two cortices in columnar information processing by comparing the spatiotemporal patterns of excitation spread in the two cortices using voltage-sensitive dye imaging. In contrast to the well known excitation spread in the horizontal direction in layer II/III induced in the barrel cortex by layer IV stimulation, the excitation caused in the insular cortex by stimulation of layer IV spread bidirectionally in the vertical direction into layers II/III and V/VI, displaying a columnar image pattern. Bicuculline or picrotoxin markedly extended the horizontal excitation spread in layer II/III in the barrel cortex, leading to a generation of excitation in the underlying layer V/VI, whereas those markedly increased the amplitude of optical responses throughout the whole column in the insular cortex, subsequently widening the columnar image pattern. Such synchronous activities as revealed by the horizontal and vertical excitation spreads were consistently induced in the barrel and insular cortices, respectively, even by stimulation of different layers with varying intensities. Thus, a unique functional column existed in the insular cortex, in which intracolumnar communication between the superficial and deep layers was prominent, and GABAA action is involved in the inhibition of the intracolumnar communication in contrast to its involvement in intercolumnar lateral inhibition in the barrel cortex. These results suggest that the columnar information processing may not be universal across the different cortical areas.
Keywords: barrel cortex, insular cortex, lateral inhibition, functional column, GABAA receptor, voltage-sensitive dye
Introduction
In the rodent barrel cortex, a distinct topographical representation of the whisker is well known (Woolsey and Van der Loos, 1970; Moore et al., 1999), and the presence of columnar organization is most apparent in this area as revealed by the barrel structure of layer IV (Feldmeyer et al., 1999, 2005; Laaris et al., 2000; Lubke et al., 2000; Petersen and Sakmann, 2000, 2001; Bender et al., 2003). In contrast, in the primary gustatory area of the insular cortex, the topographical representation of the four basic tastes (sweet, sour, salty, and bitter) appears to be much less distinct (Yamamoto et al., 1980, 1984, 1985; Kosar et al., 1986; Ogawa et al., 1991, 1992). Indeed, there were considerable overlaps among the representation areas of the four basic tastes in rats (Accolla et al., 2007). The taste neurons are intermingled with sensory neurons for various other modalities such as tactile, thermal, and nociceptive sensations (Cechetto and Saper, 1987; Yamamoto et al., 1988; Allen et al., 1991; Hanamori et al., 1998). Furthermore, the gustatory insular cortex is mainly composed of dysgranular and agranular cortices, thereby displaying either a thin layer IV or no layer IV (Yamamoto et al., 1984). Thus, information processing in the insular cortex would be predicted to differ from other sensory cortical areas.
However, several studies suggested that the gustatory insular cortex processes information through functional column. For example, when simultaneous recordings were obtained from two neurons closely located in the insular cortex, they displayed synchronous activities in response to a taste stimulation (Nakamura and Ogawa, 1997; Yokota and Satoh, 2001). Furthermore, taste-responsive mechanoreceptive neurons with the same receptive field are topographically arranged in the insular cortex (Wang and Ogawa, 2002). However, it is not yet firmly established whether the distinct functional column exists in the insular cortex, and it is also not clear whether columnar information processing is universal or distinctive between the insular and other sensory cortices.
Higher brain functions such as learning and cognition are thought to be mediated by synchronization of firing activities of pyramidal neurons located within a single column or in different columns of the cerebral cortex (Gray et al., 1989; Roy and Alloway, 1999). Synchronous activities among pyramidal neurons in the neighboring columns may be regulated by GABAergic lateral inhibition (Chagnac-Amitai and Connors, 1989). If the lateral inhibition in the barrel and insular cortices were different, such discrepancies would be helpful for a better understanding of the functional role of the lateral inhibition in the synchronization. Here, we addressed these questions by quantitatively analyzing the spatiotemporal dynamics of the excitation spread in the barrel and insular cortices, in the absence and presence of bicuculline or picrotoxin, using voltage-sensitive dye imaging.
Materials and Methods
All experiments were performed in accordance with the guidelines of Osaka University Graduate School of Dentistry for the care and use of laboratory animals.
Slice preparation.
We prepared slices using standard methods (Kang et al., 2004; Saito et al., 2006; Kang et al., 2007). Wistar rats of both sexes at 18–22 and 15–22 d of age (Charles River Breeders, Osaka, Japan) were used for the experiments on the barrel and insular cortices, respectively. They were anesthetized with diethylether, and the brain was quickly removed from the skull and immersed in ice-cold modified artificial CSF (M-ACSF) (in mm: 230 sucrose, 2.5 KCl, 10 MgSO4, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 2.5 CaCl2, and 10 d-glucose). The caudal end of the brain block was cut at 15° tilted caudally from the coronal plane. With a microslicer (Super ZERO-1; Dosaka EM, Kyoto, Japan), sections of 350 μm thickness including both the whisker barrel and gustatory insular cortices were cut parallel to the plane, referring to the anatomical landmarks of the rhinal fissure and the middle cerebral artery (Yamamoto et al., 1985; Kosar et al., 1986; Cechetto and Saper, 1987; Accolla et al., 2007) (Fig. 1A). By intracellular injection of Lucifer yellow (Fig. 1Ba) with biocytin (Fig. 1Bb) and by extracellular application of DiI (Fig. 1C,D), we confirmed that apical dendrites of pyramidal neurons in deep layers are well preserved in these slice preparations. Slices were incubated at 32°C for 30 min in 50% M-ACSF and 50% normal ACSF (N-ACSF) (in mm: 126 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1 MgSO4, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, 2 CaCl2, and 10 d-glucose). The slices were then placed in N-ACSF at room temperature (20–24°C). N-ACSF was continuously gassed with a mixture of 95% O2–5% CO2.
Optical recording using a voltage-sensitive dye.
The slices were stained for 60 min with a styryl pyridinium dye RH414 (Invitrogen, Tokyo, Japan) that was dissolved in dimethylsulfoxide and added to the gassed N-ACSF at a dilution of 1:1000 to yield a final concentration of 200 μm. The stained slices were placed in the recording chamber perfused continuously with N-ACSF at a flow rate of 1.5 ml/min at the room temperature during imaging experiments. The slices were illuminated with light of 535 ± 15 nm wavelength using a stabilized 150 W xenon lamp (Opti-Quip, Highland Mills, NY). The fluorescence emitted from voltage-sensitive dye was long-pass filtered above 580 nm and measured with a CCD camera (NeuroCCD-SM; RedShirtImaging, Fairfield, CT), which was attached to an upright microscope (BX-51WI; Olympus, Tokyo, Japan) equipped with a water immersion objective (10×, 0.3 numerical aperture; Olympus). The imaged area was 1.6 × 1.6 mm2, and each pixel (element) of the 80 × 80 array detected the optical signals generated by a square region (20 × 20 μm2) on the slice. Two hundred and fifty-six consecutive frames of the fluorescence images were captured at a sampling rate of 1 kHz, and stimulation was applied when capturing the 25th frame. Eight series of the entire 256 optical images obtained at an interval of 15 s were averaged to improve the signal-to-noise ratio. No change in fluorescence intensity was observed in the absence of RH414.
Microstimulation of 100 μs duration was delivered via a sharp monopolar tungsten electrode (DC resistance, 1 MΩ) placed at the center of a barrel in the barrel cortex (Fig. 1Ac) or at “layer IV,” which is a site 300–400 μm lateral to the tail end of granular layer IV in the transition zone between the dysgranular and agranular insular cortices (Fig. 1Ad). This tail of granular layer IV was clearly visible with infrared–differential interference contrast (IR-DIC) optics. Microstimulation was also applied to layer III or layer V. Stimulus intensities were changed between 4 and 8 V, which were 1.2- to 1.8-fold greater than the threshold intensity. The stimulation with the intensity >8 V was never used because the slice tissue was often lesioned by such stimuli. An antagonist of GABAA receptor, bicuculline, and a blocker of GABA-gated Cl− channels, picrotoxin (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO), were bath applied at concentrations of 10 and 100 μm, respectively.
Data analysis.
The optical data were analyzed by using NeuroPlex (RedShirtImaging) and were displayed as pseudocolor images. Voltage-sensitive dye signals were calculated as ΔF/F0, where ΔF is the difference (F0 − F) between the resting light intensity (F0) and the respective fluorescence responses (F) obtained before and after stimulation. For pseudocolor coding (purple to red), the optical signals obtained from the average of 3 × 3 pixels ranging between the minimal (−4.5 × 10−3) and maximal values (+7.0 × 10−3) of ΔF/F0 were scaled between 0 and 127. Optical signals smaller than the minimal and larger than the maximal were treated as 0 and 127, respectively. Amplitudes of the optical responses were normalized to the maximum amplitude of the response obtained at the crossing point (the average of 3 × 3 pixels) of the vertical and horizontal axes, and were plotted as a normalized optical response (NOR). The vertical axis was drawn from the stimulated site (location of the initial response) perpendicularly to the cortical surface, and the orthogonal horizontal axis was drawn through the midpoint of the excited band of layer II/III in the barrel cortex. In the insular cortex, the orthogonal horizontal axis was drawn through the midpoint of cytoarchitectural layer II/III itself, because there was no discrete horizontal spread of excitation confined within layer II/III (see Fig. 2C). To show the temporal profiles of optical responses in layers II/III and V, two regions of interest (ROI-1 and ROI-2) were placed at the crossing point and the site 240 μm below the lower margin of the barrel or the tip of layer IV in the insular cortex on the vertical axis.
Numerical data are expressed as the mean ± SD. Statistical significance was assessed using ANOVA with post hoc PLSD test. Curves representing temporal and spatial profiles were smoothed by using Savitzky–Golay (S–G) averaging method, unless otherwise mentioned.
Results
Spatiotemporal patterns of the excitation spread induced by layer IV stimulation in the barrel cortex
We analyzed the spatiotemporal patterns of excitation spread with reference to the septum of respective barrels, which is visible as a column boundary at a low magnification (10×) in slice preparations of the barrel cortex (Fig. 2Aa). After microstimulation of the center of a barrel in a column (Fig. 2Aa), the initial optical response was evoked in a limited area within a single barrel at a latency of 1 ms (stage 1) (Fig. 2Ab). This was likely caused by direct activation of layer IV neurons in response to the microstimulation (Gustafsson and Jankowska, 1976). Subsequently, the excitation spread toward layer II/III (stage 2) (Fig. 2Ac), and then began leaving layer IV to propagate into layer II/III where the strength of excitation increased (stage 3) (Fig. 2Ad). Thereafter, the excitation propagated completely into layer II/III, and simultaneously spread in the horizontal direction into the neighboring columns partly, in layer II/III (stage 4) (Fig. 2Ae). During stage 4, the strength of excitation in layer II/III of the stimulated (home) column reached a maximum level. However, the excitation in layer V/VI of the home column was not apparent throughout stages 1–4 (Fig. 2Ab–e), consistent with a previous report (Petersen and Sakmann, 2001).
Here, we analyzed the vertical and horizontal profiles of the spatiotemporal pattern of the excitation spread by plotting the amplitude of optical responses normalized to its maximum value (i.e., NOR) against the position on the vertical (Fig. 2Aa, interrupted line labeled with V) and horizontal (Fig. 2Aa, interrupted line labeled with H) axes. The vertical and horizontal spatial profiles of the excitation spread were documented at the four stages described above (Fig. 2Ba,b), because the time course of excitation spread varied from slice to slice. The mean lag times of stages 1–4 examined in five slices were 1 ms (black curve), 5.1 ± 0.5 ms (blue curve), 8.1 ± 1.7 ms (green curve), and 10.5 ± 1.7 ms (orange curve), respectively.
As revealed by the averaged vertical profiles obtained from five slices, the peak position of the optical response shifted from layer IV toward layer II/III and its peak amplitude increased, with the progression from stage 1 to stage 4 (Fig. 2Ba, arrows). The averaged horizontal profiles of the excitation spread in layer II/III (n = 5) also revealed that the optical response increased both in amplitude and in spatial extent with stage progression (Fig. 2Bb). The optical response reached a plateau level only in amplitude (>90% peak value indicated with an interrupted line) at the end of stage 3 (green curve), but continued to increase in the spatial extent until the end of stage 4 (orange curve), consequently exhibiting a trapezoidal pattern of the horizontal spatial profile at stage 4. The enhancement of optical responses in layer II/III has previously been predicted by the exact overlap in layer II/III between the two terminal arborization fields of layer IV and layer II/III neurons (Feldmeyer et al., 2006).
The temporal profiles of the optical responses were also analyzed at ROI-1 in layer II/III and ROI-2 in layer V in five slices (Fig. 2A, blue and red squares, respectively). The optical response in layer II/III reached a peak value at the end of stage 3 (mean lag time, 8.3 ± 0.4 ms) and remained almost unchanged for 3.0 ± 0.9 ms by the end of stage 4 (mean lag time, 11.3 ± 1.1 ms), thereafter rapidly decaying (Fig. 2Bc, blue filled circles). During this plateau phase (Fig. 2Bc, inset), the excitation spread in the horizontal direction in layer II/III, as revealed by the trapezoidal pattern in the horizontal profile (stage 4) (Fig. 2Bb, orange curve). Because of the variation of temporal profiles from slice to slice, the duration of plateau phase in the averaged temporal profile is not necessarily equivalent to the mean duration of plateau phases in five temporal profiles. The optical response in layer V displayed a similar time course to that seen in layer II/III but was much smaller in amplitude than that in layer II/III (Fig. 2Bc, red filled circles).
Spatiotemporal patterns of the excitation spread induced by layer IV stimulation in the insular cortex
The vertical and horizontal spatial profiles of excitation spread in the insular cortex were documented at the following three stages in five slices examined. After microstimulation of layer IV (defined in Materials and Methods), a prominent initial optical response was evoked in a stimulated area at a latency of 1 ms (stage 1) (Fig. 2Cb), similar to that seen in the barrel cortex (Fig. 2Ab). Subsequently, the excitation spread bidirectionally toward layers II/III and V at stage 2 with a mean lag time of 6.1 ± 1.2 ms (Fig. 2Cc). It then spread over both layer II/III and layer V entirely, forming a columnar pattern of the optical image at stage 3 with a mean lag time of 10.9 ± 2.7 ms (Fig. 2Cd). During stage 3, the amplitude of the optical response at the midpoint of layer II/III right above the location of the initial response reached a maximum value.
As revealed by the averaged vertical profile (n = 8), the initial response observed at stage 1 with a mean lag time of 1 ms was very sharp and strong (Fig. 2Da, black curve). However, both the vertical spatial profiles at stages 2 and 3 displayed no sharp peak (Fig. 2Da, blue and green curves). Thus, the excitation in the initial response appeared to spread bidirectionally into layers II/III and V (Fig. 2Da), unlike for the barrel cortex (Fig. 2Ba). Contrary to the increment of the peak amplitude of responses during the vertical spread of excitation in the barrel cortex (Fig. 2Ba), the peak amplitude of responses in the insular cortex decreased during the vertical spread of excitation (Fig. 2Da, compare blue and green curves with black one). This suggests an involvement of the inhibition of neurons in layers II/III and V by the activity of layer IV neurons in depressing the vertical spread of excitation. This possibility was examined by applying bicuculline as described in a later section. The averaged horizontal profile in layer II/III revealed that the insular cortex never displayed a trapezoidal pattern of the horizontal spatial profile, but instead showed a triangular or bell-shaped pattern even after reaching the maximum amplitude, unlike the case in the barrel cortex (Fig. 2, compare Db, Bb).
The temporal profile of the excitation in the insular cortex was also distinct from that in the barrel cortex. The optical response at ROI-1 in layer II/III (Fig. 2C, blue square) and that at ROI-2 in layer V (Fig. 2C, red square) in the insular cortex reached the peak values 10.9 ± 2.6 and 8.9 ± 1.1 ms (n = 8) after stimulation, respectively (Fig. 2Dc, blue and red filled circles, respectively). In contrast to what was observed in the barrel cortex (Fig. 2Bc), the optical responses in ROI-1 and ROI-2 decayed very slowly (Fig. 2Dc). These results indicate that a distinct functional column exists in the insular cortex and that there may be differences in the local circuits of functional column between the insular and barrel cortices, as reflected in the differences in the spatiotemporal pattern of the optical response between the two cortices.
Thus, there were distinct differences in the spatiotemporal pattern of the excitation spread between the barrel and insular cortices. Next, we examined whether GABAA inhibition is involved in generating such differences.
Effects of bicuculline on the excitation spread induced by layer IV stimulation in the barrel cortex
When the optical response was evoked in the presence of bicuculline (10 μm) by stimulation of layer IV in the barrel cortex, the horizontal spread of excitation in layer II/III remarkably extended far beyond the distal end of the neighboring columns after stage 4 (Fig. 3Be). Such a remarkable extension of the horizontal spread resulted in excitation in layer V of the same columns that exhibited the synchronous excitation in layer II/III (Fig. 3, compare Be, Ae, three arrowheads). Thereafter, the horizontal spread in layer II/III continued to extend for ∼18 ms and reached a maximum value (Fig. 3Bf), subsequently causing excitation in layer V of a more remote column (Fig. 3Bf, arrowhead). In five slices, the spatiotemporal patterns of the excitation spread were compared more quantitatively between the optical responses obtained in the absence and presence of bicuculline.
As revealed in the averaged temporal profiles in layer II/III in the absence and presence of bicuculline (Fig. 3Ca, blue filled and open circles, respectively), bicuculline had no apparent effect on the temporal profiles until the end of stage 2 (Fig. 3Ca, black and blue arrows), but subsequently increased the peak amplitude by 37 ± 16% and prolonged the half-duration from 11.4 ± 3.2 ms to a mean value at least larger than 152 ms (Fig. 3Ca). Also, in layer V, until the end of stage 2 (Fig. 3Cb, black and blue arrows), there were no marked differences in the temporal profile between the optical responses in the absence and presence of bicuculline (Fig. 3Cb, red filled and open circles, respectively). Thereafter, however, bicuculline markedly increased the peak amplitude by 232 ± 102% and prolonged the half-duration of the optical response in layer V from 16.8 ± 6.7 ms to a mean value at least larger than 169 ms (Fig. 3Cb), similar to the case with layer II/III (Fig. 3Ca). It was difficult to accurately measure the half-duration in the presence of bicuculline because it often exceeded the measurable time window of 231 ms after stimulation.
The vertical and horizontal profiles of the excitation spread obtained in the absence and presence of bicuculline (solid and dotted curves, respectively) were compared at the respective stages (Fig. 3D,E). As revealed by averaging the spatial profiles at the same stages obtained from five slices, the vertical and horizontal profiles at stages 1 and 2 (black and blue, respectively) were not markedly affected by bicuculline (Fig. 3Da,Ea, compare solid and dotted curves). Subsequently, bicuculline increased the overall amplitudes in the vertical and horizontal profiles at stages 3 and 4, leaving their spatial extents of the excitation spread almost unchanged (Fig. 3Db,Eb, compare orange solid and dotted curves). Thereafter, however, both the amplitude and the spatial extent in the horizontal profile continued to increase, and only the amplitude reached a maximum value (Fig. 3Eb, violet dotted curve) at a time, which was defined as the end of stage 5 (mean lag time, 23.5 ± 9.1 ms) (Fig. 3Ca, violet arrow). Then, after the slow decay of the optical response (Fig. 3Ca, blue open circles), the spatial extent in the horizontal profile was further increased, and reached a maximum value (Fig. 3Eb, red dotted curve) at a time that was defined as the end of stage 6 (mean lag time, 40.3 ± 9.6 ms) (Fig. 3Ca, red arrow). The amplitude at ROI-1 at the end of stage 6 was smaller than the maximum value at the end of stage 5, but was invariably larger than that of the response showing the maximum horizontal width at stage 4 in the absence of bicuculline (Fig. 3Eb, compare red dotted curve and orange solid curve). The enhancement or generation of the excitation in layer V (Fig. 3Db, asterisk), especially at stage 6, was invariably associated with the remarkable extension of the horizontal spread of excitation within layer II/III (Fig. 3Eb, asterisk).
These results indicate that GABAA inhibition began to operate after the excitation left layer IV and propagated into layer II/III, suggesting that the GABAergic interneurons were activated after activation of layer II/III pyramidal neurons. The synchronous excitation of neighboring columns within layer II/III (Fig. 3Eb, asterisk) resulted in the excitation of layer V output neurons in the same columns (Fig. 3Db, asterisk). This may be reflected in the longer time-to-peak of the excitation in layer V in comparison with that in layer II/III (Fig. 3, compare violet arrow in Ca, red arrow in Cb).
Effects of bicuculline on the excitation spread induced by layer IV stimulation in the insular cortex
When the optical response was evoked in the presence of bicuculline (10 μm) by stimulation of layer IV in the insular cortex, the width of columnar pattern increased in comparison with the case in the absence of bicuculline (Fig. 4A,B). The temporal profiles of excitation in layers II/III and V (ROI-1 and ROI-2, respectively) (Fig. 4A,B) were compared in the absence and presence of bicuculline (filled and open circles, respectively) (Fig. 4Ca,Cb). Bicuculline began to affect the temporal profiles of the excitation in layers II/III and V by the end of stage 2 (Fig. 4Ca,Cb, blue arrows). Subsequently, bicuculline markedly increased the amplitudes of the optical responses in layers II/III and V by 138 ± 64 and 170 ± 102%, respectively, and prolonged the times-to-peak from 11.0 ± 3.2 to 37.6 ± 7.9 ms in layer II/III (Fig. 4Ca, green and orange arrows) and from 9.0 ± 1.3 to 39.4 ± 11.7 ms in layer V (Fig. 4Cb, green and orange arrows).
As also revealed by the averaged vertical and horizontal profiles of the excitation spread, bicuculline did not markedly affect these profiles at stage 1 (Fig. 4C, black arrow; Da,Ea, black solid and dotted curves). At stages 2 and 3 (Fig. 4C, blue and green arrows), bicuculline increased the amplitudes of the optical responses in both layers II/III and V/VI to values larger than those of the initial response, without changing the spatial extents in the vertical and horizontal profiles markedly (Fig. 4D,E, blue and green solid and dotted curves). Thereafter, both the amplitude and the spatial extent in the vertical and horizontal profiles were considerably increased by bicuculline, for example, 34 ms after stimulation (Fig. 4Bd) during stage 4 (Fig. 4Db,Eb, orange solid and dotted curves). Then, only the amplitude reached a maximum value at a time that was defined as the end of stage 4 (mean lag time, 40.5 ± 10.3 ms) (Fig. 4Ca, orange arrow). This would indicate that inhibition of neurons in layers II/III and V/VI by the activity of the layer IV neurons is involved in depressing the vertical spread of excitation, as originally suggested by the control spatiotemporal pattern. The averaged vertical profile of the excitation spread revealed a marked synchronous excitation between neurons in layers II/III and V/VI, particularly during stage 4, in the presence of bicuculline (Fig. 4Db, asterisk). Nevertheless, the spatial extent in the horizontal profile in the insular cortex remained much smaller than in the barrel cortex (compare Figs. 3Eb, 4Eb). These results indicate that the activity of GABAA action is mainly involved in the intracolumnar inhibition in the insular cortex, rather than in the intercolumnar lateral inhibition similar to that seen in the barrel cortex.
Differential effects of bicuculline on the optical responses between the barrel and insular cortices
Between the barrel and insular cortices, the effects of bicuculline on the vertical and horizontal profiles of the maximum spread were compared. First, we defined the amplitudes in layers II/III (ROI-1) and V (ROI-2) in the vertical profile as p and q, respectively (Fig. 5Aa,b), and calculated the ratio of q/p to evaluate the vertical spread. The q/p measured at the time of the maximum vertical spread was significantly (p < 0.001) larger in the insular cortex (0.68 ± 0.10; n = 5) than in the barrel cortex (0.22 ± 0.05; n = 5) (Fig. 5B, gray and black solid bars). After application of bicuculline, the q/p in the barrel cortex significantly (p < 0.001) increased to 0.51 ± 0.03 (Fig. 5B, black solid and hollow bars), whereas the q/p in the insular cortex increased significantly (p < 0.001) but only to 0.82 ± 0.08 (Fig. 5B, gray solid and hatched bars).
Next, to compare the effects of bicuculline on the horizontal spread of excitation, we measured the width at the 90% value of the averaged maximum amplitude (0.9-max width) in the horizontal profile to estimate the maximal spatial extent of the horizontal spread of excitation (Fig. 5Ca,Cb). The averaged maximum amplitude was calculated by averaging over the amplitudes of five respective points distributed within ±40 μm from the peak point along the horizontal axis, at stage 4 (control) and stage 6 (effects of bicuculline) in the barrel cortex (Fig. 5Ca) and at stage 3 (control) and stage 4 (effects of bicuculline) in the insular cortex (Fig. 5Cb). In the absence of bicuculline, the 0.9-max width was significantly (p < 0.02) larger in the barrel cortex (392 ± 52 μm) than in the insular cortex (144 ± 56 μm) (Fig. 5D, black and gray solid bars). After bath application of bicuculline, the 0.9-max width underwent a significant (p < 0.001) increase of 940 ± 227 μm in the barrel cortex (Fig. 5D, black solid and hollow bars) without a marked increase in amplitude (Fig. 5Ca), whereas it underwent a significant (p < 0.005) but small increase of 340 ± 68 μm in the insular cortex (Fig. 5D, gray solid and hatched bars) after marked increases in the amplitudes of the optical responses (Fig. 5Cb). The extension of the horizontal spread of excitation by bicuculline was significantly (p < 0.001) larger in the barrel cortex than in the insular cortex.
In the barrel cortex, the time-to-peak of optical responses in the presence of bicuculline was significantly (p < 0.02) longer in layer V (34.2 ± 8.0 ms) than in layer II/III (23.4 ± 4.0 ms) (Fig. 3, compare violet and red arrows in Ca and Cb). This longer time-to-peak in layer V may indicate that the increase in the 0.9-max width in layer II/III by bicuculline was followed by the increases in the q/p ratio, suggesting that, in the barrel cortex, the extension of horizontal spread of excitation in layer II/III consequently enhanced the excitation in layer V. However, in the insular cortex, bicuculline similarly increased both the times-to-peak in layers II/III and V from their similar control values (Fig. 4Ca,b, orange arrows), as late as those in layer V/VI of the barrel cortex. This would suggest the involvement of mutual interaction between layers II/III and V/VI pyramidal neurons in increasing the amplitude of the optical response throughout the whole column in the insular cortex.
Thus, regardless of the absence or presence of bicuculline, there were distinct differences in the spatiotemporal pattern of excitation spread or propagation between the barrel and insular cortices, suggesting the difference at least in the excitatory local network between the two cortices. However, it is not clear whether these differences arise depending on the stimulation site or stimulus intensity. In the next series of experiments, we examined the effects of changes in the stimulus intensity and of changes in the stimulation site on the optical responses.
Effects of changes in the stimulus intensity and in the stimulation site on the optical responses in the barrel cortex
As seen in the pseudocolor images (Fig. 6A), the amplitude and the extent of horizontal excitation spread of optical responses in layer II/III at stage 4 appeared to increase with an increase in the intensity of stimulation applied to a layer IV barrel. When the intensity of stimulation applied to layer IV was linearly increased from 4.0 to 7.0 V by 1 V step in four different slice preparations, the mean maximum amplitude of optical responses at ROI-1 (Fig. 6Ba) and the mean 0.9-max width (Fig. 6Bb) at stage 4 increased in parallel. After an increase in the stimulus intensity from 4.0 to 6.0 V, the maximum amplitude of the optical response at ROI-1 increased significantly (p < 0.05) by 23 ± 13%, and the 0.9-max width also increased significantly (p < 0.01) from 315 ± 38 to 480 ± 37 μm. However, both ends of the horizontal spatial profile at the 0.9-max amplitude never exceeded the distal end of the adjacent columns.
A similar horizontal excitation spread was induced by stimulation of layer III (Fig. 6C). However, unlike those induced by layer IV stimulation (Fig. 2Bc), the optical response induced at ROI-1 by stimulation of layer III displayed a composite temporal profile (Fig. 6Ea), in which a transient plateau phase was followed by a small slow component (compare Figs. 6Ea, 2Bc). The 0.9-max width was measured just before the offset of the plateau phase (Fig. 6Ea, arrowhead). When the stimulus intensity was increased from 4.6 ± 0.6 to 6.6 ± 0.6 V (n = 4), the maximum amplitude of the optical response at ROI-1 to layer III stimulation significantly (p < 0.05) increased by 23 ± 9% (Fig. 6Ea), which was accompanied by a significant (p < 0.05) increase in the 0.9-max width from 450 ± 46 to 550 ± 91 μm (Fig. 6Gb). Although the optical responses induced by layer III stimulation tended to display a larger 0.9-max, both ends of the horizontal spatial profile at the 0.9-max amplitude never exceeded the distal end of the adjacent columns (Fig. 6C,Gb), as were the cases with layer IV stimulation (Fig. 6A). As long as the optical response displays a trapezoidal spatial profile of the horizontal excitation spread in layer II/III, the maximum amplitude of such a spatial profile may reflect the number of neurons in unit area that display synchronous activity, and the 0.9-max width may reflect the horizontal spatial extent of the area, in which layer II/III neurons display synchronous activity. Therefore, the effects of changes in the stimulus intensity on the horizontal spatial extent of synchronized area can be estimated by comparing the 0.9-max width of the horizontal spatial profiles having different amplitudes (Fig. 6Gb).
Bath application of picrotoxin markedly prolonged the temporal profiles of optical responses without markedly changing the initial responses (Fig. 6Fa), similar to the case with the bath application of bicuculline (Fig. 3Ca). Thus, the characteristic trapezoidal pattern of the horizontal spatial profile seen in response to layer IV stimulation in the barrel cortex (Fig. 3A,B,E) was preserved in response to layer III stimulation, and was not markedly affected by changing the stimulus intensity, regardless of the absence or presence of picrotoxin (Fig. 6C,D,Gb).
Next, we compared the two optical responses induced by stimulation of layer III (5.8 ± 1.8 V) and layer V (5.8 ± 0.7 V) in the same slice preparation to examine the effects of changes in the stimulation site on the optical response. In contrast to the band-like image pattern induced 10 and 16 ms after layer III stimulation (Fig. 7A), stimulation of a site of layer V in the same slice preparation induced beaded-string-like image patterns at the same timings (Fig. 7Ba). However, the same stimulation of layer V in the presence of picrotoxin caused a band-like image pattern in layer II/III similar to that caused by stimulation of layer III (Fig. 7A), only at a latency >16 ms (Fig. 7Bb). Thus, when stimulation was applied to layer V in the presence of picrotoxin, the spatial profile of an excitation spread in the horizontal direction in layer II/III was very similar to that caused by stimulation of layer IV or III (Figs. 2A, 6C), whereas the temporal profile was much slower than that caused by stimulation of layer IV or III (compare Fig. 7Da with Figs. 3Ca, 6Fa). During this slow time course, a small excitation caused in layer II/III by stimulation of layer V was amplified markedly within layer II/III (Fig. 7Ea) and spread in the horizontal direction (Fig. 7Eb), in the presence of picrotoxin. Thus, regardless of the stimulation site, a characteristic trapezoidal pattern of spatial profile of the excitation spread in the horizontal direction in layer II/III was invariably induced in the barrel cortex.
Comparison of vertical excitation spread between the optical responses induced by stimulation of layer IV and layer III with varying intensities in the insular cortex
In the insular cortex, the characteristic vertical spatial profile of excitation spread was not affected by changing stimulus intensity. Despite decreases in the intensity of stimulation applied to layer IV from 6.8 ± 1.3 to 5.1 ± 0.7 V (n = 4), the bidirectional excitation spread in the vertical direction into the superficial layer II/III and deep layer V/VI remained almost unchanged, although the amplitude of optical response was decreased (Fig. 8A–C). In contrast, the excitation initiated by stimulation of layer III appeared not to spread vertically into the deep layer V/VI but remained confined mostly within superficial layer II/III (Fig. 8D–F) regardless of the stimulus intensity changed between 5.0 ± 0.7 and 6.6 ± 0.8 V (n = 4), as indicated by the lower horizontal interrupted lines representing the border between layer III and layer V (Fig. 8D). Thus, in the insular cortex, the layer IV stimulation invariably caused the bidirectional excitation spread in the vertical direction, whereas layer III stimulation caused a much less vertical spread confined mostly within layer II/III. These were also revealed by the absence and presence of the difference in the amplitude between the temporal profiles at ROI-1 and ROI-2, in the optical responses to layer IV and layer III stimuli, respectively, regardless of stimulus intensity (Fig. 8B,E). Such differences in the vertical excitation spread between the responses to stimulation of layer IV and layer III (Fig. 8A–C,D–F, respectively) may suggest that the synaptic connection between superficial and deep layers is not simply a reciprocal one. In the next experiments, we addressed this possibility by directly comparing the two optical responses initiated by stimulation of layer III and layer V in the same slice preparation.
Consistent vertical excitation spread regardless of stimulation of layer III or layer V
In four slice preparations examined, the vertical excitation spread initiated by stimulation of layer III (6.1 ± 1.4 V) seemed to remain confined mostly within layer II/III (Fig. 9Aa), whereas that initiated by layer V stimulation (5.4 ± 1.0 V) extended from deep to superficial layers to form a narrow columnar image pattern (Fig. 9Ab). Such differential vertical excitation spreads were revealed by the differential vertical spatial profiles (Fig. 9Ea, blue and red solid curves) and also by the temporal profiles (Fig. 9Ca,Da). In contrast to the difference in the amplitude between ROI-1 and ROI-2 in response to layer III stimulation (Fig. 9Ca, compare blue and red filled circles), there was no apparent difference in the amplitude between ROI-1 and ROI-2 after the narrow columnar pattern was formed at a latency of 13 ms after stimulation of layer V (Fig. 9Da, compare blue and red filled circles). Thus, the synaptic connections between the deep and superficial layers in the insular cortex appeared not to be reciprocal.
However, in the presence of picrotoxin, the optical response measured 9 ms after layer III stimulation was markedly enhanced (Fig. 9, compare Aa, Ba; also compare filled and open circles in Ca), and then spread in the vertical direction into the deep layer V/VI, consequently displaying a wide columnar image pattern (Fig. 9Ba), as partly revealed by the vertical spatial profile (Fig. 9Eb, blue dotted curve). Picrotoxin also changed the narrow columnar pattern caused by layer V stimulation into a wide columnar one (Fig. 9Ab,Bb), which may be secondary to increases in the amplitudes of the optical responses in superficial and deep layers, as revealed by the vertical spatial profile (Fig. 9, compare red solid and dotted curves in Ea and Eb). These wide columnar image patterns in the presence of picrotoxin were very similar to those induced by stimulation of layer IV in the presence of bicuculline (Fig. 4B). Thus, a reciprocal excitatory synaptic connection between infragranular and supragranular layers emerged after removing GABAA inhibition.
In the optical responses induced by layer III stimulation in the presence of picrotoxin (Fig. 9Ca,Cb), the time-to-peak (59 ± 11 ms) (red arrowhead) of the temporal profile at ROI-2 (red open circles) was invariably later than that (43 ± 10 ms) (blue arrowhead) at ROI-1 (blue open circles), as expected from the conduction delay from the superficial layer to the deep layer. In contrast, in those induced by layer V stimulation in the presence of picrotoxin (Fig. 9Da,b), the time-to-peak at ROI-1 (71 ± 9 ms) (blue arrowhead) was unexpectedly slightly earlier than that at ROI-2 (75 ± 12 ms) (red arrowhead). Because there should be a conduction delay from the deep layer to the superficial layer as seen in the absence of picrotoxin (Fig. 9Da, red and blue filled circles), the possible difference in the time-to-peak between ROI-1 and ROI-2 might have been masked by the active interaction between layer III and layer V in the presence of picrotoxin.
Indeed, the time-to-peak at ROI-2 in the optical response induced by layer V stimulation (75 ± 12 ms) was invariably and significantly (p < 0.001) later than the time-to-peak at the same ROI-2 in the optical response induced by layer III stimulation (59 ± 11 ms). This suggests the activation of interactions or recurring inputs between layer III and layer V after the excitation spread from the deep layer to the superficial layer in response to layer V stimulation. Layer III stimulation may not have activated such interactions or recurring inputs because the amplitude of optical responses in the superficial layer reached the maximum before or not later than the complete excitation spread into the deep layer. This may be attributable to the differential synchronization mechanisms in the superficial and deep layers, as described in Discussion.
Thus, regardless of changes in the stimulation site, the insular cortex consistently displayed the characteristic columnar image pattern of excitation spread in response to stimulation of layer III, layer IV, or layer V, either in the absence or presence of picrotoxin.
Discussion
The intracolumnar and intercolumnar flow of excitation in the barrel cortex
In the barrel cortex, bicuculline had no apparent effect on the spatiotemporal patterns of the excitation spread until the excitation reached layer II/III at stage 3, but subsequently produced a slight increase in the peak amplitude and a marked increase in the spatial extent of the horizontal spread in layer II/III (Fig. 3). This would indicate that GABAA action begins immediately after the activation of layer II/III pyramidal neurons to depress the horizontal spread of excitation. This is consistent with the previous findings that layer IV stellate neurons make synaptic contacts onto pyramidal neurons, but not onto GABAergic neurons, in layer II/III (Feldmeyer et al., 2002).
In the absence of bicuculline, when the excitation reached layer II/III, it increased in strength and spread in the horizontal direction into the neighboring columns only partly, in layer II/III (Fig. 2A,B), in good agreement with the findings in the previous report (Petersen and Sakmann, 2001). However, it is reported that the axon collaterals of layer II/III pyramidal neurons extend horizontally across two or three neighboring columns (1100–1200 μm) (Lubke et al., 2003). Nevertheless, the majority of monosynaptic connections were detected between two layer II/III pyramidal neurons separated by an intercellular distance of <100–200 μm (Holmgren et al., 2003; Feldmeyer et al., 2006). Then, the excitation spread in the horizontal direction into the neighboring columns in layer II/III may be primarily mediated by disynaptic transmission between layer II/III pyramidal neurons. Such disynaptic transmissions would be more easily depressed by lateral inhibition, compared with monosynaptic transmission. Indeed, after bicuculline application, the horizontal spread in layer II/III extended remarkably beyond the distal end of the neighboring columns, during the prolonged time course from stage 4 to 6, indicative of disynaptic or polysynaptic transmission (Fig. 3). Thus, the intercolumnar synchronization in layer II/III seems to be strictly controlled by GABAA inhibition (Laaris et al., 2000; Petersen and Sakmann, 2001; Laaris and Keller, 2002), which is likely to operate in a feedforward manner after activation of layer II/III pyramidal neurons in the home column by the inputs arising from layer IV.
The intracolumnar and intercolumnar flow of excitation in the insular cortex
Our data strongly suggest that the functional column does exist in the insular cortex, because the insular cortex displayed a columnar image pattern of the excitation spread in response to stimulation of layer IV (Fig. 2C,D). Recently, it has been demonstrated by intrinsic optical imaging in rat brain that the insular cortex displays the chemotopy for the four basic tastes of sweet, salty, sour, and bitter localized sequentially along the rostrocaudal extent, although there were considerable overlaps among their representation areas (Accolla et al., 2007). Such a topographical representation would require functional columns.
In the presence of bicuculline, the spatial pattern of the excitation spread remained unchanged only at stage 1. Thereafter, however, bicuculline markedly increased the amplitude of the optical responses in layer II/III (Fig. 4Ca) and in layer V (Fig. 4Cb), which was accompanied by the extension of the width of the columnar image pattern. These observations would indicate that the two groups of GABAergic interneurons located in layers II/III and V within the same column begin to operate in parallel as soon as the excitation from layer IV reaches the respective layers, suggesting the presence of direct synaptic connections from layer IV neurons to GABAergic interneurons and pyramidal neurons in both layers. Therefore, it is likely that layer IV neurons, presumably star pyramidal neurons, in deep layer III of the insular cortex exert simultaneous feedforward inhibition on pyramidal neurons in layers II/III and V, through the activation of GABAergic interneurons in the respective layers within the same column. Indeed, the time-to-peak and amplitude of the optical response in layer V were increased, in parallel with increases in those in layer II/III, after bicuculline application. Thus, the wider columnar pattern of the excitation spread on removal of GABAergic inhibition may be simply attributable to the enlargement of the “subliminal fringe” after the enhancement of the optical response in the whole column, rather than attributable to the removal of lateral inhibition.
Distinctive and common features of columnar information processing between the barrel and insular cortices
The horizontal and vertical excitation spreads were consistently induced in the barrel and insular cortices, respectively, even by stimulation of different layers with varying intensities, indicating the distinctive features of columnar information processing in the respective cortices. Involvement of GABAA action in the columnar information processing was also distinctive between the two cortices. Therefore, the columnar information processing may not be universal across the different cortical areas.
Nevertheless, there was an important common feature in the columnar information processing between the barrel and insular cortices. Regardless of the barrel or the insular cortex, stimulation in layer III caused a prominent excitation spread radially from the stimulation site (Figs. 6C,D, 7A, 8D, 9Aa,Ba), whereas layer V stimulation never caused such a radial spread, but induced the excitation spread in layer II/III apart from the stimulation site (Figs. 7B, 9Ab,Bb). Therefore, synchronous activity appeared to be primarily caused among supragranular layer neurons, which can lead to the generation or enhancement of synchronous activity among infragranular layer neurons in the presence of bicuculline or picrotoxin (Figs. 3, 4, 7Bb, 9Bb). Indeed, in the presence of picrotoxin, the time-to-peak of the synchronized optical response in infragranular layers (ROI-2) to stimulation of layer V was invariably later than that of the synchronized optical response in infragranular layers (ROI-2) to stimulation of layer III (Fig. 9Cb,Db). This observation strongly suggests that the synchronization of neuronal activity in deep layers is secondary to the synchronous activity in superficial layers.
Such layer-dependent synchronization may be explained by the possibility that the recurrent synaptic connections among layer II/III pyramidal neurons are much stronger than those among layer V/VI pyramidal neurons. Dual recordings from pairs of layer II/III pyramidal neurons and those from pairs of layer V neurons revealed no marked differences in the mean amplitude of unitary EPSPs, release probability, and the number of release sites between the synaptic connections among layer II/III and those among layer V pyramidal neurons (Feldmeyer et al., 2006; Frick et al., 2007, 2008). Then, the amount of convergence of the recurrent collateral inputs may be larger in layer II/III pyramidal neurons than in layer V pyramidal neurons. In support of such an idea, morphological studies revealed that recurrent axon collaterals of layer II/III pyramidal neurons form much more dense terminal fields in layer II/III than those of layer V pyramidal neurons do in layer V (Feldmeyer et al., 2006; Frick et al., 2008). Thus, synchronous activity would be easily induced in supragranular layer neurons, whereas the synchronous activity of infragranular layer neurons may be secondary to that in infragranular layer neurons. This may be a universal feature of columnar information processing regardless of cortical areas.
The functional significance of columnar organization in the insular cortex
In the insular cortex, a variety of neurons that respond to taste, tactile, thermal, or nociceptive stimulation (Cechetto and Saper, 1987; Yamamoto et al., 1988; Allen et al., 1991; Hanamori et al., 1998) were layer-specifically distributed. Taste-responsive neurons together with low-threshold mechanoreceptive and wide-dynamic range neurons were mainly identified in layer V, whereas nociceptive-specific neurons were in layer II/III (Wang and Ogawa, 2002). The insular cortex is also involved in autonomic responses, because electrical stimulation of the insular cortex caused salivary secretion (Penfield and Boldrey, 1937; Cechetto and Saper, 1990), swallowing, throat sensation (Penfield and Boldrey, 1937), and changes in arterial blood pressure, heart rate (Ruggiero et al., 1987; Yasui et al., 1991), and gastric motility (Yasui et al., 1991). The neurons involved in these autonomic responses were found to be located in layer V (Ruggiero et al., 1987; Yasui et al., 1991).
Taste perception may be performed through the integration of gustatory and somesthetic sensations, such as tactile, thermal, and nociceptive ones (Katz et al., 2002). The nociceptive sensation in the oral cavity may be somehow involved in hot and spicy “taste” sensation through the activation of capsaicin receptors. If such nociceptive sensation participates in the perception of taste, the sensory information arising from diverse modalities during tasting food would be integrated within a column through the strong facilitatory interaction between neurons in layers II/III and V/VI, as observed in the present study.
Footnotes
This work was partly supported by Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Priority Areas (A) 17021027 from Japan Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology (Y.K.). We thank Hendrik W. Steenland for editing this manuscript.
References
- Accolla et al., 2007.Accolla R, Bathellier B, Petersen CC, Carleton A. Differential spatial representation of taste modalities in the rat gustatory cortex. J Neurosci. 2007;27:1396–1404. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5188-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen et al., 1991.Allen GV, Saper CB, Hurley KM, Cechetto DF. Organization of visceral and limbic connections in the insular cortex of the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1991;311:1–16. doi: 10.1002/cne.903110102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bender et al., 2003.Bender KJ, Rangel J, Feldman DE. Development of columnar topography in the excitatory layer 4 to layer 2/3 projection in rat barrel cortex. J Neurosci. 2003;23:8759–8770. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-25-08759.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cechetto and Saper, 1987.Cechetto DF, Saper CB. Evidence for a viscerotopic sensory representation in the cortex and thalamus in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1987;262:27–45. doi: 10.1002/cne.902620104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cechetto and Saper, 1990.Cechetto DF, Saper CB. Role of the cerebral cortex in autonomic functions. In: Loewy AD, Spyer KM, editors. Central regulation of autonomic functions. New York: Oxford UP; 1990. pp. 208–223. [Google Scholar]
- Chagnac-Amitai and Connors, 1989.Chagnac-Amitai Y, Connors BW. Synchronized excitation and inhibition driven by intrinsically bursting neurons in neocortex. J Neurophysiol. 1989;62:1149–1162. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.62.5.1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldmeyer et al., 1999.Feldmeyer D, Egger V, Lubke J, Sakmann B. Reliable synaptic connections between pairs of excitatory layer 4 neurones within a single “barrel” of developing rat somatosensory cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 1999;521:169–190. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.00169.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldmeyer et al., 2002.Feldmeyer D, Lubke J, Silver RA, Sakmann B. Synaptic connections between layer 4 spiny neurone-layer 2/3 pyramidal cell pairs in juvenile rat barrel cortex: physiology and anatomy of interlaminar signalling within a cortical column. J Physiol (Lond) 2002;538:803–822. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.012959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldmeyer et al., 2005.Feldmeyer D, Roth A, Sakmann B. Monosynaptic connections between pairs of spiny stellate cells in layer 4 and pyramidal cells in layer 5A indicate that lemniscal and paralemniscal afferent pathways converge in the infragranular somatosensory cortex. J Neurosci. 2005;25:3423–3431. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5227-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldmeyer et al., 2006.Feldmeyer D, Lubke J, Sakmann B. Efficacy and connectivity of intracolumnar pairs of layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in the barrel cortex of juvenile rats. J Physiol (Lond) 2006;575:583–602. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.105106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick et al., 2007.Frick A, Feldmeyer D, Sakmann B. Postnatal development of synaptic transmission in local networks of L5A pyramidal neurons in rat somatosensory cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 2007;585:103–116. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.141788. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick et al., 2008.Frick A, Feldmeyer D, Helmstaedter M, Sakmann B. Monosynaptic connections between pairs of L5A pyramidal neurons in columns of juvenile rat somatosensory cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:397–406. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray et al., 1989.Gray CM, Konig P, Engel AK, Singer W. Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature. 1989;338:334–337. doi: 10.1038/338334a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gustafsson and Jankowska, 1976.Gustafsson B, Jankowska E. Direct and indirect activation of nerve cells by electrical pulses applied extracellularly. J Physiol (Lond) 1976;258:33–61. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanamori et al., 1998.Hanamori T, Kunitake T, Kato K, Kannan H. Responses of neurons in the insular cortex to gustatory, visceral, and nociceptive stimuli in rats. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:2535–2545. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.5.2535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmgren et al., 2003.Holmgren C, Harkany T, Svennenfors B, Zilberter Y. Pyramidal cell communication within local networks in layer 2/3 of rat neocortex. J Physiol (Lond) 2003;551:139–153. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.044784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kang et al., 2004.Kang Y, Notomi T, Saito M, Zhang W, Shigemoto R. Bidirectional interactions between h-channels and Na+-K+ pumps in mesencephalic trigeminal neurons. J Neurosci. 2004;24:3694–3702. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5641-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kang et al., 2007.Kang Y, Saito M, Sato H, Toyoda H, Maeda Y, Hirai T, Bae YC. Involvement of persistent Na+ current in spike initiation in primary sensory neurons of the rat mesencephalic trigeminal nucleus. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97:2385–2393. doi: 10.1152/jn.01191.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katz et al., 2002.Katz DB, Nicolelis MA, Simon SA. Gustatory processing is dynamic and distributed. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002;12:448–454. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(02)00341-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kosar et al., 1986.Kosar E, Grill HJ, Norgren R. Gustatory cortex in the rat. I. Physiological properties and cytoarchitecture. Brain Res. 1986;379:329–341. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)90787-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laaris and Keller, 2002.Laaris N, Keller A. Functional independence of layer IV barrels. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87:1028–1034. doi: 10.1152/jn.00512.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laaris et al., 2000.Laaris N, Carlson GC, Keller A. Thalamic-evoked synaptic interactions in barrel cortex revealed by optical imaging. J Neurosci. 2000;20:1529–1537. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-04-01529.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lubke et al., 2000.Lubke J, Egger V, Sakmann B, Feldmeyer D. Columnar organization of dendrites and axons of single and synaptically coupled excitatory spiny neurons in layer 4 of the rat barrel cortex. J Neurosci. 2000;20:5300–5311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-14-05300.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lubke et al., 2003.Lubke J, Roth A, Feldmeyer D, Sakmann B. Morphometric analysis of the columnar innervation domain of neurons connecting layer 4 and layer 2/3 of juvenile rat barrel cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13:1051–1063. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.10.1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore et al., 1999.Moore CI, Nelson SB, Sur M. Dynamics of neuronal processing in rat somatosensory cortex. Trends Neurosci. 1999;22:513–520. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(99)01452-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura and Ogawa, 1997.Nakamura T, Ogawa H. Neural interaction between cortical taste neurons in rats: a cross-correlation analysis. Chem Senses. 1997;22:517–528. doi: 10.1093/chemse/22.5.517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogawa et al., 1991.Ogawa H, Ohgushi M, Hasegawa K, Murayama N. Differential development of cortical taste areas in granular and dysgranular insular cortices in rats. Brain Res Dev Brain Res. 1991;60:271–274. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(91)90057-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogawa et al., 1992.Ogawa H, Murayama N, Hasegawa K. Difference in receptive field features of taste neurons in rat granular and dysgranular insular cortices. Exp Brain Res. 1992;91:408–414. doi: 10.1007/BF00227837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penfield and Boldrey, 1937.Penfield W, Boldrey E. Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain. 1937;60:389–443. [Google Scholar]
- Petersen and Sakmann, 2000.Petersen CC, Sakmann B. The excitatory neuronal network of rat layer 4 barrel cortex. J Neurosci. 2000;20:7579–7586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-20-07579.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen and Sakmann, 2001.Petersen CC, Sakmann B. Functionally independent columns of rat somatosensory barrel cortex revealed with voltage-sensitive dye imaging. J Neurosci. 2001;21:8435–8446. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-21-08435.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roy and Alloway, 1999.Roy S, Alloway KD. Synchronization of local neural networks in the somatosensory cortex: a comparison of stationary and moving stimuli. J Neurophysiol. 1999;81:999–1013. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.3.999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruggiero et al., 1987.Ruggiero DA, Mraovitch S, Granata AR, Anwar M, Reis DJ. A role of insular cortex in cardiovascular function. J Comp Neurol. 1987;257:189–207. doi: 10.1002/cne.902570206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saito et al., 2006.Saito M, Murai Y, Sato H, Bae YC, Akaike T, Takada M, Kang Y. Two opposing roles of 4-AP-sensitive K+ current in initiation and invasion of spikes in rat mesencephalic trigeminal neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:1887–1901. doi: 10.1152/jn.00176.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang and Ogawa, 2002.Wang X, Ogawa H. Columnar organization of mechanoreceptive neurons in the cortical taste area in the rat. Exp Brain Res. 2002;147:114–123. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1228-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolsey and Van der Loos, 1970.Woolsey TA, Van der Loos H. The structural organization of layer IV in the somatosensory region (SI) of mouse cerebral cortex. The description of a cortical field composed of discrete cytoarchitectonic units. Brain Res. 1970;17:205–242. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(70)90079-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto et al., 1980.Yamamoto T, Matsuo R, Kawamura Y. Localization of cortical gustatory area in rats and its role in taste discrimination. J Neurophysiol. 1980;44:440–455. doi: 10.1152/jn.1980.44.3.440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto et al., 1984.Yamamoto T, Azuma S, Kawamura Y. Functional relations between the cortical gustatory area and the amygdala: electrophysiological and behavioral studies in rats. Exp Brain Res. 1984;56:23–31. doi: 10.1007/BF00237438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto et al., 1985.Yamamoto T, Yuyama N, Kato T, Kawamura Y. Gustatory responses of cortical neurons in rats. III. Neural and behavioral measures compared. J Neurophysiol. 1985;53:1370–1386. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.53.6.1370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto et al., 1988.Yamamoto T, Matsuo R, Kiyomitsu Y, Kitamura R. Sensory inputs from the oral region to the cerebral cortex in behaving rats: an analysis of unit responses in cortical somatosensory and taste areas during ingestive behavior. J Neurophysiol. 1988;60:1303–1321. doi: 10.1152/jn.1988.60.4.1303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yasui et al., 1991.Yasui Y, Breder CD, Saper CB, Cechetto DF. Autonomic responses and efferent pathways from the insular cortex in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1991;303:355–374. doi: 10.1002/cne.903030303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yokota and Satoh, 2001.Yokota T, Satoh T. Three-dimensional estimation of the distribution and size of putative functional units in rat gustatory cortex as assessed from the inter-neuronal distance between two neurons with correlative activity. Brain Res Bull. 2001;54:575–584. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00464-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]