Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2019 Oct 18;9:14947. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-51351-1

Flying Drosophila show sex-specific attraction to fly-labelled food

Laurie Cazalé-Debat 1, Benjamin Houot 1,2, Jean-Pierre Farine 1, Claude Everaerts 1, Jean-François Ferveur 1,
PMCID: PMC6802089  PMID: 31628403

Abstract

Animals searching for food and sexual partners often use odourant mixtures combining food-derived molecules and pheromones. For orientation, the vinegar fly Drosophila melanogaster uses three types of chemical cues: (i) the male volatile pheromone 11-cis-vaccenyl acetate (cVA), (ii) sex-specific cuticular hydrocarbons (CHs; and CH-derived compounds), and (iii) food-derived molecules resulting from microbiota activity. To evaluate the effects of these chemicals on odour-tracking behaviour, we tested Drosophila individuals in a wind tunnel. Upwind flight and food preference were measured in individual control males and females presented with a choice of two food sources labelled by fly lines producing varying amounts of CHs and/or cVA. The flies originated from different species or strains, or their microbiota was manipulated. We found that (i) fly-labelled food could attract—but never repel—flies; (ii) the landing frequency on fly-labelled food was positively correlated with an increased flight duration; (iii) male—but not female or non-sex-specific—CHs tended to increase the landing frequency on fly-labelled food; (iv) cVA increased female—but not male—preference for cVA-rich food; and (v) microbiota-derived compounds only affected male upwind flight latency. Therefore, sex pheromones interact with food volatile chemicals to induce sex-specific flight responses in Drosophila.

Subject terms: Chemical ecology, Behavioural ecology

Introduction

When searching for food sources, prey, appropriate sexual partners, and the most suitable ecological niche, animals often use multiple sensory cues1,2. Chemical sensory stimuli are ubiquitously used by vertebrates, invertebrates and even unicellular organisms to fulfil these needs3. Very often, the simultaneous emission of food-derived molecules and pheromones helps individuals to accurately orient in space during their search47.

Similar to many other flying insects, Drosophila can fly in a sustained manner over a relatively long distance811. When flying insects are far from a food source, they generally use mechanosensory stimuli to estimate wind velocity, while they rely on chemical and visual stimuli to visually orient through an odour gradient1215. At a closer distance, they simultaneously perceive visual and chemical signals before landing on the source1618. To detect volatile chemosensory cues, Drosophila melanogaster flies use sensory hairs (sensilla) covering several head appendages (antenna, maxillary palps19) and the wings20,21. The receptor neurons housed in these sensilla send a converging influx to specific—sometimes sex-specific—brain centres that process the information to allow the insect to react with an adapted behaviour according to its sex and mating status6,2225.

D. melanogaster flies mainly use two types of pheromones: 11-cis-vaccenyl acetate (cVA26,27), a male-specific volatile compound, and several sex-specific cuticular hydrocarbons (CHs), which are mostly—but not exclusively—detected by physical contact2831. During copulation, males transfer cVA into the female reproductive tract; subsequently, the eggs laid on food are covered with cVA, but only during the few days following mating32. Drosophila pheromones can modulate several subsocial behaviours. For example, cVA associated with sex-specific CHs inhibits male courtship and stimulates female sexual receptivity3338. The “male CHs + cVA” mixture can also induce male-male aggression39,40 and render food more attractive to both sexes5,41.

Moreover, some food-derived molecules (especially yeast and fermentation products) and pheromones are involved in Drosophila long- to close-distance attraction21,42. At a relatively close distance, fatty acid-derived compounds can influence aggregation and feeding43,44. Some of these products deposited in frass depend on the activity of gut-associated bacteria43. At a longer distance, cVA combined with volatile food by-products (vinegar) can increase fly aggregation on food sources6,45,46, depending on the sex, hunger state and mating status of the tested flies5,47. Furthermore, an aldehyde compound (Z4-11Al) derived from the degradation of the main cuticular pheromone (7,11-heptacosadiene) produced by females of most strains can attract wild-type males of the two sibling species D. melanogaster and D. simulans at some distance and with some intraspecific variation48. An inter- and intraspecific variation for free flight odour tracking has also been reported in several Drosophila species49.

To decipher the long-range effect of cVA and/or CHs combined with food-derived molecules, we measured free flight performance in D. melanogaster flies tested in a wind tunnel. Such devices have been used to test odour-guided flight in Drosophila in only a few studies12,13,16,18,21,42,44,45,47,48. Our experiments aimed to test the ecological significance of the chemical mixtures deposited by flies on food and not only the effect of isolated compounds. More precisely, compared to previous reports, the originality of our study resides in the combination of the six following aspects: we determined (i) both flight performance and food preference in individual flies (ii) of the two sexes (iii) from several natural and lab-manipulated lines (iv) presented with a dual food choice generally associating plain food with food labelled (v) by several lines of flies producing various CHs/cVA profiles (vi) or with an altered microbiota.

Results

Responses of control D. melanogaster flies to food labelled by other control flies

We measured several free flight parameters in individual flies: the frequency and latency of taking upwind flight from the starting platform, flight duration (corresponding to the time difference between “taking upwind flight” and “landing latency”) and the overall landing frequency (estimated only from individuals taking upwind flight; Fig. 1). Landing preference was measured in a dual-choice test based on the frequency of individuals landing on each food source. For the sake of clarity, we only show the upwind flight and overall landing frequencies together with the landing preference. The latency of taking upwind flight and the flight duration are shown in the Supplemental Info section.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Wind tunnel device used to measure single fly responses to food choice. Schematic representation of upwind free flight in the wind tunnel. The four flight features measured are the frequency and latency of taking upwind flight, flight duration and overall landing frequency. We also noted the preference (LF/PF) in the binary choice between food generally consisting of plain food (PF) and food labelled by flies from different lines (labelled food = LF). The wind speed and direction are indicated on the right.

Upwind flight and landing preference were assessed in single starved control Canton-S (CS) male and female flies presented with a dual choice between plain food (PF) and fly-labelled food (LF; Fig. 2). All LFs were marked by groups of 170 CS flies (either males or females; or 85 flies of each sex in the “mixed sex odour” treatment). Note that while the control test did not involve LFs (“PF vs. PF”; see empty histogram bars on the left side of Fig. 2), another test simultaneously used two LFs (“CS virgin female odours” vs. “CS virgin female odours + cVA”; see histogram bars with dark dots on the right side).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Flight and landing preference in single CS flies tested for food labelled by CS flies. The histograms represent (a,d) the frequency of flies taking upwind flight (calculated from the total number of flies tested: see the bottom of each histogram bar), (b,e) the overall landing frequency (calculated from flying individuals), and (c,f) the landing preferences of mature male (c) and female CS flies (f). The coloured fill of the bars indicates the sex and mating status of the CS flies labelling the food (labelled food = LF; indicated below the histograms). The blue and pink colours indicate food labelled by males and females, respectively. Light and dark pink colours indicate labelling by virgin and mated females, respectively. The mixed blue/dark pink histogram bar indicates food labelled by both sexes (mixed sex). The light pink bar with dark spots (right bar) indicates food labelled by virgin females and enriched with synthetic cis-vaccenyl acetate (cVA). The white bar on the left represents plain food (PF; without fly labelling). In all experiments, the flies were tested through a binary choice involving PF and LF, except in the control test (left bar: PF/PF) and the test involving “virgin CS female odours vs. virgin CS female odours + cVA” (right bars). Therefore, the bars above and below the baseline show the proportion of flies choosing each food type. For each sex, the difference between upwind flight and landing frequencies was tested with the Wilks G2 likelihood ratio test completed with a computation of significance by cell (Fisher’s exact test; significant differences at α = 0.05 are indicated by different letters above the bars), whereas landing preference was tested with the z-test, and the corresponding frequencies were compared between the different LFs using the Marascuilo procedure. For the two frequency parameters, significant differences are indicated by different letters at α = 0.05, while the level of significance for food preference is represented (or not) by asterisks (*α < 0.05; **α < 0.01; ***α < 0.001; no asterisk: not significant). (a) Wilks G2 likelihood ratio test, G2(5df) = 16.6, p = 0.005; (b) G2(5df) = 61.2, p < 10−4; (c) Marascuilo procedure, Khi2(5df) = 4.4, p = 0.494; (d) G2(5df) = 6.6, p = 0.249; (e) G2(5df) = 15.3, p = 0.009; (f) Khi2(5df) = 12.0, p = 0.035.

CS males and females very frequently took upwind flight in the presence of all fly-labelled foods (85–100%; Fig. 2a,d). Only males showed significant differences, presenting higher frequencies of taking upwind flight toward food labelled by females. Both sexes showed significant variation in their overall landing frequencies depending on the genotype of the flies that labelled the food. Males landed more frequently (88–92%) on food with CS female odours (either mixed sexes, mated flies or virgins) than on foods with “CS virgin female odours + cVA” or with CS male odours (55 and 48%, respectively; Fig. 2b) and on PF in the choice test without fly-labelled food (PF/PF; 39%). Females generally landed more frequently on food with CS male odours (70%) than on any other fly-labelled food, while the PF/PF choice induced much less landing (34%, Fig. 2e). No marked difference was detected between the sexes and LFs for the latency of taking upwind flight or the flight duration (Fig. S1).

Landing preference significantly varied depending on both the sex of the fly and the type of food tested (Fig. 2c,f). Males preferred food with CS mix sex odours (p < 0.01; Fig. 2c) than food labelled with CS male or mated or virgin female odours (p < 0.05). However, males showed no landing preference toward food with CS virgin female odours enriched with cVA compared to CS virgin female odours without cVA (p > 0.05). Females strongly preferred food with CS mixed sex odours (p < 0.001; Fig. 2f) over food with CS male or mated female odours (p < 0.01). However, in contrast to males, females showed no landing preference toward food with CS virgin female odours (p > 0.05), while they clearly preferred food labelled with “CS virgin female odours + cVA” (p < 0.01).

To check whether the landing preference observed in the wind tunnel could also be observed with another behavioural device, we measured individual fly preference in a Y-maze olfactometer. In addition to the “PF vs. PF” choice (control test), we tested fly preference in the choice between PF and food with CS mixed sex odours (Fig. S2). In the latter choice test, males showed a faster and stronger response than females: after 20 min, 50% of males and 25% of females had moved to either arm (Fig. S2c); after 240 min, 75% of males and 70% of females had moved in either olfactometer arm, and they showed a clear preference toward food with CS mixed sex odours (p < 0.001 and p < 0.01, respectively; Fig. S2a,b).

Responses of control D. melanogaster flies to food labelled by flies showing diverse cuticular hydrocarbon and cVA profiles

To further determine the nature of the fly compounds potentially influencing the flight response, food labelled by flies from several lines differing in their cuticular hydrocarbon (CH) profiles or cVA levels, or with a potentially altered microbiota was used (Fig. 3; Tables S1 and S2).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Chemical profiles of the flies labelling the food. For each fly genotype (indicated on the left), the mean absolute contents (in ng) of the principal cuticular hydrocarbons (CHs) detected and cVA are shown. For the sake of clarity, CHs were separated based on their chain length (indicated above the histograms for 23 to 29 carbons (C23-C29, and below histogram bars on the left for the lighter CHs with 21 and 22 carbons (C21, C22). The sum of CHs (∑CHs; in ng) is represented with unfilled bars on the right. N = 20 flies except for the Desat1 males and females, where N = 15. For the nomenclature of chemicals and fly line identity, please refer to Table S1 and Fig. 4.

In these conditions, most flies took upwind flight (81–98%; Fig. 4a,d). Females showed a higher upwind flight frequency in the presence of food labelled with D. simulans female odours than related to any other fly-labelled food. D. simulans virgin females were used because they produce similar CHs to CS males but no cVA28. Food labelled with D. simulans female odours induced 60% and 36% overall landing frequencies in males and females, respectively; this food was preferred by both sexes (p < 0.05; Fig. 4c,f). Food labelled by oenocyteless males (Oe; producing very low CH levels but wild-type-like cVA levels50) induced low overall landing frequencies in males and females (48% and 29%, respectively; Fig. 4b,d) but a strong landing preference (p < 0.01 and p < 0.001, respectively; Fig. 4c,f). In contrast, food labelled by desat1 mutant males and females (producing low levels of desaturated CHs and high levels of linear saturated CHs51,52) induced high overall landing frequencies in both sexes (75 to 86%), but a significant landing preference was only shown by CS males toward food with desat1 male odours (p < 0.001).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Free flight and landing preference in single CS flies toward food labelled by flies of various genotypes. The histograms represent (a,d) the frequency of flies taking upwind flight, (b,e) the overall landing frequency, and (c,f) the landing preference in mature male (ac) and female CS flies. (df) The fly-labelled foods are shown below the histograms, from left to right: CS males, D. simulans females, oenocyteless males (Oe), Desat1 mutant males and females, CS males originating from dechorionated eggs (Dechor. CS males) and antibiotic-treated eggs (Antibio. CS males). For the parameters and statistics, please refer to the Fig. 2 legend. (a) G2(6df) = 3.2, p = 0.789; (b) G2(6df) = 48.5, p = 0.0001; (c) Khi2(6df) = 13.9, p = 0.031; (d) G2(6df) = 13.1, p = 0.042; (e) G2(6df) = 85.3, p = 0.0001; (c) Khi2(6df) = 39.4, p = 0.0001. The sample size is indicated at the bottom of each histogram bar (a,d).

To test the possible effect of the microbiota potentially transferred by the flies labelling the food, we treated the eggs from which the “labelling” flies were produced. Practically, we either removed the egg chorion or raised control eggs on antibiotic-rich food53,54. While the two foods labelled by dechorionated- or antibiotic-treated CS males induced low to moderate overall landing frequencies (38–59%), both were strongly preferred by males (p < 0.01) and females (p < 0.001). No difference was detected for upwind flight latency, and only the female flight duration toward food with Oe odours was slightly shorter compared to the responses to other labelled foods (Fig. S3).

Testing the relationship between fly chemicals, flight parameters and food preference

In an attempt to determine the roles of cVA, CHs, food and/or their derived products in affecting upwind flight and landing preference, we performed a principal component analysis (PCA) taking into account the variation of all relevant flight parameters (upwind flight latency, flight duration, overall landing frequency) and landing preference (“LF/PF”), together with the relative proportions of each hydrocarbon and cVA detected in the cuticular extracts of the flies used to label food (Fig. 3; Table S2). All these parameters were used as variables, whereas the “labelling” fly types were considered as individuals (Fig. 5). The three first axes of this PCA explained 84.5% of the total variation of our sample (170 flies for CH analysis; 769 males and 943 females for behaviour). Most of the flight parameters were highly correlated between the sexes. In both sexes, the overall landing frequency was strongly negatively correlated with the (i) labelled food preference and (ii) flight duration. In other words, when the total number of flies landing on both food sources increased, their preference for landing on the labelled food decreased, and their flight duration increased. Although we cannot infer the effect of each single chemical in this PCA given that multiple chemicals often act in synergy to modulate one or several aspects of flight, we noted several clear correlations between behavioural parameters and (groups of) chemical compounds. More precisely, in both sexes, the landing preference toward labelled food was highly correlated with the labelling of the preferred food by flies producing high levels of shorter-chain male-predominant monodesaturated CHs (monoenes with 23C-carbon chains: 5 T, 7 T, 9 T; with 25C-carbon chains: 7 P, 9 P). In contrast, the overall landing frequency of males and females showed a positive relationship with the level of longer-chain monoenes (with 27 C: 5 H, 7 H; with 29 C: 7 N, 9 N) on the labelled food. Increased overall landing frequencies were also marginally correlated with female-specific dienes (double desaturated CHs) in males and with methyl-branched (23Br-31Br) plus linear saturated CHs (C21-C29) in females. Note that the upwind flight latency showed a negative relationship between the sexes. As a consequence, the female latency of taking upwind flight and, to a lesser extent, the female flight duration were correlated with the variation of the cVA level, which were effects that were not detected in males.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Statistical relationship between chemicals, flight parameters and labelling flies. Three-dimensional plot of the 3 first axes of the principal component analysis (PCA; type Pearson’s correlation matrix; with standardized values) using relevant flight parameters (upwind flight latency, flight duration, overall landing frequency), landing preference (labelled food vs. plain food = LF/PF) and the relative proportions of each hydrocarbon and cVA detected in the cuticular extract of labelling flies as variables and the “labelling” fly types as individuals. The 3rd dimension (axis 3) is represented by the size of the symbols (circle, square, star). These three first PCA axes explain 84.5% of the total variation of our sample (axis 1 = 43.2%, axis 2 = 31.4%, axis 3 = 9.9%). Blue and pink filled circles represent male and female flight parameters, respectively. The star represents cVA, while empty circles represent the content of CHs carried by the labelling flies (red = monoenes; purple = dienes; blue = methyl-branched CHs; green = linear CHs). The fly lines used to label food are represented by empty black squares.

Flight and food preference in naïve male flies of diverse genotypes

We compared the response shown by control CS males toward food labelled with CS male odours with the response of males from (i) an antibiotic-treated CS strain, (ii) another D. melanogaster wild-type strain producing variant CHs (Zimbabwe30 = Z3055), or (iii) the D. simulans sibling species (Fig. 6). These males showed significant variation in their upwind flight frequencies (76% in D. simulans, 98% in Z30 males) and landing frequencies (approximately 74% in Z30 and D. simulans; 34% in antibiotic-treated males). Both the latency of taking upwind flight and flight duration varied strongly between males (p < 0.001; Fig. S4). More particularly, antibiotic-treated CS males showed a shorter upwind flight latency compared to the three wild-type males (p < 0.001). Moreover, D. simulans and antibiotic-treated CS males showed a greater landing preference toward food labelled with CS male odours (p < 0.001 and p < 0.01, respectively) compared to those of the two other males (p < 0.05).

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Free flight and landing preference in single male flies from various genotypes toward food labelled by CS males. Histograms represent (a) the frequency of flies taking upwind flight, (b) the overall landing frequency, and (c) the landing preference between plain food (PF; empty bars) and food labelled by CS males (coloured bars). In addition to CS control males (blue coloured bars), we tested the response of individual males derived from CS antibiotic-treated eggs (Antibio. CS males; green coloured bars), from the Zimbabwe30 wild-type line (Z30; yellow coloured bars), and from D. simulans (D. simulans; purple coloured bars). For parameters and statistics, please refer to the Fig. 2 legend. (a) G2(3df) = 11.0, p = 0.012; (b) G2(3df) = 7.9, p = 0.049; (c) Khi2(3df) = 5.5, p = 0.142. The sample size is indicated at the bottom of each histogram bar (a).

Material and Methods

Drosophila strains and rearing

We used the following strains: two Drosophila melanogaster wild-type strains: Canton-S (CS) and Zimbabwe30 (Z30), a Drosophila simulans wild-type strain (line #K509, a gift from Prof. Yamamoto), and homozygous desat11573 mutant flies, showing altered production and perception of cuticular hydrocarbons51,52,56. We also used oenocyteless flies (Oe50) resulting from a cross between UAS-hid/CyO females and +: PromE(800)-Gal4[2 M],Tub:Gal80ts, + males (kindly provided by Prof. J-C Billeter). All strains were raised on yeast/cornmeal/agar medium [for 6 L of food: 300 g of yeast, 392 g of maize flour, 55 g of agar and 185 ml of Tegosept (®Apex) completed with distilled water] and kept under a 12:12 hour light/dark cycle at 24 ± 0.5 °C with 65 ± 5% humidity. Oe transgenic parents were mated and kept at 18.5 °C until the emergence of their adult progeny, which were kept at 29 °C for 3–5 days.

All flies were derived from mass-rearing stocks transferred every 2–3 days to avoid competition and regularly provide progeny. The flies were screened 4 to 6 hours after emergence under light CO2 anaesthesia and kept at 24 ± 0.5 °C (at 29 °C for Oe flies) in same sex groups (20 flies) in all labelling flies and the female fly subjects. Male subjects were held individually to prevent social interactions potentially affecting behaviour57.

Food labelling

To investigate the effect of the molecules potentially involved in free flight odour tracking and landing preference, we labelled fresh plain laboratory food with living flies. To label food, 170 “labelling” flies were kept in a petri dish (∅ = 5.4 cm) filled with fresh food and covered by a plastic lid (h = 6.4 cm) for 24 hours under similar experimental conditions as described above. Flies used to label food (“labelling flies”) were removed prior to the behavioural experiment.

Microbiota alteration

Three- to four-day-old mass-reared CS females were allowed to lay eggs for 6 hours in an egg-laying device consisting of a 50 mm Petri dish filled with 1 ml of 3% agar striped with fresh yeast to stimulate egg laying. After this period, the flies were removed, and their eggs were collected. To remove the chorion associated with the microbiota present on the surface, eggs were soaked for a few seconds in concentrated -bleach water (9.6%). These eggs were then thoroughly rinsed with distilled water and deposited in groups of approximately 130 in vials with fresh food. For the antibiotic treatment, a dozen of 4-day-old flies of each sex were kept together overnight for mass mating. The next morning, males and females were separated under light CO2 anaesthesia. Then, females were allowed to lay eggs on antibiotic-treated food (50 μg/mL tetracycline +200 μg/mL rifampicin +100 μg/mL streptomycin) during a 12-hour period53. Adult males resulting from these antibiotic-treated eggs were used to label food under similar conditions to those described above.

Wind tunnel

The design of the wind tunnel has been previously described in detail21,49,58. Briefly, it consists of clear acrylic (length = 155 cm; width and height = 30.5 cm) and is illuminated by four band strips of white LEDs (BDL- F300W-05-3528, Boulevard des Leds, France; length = 1 m) located below the tunnel base and separated with a red screen. Tracing paper was placed over the tunnel to homogenize the light intensity inside the flying section. The two lateral panels of the tunnel were covered with a randomized pattern consisting of black and white squares (side = 3 cm). A “departure/starting” platform (height = 16 cm) was placed in the downwind section at 90 cm from the two landing platforms (height = 16 cm, ∅ = 1.7 cm) located in the upwind section. The two landing platforms were placed 10 cm from each lateral panel and were separated from each other by 7.5 cm. Before each behavioural test, approximately 1 cm3 of food was deposited on a microscope slide at the top of each platform. A humidifier (® OKOIA, AH400; Tianjin, China) was placed at the entrance of the airflow to maintain a constant humidity (65–75%) in the flying section. A laminar airflow (0.4 ms−1) was running through the section. After each session of tests (performed between 9:00 am and 3:00 pm), the wind tunnel was washed with a 70% ethanol solution, and the room was ventilated until the next day. One hour before the start of the experiment, the room temperature was adjusted to 24±1 °C.

Free flight parameters and food choice

The experiments were conducted under red light with starving flies to stimulate upwind flight attraction46. More precisely, 16 hours before the test, each fly was placed in a glass vial containing only a piece of absorbent paper (length = 4 cm, width = 2 cm) moistened with 90 μL of distilled water and kept at 25 °C. During flight, we measured several parameters and landing preference in a binary food choice assay. Individual flies were introduced with a mouth aspirator to an acclimation chamber (consisting of an acrylic tube; ∅ = 5 mm) separated by a gate from the inside of the wind tunnel and were left for 3 min. After acclimation, the flies were allowed to reach the part of the tube opening inside the wind tunnel. Once the fly reached the lift off platform, we successively noted its latency (and frequency) of taking upwind flight, flight duration (between lift off and landing on either platform, also corresponding to landing latency), and in the case of landing, the platform chosen. We also determined the overall landing (based on the sum of landings on the two platforms). Each experiment lasted 10 min or less when the fly landed on a platform. Each binary choice experiment generally associated one platform with plain food and the second one with fly-labelled food. In the case of food labelled by virgin females and enriched with cVA, 100 ng of cVA (® Cayman Chemical, Ann Arbor, MI, USA; 50 mg/ml solution in ethanol; purity >98%) in 5 μl of hexane was added to a Whatman filter paper patch (∅ = 1 cm, ® GE Healthcare Life Sciences) and deposited on fly-labelled food a few minutes prior to each test. In this test, 5 μl of hexane was added to filter paper on food labelled by virgin females (without cVA) on the “control” platform.

Y-maze olfactometer

Food preference was also measured in a glass Y-maze, which allowed us to record the walking decisions made by individual flies confronted with two odours present in either arm of the maze. The device consisted of a regular empty glass vial connected to a straight tube (length = 6 cm; ∅ = 0.3 cm) divided into two 5-cm long arms, each of which was connected to a glass vial containing a filter paper impregnated with the food to be tested. The Y-maze was placed in darkness, and the tests were performed between 9 am and 2 pm. The flies and fly-labelled food were prepared as for the wind tunnel experiment. Each fly was individually introduced into the starting vial connected to the straight arm of the Y-shape olfactometer. Every 20 min, after the start of the test (and during the first two hours of the test), the position of the fly was noted in the device relative to the arm (type of food) chosen. The final choice of the fly was also noted 240 min after the start of the test.

Chemical analysis

To extract adult cuticular hydrocarbons, 5-day-old flies were individually immersed for 10 min at room temperature in 30 μl of hexane containing 100 ng of C26 (n-hexacosane) and 100 ng of C30 (n-triacontane) as internal standards. Each fly was removed, and the extracts were stored at −18 °C until analysis. All extracts were analysed using a Varian CP3380 gas chromatograph fitted with a flame ionization detector, a CP Sil 5CB column (25 m × 0.25 mm internal ∅; 0.1 μm film thickness; Agilent), and a split–splitless injector (60 ml/min split-flow; valve opening 30 s after injection) with helium as the carrier gas (velocity = 50 cm/s at 120 °C). The temperature program began at 120 °C, was then ramped at 10 °C/min up to 140 °C, followed by 2 °C/min to 290 °C and holding for 10 min. The chemical identities of the major cuticular hydrocarbons and cVA were checked using gas chromatography-mass spectrometry30. The amount (in ng) of each compound was calculated based on the values provided by internal standards.

Statistics

Behavioural frequencies (upwind flight and landing) were compared using the Wilks G2 likelihood ratio test completed with a computation of significance by cell (Fisher’s exact test). The choice between the two platforms was tested using the z-test, and the corresponding frequencies were compared between the different fly-labelled foods using the Marascuilo procedure. We performed a principal component analysis (PCA; Pearson’s correlation matrix type; with standardized values) with the relevant flight parameters (upwind flight latency, flight duration, overall landing frequency), food preference (LF/PF) and the level of each hydrocarbon and of cVA detected in the cuticular extracts of the “labelling flies” taken as variables, while the “labelling fly” strains were used as individuals. PCA is a powerful type of analysis that has often been used in similar studies to discover correlations between individual chemical compounds present in complex mixtures and behavioural events potentially induced by these compounds5966. Finally, the dynamic curves obtained for the latency of upwind flight and flight duration (shown in Supplemental Figures) were analysed using the Kaplan-Meier procedure followed by a log-rank test and a post hoc multiple pairs comparison (α = 0.05 with Bonferroni correction). All statistical analyses were performed using XLSTAT 201967.

Discussion

Our study aimed to investigate the potential effect of CHs and cVA mixed with laboratory food on the long-range olfactory tracking behaviour of Drosophila flies. Compared to other Drosophila studies performed in a wind tunnel, the originality of our approach lies in the binary choice between two food sources, allowing us to determine the landing preference after upwind flight. The wide array of chemical profiles shown by the flies used to label food was helpful in attempting to establish a relationship between pheromonal compounds and flight response. The comparison of the responses shown by the control flies of both sexes and by several variant males provided us with additional data. The dissociation between the “overall landing frequency” and “landing preference” provides novel information suggesting that the perception of some volatile olfactory cues—mostly non-specific—emanating from fly-labelled food can influence upwind flight and non-discriminatory landing, while other cues that are more specific and perceived at a closer distance to the food sources can influence landing preference and increase flight duration. Nevertheless, when testing chemical blends, one can never assume the influence played by each single component on each single behavioural parameter68. Another limitation is due to the fact that the concentration of the various chemicals participating to the blend can change between the source point to the fly position during its flight. Therefore, it is never easy to determine which component(s) of the blend is (are) active at every position of the fly in the wind tunnel. The different aspects of this hypothesis are discussed below.

Taking upwind flight

The different volatile substances emanating from the fly-labelled food may not have been perceived by the control flies (CS) starting their upwind flight since they showed no—or very little—food-related difference in either the latency or the frequency of upwind flight. This was confirmed by the absence of a correlation between upwind flight latency and CHs in our PCA. However, the negative correlation between the sexes for the latency of taking upwind flight indicates sex-specific variation relative to the perception of cVA when flies decide to take upwind flight. Three more observations made in the present study reinforce this hypothesis: (i) both the female latency of taking flight and female flight duration were moderately correlated with cVA amounts; (ii) females—but not males—preferred to land on food labelled with virgin CS female odours + cVA compared to food labelled with virgin CS female odours without cVA; and, (iii) food with mixed sex odours (described as copulating fly46 or mated5 odour) was slightly preferred by control females compared to control males. Together, these data support the finding that starved females exhibit a higher sensitivity than males to cVA associated with food-derived odour(s)5. However, the stronger preference shown by males toward food with mixed sex odours in the Y-olfactometer suggested that the sex difference observed in the wind tunnel disappeared when fly odours were more concentrated in a smaller-volume behavioural device without wind or when flies walked instead of flying.

In contrast to previous reports, the control males did not show any long-range attraction to food that was either naturally or experimentally labelled with cVA45. At least five experimental factors could explain the discrepancies between our study and other reports: (i) we tested single flies, while others tested groups of flies (between 1046 and 100045), and we suspect that in groups, the first flies contacting the source could emit additional pheromones that subsequently affect the behaviour of other flies; (ii) we tested food choice in a wind tunnel, in contrast to the two previous studies that used an olfactometer placed inside a wind tunnel; (iii) we tested food labelled by flies but not fly extracts45 or intact flies kept behind a mesh46; (iv) in other studies, cVA was not mixed with food but was presented concurrently with vinegar, a food-derived compound that strongly reinforces the cVA effect6,46; and (v) the sex-specific response to cVA may also be due to the sex-specific effect of cVA conditioning during early developmental exposure32.

In addition to cVA, long-range attraction to male odours or copulating fly odours could also be induced by some as-yet-unknown volatile male substance(s) emitted just prior to copulation46,69,70. This possibility was not clear in our hands given that the control flies only showed a slightly increased preference toward food labelled with CS mixed sex odours compared to food with CS male odours. Both CS sexes also showed a strong preference toward landing on food labelled by CS males with an altered microbiota. Given that such “labelling” males (either originating from dechorionation or antibiotic treatment) showed no CH defect compared to control males (Fig. 3 and in contrast with a study involving antibiotic-treated flies53), it is possible that the hypothetical compounds that are normally derived from fly microbiota activity tend to partially repel fly landing. Compared to previous studies revealing the strong effect of frass deposited both by control and bacterially infested flies43,71, our results were surprising given that the food on which 170 flies were kept during 24 hours may be rich in frass. However, the apparent discrepancy between studies could again be related to the very different sizes of the experimental devices used to test fly behaviour.

The Z30 and CS antibiotic-treated males diverged from the control males in several flight parameters in the presence of food labelled with CS male odours. The flight duration of Z30 males was longer compared to those of other males, but this did not affect their overall landing frequency, which was still higher than that of CS males. The variation of Z30 behaviour could be linked to the lower activity of these flies in the presence of plain food49 and more generally to their lower behavioural activity72. In contrast, CS antibiotic-treated males took flight more quickly, which could have resulted from either their altered olfactory perception, as shown in germfree mice73, or the lack of exposure to microbiota-derived pheromones during their early development54.

Landing on the food source

The control flies approaching food/pheromonal sources spent a greater proportion of time there, likely due to more intense sampling of cues in the surroundings these sources49. Then, based on the simultaneous integration and comparison of the visual and chemical stimuli emanating from each platform (in the case of a binary choice), the fly chooses to land on one of the platforms18. We found that a higher preference for landing on fly-labelled food was correlated with a longer flight duration. This finding supports the idea that this decision-making process takes more time than non-discriminatory landing. This effect may also explain the shorter flight duration shown by desat1 mutant males (with altered sex pheromone perception), especially near a food source21,49,52.

The difference between the low overall landing frequency induced by the control test without fly-labelled food (PF/PF; 34% in females, 39% in males) and the higher frequency (49–92%) induced by most experiments involving at least one type of fly-labelled food provides crucial information indicating that the compounds left by flies on food generally increase the overall landing frequency and never decrease it. Reciprocal sex-related effects were observed: the male landing frequency increased on food labelled by CS females, whereas the CS female landing frequency slightly increased on food labelled by CS males. Moreover, the food labelled with CS mixed sex odours induced the greatest landing preference in both sexes. This finding is in accord with a report showing male preference for mixed sex odours (added with vinegar in the physical absence of flies46). However, the last study did not report on female preference.

The high correlation shown by our PCA indicates that longer-chain—mostly non-sex-specific— CHs promote fast and frequent but non-discriminatory landing overall in both sexes (5-, 7- and 9-monenes—mono-desaturated CHs—with 27 and 29 carbons) or in females (saturated linear CHs, methyl-branched CHs). Once taken into account the limitation to evaluate the role that single components of a blend can play on distinct flight parameters, we propose the following hypothesis. At a longer distance (corresponding to the 80 cm of our wind tunnel), attraction (but no landing preference) could be induced by chemicals derived from these long chain CHs, as shown for (Z)−4-undecenal (Z4-11Al), derived from 7,11-heptacosadiene, in a no-choice experiment46. However, when the CS flies had the choice, they preferred to land on the food labelled with shorter-chain CHs (monoenes with 23 and 25 carbons) than with longer-chain CHs (and/or with saturated linear CHs). Given the wind velocity and the relatively low volatility of these shorter-chain monoenes31, they may be only perceived at a relatively short distance, just before landing. In addition to CH-derived products, food-derived molecules resulting in microbiota activity could also be involved in landing preference43,71. However, our study does not support this hypothesis given that food labelled by microbiota-treated flies induced a greater landing preference compared to food labelled by control (untreated) male flies.

In summary, our data indicate that odour-guided flight and landing preference toward a food source that is either marked with fly substances or not depend on both the chemicals left on the food by flies and the sex or strain of the flying insect. The behavioural response induced by such chemicals mixed in the food can vary when they are presented alone or simultaneously with a second food source.

Supplementary information

Supplemental information (340KB, xlsx)

Acknowledgements

We thank Jérôme Cortot for technical help. This work was funded by the University of Bourgogne-Franche Comté, the Bourgogne-Franche Comté Regional Council (PARI 2016), the “Fonds Européen de Développement Régional” (FEDER), and the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS; Insb).

Author Contributions

L.C.D., B.H. and J.P.F. performed the experiments. L.C.D., B.H. and J.F.F. conceived the experiments. L.C.D., C.E. and J.F.F. analyzed the data. J.F.F. wrote the ms. C.E. made the figures. All authors revised the ms.

Data Availability

A xlsx File containing all raw data is available as supplemental material.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-019-51351-1.

References

  • 1.McMeniman CJ, Corfas RA, Matthews BJ, Ritchie SA, Vosshall LB. Multimodal integration of carbon dioxide and other sensory cues drives mosquito attraction to humans. Cell. 2014;156:1060–1071. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.12.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lucas‐Barbosa D, et al. Visual and odour cues: plant responses to pollination and herbivory affect the behaviour of flower visitors. Funct. Ecol. 2016;30:431–441. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12509. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wyatt, T. D. Pheromones and animal behavior: chemical signals and signatures. 2nd edn, (Cambridge University Press, 2014).
  • 4.Landolt PJ, Phillips TW. Host plant influences on sex pheromone behavior of phytophagous insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 1997;42:371–391. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.42.1.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lebreton S, et al. Feeding regulates sex pheromone attraction and courtship in Drosophila females. Sci. Rep. 2015;5:13132. doi: 10.1038/srep13132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Das S, et al. Electrical synapses mediate synergism between pheromone and food odors in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:E9962–E9971. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1712706114.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Chan, H. K. et al. Odorant mixtures elicit less variable and faster responses than pure odorants. PLoS Comput. Biol. 14, 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006536 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 8.Dickinson MH. Death Valley, Drosophila, and the Devonian toolkit. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 2014;59:51–72. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ento-011613-162041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Chapman JW, Reynolds DR, Wilson K. Long-range seasonal migration in insects: mechanisms, evolutionary drivers and ecological consequences. Ecol. Lett. 2015;18:287–302. doi: 10.1111/ele.12407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Reynolds AM, Reynolds DR, Sane SP, Hu G, Chapman JW. Orientation in high-flying migrant insects in relation to flows: mechanisms and strategies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London [Biol] 2016;371:20150392. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Manjila SB, Kuruvilla M, Ferveur JF, Sane SP, Hasan G. Extended flight bouts require disinhibition from GABAergic mushroom body neurons. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:283–293. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.11.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Budick SA, Reiser MB, Dickinson MH. The role of visual and mechanosensory cues in structuring forward flight in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Exp. Biol. 2007;210:4092. doi: 10.1242/jeb.006502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Duistermars BJ, Chow DM, Frye MA. Flies require bilateral sensory input to track odor gradients in flight. Curr. Biol. 2009;19:1301–1307. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Krishnan A, Sane SP. Visual feedback influences antennal positioning in flying hawk moths. J. Exp. Biol. 2014;217:908. doi: 10.1242/jeb.094276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Dahake A, Stöckl AL, Foster JJ, Sane SP, Kelber A. The roles of vision and antennal mechanoreception in hawkmoth flight control. eLife. 2018;7:e37606. doi: 10.7554/eLife.37606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Budick SA, Dickinson MH. Free-flight responses of Drosophila melanogaster to attractive odors. J. Exp. Biol. 2006;209:3001–3017. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bhandawat V, Olsen SR, Gouwens NW, Schlief ML, Wilson RI. Sensory processing in the Drosophila antennal lobe increases reliability and separability of ensemble odor representations. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:1474–1482. doi: 10.1038/nn1976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Saxena N, Natesan D, Sane SP. Odor source localization in complex visual environments by fruit flies. J. Exp. Biol. 2018;221:jeb172023. doi: 10.1242/jeb.172023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Stocker RF. The organization of the chemosensory system in Drosophila melanogaster: a rewiew. Cell Tissue Res. 1994;275:3–26. doi: 10.1007/BF00305372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Raad H, Ferveur JF, Ledger N, Capovilla M, Robichon A. Functional gustatory role of chemoreceptors in Drosophila wings. Cell Rep. 2016;15:1442–1454. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.04.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Houot, B., Gigot, V., Robichon, A. & Ferveur, J.-F. Free-flight odor tracking in Drosophila: effect of wing chemosensors, sex, and pheromonal gene regulation. Sci. Rep. 7, 10.1038/srep40221 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 22.Couto A, Alenius M, Dickson BJ. Molecular, anatomical, and functional organization of the Drosophila olfactory system. Curr. Biol. 2005;15:1535–1547. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.07.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Fishilevich E, et al. Chemotaxis behavior mediated by single larval olfactory neurons in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2005;15:2086–2096. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Datta SR, et al. The Drosophila pheromone cVA activates a sexually dimorphic neural circuit. Nature. 2008;452:473–477. doi: 10.1038/nature06808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ruta V, et al. A dimorphic pheromone circuit in Drosophila from sensory input to descending output. Nature. 2010;468:686–690. doi: 10.1038/nature09554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Butterworth FM. Lipids of Drosophila: a newly detected lipid in the male. Science. 1969;163:1356–1357. doi: 10.1126/science.163.3873.1356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Guiraudie-Capraz G, Pho DB, Jallon JM. Role of the ejaculatory bulb in biosynthesis of the male pheromone cis-vaccenyl acetate in Drosophila melanogaster. Integr. Zool. 2007;2:89–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-4877.2007.00047.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jallon JM. A few chemical words exchanged byDrosophila during courtship and mating. Behav. Genet. 1984;14:441–478. doi: 10.1007/BF01065444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Everaerts C, Lacaille F, Ferveur JF. Is mate choice in Drosophila males guided by olfactory or gustatory pheromones? Anim. Behav. 2010;79:1135–1146. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Everaerts C, Farine JP, Cobb M, Ferveur JF. Drosophila cuticular hydrocarbons revisited: mating status alters cuticular profiles. PLoS One. 2010;5:e9607. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Farine JP, Ferveur JF, Everaerts C. Volatile Drosophila cuticular pheromones are affected by social but not sexual experience. PLoS One. 2012;7:e40396. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Everaerts, C. et al. Pre-imaginal conditioning alters adult sex pheromone response in Drosophila. PeerJeCollection 2018. 10.7717/peerj.5585 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 33.Jallon JM, Antony C, Benamar O. An anti-aphrodisiac produced by Drosophila melanogaster males and transferred to females during copulation. C. R. Acad. Sci. Ser. III Sci. Vie. 1981;292:1147–1149. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Grillet M, Dartevelle L, Ferveur JF. A Drosophila male pheromone affects female sexual receptivity. Proc. R. Soc. Biol. Sci. Ser. B. 2006;273:315–323. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kurtovic A, Widmer A, Dickson BJ. A single class of olfactory neurons mediates behavioural responses to a Drosophila sex pheromone. Nature. 2007;446:542–546. doi: 10.1038/nature05672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Billeter JC, Levine J. The role of cVA and the Odorant binding protein Lush in social and sexual behavior in Drosophila melanogaster Front. Ecol. Evol. 2015;3:75. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2015.00075. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Laturney M, Billeter JC. Drosophila melanogaster females restore their attractiveness after mating by removing male anti-aphrodisiac pheromones. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:12322. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Mercier D, Tsuchimoto Y, Ohta K, Kazama H. Olfactory landmark-based communication in interacting Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2018;28:2624–2631.e2625. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Fernandez MP, Kravitz EA. Aggression and courtship in Drosophila: pheromonal communication and sex recognition. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 2013;199:1065–1076. doi: 10.1007/s00359-013-0851-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Wang L, et al. Hierarchical chemosensory regulation of male-male social interactions in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:757–762. doi: 10.1038/nn.2800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Duménil C, et al. Pheromonal cues deposited by mated females convey social information about egg-laying sites in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Chem. Ecol. 2016;42:259–269. doi: 10.1007/s10886-016-0681-3.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Becher PG, et al. Yeast, not fruit volatiles mediate Drosophila melanogaster attraction, oviposition and development. Funct. Ecol. 2012;26:822–828. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2012.02006.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Keesey IW, et al. Adult frass provides a pheromone signature for Drosophila feeding and aggregation. J. Chem. Ecol. 2016;42:739–747. doi: 10.1007/s10886-016-0737-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Dweck HKM, et al. Drosophila aggregation and mating pheromones. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:E2829–E2835. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504527112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Bartelt RJ, Schaner AM, Jackson LL. Cis-Vaccenyl acetate as an aggregation pheromone In Drosophila melanogaster. J. Chem. Ecol. 1985;11:1747–1756. doi: 10.1007/bf01012124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Lebreton S, Becher PG, Hansson BS, Witzgall P. Attraction of Drosophila melanogaster males to food-related and fly odours. J. Insect Physiol. 2012;58:125–129. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2011.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lebreton S, et al. Love makes smell blind: mating suppresses pheromone attraction in Drosophila females via Or65a olfactory neurons. Sci. Rep. 2014;4:7119. doi: 10.1038/srep07119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lebreton, S. et al. A Drosophila female pheromone elicits species-specific long-range attraction via an olfactory channel with dual specificity for sex and food. BMC Biology15, 10.1186/s12915-017-0427-x (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 49.Houot B, et al. Gene regulation and species-specific evolution of free-flight odor-tracking in. Drosophila. Mol. Biol. Evol. 2018;35:3–15. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msx241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Billeter JC, Atallah J, Krupp JJ, Millar JG, Levine JD. Specialized cells tag sexual and species identity in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature. 2009;461:987–991. doi: 10.1038/nature08495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Marcillac F, Bousquet F, Alabouvette J, Savarit F, Ferveur JF. A mutation with major effects on Drosophila melanogaster sex pheromones. Genetics. 2005;171:1617–1628. doi: 10.1534/genetics.104.033159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Marcillac F, Grosjean Y, Ferveur JF. A single mutation alters production and discrimination of Drosophila sex pheromones. Proc. R. Soc. Biol. Sci. Ser. B. 2005;272:303–309. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Sharon G, et al. Commensal bacteria play a role in mating preference of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:20051–20056. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009906107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Farine, J. P., Habbachi, W., Cortot, J., Roche, S. & Ferveur, J. F. Maternally-transmitted microbiota affects odor emission and preference in Drosophila larva. Sci. Rep. 7, 10.1038/s41598-017-04922-z (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 55.Grillet M, et al. Incipient speciation in Drosophila melanogaster involves chemical signals. Sci. Rep. 2012;2:224. doi: 10.1038/srep00224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Bousquet F, et al. Expression of a desaturase gene, desat1, in neural and nonneural tissues separately affects perception and emission of sex pheromones in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:249–254. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1109166108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Svetec N, Ferveur JF. Social experience and pheromonal perception can change male-male interactions in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Exp. Biol. 2005;208:891–898. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Fry SN, Rohrseitz N, Straw AD, Dickinson MH. TrackFly: virtual reality for a behavioral system analysis in free-flying fruit flies. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2008;171:110–117. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.van Zweden JS, Dreier S, d’Ettorre P. Disentangling environmental and heritable nestmate recognition cues in a carpenter ant. J. Insect Physiol. 2009;55:159–164. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2008.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Rajpurohit S, et al. Adaptive dynamics of cuticular hydrocarbons in Drosophila. J. Evol. Biol. 2017;30:66–80. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Piskorski R, Trematerra P, Dorn S. Cuticular hydrocarbon profiles of codling moth larvae, Cydia pomonella (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae), reflect those of their host plant species. Biological Biol. J. Linnean Soc. 2010;101:376–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2010.01511.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Hanus R, Vrkoslav V, Hrdy I, Cvacka J, Sobotnık J. Beyond cuticular hydrocarbons: evidence of proteinaceous secretion specific to termite kings and queens. Proc. R. Soc. B. 2010;227:995–1002. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Dembeck, L. M. et al. Genetic architecture of natural variation in cuticular hydrocarbon composition in Drosophila melanogaster. eLife4, 10.7554/eLife.09861 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 64.Budaev SV. Using Principal Components and Factor Analysis in animal behaviour research: caveats and guidelines. Ethology. 2010;116:472–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01758.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Qlucore Omics Explorer. What is Principal Component Analysis?, https-/qlucore.com/an-introduction-to-pca.pdf (2015).
  • 66.Joliffe IT, Cadima J. Principal component analysis: a review and recent developments. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. A. 2016;374:20150202. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2015.0202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.XLSTAT 2019: Data Analysis and Statistical Solution for Microsoft Excel. (Addinsoft, Paris, France, 2019).
  • 68.Linn CE, Jr., Campbell MG, Roelofs WL. Male moth sensitivity to multicomponent pheromones: critical role of female-released blend in determining the functional role of components and active space of the pheromone. J. Chem. Ecol. 1986;12:659–668. doi: 10.1007/BF01012100.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Lasbleiz C, Ferveur JF, Everaerts C. Courtship behaviour of Drosophila melanogaster revisited. Anim. Behav. 2006;72:1001–1012. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.01.027. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.van der Goes van Naters W, Carlson JR. Receptors and neurons for fly odors in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2007;17:606–612. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Keesey, I. W. et al. Pathogenic bacteria enhance dispersal through alteration of Drosophila social communication. Nat. Commun. 8(1): 265. 8, 10.1038/s41467-017-00334-9 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 72.Grillet, M., Ferveur, J. F. & Everaerts, C. Behavioural elements and sensory cues involved in sexual isolation between Drosophila melanogaster strains. Royal Soc. Open Sci. 5, 10.1098/rsos.172060 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 73.François A, et al. Olfactory epithelium changes in germfree mice. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:24687. doi: 10.1038/srep24687. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental information (340KB, xlsx)

Data Availability Statement

A xlsx File containing all raw data is available as supplemental material.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES