Summary
Mosquitoes rely on the integration of multiple sensory cues, including olfactory, visual, and thermal stimuli, to detect, identify and locate their hosts [1–4]. Although we increasingly know more about the role of chemosensory behaviours in mediating mosquito-host interactions [1], the role of visual cues is comparatively less studied [3], and how the combination of olfactory and visual information is integrated in the mosquito brain remains unknown. In the present study, we used a tethered-flight LED arena, which allowed for quantitative control over the stimuli, and a control theoretic model, to show that CO2 modulates mosquito steering responses towards vertical bars. To gain insight into the neural basis of this olfactory and visual coupling, we conducted two-photon microscopy experiments in a new GCaMP6s-expressing mosquito line. Imaging revealed that neuropil regions within the lobula exhibited strong responses to objects such as a bar, but showed little response to a large-field motion. Approximately 20% of the lobula neuropil we imaged were modulated when CO2 preceded the presentation of a moving bar. By contrast, responses in the antennal (olfactory) lobe were not modulated by visual stimuli presented before or after an olfactory stimulus. Together, our results suggest that asymmetric coupling between these sensory systems provides enhanced steering responses to discrete objects.
Keywords: vision, olfaction, sensory integration, visual arena, mosquitoes, Aedes aegypti
Graphical Abstract
eTOC:
When mosquitoes encounter CO2, they become attracted to dark, visual objects. Using tethered mosquitoes and calcium imaging experiments, Vinauger, Van Breugel et al. find that CO2 modulates lobula neuropil responses to discrete visual stimuli. This modulation may facilitate host-tracking by navigating mosquitoes.
Results and Discussion
Many animals integrate different sensory modalities to make robust behavioural decisions. For example, in bees and humans, prior exposure to a visual stimulus can modify olfactory responses [5–7], and vice versa [8]. To detect and locate suitable hosts, mosquitoes rely on multiple sensory cues, including olfactory, visual, and thermosensory information [1–4, 9] while flying through a dynamic environment [10]. Whereas mosquitoes’ responses to olfactory [11–13] and thermal stimuli [14,15] have been well studied, comparatively less is known about their visually mediated behaviours (but see [16,17]). A recent study with freely flying mosquitoes showed that CO2 detection activates a strong attraction to visual features that is critical for mediating interaction with close-range cues such as heat and other host volatiles [3]. How the visual and olfactory signals are integrated in the brain, however, remains an open question.
Here, to study the integration of multimodal host signals in Ae. aegypti, we placed tethered, mated females within a cylindrical LED arena [18] that permitted simultaneous presentation of olfactory stimuli with the motion of high-contrast visual objects in a controlled manner (Figure 1A). We monitored the mosquitoes’ responses to visual and olfactory cues by tracking changes in wingbeat frequency and stroke amplitude of their left and right wings; proxies for acceleration and turning behaviour. After establishing behavioural evidence of visual-olfactory integration in this tethered preparation, we used 2-photon calcium imaging to investigate the neural basis for this integration.
Tethered mosquitoes increase their wingbeat frequency and amplitude in response to CO2
Our first step was to characterise tethered mosquitoes’ behavioural responses to host cues. We placed the animals in a static visual environment composed of multiple dark vertical bars (22.5° wide × 54° tall, spaced by 22.5°) and presented them with pulses of CO2, a key attractant for host localization [19]. To characterize the behavioural response to CO2, we measured changes in wingbeat frequency (WBF), and the sum and difference in wingbeat amplitude (L+R WBA and L-R WBA) (Figure 1B). In free flight, insects, including mosquitoes, exhibit an upwind surge behaviour when they encounter an attractive odor [20–24]; in the tethered preparation this surge is manifested by increases in WBF and L+R WBA. Increases in the absolute value of L-R WBA, on the other hand, are correlated with turning manoeuvres. In our preparation, we found increases of WBF and L+R WBA, and to a lesser extent, L-R WBA. The concentration of CO2 emitted by humans is ~4.5% [25], and we observed the strongest kinematic changes in the tethered mosquitoes at concentrations of 5–10% (Figure S1). We also tested pulse durations from 0.5 to 20 seconds, and found 1 sec pulses to elicit robust transient changes in kinematics, with longer pulses resulting in more sustained changes (Table S1).
Carbon dioxide modulates responses to object motion, but not translational motion.
Given the robust responses obtained with 1 sec pulses at 5% CO2, we chose this concentration and pulse duration to investigate the effect of CO2 on the responses to visual stimuli. Previous studies by Kennedy and others have examined the visual responses of mosquitoes in both free and tethered flight preparations and established the attraction of mosquitoes to dark objects [17, 26–28]. However, the responses of tethered mosquitoes to other types of visual stimuli have not been systematically studied.
To characterize the effect of CO2 on tethered mosquitoes’ visual responses, we first used several translational visual stimuli (Figure S1E), including expanding and regressing objects and patterns of optic flow [29], presented under open-loop conditions. In both the presence and absence of CO2, these open-loop visual stimuli elicited two general types of responses in WBF. Frontally expanding patterns such as looming objects or vertical bars and starfields creating progressive optic flow all elicited increases in wingbeat frequency, whereas contracting patterns such as regressive motion and shrinking objects elicited small decreases. All of these visual patterns, regardless of whether they were expanding or contracting, elicited small increases in wingbeat amplitude (L+R WBA). None of these responses were significantly altered in the presence of CO2 (Figure S1E).
We next examined responses to horizontally drifting patterns (e.g., bar and starfield, Figure 1C; square, Figures S1F and S2A). For all horizontally drifting visual patterns, the mosquitoes followed the motion direction in both the presence and absence of CO2 (Figure 1C, S1F). This contrasts the behavior of D. melanogaster, which turn towards bars taller than ~25° and turn away from smaller objects (8–25° in height) in the absence of odour [30]. D. mojaviensis, however, exhibit behavior similar to the mosquitoes [31].
CO2 had little effect on the mosquito’s response to the wide field starfield stimulus. However, CO2 did elicit subtle differences in all three kinematic measures in response to the bar and square. A decrease in WBF caused by CO2 occurred after the end of the odor and visual presentation, making it difficult to interpret its behavioural significance. The wingbeat amplitude sum and difference, however, show CO2 modulated differences throughout the stimulus. In the next section we use a model to analyse the differences in the wingbeat amplitudes in more detail.
Carbon dioxide increases object tracking fidelity in tethered mosquitoes
In both the presence and absence of CO2, mosquitoes turned in the direction of horizontally moving bars and squares (Figure S1F). However, the dynamics of their turning behaviour are modulated by the odour (green arrow, Figure 1C). To quantify these changes in dynamics, we modelled their behaviour using the approach originated by Reichardt and Poggio [32]. This method makes it possible to characterize the closed-loop behaviour using data collected with more controlled open-loop experiments in which the stimulus spans a range of object positions and speeds. In particular, the approach allows us to combine the L-R WBA time series data (Figure 1C) from experiments done with clockwise and counter-clockwise moving objects (Figure 1D) to calculate functions that describe the velocity- and position-dependent steering responses of the insect (Figure 1E and 1F, respectively). Details of the analysis can be found in the supplementary methods.
The key insight from this analysis, is that for the bar, CO2 increases the magnitude of the mosquitoes’ velocity dependent steering responses (Figure 1E), whereas the position dependent response is only slightly modified (see Figure S2F–G for statistics). This is equivalent to increasing the damping of their response to target motion, which stabilizes their tracking behaviour. To illustrate what this means for closed-loop behavior, we ran a simulation using the differential equations from the Reichardt and Poggio model together with our data (Figure 1G). This simulation shows that CO2 reduces the overshoot and settling time of the bar tracking response, producing better tracking fidelity. We also found that CO2-induced changes in the dynamics of the mosquitoes’ tracking behaviour of a small square target (Figure S2A–F). The changes in their responses were more variable, and the effect sizes were smaller, making it difficult to interpret the effects of CO2 with confidence. Thus, for the rest of this paper we will focus on the mosquitoes’ responses to moving bars.
It is important to emphasize the challenges of relating the temporal dynamics of tethered animals to freely flying animals, because when on the tether there is no proprioceptive feedback, and we are only able to measure a few of the kinematic variables that are involved in flight manoeuvres. Thus, our conclusion from these experiments is simply that CO2 changes the dynamics with which mosquitoes track discrete objects, such as a bar, and these changes are generally consistent with their free flight behaviour. These results, however, allow us to begin searching for the neural basis of this modulation.
Odour selectively modulates optic lobe responses
Given how odour stimuli modified steering responses to moving visual objects, we took the first steps towards localizing where in the brain this integration takes place by monitoring neural activity in the antennal lobe, a primary olfactory processing centre, and the lobula, a 3rd order neuropil in the mosquito optic lobe. We imaged calcium levels in groups of neurons with two-photon excitation microscopy (Figures 2A; 3A,H) in an Ae. aegypti line (PUb-GCaMP6s) that expresses a genetically encoded Ca2+ indicator under the control of a ubiquitin promotor. Although this line does not permit cell-type specific targeting, PUb-GCaMP6s is expressed strongly in axons and neuropil in the central nervous system and shows clear stimulus-evoked responses in the visual and olfactory brain regions (Figures 2 and 3) [33]. After image alignment and filtering (Figure 2A–B), we manually selected regions of interest (ROIs) that exhibited strong GCaMP6s expression. The ROIs we chose appeared to represent small bundles of axon terminals or dendrites, and we limited their size (40–100 μm2) to prevent recording from large numbers of cells (see methods for details). After experiments, optical sectioning allowed 3D reconstructions of the imaged neuropil (Figures 2C; 3A,H).
We focused on the lobula, because this area contains object-selective neurons in other dipteran species [34–37]. As a first step to characterizing responses within the lobula, we presented mosquitoes with a single moving bar, square (15°), or star-field pattern. We imaged 59 ROIs across 6 individual mosquitoes, and each stimulus type was presented 9 times. For each ROI, we compared the mean fluorescence during the first two seconds of visual motion to the mean fluorescence two seconds prior to the stimulus onset, and compared these two datasets to a null distribution of 10,000 bootstrapped pairwise differences drawn from the combined datasets. Results from these analyses revealed that the moving bar evoked strong and significant responses in approximately 67% of the ROIs (Figure 2D–E). Consistent with our behavioural experiments, moving squares and wide-field motion of a star-field elicited less robust responses (28% and 13% of ROIs, respectively) (Figures 2F–G; S3A–B).
To examine how odour modulates the visually-evoked responses in these ROIs (Figure 3A), we presented the mosquito with a moving bar, with and without CO2 pulses, prior to the onset of the visual stimulus (Figure 3A–B). Similar to the above analysis, for each ROI we assessed the difference between the odour and no-odour responses by comparing the data to a bootstrapped distribution of the combined odour and no-odour datasets (Figure 3B–C). In 14 of the ROIs (23%, including data from 5 out of 6 individuals (Figure S3C)), the visually evoked responses were significantly larger (p<0.05) when preceded by CO2 (Figure 3D). On average, the relative increase in fluorescence for these modulated ROIs was 0.037, a 47% increase (Figure 3E–G). In 2 of the ROIs (3%), the responses were significantly smaller (p<0.05) (Figure 3E–G). Note that at a cut-off of p=0.05, we would only expect 3 ROIs to exhibit a significant difference by chance, suggesting that most of the 16 ROIs that exhibited different visual responses are indeed being modulated by the preceding odour.
When a 2-second pulse of odour was presented without an accompanying visual stimulus, we found that the 14 positively modulated ROIs responded with an increase in fluorescence of 0.015 (Figure S3D). This increase in fluorescence could explain 40% (0.015/0.037) of the odour-induced modulation of the visual response. ROIs that did not exhibit modulation of visual responses showed no response to the odour when presented alone, and ROIs that exhibited negative modulation also exhibited reduced fluorescence in response to the odour pulse (Figure S3D).
In conclusion, 67% of the 59 lobula ROIs we imaged exhibited significant responses to a moving bar, without any odour, and 23% of the ROIs exhibited a significant positive modulation of this visual response when the visual motion was preceded by a CO2 pulse. These modulated ROIs also responded to a CO2 pulse without any visual stimulus, but the magnitude of this olfactory response alone was less than half of the magnitude of the odour-induced change of the visual response. This suggests that the phenomenon is not simply a superposition of olfactory and visual responses, but rather a super-linear, modulatory effect. Approximately 30% of the modulated ROIs only responded to the visual stimulus when it was preceded by a CO2 pulse.
Visual stimuli do not modulate responses in the mosquito antennal lobe.
If odour can modulate the visual responses in the lobula, might visual stimuli have a similar effect on olfactory responses in the antennal lobe, perhaps via visual feedback from the mushroom bodies [38]? We conducted calcium imaging experiments on the olfactory glomeruli in eight animals using stimuli similar to those described in the previous section, in which an individual mosquito was presented with visual stimuli (moving bar) with and without pulses of CO2 and nonanal, another host-emitted odorant [39]. Prior to odour stimulation, glomerular boundaries were discernible based on the baseline GCaMP6s expression, although significant calcium transients were not apparent. However, upon odour stimulation, the ventral AL glomeruli (AL3 and LC2), which are responsive to nonanal and other host odours, showed strong calcium responses that were time-locked to the odour stimulus (Kruskal-Wallis test: χ2 >43.7, p<0.0001; multiple comparisons relative to control: p<0.001) (Figure 3H,I). When stimulated, dendritic arbours filling the AL3 and LC2 glomeruli and axons projecting into the coarse neuropil became observable (Figure 3H). When visual stimuli were presented with the odour – either 1 sec before, or 1 sec after – we observed no difference in the response compared to when the odour was delivered alone (p>0.99) (Figure 3I–M). Moreover, responses to isolated visual stimuli were not significantly different from the mineral oil (no odour) control (p>0.97) (Figure 3L,M).
Conclusions
Free flight behavioural experiments with mosquitoes have shown that they integrate olfactory, visual, and thermal cues to function efficiently and robustly in complex environments [3]. In this current study, we took advantage of recent advances in genetic tools to probe where in the brain that integration occurs. These calcium imaging methods, however, require that the animals be rigidly tethered to a head stage, which significantly alters sensory feedback. Furthermore, in these tethered preparations we are only able to measure one of many parameters describing free flight wing kinematics. This effects not only the behavioural readout, but also our ability to provide realistic closed-loop virtual visual experiences. As a result of these limitations, it is difficult to directly compare the behavioural responses of tethered and freely flying animals. Despite these limitations, we were able to see significant effects of CO2 on the behaviour that are generally consistent with free flight behaviour, which allowed us to ask the question, “where in the brain are olfactory and visual signals integrated in the mosquito?”
Like mammals, insects exhibit symmetric sensory integration and modulation in higher order brain areas, such as the mushroom bodies [40–42] and central complex [43]. However, in contrast to mammals, insects also exhibit sensory integration and modulation in early sensory areas, such as in the antennal lobe [44] and optic lobe [45–46]. Nonetheless, the degree to which sensory integration occurs in more peripheral processing areas for insects remains an open question. Our experiments suggest that in Ae. aegypti sensory modulation is asymmetric: odour modulates vision, but not vice versa. Making a direct comparison between the two systems is difficult, as there are more regions and connections in the visual system compared to the olfactory system [47–50]. Prior studies, however, suggest that an output region of the lobula, referred to as the optic glomeruli, shares many anatomical similarities with the antennal lobe [49], making these brain regions ideal for functional comparisons. Although we did not image from the optic glomeruli, the fact that we saw olfactory modulation in the lobula, one processing stage before the optic glomeruli, but no visual modulation in the antennal lobe, suggests that the modulation is indeed asymmetric in these brain loci.
Why might sensory modulation in the mosquito be asymmetric? Insects such as mosquitoes have relatively poor visual resolution (5°, compared to humans’ 0.02°). Thus, for a mosquito, vision is unlikely to provide information about what something is. Instead, the odour may provide information for what the animal is smelling, while vision provides information for where the odour is located. These differences might explain the asymmetric sensory modulation we observed. Comparative studies across species with varying degrees of resolution in sensory modalities will be needed to address this hypothesis. Finally, there is a growing understanding of the molecular and neurophysiological bases of olfactory behaviours in mosquitoes [11,13], but we know comparatively little about visual behaviours despite their importance for locating hosts and selection of biting sites [3, 51, 52]. Our results here provide motivation for addressing this research gap, as well as identifying the mechanisms by which olfactory input modulates other sensory systems. Fortunately, thanks to the recent development of new genetic tools, these types of experiments are now possible.
STAR★Methods
LEAD CONTACT AND MATERIALS AVAILABILITY
Further information and requests for materials, resources and reagents, including mosquito lines, should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Jeff Riffell (jriffell@uw.edu).
EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS
Wild type Aedes aegypti mosquitoes (line Rockefeller F25, MR4–735) were used for the tethered flight experiments. The colony was maintained in a climatic chamber at 25±1°C, 60±10% relative humidity (RH) and under a 12–12h light-dark cycle. An artificial feeder (D.E. Lillie Glassblowers, Atlanta, Georgia; 2.5 cm internal diameter) supplied with heparinized bovine blood (Lampire Biological Laboratories, Pipersville, PA, USA) placed on the top of the cage and heated at 37°C using a water-bath circulation, allowed us to feed mosquitoes on weekdays. Cotton balls soaked with 10% sucrose were continuously provided to the mosquitoes. Groups of 200 larvae were placed in 26×35×4cm covered pans containing tap water and were fed on fish food (Hikari Tropic 382 First Bites - Petco, San Diego, CA, USA). Groups of 120 pupae were then isolated in 16 Oz containers (Mosquito Breeder Jar, Bioquip Products, Rancho Dominguez, CA, USA) until emergence. Adults were then transferred into mating cages (BioQuip Products, Rancho Dominguez, CA, USA) and maintained on 10% sucrose.
Mosquitoes used in the calcium imaging experiments were from of the Ae. aegypti Liverpool strain, which was the source strain for the reference genome sequence. Briefly, this mosquito line was generated by injecting a construct that included the GCaMP6s plasmid (ID# 106868) cloned into the piggyBac plasmid pBac-3xP3-dsRed and using Ae. aegypti polyubiquitin (PUb) promoter fragment. Mosquito pre-blastoderm stage embryos were injected with a mixture of the GCaMP6s plasmid described above (200ng/ul) and a source of piggyBac transposase (phsp-Pbac, (200ng/ul)). Injected embryos were hatched in deoxygenated water and surviving adults were placed into cages and screened for expected fluorescent markers. Mosquitoes were backcrossed for five generations to our wild-type stock, and subsequently screened and selected for at least 20 generations to obtain a near homozygous line. The location and orientation of the insertion site was confirmed by PCR (see [33] for details).
To characterize the expression of GCaMP in different cell types in the AL, we double-stained for GFP (for the GCaMP6s; Abcam, Cambridge, MA, USA – Cat. no. ab6556; 1:1000 concentration) and glutamine synthase (GS; a glial marker; Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA - Cat. no. MAB302; 1:500 concentration). Double-labelling of GFP (for GCaMP6s) and glutamine synthase (for glia) revealed that ubiquitous expression of GCaMP occurred in glia, local interneurons, and projection neurons. However, glia-like processes occurred on the exterior ‘rind’ of AL glomeruli and was restricted compared to the GFP labelling, thus enabling us to record from the central interior regions of the glomerular neuropil (Figure S3F). Similarly, GFP was strongly expressed in the optic lobe lamina and other loci, with restricted GS-labeling and little overlap (Figure S3F). In both brain regions, the GCaMP6s expression was very high in lobula cell types and AL projection neurons (PNs), such that during stimulation the cells could be imaged and tentatively reconstructed via optical sectioning.
For all the experiments, 6–8 day old female mosquitoes were used. For behavioural experiments, this gave mosquitoes the time to mate in the containers before the tethered flight experiments (random dissection of females revealed that 95% of them had oocytes); all experiments in the flight arena occurred during the last three hours of the mosquitoes’ subjective day [52–55]. Female mosquitoes used in calcium imaging experiments were unmated and kept in isolation allowing fine-scale control of their age, reproductive status, physiological state, and sugar feeding. Previous studies have shown no differences between mated and unmated females in their host-seeking responses to odour cues [55]; as a first step we wanted to ensure that any neural modulation was due to the stimuli presented to the animals. Sugar feeding (10% sucrose) up to 16 h before experiments increased the calcium fluorescence and duration of the experiments.
METHOD DETAILS
Tethered Flight Visual Arena
Tethered flight responses by mosquitoes to olfactory and visual stimuli were tested in an LED-based arena (sensu [18]; Fig 1A). The arena consists of an array of 96×16 LEDs, each subtending 3.75° on the eye, subtending 360° horizontally and 54° vertically. Mosquitoes were cold anesthetized on ice and tethered to a tungsten wire using UV-activated glue (Loctite 3104 Light Cure Adhesive, Loctite, Düsseldorf, Germany) applied on the thorax. The main body axis was positioned at a 30° angle from the tether. Mosquitoes were then stored at room temperature in a closed container for an approximate 30 minute recovery period. Tethered mosquitoes were centred in a hovering position within the arena (Figure 1A; [18]).
Mosquitoes were placed directly under an infrared (IR) diode and situated above an optical sensor coupled to a wingbeat analyser (JFI Electronics, University of Chicago; [18,56]). The beating wings cast a shadow onto the sensor, allowing the analyser to track the motion of both wings and measure the amplitude and frequency of each wingbeat. Measurements were sampled at 5 kHz and acquired with a National Instrument Acquisition board (BNC −2090A, National Instruments, Austin, Texas, USA).
Odour delivery
The mosquito was centred between an air inlet and a vacuum line aligned diagonally with one another, 30° from the vertical axis (Fig 1A). The air inlet was positioned 12 mm in front of and slightly above the mosquito’s head, targeting the antennae from an angle of 15°. The vacuum line was positioned behind the mosquito 25 mm away from the tip of the abdomen. Two different airlines independently controlled by a solenoid valve (The Lee Company, Essex, CT, USA, LHDA0533115H) intersected this main air inlet, one delivering nitrogen and, the other, CO2. Mass flow controllers for both the CO2 and nitrogen delivery allowed for the CO2 to be set at different concentrations (0, 1, 2.5, 5 and 10%) and pulse durations. Nonanal was diluted at 1:100 in mineral oil and 2 μL was pipetted on to a filter paper (2M Whatman) in a Pasteur pipette.
Mosquitoes responses to different CO2 concentrations and pulse durations
For these experiments, a visual pattern of alternating vertical bars comprised of either inactive or fully-lit LEDs, each 16×6 pixels in size (i.e. 22.5° wide, 54° tall) was used. The pattern was briefly placed in closed-loop at the beginning of the experiment in order to encourage the mosquitoes to fly and then held motionless during the presentation of CO2. Closed-loop control of the pattern position was achieved using the difference between the left and right amplitude signals. Concentrations of 5% and 10% CO2 were initially tested, delivered for durations of 20, 10, 5, 1, and 0.5 seconds. One second pulses of CO2 at 2.5% and 1% were also tested. Potential mechanical stimulation associated with the onset of the pulses was controlled for by delivering N2 pulses for all the tested durations. Because a 1 sec pulse of 5% CO2 was sufficient to produce a reliable, robust frequency response, this was the concentration and pulse duration used throughout the remainder of this study.
Moving visual patterns
To test the response to looming and drifting objects, large-field patterns of optic flow, and rotating field patterns, we adapted a broad panel of visual stimuli that are known to be important for guidance and stability during flight in other insects [29]: looming and fading squares, progressive and regressive bars, and starfield patterns (75% of pixels ON), yaw, a 22.5° wide square-like object (6×6 pixels, 20.25° tall) or a 22.5° wide and 54° tall bar moving either from left to right (Clockwise; CW) or from right to left (Counter-clockwise, CCW) (Figures 1 and S2). The stimuli were each presented for two seconds and were separated by a 4 sec period during which all LEDs in the arena were lit. The angular velocity of objects moving on the display was 150°/sec. The entire experiment consisted of five trials of twelve visual stimuli presented twice (either immediately following a 1 sec pulse of CO2, or alone), the order of which was randomized at the beginning of each trial, using Matlab’s random number generator.
Dynamics model
To quantify the changes in visuomotor turning dynamics elicited by CO2, we modelled their behaviour using the approach described by Reichardt and Poggio [32]. Reichardt and Poggio describe the closed-loop behaviour of a tethered insect steering towards an object with the following dynamics:
Eqn. 1 |
Where ѱ(t)is the angular position of the object on the mosquito’s retina, Θ is the mosquito’s moment of inertia, k is the aerodynamic friction, N(t) is mean-zero gaussian noise, S(t) describes the motion of the object relative to stationary objects in the world, and R(ѱ(t)),t) describes the mosquito’s steering response. The steering response, R(ѱ(t)),t), is a nonlinear function of the object’s position and velocity on the retina, which may be approximated by [32]:
Eqn. 2 |
where describes the mosquito’s response to the velocity of the object, and D(ѱ(t)) describes the mosquito’s response to the position of the object.
The advantage of using open-loop data is that they provide information over the entire range of ѱ = [−π,π]. These data can then be used to estimate r(ѱ(t)) and D(ѱ(t)) by comparing the mosquito’s turning responses (L-R WBA) for objects moving in the clockwise (CW) and counter-clockwise (CCW) directions. The velocity component can be calculated from the difference, r(ѱ) = CW(ѱ) − CCW(ѱ) (Figure 1F), because the position components cancel out, whereas the position component can be calculated from the sum D(ѱ) = CW(ѱ) + CCW(ѱ) (Figure 1G), because the velocity components cancel out [37]. The canonical shape for r(ѱ) is a positive even function, such as a horizontal line or cosine curve, and D(ѱ) is typically an odd function, such as a line with a positive slope or sine curve (corresponding to saturation at peripheral angles) [32]. These canonical shapes correspond to steering responses that are simultaneously proportional to the objects position and velocity.
In both the presence and absence of CO2, mosquitoes’ responses are proportional to the object’s position and velocity, corresponding to object tracking. The precise shape of D(ѱ) and the magnitude of r(ѱ), however, changes in the presence of CO2. To characterize these changes, we modelled r(ѱ) as a cosine, and D(ѱ) as a sine curve (Figure S2F–G). The cosine approximation of r(ѱ) is not perfect, however, the changes in magnitude are appropriately reflected in the model. For the square, CO2 had little effect on r(ѱ), whereas it significantly increased the frequency of D(ѱ) (p=0.003), contracting the sinewave, which corresponds to an increase in the slope of the proportional response when ѱ is in front of the animal. For the bar, CO2 significantly increased the magnitude of r(ѱ) (p=0.007), corresponding to an increase in the velocity dependent response, and modestly increased the frequency of D(ѱ) (p=0.06).
How might these changes in open-loop responses relate to free flight behaviour? To gain a better intuition for how the functions r(ѱ) and D(ѱ) shape the mosquito’s behaviour, we simplified the dynamical system to bring it into a standard form. If we consider the mosquito interacting with a static object with some initial condition, we can eliminate N(t) and S(t) in Eqn. 1, since both are equal to zero, leaving us with the following nonlinear second order differential equation:
Eqn. 3 |
We used cosine and sine approximations of r(ѱ) and D(ѱ) in Figure S3 to linearize the system about the stable equilibrium, ѱ = 0, allowing us to approximate r(ѱ) as a constant, and D(ѱ) as a line, resulting in:
Eqn. 4 |
where r0 = r(ѱ) = 0 and ds is the slope of r(ѱ) |ѱ=0. This is a classic second order differential equation, equivalent to a mass-spring-damper system in which the slope of D(ѱ) determines the natural frequency and the magnitude of r0 determines the damping. These parameters can be used to calculate how quickly the system responds to a step input, such as a mosquito seeing an object and steering towards or away from it. Larger values of ds will reduce the response delay and increase the amount of oscillations, and larger values of r0 will reduce the extent of any oscillations, thereby increasing the stability.
The results of our analysis suggest that CO2 modulates flight behaviour such that mosquitoes respond to visual objects with faster and more stable responses. To illustrate this, we numerically integrated Eqn. 3 using the sine and cosine curves from Figure S2 for r(ѱ) and D(ѱ) (Figure 1G) to simulate a mosquito turning towards a fixed object. Because the dynamics for tethered flight are slower than free flight, and because of values for r(ѱ) and D(ѱ) are in relative units (based on the amplifier gains in the wing beat analyser), we chose values for Θ and k to emphasize the relationship between r(ѱ) and the stability. The values we chose were Θ = 1 (relative units) . The ratio represents the time constant of the passive rotational dynamics; values smaller than 1 ensure that the oscillations would be damped even with a small value for r(ѱ). The simulations show that CO2 increases the speed of mosquitoes’ responses to squares, at the expense of stability, and CO2 dampens the oscillations of mosquitoes’ responses to bars, increasing the stability at the expense of speed. Because the magnitude of the velocity response function was larger for bars (Figure 1F), and because this modulation increased the stability of the response, we chose to use bars as our primary visual stimulus for the calcium imaging.
Although it seems as though CO2 has opposite effects on the dynamics for the bar and bar (compare Figures 1G and S2E), there are several explanations for this. First, the mosquitoes’ behaviour in response to the square was more variable, and the changes more subtle, thus the response may not be representative of their free behaviour. Second, it is possible for one dynamical system to have opposite effects with an increase in gain depending on the initial gain. For example, when the system G(S) = (s +10)(s + 0.5 ± 1.5j)/((s + 0.001)(s + 0.5 ± 0.5j)) transitions from low to intermediate gain the stability decreases, but when it transitions from intermediate to high gain, the stability increases. If the mosquito’s responses to bars and squares resulted in high and low initial gains, respectively, and the gain for each response increased multiplicatively due to CO2, it would explain our results. However, we do not have sufficient data to test this hypothesis at present.
Calcium imaging
Image acquisition:
Visual and odor-evoked responses were imaged in the lobula region of the mosquito optic lobe, and the antennal lobe region, taking advantage of our genetically-encoded ubiquitin-GCaMPs mosquito line [33](Figures 2A–C; 3H). Calcium-evoked responses were imaged using the Prairie Ultima IV multiphoton microscope (Prairie Technologies) and Ti-Sapphire laser (Chameleon Ultra; Coherent). The laser power was adjusted to 20mW at the rear aperture of the objective lens (Nikon NIR Apo, 40X water immersion lens, 0.8 NA), and bandpass filtered the GCaMP fluorescence with a HQ 525/50 m-2p emission filter (Chroma Technologies) and collected the photons using a multialkali photomultiplier tube. Images were collected at 2 Hz for each visual and visual+odour stimulus, for a total duration of 350 s (Figure 2), and calcium-evoked responses are calculated as the change in fluorescence and time-stamped and synced with the stimuli. Individual mosquitoes were tethered to a holder, and their cuticle removed to provide access to the antennal lobe or lobula regions of the brain [56]. The mosquitoes were placed at the centre of a semi-cylindrical visual arena (frosted mylar, 20 cm diameter, 20 cm high); a video projector (Acer K132 WXGA DLP LED Projector, 600 Lumens) positioned in front of the arena projected the visual stimuli. To separate the wavelength of the light emitted by the projector from the GCaMP6 fluorescence, we used the projector’s blue channel (peak at 451 nm, 18 lux, 0.02 W/m2) and further reduced the longer wavelength component by covering the projector with three layers of blue gel filter (ROSCOLUX #59 Indigo). Select visual stimuli were the same as those used in the arena experiments: a bar, square (15°) and star-field pattern (comprising 75% of the screen).
Image analysis for Lobula ROIs:
The ubiquitous expression of GCaMP6s made it difficult to distinguish between different cell types in the imaging planes. We thus used a series of criteria and image analyses to select ROIs manually. To ensure that mosquitoes were viable, we used animals that showed both odour-evoked changes glomerular fluorescence in the AL and changes in lobula fluorescence from stimulation with strong puffs of air to the head (via hand-held syringe) and from presentations of visual stimuli. Images were initially examined in ImageJ and imported into Matlab for alignment using a single frame as the reference at a given imaging depth and subsequently registered to every frame to within ¼ pixel, and subsequently Gaussian filtered (2×2 pixel; σ = 1.5–3). For detection of the calcium dynamics, pixels were chosen based on fluorescence changes above the background threshold (1.02 to 15.9-times the baseline fluorescence), and ROIs were manually selected based on pixel intensities and appearances similar to axonal regions; restricted ROI surface areas (40–100 μm2) were used to minimize recording multiple cells. Images were acquired at approximately 40 to 100 μm from the ventral surface (Figures 2 and 3) – neuropil in this region showed strong responses to visual stimuli, and odour-evoked modulation –, and optical sections (1 m) were taken to tentatively reconstruct axonal regions associated with the regions of interest (Amira v.5, Thermo Fisher Scientific). We typically had stable imaging for approximately 1.5 h allowing complete testing of the experimental series.
Image analysis for AL ROIs:
Antennal lobe ROIs were selected mainly based on the criteria listed above, except ROI selection was based on the clear delineation between glomerular boundaries. Glomerular ROIs were imaged at 40 μm from the ventral surface. Glomeruli at this depth show strong responses to either host- or plant-related odorants. For instance, the lateral cluster of glomeruli (AL3, LC2, V1) are especially responsive to host odorants, including nonanal, octanal, and hexanoic acid, and to a lesser extent, CO2 (AL3, a glomerulus that is broadly responsive to stimuli), whereas glomeruli in the anteriomedial cluster respond to plant-related compounds, such as linalool, lilac aldehyde and myrtenol. At this depth, 14–18 glomeruli were neuroanatomically identified and registered between preparations. Calcium-evoked responses are calculated as the change in fluorescence and time-stamped and synced with the stimulus pulses. After an experiment, the AL was serially scanned at 1 μm depths from the ventral to the dorsal surface to provide glomerular registration to our tentative AL atlas (n = 6 female mosquitoes) as well as one that was previously published [57]. We note that glomeruli identified in our imaging experiments did not always conform, regarding glomerular number and position, to the previously published atlas; however, the two atlases provide a first principles approach for identifying and registering glomeruli.
QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Analyses were performed in R. For each stimulus, a baseline wingbeat frequency was determined by averaging the frequency across a 1 sec time window preceding the stimulus delivery (either visual or olfactory, according to the experiment) and then subtracting this value from the max frequency values following the stimulus. Trials were discarded in which the mosquitoes stopped flying, indicated by a drop in wingbeat frequency below 200 Hz. The mean response for each individual was calculated from the saved trials and used as a replicate to calculate the mean response for each treatment group. This latter was calculated using the difference in frequency, turning tendency (L-R WBA), and total amplitude (L+R WBA) before and after the stimulus. One-tailed Student’s t-tests for paired samples were used to test for differences from baseline and t-tests for independent samples were used to test for differences between groups. As stimuli that were presented with two directions of movement (i.e. square, bar and yaw from left to right or from right to left) did not elicit significantly different responses (Student t test; 0.06<t>1.15; 40<df<84; p>0.21 for all comparisons), both directions of rotations were combined for the analysis. ANOVA and Tukey post-hoc tests were employed for multiple comparisons. When specified, multiple pairwise t-tests with Holm corrections were used to compare responses to visual stimuli. Whenever samples did not meet the normality assumption of the t-test, a Wilcoxon test was performed. The delay before return to baseline wingbeat frequency was determined by determining the time at which the frequency signal crossed a threshold set at ½ standard deviation above the baseline mean frequency. The correlation between the turning response of the mosquitoes and the position of moving visual objects (i.e. squares and bars), were quantified using equation 1–3 in the text, and compared statistically using a resampling test.
Calcium imaging data were extracted in Fiji/ImageJ and analysed in Matlab and python. The trigger-averaged F/F were used for comparing responses to visual and odour stimuli. To statistically determine visual-evoked responses, for each ROI we assessed the difference in mean fluorescence between the time period during the visual stimulus presentation and the time period preceding the visual stimulus by comparing the two datasets to a null distribution of 10,000 bootstrapped pairwise differences drawn from the combined pre-visual stimulus and visual stimulus datasets. Similarly, for examining odour-evoked modulation, for each ROI we assessed the difference between the odour and no-odour responses by comparing the difference in the mean fluorescence during the visual stimulus for these two experiments to a null distribution of 10,000 bootstrapped pairwise differences drawn from the combined odour and no-odour datasets.
Steering responses (e.g. Figures 1, S1 and S2) were compared using a resampling test, written in Python. For these tests we generated an exact null distribution by combining the CO2 and control datasets, and randomly chose two samples (equal in size to the original datasets) from this combined dataset, fit sine and cosine curves to these samples, and compared the frequency and amplitude. We repeated this process 300 times to generate the null distributions shown in Figure S2. Then, we compared the actual difference in frequency and amplitude for the sine and cosine curves corresponding to the CO2 and control trials, and compared these actual differences to the null distribution. This comparison provided a two-tailed p-value indicating how likely the difference in these datasets was due to random sampling error.
DATA AND CODE AVAILABILITY
The behavioral and calcium imaging data generated during this study are available at Mendeley Data (DOI: 10.17632/57pc9mkvft.1). Code is available upon request.
Supplementary Material
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
Antibodies | ||
GFP | Abcam | ab6556 |
Glutamine synthase | Sigma-Aldrich | MAB302 |
Bacterial and Virus Strains | ||
N/A | N/A | N/A |
Biological Samples | ||
Heparinized bovine blood | Lampire Biological Laboratories | Bovine blood |
Chemicals, Peptides, and Recombinant Proteins | ||
All odours for arena and imaging experiments | Sigma-Aldrich | N/A |
Critical Commercial Assays | ||
N/A | N/A | N/A |
Experimental Models: Organisms/Strains | ||
Aedes aegypti Rockefeller strain | BEI | ROCK |
Aedes aegypti GCaMP6s mutant strain | This study | GCaMP6 mutant |
Oligonucleotides | ||
N/A | N/A | N/A |
Recombinant DNA | ||
N/A | N/A | N/A |
Software and Algorithms | ||
Kinefly | Custom | https://github.com/ssafarik/Kinefly |
R | R Development Core Team | N/A |
MATLAB | The MathWorks, Inc., | MATLAB and Statistics Toolbox Release 2012b, The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachusetts, United States |
ImageJ | NIH | https://imagej.nih.gov/ij/ |
Python (numpy, scipy, matplotlib) | Python Software Foundation and others. |
http://www.python.org http://www.scipy.org/ http://www.numpy.org/ https://matplotlib.org/ |
Custom software | This study | J. Riffell (jriffell@uw.edu) |
Other | ||
Wingbeat Analyzer | JFI Electronics / University of Chicago | N/A |
Mosquito Electrophysiology Holder | This study | J. Riffell (jriffell@uw.edu) |
Deposited Data | ||
Calcium imaging and behavioral data | This study | Mendeley Data (DOI: 10.17632/57pc9mkvft.1) |
Highlights.
CO2 modulates mosquito behavioral responses to discrete visual stimuli
CO2 modulates lobula neuropil responses to discrete visual stimuli
Visual stimuli do not modulate responses to CO2 in olfactory glomeruli
Modulation of peripheral visual and olfactory sensory stimuli is asymmetric
Acknowledgements
Comments from three anonymous reviewers greatly improved the manuscript and analyses. We thank B. Nguyen for mosquito colony maintenance, J. Tuthill, A. Mamiya, and P. Weir for comments and help with the arena and imaging experiments, G. Wolff for comments and imaging assistance, and D. Alonso San Alberto for technical support. We acknowledge the support of the Air Force Office of Sponsored Research under grant FA9550-14-1-0398 and FA9550-16-1-0167, National Institutes of Health under grants 1RO1DCO13693 and 1R21AI137947, an Endowed Professorship for Excellence in Biology (J.A.R.), the University of Washington Innovation Award. O.S.A was supported in part by NIH grants 5K22AI113060 and 1R21AI123937.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Declaration of Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
References
- 1.Lehane MJ. (2005). The Biology of Blood-Sucking in Insects (Cambridge University Press; ). [Google Scholar]
- 2.Turner SL, Li N, Guda T, Githure J, Cardé RT, and Ray A (2011). Ultra-prolonged activation of CO2-sensing neurons disorients mosquitoes. Nature 474, 87–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.van Breugel F, Riffell J, Fairhall A, and Dickinson MH (2015). Mosquitoes use vision to associate odor plumes with thermal targets. Curr. Biol 25, 1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.McMeniman CJ, Corfas R. a., Matthews BJ, Ritchie S. a., and Vosshall LB (2014). Multimodal integration of carbon dioxide and other sensory cues drives mosquito attraction to humans. Cell 156, 1060–1071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gottfried JA and Dolan RJ (2003). The nose smells what the eye sees: crossmodal visual facilitation of human olfactory perception. Neuron, 39, 375–386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gerber B and Smith BH (1998). Visual modulation of olfactory learning in honeybees. J. Exp. Biol, 201, 2213–2217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Leonard AS, Dornhaus A, and Papaj DR (2011) Flowers help bees cope with uncertainty: signal detection and the function of floral complexity. J. Exp. Biol 214, 113–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Zhou W, Jiang Y, He S and Chen D (2010). Olfaction modulates visual perception in binocular rivalry. Curr. Biol, 20, 1356–1358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cardé RT (2015). Multi-cue integration: How female mosquitoes locate a human host. Curr. Biol 25, R793–R795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Geier MA, Bosch OJ, Boeckh JÜ (1999). Influence of odour plume structure on upwind flight of mosquitoes towards hosts. J. Exp. Biol 202,1639–1648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Zwiebel LJ, and Takken W (2004). Olfactory regulation of mosquito-host interactions. Insect Biochem. Mol. Biol 34, 645–652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Stanczyk NM, Mescher MC, and De Moraes CM (2017). Effects of malaria infection on mosquito olfaction and behavior: extrapolating data to the field. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 20, 7–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Lutz EK, Lahondère C, Vinauger C, and Riffell JA (2017). Olfactory learning and chemical ecology of olfaction in disease vector mosquitoes: a life history perspective. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 20:75–83 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Corfas RA, and Vosshall LB (2015). The cation channel TRPA1 tunes mosquito thermotaxis to host temperatures. Elife 4, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Zermoglio PF, Robuchon E, Leonardi MS, Chandre F, and Lazzari CR (2017). What does heat tell a mosquito? Characterization of the orientation behaviour of Aedes aegypti towards heat sources. J. Insect Physiol. 100, 9–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Muir LE, Thorne MJ, and Kay BH (1992). Aedes aegypti (Diptera: Culicidae) vision: spectral sensitivity and other perceptual parameters of the female eye. J. Med. Entomol 29, 278–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kennedy JS (1940). The visual responses of flying mosquitoes. Proc. Zool. Soc. London 109 A, 221–242. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Reiser MB, and Dickinson MH (2008). A modular display system for insect behavioral neuroscience. J. Neurosci. Methods 167, 127–139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gillies MT (1980). The role of carbon dioxide in host-finding by mosquitoes (Diptera: Culicidae): a review. Bull. Entomol. Res 70, 525–532. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Frye MA (2004). Motor output reflects the linear superposition of visual and olfactory inputs in Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol 207, 123–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Chow DM, and Frye MA (2008). Context-dependent olfactory enhancement of optomotor flight control in Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol 211, 2478–2485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fox JL, Aptekar JW, Zolotova NM, Shoemaker PA, and Frye MA (2014). Figure-ground discrimination behavior in Drosophila. I. Spatial organization of wing-steering responses. J. Exp. Biol 217, 558–569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Fox JL, and Frye MA (2014). Figure-ground discrimination behavior in Drosophila. II. Visual influences on head movement behavior. J. Exp. Biol 217, 570–579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tammero LF, Frye M. a, and Dickinson MH (2004). Spatial organization of visuomotor reflexes in Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol 207, 113–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dekker T, and Carde RT (2011). Moment-to-moment flight manoeuvres of the female yellow fever mosquito (Aedes aegypti L.) in response to plumes of carbon dioxide and human skin odour. J. Exp. Biol 214, 3480–3494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ramachandra RT (1947). Visual responses of mosquitoes artificially rendered flightless. J. Exp. Biol 24, 64–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bidlingmayer WL, and Hem DG (1980). The range of visual attraction and the effect of competitive visual attractants upon mosquito (Diptera: Culicidae) flight. Bull. Entomol. Res 70, 321–342. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bidlingmayer WL (1994). How mosquitoes see traps: role of visual responses. J. Am. Mosq. Control Assoc. 10, 272–279. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Weir PT, and Dickinson MH (2015). Functional divisions for visual processing in the central brain of flying Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 112, 5523–5532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Maimon G, Straw AD, and Dickinson MH (2008). A simple vision-based algorithm for decision making in flying Drosophila. Curr. Biol 18, 464–470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Park EJ and Wasserman SM (2018). Diversity of visuomotor reflexes in two Drosophila species. Curr. Biol 28, R865–R866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Reichardt W, & Poggio T (1976). Visual control of orientation behaviour in the fly: Part I. A quantitative analysis. Q. Rev. Biophys, 9(3), 311–375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bui M, Lutz EK, Shyong J, Yang T, Li M, Truong K, Arvidson P, Buchman A, Riffell JA, and Akbari O (2018). Live calcium imaging of Aedes aegypti neuronal tissues reveals differential importance of chemosensory systems. bioRxiv, p.345389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Collett T (1971). Visual neurons for tracking moving targets. Nature 232, 127–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Nordström K, and O’Carroll DC (2006). Small object detection neurons in female hoverflies. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci 273, 1211–1216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Nordström K, Bolzon DM, and O’Carroll DC (2011). Spatial facilitation by a high-performance dragonfly target-detecting neuron. Biol. Lett 7, 588–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Keleş MF, and Frye MA (2017). Object-detecting neurons in Drosophila. Curr. Biol 27, 680–687. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hu A, Zhang W, and Wang Z (2010). Functional feedback from mushroom bodies to antennal lobes in the Drosophila olfactory pathway Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 107, 10262–10267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Syed Z, and Leal WS (2009). Acute olfactory response of Culex mosquitoes to a human- and bird-derived attractant. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 18803–18808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Gronenberg W, 2001. Subdivisions of hymenopteran mushroom body calyces by their afferent supply. J. Comp. Neurol, 435, 474–489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Vogt K, Aso Y, Hige T, Knapek S, Ichinose T, Friedrich AB, Turner GC, Rubin GM and Tanimoto H (2016). Direct neural pathways convey distinct visual information to Drosophila mushroom bodies. Elife, 5, 14009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Strube-Bloss MF and Rössler W (2018). Multimodal integration and stimulus categorization in putative mushroom body output neurons of the honeybee. Roy. Soc. Open Sci, 5, 171785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Pfeiffer K and Homberg U (2014). Organization and functional roles of the central complex in the insect brain. Annu. Rev. Entomol 59, 165–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Nishino H, Yamashita S, Yamazaki Y, Nishikawa M, Yokohari F and Mizunami M (2003). Projection neurons originating from thermo-and hygrosensory glomeruli in the antennal lobe of the cockroach. J. Comp. Neurol 455, 40–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Suver MP, Mamiya A and Dickinson MH (2012). Octopamine neurons mediate flight-induced modulation of visual processing in Drosophila. Curr. Biol, 22, 2294–2302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Maimon G, Straw AD and Dickinson MH (2010). Active flight increases the gain of visual motion processing in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci, 13, 393–399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Otsuna H, and Ito K (2006). Systematic analysis of the visual projection neurons of Drosophila melanogaster. I. Lobula-specific pathways. J. Comp. Neurol 497, 928–958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Wu M, Nern A, Williamson WR, Morimoto MM, Reiser MB, Card GM and Rubin GM (2016). Visual projection neurons in the Drosophila lobula link feature detection to distinct behavioral programs. Elife, 5, e21022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Mu L, Ito K, Bacon JP and Strausfeld NJ (2012). Optic glomeruli and their inputs in Drosophila share an organizational ground pattern with the antennal lobes. J. Neurosci 32, 6061–6071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Fischbach KF and Dittrich APM (1989). The optic lobe of Drosophila melanogaster. I. A Golgi analysis of wild-type structure. Cell Tissue Res, 258, 441–475. [Google Scholar]
- 51.De Jong R and Knols BGJ (1995). Selection of biting sites on man by two malaria mosquito species. Experientia, 51, 80–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Takken W, and Verhulst NO (2013). Host preferences of blood-feeding mosquitoes. Annu. Rev. Entomol 58, 433–453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Trpis M, McClelland G. a H., Gillett JD, Teesdale C, and Rao TR (1973). Diel periodicity in the landing of Aedes aegypti on man. Bull. World Health Organ. 48, 623–629. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Vinauger C, Lutz EK, and Riffell JA (2014). Olfactory learning and memory in the disease vector mosquito Aedes aegypti. J. Exp. Biol 217, 2321–2330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Klowden MJ (1995). Blood, sex, and the mosquito. Bioscience, 45, 326–331. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Vinauger C, Lahondère C, Wolff GH, Locke LT, Liaw JE, Parrish JZ, Akbari OS, Dickinson MH, and Riffell JA (2018). Modulation of host learning in Aedes aegypti mosquitoes. Curr. Biol 28,1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Ignell R, Dekker T, Ghaninia M & Hansson BS (2005). Neuronal architecture of the mosquito deutocerebrum. J. Comp. Neurol 493, 207–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The behavioral and calcium imaging data generated during this study are available at Mendeley Data (DOI: 10.17632/57pc9mkvft.1). Code is available upon request.