Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2019 Aug 6;8:e39495. doi: 10.7554/eLife.39495

Neuropeptide B mediates female sexual receptivity in medaka fish, acting in a female-specific but reversible manner

Towako Hiraki-Kajiyama 1,2, Junpei Yamashita 1, Keiko Yokoyama 1, Yukiko Kikuchi 1, Mikoto Nakajo 1,3, Daichi Miyazoe 1, Yuji Nishiike 1, Kaito Ishikawa 1, Kohei Hosono 1, Yukika Kawabata-Sakata 1,4, Satoshi Ansai 5,6, Masato Kinoshita 5, Yoshitaka Nagahama 7, Kataaki Okubo 1,
Editors: Catherine Dulac8, Joel K Elmquist9
PMCID: PMC6684226  PMID: 31383257

Abstract

Male and female animals display innate sex-specific mating behaviors. In teleost fish, altering the adult sex steroid milieu can effectively reverse sex-typical mating behaviors, suggesting remarkable sexual lability of their brains as adults. In the teleost medaka, neuropeptide B (NPB) is expressed female-specifically in the brain nuclei implicated in mating behavior. Here, we demonstrate that NPB is a direct mediator of estrogen action on female mating behavior, acting in a female-specific but reversible manner. Analysis of regulatory mechanisms revealed that the female-specific expression of NPB is dependent on direct transcriptional activation by estrogen via an estrogen-responsive element and is reversed in response to changes in the adult sex steroid milieu. Behavioral studies of NPB knockouts revealed that female-specific NBP mediates female receptivity to male courtship. The female-specific NPB signaling identified herein is presumably a critical element of the neural circuitry underlying sexual dimorphism and lability of mating behaviors in teleosts.

Research organism: Other

Introduction

From invertebrates to humans, males and females of a given species exhibit profound differences in mating behavior; in general, males perform elaborate courtship displays to attract females for mating, and females evaluate male courtship displays to decide whether to mate. Such differences in mating behavior between males and females result from sexually dimorphic development and activation of the underlying neural circuits (Yang and Shah, 2016). In vertebrates, sex-typical mating behaviors and the underlying neural circuits are highly dependent on the sex steroid hormone milieu, which is unique to each sex (McCarthy and Arnold, 2011; McCarthy et al., 2017). Males have higher circulating levels of androgen derived from the testis, whereas females have a cyclic pattern of estrogen and progestin arising from the ovary. Sex steroids largely exert their effects by binding to intracellular receptors, which subsequently interact with sex steroid-responsive elements in the genome to modulate the transcription of target genes. Recent work, particularly in rodents and songbirds, has identified sex steroid-responsive neural circuits that underlie the divergent mating behaviors of males versus females, as well as the genes downstream of sex steroids that mediate these behaviors. However, the identity of the sex-specific direct transcriptional targets of sex steroids that mediate sex-typical mating behaviors remains elusive (Yang and Shah, 2014).

It is generally viewed that differences between the sexes in mating behavior are robust and essentially irreversible in mammals and birds. Several studies, however, have challenged this view and showed that they exhibit some degree of sexual lability in mating behavior (Edwards and Burge, 1971; Södersten, 1972; Adkins-Regan, 2009; Balthazart et al., 2010; Ball et al., 2014; Kimchi et al., 2007). Interestingly, sexual lability in mating behavior can be seen much more frequently and completely in teleost fish, where experimental manipulations that alter the sex steroid milieu effectively lead to the reversal of sex-typical mating behaviors, even in adulthood (Munakata and Kobayashi, 2010; Paul-Prasanth et al., 2013; Göppert et al., 2016; Ghosal and Sorensen, 2016). Furthermore, a large number of teleost species spontaneously undergo phenotypic sex reversal, involving both behavioral and anatomical changes, in response to social and physiological stimuli (Liu et al., 2017; Capel, 2017). Despite several studies, the neural mechanisms responsible for the reversal of sex-typical mating behaviors remain unknown.

In the teleost species medaka (Oryzias latipes), we previously found that estrogen and androgen receptors are expressed almost exclusively in females in two brain regions: the supracommissural/posterior nucleus of the ventral telencephalic area (Vs/Vp) and the magnocellular/gigantocellular portion of the magnocellular preoptic nucleus (PMm/PMg) (Hiraki et al., 2012) (Figure 1A,B). These regions are therefore likely to represent female-specific target sites for both estrogen and androgen in the teleost brain. This finding, together with classic lesion and stimulation studies suggesting that Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg are involved in mating behavior (Demski et al., 1975; Kyle and Peter, 1982; Koyama et al., 1984; Satou et al., 1984), led us to search for genes that are directly activated by estrogen and/or androgen in these brain regions and mediate female-typical mating behavior. As a result, we identified a candidate gene, npb (encoding neuropeptide B; NPB), whose expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg is virtually confined to females and dependent on gonadal estrogen (Hiraki et al., 2014). NPB, together with its close relative, neuropeptide W (NPW), was originally identified as a ligand for the orphan receptors GPR7 and GPR8 (now designated NPBWR1 and NPBWR2) (Fujii et al., 2002; Brezillon et al., 2003; Tanaka et al., 2003). NPB and NPW have been implicated in a wide array of physiological processes, including food intake, energy homeostasis, inflammatory pain response, regulation of pituitary hormones, and social interaction (Sakurai, 2013; Watanabe and Yamamoto, 2015); to our knowledge, however, there is no information regarding their role in mating behavior.

Figure 1. Sexually dimorphic npba expression is independent of sex chromosome complement but dependent on direct transcriptional activation by estrogen.

(A) Lateral view (anterior to left) of the medaka brain showing approximate levels of sections in panel B. For abbreviations of brain regions, see Supplementary file 1. (B) Coronal sections showing the location of Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg. (C) Levels of npba expression in the whole brain of artificially sex-reversed XX males/XY females, and wild-type XY males/XX females as determined by real-time PCR (n = 8 per group). ***, p<0.001 (Bonferroni’s post hoc test). (D) Representative micrographs of npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of sex-reversed XX males/XY females and wild-type XY males/XX females (n = 5 per group). Scale bars represent 50 μm. (E) Levels of npba expression in the whole brain of male and female mutants lacking the estrogen-responsive element in the npba promoter (ΔERE), and wild-type (WT) males and females as determined by real-time PCR (n = 6 per group, except WT females, where n = 5). ***, p<0.001 (Bonferroni’s post hoc test). (F) Total area of npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of ΔERE (n = 5) and WT (n = 4) females. *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01 (unpaired t-test). (G) Levels of npba expression in the whole brain of ΔERE (beige columns) and WT (blue columns) females that were sham-operated (Sham), ovariectomized (OVX), or ovariectomized and treated with estradiol-17β (OVX +E2) as determined by real-time PCR (n = 6 per group). There were significant main effects of genotype (F (1, 30)=45.03, p<0.0001) and treatment (F (2, 30)=60.19, p<0.0001) and a significant interaction between genotype and treatment (F (2, 30)=9.944, p=0.0005). *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001 (Bonferroni’s post hoc test). See also Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Genetic scheme for the ΔERE mutant medaka.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Mutant medaka in which an estrogen-responsive element (ERE) in the npba promoter was deleted (designated ΔERE mutant medaka) were generated by transcription activator-like effector nuclease (TALEN)-mediated genome editing. (A) Structure of the npba locus showing the position of the deleted ERE, which is expanded below to indicate the nucleotide sequences of the wild-type (WT) and ΔERE alleles. ERE is indicated by white letters on a black background. TALEN binding sites are underlined and deleted nucleotides are indicated by dashes. (B) Comparison of the nucleotide sequences of WT ERE and ΔERE, the deletion of which is both left- and right-aligned. Nucleotides identical to the consensus ERE sequence are indicated by white letters on a black background.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Levels of estradiol-17β (E2) in the brains of ∆ERE mutant and wild-type (WT) females.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

n = 4 per group.

In the present study, we investigated whether the sexually dimorphic expression of npb (renamed npba after the discovery of its paralog named npbb) in the medaka brain is directly elicited by estrogen, can be reversed by changes in the adult sex steroid milieu, and is relevant to female-typical mating behavior. Our results demonstrate that NPB acts directly downstream of estrogen in a female-specific but reversible manner to mediate female sexual receptivity.

Results

Sexually dimorphic npba expression is independent of sex chromosome complement but dependent on direct transcriptional activation by estrogen

First, we estimated the magnitude of chromosomal and hormonal influences on the sexually dimorphic expression of npba. We addressed whether the pattern of npba expression coincides with the chromosomal sex and/or gonadal sex by producing sex-reversed medaka and examining npba expression in their brains. Real-time PCR performed on the whole brain revealed that sex-reversed XY females exhibited the same high level of overall npba expression as wild-type XX females, whereas sex-reversed XX males showed a much lower level of overall npba expression (p<0.0001 versus XX and XY females), comparable to that in wild-type XY males (Figure 1C). Consistent with these results, in situ hybridization analysis revealed abundant npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of sex-reversed XY females, similar to wild-type XX females, whereas npba expression in these brain nuclei was not detected in either sex-reversed XX males or wild-type XY males (Figure 1D). Thus, the sexually dimorphic pattern of npba expression is independent of sex chromosome complement, but linked to gonadal phenotype.

Next, we investigated whether estrogen directly activates the transcription of npba in vivo by generating and analyzing ΔERE mutant medaka lacking the estrogen-responsive element (ERE) in the npba promoter, which has been shown to be functional in vitro (Hiraki et al., 2014) (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Real-time PCR performed on the whole brain demonstrated that ΔERE mutant females had a significantly lower level of overall npba expression as compared with wild-type females (p<0.0001), whereas ΔERE mutant males had an even lower level (p<0.0001 versus wild-type and ΔERE mutant females), comparable to that in wild-type males (Figure 1E). In agreement with these results, in situ hybridization analysis revealed a smaller total area of npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of ΔERE mutant females as compared with wild-type females (p=0.0202 for Vs/Vp, and 0.0058 for PMm/PMg) (Figure 1F), whereas npba expression in these nuclei was not detected in ΔERE or wild-type males. We also evaluated npba expression in the whole brain of ΔERE mutant females that were sham-operated, ovariectomized, or ovariectomized and treated with estradiol-17β (E2; the primary estrogen in vertebrates including teleosts) by real-time PCR. In the mutant female brain, npba expression was significantly reduced by ovariectomy and restored by E2 treatment, but E2 was not able to restore npba expression to the level present in wild-type females (p=0.0023 for the mutant versus wild-type females) (Figure 1G). Taken together, these results demonstrate that estrogen elicits the female-specific expression of npba by direct transcriptional activation through the ERE present in the npba promoter, although other mechanisms are also likely involved in this estrogen effect.

The possibility that decreased npba expression in ∆ERE mutant females was due to a decrease in estrogen levels in their brains was ruled out by the observation that ∆ERE mutant females had brain levels of E2 comparable to those of wild-type females (p=0.782) (Figure 1—figure supplement 2).

Sexually dimorphic npba expression can be reversed by altering the sex steroid milieu

We determined whether the sexual phenotype of npba expression can be reversed from the female to the male pattern, and vice versa, by altering the adult sex steroid milieu. In a previous study, we showed that npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of females was abolished by ovariectomy but then restored by estrogen replacement (Hiraki et al., 2014). Here, we first tested whether npba expression in these brain nuclei can be induced in males by castration and sex steroid supplementation. Indeed, in situ hybridization results showed that treating castrated males with E2 induced npba expression in both Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg (p=0.0013 for Vs/Vp and 0.0006 for PMm/PMg) (Figure 2—figure supplement 1).

The effects of altering the sex steroid milieu on the sexually dimorphic pattern of npba expression was further evaluated in fish with intact gonads. In situ hybridization results showed that treating ovary-intact adult females continuously with 11-ketotestosterone (KT; the most prominent, non-aromatizable androgen in teleosts) caused a gradual decline in npba expression in Vs/Vp, decreasing both the number of npba-expressing neurons (p<0.0001 on days 5 and 9) and the total area of npba expression (p=0.0002 on day 2; p<0.0001 on days 5 and 9) to undetectable or nearly undetectable levels over the treatment period (Figure 2A–C). A significant decline in the area of npba expression was also observed in PMm/PMg (p=0.0068 for the total area on day 9), although the number of npba-expressing neurons was unchanged (Figure 2A–C). Treating ovary-intact adult females with an aromatase inhibitor (AI; aromatase converts androgen to estrogen) yielded essentially the same results (p<0.0001 for number of neurons in Vs/Vp on days 2, 5, and 9; p=0.0063 and 0.0025 for total area in Vs/Vp on days 5 and 9, respectively; p=0.0108 for total area in PMm/PMg on day 9) (Figure 2D–F). To aid in interpreting these results, we measured brain levels of E2 in females treated with KT in exactly the same way as for the npba expression analysis. The results showed that E2 levels fell to less than 5% of untreated controls within 2 days and remained low during the remainder of the treatment period (p<0.0001 on days 2, 5, and 9), demonstrating a substantial decrease in E2 levels in the brain of KT-treated females (Figure 2—figure supplement 2).

Figure 2. Sexually dimorphic npba expression can be reversed by altering the sex steroid milieu.

Temporal changes in npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of 11-ketotestosterone (KT)-treated females (A, B, C), aromatase inhibitor (AI)-treated females (D, E, F), and estradiol-17β (E2)-treated males (G, H, I) (n = 5 per treatment and sampling day). (A, D, G) Number of npba-expressing neurons in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg. ***, p<0.001 (versus day 0, Dunnett’s post hoc test). (B, E, H) Total area of npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg. *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001 (versus day 0, Dunnett’s post hoc test). (C, F, I) Representative micrographs showing npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg. Scale bars represent 50 μm. See also Figure 2—figure supplement 1 and Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Effects of castration and sex steroid supplementation on npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of males.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Number of npba-expressing neurons in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of sham-operated males (Sham) and castrated males that were exposed to vehicle alone (CX), 11-ketotestosterone (CX + KT), or estradiol-17β (CX + E2) (n = 5 per group). *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001 (Bonferroni’s post hoc test). (B) Representative micrographs of npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of Sham, CX, CX + KT, and CX + E2 treated males. Scale bars represent 50 μm.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Levels of estradiol-17β (E2) in the brains of females treated with 11-ketotestosterone (KT).

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

n = 4 per sampling day. ***, p<0.001 (versus day 0, Dunnett’s post hoc test).

In contrast, treating testis-intact adult males continuously with E2 resulted in the induction of npba expression in both Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg, where both the number of npba-expressing neurons and the total area of npba expression steadily increased over time during treatment (p<0.0001 for number of neurons in Vs/Vp on days 2, 5, and 9; p=0.0013 and<0.0001 for total area in Vs/Vp on days 5 and 9, respectively; p<0.0001 for number of neurons in PMm/PMg on day 9; p=0.0023 for total area in PMm/PMg on day 9) (Figure 2G–I). Thus, the sexually dimorphic pattern of npba expression can be reversed in both directions in response to changes in the sex steroid milieu.

Female-specific Npba likely acts widely in the brain and spinal cord

Next, we investigated the sites of action of the Npba peptide produced specifically by females. First, the axonal projections of npba-expressing neurons were determined by immunohistochemical detection of Npba and compared between the sexes. The specificity of the anti-Npba antibody was ascertained by a double-labeling experiment with immunohistochemistry and in situ hybridization, where it selectively identified neurons expressing the npba transcript (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). In addition, this antibody was found not to cross-react with Npbb as confirmed by the complete absence of labeling with this antibody in npba knockout females (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Although the focus of the present study is female-specific subsets of npba-expressing neurons in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg and these neurons account for a large proportion of overall npba expression in the female brain, npba is expressed in both sexes in several brain nuclei, including Pbl in the ventrolateral preoptic area (Hiraki et al., 2014). Consistent with this fact, Npba-immunoreactive axons arising from the neuronal cell bodies in Pbl and extending through the hypothalamus into the pituitary were observed in both sexes (Figure 3A). In females, additional Npba-immunoreactive axons were found in many areas of the central nervous system, with particular abundance in the thalamus, hypothalamus, optic tectum, midbrain tegmentum, medulla oblongata, and spinal cord (Figure 3A).

Figure 3. Female-specific Npba likely acts widely in the brain and spinal cord.

(A) Comparison of the distribution of Npba-immunoreactive axons in the brain, pituitary, and spinal cord between males and females. All images are sagittal sections with anterior to the left. Arrows and arrowheads indicate female-specific Npba-immunoreactive neuronal cell bodies in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg, respectively. Asterisks indicate Npba-immunoreactive neuronal cell bodies in Pbl occurring in both sexes. Scale bars represent 500 μm. For abbreviations of brain regions, see Supplementary file 1. (B) Comparison of the distribution of GFP-labeled axons in the brain, pituitary, and spinal cord between npba-GFP transgenic males and females. Images in the left and middle panels are lateral views with anterior to the left; images in the right panels are horizontal views with anterior to the left. Arrows and arrowheads indicate female-specific GFP-labeled neuronal cell bodies in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg, respectively. Scale bars represent 500 μm. (C) Phylogenetic tree of NPBWR1 and NPBWR2. The number at each node indicates bootstrap values for 1000 replicates. Human opioid receptors μ1 (OPRM1) and δ1 (OPRD1) were used as the outgroup for tree reconstruction. Scale bar represents 0.1 substitution per site. For species names and GenBank accession numbers, see Supplementary file 3. (D) Distribution of npbwr2 expression in the brain, pituitary, and spinal cord. All images are coronal sections. Images of only males are presented, because there were no obvious sex differences in the distribution of expression (n = 5 per sex). Scale bars represent 100 μm. For abbreviations of brain and spinal cord regions and brain nuclei, see Supplementary file 1. See also Figure 3—figure supplement 1, Figure 3—figure supplement 2, Figure 3—figure supplement 3, and Figure 3—figure supplement 4.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Verification of the specificity of the anti-Npba antibody.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) The specificity of the anti-Npba antibody was verified by a pattern of labeling consistent with npba-expressing neurons detected by in situ hybridization in the medaka brain. Left and middle panels show images of immunohistochemistry (IHC) using the anti-Npba antibody (green) and in situ hybridization (ISH) detecting npba expression (magenta), respectively, in the same sections; right panels show the merged images with nuclear counterstaining (blue). Arrowheads indicate representative neuronal cell bodies labeled by both IHC and ISH. The IHC signals that do not overlap with the ISH signals most likely represent the axons of Npba-expressing neurons (but not the cell bodies of other neurons), given their relatively small size and typical varicosity-like structures. Scale bars represent 50 μm. (B) The lack of cross-reactivity of the anti-Npba antibody with Npbb was confirmed by the observation that labeled cell bodies and axons (green) were present in npbb knockout (npbb-/-) as well as wild-type (WT) females, npbb-/- but totally absent in npba knockout (npba-/-) females (n = 15, 9, and eight for wild-type, npba-/-, and npbb-/- females). Representative micrographs of PMm/PMg, which contains neurons expressing both npba and npbb, and PPv/DP/CP, which contains hundreds of npbb-expressing neurons but no npba-expressing neurons, are shown. Scale bars represent 50 μm. For abbreviations of brain nuclei, see Supplementary file 1.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Generation and verification of npba-GFP transgenic medaka.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(A) Structure of the transgene in npba-GFP transgenic medaka. A 22 bp sequence containing the translation initiation site of npba in a medaka bacterial artificial chromosome (BAC) clone (clone ID: 180_I09) was replaced by a 2136 bp DNA cassette containing the humanized Renilla reniformis GFP II (hrGFPII)-coding sequence, bovine growth hormone polyadenylation signal (BGH pA), and kanamycin resistance gene (Km). This BAC clone contains the whole transcriptional unit of npba together with 38 kb of 5′-flanking and 21 kb of 3′-flanking sequence. (B) The specificity of GFP expression was verified by double in situ hybridization (ISH) of GFP and npba in the brain of npba-GFP transgenic medaka. Left and middle panels show images of GFP (green) and npba expression (magenta), respectively, in the same sections; right panels show the merged images with nuclear counterstaining (blue). Note that GFP fluorescence remained visible after processing for ISH. Arrowheads indicate representative neuronal cell bodies labeled by both GFP and npba ISH. Scale bars represent 50 μm. For abbreviations of brain nuclei, see Supplementary file 1.
Figure 3—figure supplement 3. Distribution of npba-expressing neurons in the medaka medulla oblongata and spinal cord.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

(A) Distribution of npba-expressing neurons in the medulla oblongata and the anterior part of the spinal cord of males (blue columns) and females (beige columns) (n = 5 per sex). (B) Representative micrographs showing npba-expressing neurons in the respective regions of the medulla oblongata and anterior part of the spinal cord. Scale bars represent 100 μm. (C) Number of npba-expressing neurons in the middle to posterior part of the spinal cord (mpSC) of males (blue columns) and females (beige columns) (n = 3 per sex). The number of neurons per 30 coronal sections of 10 μm thickness was counted. (D) Representative micrographs showing npba-expressing neurons in mpSC. Scale bars represent 100 μm. For abbreviations of brain and spinal cord regions and brain nuclei, see Supplementary file 1.
Figure 3—figure supplement 4. Sequence information for medaka npbwr2.

Figure 3—figure supplement 4.

(A) Nucleotide and deduced amino acid sequences of the medaka npbwr2 cDNA. Asterisk indicates the stop codon. Nucleotide numbers are shown at the right of each sequence line. (B) Alignment of deduced amino acid sequences of Npbwr2 from medaka and other vertebrates. Identical amino acids in all sequences are shaded in beige. For species names and GenBank accession numbers, see Supplementary file 3.

Axonal projections were additionally evaluated by generating and examining transgenic medaka in which npba-expressing neurons were labeled with GFP (npba-GFP transgenic medaka). The specificity of GFP expression was confirmed by double in situ hybridization detecting GFP and endogenous npba transcripts, which demonstrated that the distribution of the two transcripts almost completely overlapped (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Fluorescence imaging of the transgenic fish yielded results that were fairly consistent with immunohistochemistry: that is, a subset of axons common to both sexes and a subset specific to females were apparent (Figure 3B). In addition, imaging of whole brains and spinal cords revealed that the female-specific axons reached the posterior end of the spinal cord and that all female-specific axons most probably originated from the female-specific npba-expressing neurons in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg.

However, we could not rule out the possibility that additional female-specific npba-expressing neurons were present in the medulla oblongata and/or spinal cord (regions that have not previously been studied) and were the source of the female-specific axons therein. Accordingly, we surveyed npba-expressing neurons in these regions by in situ hybridization. Although a large number of npba-expressing neurons were widely scattered over the medulla oblongata and spinal cord, all of these subsets of neurons were common to both sexes and were not confined to, or predominant in, females (Figure 3—figure supplement 3), thereby showing that the female-specific axons did not originate from these regions.

The sites of action of Npba were further delineated by identifying the gene encoding the NPB/NPW receptor (NPBWR) in medaka and assessing the spatial distribution of its expression. Screening of an expressed sequence tag (EST) library generated from medaka brain, followed by 5′-rapid amplification of cDNA ends (5′-RACE), led to the isolation of a full-length cDNA of 4045 bp (deposited in DDBJ/EMBL/GenBank with accession number LC375958), whose deduced amino acid sequence shared a high degree of identity with NPBWR2 in other species (Figure 3—figure supplement 4). Phylogenetic tree analysis demonstrated that this cDNA encoded a medaka protein orthologous to NPBWR2 and paralogous to NPBWR1 of other species (Figure 3C). No other NPBWR1/NPBWR2-like genes were found in our EST library or public medaka genome/EST databases, indicating the absence of NPBWR1 in medaka as in zebrafish (Danio rerio) (Bu et al., 2016).

In situ hybridization identified npbwr2 expression in the following nuclei in both males and females: Vv, Vl, and Vs/Vp in the ventral telencephalon; Dm/Dl in the dorsal telencephalon; PMp, PPa, and PPp in the preoptic area, rHd and lHd in the habenula; VM and DP/CP in the thalamus; PGZ3 in the optic tectum; NDLI, NAT, NVT, NRL, NPT, and NRP in the hypothalamus; IR/MR/IQ and DT/TS/is in the midbrain tegmentum; ra and gc/RS in the brain stem; the peripheral region of the anterior pituitary; and the medial part of the dorsal horn and the lateral part of the ventral horn throughout the spinal cord (Figure 3D; abbreviations of brain and spinal cord regions and brain nuclei are defined in Supplementary file 1). There were no overt differences in the distribution pattern of npbwr2 expression between the sexes.

Collectively, these results suggest that Npba that is produced female-specifically in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg is transported to and acts in many different brain areas and the spinal cord.

Medaka possess an additional NPB gene, designated npbb

Via a survey of the medaka genome and EST databases, we identified a previously uncharacterized gene encoding a protein highly homologous to, but distinct from, Npba. Structural analysis of the protein sequence predicted a mature NPB polypeptide at residues 26–54, which shared a high degree of sequence identity with known mature NPB polypeptides in medaka and other species (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). The protein sequence was also predicted to contain a signal peptide at the N-terminus with a cleavage site between residues 25 and 26 (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Phylogenetic tree analysis indicated that this protein represents an additional NPB and was thus designated Npbb with re-designation of the original Npb as Npba (Figure 4A).

Figure 4. Medaka possess an additional NPB gene, designated npbb.

(A) Phylogenetic tree of NPB and NPW. The number at each node indicates bootstrap values for 1000 replicates. Scale bar represents 0.1 substitution per site. For species names and GenBank accession numbers, see Supplementary file 3. (B) Ability of medaka Npba and Npbb to activate medaka Npbwr2. Receptor activation was assessed by measuring cAMP-responsive element-driven luciferase activity in cells transfected with Npbwr2. The x-axis shows the concentration of Npba and Npbb, and the y-axis shows luciferase activity as a percentage of that observed in the absence of Npba/Npbb. (C) Overall levels of npbb expression in the male (blue columns) and female (beige columns) brain dissected into three parts as determined by real-time PCR (n = 6 per sex). *, p<0.05 (unpaired t-test). For abbreviations of brain regions, see Supplementary file 1. (D) Distribution of npbb-expressing neurons in the male (blue columns) and female (beige columns) brain (n = 5 per sex). The data are split into three graphs for clarity. *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001 (unpaired t-test). For abbreviations of brain nuclei, see Supplementary file 1. (E) Total area of npbb expression in PMm/PMg/PPp in males (blue column) and females (beige column) (n = 5 per sex). ***, p<0.001 (unpaired t-test). (F) Representative micrographs showing npbb-expressing neurons in different brain nuclei. Micrographs of both sexes are shown for Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg/PPp, where npbb expression is confined or almost confined to females. Micrographs of males only are shown for other nuclei, where sex differences were not detected or, if present, were not sufficiently demonstrated by photographs. (G) Coexpression of npba and npbb in the same neurons in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg, but not in other brain nuclei. In each set of panels, the left and middle ones show images of npba (green) and npbb (magenta) expression, respectively, in the same sections; the right ones show the merged images with nuclear counterstaining (blue). Arrowheads indicate representative neurons coexpressing npba and npbb. Scale bars represent 50 μm. For abbreviations of brain nuclei, see Supplementary file 1. See also Figure 4—figure supplement 1 and Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Sequence information for medaka npbb.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) Nucleotide and deduced amino acid sequences of the medaka npbb cDNA. The predicted signal peptide is underlined and the mature Npbb polypeptide is boxed. Asterisk indicates the stop codon. Nucleotide numbers are shown at the right of each sequence line. (B) Comparison of mature NPB polypeptide sequences from medaka and other vertebrate species. Identical amino acids in all sequences are shaded in beige. For species names and GenBank accession numbers, see Supplementary file 3.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Comparison of syntenic relationships of genes in the vicinity of gar, arowana, catfish, and medaka NPB genes.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

Genes are represented by arrows denoting the direction of transcription. gnav1, guanine nucleotide-binding protein (G protein) α v1; mafg, MAF bZIP transcription factor G; pcyt2, phosphate cytidylyltransferase 2, ethanolamine; rnf213, ring finger protein 213; sgsh, N-sulfoglucosamine sulfohydrolase; sirt7, sirtuin 7; slc26a11, solute carrier family 26 member 11; sox9, SRY (sex determining region Y)-box 9; sstr2, somatostatin receptor 2.

Database searches using the medaka NPB genes as queries led to the identification of two distinct NPB genes in many other teleost species, including arowana (Scleropages formosus), catfish (Ictalurus punctatus), salmon (Salmo salar), trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss), pufferfish (Tetraodon nigroviridis), and tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus), while only one NPB gene was identified in non-teleost species, including gar (Lepisosteus oculatus), a non-teleost ray-finned fish. Comparative synteny analysis of NPB genes in the gar, basal (early-branching) teleost (arowana and catfish), and medaka genomes revealed that, in the basal teleost and medaka genomes, npba is located in a segment of conserved synteny containing sstr2, sox9, rnf213, pcyt2, and mafg, while npbb is in a segment of conserved synteny containing slc26a11, sgsh, gnav1, and pcyt2. On the other hand, the NPB gene in the gar genome is located in a segment containing all of these genes (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). These lines of evidence strongly suggest that the two NPB genes arose from the whole-genome duplication that occurred early in teleost evolution (Amores et al., 1998). No other NPB/NPW-like genes were found in our EST library or public medaka genome/EST databases, indicating the absence of NPW in medaka as in zebrafish (Bu et al., 2016). Receptor activation assays revealed that both Npba and Npbb dose-dependently activated medaka Npbwr2, resulting in a decrease in intracellular cAMP levels, as reported for NPBWR1/2 of other species (Fujii et al., 2002; Tanaka et al., 2003). Npbb displayed greater potency than Npba with a 50% inhibitory concentration (IC50) of 4.3 nM versus 240 nM (Figure 4B).

Next, we examined the spatial expression of npbb with a focus on possible differences between the sexes and colocalization with npba expression. First, the relative levels of regional npbb expression were measured by real-time PCR and compared between male and female brains dissected into three parts: (i) the olfactory bulb, telencephalon, diencephalon, and midbrain tegmentum; (ii) the optic tectum; and (iii) the cerebellum and medulla oblongata.Expression of npbb was observed in all three parts, but was highest in the optic tectum, where males had slightly higher levels than females (p=0.014) (Figure 4C). Subsequent examination by in situ hybridization revealed that npbb was expressed in the following nuclei: Vs/Vp in the ventral telencephalon; Pbl, PMp, and PMm/PMg/PPp in the preoptic area (note that npbb was continuously expressed across PMm/PMg and PPp, whereas npba was expressed separately in PMm/PMg and PPp); VL and PPv/DP/CP in the pretectum/thalamus; NVT, NRP, NPT, and PGc in the hypothalamus; DT/IR/MR and IQ/RT in the midbrain tegmentum; gc, ra/ttb, and RS in the brain stem; and OT and PGZ3 in the optic tectum (Figure 4D,F). A marked difference between the sexes was observed in Vs/Vp (p<0.0001), where npbb expression was observed in females, but completely absent in males (Figure 4D,F). Similarly, npbb-expressing neurons in PMm/PMg/PPp were more abundant in number and displayed a much larger total area of expression in females than in males (p=0.0011 for number of neurons; p<0.0001 for total area) (Figure 4D–F). In contrast, in VL and PGZ3, a marginally higher number of npb-expressing neurons were seen in males than in females (p=0.020 for VL, and 0.045 for PGZ3) (Figure 4D).

The simultaneous detection of npba and npbb expression showed that these genes are expressed in the same subset of neurons within Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg/PPp; in contrast, no neurons in other brain nuclei were found to express both genes (Figure 4G). Taken together, these findings show that there is an additional NPB gene, npbb, in medaka that is coexpressed with npba in a female-biased manner in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg/PPp.

Sexually dimorphic npbb expression can also be reversed by altering the sex steroid milieu

Following identification of npbb, we investigated whether the sexually dimorphic pattern of npbb expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg/PPp can be reversed by altering the adult sex steroid milieu, as observed for npba. First, we evaluated the effects of sex steroids on npbb expression by means of gonadectomy and sex steroid supplementation, followed by in situ hybridization analysis. In adult females, ovariectomy caused a significant reduction in npbb expression in both Vs/Vp (p=0.0271) and PMm/PMg/PPp (p=0.0041), which was restored by treatment with E2 (p=0.0091 for Vs/Vp, and 0.0173 for PMm/PMg) but not by KT (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). In adult males, E2 treatment following castration induced a few npbb-expressing neurons in Vs/Vp (p<0.0001), whereas npbb expression in PMm/PMg/PPp did not show clear responses to any treatment (Figure 5—figure supplement 1).

The effects of the altered sex steroid milieu on npbb expression was further tested in fish with intact gonads. Both KT and AI treatment of ovary-intact adult females resulted in a gradual decline in npbb expression in Vs/Vp, both in the number of npbb-expressing neurons and in the total area of npbb expression, to undetectable or nearly undetectable levels (p<0.0001 for number of neurons in KT-treated fish on days 5 and 9; p=0.0235,<0.0001, and <0.0001 for total area in KT-treated fish on days 2, 5, and 9, respectively; p=0.0002 and<0.0001 for number of neurons in AI-treated fish on days 5 and 9, respectively; p=0.0013 and 0.0009 for total area in AI-treated fish on days 5 and 9, respectively) (Figure 5A–F). KT treatment, but not AI treatment, also led to a significant decrease in npbb expression in PMm/PMg/PPp (p=0.0038 for number of neurons in KT-treated fish on day 9; p=0.0021 and<0.0001 for total area in KT-treated fish on days 5 and 9, respectively) (Figure 5A–F). E2 treatment of testis-intact adult males caused no clear changes in npbb expression in either Vs/Vp or PMm/PMg/PPp, but a few npbb-expressing neurons were newly induced in Vs/Vp (p=0.0369) (Figure 5G–I). Collectively, these results show that, similar to npba expression, the sexually dimorphic pattern of npbb expression can be reversed, at least in part, in response to changes in the sex steroid milieu.

Figure 5. Sexually dimorphic npbb expression can also be reversed by altering the sex steroid milieu.

Temporal changes in npbb expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of 11-ketotestosterone (KT)-treated females (A, B, C), aromatase inhibitor (AI)-treated females (D, E, F), and estradiol-17β (E2)-treated males (G, H, I) (n = 5 per treatment and sampling day). (A, D, G) Number of npbb-expressing neurons in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg. *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001 (versus day 0, Dunnett’s post hoc test). (B, E, H) Total area of npbb expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg. *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001 (versus day 0, Dunnett’s post hoc test). (C, F, I) Representative micrographs showing npbb expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg. Scale bars represent 50 μm. See also Figure 5—figure supplement 1 and Figure 5—figure supplement 2.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Effects of gonadectomy and sex steroid supplementation on npbb expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A) Number of npbb-expressing neurons in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg/PPp of sham-operated females (Sham) and ovariectomized females that were exposed to vehicle alone (OVX), 11-ketotestosterone (OVX + KT), or estradiol-17β (OVX + E2) (n = 5 per group). *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01 (Bonferroni’s post hoc test). (B) Representative micrographs of npbb expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg/PPp of Sham, OVX, OVX + KT, and OVX + E2 females. Scale bars represent 50 μm. (C) Number of npbb-expressing neurons in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg/PPp of Sham males and castrated males that were exposed to vehicle alone (CX), 11-ketotestosterone (CX + KT), or estradiol-17β (CX + E2) (n = 5 per group). ***, p<0.001 (Bonferroni’s post hoc test). (D) Representative micrographs of npbb expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg/PPp of Sham, CX, CX + KT, and CX + E2 males. Scale bars represent 50 μm.
Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Putative estrogen-responsive elements (EREs) predicted in the proximal promoter regions of gar, arowana, catfish, medaka, and tilapia NPB genes.

Figure 5—figure supplement 2.

Ocher circles represent putative EREs. Numbers indicate nucleotide positions relative to the translation initiation site (+1).

Search for putative EREs in the npbb promoter led us to identify a putative ERE at a similar position as in the npba promoter. In addition, the npb promoter in gar and the npba/npbb promoters in several teleost species examined were also found to contain a putative ERE at a similar position (Figure 5—figure supplement 2).

Npba/Npbb/Npbwr2 signaling is involved in female sexual receptivity

To examine the role of npba and npbb in female mating behavior, we generated npba knockout (npba-/-) and npbb knockout (npbb-/-) medaka by transcription activator-like effector nuclease (TALEN)-mediated genome editing (Figure 6—figure supplement 1) and observed the mating behavior of females. Most homozygous females of both knockout strains spawned successfully, similar to wild-type and heterozygous females. However, subsequent quantitative behavioral testing uncovered a latent abnormality in npba knockout strain. The mating behavior of medaka consists of a sequence of stereotyped actions that are easily quantified (Ono and Uematsu, 1957; Walter and Hamilton, 1970). The sequence begins with the male approaching and following the female closely. The male then performs a courtship display, in which he swims quickly in a circular pattern in front of the female. If she is receptive, the male grasps her with his dorsal and anal fins (termed ‘wrapping’), and they quiver together (termed ‘quivering’) until eggs and sperm are released. If the female is not receptive, she either assumes a rejection posture in which she raises her head or rapidly moves away from the male. Here, we found a significant increase in latencies from the first courtship display to the first wrapping and to the wrapping that resulted in spawning for npba-/- females as compared with npba+/+ and npba+/- females (p=0.0256 and 0.0002, respectively, for the latency to the first wrapping; p=0.0104 and 0.0026, respectively, for the latency to the wrapping that resulted in spawning) (Figure 6A). In contrast, npbb-/- females compared with controls showed no significant difference in any of the behavioral parameters (Figure 6B). To further evaluate the role of NPB signaling, we generated npba/npbb double knockout (npba-/-/npbb-/-) and npbwr2 knockout (npbwr2-/-) medaka (Figure 6—figure supplement 1) and assessed the mating behavior of these females. Homozygous females, as well as males, of both strains were found to be fertile, sexually active, and to spawn successfully. However, as was the case with npba-/- females, a significant increase in the latency from the first courtship display to the first wrapping was observed for both npba-/-/npbb-/- and npbwr2-/- females as compared with wild-type females (p=0.0064 for npba-/-/npbb-/-, and 0.0002 for npbwr2-/-) (Figure 7A). npbwr2-/- females also showed a significant increase in latency from the first courtship display to the wrapping that resulted in spawning (p=0.0013) (Figure 7A). Although not statistically significant, npba-/-/npbb-/- females showed a similar trend (p=0.0609) (Figure 7A). In addition, there was a significant increase in the proportion of females that spawned without any preceding courtship display from the male for both npba-/-/npbb-/- (p=0.0405) and npbwr2-/- (p=0.0010) strains (Figure 7A).

Figure 6. npba is involved in female sexual receptivity.

(A, B) Various parameters in the mating behavior of npba (A) and npbb (B) single knockout females were measured and compared with wild-type females (n = 34, 33, and 31 for npba+/+, npba+/-, and npba-/- females, respectively; n = 29, 33, and 32 for npbb+/+, npbb+/-, and npbb-/- females, respectively). Blue, ocher, and beige columns represent wild-type, heterozygous knockout, and homozygous knockout females, respectively. *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001 (Bonferroni’s post hoc test). (C, D) The latency data for npba (C) and npbb (D) single knockouts were further analyzed using Kaplan-Meier plots. Blue triangles, ocher diamonds, and beige circles represent wild-type, heterozygous knockout, and homozygous knockout females, respectively. *, p<0.05 (Gehan-Breslow-Wilcoxon test with Bonferroni’s correction). See also Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Genetic scheme for the npba-/-, npbb-/-, and npbwr2-/- mutant medaka.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

Knockout medaka deficient for npba (A, B), npbb (C, D), and npbwr2 (E, F) were generated by transcription activator-like effector nuclease (TALEN)-mediated genome editing. (A) Gene structure of npba showing the location of the deletion target site, which is enlarged to show the nucleotide sequences of the npba+/+ and npba-/- alleles. The mature Npba polypeptide-coding sequence is indicated in white letters on a black background. TALEN binding sites are underlined, and deleted nucleotides are indicated by dashes. (B) Comparison of the deduced Npba precursor protein sequences of the npba+/+ and npba-/- alleles. The mature Npba polypeptide is indicated in white letters on a black background. The altered sequence caused by a frameshift is shaded in gray. Asterisks indicate stop codons. (C) Gene structure of npbb showing the location of the deletion target site, which is enlarged to show the nucleotide sequences of the npbb+/+ and npbb-/- alleles. The mature Npbb polypeptide-coding sequence is indicated in white letters on a black background. TALEN binding sites are underlined and deleted nucleotides are indicated by dashes. (D) Comparison of the deduced Npbb precursor protein sequences of the npbb+/+ and npbb-/- alleles. The mature Npbb polypeptide is indicated in white letters on a black background. The altered sequence caused by a frameshift is shaded in gray. Asterisks indicate stop codons. (E) Gene structure of npbwr2 showing the location of the deletion target site, which is enlarged to show the nucleotide sequences of the npbwr2+/+ and npbwr2-/- alleles. TALEN binding sites are underlined and deleted nucleotides are indicated by dashes. (F) Comparison of the deduced Npbwr2 protein sequences of the npbwr2+/+ and npbwr2-/- alleles. The altered sequence caused by a frameshift is shaded in gray. Asterisks indicate stop codons.

Figure 7. Npba/Npbb/Npbwr2 signaling is involved in female sexual receptivity.

Figure 7.

(A) Various parameters in the mating behavior of npba/npbb double knockout (npba-/-/npbb-/-) females (n = 31) and npbwr2 knockout (npbwr2-/-) females (n = 38) were measured and compared with wild-type females (n = 55). Blue, ocher, and beige columns represent wild-type, npba/npbb double knockout, and npbwr2 knockout females, respectively. *, p<0.05; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001 (Dunnett’s post hoc test for data on number, latency, and duration; Fisher’s exact test for data on percentage). (B) The latency data were further analyzed using Kaplan-Meier plots. Blue triangles, ocher diamonds, and beige circles represent wild-type, npba/npbb double knockout, and npbwr2 knockout females, respectively. *, p<0.05; ***, p<0.001 (Gehan-Breslow-Wilcoxon test with Bonferroni’s correction). See Video 1, Video 2, and Video 3. Supplementary Data List.

Video 1. A representative video showing the mating behavior of wild-type females.

Download video file (9.6MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39495.022

Video 2. A representative video showing the mating behavior of npba/npbb double knockout females.

Download video file (11.8MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39495.023

Video 3. A representative video showing the mating behavior of npbwr2 knockout females.

Download video file (11.8MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39495.024

Behavioral time-series data sets were further analyzed using Kaplan-Meier plots with the inclusion of fish that did not spawn within the test period. Although the significant difference disappeared in the latency from the first courtship display to the wrapping that resulted in spawning between npba+/- and npba-/-, all other significant differences detected were also detected by this analysis, supporting the robustness of our results (the latency to the first wrapping; p=0.0480 for npba+/+ versus npba-/- females, p=0.0291 for npba+/- versus npba-/- females, p=0.0162 for wild-type versus npba-/-/npbb-/- females, p<0.0002 for wild-type versus npbwr2-/- females: the latency to the wrapping that resulted in spawning; p=0.0129 for npba+/+ versus npba-/- females, p=0.1158 for npba+/- versus npba-/- females, p=0.1068 for wild-type versus npba-/-/npbb-/- females, p=0.0006 for wild-type versus npbwr2-/- females) (Figure 6C,D and Figure 7B). Collectively, these results indicate that NPB signaling affects female receptivity to male courtship.

Discussion

In a previous study, we showed that npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg, two brain nuclei implicated in mating behavior, is specific to females in medaka (Hiraki et al., 2014). That finding, together with the fact that sex differences in mating behavior can be reversed in teleosts, led us to investigate whether npba has a role in female-typical mating behavior and whether its sex-specific expression pattern may be reversed between males and females. The present results indicate that NPB signaling constitutes a female-specific but reversible component of the female sexual receptivity pathway.

Our previous study showed that female-specific npba expression is dependent on gonadal estrogen (Hiraki et al., 2014). In mammals and birds, many sex differences in behavior and gene expression in the brain are robustly influenced by sex chromosome complement, with chromosomal sex occasionally exerting an influence as large as that of gonadal sex steroids (McCarthy and Arnold, 2011; Forger et al., 2016). It therefore seemed possible that sex chromosome complement as well as gonadal estrogen might contribute to the female-specific expression of npba in medaka brain. Here, however, we found that the sexually dimorphic pattern of npba expression is independent of sex chromosome complement, and relies exclusively on gonadal sex steroids. The lack of sex chromosome effects certainly underlies the reversibility of the sexually dimorphic npba expression.

We also showed that the female-specific expression of npba results, at least in part, from direct transcriptional activation of npba by estrogen via an ERE present in its promoter. Consistent with this, female-specific npba-expressing neurons coexpress estrogen receptors (ERs) (Hiraki et al., 2014). The identification of npba as a direct target of estrogen action is interesting because very few genes involved in female mating behavior in any species have been found to be direct targets of estrogen, although such genes can be highly estrogen-responsive (Yang and Shah, 2014). The exceptions are the genes encoding the progesterone receptor, oxytocin, and oxytocin receptor; however, those genes are expressed in behaviorally relevant brain regions of both males and females, and affect the mating behaviors of both sexes, in contrast to npba in medaka (Wagner, 2006; Yang et al., 2013; Veening et al., 2015; Dumais and Veenema, 2016; Pfaff et al., 2018). Collectively, our findings indicate that the female-specific expression of npba has a distinct regulatory mechanism.

Another distinctive aspect of npba expression is the bidirectional reversibility of sexual dimorphism (both from female-typical to male-typical pattern and from male-typical to female-typical pattern). We found that npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg disappeared in females and appeared in males in response to changes in the adult sex steroid milieu, in contrast to genes that exhibit sexually dimorphic expression and mediate sex-typical mating behaviors in mammals. In mice, for example, the cholecystokinin receptor gene (Cckar) implicated in female sexual receptivity is expressed in the ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus at much higher levels in females owing to estrogen stimulation. Although Cckar expression in this nucleus is reduced by ovariectomy and reinstated by treatment with estrogen in adult females, treating adult males with estrogen does not induce expression of Cckar in this nucleus (Xu et al., 2012). Such irreversibility of sexually dimorphic gene expression in the adult brain is attributed to the specific action of gonadal sex steroids during the perinatal period, in addition to the sex chromosome effects mentioned above. Studies in rodents indicate that perinatal sex steroids have long-lasting or even permanent effects on brain gene expression and behavior via epigenetic mechanisms involving DNA methylation and histone modifications, which establish and maintain differences between the sexes across the lifespan (McCarthy and Nugent, 2015; Forger et al., 2016; McCarthy et al., 2017). The reversible sex difference in adult npba expression in medaka suggests either that perinatal sex steroids induce few, if any, epigenetic modifications in npba or that such modifications can easily be reversed for some reason. This unique mode of sexual differentiation of npba expression probably contributes to the adult reversal of sex-typical mating behaviors observed in teleosts. Given that mammals and birds also exhibit some degree of sexual lability in mating behavior, behavior-relevant genes for which expression in the adult brain is sexually dimorphic but bidirectionally reversible in response to sex steroids may remain to be identified.

Importantly, our results provide insight into the mechanisms underlying the reversal of sexually dimorphic npba expression. The profound decrease in npba expression observed in ovary-intact females treated with non-aromatizable androgen indicates that androgen/androgen receptor (AR) signaling plays an inhibitory role in npba expression, alongside the stimulatory role of estrogen/ER signaling. However, expression of npba was not induced in males deprived of androgen by castration, suggesting that the androgen/AR signaling does not directly inhibit npba expression. Considering a concomitant decrease in brain levels of E2 in androgen-treated males, the androgen/AR signaling pathway presumably exerts its influence by attenuating the stimulatory action of estrogen through a reduction of brain E2 levels. Given that ER expression in Vs/Vp and PMp/PMg is strongly suppressed by androgen (Hiraki et al., 2012), the inhibitory action of androgen on npba expression may involve repression of estrogen/ER signaling not only at ligand, but also at receptor level.

The estrogenic induction of npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of males showed a slower kinetic response than would be expected of a model of direct transcriptional activation. However, we previously found that ERs are not expressed at detectable levels in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of males and that treating males with estrogen induces ER expression therein (Hiraki et al., 2012). Considering these findings, it seems plausible that this estrogen-induced npba expression is not fully realized until sufficient ER expression is achieved.

The brains of teleosts exhibit extensive adult neurogenesis (Chapouton et al., 2007; Ganz and Brand, 2016) and estrogen has significant effects on adult neurogenesis (Mahmoud et al., 2016; Heberden, 2017; Ponti et al., 2018). These facts suggest the possibility that adult neurogenesis may be involved in the reversal of sexually dimorphic npba expression. However, our recent work showed that the Npba-expressing neurons emerged in adult males treated with estrogen, not through neurogenesis, but through the activation of pre-existing, quiescent male counterpart neurons (Kikuchi et al., 2019). This evidence suggests the relevance of an activation–inactivation process on existing neurons, rather than neurogenesis, in the reversible sexual dimorphism in npba expression.

We found that the female-specific npba-expressing neurons project their axons to various regions of the central nervous system, particularly the brainstem and spinal cord, but not to the pituitary. The latter observation is surprising, given that most of the isotocin neurons that are intermingled with the female-specific npba-expressing neurons in PMm/PMg project to the pituitary in teleosts including medaka (Hiraki et al., 2014; Yamashita et al., 2017). Consistent with the widespread projection of npba-expressing neurons, npbwr2 was found to be widely expressed in the central nervous system, corroborating the idea that the female-specific Npba peptide has multiple action sites. In the spinal cord, npbwr2 was expressed in the medial part of the dorsal horn and the lateral part of the ventral horn, which respectively contain sympathetic preganglionic neurons and motoneurons in teleosts as well as in mammals (Westerfield et al., 1986; Schneider and Sulner, 2006; Funakoshi and Nakano, 2007). Thus, the female-specific Npba probably acts on either or both of these neurons in the spinal cord, as well as in various neuronal subsets in the brain. Although NPBWR1/2 primarily couples to the inhibitory G protein (Gi) (Fujii et al., 2002; Tanaka et al., 2003), activation of NPBWR1 reportedly leads to increased excitability of some neuronal populations in rats (Price et al., 2009). Thus, whether Npbwr2 activation has inhibitory or stimulatory effects on each type of neuronal population in medaka remains to be defined.

Our study identified a second functional NPB gene, designated npbb, in medaka. Phylogenetic and syntenic analyses indicated that this gene probably originated from the teleost-specific whole-genome duplication (Amores et al., 1998) and is thus confined to teleosts. Similar to npba, npbb was found to be preferentially expressed in females in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg/PPp; in addition, npba and npbb were expressed in the same neurons in these brain nuclei, suggesting that the two genes share common regulatory mechanisms. Indeed, the sexually dimorphic expression of npbb in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg/PPp could also be reversed by altering the sex steroid milieu, although the changes were less marked as compared with npba. In addition, a putative ERE, though not yet proven to be functional, was found in the npbb promoter at a similar position as in the npba promoter, suggesting that npbb is also directly activated by estrogen and that a cis-regulatory region that confers estrogen-responsive NPB gene expression is evolutionary conserved. This idea is further supported by the observation that NPB genes in gar and several other teleosts also contain a putative ERE at a similar position. The results also suggested that the two genes have a shared function in female mating behavior; indeed, part of the altered behavioral phenotypes was evident in npba-/-/npbb-/- females, but not in single knockouts of either of the two genes, presumably due to functional redundancy. Importantly, coexpression of npba and npbb was observed exclusively in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg/PPp and not in other brain nuclei, which strongly suggests that their expression in these nuclei is responsible for the phenotypes observed.

npba-/-, npba-/-/npbb-/-, and npbwr2-/- females showed increased latency from courtship display to wrapping, and furthermore, npba-/-/npbb-/- and npbwr2-/- females showed increased incidence of spawning without any preceding courtship display. These results illustrate the relevance of NPB signaling to female sexual receptivity. However, the two phenotypes are seemingly inconsistent in that the increased latency from courtship to mating may be associated with decreased receptivity, whereas the increased incidence of spawning without receiving courtship may be associated with increased receptivity. The most likely explanation is that the deficiency of NPB signaling reduces the significance of courtship display in female receptivity. In medaka, as in other animals, courtship display serves to stimulate female receptivity (Grant et al., 1995; Grant and Green, 1996). Conceivably, courtship display is somehow less important and less effective for females deficient in NPB signaling; in other words, they require more time to accept males after receiving courtship stimulation, but simultaneously tend to accept males without being courted. Thus, female-specific NPB signaling may play an important role in female mate choice by facilitating the acceptance of males performing courtship display and the refusal of males exhibiting no courtship.

This raises the question of what specific process in female mate choice is primarily influenced by female-specific NPB signaling. The answer remains to be found, but Npbwr1-/- mice have been shown to exhibit behavioral abnormalities in social interaction: when confronted by a conspecific intruder, they approach rapidly and spend more time close to the intruder (Nagata-Kuroiwa et al., 2011). In addition, a single nucleotide polymorphism in human NPBWR1 has been shown to influence the evaluation of facial expressions (Watanabe et al., 2012). These observations, together with the extensive effective area of female-specific NPB signaling in the brain, suggest that NPB signaling may be involved in the process of perceiving and/or evaluating male performance during courtship. Alternatively, given that the spinal cord is the major target site of female-specific NPB signaling, it is possible that NPB signaling, by influencing motor and/or sympathetic function, may be involved in executing the decision of whether or not to mate.

In summary, the present study has identified NPB as a critical element of the female sexual receptivity circuitry in medaka, exhibiting reversible sexual dimorphism under the direct control of estrogen. The female-specific NPB neurons are located in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg, regions that are considered homologous to the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis/subpallial amygdala and the supraoptic nucleus/paraventricular nucleus, respectively, in the mammalian brain (Northcutt, 1995; Moore and Lowry, 1998; O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011; Goodson and Kingsbury, 2013). Similar to medaka, NPB/NPW neurons have been detected in these nuclei in mammals (Dun et al., 2003; Jackson et al., 2006). Thus, the role of NPB in female sexual receptivity may be conserved across a wide array of vertebrate taxa. Further studies will be required to test this possibility.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type
(species) or resource
Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional
information
Gene (Oryzias latipes) npba DOI: 10.1210/en.2013-1806 Genbank:NM_001308979
Gene
(O. latipes)
npbb NBRP Medaka clone ID:olova57o22; clone ID:olova6h09; clone ID:olova8d12; clone ID:olova13m04; clone ID:olova28d23; clone ID:olova58i17
Gene (O. latipes) npbwr2 Genbank:LC375958
Gene (O. latipes) actb Genbank:NM_001104808
Strain, strain background (O. latipes) d-rR NBRP Medaka strain ID:MT837 maintained in a closed colony over 10 years in Okubo lab
Genetic reagent (O. latipes) ΔERE mutant this paper generated and maintained in Okubo lab
Genetic reagent (O. latipes) npba-GFP transgenic this paper generated and maintained in Okubo lab
Genetic reagent (O. latipes) npba-/- this paper generated and maintained in Okubo lab
Genetic reagent (O. latipes) npbb-/- this paper generated and maintained in Okubo lab
Genetic reagent (O. latipes) npbwr2-/- this paper generated and maintained in Okubo lab
Cell line (Homo sapiens) HEK293T Riken BRC Cell Bank Cell number:RCB2202; RRID:CVCL_0063
Cell line (Escherichia coli) DY380 DOI: 10.1038/35093556; DOI: 10.1006/geno.2000.6451
Transfected construct pcDNA3.1/V5-His-TOPO Thermo Fisher Scientific Thermo Fisher Scientific:K480001
Transfected construct pGL4.29 Promega Promega:E8471
Transfected construct pGL4.74 Promega Promega:E6921
Antibody anti-Npba antibody this paper RRID:AB_2810229 rabbit polyclonal; against the entire Npba polypeptide (1:500 or 1:1000)
Antibody Dylight 549-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG Vector Laboratories Vector Laboratories:DI-1549;
RRID:AB_2336407
(1:500 or 1:1000)
Antibody Alexa Flour 488-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG Thermo Fisher Scientific Thermo Fisher
Scientific:A-11070; RRID:AB_2534114
(1:500)
Antibody horseradish peroxidase-conjugated anti-fluorescein antibody PerkinElmer PerkinElmer:NEF710001EA; RRID:AB_2737388 (1:500 or 1:1000)
Antibody alkaline phosphatase-conjugated anti-DIG antibody Roche Diagnostics Roche Diagnostics:11093274910; RRID:AB_514497 (1:500–1:10000)
Recombinant DNA reagent medaka bacterial artificial chromosome (BAC) clone NBRP Medaka clone ID:180_I09
Recombinant DNA reagent phrGFP II-1 mammalian expression vector Agilent Technologies Agilent Technologies:240143
Recombinant DNA reagent pGEM-Teasy vector Promega Promega:A1360
Peptide, recombinant protein Npba polypeptide this paper WYKQVAGPSYYSVGRASGLLSGIRRSPHV-NH2
Peptide, recombinant protein Npbb polypeptide this paper WYKQSTGPIFYPVGRASGLLSGIRRSPYV-NH2
Commercial assay or kit Dual-Luciferase Reporter Assay System Promega Promega:E1910
Commercial assay or kit RNeasy Lipid Tissue Mini Kit Qiagen Qiagen:74804
Commercial assay or kit RNeasy Plus Universal Mini Kit Qiagen Qiagen:73404
Commercial assay or kit Omniscript RT Kit Qiagen Qiagen:205111
Commercial assay or kit SuperScript VILO cDNA Synthesis Kit Thermo Fisher Scientific Thermo Fisher Scientific:11754050
Commercial assay or kit LightCycler 480 SYBR Green I Master Roche Diagnostics Roche Diagnostics:04887352001
Commercial assay or kit Golden Gate TALEN and TAL Effector Kit 2.0 Addgene Addgene:1000000024
Commercial assay or kit mMessage mMachine SP6 Kit Thermo Fisher Scientific Thermo Fisher Scientific:AM1340
Commercial assay or kit Marathon cDNA Amplification Kit Takara Bio Takara Bio:634913
Commercial assay or kit Power SYBR Green PCR Master Mix Thermo Fisher Scientific Thermo Fisher Scientific:4367659
Commercial assay or kit TSA Plus Fluorescein System PerkinElmer PerkinElmer:NEL741001KT
Chemical compound, drug aromatase inhibitor (AI); Fadrozole Sigma-Aldrich Sigma-Aldrich:F3806-10MG
Chemical compound, drug estradiol-17β; E2 Fujifilm Wako Pure Chemical Corporation Fujifilm Wako Pure Chemical Corporation:058–04043
Chemical compound, drug 11-ketotestosterone; KT Cosmo Bio Cosmo Bio:117 ST
Software, algorithm GraphPad Prism GraphPad Software RRID:SCR_002798
Software, algorithm Adobe Photoshop Adobe Systems RRID:SCR_014199
Software, algorithm ImageJ http://rsbweb.nih.gov/ij/ RRID:SCR_003070
Software, algorithm InterProScan http://www.ebi.ac.uk/interpro/search/sequence-search RRID:SCR_005829
Software, algorithm SignalP http://www.cbs.dtu.dk/services/SignalP/ RRID:SCR_015644
Software, algorithm ClustalW http://clustalw.ddbj.nig.ac.jp/index.php RRID:SCR_017277
Software, algorithm Jaspar http://jaspar.genereg.net/ RRID:SCR_003030
Other DAPI stain Thermo Fisher Scientific Thermo Fisher Scientific:D1306; RRID:AB_2629482 (1:1000)

Animals

Medaka of the d-rR strain were bred and maintained at 28°C under a 14 hr light/10 hr dark photoperiod. They were fed 3–4 times per day with live brine shrimp and commercial pellet food (Otohime; Marubeni Nissin Feed, Tokyo, Japan). Sexually mature, spawning adults (aged 3–5 months) were used for all analyses. They were randomly assigned to experimental groups. All sampling was conducted at 1–4 hr after onset of the light period.

Production of sex-reversed medaka

Sex-reversed XX males and XY females were produced as described previously (Okubo et al., 2011). In brief, fertilized eggs were treated with methyltestosterone at high temperature (32°C) for the production of XX males, or with E2 at 28°C for the production of XY females.

Real-time PCR

The brain was sampled whole (excluding the cerebellum and medulla oblongata in ∆ERE mutant analysis) or dissected into three parts (the olfactory bulb, telencephalon, diencephalon, and midbrain tegmentum; the optic tectum; and the cerebellum and medulla oblongata), and total RNA was isolated by using the RNeasy Lipid Tissue Mini Kit or RNeasy Plus Universal Mini Kit (Qiagen, Hilgen, Germany). Complementary DNA was synthesized by using the Omniscript RT Kit (Qiagen) or SuperScript VILO cDNA Synthesis Kit (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA). Real-time PCR was run either on the ABI Prism 7000 Sequence Detection System using the Power SYBR Green PCR Master Mix (Thermo Fisher Scientific) or on the LightCycler 480 System II using the LightCycler 480 SYBR Green I Master (Roche Diagnostics, Basel, Switzerland). For every reaction, melting curve analysis was conducted to ensure that a single amplicon was produced in each sample. The β-actin gene (actb; GenBank accession number NM_001104808) was used to normalize levels of npba/npbb transcripts in each sample. The primers used for real-time PCR are listed in Supplementary file 2. The numbers of fish examined in each experimental group are indicated in the figure legends.

Single in situ hybridization

DNA fragments of 645 bp, 647 bp, and 1520 bp corresponding to nucleotides 1–645 of the medaka npba cDNA (GenBank accession number NM_001308979), 1–647 of the medaka npbb EST clone of National BioResource Project (NBRP) Medaka (http://www.shigen.nig.ac.jp/medaka/) (clone ID: olova57o22), and 1699–3218 of the medaka npbwr2 cDNA (deposited in DDBJ/EMBL/GenBank with accession number LC375958), respectively, were PCR-amplified and subcloned into the pGEM-Teasy vector (Promega, Madison, WI). The resultant constructs were used to generate digoxigenin (DIG)-labeled cRNA probes for npba, npbb, and npbwr2 by using the DIG RNA Labeling Mix and T7 RNA polymerase (Roche Diagnostics). The procedure for in situ hybridization has been described previously (Hiraki et al., 2012). In brief, the brain, pituitary, and/or spinal cord were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA), embedded in paraffin, and cut into 10 μm sections in the coronal plane. Hybridization signals were visualized by using alkaline phosphatase-conjugated anti-DIG antibody (Roche Diagnostics) and 5-bromo-4-chloro-3-indolyl phosphate/nitro blue tetrazolium (BCIP/NBT) substrate (Roche Diagnostics). Color development was allowed to proceed overnight or was stopped after 2–3 hr (for quantification of npba expression in Vs/Vp) or 15–30 min (for quantification of npba expression in PMm/PMg) to avoid saturation. All sections in each comparison were processed simultaneously under the same conditions. The numbers of fish examined in each experimental group are indicated in the figure legends.

To obtain quantitative data, the number of npba/npbb-expressing neurons in each brain nucleus or each brain/spinal cord region was counted manually. In some analyses, the resulting sections were photographed and converted to black and white binary images by thresholding using Adobe Photoshop (Adobe Systems, San Jose, CA), and the total area of npba/npbb expression was calculated by using ImageJ (http://rsbweb.nih.gov/ij/). The threshold was set manually by visual inspection of the images to successfully separate specific signals from background noise. Brain nuclei were identified by using medaka brain atlases (Anken and Bourrat, 1998; Ishikawa et al., 1999); http://www.shigen.nig.ac.jp/medaka/medaka_atlas/), supplemented with information from our Nissl-stained sections (Kawabata et al., 2012).

Generation of the ere mutant medaka

Mutant medaka in which an ERE sequence in the npba promoter was deleted (ΔERE) were generated by TALEN-mediated genome editing. TALENs were designed to target the ERE that has been shown to be functional in vitro (Hiraki et al., 2014). The position and sequence of the targeted ERE and TALEN binding sites are shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 1. The TALE repeat arrays were assembled by the Golden Gate method (Cermak et al., 2011) as described elsewhere (Ansai et al., 2013), using the Golden Gate TALEN and TAL Effector Kit 2.0 (Addgene 1000000024). TALEN mRNAs were synthesized by in vitro transcription using the mMessage mMachine SP6 Kit (Thermo Fisher Scientific) and microinjected into the cytoplasm of embryos at the one-cell stage. Potential founders were screened by outcrossing to wild-type fish and testing progeny for mutations at the target site using a mismatch-sensitive T7 endonuclease I assay (Kim et al., 2009) followed by direct sequencing. A founder was identified that yielded a high proportion of progeny carrying a 7 bp deletion; the progeny were subsequently intercrossed to generate fish homozygous for the deletion. The genotype of each fish was determined by direct sequencing using the primers listed in Supplementary file 2. In all analyses of ∆ERE mutants, their wild-type siblings were used as controls.

Gonadectomy and drug treatment

For both male and female fish, the gonad was removed under tricaine methane sulfonate anesthesia (0.02%) through a small incision made in the ventrolateral abdominal wall. Immediately after removal of the gonad, the incision was sutured with nylon thread. Sham-operated fish received the same surgical treatment as gonadectomized fish, except for removal of the gonad. After a recovery period of 3 days, gonadectomized males and females were immersed in water containing 100 ng/ml of KT or E2, or vehicle (ethanol) alone for 6 days and then sampled. Sham-operated fish were treated with vehicle alone and used as controls.

In another experiment, females with intact ovaries were treated with 100 ng/ml of KT or AI (Fadrozole; Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) by immersion in water for 9 days. Similarly, males with intact testes were treated with 100 ng/ml of E2 by immersion in water for 9 days. These fish were sampled on days 0 (untreated controls), 2, 5, and 9. The sex steroid concentration used was based on previously reported serum steroid levels in medaka (Foran et al., 2002; Foran et al., 2004; Tilton et al., 2003).

Measurement of brain levels of E2

Dissected whole brains were frozen and stored at −80°C until analysis. Levels of E2 in the brains were analyzed by liquid chromatography coupled to tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS) at Aska Pharmamedical (Kanagawa, Japan). In the KT treatment experiment, E2 levels were measured over the same time course as that used to assess npba/npbb expression (on days 0 (untreated controls), 2, 5, and 9 of treatment).

Double labeling with immunohistochemistry and in situ hybridization

A polyclonal anti-Npba antibody was raised in rabbit against a synthetic peptide corresponding to the entire mature polypeptide sequence of medaka Npba (Medical and Biological Laboratories, Aichi, Japan). The specificity of the anti-Npba antibody in immunohistochemical studies was confirmed by a double-labeling experiment with immunohistochemistry and in situ hybridization. In brief, whole brains of females (n = 6) were fixed in 4% PFA and embedded in 5% agarose (Type IX-A; Sigma-Aldrich) supplemented with 20% sucrose. Frozen coronal sections of 20 μm thickness were cut and hybridized with the above-mentioned npba probe, which was labeled with fluorescein by using Fluorescein RNA Labeling Mix and T7 RNA polymerase (Roche Diagnostics). After blocking with PBS containing 2% normal goat serum for 30 min, the sections were incubated overnight at 4°C with the anti-Npba antibody diluted 1:500 in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) containing 2% normal goat serum, 0.1% bovine serum albumin, and 0.02% keyhole limpet hemocyanin. The sections were incubated overnight at 4°C with horseradish peroxidase-conjugated anti-fluorescein antibody (diluted 1:1000 and 1:500 for detection in PMm/PMg and other nuclei, respectively; PerkinElmer, Waltham, MA) and Dylight 549-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG (diluted 1:500; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) in Tris-buffered saline (TBS) containing 1.5% blocking reagent (Roche Diagnostics) and 5 μg/ml 4′,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole (DAPI). The anti-fluorescein antibody was visualized by using the TSA Plus Fluorescein System (PerkinElmer) in accordance with the manufacturer’s instructions. Fluorescent images were acquired by a confocal laser scanning microscope (C1; Nikon, Tokyo, Japan). The following excitation and emission wavelengths were used for detection: DAPI for nuclear staining, 405 nm and 450/35 nm; fluorescein, 488 nm and 515/30 nm; Dylight 549, 543 nm and 605/75 nm.

Immunohistochemistry

Immunohistochemistry was performed with the anti-Npba antibody described above. Whole brains, with pituitaries attached, and spinal cords of both sexes (n = 3 each) were fixed in 4% PFA and embedded in 5% agarose (Type IX-A; Sigma-Aldrich) supplemented with 20% sucrose. Frozen 40 μm thick sections were cut in the sagittal plane. After blocking as described above, the sections were incubated overnight at 4°C with the anti-Npba antibody diluted at 1:1000 in PBS containing 2% normal goat serum, 0.1% bovine serum albumin, and 0.02% keyhole limpet hemocyanin. The sections were incubated overnight at 4°C with Dylight 549-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG (diluted 1:1000; Vector laboratories) or Alexa Fluor 488-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG (diluted 1:500; Thermo Fisher Scientific) in PBS. Fluorescent images were obtained as described above. The excitation and emission wavelengths for Alexa Fluor 488 were 488 nm and 515/30 nm, respectively.

Generation of the npba-GFP transgenic medaka

A medaka bacterial artificial chromosome (BAC) clone (clone ID: 180_I09) containing the npba locus was obtained from NBRP Medaka and modified by homologous recombination in Escherichia coli strain DY380, essentially as described previously (Copeland et al., 2001; Lee et al., 2001). A 22 bp sequence containing the translation initiation site of npba in this BAC clone was replaced by a 2136 bp DNA cassette containing the humanized Renilla reniformis GFP II-coding sequence (Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA), bovine growth hormone polyadenylation signal, and kanamycin resistance gene (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). The resulting BAC transgene was microinjected into the cytoplasm of embryos at the one-cell stage. Transgenic founders were screened by outcrossing to wild-type fish and examining progeny embryos for GFP fluorescence. Two founders were identified that produced progeny expressing GFP in a pattern reflective of endogenous npba expression during embryonic development (Hiraki et al., 2014). These progeny were raised to adulthood and intercrossed to establish homozygous transgenic lines.

Double in situ hybridization

To confirm that GFP expression in the npba-GFP transgenic fish recapitulated endogenous npba expression, brains from transgenic fish (n = 10; 3 males and seven females) were processed for double in situ hybridization using a fluorescein-labeled GFP probe and a DIG-labeled npba probe. Double in situ hybridization was also performed on wild-type female brains (n = 5) with a fluorescein-labeled npba probe and a DIG-labeled npbb probe, in order to examine whether npba and npbb are coexpressed in the same neurons. Double in situ hybridization was done according to Takeuchi and Okubo (2013). In brief, whole brains were fixed in 4% PFA and embedded in 5% agarose (Type IX-A; Sigma-Aldrich) supplemented with 20% sucrose. Frozen 20 μm thick coronal sections were cut and hybridized with the fluorescein- and DIG-labeled probes. The GFP probe was generated from a 733 bp fragment corresponding to nucleotides 733–1465 of the phrGFP II-1 mammalian expression vector (Agilent Technologies). The npba and npbb probes used were the same as those used for single in situ hybridization. The fluorescein-labeled probe was visualized by using the TSA Plus Fluorescein System (PerkinElmer); the DIG-labeled probe was visualized by using alkaline phosphatase-conjugated anti-DIG antibody (Roche Diagnostics) and Fast Red (Roche Diagnostics). Cell nuclei were counterstained with DAPI. Fluorescent images were obtained as described above.

GFP imaging

Wide-field fluorescence imaging was performed on whole, unsectioned brains and spinal cords of npba-GFP transgenic fish (n = 2 for each sex). Whole brains, with pituitaries attached, and spinal cords were fixed in 4% PFA and optically cleared by immersion in Scaleview-A2 (Olympus, Tokyo, Japan) at room temperature overnight. Confocal images were acquired by using a fluorescence macroscope (MVX10; Olympus) equipped with a DSU spinning disk confocal system and EGFP band pass filter set (Olympus).

Molecular cloning of medaka npbwr2

An EST library was generated from the medaka brain and approximately 32000 ESTs were randomly selected and sequenced in the 5′ to 3′ direction as described previously (Okubo et al., 2011). After assembly and annotation, an EST (clone ID: 11_L07), which had best BLAST hits to NPBWR1/NPBWR2 in other vertebrates, was identified and fully sequenced. Upon sequencing, this EST was found to be truncated at the 5′ end and to lack the translation initiation site. The remaining 5′ sequence was obtained by 5′-RACE on medaka brain poly(A)+ RNA using the Marathon cDNA Amplification Kit (Takara Bio, Shiga, Japan), essentially as described previously (Kawabata et al., 2012).

The deduced amino acid sequence of the resultant full-length medaka cDNA was aligned with NPBWR1/NPBWR2 in other vertebrates by using ClustalW. The resulting alignment was used to construct a bootstrapped (1000 replicates) neighbor-joining tree (http://clustalw.ddbj.nig.ac.jp/index.php). Opioid receptors µ1 and δ1 (OPRD1 and OPRM1) in humans were used as outgroups. The species names and GenBank accession numbers of the sequences used in the analysis are listed in Supplementary file 3.

Molecular cloning of medaka npbb

A survey of the medaka genome assembly in Ensembl (http://www.ensembl.org/index.html) identified a previously uncharacterized predicted gene (gene ID: ENSORLG00000012098) that bears some sequence similarity to npba. A BLAST search of the medaka EST database at NBRP using this predicted gene, designated npbb, as the query identified the corresponding ESTs (clone ID: olova57o22, olova6h09, olova8d12, olova13m04, olova28d23, olova58i17), which were then assembled to obtain the full-length cDNA sequence for npbb.

The deduced amino acid sequence of medaka npbb was analyzed for the presence of specific domains or motifs by using InterProScan (http://www.ebi.ac.uk/interpro/search/sequence-search) and SignalP (http://www.cbs.dtu.dk/services/SignalP/). This sequence was then aligned with NPB/NPW in other vertebrates by using ClustalW, and an unrooted neighbor-joining tree with 1000 bootstrap replicates was constructed (http://clustalw.ddbj.nig.ac.jp/index.php). The species names and GenBank accession numbers of the sequences used are listed in Supplementary file 3.

Syntenic relationships of genes in the vicinity of gar, arowana, catfish, and medaka NPB genes were established using the Ensembl genome browser. The proximal promoter sequence of medaka npbb (up to 6 kb upstream of the translation initiation site) was retrieved from the Ensembl genome browser and analyzed for the presence of putative EREs by Jaspar (http://jaspar.genereg.net/). Only putative EREs with a relative profile score higher than 80% on both strands were considered as positive hits. The proximal promoter sequences of gar, arowana, catfish, and tilapia NPB genes were also obtained and analyzed for putative EREs by the same procedure.

Receptor activation assay

Medaka Npba and Npbb polypeptides with amidated C termini were synthesized by Scrum (Tokyo, Japan). The cDNA fragment encoding the full-length Npbwr2 was PCR-amplified and subcloned into the expression vector pcDNA3.1/V5-His-TOPO (Thermo Fisher Scientific). The resulting Npbwr2 expression construct was transiently transfected into HEK293T cells together with the cAMP-responsive luciferase reporter vector pGL4.29 (Promega) and the internal control vector pGL4.74 (Promega) at a ratio of 11:18:1 using FuGENE HD transfection reagent (Promega). Forty-two hours after transfection, cells were stimulated with peptide at doses of 0, 10−11, 10−10, 10−9, 10−8, 10−7, 10−6, and 10−5 M in the presence of 2 μM forskolin for 6 hr. The extracted cells were assayed for luciferase activity by using the Dual-Luciferase Reporter Assay System (Promega). Assays were performed in triplicate and repeated twice independently. HEK293T cells used in this study were authenticated by short tandem repeat profiling (National Institute of Biomedical Innovation, Osaka, Japan) and confirmed to be mycoplasma free (Biotherapy Institute of Japan, Tokyo, Japan).

Generation of knockout medaka

Knockout medaka deficient for npba, npbb, or npbwr2 were generated by using TALEN technology, targeting the sequences corresponding to the N-terminus of the mature Npba/Npbb polypeptide and the middle portion of Npbwr2 protein (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). TALEN construction, mRNA synthesis, microinjection, and founder screening were performed as described above. A founder was identified for each knockout strain that yielded a high proportion of progeny carrying a deletion that caused a frameshift and premature termination of translation (10 bp, 11 bp, and 7 bp deletions for npba-/-, npbb-/-, and npbwr2-/- strains, respectively) (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). These progeny were intercrossed to generate fish homozygous for the deletions. The genotype of each fish was determined by direct sequencing using the primers listed in Supplementary file 2. In addition, npba/npbb double knockout (npba-/-/npbb-/-) medaka were obtained by intercrossing the npba-/- and npbb-/- medaka.

Mating behavior analysis

All behavioral procedures were conducted in 2-liter rectangular tanks contained within a large recirculating water system with a constant influx of dechlorinated tap water. Fish were thinned to 2–4 females and 2–4 males per tank and allowed to acclimatize for 3–5 days to ensure and standardize their reproductive conditions. On the day before behavioral testing, each focal female was placed with a stimulus male in a tank, separated by a transparent, perforated partition. The partition was removed 1.0–2.5 hr after onset of the light period of the following day, and fish were allowed to interact for 30 min. All interactions were recorded with a digital video camera (iVIS HF S11/S21, Canon, Tokyo, Japan, or Everio GZ-G5, Jvckenwood, Kanagawa, Japan). Water inflow to the tanks was turned off during recording.

The percentage of females that spawned within the test period (30 min) was calculated for each genotype. The following behavioral parameters were also calculated from the video recordings: the total number of courtship displays; the latency from the first courtship display to the first wrapping and to the wrapping that resulted in spawning; the number of wrapping attempts refused by the female; the duration of quivering; and the percentage of females that spawned without any preceding courtship display from the male. Each action during mating behavior was identified following Ono and Uematsu (1957) and Walter and Hamilton (1970) (also see a brief description of each action in the Results section). Females that did not spawn were excluded from the analysis of these parameters. Females that spawned without any courtship display were excluded from the analysis of latency from the first courtship display.

In the behavioral analysis of npba single knockouts, their wild-type and heterozygous siblings served as controls. In the analysis of npbb single knockouts, their heterozygous siblings and non-sibling wild-type fish served as controls, because npbb is located on the sex chromosomes and it is impossible to obtain wild-type siblings of npbb knockouts by the mating of any pair of genotypes. For the same reason, non-sibling wild-type fish were used as controls in the analysis of npba/npbb double knockouts and npbwr2 knockouts. These non-sibling wild-type fish were derived from the same genetic background and reared under the same conditions as the knockouts.

Statistical analysis

Values are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) for continuous variables and as percentages for categorical variables. Individual data points are also shown to give a better indication of the underlying distribution. To facilitate comparisons in real-time PCR analysis, the expression level of each target gene (normalized to that of actb) in male whole brain was arbitrarily set to 1, and the relative difference was calculated.

Statistical analyses were performed by using GraphPad Prism (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA). Continuous variables were compared between two groups by the unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test. If the F-test indicated that the variance differed significantly between groups, Welch’s correction to the Student’s t-test was employed. For more than two groups, continuous variables were compared by one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), followed by either Bonferroni’s (for comparisons among experimental groups) or Dunnett’s (for comparisons of experimental versus control groups) post hoc test. If Bartlett’s and Brown-Forsythe tests indicated that the variance differed significantly among groups, data were log-transformed to normalize distributions prior to analysis. If the variance remained heterogeneous after transformation, data were analyzed by the non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis test followed by Dunn’s post hoc test. For the analysis of npba expression in ovariectomized and E2-treated ΔERE mutant females, a two-way ANOVA was conducted to test for main effects and interactions between genotype and treatment. Behavioral time-series data sets were further analyzed using Kaplan-Meier plots with the inclusion of fish that did not spawn within the test period, following Jahn-Eimermacher et al. (2011). Differences between Kaplan-Meier curves were tested for statistical significance using Gehan-Breslow-Wilcoxon test with Bonferroni’s correction. Fisher’s exact test was used to compare categorical variables. Statistical outliers were determined with a ROUT test, using a false-positive rate (Q) of 0.1%, and were removed from the behavioral time-series data sets.

Acknowledgements

We thank the National BioResource Project (NBRP) Medaka for providing the BAC clone used in this study and Drs. Lino Tessarollo and Donald L Court for the DY380 bacteria. We also thank Dr. Shin-ichi Higashijima for technical advice on generating the transgenic construct; Dr. Guro K Sandvik for help with transgenic fish; Tatsuya Fukataki for performing the receptor activation assay; and Thomas Fleming for language editing of the revised manuscript.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Kataaki Okubo, Email: okubo@marine.fs.a.u-tokyo.ac.jp.

Catherine Dulac, Harvard University, United States.

Joel K Elmquist, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology 25132705 to Kataaki Okubo.

  • Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology 17H06429 to Kataaki Okubo.

  • Japan Society for the Promotion of Science 16H04979 to Kataaki Okubo.

  • Japan Society for the Promotion of Science 19H03044 to Kataaki Okubo.

  • Japan Society for the Promotion of Science 12J07446 to Towako Hiraki-Kajiyama.

  • RIKEN Special Postdoctoral Researcher Program to Towako Hiraki-Kajiyama.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All animal procedures were performed in accordance with the guidelines of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Tokyo. The committee requests the submission of an animal-use protocol only for use of mammals, birds, and reptiles, in accordance with the Fundamental Guidelines for Proper Conduct of Animal Experiment and Related Activities in Academic Research Institutions under the jurisdiction of the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan (Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, Notice No. 71; June 1, 2006). Accordingly, we did not submit an animal-use protocol for this study, which used only teleost fish and thus did not require approval by the committee.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Abbreviations of brain and spinal cord regions and brain nuclei.
elife-39495-supp1.docx (14.6KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39495.025
Supplementary file 2. Primers used in this study.
elife-39495-supp2.docx (13.9KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39495.026
Supplementary file 3. Species names and GenBank accession numbers of the protein sequences used in this study.
elife-39495-supp3.docx (14.1KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39495.027
Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39495.028

Data availability

Sequence data have been deposited in DDBJ/EMBL/GenBank with accession number LC375958.

The following dataset was generated:

Kataaki Okubo. 2019. Oryzias latipes npbwr2 mRNA for neuropeptides B and W receptor 2. NCBI Genbank. LC375958

References

  1. Adkins-Regan E. Hormones and sexual differentiation of avian social behavior. Developmental Neuroscience. 2009;31:342–350. doi: 10.1159/000216545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amores A, Force A, Yan YL, Joly L, Amemiya C, Fritz A, Ho RK, Langeland J, Prince V, Wang YL, Westerfield M, Ekker M, Postlethwait JH. Zebrafish hox clusters and vertebrate genome evolution. Science. 1998;282:1711–1714. doi: 10.1126/science.282.5394.1711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anken R, Bourrat F. Brain Atlas of the Medakafish. Paris: INRA Editions; 1998. 978-2738008183 [Google Scholar]
  4. Ansai S, Sakuma T, Yamamoto T, Ariga H, Uemura N, Takahashi R, Kinoshita M. Efficient targeted mutagenesis in medaka using custom-designed transcription activator-like effector nucleases. Genetics. 2013;193:739–749. doi: 10.1534/genetics.112.147645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ball GF, Balthazart J, McCarthy MM. Is it useful to view the brain as a secondary sexual characteristic? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;46:628–638. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Balthazart J, Charlier TD, Barker JM, Yamamura T, Ball GF. Sex steroid-induced neuroplasticity and behavioral activation in birds. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;32:2116–2132. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07518.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brezillon S, Lannoy V, Franssen JD, Le Poul E, Dupriez V, Lucchetti J, Detheux M, Parmentier M. Identification of natural ligands for the orphan G protein-coupled receptors GPR7 and GPR8. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2003;278:776–783. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M206396200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bu G, Lin D, Cui L, Huang L, Lv C, Huang S, Wan Y, Fang C, Li J, Wang Y. Characterization of neuropeptide B (NPB), Neuropeptide W (NPW), and their receptors in chickens: evidence for NPW being a novel inhibitor of pituitary GH and prolactin secretion. Endocrinology. 2016;157:3562–3576. doi: 10.1210/en.2016-1141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Capel B. Vertebrate sex determination: evolutionary plasticity of a fundamental switch. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2017;18:675–689. doi: 10.1038/nrg.2017.60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cermak T, Doyle EL, Christian M, Wang L, Zhang Y, Schmidt C, Baller JA, Somia NV, Bogdanove AJ, Voytas DF. Efficient design and assembly of custom TALEN and other TAL effector-based constructs for DNA targeting. Nucleic Acids Research. 2011;39:e82. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkr218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chapouton P, Jagasia R, Bally-Cuif L. Adult neurogenesis in non-mammalian vertebrates. BioEssays. 2007;29:745–757. doi: 10.1002/bies.20615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Copeland NG, Jenkins NA, Court DL. Recombineering: a powerful new tool for mouse functional genomics. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2001;2:769–779. doi: 10.1038/35093556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Demski LS, Bauer DH, Gerald JW. Sperm release evoked by electrical stimulation of the fish brain: a functional-anatomical study. Journal of Experimental Zoology. 1975;191:215–231. doi: 10.1002/jez.1401910209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dumais KM, Veenema AH. Vasopressin and oxytocin receptor systems in the brain: sex differences and sex-specific regulation of social behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2016;40:1–23. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2015.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dun SL, Brailoiu GC, Yang J, Chang JK, Dun NJ. Neuropeptide W-immunoreactivity in the hypothalamus and pituitary of the rat. Neuroscience Letters. 2003;349:71–74. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00804-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Edwards DA, Burge KG. Early androgen treatment and male and female sexual behavior in mice. Hormones and Behavior. 1971;2:49–58. doi: 10.1016/0018-506X(71)90037-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Foran CM, Peterson BN, Benson WH. Transgenerational and developmental exposure of Japanese medaka (Oryzias latipes) to ethinylestradiol results in endocrine and reproductive differences in the response to ethinylestradiol as adults. Toxicological Sciences. 2002;68:389–402. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/68.2.389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Foran CM, Weston J, Slattery M, Brooks BW, Huggett DB. Reproductive assessment of japanese medaka (Oryzias latipes) following a four-week fluoxetine (SSRI) exposure. Archives of Environmental Contamination and Toxicology. 2004;46:511–517. doi: 10.1007/s00244-003-3042-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Forger NG, Strahan JA, Castillo-Ruiz A. Cellular and molecular mechanisms of sexual differentiation in the mammalian nervous system. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2016;40:67–86. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2016.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fujii R, Yoshida H, Fukusumi S, Habata Y, Hosoya M, Kawamata Y, Yano T, Hinuma S, Kitada C, Asami T, Mori M, Fujisawa Y, Fujino M. Identification of a neuropeptide modified with bromine as an endogenous ligand for GPR7. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2002;277:34010–34016. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M205883200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Funakoshi K, Nakano M. The sympathetic nervous system of anamniotes. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2007;69:105–113. doi: 10.1159/000095199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ganz J, Brand M. Adult neurogenesis in fish. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 2016;8:a019018. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a019018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ghosal R, Sorensen PW. Male-typical courtship, spawning behavior, and olfactory sensitivity are induced to different extents by androgens in the goldfish suggesting they are controlled by different neuroendocrine mechanisms. General and Comparative Endocrinology. 2016;232:160–173. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2016.04.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Goodson JL, Kingsbury MA. What's in a name? considerations of homologies and nomenclature for vertebrate social behavior networks. Hormones and Behavior. 2013;64:103–112. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Göppert C, Harris RM, Theis A, Boila A, Hohl S, Rüegg A, Hofmann HA, Salzburger W, Böhne A. Inhibition of aromatase induces partial sex change in a cichlid fish: distinct functions for sex steroids in brains and gonads. Sexual Development. 2016;10:97–110. doi: 10.1159/000445463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Grant JWA, Bryant MJ, Soos CE. Operational sex ratio, mediated by synchrony of female arrival, alters the variance of male mating success in Japanese medaka. Animal Behaviour. 1995;49:367–375. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1995.9998. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Grant JWA, Green LD. Mate copying versus preference for actively courting males by female Japanese medaka (Oryzias latipes) Behavioral Ecology. 1996;7:165–167. doi: 10.1093/beheco/7.2.165. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Heberden C. Sex steroids and neurogenesis. Biochemical Pharmacology. 2017;141:56–62. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2017.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hiraki T, Takeuchi A, Tsumaki T, Zempo B, Kanda S, Oka Y, Nagahama Y, Okubo K. Female-specific target sites for both oestrogen and androgen in the teleost brain. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012;279:5014–5023. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hiraki T, Nakasone K, Hosono K, Kawabata Y, Nagahama Y, Okubo K. Neuropeptide B is female-specifically expressed in the telencephalic and preoptic nuclei of the medaka brain. Endocrinology. 2014;155:1021–1032. doi: 10.1210/en.2013-1806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Ishikawa Y, Yoshimoto M, Ito H. A brain atlas of a wild-type inbred strain of the medaka, Oryzias latipes. Fish Biology Journal Medaka. 1999;10:1–26. [Google Scholar]
  32. Jackson VR, Lin SH, Wang Z, Nothacker HP, Civelli O. A study of the rat neuropeptide B/neuropeptide W system using in situ techniques. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2006;497:367–383. doi: 10.1002/cne.20989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jahn-Eimermacher A, Lasarzik I, Raber J. Statistical analysis of latency outcomes in behavioral experiments. Behavioural Brain Research. 2011;221:271–275. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kawabata Y, Hiraki T, Takeuchi A, Okubo K. Sex differences in the expression of Vasotocin/isotocin, gonadotropin-releasing hormone, and tyrosine and tryptophan hydroxylase family genes in the medaka brain. Neuroscience. 2012;218:65–77. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kikuchi Y, Hiraki-Kajiyama T, Nakajo M, Umatani C, Kanda S, Oka Y, Matsumoto K, Ozawa H, Okubo K. Sexually dimorphic neuropeptide B neurons in medaka exhibit activated cellular phenotypes dependent on estrogen. Endocrinology. 2019;160:827–839. doi: 10.1210/en.2019-00030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kim HJ, Lee HJ, Kim H, Cho SW, Kim JS. Targeted genome editing in human cells with zinc finger nucleases constructed via modular assembly. Genome Research. 2009;19:1279–1288. doi: 10.1101/gr.089417.108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kimchi T, Xu J, Dulac C. A functional circuit underlying male sexual behaviour in the female mouse brain. Nature. 2007;448:1009–1014. doi: 10.1038/nature06089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Koyama Y, Satou M, Oka Y, Ueda K. Involvement of the telencephalic hemispheres and the preoptic area in sexual behavior of the male goldfish, Carassius auratus: a brain-lesion study. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1984;40:70–86. doi: 10.1016/S0163-1047(84)90182-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kyle AL, Peter RE. Effects of forebrain lesions on spawning behaviour in the male goldfish. Physiology & Behavior. 1982;28:1103–1109. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(82)90183-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lee EC, Yu D, Martinez de Velasco J, Tessarollo L, Swing DA, Court DL, Jenkins NA, Copeland NG. A highly efficient Escherichia coli-based chromosome engineering system adapted for recombinogenic targeting and subcloning of BAC DNA. Genomics. 2001;73:56–65. doi: 10.1006/geno.2000.6451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Liu H, Todd EV, Lokman PM, Lamm MS, Godwin JR, Gemmell NJ. Sexual plasticity: a fishy tale. Molecular Reproduction and Development. 2017;84:171–194. doi: 10.1002/mrd.22691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Mahmoud R, Wainwright SR, Galea LA. Sex hormones and adult hippocampal neurogenesis: regulation, implications, and potential mechanisms. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2016;41:129–152. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2016.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. McCarthy MM, Nugent BM, Lenz KM. Neuroimmunology and neuroepigenetics in the establishment of sex differences in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2017;18:471–484. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. McCarthy MM, Arnold AP. Reframing sexual differentiation of the brain. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:677–683. doi: 10.1038/nn.2834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. McCarthy MM, Nugent BM. At the frontier of epigenetics of brain sex differences. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2015;9:221. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Moore FL, Lowry CA. Comparative neuroanatomy of vasotocin and vasopressin in amphibians and other vertebrates. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part C: Pharmacology, Toxicology and Endocrinology. 1998;119:251–260. doi: 10.1016/S0742-8413(98)00014-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Munakata A, Kobayashi M. Endocrine control of sexual behavior in teleost fish. General and Comparative Endocrinology. 2010;165:456–468. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2009.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Nagata-Kuroiwa R, Furutani N, Hara J, Hondo M, Ishii M, Abe T, Mieda M, Tsujino N, Motoike T, Yanagawa Y, Kuwaki T, Yamamoto M, Yanagisawa M, Sakurai T. Critical role of neuropeptides B/W receptor 1 signaling in social behavior and fear memory. PLOS ONE. 2011;6:e16972. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016972. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Northcutt RG. The forebrain of gnathostomes: in search of a morphotype. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 1995;46:275–288. doi: 10.1159/000113279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. O'Connell LA, Hofmann HA. The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: a comparative synthesis. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2011;519:3599–3639. doi: 10.1002/cne.22735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Okubo K, Takeuchi A, Chaube R, Paul-Prasanth B, Kanda S, Oka Y, Nagahama Y. Sex differences in aromatase gene expression in the medaka brain. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2011;23:412–423. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2011.02120.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Ono Y, Uematsu T. Mating ethogram in Oryzias latipes.  Journal of the Faculty of Science, Hokkaido University. 1957;13:197–202. [Google Scholar]
  53. Paul-Prasanth B, Bhandari RK, Kobayashi T, Horiguchi R, Kobayashi Y, Nakamoto M, Shibata Y, Sakai F, Nakamura M, Nagahama Y. Estrogen oversees the maintenance of the female genetic program in terminally differentiated gonochorists. Scientific Reports. 2013;3:2862. doi: 10.1038/srep02862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Pfaff DW, Gagnidze K, Hunter RG. Molecular endocrinology of female reproductive behavior. Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology. 2018;467:14–20. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2017.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Ponti G, Farinetti A, Marraudino M, Panzica G, Gotti S. Sex steroids and adult neurogenesis in the ventricular-subventricular zone. Frontiers in Endocrinology. 2018;9:156. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2018.00156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Price CJ, Samson WK, Ferguson AV. Neuropeptide W has cell phenotype-specific effects on the excitability of different subpopulations of paraventricular nucleus neurones. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2009;21:850–857. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2009.01904.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Sakurai T. NPBWR1 and NPBWR2: implications in energy homeostasis, pain, and emotion. Frontiers in Endocrinology. 2013;4:23. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2013.00023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Satou M, Oka Y, Kusunoki M, Matsushima T, Kato M, Fujita I, Ueda K. Telencephalic and preoptic areas integrate sexual behavior in hime salmon (landlocked red salmon, Oncorhynchus nerka): results of electrical brain stimulation experiments. Physiology & Behavior. 1984;33:441–447. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90167-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Schneider H, Sulner B. Innervation of dorsal and caudal fin muscles in adult zebafish Danio rerio. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2006;497:702–716. doi: 10.1002/cne.21038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Södersten P. Mounting behavior in the female rat during the estrous cycle, after ovariectomy, and after estrogen or testosterone administration. Hormones and Behavior. 1972;3:307–320. doi: 10.1016/0018-506X(72)90020-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Takeuchi A, Okubo K. Post-proliferative immature radial glial cells female-specifically express aromatase in the medaka optic tectum. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e73663. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Tanaka H, Yoshida T, Miyamoto N, Motoike T, Kurosu H, Shibata K, Yamanaka A, Williams SC, Richardson JA, Tsujino N, Garry MG, Lerner MR, King DS, O'Dowd BF, Sakurai T, Yanagisawa M. Characterization of a family of endogenous neuropeptide ligands for the G protein-coupled receptors GPR7 and GPR8. PNAS. 2003;100:6251–6256. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0837789100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Tilton SC, Foran CM, Benson WH. Effects of cadmium on the reproductive Axis of Japanese medaka (Oryzias latipes) Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part C: Toxicology & Pharmacology. 2003;136:265–276. doi: 10.1016/j.cca.2003.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Veening JG, de Jong TR, Waldinger MD, Korte SM, Olivier B. The role of oxytocin in male and female reproductive behavior. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2015;753:209–228. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2014.07.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wagner CK. The many faces of progesterone: a role in adult and developing male brain. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2006;27:340–359. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2006.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Walter RO, Hamilton JB. Head-up movements as an Indicator of sexual unreceptivity in female medaka, Oryzias latipes. Animal Behaviour. 1970;18:125–127. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(70)90079-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Watanabe N, Wada M, Irukayama-Tomobe Y, Ogata Y, Tsujino N, Suzuki M, Furutani N, Sakurai T, Yamamoto M. A single nucleotide polymorphism of the neuropeptide B/W receptor-1 gene influences the evaluation of facial expressions. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e35390. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Watanabe N, Yamamoto M. Neural mechanisms of social dominance. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2015;9:154. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Westerfield M, McMurray JV, Eisen JS. Identified motoneurons and their innervation of axial muscles in the zebrafish. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1986;6:2267–2277. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.06-08-02267.1986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Xu X, Coats JK, Yang CF, Wang A, Ahmed OM, Alvarado M, Izumi T, Shah NM. Modular genetic control of sexually dimorphic behaviors. Cell. 2012;148:596–607. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Yamashita J, Kawabata Y, Okubo K. Expression of isotocin is male-specifically up-regulated by gonadal androgen in the medaka brain. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2017;29:e12545. doi: 10.1111/jne.12545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Yang CF, Chiang MC, Gray DC, Prabhakaran M, Alvarado M, Juntti SA, Unger EK, Wells JA, Shah NM. Sexually dimorphic neurons in the ventromedial hypothalamus govern mating in both sexes and aggression in males. Cell. 2013;153:896–909. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Yang CF, Shah NM. Representing sex in the brain, one module at a time. Neuron. 2014;82:261–278. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.03.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Yang T, Shah NM. Molecular and neural control of sexually dimorphic social behaviors. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2016;38:89–95. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.04.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Joel K Elmquist1
Reviewed by: Scott Juntti

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Neuropeptide B mediates female sexual receptivity in medaka fish, acting in a female-specific but reversible manner" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and a Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal his identity: Scott Juntti (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

Hiraki-Kajiyama et al. follow up work on the sexually dimorphic expression of NPB with a series of experiments to understand its gene regulation and directly test its role in sexual behaviors using reverse genetics. An outstanding problem in molecular biology has been the linking of hormone receptors to individual target genes. A second problem is in discovering genes in the pathways that drive mating behavior. This paper addresses both, and makes an attempt to link the two. They report that the two paralogs of npb, nbpa and npbb, display female specific expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg region of the medaka brain. They found that this female specific expression depends on estrogen and an ERE site in the case of npba, and is responsive to altered sex hormone milieu. Furthermore, they uncovered that in mutants lacking either the ligands (Npba and Npbb) or the receptor (Npbwr2) are less receptive to male courtship. Overall, the manuscript is well written, the data is of high quality, and the conclusions are supported by the results. There are a few issues need to be addressed.

Essential revisions:

The decreased expression of npba in the ∆ERE mutants supports the hypothesis that ER acts directly on the ERE to activate npba expression. However, the conclusion may be strengthened if npba expression in the mutants is not or less responsive to exogenous estrogen treatment.

The slow kinetics of estrogen induction of npba and npbb expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg regions does not agree with the model of direct ER regulation. Other permissive changes may be necessary.

Regarding the change in the number of npba/npbb expression neurons, the authors only considered changes in gene expression. Is it possible that some of changes are due to neuronal birth/death in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg during induction of sex reversal or change of sex hormone milieu?

The crosses that gave rise to fish used in behavioral experiments are not spelled out, and so the relationships between controls and experimental fish are unknown to the reader. This may be problematic as genetic variation may not be properly controlled for. Ideally, the genotypes analyzed would be derived from the same crosses and share parents. This is particularly important in the context of gene editing experiments due to off-target effects. If the wild-type controls are not derived from the same crosses as the mutants, these may behave differently due to factors other than the mutations under study.

A similar critique of crosses applies to the ∆ERE fish; are WT controls their siblings? It's worrisome that the ∆ERE females in Figure 1E have similar Npba mRNA levels to the WT females in 1C, and one explanation could be genetic- or cohort-level variations.

The authors postulate that single Npb gene deletions are insufficient to reveal a behavioral phenotype due to functional redundancy. However, some limitations of the data depicted in Figure 6—figure supplement 2 make this conclusion tenuous. First, the questions of genetic variability listed above apply here too. Second-and perhaps relatedly-the variability in the data appears large. WT fish have dramatically differing values when comparing row A and B in Figure 6—figure supplement 2 and Figure 6. Finally, the sample size is only ~1/3 that of Figure 6. Therefore this experiment appears underpowered to conclude that the single mutations don't have an effect. A power analysis would be appropriate to show that there is enough statistical power given the variability.

Discussion, fifth paragraph and elsewhere. Do androgens inhibit Npb expression? Androgen/estrogen balance is a parsimonious explanation, but Npb mRNA decrease after KT treatment could also be secondary to a decrease in circulating estrogen levels (or decreased neural synthesis of estrogen). Would not a better way to test this be the simultaneous treatment with KT and E2?

As estradiol levels affect Npba expression, it is important to confirm that E2 levels are not changed in the ∆ERE fish. As Npba is expressed in the pituitary, it's not improbable that the decreased Npba expression is due to low estrogen. Furthermore, treating these animals with E2 to test whether the gene has become unresponsive to ER would be a nice demonstration.

To appropriately test latency to perform behavior, without removing any animals from analysis, one should use Kaplan-Meier plots (Jahn-Eimermacher, 2011) Statistical analysis of latency outcomes in behavioral experiments). This does not require any assumptions about data distributions and allows the inclusion of animals that do not perform a behavior.

eLife. 2019 Aug 6;8:e39495. doi: 10.7554/eLife.39495.033

Author response


Essential revisions:

The decreased expression of npba in the ∆ERE mutants supports the hypothesis that ER acts directly on the ERE to activate npba expression. However, the conclusion may be strengthened if npba expression in the mutants is not or less responsive to exogenous estrogen treatment.

Following this comment, we have examined npba expression in the brain of ∆ERE mutant females that were sham-operated, ovariectomized, or ovariectomized and treated with E2 by real-time PCR. In ∆ERE mutant females, npba expression was significantly reduced by ovariectomy and restored by E2 treatment; however, E2 was not able to restore npba expression to the level present in wild-type females. These results further support the conclusion that estrogen directly activates the expression of npba via the ERE found in its promoter, while simultaneously suggesting that other mechanisms are also involved in this estrogen effect.

These data have been included in the revised manuscript as follows:

1) Results section: “These results demonstrate that estrogen elicits the female-specific expression of npba, at least in part, by direct transcriptional activation through the ERE present in the npba promoter.” has been replaced with “We also evaluated npba expression in the whole brain of ΔERE mutant females that were sham-operated, ovariectomized, or ovariectomized and treated with estradiol-17β (E2; the primary estrogen in vertebrates including teleosts) by real-time PCR. […] Taken together, these results demonstrate that estrogen elicits the female-specific expression of npba by direct transcriptional activation through the ERE present in the npba promoter, although other mechanisms are also likely involved in this estrogen effect.”. Following the addition of this description, “estradiol-17β (E2; the primary estrogen in vertebrates including teleosts)” has been replaced with “E2”.

2) Materials and methods section: “For the analysis of npba expression in ovariectomized and E2-treated ΔERE mutant females, a two-way ANOVA was conducted to test for main effects and interactions between genotype and treatment.” has been added.

3) Legend for Figure 1: “(G) Levels of npba expression in the whole brain of ΔERE (beige columns) and WT (blue columns) females that were sham-operated (Sham), ovariectomized (OVX), or ovariectomized and treated with estradiol-17β (OVX+E2) as determined by real-time PCR (n = 6 per group). There were significant main effects of genotype (F (1, 30) = 45.03, p < 0.0001) and treatment (F (2, 30) = 60.19, p < 0.0001) and a significant interaction between genotype and treatment (F (2, 30) = 9.944, p = 0.0005). *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01; ***, p < 0.001 (Bonferroni’s post hoc test).” has been added.

4) Figure 1: A new panel showing these results (panel G) has been added and the layout of the other panels has been modified.

The slow kinetics of estrogen induction of npba and npbb expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg regions does not agree with the model of direct ER regulation. Other permissive changes may be necessary.

As this comment indicates, the estrogenic induction of npba/npbb expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of males occurred rather slowly. We have previously reported that ERs are not expressed at detectable levels in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of males and that treating males with estrogen induces ER expression therein (Hiraki et al., 2012). Considering these findings, it seems plausible that npba/npbb expression is dependent on ER expression and thus the response to estrogen is slower than expected. In support of this idea, we have obtained preliminary evidence that a cumulative increase in ER expression precedes the elevation of npba/npbb expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of estrogen-treated males. We are currently investigating the mechanism by which estrogen induces ER expression and will report our findings in a future paper.

We have included part of this discussion in the revised manuscript by adding the following paragraph in the Discussion section: “The estrogenic induction of npba expression in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg of males showed a slower kinetic response than would be expected of a model of direct transcriptional activation. […] Considering these findings, it seems plausible that this estrogen-induced npba expression is not fully realized until sufficient ER expression is achieved.”

Regarding the change in the number of npba/npbb expression neurons, the authors only considered changes in gene expression. Is it possible that some of changes are due to neuronal birth/death in Vs/Vp and PMm/PMg during induction of sex reversal or change of sex hormone milieu?

While the present paper focuses on deciphering gene expression patterns in npba/npbb-expressing neurons, we have also been interested in this possibility, given that the brains of teleosts exhibit extensive adult neurogenesis and that estrogen has significant effects on adult neurogenesis. We have published a paper relevant to this issue recently (Kikuchi et al., 2019). In this paper, we found that Npb-expressing neurons emerged in adult male medaka treated with estrogen, not through neurogenesis, but rather through the activation of pre-existing, quiescent male counterpart neurons. This finding strongly suggests that an activation–inactivation process on existing neurons is regulating the number of npba/npbb-expressing neurons.

This discussion has been included in the revised manuscript as follows:

1) Discussion section: “The brains of teleosts exhibit extensive adult neurogenesis (Chapouton et al., 2007; Ganz and Brand, 2016) and estrogen has significant effects on adult neurogenesis (Mahmoud et al., 2016; Heberden, 2017; Ponti et al., 2018). […] This evidence suggests the relevance of an activation–inactivation process on existing neurons, rather than neurogenesis, in the reversible sexual dimorphism in npba expression.” has been added.

2) Reference list - the following references, which are cited in the above discussion, have been added:

Chapouton, Jagasia and Bally-Cuif, 2007; Ganz and Brand, 2016; Heberden, 2017; Kikuchi et al., 2019; Mahmoud, Wainwright and Galea, 2016; Ponti et al., 2018.

The crosses that gave rise to fish used in behavioral experiments are not spelled out, and so the relationships between controls and experimental fish are unknown to the reader. This may be problematic as genetic variation may not be properly controlled for. Ideally, the genotypes analyzed would be derived from the same crosses and share parents. This is particularly important in the context of gene editing experiments due to off-target effects. If the wild-type controls are not derived from the same crosses as the mutants, these may behave differently due to factors other than the mutations under study.

In the behavioral analysis of npba single knockouts, their wild-type and heterozygous siblings served as controls. Similarly, in the analysis of npbb single knockouts, their heterozygous siblings served as controls. However, their wild-type siblings could not be used because npbb is located on the sex chromosomes and it is therefore impossible to obtain wild-type siblings of npbb knockouts through the mating of any pair of genotypes. For this reason, non-sibling wild-type fish were used as controls in the analysis of npba/npbb double knockouts and npbwr2 knockouts. Additionally, we believe there is sufficient reason to support the reliability of our results considering similar phenotypes were detected in npba single knockouts, npba/npbb double knockouts, and npbwr2 knockouts; non-sibling wild-type and sibling heterozygous controls gave similar results in all analyzed parameters in npbb single knockouts; all knockouts and wild-type controls used in the study were derived from a d-rR strain maintained in a closed colony (sharing the same genetic background) in our laboratory for over 10 years; and all fish used for each experiment were reared under the same conditions to minimize the influence of environmental factors.

We have included these pieces of information in the Materials and methods section as follows: “In the behavioral analysis of npba single knockouts, their wild-type and heterozygous siblings served as controls. In the analysis of npbb single knockouts, their heterozygous siblings and non-sibling wild-type fish served as controls, because npbb is located on the sex chromosomes and it is impossible to obtain wild-type siblings of npbb knockouts by the mating of any pair of genotypes. […] These non-sibling wild-type fish were derived from the same genetic background and reared under the same conditions as the knockouts.” has been added.

A similar critique of crosses applies to the ∆ERE fish; are WT controls their siblings? It's worrisome that the ∆ERE females in Figure 1E have similar Npba mRNA levels to the WT females in 1C, and one explanation could be genetic- or cohort-level variations.

In the analyses of ∆ERE mutants, their wild-type siblings were used as controls. Therefore, the difference in the level of npba expression between Figures 1C and 1E cannot be attributed to differences in the genetic background of the controls used but is most likely due to variations in the physiological conditions.

To specify the genetic background of the control fish used in this study, the following sentence has been added to the Materials and methods section: “In all analyses of ∆ERE mutants, their wild-type siblings were used as controls.”

The authors postulate that single Npb gene deletions are insufficient to reveal a behavioral phenotype due to functional redundancy. However, some limitations of the data depicted in Figure 6—figure supplement 2 make this conclusion tenuous. First, the questions of genetic variability listed above apply here too. Second-and perhaps relatedly-the variability in the data appears large. WT fish have dramatically differing values when comparing row A and B in Figure 6—figure supplement 2 and Figure 6. Finally, the sample size is only ~1/3 that of Figure 6. Therefore this experiment appears underpowered to conclude that the single mutations don't have an effect. A power analysis would be appropriate to show that there is enough statistical power given the variability.

As described above for the behavioral analysis of npba single knockouts, their wild-type and heterozygous siblings served as controls. All knockout and wild-type fish used in the present study, including npbb knockouts and their wild-type controls, share the same genetic background since they were derived from the d-rR strain maintained in a closed colony in our laboratory for over 10 years. The possibility can therefore be eliminated that the large variability in the data was due to the genetic diversity of the fish analyzed. Additionally, all fish used for each analysis were reared under the same conditions to minimize the influence of environmental factors. These facts notwithstanding, a relatively large variability was seen in the data of npba and npbb single knockouts, which may have limited our ability to detect significant differences in the data sets, as pointed out in this comment. Indeed, post hoc power analysis on the latency data of npba and npbb single knockouts indicated that we had very low power to detect differences (statistical power = 0.17–0.22). Accordingly, we have substantially increased the sample size as follows:

1) The numbers of wild-type, heterozygous, and homozygous fish used in the analysis of npba knockouts have been increased from 11, 12, and 12 to 34, 33, and 31, respectively.

2) The numbers of wild-type, heterozygous, and homozygous fish used in the analysis of npbb knockouts have been increased from 9, 10, and 12 to 29, 33, and 32, respectively.

As expected, this increase in sample size greatly improved the statistical power of the analyses (0.46–0.54), which is comparable to the analysis of npba/npbb double knockouts and npbwr2 knockouts (0.51–0.53).

To our surprise (but perhaps not to this reviewer’s), the increase in sample size has yielded significant differences in the latencies from the first courtship display to the first wrapping and to the wrapping that resulted in spawning between npba+/+ and npba-/- and between npba+/- and npba-/- (please see Figure 6). This is important since the phenotypes observed in npba/npbb double knockouts and npbwr2 knockouts have now also been observed in npba single knockouts. These results further strengthen the role of NPB signaling in female sexual receptivity. We deeply appreciate this discerning comment from this reviewer.

The text and figures have been revised to reflect these results as follows:

1) Results section: “Moreover, there was no significant difference in any of the detailed behavioral parameters among wild-type, heterozygous, and homozygous females of either knockout strain (Figure 6—figure supplement 2). […] However, subsequent quantitative behavioral testing showed abnormalities in several parameters.” has been changed to “However, subsequent quantitative behavioral testing uncovered a latent abnormality in npba knockout strain.”.

2) Results section: “Here, we found a significant increase in the latency from the first courtship display to the first wrapping for both npba-/-/npbb-/- and npbwr2-/- females as compared with wild-type females” has been changed to “Here, we found a significant increase in latencies from the first courtship display to the first wrapping and to the wrapping that resulted in spawning for npba-/- females as compared with npba+/+ and npba+/- females (p = 0.0256 and 0.0002, respectively, for the latency to the first wrapping; p = 0.0104 and 0.0026, respectively, for the latency to the wrapping that resulted in spawning) (Figure 6A). […] However, as was the case with npba-/- females, a significant increase in the latency from the first courtship display to the first wrapping was observed for both npba-/-/npbb-/- and npbwr2-/- females as compared with wild-type females”.

3) Discussion section: “altered behavioral phenotypes were evident in npba-/-/npbb-/- females” has been changed to “part of the altered behavioral phenotypes was evident in npba-/-/npbb-/- females”.

4) Discussion section: “npba-/-/npbb-/- females showed increased latency from courtship display to wrapping and increased incidence of spawning without any preceding courtship display. Consistent with this, npbwr2-/- females showed identical phenotypes.” has been changed to “npba-/-, npba-/-/npbb-/-, and npbwr2-/- females showed increased latency from courtship display to wrapping, and furthermore, npba-/-/npbb-/- and npbwr2-/- females showed increased incidence of spawning without any preceding courtship display.”.

5) Figure 6—figure supplement 2 has been revised to reflect the new results and renumbered as Figure 6 to be included in the main body of the paper.

6) As a consequence, Figure 6 has been renumbered as Figure 7. “Figure 6” has been replaced with “Figure 7A”.

7) Legend for new Figure 6 (previous Figure 6—figure supplement 2) now reads: “Figure 6. npba is involved in female sexual receptivity. Various parameters in the mating behavior of npba (A) and npbb (B) single knockout females were measured and compared with wild-type females (n = 34, 33, and 31 for npba+/+, npba+/-, and npba-/- females, respectively; n = 29, 33, and 32 for npbb+/+, npbb+/-, and npbb-/- females, respectively). Blue, ocher, and beige columns represent wild-type, heterozygous knockout, and homozygous knockout females, respectively. *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01; ***, p < 0.001 (Bonferroni’s post hoc test). See also Figure 6—figure supplement 1.”

8) Legend for new Figure 7 (previous Figure 6): “See also Figure 6—figure supplement 1 and Figure 6—figure supplement 2.” has been deleted.

Discussion, fifth paragraph and elsewhere. Do androgens inhibit Npb expression? Androgen/estrogen balance is a parsimonious explanation, but Npb mRNA decrease after KT treatment could also be secondary to a decrease in circulating estrogen levels (or decreased neural synthesis of estrogen). Would not a better way to test this be the simultaneous treatment with KT and E2?

To examine the possibility raised in this comment, we have measured E2 levels in the brains of KT-treated females. More specifically, females were treated with KT in exactly the same way as for the npba/npbb expression analysis and brain levels of E2 were measured on days 0, 2, 5, and 9 by LC-MS/MS. The results showed that brain levels of E2 fell to less than 5% of untreated control levels within 2 days and remained low during the remainder of the treatment period, demonstrating a substantial decrease in E2 levels in the brain of KT-treated females. This result strongly indicates that the decreased npba/npbb expression after KT treatment is secondary to a decrease in E2 levels, as suggested by this comment. We greatly appreciate this comment, which has much improved our understanding of the mechanisms underlying the reversal of sexually dimorphic npba/npbb expression.

We have included this data in the manuscript and revised the relevant discussion as follows:

1) Results section: “To aid in interpreting these results, we measured brain levels of E2 in females treated with KT in exactly the same way as for the npba expression analysis. The results showed that E2 levels fell to less than 5% of untreated controls within 2 days and remained low during the remainder of the treatment period (p < 0.0001 on days 2, 5, and 9), demonstrating a substantial decrease in E2 levels in the brain of KT-treated females (Figure 2—figure supplement 2).” has been added.

2) Discussion section: “Because ER expression in Vs/Vp and PMp/PMg is strongly suppressed by androgen (Hiraki et al., 2012), the androgen/AR signaling pathway presumably exerts its influence by attenuating the stimulatory action of estrogen through a reduction of ER in npba-expressing neurons. […] This represents an efficient system whereby brain and behavior are sexually differentiated, but the potential for sex reversal is retained.” has been replaced with “Considering a concomitant decrease in brain levels of E2 in androgen-treated males, the androgen/AR signaling pathway presumably exerts its influence by attenuating the stimulatory action of estrogen through a reduction of brain E2 levels. Given that ER expression in Vs/Vp and PMp/PMg is strongly suppressed by androgen (Hiraki et al., 2012), the inhibitory action of androgen on npba expression may involve repression of estrogen/ER signaling not only at ligand, but also at receptor level.”.

3) Materials and methods: “Measurement of brain levels of E2. Dissected whole brains were frozen and stored at -80ºC until analysis. Levels of E2 in the brains were analyzed by liquid chromatography coupled to tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS) at Aska Pharmamedical (Kanagawa, Japan). In the KT treatment experiment, E2 levels were measured over the same time course as that used to assess npba/npbb expression (on days 0 (untreated controls), 2, 5, and 9 of treatment).” has been added.

4) Legend for Figure 2: “See also Figure 2—figure supplement 1.” has been changed to “See also Figure 2—figure supplement 1 and Figure 2—figure supplement 2.”.

5) The legend for Figure 2—figure supplement 2 has been added. It reads: “Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Levels of estradiol-17β (E2) in the brains of females treated with 11-ketotestosterone (KT). n = 4 per sampling day. ***, p < 0.001 (versus day 0, Dunnett’s post hoc test).”

6) Figure 2—figure supplement 2 has been added.

As estradiol levels affect Npba expression, it is important to confirm that E2 levels are not changed in the ∆ERE fish. As Npba is expressed in the pituitary, it's not improbable that the decreased Npba expression is due to low estrogen. Furthermore, treating these animals with E2 to test whether the gene has become unresponsive to ER would be a nice demonstration.

In response to this comment, we have measured E2 levels in the brains of ∆ERE mutant females. The results showed that ∆ERE mutant females had E2 levels comparable to those of their wild-type sibling females, addressing the first concern in this comment.

This data has been included in the revised manuscript as follows:

1) Results section: “The possibility that decreased npba expression in ∆ERE mutant females was due to a decrease in estrogen levels in their brains was ruled out by the observation that ∆ERE mutant females had brain levels of E2 comparable to those of wild-type females (p = 0.782) (Figure 1—figure supplement 2).” has been added.

2) Legend for Figure 1: “See also Figure 1—figure supplement 1.” has been changed to “See also Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 2.”.

3) The legend for Figure 2—figure supplement 2 has been added. It reads: “Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Levels of estradiol-17β (E2) in the brains of ∆ERE mutant and wild-type (WT) females. n = 4 per group.”

4) Figure 1—figure supplement 2 has been added.

The second concern in this comment is the same as that in the first comment. As described in response to the first comment, we have examined npba expression in the brain of ∆ERE mutant females that were sham-operated, ovariectomized, or ovariectomized and treated with E2. The results showed that, in the mutant female brain, npba expression was significantly reduced by ovariectomy and restored by E2 treatment, but E2 was not effective to increase npba expression to the level in wild-type females.

To appropriately test latency to perform behavior, without removing any animals from analysis, one should use Kaplan-Meier plots (Jahn-Eimermacher, 2011) Statistical analysis of latency outcomes in behavioral experiments). This does not require any assumptions about data distributions and allows the inclusion of animals that do not perform a behavior.

Following this comment, the latency data were analyzed using Kaplan-Meier plots with the inclusion of fish that did not spawn within the test period. Differences between Kaplan-Meier curves were tested for statistical significance using Gehan-Breslow-Wilcoxon test with Bonferroni’s correction, according to the Prism 8 Statistics Guide (https://www.graphpad.com/guides/prism/8/statistics/index.htm). Fish that spawned without any courtship display were excluded from this analysis as before, because it was deemed inappropriate to treat these fish as either those that did not spawn within the test period or those that spawned “zero” seconds after the first courtship display.

Although the significant difference disappeared in the latency from the first courtship display to the wrapping that resulted in spawning between npba+/- and npba-/- fish, all other significant differences detected by ANOVA and post hoc test were also detected by Kaplan-Meier plots and Gehan-Breslow-Wilcoxon test, which did not affect the final conclusion of this study. The results have been summarized and presented in Figures 6 and 7. Since ANOVA/post hoc test and Kaplan-Meier plots/Gehan-Breslow-Wilcoxon test gave an inconsistent statistical result for the latency between npba+/- and npba-/- fish as described above, we would like to present both the Kaplan-Meier plots and the original bar graphs (showing the results of ANOVA/post hoc test), rather than to replace the bar graphs with the Kaplan-Meier plots.

Specifically, we have revised the text and figures as follows:

1) Results section: “Behavioral time-series data sets were further analyzed using Kaplan-Meier plots with the inclusion of fish that did not spawn within the test period. Although the significant difference disappeared in the latency from the first courtship display to the wrapping that resulted in spawning between npba+/- and npba-/-, all other significant differences detected were also detected by this analysis, supporting the robustness of our results (the latency to the first wrapping; p = 0.0480 for npba+/+ versus npba-/- females, p = 0.0291 for npba+/- versus npba-/- females, p = 0.0162 for wild-type versus npba-/-/npbb-/- females, p < 0.0002 for wild-type versus npbwr2-/- females: the latency to the wrapping that resulted in spawning; p = 0.0129 for npba+/+ versus npba-/- females, p = 0.1158 for npba+/- versus npba-/- females, p = 0.1068 for wild-type versus npba-/-/npbb-/- females, p = 0.0006 for wild-type versus npbwr2-/- females) (Figure 6C, D and Figure 7B).” has been added.

2) Materials and methods: “Behavioral time-series data sets were further analyzed using Kaplan-Meier plots with the inclusion of fish that did not spawn within the test period, following Jahn-Eimermacher et al., 2011. Differences between Kaplan-Meier curves were tested for statistical significance using Gehan-Breslow-Wilcoxon test with Bonferroni’s correction.” has been added.

3) Reference list: the following reference, which is cited in the above text, has been added.

Jahn-Eimermacher, Lasarzik I, and Raber, 2011.

4) Legend for Figure 6: “(A, B)” has been added.

5) Legend for Figure 6: “(C, D) The latency data for npba (C) and npbb (D) single knockouts were further analyzed using Kaplan-Meier plots. Blue triangles, ocher diamonds, and beige circles represent wild-type, heterozygous knockout, and homozygous knockout females, respectively. *, p < 0.05 (Gehan-Breslow-Wilcoxon test with Bonferroni’s correction).” has been added.

6) Legend for Figure 7: “(A)” has been added.

7) Legend for Figure 7: “(B) The latency data were further analyzed using Kaplan-Meier plots. Blue triangles, ocher diamonds, and beige circles represent wild-type, npba/npbb double knockout, and npbwr2 knockout females, respectively. *, p < 0.05; ***, p < 0.001 (Gehan-Breslow-Wilcoxon test with Bonferroni’s correction).” has been added.

8) Figures 6 and 7: The Kaplan-Meier plots for the latency data have been added in these figures (panels C and D in Figure 6 and panel B in Figure 7).

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Kataaki Okubo. 2019. Oryzias latipes npbwr2 mRNA for neuropeptides B and W receptor 2. NCBI Genbank. LC375958

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Abbreviations of brain and spinal cord regions and brain nuclei.
    elife-39495-supp1.docx (14.6KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39495.025
    Supplementary file 2. Primers used in this study.
    elife-39495-supp2.docx (13.9KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39495.026
    Supplementary file 3. Species names and GenBank accession numbers of the protein sequences used in this study.
    elife-39495-supp3.docx (14.1KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39495.027
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39495.028

    Data Availability Statement

    Sequence data have been deposited in DDBJ/EMBL/GenBank with accession number LC375958.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Kataaki Okubo. 2019. Oryzias latipes npbwr2 mRNA for neuropeptides B and W receptor 2. NCBI Genbank. LC375958


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES