Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2019 Oct;31(4):1527–1540. doi: 10.1017/S0954579418001207

Simultaneously Examining Negative Appraisals, Emotion Reactivity, and Cognitive Reactivity in Relation to Depressive Symptoms in Children

David A Cole 1, Rachel L Zelkowitz 2, Elizabeth A Nick 3, Sophia R Lubarsky 4, Jason D Rights 5
PMCID: PMC6722000  NIHMSID: NIHMS1026758  PMID: 30837008

Abstract

Prior theory and research have linked negative appraisals (NA), emotion reactivity (ER), and cognitive reactivity (CR) to depression; however, few studies have examined even two of these constructs simultaneously, and none have done so in child or adolescent populations. 571 youths (ages 9–13) completed a novel procedure in which all three constructs were assessed in response to the same personally relevant, hypothetical, peer victimization events. Multilevel modeling enabled the extraction of dynamic, within-person, latent-variable measures of NA, ER, and CR. All three constructs were related to children’s depressive symptoms in ways that were commensurate with most (but not all) theoretical frameworks. Gender and age differences also emerged. Support for an NA-predicts-ER-predicts-CR model suggests ways that these constructs can be integrated into a more complete, trans-theoretical understanding of the cognitive-emotional substrate of depression in children.

Keywords: Negative Appraisals, Emotion Reactivity, Cognitive Reactivity Depression, Peer victimization, Children, Adolescents


One set of theories has focused on the role of negative appraisals in depression (Ingram, 1984; see also Everaert, Koster, & Derakshan, 2012). A second set of theories has linked depression to emotion reactivity (e.g., Davidson, 1998; Luby & Belden, 2006; Rottenberg, 2017). A third set of theories has associated depression with cognitive reactivity (e.g., Beck, 1963; Ingram, 1984). Despite the fact that appraisals, emotions, and cognitive reactions are almost inextricably interrelated processes, empirical studies have not rigorously examined these constructs together, let alone examined them as responses to the same set of stimuli. Consequently, evidence of their association to depressive symptoms is piecemeal, and their associations with each other are unexamined. Studies of these variables in childhood and adolescence are even rarer, despite the likelihood that this is when these processes are under construction. The goals of the current study were to examine the relation of negative appraisals, negative emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity to each other and to depressive symptoms in children and adolescents.

Negative appraisal (NA).

Cognitive models of depression have suggested that depression is closely related to negative or pessimistic appraisals of personally relevant life events. Building on work by Arnold (1960), Billings and Moos (1982), and Lazarus (1966), Ingram’s information processing model of depression defined negative appraisal (NA) as the process whereby individuals give subjective meaning to external events via the application of beliefs about the parameters and effects of the events (Ingram, 1984). Negative appraisals of life events activate depressive memory networks, which triggers biased elaborations about the negative material and facilitates deeper encoding of this material, thus increasing the probability of its elicitation in the future. For example, a child with a history of loss might regard a new loss experience as more devastating than would a child without such a history. Literature reviews support the association of depression with negatively biased interpretations, in youth as well as adults (Ackermann & DeRubeis, 1991; Dobson & Franche, 1989; Everaert et al., 2012; Platt, Waters, Schulte-Koerne, Engelmann, & Salemink, 2017), especially when the stimulus events are ecologically valid and participants’ attention is self-focused.

Emotion reactivity (ER).

Three broad theoretical frameworks anticipate relations of depression to emotion reactivity (ER). First, cognitive theories of depression predict potentiation of negative emotions. Depression is associated with negative self-schemas that, when activated, trigger strong negative affective responses (Beck, 1967). The second is general emotion theory from which one can infer that depressed mood will have widespread cognitive and affective repercussions. Rosenberg’s (1998) hierarchy of affective responses posits that depressed mood will potentiate negative emotions such as sadness (p. 253). Third, emotion context insensitivity theory predicts the opposite: that depression attenuates (or blunts) negative emotional reactions (Bylsma, Morris & Rottenberg, 2008). A meta-analysis of emotion reactivity studies showed a small but significant blunting effect of major depression on negative ER and a medium blunting effect on positive emotions (Bylsma et al., 2008). Rottenberg (2017) noted the consistency of these results with emotion context insensitivity, although he also raises several questions concerning ER and major depression (e.g., in emotion reactivity, to what is an individual is emotionally reacting?)

Typically, researchers have operationalized ER as the change in negative emotion from a baseline condition to a negative event condition; however, given individual differences in reactions to similar stimuli, researchers have emphasized the role of appraisals in both emotion generation and regulation (Davidson, 1998; Gross, 1998; Gross & Feldman Barrett, 2011). Although many studies assess negative appraisals (NA) as part of their research protocol, they do not incorporate such information into the calculation of ER. Assessments of appraisals are used only to verify event typology or to validate the experimental manipulation; they do not serve directly in the quantification of ER (e.g., Bylsma et al., 2011). Most studies operationalize ER as the change in negative emotion from a baseline condition to a negative event condition (Δ Negative Emotion), irrespective of how negatively either the baseline or negative event condition was assessed. A more refined approach would be to calibrate the change in emotion relative to the change in NA across conditions (Δ Negative Emotion / Δ Negative Appraisal). The conventional computation tacitly assumes that the difference between the appraisals of the baseline and event conditions is the same for everyone. In depression research, this is almost certainly not true, as we know that depressed individuals often perceive their current (or baseline) situation as more negative than do nondepressed individuals. Failure to take appraisals into account could seriously affect the relation of depression to ER.

Cognitive reactivity (CR).

Beck (1963, 1967; Clark, Beck & Alford, 1999) posited that negative cognitive schemas develop in childhood in response to negative life events. These schemas remain latent until activated by new events that evoke negative emotions reminiscent of those experienced when the schemas were first formed. When activated, such schemas affect attention, memory, and cognitive distortions (Ingram et al., 1998; Segal, 1988; Williams, Watts, MacLeod, & Mathews, 1988). Mood-activated cognitive schemas are central to the development, maintenance, and recurrence of depression (Kovacs & Beck, 1978). The term cognitive reactivity refers to “the relative ease with which maladaptive cognitions or cognitive styles are triggered by mild (nonpathological) mood fluctuations” (Williams, Van der Does, Barnhofer, Crane, & Segal, 2008). CR is not simply the existence of maladaptive cognitions; nor is it simply the presence of negative mood. It is a dynamic strength-of-association construct, reflecting the extent to which negative mood triggers maladaptive self-cognitions. As such, it is conceptually similar to the slope of a regression line representing the strength of relation between two variables: slope = Δ Negative Cognitions /Δ Negative Emotion (i.e., how much negative cognition changes for a given change in negative emotion). Literature reviews consistently support the relation of mood-activated cognitive schemas to many aspects of and risk for depression (Ingram, 1984; Scher, Ingram, & Segal, 2005).

Gender and age.

Despite the enormous literature on gender differences in depression (Kessler, McGonagle, Swartz, Blazer, & Nelson, 1993; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2012), relatively few studies have examined gender differences in negative appraisals, emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity (NA, ER, and CR). Studies on gender and NA have largely focused on anxiety and threat appraisal not depression (Rasa & Hopp, 1989; Stroud, Salovey & Epel, 2002; Sass et al., 2010). In a cross-sectional study of young Polish adults, women appraised traumatic events more negatively than did men even after symptom severity was statistically controlled; negative appraisal and gender also interacted to predict internalizing symptoms (Kucharska, 2017). Gender studies on emotion are numerous (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2012) but complicated by the conflation of ER with emotionality, emotion regulation, and mood, which are different constructs. Literature reviews descry the need for studies examining gender as a potential moderator (Bylsma et al., 2008; Rottenberg, 2017). Gender studies on CR are rarer, although some theoretical connections can be drawn from the tend-and-befriend literature (Taylor, 2012; Taylor, Klein, Lewis, Gruenewald, Gurung, & Updegraff, 2000). Other work has focused on cognitive style and negative inferences, suggesting that they may be responsible for part of the gender difference in adolescent depression (e.g., Hankin & Abramson, 2002). Focusing explicitly on CR, Booij and Van der Does (2007) reported that females scored higher on a paper-and-pencil measure of CR, as well as on a concomitant measure of serotonergic vulnerability to depression. Our focus on middle childhood provides an opportunity to examine gender differences in the roles of NA, ER, and CR just prior to the time when gender differences in depression begin to emerge.

Negative appraisals, emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity (NA, ER, and CR) almost certainly have at least some of their roots in childhood. Beck originally suggested that CR was born out of early negative life events. More specifically, Cole’s learning-based model regards NA and CR as early internalizations of repeated pattern of aversive interactions with parents and peers (Cole et al., 2010, 2014a, 2014b, 2016; Sinclair et al., 2012; Tran et al., 2012). Some research suggests that these relations strengthen with age. Studying children of divorce, Sheets, Sandler, and West (1996) found that older children (10–12 year olds compared to 8–9 year olds) had more differentiated appraisals of divorce-related events, which predicted psychological symptoms over and above the negative events themselves. Although NA, ER, and CR have all been examined in children, no study has examined age as a moderator of these processes.

Methodological concerns.

In an attempt to investigate NA, ER, and CR simultaneously, at least four methodological considerations are critical. First, assessment strategies must be sensitive to the dynamic nature of these constructs. Cole et al. (2014b) advocated methods that directly quantify these constructs in terms of within-person strengths of association. NA is a function of the within-person association of negative events to the subjective appraisal of these events. ER is the within-person association of subjective event appraisals to one’s emotional responses. CR is the within-person association of emotional responses to negative self-cognitions.

Second, these processes should be assessed in response to the same set of events. Taken together, the above theoretical models describe a collection of cognitive or emotional responses to a particular event: e.g., (1) an event is interpreted as negative; (2) the negatively interpreted event evokes an emotional response; (3) the emotional response to the event activates depressive schemas. Assessing appraisals, emotions, and cognitions as responses to different events will diminish the inherent connectivity among these constructs (Dagleish et al., 2003; Gotlib et al., 2004; Mogg et al., 2000).

Third, in the assessment paradigm, the events that trigger these processes should be ecologically valid. Previous research has shown that effects are smaller when the emotion- or cognition-eliciting events involve arbitrary laboratory stimuli as opposed to stimuli that are personally relevant to the participant (Dobson & Franche, 1989).

Fourth, effects are more evident when these processes are assessed under conditions of self-focused attention. Beck’s theory stipulates that negative cognitive schemas are more effectively triggered by events that focus attention on the self. Literature reviews suggest that effect sizes are stronger when participants have their attention focused on themselves (Everaert et al., 2012; Ingram, 1990; Ingram & Smith, 1984).

Goals and hypotheses.

The overarching goal of the current study was to examine the relation of children’s depression symptoms to negative appraisals, emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity (NA, ER, and CR), using methods that are sensitive to the dynamic nature of these constructs and assess all three constructs in response to the same set of ecologically valid, self-focused events. We had five goals or hypotheses. First, we hypothesized that depressive symptoms would be related to NA, irrespective of the objective severity of the negative events being appraised. Our second goal was to examine the relation between depressive symptoms and ER; however, because of conflicting theoretical predictions, we did not make directional hypotheses. Third, we hypothesized that depressive symptoms would be positively related to CR. Our fourth goal was to examine the relations among NA, ER, and CR as responses to the same set of peer victimization scenarios. Fifth, a more exploratory goal was to examine gender and age as moderators of the NA, ER, and CR processes.

Method

Participants

Participants were students in grades 3–6 attending one of five metropolitan public schools in Tennessee. School principals and the University IRB approved the study. Out of 1,021 students from 55 classrooms, 571 students obtained parental consent and participated in the study (n = 114 in Grade 3, n = 104 in Grade 4, n = 176 in Grade 5, and n = 177 in Grade 6). Differences between participants and nonparticipants on gender, age, and race/ethnicity were not significant (ps > .20). Participants were 55.87% female and had an average age of 10.89 years (SD = 1.20). The sample race/ethnicity proportionately represented the school district: 69.88% Caucasian, 33.10% African American, 7.36% Hispanic or Mexican American, 5.60% Asian or Asian American, and 4.03% other (participants could endorse more than one race/ethnicity).

Measures

Depressive symptoms.

The Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale - version 2 (RADS-2; Reynolds, 2002) is a 30-item self-report inventory assessing depressive symptoms in young people. The measure assesses dysphoric mood, anhedonia, negative affect, negative self-evaluation, somatic complaints. We used the total RADS-2 score. Example items include “I feel lonely” and “I feel I am no good.” Response options range from 1 to 4 (almost never to most of the time). In school-age samples, the RADS-2 has coefficient alphas of .91 to .93, 3-week test–retest reliability of .87, and strong criterion-related validity (Reynolds & Mazza, 1998). The measure has been used successfully with children as young as nine years old (Laurent, Joiner, & Catanzaro, 2011; Painter, 2012). In the current sample, coefficient alpha was .91.

Negative appraisal, emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity (NA, ER, and CR).

We assessed NA, ER, and CR via an adaptation of Cole et al.’s (2014) Behind Your Back procedure. This method involves audio recordings of 21 brief conversations plus a practice item of a boy and girl talking to each other about a third student. The content of these conversations ranged from mild to mean. Different versions used gendered pronouns so that the third student was of the same gender as the participant. A printed version of the same conversations accompanied the recordings. Instructions are, “Try to imagine that you hear two people talking about you behind your back. Below is what they say. Read each conversation as you listen to them and pretend you could actually hear them. Also pretend that they are talking about you. After each conversation, there will be some questions for you to answer. Circle a number to answer each question.”

Each scenario was enacted and recorded by trained actors with youthful voices. To make the conversations seem more personal, the recordings were transferred to MP3 players, and each participant listened to them through noise-cancelling headphones. Each scenario was preceded by 2 s of ambient noise recorded from a school hallway or cafeteria. The noise abated, cueing the participant’s attention to the beginning of the recorded scenario.

Four questions follow each scenario: one assesses perceived meanness of the scenario; one assesses the respondents’ emotional response to the scenario, and two inquire about thoughts the respondent might have (all using 5-point Likert scales: 1 = not at all, 2 = just a little, 3 = somewhat, 4 = pretty much, 5 = a lot.) To control for order effects, we used two versions of the recordings that differed only with regard to the order in which the scenarios were presented. The following is an example of a mild scenario from the girl version, followed by the four questions:

Speaker 1: Here she comes. Do you want to let her work with us?

Speaker 2: I don’t know. What do you think?

Speaker 1: Well, I guess I don’t mind.

Speaker 2: OK, we’ll let her this one time.

Think about what they said:

  1. How mean were they being?

  2. If they were talking about you, how sad would this make you feel?

    If you heard this, how much would it make you think:

  3. I’ll probably mess this up.

  4. They aren’t going to like me.

The following is an example of a mean scenario from the boy version, followed by the four questions:

Speaker 1: Why did he do that? What’s wrong with him anyway?

Speaker 2: He’s so clueless!

Speaker 1: I know, right?! Maybe someone should tell him.

Speaker 2: It wouldn’t do any good.

Think about what they said:

  1. How mean were they being?

  2. If they were talking about you, how sad would this make you feel?

    If you heard this, how much would it make you think,

  3. I am an idiot.

  4. I don’t like myself.

BYB data reduction.

Objective (or nomothetic) meanness scores were the average question 1 ratings across all participants for each scenario. Average meanness scores for the 21 scenarios ranged from 2.02 to 4.59 (M = 3.48, SD = 0.99). Exploratory factor analysis revealed that the 42 negative cognition items (questions 3 and 4 for 21 scenarios) loaded onto a single general factor. Internal consistency was high with a coefficient alpha of .98. Consequently, we averaged the two cognitive items for each scenario, yielding one cognitive score for each of the 21 scenarios.

Slopes and intercepts.

We conceptualized each person’s NA, ER, and CR (i.e., negative appraisals, emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity) as a function of the within-person relation between two variables across scenarios (see Figure 1, plots A-F). First, NA is a function of the relation of perceived meanness to objective meanness. In plot A, the x-axis represents the objective meanness of each scenario. The y-axis represents each person’s perceived meanness of the same scenarios. Regression lines represent the relation of each person’s subjective ratings to the objective ratings across the scenarios. All participants would have such lines, and the person-specific slopes and intercepts of these lines could vary as a function of the person’s level of depressive symptoms. The person-specific slopes and intercepts can be aggregated across subsets of individuals to create average lines. In this context, NA becomes the average perceived meanness at a specific level of objective meanness. This definition allows NA to vary as a function of the objective meanness of the stimulus event. As such, NA becomes especially interesting as a relativistic construct. For example, more depressed individuals might exhibit more NA than less depressed individuals in response to events containing the same amount of objective meanness. In plot A, the upper line might represent a relatively depressed person; the lower line could represent a relatively nondepressed person. Here the lines are parallel, so the difference in intercepts for these lines would signify that people with few depressive symptoms have greater NA than people with more depressive symptoms across all levels of objective meanness. In plot B, however, the lines converge (because of a depression x objective meanness interaction), suggesting that relatively depressed and nondepressed people’s levels of NA become more similar when events have high levels of objective meanness. In the absence of such an interaction, we operationalized NA as a person’s average subjective meanness. In the presence of an interaction, NA is conditional upon the level of objective meanness.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Hypothetical depictions of depression affecting the slopes and intercepts embedded in the negative appraisal, emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity (NA, ER, and CR) concepts.

Second, ER is the strength of a person’s emotional response as a function of perceived scenario meanness. For ER (plots C and D in Figure 1), subjective appraisals of the events are plotted on the x-axis, and emotional responses to the events on the y-axis. ER is operationalized as the slope of these regression lines. Plot C shows an interaction representing the potentiation hypothesis: the effect of perceived event negativity on negative emotion is stronger for people with more versus fewer depressive symptoms. Plot D shows a different interaction, representing the emotion context insensitivity hypothesis: the effect of perceived event negativity on emotions is attenuated by higher levels of depression.

Third, CR is the strength of a person’s negative cognitive responses as a function of a person’s reported emotional responses to the scenarios. For CR (plots E and F in Figure 1), negative emotional responses are plotted on the x-axis and negative self-cognitive responses on the y-axis. CR is operationalized as the slopes of the lines. Plot E shows two main effects but no interaction. The strength of relation between negative emotion and negative self-cognitions is not affected by depressive symptoms. Plot F shows an interaction supporting the hypothesis that depressive symptoms affect CR: the effect of negative emotion on negative self-cognitions is greater for people with higher levels of depression.

These operationalizations differ from previous ones. For example, Nock et al.’s (2008) emotion reactivity scale focuses on emotional response characteristics such as the threshold for reactivity, the peak amplitude of response, the rise time to peak and the recovery time, but does not consider the stimulus strength of the triggering event. Mood-induction approaches to CR do not incorporate individual differences in the emotional response into the index of CR. Questionnaire measures of CR (e.g., Solis, Antypa, Conijn, Kelderman & Van der Does, 2017) ask people to intuit the strength of relation between emotion and cognition instead of measuring it directly. The closest to the proposed methods is Bylsma et al.’s (2011) hierarchical linear modeling approach to ER; however, even it dichotomized events into negative or not, instead of using people’s actual ratings of negativity.

Procedure

Data collection occurred in the schools. Research assistants escorted groups of consented students to a meeting room in the school, described the study, answered questions, and administered assent forms for signatures. After distributing paper-and-pencil versions of all measures to the participants, one research assistant read aloud the initial demographic questions, while participants marked responses on their printed questionnaires. Other research assistants circulated around the classroom, addressing questions that arose and preventing the occasional off-task behavior. Next, headsets and MP3 players were distributed for the Behind Your Back procedure. Students listened and marked their answers to the questions on the forms provided. Next, the RADS-2 was administered; it was read aloud while participants responded on their own hard copies. At the end of data collection, students were thanked for their participation, offered a snack, and given a decorative pencil.

Results

Multilevel Modeling

In order to model the effects of within-person operationalizations of negative appraisals, emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity (NA, ER, and CR), we used multilevel models in which the 21 scenarios (Level 1) were nested within person (Level 2). This method enabled us to compare the within-person effects to the effects of the more conventional between-person approach. To isolate the within-person variability of perceived meanness and reported sadness, we person-centered both variables by subtracting the person’s mean from each of the 21 scenario-specific scores. We refer to these as c_meanness and c_sadness, respectively. The between-person information consisted of the person means for these variables, m_meanness and m_sadness, respectively. For ER, we then fit the following two-level model with random intercepts and random slopes:

Level 1:sadnessij=β0j+β1jc_meannessij+eijLevel2:β0j=γ00+γ01m_meanness+u0jβ1j=γ10+u1j

with eijN(0,σe2) and [u0ju1j]N([00],[τ00τ10τ11]). The analysis was repeated for CR, where the dependent variable was cognition and the independent variables were c_sadness and m_sadness. The correspondence between the verbal and technical definitions of NA, ER, and CR are detailed in Table 1. The maximum likelihood results in Table 2 clearly show that the within- and between-person components of both variables significantly predicted their respective dependent variables. Furthermore, using Rights and Sterba (in press) new method for partitioning variance in multilevel models, we also see that the variance explained by the within-person components was substantial (see the right-most column of Table 2). For ER, 31.8% of the total variance in sadness was explained by c_meanness whereas 10.2% was explained by m_meanness, meaning that 75.7% of the overall effect of perceived meanness on sadness was due to the within-person component (and 24.3% due to the between-person component). For CR, 31.3% of the effect of sadness on cognition was due to the within-person component (and 68.7% due to the between-person component).1

Table 1.

Technical and Conceptual Definitions of Negative Appraisals (NA), Emotion Reactivity (ER), and Cognitive Reactivity (CR)

Concept Definition Technical operationalization

NA Across a series of situations, the average within-person tendency to perceive situations as meaner (or less mean) than objective meanness ratings. NA=β0jMasshowninthelevel1equation:meannessij=β0jM+β1jMavgMEANij+eijM
ER Across a series of situations, the within-person strength of association between perceived situation meanness and degree of sadness evoked. ER=β1jSasshowninthelevel1equation:sadnessij=β0jS+β1jSc_meannessij+eijS
CR Across a series of situations, the within-person strength of association between induced sadness and degree of negative cognitions evoked. CR=β1jMasshowninthelevel1equation:cognitionij=β0jC+β1jCc_sadnessij+eijC

Table 2.

Results of Two-level Model with Random Intercepts and Random Slopes, Showing Between- and Within-person Effects

Parameter Est. SE df t p < Proportion of
total variance
explained (R 2)
Proportion
of R2
explained

   Emotion Reactivity: Perceived Meanness → Sadness
Intercept 0.536 0.126 682.18 4.26 0.001 -- --
Within- person (γ01) 0.631 0.013 540.63 48.66 0.001 .318 .757
Between- person (γ10) 0.694 0.035 661.18 19.74 0.001 .102 .243

  Cognitive Reactivity: Sadness → Cognition
Intercept 0.402 0.103 643.08 3.92 0.001 -- --
Within- person (γ01) 0.332 0.013 555.31 26.25 0.001 .094 .313
Between- person (γ10) 0.655 0.033 637.37 19.90 0.001 .206 .687

In keeping with our within-person variable definitions, all subsequent analyses retained only the person-mean-centered BYB predictors. Level 2 predictors were added to the model to test the effects of depressive symptoms (RADS-2), Gender, and Age. For example, the following equations test ER hypotheses regarding the fixed effects of depressive symptoms on random slopes and intercepts from the level-1 regression of sadness onto c_meanness:

Level1:sadnessij=β0j+β1jc_meannessij+eijLevel2:β0j=γ00+γ01RADS+γ02Gender+γ03Age+u0jβ1j=γ10+γ11RADS+γ12Gender+γ13Age+u1j

Similar methods were used to test hypotheses about CR and NA. All models were tested using maximum likelihood estimation in SPSS MIXED (version 25) and/or Mplus (version 7.4).

Descriptive Statistics

Table 2 contains correlations, means, and standard deviations for all study measures. No measure of negative appraisals, emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity (NA, ER, or CR) appears in this table, as they represent dynamic latent variables implicit in our multilevel statistical model. Descriptive statistics are included for BYB-based measures of meanness, sadness, and negative cognitions. RADS-2 data were similar to those reported in other studies of school-based samples (e.g., Reynolds & Mazza, 1998). Approximately 5.8% of participants (n = 33) had RADS-2 raw scores greater than 82 (a conventional cutoff for moderate to severe depression; Reynolds, 2002).2 Intraclass correlations ranged from .17 to .54, necessitating the use of multilevel modeling to account for the dependency of scenarios within person (see Table 3).

Table 3.

Correlation, Means, and Standard Deviations


Measure RADS-2 Meanness Sadness Cognition Mean SD ICC

RADS-2 1.00 53.61 15.13 --
Meanness .195 1.00 3.48 0.68 .17
Sadness .230 .619 1.00 2.95 0.93 .33
Cognition .464 .429 .645 1.00 2.33 1.04 .54

Negative Appraisal (NA)

To test the effects of the RADS-2, Gender, and Age on NA, we estimated a two-level multilevel model with random intercepts and slopes. The dependent variable was subjective meanness ratings of the scenarios. The level-1 predictor consisted of the person mean-centered objective meanness scores for the same scenarios (avgMEAN). Level-2 predictors of the level-1 slopes and intercepts were RADS-2, Gender, and Age. Each person-specific intercept represents the person’s average NA. As shown in Table 4, the cross-level Age x Objective meanness interaction was significant, as were all four main effects. This pattern of results is evident in Figure 2. In the upper plot, the main effect of RAD-2 is signified by the difference between the intercepts of the two lines (representing minimum and maximum RADS-2), indicating higher levels of NA among more depressed children (p < .001, d < .80). This effect is commensurate with hypothetical model A in Figure 1. In the middle plot, the intercept was significantly greater for females than males (p < .001, d = .54), indicating moderately higher NA for females. In the bottom plot, the significant interaction and Age main effect indicate that younger participants’ subjective ratings were not as strongly associated with the objective ratings (p <.002) and were more negative (p < .001) than were the older participants’ ratings of the same scenarios (d = .31).

Table 4.

Results for Two-level Multilevel Models with Random Intercepts and Random Slopes, Testing Main Effects and Cross-level Interactions of Depression (RADS-2), Gender, and Age in the Analysis of NA, ER, and CR.

Predictor Estimate SE df t p < 95% CI

Negative Appraisal: DV = Subjective meanness
Intercept 4.115 0.268 559.23 15.38 0.001 3.589 to 4.640
Objective meanness 0.551 0.193 558.55 2.85 0.005 0.172 to 0.931
RADS-2 0.007 0.002 557.22 3.81 0.001 0.003 to 0.011
Gender (male) −0.369 0.055 558.04 −6.66 0.001 −0.478 to −0.260
Age (years) −0.078 0.023 558.89 −3.43 0.001 −0.122 to −0.033
Obj. meanness * RADS-2 −0.002 0.001 555.02 −1.63 0.104 −0.005 to 0.000
Obj. meanness * Gender 0.002 0.040 556.39 0.06 0.952 −0.076 to 0.081
Obj. meanness * Age 0.052 0.016 557.89 3.15 0.002 0.019 to 0.084

Emotion Reactivity: DV = Reported sadness
Intercept 3.619 0.355 558.95 10.20 0.001 2.922 to 4.316
Subjective meanness 1.011 0.125 523.35 8.20 0.001 0.766 to 1.257
RADS-2 0.012 0.002 557.89 4.82 0.001 0.007 to 0.016
Gender (male) −0.599 0.074 558.31 −8.15 0.001 −0.743 to −0.454
Age (years) −0.095 0.030 558.77 −3.15 0.002 −0.154 to −0.036
Sub. meanness * RADS-2 0.002 0.001 522.59 2.50 0.013 0.000 to 0.004
Sub. meanness * Gender −0.153 0.026 513.00 −5.96 0.001 −0.204 to −0.103
Sub. meanness * Age −0.038 0.011 523.35 −3.61 0.001 −0.059 to −0.017

Cognitive Reactivity: DV = Negative cognitions
Intercept 1.950 0.377 559.19 5.17 0.001 1.209 to 2.690
Sadness 0.317 0.125 516.96 2.53 0.012 0.070 to 0.563
RADS-2 0.031 0.003 558.57 12.07 0.001 0.026 to 0.036
Gender (male) −0.209 0.078 558.78 −2.68 0.008 −0.363 to −0.056
Age (years) −0.109 0.032 559.06 −3.41 0.001 −0.172 to −0.046
Sadness * RADS-2 0.002 0.001 521.44 2.58 0.010 0.001 to 0.004
Sadness * Gender −0.013 0.026 539.72 −0.49 0.626 −0.065 to 0.039
Sadness * Age −0.010 0.011 526.42 −0.95 0.341 −0.031 to 0.011

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Negative Appraisals: Effects of objective story meanness (+/−1 SD), depression, gender, and age on subjective ratings of scenario meanness

Emotion Reactivity (ER)

We used a similar multilevel model to test hypotheses about ER. Specifically, we regressed scenario-based sadness ratings onto person-mean centered subjective meanness at level 1. Level-2 RADS-2, Gender, and Age predicted level-1 slopes and intercepts. All four main effects and all three cross-level interactions were significant (see Table 4). Each slope depicted in Figure 3 represents the strength of relation between perceived story meanness and the degree of a sad emotional response to the scenarios. In other words, the slopes represent ER. Taken together, the three significant interactions indicate that more depressed participants, female participants, and younger participants exhibited greater ER in response to the peer victimization scenarios (ds = .60, .59, and .59, respectively). These effects are similar to the hypothetical model C in Figure 1.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Emotion Reactivity: Effects of subjective story meanness (+/−1 SD), depression, gender, and age on sad responses to peer victimization scenarios.

Cognitive Reactivity (CR)

To test hypotheses about CR, we regressed scenario-based ratings of negative self-cognition onto person-mean centered sadness at level 1. At level 2, we again tested the effect of RADS-2, Gender, and Age on level-1 random slopes and intercepts. All level-1 and level-2 main effects were significant, as was the cross-level RADS-2 × Sadness interaction (see Table 4). The Figure 4 slopes represent CR (i.e., the strength of relation between sad emotion and negative self-cognitions). The significant RADS-2 × Sadness interaction therefore indicates that higher levels of depression are associated with greater CR (i.e., the slopes; d = .63). This phenomenon resembles hypothetical model F in Figure 1. Figure 4 also shows that depressive symptoms had a very large effect (d = 2.19), being female had a small but significant effect (d = .20), and being younger had a small to medium effect (d = .42) on negative cognitions (i.e., the intercepts).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Cognitive Reactivity: Effects of sad emotional responses (+/−1 SD), depression, gender, and age on negative self-cognitions.

Relations among NA, ER, and CR

To understand the relations among negative appraisals, emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity (NA, ER, and CR), we used multilevel structural equation modeling. By definition, ER is the tendency of a person to respond emotionally to events that are perceived as negative. As such, ER presupposes NA. Consequently, we incorporated the NA-predicts-ER relation into our structural model. Also by definition, CR is the tendency to have negative thoughts about oneself in response to negative emotion. As such CR presupposes ER. Therefore, we incorporated the ER-predicts-CR relation into our structural model as well. Taken together, these components combine to form a (cross-sectional) level 2 NA-ER-CR model (i.e., NA statistically predicts ER which statistically predicts CR). The level 1 portion of the model was a multivariate combination of the level 1 portions of the three previous models. The complete model is represented by the following equations:

Level1:meannessij=β0jM+β1jMavgMEANij+eijMsadnessij=β0jS+β1jSc_meannessij+eijScognitionij=β0jC+β1jCc_sadnessij+eijCLevel2:β0jM=γ00M+u0jMβ1jM=γ10M+u1jMβ0jS=γ00S+u0jSβ1jS=γ10S+γaβ0jM+u1jSβ0jC=γ00C+u0jCβ1jC=γ10C+γbβ1jS+γcβ0jM+u1jC,

where [eijMeijSeijC]~MVN(σM2σMS2σS2σMC2σSC2σC2) and [u0jMu1jMu0jSu1jSu0jCu1jC]~MVN(τ00Mτ10Mτ11Mτ00MSτ10MSτ00S0τ11MSτ10Sτ11Sτ00MCτ10MCτ00SCτ10SCτ00C0τ11MCτ10SC0τ10Cτ11C).

The level 1 results were very similar to those reported in the previous univariate models. The level 2 structural parameter estimates appear in Figure 5. Both the NA-predicts-ER relation (γa) and the ER-predicts-CR relation (γb) were significant. The NA-predicts-CR relation (γc) was small and nonsignificant, indicating that most of the NA relation to CR was explained by ER. Using Selig and Preacher’s (2008) simulation method, we estimated the indirect relation to be γaγb=0.065 (95% CI = 0.040 to 0.095), hence significant at α = .05.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Level 2 model of level 1 random intercepts and slopes, where NA = β0jM, ER = β1jS, and CR = β1jC.

Discussion

In the current paper, we successfully implemented a new method for assessing three dynamic responses (negative appraisals, emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity: NA, ER, CR) to personally relevant, stressful events under conditions of self-focused attention. Five major sets of results emerged. First, children with higher levels of depressive symptoms exhibited greater NA, signifying that they evaluated peer victimization scenarios more negatively than did children with low levels of depressive symptoms. Second, children with higher levels of depressive symptoms exhibited more negative ER than did children with low levels of depressive symptoms; that is, the perceived negativity of an event had a stronger negative emotional effect on children with higher levels of depressive symptoms. Third, children with higher levels of depressive symptoms exhibited greater CR than did children with lower levels of such symptoms; that is, sad emotional responses were more strongly associated with negative self-cognitions for children with higher levels of depressive symptoms. Fourth, NA, ER, and CR, related to each other in a manner consistent with a NA-ER-CR model. Fifth, gender and age differences emerged in NA, ER, and CR. In general, these innovative methods yielded important results that help to integrate aspects of diverse cognitive- and emotion-based theories of depression.

Our first set of findings pertains to our novel method for measuring NA, ER, and CR simultaneously and in response to the same set of social stimuli. This method assesses all three constructs as parts of dynamic within-person processes. We operationalized NA as the tendency to over- (or under-) estimate the negativity of hypothetical peer victimization scenarios at a given level of objective meanness. (In the current study this was the intercept in the within-person regression of perceived meanness onto objective meanness; however, this method allows for the possibility that NA could vary with the degree of objective meanness). Our measure of ER was the strength of relation between the perceived negativity of and a negative emotional response to the same victimization scenarios (i.e., the slope of the within-person regression of sadness onto perceived meanness). Our measure of CR was the strength of relation between one’s emotional responses to these scenarios and the endorsement of negative self-cognitions (i.e., the slope of the within-person regression of self-cognitions onto reported sadness).

With these methods, support accrued to a model in which NA predicts ER predicts CR. This statistical model is consistent with two important theoretical phenomena: (1) that ER may be triggered by the subjective negative appraisals of stressful events, and (2) that negative self-cognitions may be triggered by emotional reactions to these same events. These results integrate elements of three relatively separate avenues of depression-related research: information-processing models of depression (Ingram, 1984; Everaert et al., 2012), emotion reactivity and emotion context insensitivity models of depression (Davidson et al., 2002; Rottenberg, 2017; Rottenberg, Gross & Gotlib, 2005), and emotion-activated cognitive schema models of depression (Beck, 1963; Clark et al., 1999). Previous research has not found substantial correlations among such constructs (Everaert et al., 2012). The difference between our study and previous work may be due to our assessment of all three constructs as responses to the same set of stimulus events – events that were personally relevant and focused attention on the self.

Our second, third, and fourth findings were that depressive symptoms were associated with stronger negative appraisals, emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity (NA, ER, and CR) when measured as parts of children’s responses to the same set of hypothetical, negative, social events. These results support previous NA research, concluding that depressive symptoms are associated with more negative interpretations of events (Everaert et al., 2012; Platt et al., 2017). Although debate continues as to whether depressed people’s more negative views are negatively biased (or whether nondepressed people’s more positive views are positively biased), the current study adds to a growing body of literature supporting the NA hypothesis in child populations (Bistricky, Ingram, Siegle & Short, 2015; Cole, Martin, Peeke, Seroczynski & Fier, 1999; Cole, Martin, Peeke, Seroczynski & Hoffman, 1998; Malcarne, Hamilton, Ingram & Taylor, 2000).

Our ER results are compatible with models that suggest depressive symptoms are related to greater (not blunted) negative emotion reactivity: compare Rosenberg (1998), Luby and Belden (2006), and Beck (1963) versus Bylsma et al. (2008) and Rottenberg (2017). Methodological differences could account for the disparity between our results and studies that appear to support the emotion context insensitivity hypothesis. Specifically, studies of ER typically compare emotional reactions after a negative event to similar measures obtained during a baseline condition (Rottenberg, 2017). The possibility exists, however, that the natural baseline condition may actually be (or at least be perceived to be) more negative for people with high levels of depression. If true, the discrepancy between the baseline and the negative event conditions may be smaller for relatively depressed people, creating effectively a smaller “dose” of negativity for high-depression individuals, which could create the appearance of a blunted response for people with more depressive symptoms. Our new method for assessing ER avoids this possibility by calibrating the emotion reaction against the empirically assessed change in perceived negativity.3

Our CR results expand upon a small but important literature demonstrating a link between depressive symptoms and emotion-induced negative self-cognitions in children (e.g., Martin & Cole, 2000; Taylor & Ingram, 1999), similar to the literature for adults (Scher et al., 2005). Somewhat surprising in both literatures, however, is the poor correspondence between different methods of assessing depression-related cognitive schemas (Everaert et al., 2012). We speculate that one reason for such low convergence may be contextual differences among the different assessments. The stimuli that affect responses in an emotional Stroop task are quite different from those that affect recall in an incidental memory task or one’s answers to a questionnaire. Even though multiple aspects of depressive schemas all relate to depression, they may not correlate with each other when assessed in response to qualitatively different stimuli. The current paradigm enables researchers to examine multiple aspects of complex cognitive-emotion systems in response to the same set of stimuli.

Our fifth set of results pertains to gender and age differences in NA, ER, and CR. Several literature reviews call for more studies focused on potential moderators of these processes (Bylsma et al., 2008; Ingram, 1990; Rottenberg, 2017; Scher et al., 2005). Given the profound age-related increase in depression during early adolescence and given the emergent gender difference in depression over this same time interval, gender and age are two potentially important demographic moderators. Gender differences in NA, ER, and CR during middle childhood could pave the way for gender differences in depression during adolescence.

In the current study, several gender differences emerged. Girls evinced higher NA than did boys, in that they perceived the perpetrators of hypothetical peer victimization events as being slightly meaner. Girls also reported slightly more sadness and having slightly more negative self-cognitions in response to these scenarios compared to boys. Girls also had higher levels of ER (but not greater CR) compared to boys. That is, in response to the same increase in perceived scenario meanness, girls’ sadness increased more than did boys’. These findings are consistent with previous studies on gender differences in ER and stress reactivity, in which girls were shown to have higher reactivity than boys, at least for specific emotions and circumstances; e.g., Charbonneau, Mezulis, & Hyde, 2009; Hanken, Mermelstein, & Roesch, 2007; Luby et al., 2009; Prinstein, Cheah & Guyer, 2005; Rudolph, 2002). The results are also consistent with Hyde et al.’s (2008) model of gender differences in depression, in which enhanced ER combines with other vulnerabilities to produce higher rates of depression among adolescent females versus males.

Several age differences also emerged. Younger children displayed greater NA in response to mild or ambiguous negative scenarios, compared to older participants. This age difference disappeared when the events were extremely mean, in which cases both older and younger children perceived the events as being extremely negative. On ER and CR, age differences resembled the gender differences. Younger participants reported somewhat more sadness and slightly more negative cognitions than did older participants. Younger children also had greater ER (but not greater CR) compared to older children; that is, in response to increased perceived scenario meanness, younger children’s sadness increased more than did older children’s. These findings may reflect developmental advances in emotion regulation. As Davidson (1998) noted, “rarely does an emotion get generated in the absence of recruiting associated regulatory processes” (p. 308). Children may become more facile at recruiting emotion regulation responses as they grow older, thus dampening their reactivity to less-intense stimuli.

At least four limitations of the current study suggest directions for future research. First, we elected to focus on a community sample of children in grades 3 through 6. The generalizability of these results to clinical populations with higher levels of depression or to adolescents remains to be tested. As Salk, Hyde, and Abramson’s (2017) large meta-analysis noted, the largest gender difference in diagnoses of depression occurs between 13 and 15. Examining the relations among NA, ER, and CR in a slightly older age group may help elucidate this gender discrepancy. Second, although the current methods of assessing NA, ER, and CR hold considerable promise, an important next step will be direct comparisons to more conventional measures of these constructs. Third, the current study focused on reactivity to hypothetical peer victimization events, a salient and personally relevant set of stressors in children’s lives. An advantage of this approach is that all participants respond to the same (carefully constructed) set of events; a disadvantage is that these events may not be equally relevant to all children. An important direction for future research would be to compare methods that use hypothetical events (Cole et al., 2014) to those that use real events (Byslma et al., 2011). Fourth, the goal of the current study was to characterize the relation of depressive symptoms to NA, ER, and CR, for which cross-sectional methods are appropriate. Given the significant pattern of results, longitudinal research is warranted to test prospective (and possibly mediational) relations among these constructs.

In conclusion, the present study adds to the growing literature on the relation of negative appraisals, emotion reactivity, and cognitive reactivity to depressive symptoms and the potential interplay among these constructs. We demonstrate a unique methodology that enables the concomitant, dynamic measurement of all three constructs, which enables researchers to test multiple aspects of cognitive-emotion theories of depression simultaneously. Utilization of these methods enabled us to find support for several hypotheses about NA, ER, and CR, assessed simultaneously and in response to the same set of stimuli, potentially paving the way for a more complete, trans-theoretical understanding of the cognitive-emotional substrate of depression in children.

Footnotes

D. A. Cole receives support from a donation from the Patricia and Rodes Hart family. E. A. Nick was supported from National Institute of Mental Health training grant T32MH018921-26. R. L. Zelkowitz was supported by National Institute of Mental Health National Research Service Award F31MH108241-01A1.

1

These represent proportions of the variance in the dependent variable that are explained by predictors via fixed effects (as opposed to random effect variation). Note: we do not partition the variance for NA, as there is no between-person variability in objective meanness.

2

For all such students, we contacted parents and school counselors, in keeping with the informed consent procedures and our IRB protocol.

3

One could also use an objective measure of event negativity for this calibration, but we regarded the relation of objective to subjective event negativity as an important process in its own right.

Contributor Information

David A. Cole, Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University

Rachel L. Zelkowitz, Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University

Elizabeth A. Nick, Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University

Sophia R. Lubarsky, Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University

Jason D. Rights, Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University

References

  1. Ackermann R, & DeRubeis RJ (1991). Is depressive realism real? Clinical Psychology Review, 11(5), 565–584. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(91)90004-E [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arnold MB (1960). Emotion and personality. New York: Columbia University Press. [Google Scholar]
  3. Beck AT (1963). Thinking and depression: I. Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 9, 324–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beck AT (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Hoeber. [Google Scholar]
  5. Billings AG, & Moos RH (1982). Psychosocial theory and research on depression: An integrative framework and review.Clinical Psychology Review, 2 213–237. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bistricky SL, Ingram RE, Siegle GJ, & Short M (2015). Parental depression risk and reduced physiological responses during a valence identification task. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39(3), 318–331. doi: 10.1007/s10608-014-9660-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Booij L, & Van, d. D. (2007). Cognitive and serotonergic vulnerability to depression: Convergent findings. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 86–94. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bylsma LM, Morris BH, & Rottenberg J (2008). A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(4), 676–691. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bylsma LM, Taylor-Clift A, & Rottenberg J (2011). Emotional reactivity to daily events in major and minor depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(1), 155–167. doi: 10.1037/a0021662 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Clark DA, Beck AT, & Alford BA (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy of depression. New York: John Wiley and Sons Ltd. [Google Scholar]
  11. Cole DA, Martin JM, Peeke LA, Seroczynski AD, & Fier J (1999). Children’s over- and underestimation of academic competence: A longitudinal study of gender differences, depression, and anxiety. Child Development, 70(2), 459–473. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cole DA, Martin JM, Peeke LG, Seroczynski AD, & Hoffman K (1998). Are cognitive errors of underestimation predictive or reflective of depressive symptoms in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(3), 481–496. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.107.3.481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cole DA, Dukewich TL, Roeder K, Sinclair KR, McMillan J, Will E, Bilsky S, Martin N, & Felton JW (2014a). Linking peer victimization to the development of depressive self-schemas in children and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(1), 149–160. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9769-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cole DA, Martin NC, Sterba SK, Sinclair-McBride K, Roeder KM, Zelkowitz R, & Bilsky SA (2014b). Peer victimization (and harsh parenting) as developmental correlates of cognitive reactivity, a diathesis for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(2), 336–349. doi: 10.1037/a0036489 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cole DA, Maxwell MA, Dukewich TL, & Yosick R (2010). Targeted peer victimization and the construction of positive and negative self-cognitions: Connections to depressive symptoms in children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39(3), 421–435. doi: 10.1080/15374411003691776 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cole DA, Sinclair-McBride K, Zelkowitz R, Bilsk SA, Roeder K, & Spinelli T (2016). Peer victimization and harsh parenting predict cognitive diatheses for depression in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 45(5), 668–680. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1004679 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dalgleish T, Taghavi R, Neshat-Doost H, Moradi A, Canterbury R, & Yule W (2003). Patterns of processing bias for emotional information across clinical disorders: A comparison of attention, memory, and prospective cognition in children and adolescents with depression, generalized anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32(1), 10–21. doi: 10.1207/15374420360533022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Davidson RJ (1998). Affective style and affective disorders: Perspectives from affective neuroscience. Cognition and Emotion, 12(3), 307–330. doi: 10.1080/026999398379628 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Davidson RJ (2003). Affective neuroscience and psychophysiology: Toward a synthesis. Psychophysiology, 40(5), 655–665. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Davidson RJ, Lewis DA, Alloy LB, Amaral DG, Bush G, Cohen JD, et al. (2002). Neural and behavioral substrates of mood and mood regulation. Biological Psychiatry, 52(6), 478–502, 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01458-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dobson K, & Franche R (1989). A conceptual and empirical review of the depressive realism hypothesis. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science / Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement, 21(4), 419–433. doi: 10.1037/h0079839 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Everaert J, Koster EHW, & Derakshan N (2012). The combined cognitive bias hypothesis in depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(5), 413–424. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Feder A, Nestler EJ, & Charney DS (2009). Psychobiology and molecular genetics of resilience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 446–457. doi: 10.1038/nrn2649 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gotlib IH, Kasch KL, Traill S, Joormann J, Arnow BA, & Johnson SL (2004). Coherence and specificity of information-processing biases in depression and social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113(3), 386–398, doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.113.3.386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gratz KL (2003). Risk factors for and functions of deliberate self-harm: An empirical and conceptual review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 192–205. doi: 10.1093/clipsy/bpg022 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hankin BL, & Abramson LY (2002). Measuring cognitive vulnerability to depression in adolescence: Reliability, validity and gender differences. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31(4), 491–504. doi: 10.1207/153744202320802160 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ingram RE (1984). Toward an information-processing analysis of depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8(5), 443–477, 10.1007/BF01173284 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ingram RE (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: Review and a conceptual model. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 156–176. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Ingram RE, & Ritter J (2000). Vulnerability to depression: Cognitive reactivity and parental bonding in high-risk individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(4), 588–596. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.4.588 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ingram RE, Miranda J, & Segal ZV (1998). Cognitive vulnerability to depression. New York, Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  31. Ingram RE, & Smith TW (1984). Depression and internal versus external focus of attention. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8(2), 139–151. doi: 10.1007/BF01173040 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ingram RE, Cruet D, Johnson BR, & Wisnicki KS (1988). Self-focused attention, gender, gender role, and vulnerability to negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(6), 967–978. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.55.6.967 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Joormann J (2010). Cognitive inhibition and emotion regulation in depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 161–166. 10.1177/0963721410370293. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Joormann J, & Gotlib IH (2010). Emotion regulation in depression: Relation to cognitive inhibition. Cognition & Emotion, 24(2), 281–298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Joormann J, Talbot L, & Gotlib IH (2007). Biased processing of emotional information in girls at risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 135–143. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Joormann J, Yoon KL, & Zetsche U (2007). Cognitive inhibition in depression. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 12(3), 128–139. [Google Scholar]
  37. Kessler RC, McGonagle K, Swartz M, Blazer D, & Nelson C (1993). Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey: I. Lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrence. Journal of Affective Disorders, 29, 85–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kucharska J (2017). Sex differences in the appraisal of traumatic events and psychopathology. Psychological Trauma: Research, Practice, and Policy, 9(5), 575–582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lazarus RS (1966).Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
  40. Lu W, Daleiden E, & Lu S (2007). Threat perception bias and anxiety among Chinese school children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36(4), 568–580. doi: 10.1080/15374410701776301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Luby JL, & Belden AC (2006). Mood disorders: Phenomenology and a developmental emotion reactivity model In Luby JL (Ed.), Handbook of preschool mental health: Development, disorders, and treatment; handbook of preschool mental health: Development, disorders, and treatment (pp. 209–230, Chapter xviii, 430 Pages) Guilford Press, New York, NY. [Google Scholar]
  42. Malcarne VL, Hamilton NA, Ingram RE, & Taylor L (2000). Correlates of distress in children at risk for affective disorder: Exploring predictors in the offspring of depressed and nondepressed mothers. Journal of Affective Disorders, 59(3), 243–251. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(99)00155-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Martin JM, & Cole DA (2000). Using the personal stroop to detect children’s awareness of social rejection by peers. Cognition and Emotion, 14(2), 241–260. doi: 10.1080/026999300378950 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mogg K, Bradley BP, Dixon C, Fisher S, Twelftree H, & McWilliams A (2000). Trait anxiety, defensiveness and selective processing of threat: An investigation using two measures of attentional bias. Personality and Individual Differences, 28(6), 1063–1077. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00157-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Montagner R, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Pine DS, Czykiel MS, Miguel EC, … Salum GA (2016). Attentional bias to threat in children at-risk for emotional disorders: Role of gender and type of maternal emotional disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 25(7), 735–742. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0792-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Nock MK, Wedig MM, Holmberg EB, & Hooley JM (2008). The emotion reactivity scale: Development, evaluation, and relation to self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. Behavior Therapy, 39(2), 107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2007.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Nolen-Hoeksema S (2012). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: The role of gender. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 61–87. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Pfeifer JH, & Blakemore S (2012). Adolescent social cognitive and affective neuroscience: Past, present, and future. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 1–10. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Platt B, Waters AM, Schulte-Koerne G, Engelmann L, & Salemink E (2017). A review of cognitive biases in youth depression: Attention, interpretation and memory. Cognition and Emotion, 31(3), 462–483. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1127215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Rasa OA, & Hopp M (1989). Age and sex-related differences in threat perception in a modern urban society. Aggressive Behavior, 15(5), 389–398. [Google Scholar]
  51. Rausch SM, Auerbach SM, & Gramling SE (2008). G ender and ethnic differences in stress reduction, reactivity, and recovery. Sex Roles, 59(9–10), 726–737. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-9460-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Rettew DC, & McKee L (2005). Temperament and its role in developmental psychopathology. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 13(1), 14–27. doi: 10.1080/10673220590923146 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Reynolds CR (2002). Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale—Second Edition (RADS-2) professional manual. Los Angeles, CA: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
  54. Reynolds CR, & Mazza JJ (1998). Reliability and validity of the Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale with young adolescents. Journal of School Psychology, 36, 295–312. doi: 10.1016/S0022-4405(98)00010-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Rights JD, & Sterba SK (in press). Quantifying explained variance in multilevel models: An integrative framework for defining R-squared measures. Psychological Methods. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Rosenberg EL (1998). Levels of analysis and the organization of affect. Review of General Psycholology, 2, 247–270, [Google Scholar]
  57. Rothbart MK, & Jones LB (1998). Temperament, self-regulation and education. School Psychology Review, 27(4), 479–491. [Google Scholar]
  58. Rottenberg J (2017). Emotions in depression: What do we really know? Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 13, 241–263. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Salk RH, Hyde JS, & Abramson LY (2017). Gender differences in representative national samples: Meta-analysis of diagnoses and symptoms. Psychological Bulletin, 143(8), 783–822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sass SM, Heller W, Stewart JL, Silton RL, Edgar JC, Fisher JE, & Miller GA (2010). Time course of attentional bias in anxiety: Emotion and gender specificity. Psychophysiology, 47(2), 247–259. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00926.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Scher CD, Ingram RE, & Segal ZV (2005). Cognitive reactivity and vulnerability: Empirical evaluation of construct activation and cognitive diatheses in unipolar depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(4), 487–510. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Segal ZV (1988). Appraisal of the self-schema construct in cognitive models of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 147–162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Selig JP, & Preacher KJ (2008, June). Monte Carlo method for assessing mediation: An interactive tool for creating confidence intervals for indirect effects [Computer software; ]. [Google Scholar]
  64. Sinclair KR, Cole DA, Dukewich T, Felton J, Weitlauf AS, Maxwell MA, … Jacky A (2012). Impact of physical and relational peer victimization on depressive cognitions in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41(5), 570–583. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.704841 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Stroud LR, Salovey P, & Epel ES (2002). Sex differences in stress responses: Social rejection versus achievement stress. Biological Psychiatry, 52(4), 318–327. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01333-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Tamres LK, Janicki D, Helgeson VS. 2002. Sex differences in coping behavior: a meta-analytic review and an examination of relative coping. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6:2–30 [Google Scholar]
  67. Taylor L, & Ingram RE (1999). Cognitive reactivity and depressotypic information processing in children of depressed mothers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(2), 202–210. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.108.2.202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Taylor SE (2012). Tend and befriend theory In Van Lange PAM, Kruglanski AW & Higgins ET (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (vol. 1); handbook of theories of social psychology (vol. 1) (pp. 32–49, Chapter xx, 562 Pages) Sage Publications Ltd, Thousand Oaks, CA. doi: 10.4135/9781446249215.n3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  69. Taylor SE, Klein LC, Lewis BP, Gruenewald TL, Gurung RAR, & Updegraff JA (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review, 107(3), 411–429. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.411 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Tracy JL, Klonsky ED, & Proudfit GH (2014). How affective science can inform clinical science: An introduction to the special series on emotions and psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(4), 371–386. doi: 10.1177/2167702614537627 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. Tran CV, Cole DA, & Weiss B (2012). Testing reciprocal longitudinal relations between peer victimization and depressive symptoms in young adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41(3), 353–360. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.662674 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Solis E, Antypa N, Conijn JM, Kelderman H, & Van d. D. (2017). Psychometric properties of the Leiden index of depression sensitivity (LEIDS).Psychological Assessment, 29(2), 158–171. doi: 10.1037/pas0000326 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Williams JMG, Van d. D., Barnhofer T, Crane C, & Segal ZS (2008). Cognitive reactivity, suicidal ideation and future fluency: Preliminary investigation of a differential activation theory of hopelessness/suicidality. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(1), 83–104. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9105-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. Williams JMG, Watts FN, MacLeod C, & Mathews A (1988). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. New York: John Willey and Sons Ltd. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES