Abstract
Myelination increases the conduction velocity in long‐range axons and is prerequisite for many brain functions. Impaired myelin regulation or impairment of myelin itself is frequently associated with deficits in learning and cognition in neurological and psychiatric disorders. However, it has not been revealed what perturbation of neural activity induced by myelin impairment causes learning deficits. Here, we measured neural activity in the motor cortex during motor learning in transgenic mice with a subtle impairment of their myelin. This deficit in myelin impaired motor learning, and was accompanied by a decrease in the amplitude of movement‐related activity and an increase in the frequency of spontaneous activity. Thalamocortical axons showed variability in axonal conduction with a large spread in the timing of postsynaptic cortical responses. Repetitive pairing of forelimb movements with optogenetic stimulation of thalamocortical axon terminals restored motor learning. Thus, myelin regulation helps to maintain the synchrony of cortical spike‐time arrivals through long‐range axons, facilitating the propagation of the information required for learning. Our results revealed the pathological neuronal circuit activity with impaired myelin and suggest the possibility that pairing of noninvasive brain stimulation with relevant behaviors may ameliorate cognitive and behavioral abnormalities in diseases with impaired myelination.
Keywords: axonal conduction, calcium imaging, motor learning, myelination, neuron–glia interactions, oligodendrocyte, optogenetics
Main Points
Myelin impairment results in temporal dispersion of axonal conduction in long‐range axons.
Low task‐related and high spontaneous activity induced by myelin impairment causes motor learning which is compensated by synchronized optical stimulation
1. INTRODUCTION
Myelin formation by oligodendrocytes surrounding axons in the central nervous system regulates conduction velocity (CV), which is a key component of fast and efficient information processing in the brain (Emery, 2010; Fields, 2008; Nave, 2010). Myelinated long‐range axons form white matter and transmit information between many cortical and subcortical gray matter regions. Human studies using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) have shown that white matter plasticity is associated with motor learning (Scholz, Klein, Behrens, & Johansen‐Berg, 2009; Zatorre, Fields, & Johansen‐Berg, 2012). In the mouse, motor learning or motor behavioral improvements require proliferation and differentiation of oligodendrocyte precursor cells (OPCs) in the white matter and motor cortex (Gibson et al., 2014; McKenzie et al., 2014; Xiao et al., 2016). A pharmacological block of action potentials in the retina inhibits proliferation of OPCs in optic nerves (Barres & Raff, 1993). Electrical stimulation of medullary pyramids (Li, Brus‐Ramer, Martin, & McDonald, 2010) and optogenetic stimulation of cortical layer 5 (L5) excitatory neurons (Gibson et al., 2014) promotes the differentiation of OPCs. Inhibition of vesicular release from neurons to mature oligodendrocytes impairs myelination, both in vitro (Wake et al., 2015; Wake, Lee, & Fields, 2011) and in vivo (Hines, Ravanelli, Schwindt, Scott, & Appel, 2015; Mensch et al., 2015). These results indicate that neuronal activity regulates myelination through OPCs and mature oligodendrocytes and thus impairment of activity‐dependent myelination or myelin itself may contribute on learning deficits. Given that distinct myelinated long‐range axons input to a common cell or circuit, appropriate regulation of the CVs is assumed to help synchronization of these distinct neural inputs (Pajevic, Basser, & Fields, 2014; Roelfsema, Engel, Konig, & Singer, 1997). An assembly of time‐locked inputs would consolidate information processing by inducing spike‐timing‐dependent plasticity (STDP) between optimally myelinated axons and their postsynaptic neurons, or in the local cortical circuit (Feldman, 2012; Nave, 2010; Veniero, Ponzo, & Koch, 2013). This is believed to promote the formation of the cortical ensemble required for behavioral learning.
Impaired regulation of myelination or impairment of myelin itself frequently accompanies learning deficits, cognitive dysfunction, and aging (Bennett & Madden, 2014; Liu et al., 2012; Makinodan, Rosen, Ito, & Corfas, 2012;McKenzie et al., 2014 ; Xiao et al., 2016). In MRI using diffusion tensor imaging, white matter changes are frequently observed in dementia and considered as one of the earliest signs of cognitive decline (Amlien & Fjell, 2014; Back et al., 2011). Genome‐wide analysis reveals differential expression of myelination‐related genes in schizophrenia (Hakak et al., 2001). Genetic deletion of myelin regulatory factor causes deficient maturation of OPCs in mature mice, resulting in impaired motor learning (McKenzie et al., 2014; Xiao et al., 2016). Social isolation impairs adult myelination in the mouse prefrontal cortex and causes behavioral deficits (Liu et al., 2012; Makinodan et al., 2012). Although it has been reported that impaired myelin regulation increases the conduction time of myelinated axons in vivo (Bando et al., 2008; Tanaka, Ikenaka, & Isa, 2006), it is poorly understood how this impairs cortical circuit properties that are relevant to learning and cognition. At a single neuron level, cortical activity is high in mice models of Alzheimer's disease (Busche et al., 2008), fragile X syndrome (Goncalves, Anstey, Golshani, & Portera‐Cailliau, 2013), and Rett syndrome (Zhang, Peterson, Beyer, Frankel, & Zhang, 2014), and in these mice models, the abnormalities in cortical neuronal activity are associated with defects in inhibition. It is unknown whether a mouse model of impaired myelination shows similar properties.
Here, we hypothesize that impaired myelin disrupts cortical neuronal activity and results in impaired motor learning. As disrupting myelination can have widespread and severe consequences, including inflammation, as seen in the debilitating demyelination disorders and leukodystrophies (Saugier‐Veber et al., 1994), an animal model with slight deficits in CV and learning, but without apparent histological or motor deficits, is suitable for testing this hypothesis. Transgenic mice with extra copies of the myelin proteolipid protein 1 gene (PLP‐tg mice [Kagawa et al., 1994]) have reduced CV in spinal ascending and descending tracts at 2 months of age, including the pyramidal tract (Tanaka et al., 2006) due to thinner myelin thickness (Tanaka et al., 2009) and abnormal processes of oligodendrocytes (Shimizu et al., 2013). PLP‐tg mice also exhibit some impairment of learning and behavior at the same age, although neither histological demyelination nor motor deficits are apparent (Tanaka et al., 2006; Tanaka et al., 2009). In this study, we used PLP‐tg mice at 2 months of age and a forelimb movement learning task to test our hypothesis. We focused on the primary motor cortex (M1), as the proliferation and differentiation of M1 OPCs is accelerated, and the activity of M1 cortical neurons dynamically changes during motor learning (Huber et al., 2012; Masamizu et al., 2014; McKenzie et al., 2014; Peters, Chen, & Komiyama, 2014; Xiao et al., 2016). We performed two‐photon Ca2+ imaging, electrical recording, and optogenetics to identify the abnormalities in M1 activity in these mice. Then, we demonstrated that the repetitive pairing of forelimb movements with synchronous activation of thalamocortical axon terminals was able to restore learning.
2. MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1. Animals and head plate fixation surgery
All experiments were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the National Institutes of Natural Sciences and the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Kobe University Graduate School of Medicine. The animals were given free access to food and water in a 12 hr light/dark cycle. For experiments with virus injections, 6‐week‐old male hemizygous PLP‐tg mice (BDF1 background) (Kagawa et al., 1994; Tanaka et al., 2009) and parvalbumin (PV)‐Cre mice (C57BL/6 background) (Tanahira et al., 2009) were anesthetized by intraperitoneal injection of ketamine (74 mg/kg) and xylazine (10 mg/kg). Prior to surgery, the skin was disinfected with 70% alcohol and the skull was exposed and cleaned, and a head plate was firmly attached with dental cement (Fuji luto BC; GC, Tokyo, Japan, Bistite II; Tokuyama Dental, Tokyo, Japan). The surface of the intact skull was subsequently coated with acrylic‐based dental adhesive resin cement (Super bond; Sun Medical, Shiga, Japan) to avoid drying, as previously described (Masamizu et al., 2014). Mice were allowed to recover for 1 day before virus injection. Up to five animals were kept together in a cage before the head plate was attached. After surgery, mice were housed singly to avoid damage to the head plate and the glass window.
2.2. AAV surgery and injection
Adeno‐associated virus (AAV) was injected 2–3 weeks before imaging. Dexamethasone sodium phosphate (1.32 mg/kg) and carprofen (6 mg/kg) were administered intraperitoneally 1 hr before surgery to prevent cerebral edema and inflammatory response, respectively. Under 1% isoflurane anesthesia, a ~2 mm diameter circular craniotomy (circle centered at ~0.2 mm anterior and ~1 mm lateral from bregma) was performed over the left primary motor cortex (M1; circle centered ~0.2 mm anterior and ~1 mm lateral from bregma), as previously described (Masamizu et al., 2014). A total of 1 μl recombinant AAV (rAAV2/1‐Syn‐GCaMP3: 2.76 × 1013 vector genomes/ml or rAAV2/1‐CAG‐flex‐GCaMP6f: 1.35 × 1013 vector genomes/ml, UPenn Vector Core) solution was injected through a glass pipette (tip diameter, 25–30 μm) connected to a picospritzer into layer 2/3 (L2/3) of the left M1. The pipette was then maintained in place for an additional 10 min. After the injection, 3% (w/v) agarose L (Nippon Gene, Tokyo, Japan) was placed over the craniotomy, a 4.5 mm diameter glass coverslip (Matsunami Glass, Osaka, Japan) was pressed onto the agarose surface, and the edges were sealed with dental cement and dental adhesive resin cement. Mice were then returned to their cages. Postoperative care included water supplemented with the antibiotics sulfadiazine (24 mg/kg) and trimethoprim (4.8 mg/kg), and daily carprofen (6 mg/kg, ip).
2.3. Self‐initiated lever‐pull task
The voluntary (self‐initiated) forelimb movement task was modified from a previously described procedure (Hira et al., 2013; Masamizu et al., 2014). Mice with head plates were habituated to the task device for 1 hr per day for 2 days before the start of the experiment. The mice were inserted into a body chamber and their head was fixed by placing the head plate between holders attached to a stage. After habituation, mice were water‐deprived in their home cages and maintained at 85% of their normal body weight throughout the experiments. Each mouse was trained to perform the voluntary right‐forelimb movement for 1 hr per day for 12 training days. When mice pulled the lever by 5 mm and held it there for 600 ms, they were rewarded with a 4 μL drop of water from a spout near their mouths. The lever was then quickly returned to the wait position by a solenoid valve. The lever could not be moved again from the wait position for 1 s. After this immobilization period, mice were allowed to pull the lever again. If a lever pull failed to be held for the full 600 ms (i.e., a failed trial), the lever was not immobilized and the mice were allowed to again pull the lever at any time. A program written in LabVIEW (National Instruments, Austin, TX) was used to control the timing of the reward, the lever return, and the immobilization of the lever, and continuously monitor the lever position. Mouse behavior and performance was monitored with an infrared video camera. Mice were allowed ad libitum access to 1 ml water following each training session. For Figure 1b–d, 20 wild‐type (WT) mice (10 with and 10 without AAV transduction) and 19 PLP‐tg mice (9 with and 10 without AAV transduction) were used.
2.4. Two‐photon imaging
Two‐photon images were acquired from the left M1 during performance of the lever‐pull task (on Days 1, 3, 5, 7, and 9) using either an FV1000‐MPE microscope (Olympus, Tokyo, Japan) with a ×25 objective (XLPlan, NA 1.05; Olympus) and a mode‐locked Ti:sapphire laser (MaTai HP; Spectra Physics, CA) at a wavelength of 910–920 nm, or an LSM 7 MP system (Zeiss, Jena, Germany) with a ×20 objective (XLPlan, NA 1.0; Zeiss) and a mode‐locked Ti:sapphire Chameleon Ultra II laser (Coherent, Santa Clara, CA) tuned to 920 nm. Fluorescence was collected with GaAsP photomultiplier tubes (Hamamatsu Photonics, Shizuoka, Japan). Laser intensity was 20–40 mW. The imaged fields were 508.93 × 508.93 μm at 150–200 μm depth below the cortical surface. Pixel size was 1.988 μm. Frame duration was 420 ms. Continuous 1,000‐frame imaging was repeated in each field for 10–30 min. The lever position and solenoid signal were recorded simultaneously with the Ca2+ imaging. Two‐photon imaging in WT/PV‐Cre and PLP‐tg/PV‐Cre mice was performed on Day 1.
2.5. Image analysis
Analyses were performed with an ImageJ plug‐in (1.37v; NIH) and scripts written in MATLAB (version 7; MathWorks, Natick, MA). Movies were corrected for focal plane displacements (X, Y, and rotation‐angle shifts) as previously described (Hira et al., 2013; Masamizu et al., 2014). Fields in which motion artifacts remained after correction and crowded fields were removed. The outlines of the cell‐based regions of interest (ROIs) were determined using a semi‐automated algorithm based on maximization of the correlation coefficients of the intensities of nearby pixels over time, along with cell sizes and shapes, and were confirmed by visual inspection (Masamizu et al., 2014). The numbers of analyzed ROIs were as follows: WT mice (five fields from five mice on each day), 97 on Day 1, 143 on Day 3, 107 on Day 5, 141 on Day 7, and 188 on Day 9; PLP‐tg mice (five fields from five mice on each day), 125 on Day 1, 203 on Day 3, 213 on Day 5, 240 on Day 7, and 199 on Day 9. The numbers of analyzed PV‐positive ROIs were as follows: WT mice, 115 (five fields from four mice); PLP‐tg mice, 145 (nine fields from five mice). The fluorescence within each ROI was averaged, and background fluorescence was subtracted from this average value. Ca2+ transients were defined according to the method in a previous report (Eto et al., 2011). All frames were divided into a sequence of segments with monotonically increasing fluorescence values that started at the initial time point and finished at the end time point. For each segment, when the increase in the fluorescence value from the initial to end time points was more than fourfold the SD of the 35th percentile of total fluorescence, one Ca2+ transient was assigned to the end time point. We then defined the task‐related Ca2+ transient as the time point existing 1.2 s before the start point of successful lever pulling behavior and 1.6 s after the start point of successful lever pulling behavior. Imaging sessions with more than three successful lever pulls were analyzed. If the time point of a Ca2+ transient was outside of the period from 1.2 s before the start point of any lever pull to 1.6 s after the start point of any lever pull, it was defined as a spontaneous Ca2+ transient.
2.6. In vivo electrophysiology
Sixteen‐channel silicone probes with 177 μm2 recording sites (NeuroNexus Technologies, Ann Arbor, MI) spaced 25 μm apart at depths of 500–600 μm below the cortical surface were used to record L2/3 and L5 neuronal activity in the left M1 in 8–9‐week‐old mice. All in vivo recordings were acquired using the Omniplex system (Plexon, Dallas, TX). Spike signals were filtered at a bandpass of 300 Hz to 8 kHz. Spikes were detected by threshold‐level crossing, generally set at 50 μV, and sorting of single units was carried out using principal component analysis in an offline sorter (Plexon). In L2/3, 68 units from 10 awake WT mice, 63 units from 10 anesthetized WT mice, 56 units from 13 awake PLP‐tg mice, and 51 units from 12 anesthetized PLP‐tg mice were recorded. In L5, 55 units from nine awake WT mice, 55 units from nine anesthetized WT mice, 58 units from nine awake PLP‐tg mice, and 58 units from nine anesthetized PLP‐tg mice were recorded. In the CNQX experiment, 10 units were recorded from four PLP‐tg mice.
2.7. Optogenetic stimulation of thalamic neurons
A total of 500 nl AAV2/1‐Syn‐ChR2 (H134R)‐EYFP (2.11 × 1013 vector genomes/ml, UPenn Vector Core) was injected into the left thalamus (1 mm posterior and 1 mm lateral from the bregma, and 3.2 mm below the pia). After 2–3 weeks of virus injection, electrophysiological experiments with optogenetic stimulation were performed using planar electrodes under 0.8–1% isoflurane anesthesia to reduce behavioral effects in the awake state. For optogenetic stimulation of thalamic afferents in the cortex, the fiber‐optic cable was placed close to the cortical surface that had been exposed by the craniotomy. For optogenetic stimulation of the thalamus, a fiber‐optic cable (~500 μm diameter) was acutely implanted at the same coordinates as the thalamic viral injection but at a depth of 2.2 mm. Then, 20 × 20 ms pulses of a 470 nm LED (Lumencor, Beaverton, OR) were delivered at 1 Hz to elicit responses. The power at the fiber‐optic tip was approximately 4 mW. Units that showed a stable (at least one) response to every optogenetic stimulation were recorded, and spikes that occurred within 50 ms after the onset of the stimulation were analyzed. Antidromic spikes were defined according to a previous report (Ciocchi, Passecker, Malagon‐Vina, Mikus, & Klausberger, 2015), with slight modification (low spike jitter [<0.3 ms] and high fidelity [>50%] of light‐induced spikes). One hundred and forty‐one antidromic spikes from eight WT mice and 130 antidromic spikes from seven PLP‐tg mice were analyzed. To estimate the temporal dispersion of antidromic spike latency, we calculated the SD and coefficient of variation using antidromic spikes that were detected simultaneously from more than two different channels in an experimental recording (20 × 20 ms pulses of a 470 nm LED stimulation at 1 Hz; WT, 136 antidromic spikes from 18 recordings; PLP‐tg, 122 antidromic spikes from 19 recordings). Cortical spikes were analyzed from 99 units from seven WT mice and 112 units from six PLP‐tg mice for optogenetic stimulation of the thalamocortical axons, and 95 units from six WT mice and 91 units from seven PLP‐tg mice for optogenetic stimulation of the thalamus.
For the experiment on optogenetic stimulation of thalamic axons in M1 during motor learning, a fiber‐optic cable was surgically implanted and secured over the left M1 in 5 WT mice or 16 PLP‐tg mice, with the lever‐pull task being commenced a week after the implantation. Blue light stimulation was provided by the same 470 nm LED in trains of 20 × 20 ms pulses delivered at 10 Hz. As a control, 5 WT mice or 10 PLP‐tg mice injected with AAV2/1‐Syn‐GFP (1.18 × 1013 vector genomes/ml, UPenn Vector Core) received identical light stimulation during the lever‐pull task. After 12 day training with the optogenetic stimulation, 10 of 16 PLP‐tg mice with ChR2 expression further performed the task on days 13 and 14, without the optogenetic stimulation. The success rate in either early or late sessions was not different between PLP‐tg mice and PLP‐tg mice with GFP and the light stimulation (F 1, 54 = 1.27, p = .95 [early], p = .72 [late], two‐way ANOVA followed by Tukey's test). For the experiment on optogenetic stimulation of thalamic neurons during motor learning, a fiber‐optic cable was implanted into the left motor thalamus in five PLP‐tg mice. As a control, five PLP‐tg mice injected with AAV2/1‐Syn‐GFP were used.
2.8. Semi‐quantitative RT‐PCR
For analysis of mRNA expression, brains were removed from mice immediately after the final training session (Day 12), and cortical tissue from left and right coronal slices including M1 (1.5–0 mm from the bregma) was collected with a 1.0 mm diameter biopsy needle and frozen. RNA was extracted from the cortical tissue using TRIzol reagent (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA). Total RNA (2 μg) was reverse‐transcribed with Superscript II and oligo‐dT, and PCR was performed on a Roche Lightcycler (Roche, Minneapolis, MN) with FastStart DNA Master SYBR Green 1 PCR reaction mix. Relative expression levels in samples from the left and right hemispheres were quantified as previously described (Lee, Cohen, Tendi, Farrer, GH DEV, Becker KG, & Fields RD., 2004). Primer sequences were as follows: a housekeeping gene, glyceraldehyde phosphate dehydrogenase (GAPDH), 5‐AATGCATCCTGCACCACCAAC‐3', 5'‐TGGATGCAGGGATGATGTTCTG‐3'; MBP, 5'‐CGATTGGGTGTCACTCTGAAA‐3', 5'‐CCCAGCAGAGAATGAACACAA‐3'. For each tissue sample, the MBP mRNA expression level was normalized to that of GAPDH mRNA. For Figure 2d, 14 WT mice (six with and eight without training) and 15 PLP‐tg mice (six with and nine without training) were used.
2.9. Immunohistochemistry
After cardiac perfusion and overnight fixation with 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffer (PB, pH 7.4), fixed brains were equilibrated in 30% sucrose solution in PB and cut into 20 μm‐thick sections with a microtome (Leica Microsystems, Wetzlar, Germany). Sections were incubated overnight with anti‐PV antibody (rabbit, Abcam ab 11,427; 1:1,000) in 10% normal goat serum/phosphate buffered saline with 0.3% Triton‐X (pH 7.4) at 4°C, followed by 1 hr incubation with secondary antibody (Alexa Fluor 488 or 594 conjugated donkey anti‐rabbit, Invitrogen). For 5‐ethynyl‐2′‐deoxyuridine (EdU) labeling experiment, EdU was intraperitoneally administered (25 mg/kg) (Chehrehasa, Meedeniya, Dwyer, Abrahamsen, & Mackay‐Sim, 2009) for 10 days before motor learning. Brain samples were removed from mice immediately after the final training session (Day 12). Fixed brain sections (20 μm) were incubated with anti‐adenomatous polyposis coli (clone CC1) antibody (mouse, Calbiochem OP80; 1:500) followed by detection of EdU using the Alexa Fluor 555 Click‐iT detection kit (Invitrogen). EdU and CC1 stained signals in the both motor cortex and corpse callosum were taken on an LSM 700 microscope (Zeiss) with a ×10 water‐immersion objective (EC Plan‐Neofluor, 0.3 NA; Zeiss) and then counted (motor cortex; 12 fields in four WT mice and 12 fields from four PLP‐tg, subcortical white matter; nine fields in four WT mice and nine fields from four PLP‐tg mice).
2.10. Statistics
All data are presented as the mean ± SEM. Unpaired, paired, and one‐sample t tests, Wilcoxon rank sum tests, ANOVA followed by Tukey's post hoc test and Pearson's correlation tests were used to test for statistical significance. The variances within each group of data were estimated. For Ca2+ imaging and electrophysiology data, the D'Agostino and Pearson normality test was first performed. Then, parametric or nonparametric tests were used to test for significance, depending on the data distribution.
3. RESULTS
3.1. PLP‐tg mice show impairments of learning in a lever‐pull task
We examined whether 2‐month‐old PLP‐tg mice show impaired learning of a motor task: a self‐initiated lever‐pull task using the right forelimb (Hira et al., 2013; Masamizu et al., 2014) (Figure 1a). In this task, mice had to pull the lever and hold it there for 600 ms to receive a drop of water. Mice performed this task for 60 min per session (one session per day) during 12 sessions. First, we compared the time course of the 60 min‐averaged task performance between wild‐type (WT) and PLP‐tg mice. In the early training stage (Days 1–4), there was no difference between WT littermates and PLP‐tg mice in either the success rate or the number of successful lever pulls (Figure 1b,c). In addition, the total number of lever‐pull trials did not differ (Figure 1b,c). Thus, PLP‐tg mice had the ability to move the forelimb, and recognized the association between the action (the lever‐pull movement) and outcome (water reward). This is consistent with the lack of deficits exhibited by 2‐month‐old PLP‐tg mice in other motor tests (wire hang, grip strength, and rotarod tests) (Tanaka et al., 2009). After sessions 1–4, the success rate of lever‐pull trials continued to increase in both strains of mice (Figure 1b). However, in the late training stage (Days 9–12), the success rate and success number were lower in PLP‐tg mice than in WT mice, although the total number of lever‐pull trials was not different between WT and PLP‐tg mice (Figure 1b,c). Even when we used the behavioral data during the initial 10 min of the session, when the mice were thought to be more highly motivated than during the following 50 min period, the success rate and success number in the late stage were lower in PLP‐tg mice than in WT mice (Figure 1d). This indicates that although the motivation to receive the reward was not apparently different between WT and PLP‐tg mice, the late‐stage improvement in motor performance was impaired in PLP‐tg mice.
It is possible that this impairment could be caused by enhanced anxiety (Tanaka et al., 2009); when the experiment was repeated with mice that had been habituated to the chamber for 1 week prior to the start of training, the difference in the success number in the late stage disappeared, but the difference in the success rate remained (Figure 1e). In the early sessions, the success rate or the number of successful trials did not differ (Figure 1e). Even when the training period was extended, the success rate in PLP‐tg mice did not reach the level of that achieved by WT mice (Figure 1f). Furthermore, the number of successful or total lever pulls was not different between these two groups (Figure 1f). These results indicate that the low success rate in the late training stage in PLP‐tg mice was not caused by high anxiety under the task condition, and neither was it a direct result of their previously characterized abnormal behaviors.
Although the 60 min or initial 10 min‐averaged task performance in the early sessions was not different between WT and PLP‐tg mice (Figure 1b–d), changes in the performance within a session could still be different. If the memory for the task acquired in a session was maintained, the success rate should be high at the beginning of the next session (Costa, Cohen, & Nicolelis, 2004; Yin et al., 2009) and gradually decrease within the session as the water consumption increased. We divided the 60 min period into two 30 min periods, and compared the success rate between the initial 30 min period and the last 30 min periods. In WT mice, from session 4, the initial 30 min period showed a higher success rate than the last 30 min period (Supporting Information Figure S1). By contrast, in PLP‐tg mice, the success rate of the initial 30 min was higher than that of the last 30 min period after session 7 (Supporting Information Figure S2). This result suggests that PLP‐tg mice were relatively slow in retaining the motor memory from an earlier session in comparison with the WT mice.
In the following experiments, we used the success rate (60 min‐averaged) as a robust behavioral indicator for the degree of motor learning, and examined what abnormal cellular functioning in the brain was associated with the low success rate in PLP‐tg mice.
3.2. Increase in myelin‐related mRNA correlates with improvement of the task performance in WT mice
To determine whether lever‐pull learning is related with the increase in myelination in gray and white matter of the left M1, as observed in other motor learning tasks (McKenzie et al., 2014; Sampaio‐Baptista et al., 2013; Xiao et al., 2016), we first examined production of new myelinating oligodendrocytes associated with lever‐pull learning in the gray and white matter of the left and right M1using immunohistochemistry. PLP‐tg mice showed a lower ratio of the number of density (cells per mm2) of EdU+ CC1+ cells in both left gray and white matter of M1 relative to right gray and white matter of M1 compared with those in WT (Figure 2a–c). This is consistent with the deficit of activity‐dependent myelination in PLP‐tg mice. Next, we measured the expression level of myelin basic protein (MBP) mRNA in trained and untrained WT mice because the thickness of myelin is mediated by MBP and correlates with MBP gene expression (Martini & Schachner, 1997; Shine, Readhead, Popko, Hood, & Sidman, 1992). Trained WT mice showed a higher ratio of MBP mRNA in the left M1 relative to the right M1 in comparison with untrained WT mice, whereas the ratio did not differ between trained and untrained PLP‐tg mice (Figure 2d). The change in MBP mRNA positively correlated with the late training stage success rate in WT mice (p = .049, r = 0.813; Figure 2e), whereas in PLP‐tg mice, there was not a significant association between the change in MBP mRNA and the late training stage success rate (p = .37, r = −0.451; Figure 2e). These results suggest that the increase in new myelinating oligodendrocytes (EdU+ CC1+) production and MBP mRNA expression were critical for the normal motor learning which was impaired in PLP‐tg mice.
3.3. Abnormalities of spontaneous cortical activity and task‐related activity in PLP‐tg mice
We examined how the deficits in the regulation of myelination in PLP‐tg mice impaired the neural activity required for motor learning. We tested two simple possibilities in M1 neurons: whether PLP‐tg mice showed lower task‐related activity than WT mice, which would directly impair the motor execution, and whether PLP‐tg mice showed higher spontaneous activity than WT mice, which would decrease the signal‐to‐noise ratio in the learning‐relevant circuits. First, in the early and late training sessions, we conducted two‐photon imaging of layer 2/3 (L2/3) neurons in M1, which expressed GCaMP3 (Tian et al., 2009) (Figure 3a). Two to 3 weeks before the training started, we injected an adeno‐associated virus (AAV) carrying the GCaMP3 gene into the left M1. We used synapsin I promotor so that GCaMP3 was mainly expressed in excitatory neurons (Masamizu et al., 2014; O'Connor, Peron, Huber, & Svoboda, 2010). Many GCaMP3‐expressing neurons showed Ca2+ transients, both during the lever‐pull movement and outside of the movement (Figure 3b). We divided each imaging session into three periods: a task‐related period that started 1.2 s before and ended 1.6 s after the start point of successful lever pulling behavior, a spontaneous period that was outside of the period from 1.2 s before and ending 1.6 s after the start point of any lever pull, and a third “other” period, which included failed trials (Figure 3c). We then calculated the frequency and amplitude of Ca2+ transients in the task‐related and spontaneous periods (Figure 3c; see more details in Materials and Methods). There was no difference between WT and PLP‐tg mice in the frequency of task‐related Ca2+ transients in either the early or the late training stage. By contrast, in both training stages, the amplitude of task‐related Ca2+ transients was smaller in PLP‐tg mice than in WT mice (Figure 3d,e and Table 1; see Materials and Methods). The frequency of spontaneous Ca2+ transients was significantly larger in PLP‐tg mice than in WT mice in both training stages, while the amplitude was significantly smaller (Figure 3d,e and Table 1). When the task‐related period was set to be longer or shorter than that stated above, the results were similar (Table 1). Thus, in both early and late sessions, PLP‐tg mice showed lower task‐related Ca2+ transients, and a higher frequency and smaller amplitude of spontaneous Ca2+ transients than WT mice.
Table 1.
Task‐related Ca2+ transient (early stage) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Time window | 1 | 2 | ||||
Mouse, p value | WT | PLP‐tg | p value | WT | PLP‐tg | p value |
Frequency (Hz) | 0.075 ± 0.005 | 0.099 ± 0.008 | .49 | 0.069 ± 0.004 | 0.097 ± 0.008 | .056 |
Amplitude (ΔF/F) | 1.503 ± 0.091 | 0.866 ± 0.044 | 3.0 × 10−9 | 1.540 ± 0.091 | 0.886 ± 0.043 | 9.6 × 10−10 |
Spontaneous Ca2+ transient (early stage) | ||||||
Time window | 1 | 2 | ||||
Mouse, p value | WT | PLP‐tg | p value | WT | PLP‐tg | p value |
Frequency (Hz) | 0.027 ± 0.002 | 0.031 ± 0.002 | .049 | 0.026 ± 0.002 | 0.030 ± 0.002 | .034 |
Amplitude (ΔF/F) | 1.338 ± 0.074 | 0.960 ± 0.041 | 1.3 × 10−5 | 1.304 ± 0.079 | 0.951 ± 0.040 | 9.7 × 10−5 |
Task‐related Ca2+ transient (late stage) | ||||||
Time window | 1 | 2 | ||||
Mouse, p value | WT | PLP‐tg | p value | WT | PLP‐tg | p value |
Frequency (Hz) | 0.085 ± 0.008 | 0.070 ± 0.006 | .075 | 0.075 ± 0.006 | 0.069 ± 0.006 | .18 |
Amplitude (ΔF/F) | 1.107 ± 0.050 | 0.718 ± 0.043 | 1.0 × 10−10 | 1.120 ± 0.056 | 0.703 ± 0.038 | 2.3 × 10−9 |
Spontaneous Ca2+ transient (late stage) | ||||||
Time window | 1 | 2 | ||||
Mouse, p value | WT | PLP‐tg | p value | WT | PLP‐tg | p value |
Frequency (Hz) | 0.025 ± 0.001 | 0.029 ± 0.001 | 7.1 × 10−3 | 0.022 ± 0.001 | 0.027 ± 0.001 | 1.9 × 10−4 |
Amplitude (ΔF/F) | 1.096 ± 0.045 | 0.887 ± 0.040 | 5.4 × 10−4 | 1.080 ± 0.044 | 0.885 ± 0.040 | 1.4 × 10−3 |
Task‐related Ca2+ transient (PV neurons) | ||||||
Time window | 1 | 2 | ||||
Mouse, p value | WT | PLP‐tg | p value | WT | PLP‐tg | p value |
Frequency (Hz) | 0.409 ± 0.016 | 0.372 ± 0.014 | .054 | 0.362 ± 0.012 | 0.341 ± 0.012 | .11 |
Amplitude (ΔF/F) | 2.880 ± 0.235 | 2.940 ± 0.188 | .84 | 2.825 ± 0.221 | 2.945 ± 0.195 | .68 |
Spontaneous Ca2+ transient (PV neurons) | ||||||
Time window | 1 | 2 | ||||
Mouse, p value | WT | PLP‐tg | p value | WT | PLP‐tg | p value |
Frequency (Hz) | 0.036 ± 0.002 | 0.044 ± 0.002 | .054 | 0.033 ± 0.002 | 0.041 ± 0.001 | .071 |
Amplitude (ΔF/F) | 1.790 ± 0.109 | 1.912 ± 0.109 | .43 | 1.728 ± 0.106 | 1.859 ± 0.107 | .39 |
To validate the statistical significance of Figure 3d,e and Figure 5c, two different time windows were used to redefine the task‐related Ca2+ transients and spontaneous Ca2+ transients. Time window 1 started 0.8 s before and ended 1.2 s after the start of successful lever‐pull trials (short). Time window 2 started 1.2 s before and ended 2 s after the start of successful lever‐pull trials (long). All data are presented as the mean ± SEM. Unpaired t tests and Wilcoxon rank sum tests were used to test for statistical significance.
We confirmed the increased spontaneous firing in awake PLP‐tg mice without any training by performing multi‐unit recordings from L2/3 and L5 in M1 (Figure 4a–c). When excitatory synaptic transmission was blocked, spontaneous firing in PLP‐tg mice reduced (Figure 4d,e). This was similar to the result obtained in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease (Busche et al., 2008). Anesthesia substantially reduced the frequency of spontaneous firing and abolished the difference between WT and PLP‐tg mice (Figure 4f,g). The frequency of spontaneous Ca2+ transients on each day of training was inversely correlated with the change in success rate from that day to the next, with this being the case in any WT and PLP‐tg mice, which was statistically significant (Figure 3f). This implies that day‐by‐day fluctuations in spontaneous activity could affect the motor learning performance at each day.
3.4. PLP‐tg mice had no defects in spontaneous activity of inhibitory neurons
The increased spontaneous activity in mouse models of Alzheimer's disease (Busche et al., 2008) and fragile X syndrome (Goncalves et al., 2013) is related to defects in inhibition, and is detected even in the anesthetized state. Thus, we tested whether the origin of the increased spontaneous activity in PLP‐tg mice was related to the abnormality in inhibitory neurons. We performed two‐photon Ca2+ imaging of L2/3 PV‐positive neurons on Day 1 of training (Figure 5a,b). We crossbred PLP‐tg with PV‐Cre mice (Tanahira et al., 2009), and injected an AAV coding GCaMP6f (Chen et al., 2013), which expressed in a Cre‐dependent manner. The frequency and amplitude of the spontaneous Ca2+ transients of these neurons were not different between WT and PLP‐tg mice (Figure 5c and Table 1). The frequency and amplitude of task‐related Ca2+ transients also showed no differences between these two groups (Figure 5c and Table 1). These results indicate that the learning impairment in PLP‐tg mice is not mainly caused by a defect in the activity of inhibitory neurons.
In the following experiments, we tested whether changes in the axonal conduction of myelinated long‐range axons in PLP‐tg mice were associated with the abnormal M1 neuronal activity and low success rate.
3.5. Temporal dispersion of axonal conduction in PLP‐tg mice
First, we tested whether the axonal conduction of myelinated long‐range axons projecting to M1 varied in PLP‐tg mice. Thalamic neurons, including motor thalamic neurons, strongly innervate M1, and regulate cortical activity (Bosch‐Bouju, Hyland, & Parr‐Brownlie, 2013; Hooks et al., 2013); moreover, regulation of CV in myelinated thalamocortical axons during development ensures that inputs arrive synchronously (Salami, Itami, Tsumoto, & Kimura, 2003). Therefore, we measured the conduction time of antidromic spikes of thalamic axons projecting to M1 in PLP‐tg mice. We injected an AAV carrying the channelrhodopsin‐2 (ChR2) gene into the thalamus, including the motor thalamus (Figure 6a). We then recorded antidromically evoked spikes in the thalamus, while optogenetically stimulating the thalamic axons in M1 with blue light at 1 Hz (Figure 6a,b). In both WT and PLP‐tg mice, spikes with very short jitter (~0.15 ms) and high fidelity (~80%) were evoked (Figure 6c,d). These parameters did not differ between WT and PLP‐tg mice (Figure 6d). Thus, optogenetic activation reliably evoked antidromic spikes in the same thalamic neurons in both mice. In the antidromic spikes, the spike latency from the onset of the stimulation was longer in PLP‐tg mice than in WT mice (Figure 6e). Furthermore, the temporal dispersion of the antidromic spike latency across different recording channels in the same animal was broader in PLP‐tg mice than in WT mice (Figure 6f). These results demonstrate that in PLP‐tg mice the axonal conduction in thalamocortical axons was not only reduced but also temporally spread.
3.6. Stimulation of thalamic neurons evoked asynchronous cortical activity in PLP‐tg mice
Second, we measured M1 activity responding to the optogenetic stimulation of the thalamic axons in M1 (Figure 7a). When these were stimulated at 1 Hz, spikes in L2/3 of M1 were evoked in similar patterns across stimuli (Figure 7a–c). The durations of evoked spike volleys, which were defined as the average durations between the first and last evoked spikes in individual stimuli, were no different between WT and PLP‐tg mice (Figure 7d). This indicates that neither the timing to evoke the axonal firing after the optogenetic stimulation, nor the process from the thalamocortical synaptic transmission to the firing of cortical neurons was different between the WT and PLP‐tg mice.
Third, we measured the M1 activity responding to the optogenetic stimulation of the somata of the thalamic neurons (Figure 7a). In this case, the duration of the spike volleys evoked in M1 was significantly longer in PLP‐tg mice than in WT mice (Figure 7a–c,e). This indicates that in PLP‐tg mice the long‐range thalamocortical axons with a wide range of axonal conduction substantially spread the timings of presynaptic neurotransmitter release and postsynaptic firing in M1. In addition, the number of spikes evoked by optogenetic stimulation of the thalamus was significantly larger in PLP‐tg mice than in WT mice (Figure 7f). This implies that the prolonged postsynaptic spike volleys could be a factor increasing the spontaneous activity.
3.7. Synchronous stimulation of thalamic afferents rescues the learning performance in PLP‐tg mice
If asynchronous activity in thalamocortical axons impaired the late‐stage success rate in PLP‐tg mice, artificial synchronous activity in thalamocortical axons during the lever‐pull movement would raise the decreased task‐related activity, and could restore the success rate. The electrical stimulation of thalamocortical axons at 10 Hz augments motor cortical activity (Castro‐Alamancos, 2013; Miall et al., 1998). ChR2 stimulation of thalamocortical axons at 5 or 10 Hz enhances the recovery of somatosensory cortical circuit function and forepaw sensorimotor abilities after stroke (Tennant, Taylor, White, & Brown, 2017). In our experimental condition, 10 Hz stimulation of thalamocortical axons reliably induced the cortical response in WT mice (the average number of M1 unit responses to the stimulation was 1.93 ± 0.08, n = 31 units from two mice). Therefore, for the mice with ChR2 or GFP expression in thalamic neurons, repetitive optogenetic stimulation at 10 Hz (Castro‐Alamancos, 2013; Miall et al., 1998) was applied to thalamocortical axons in M1 immediately after the onset of each lever pull in each training session (Figure 8a). In the early training stage, the success rate was not different between ChR2‐expressing and GFP‐expressing PLP‐tg mice. However, in the late stage, ChR2‐expressing PLP‐tg mice showed slightly but significantly higher lever‐pull success rate than GFP‐expressing mice (Figure 8b,c). In addition, the success rate in both early and late stage was not different between ChR2‐expressing and GFP‐expressing WT mice that already received synchronized inputs from thalamus before thalamocortical axons stimulation (Figure 8d,e). Thus, the formation of synchronous activity in thalamocortical axons during learning‐relevant movements was able to partially restore the success rate of PLP‐tg mice, not WT mice, in the late training stage, indicating that this contribution was only present in myelin deficient mice.
Next, to assess whether activation of thalamic activity also contributes to behavior improvement, we trained the PLP‐tg mice having ChR2 or GFP expression in the thalamus with the stimulation of thalamic cell bodies. In the early training stage, the success rate was not different between ChR2‐expressing and GFP‐expressing mice. However, in the late stage, ChR2‐expressing mice showed significantly higher success rates than GFP‐expressing mice (Figure 8f,g). Optogenetic stimulation of thalamic cell bodies with lever pulling might improve thalamic activity by compensating dispersed inputs from basal ganglia and cerebellum that are crucial for motor learning (Bosch‐Bouju et al., 2013; Tanaka et al., 2018). Furthermore, in ChR2‐expressing mice, from session 5 onward, the initial 30 min period showed a higher success rate than the last 30 min periods (Supporting Information Figure S3), in contrast, in GFP‐expressing mice, the success rate of the initial 30 min was higher than that of the last 30 min period after session 9 (Supporting Information Figure S4). Therefore, the facilitation of thalamic activity during learning‐relevant movements was also able to restore the success rate in PLP‐tg mice.
Finally, we determined whether the optogenetic stimulation of thalamocortical axons was still helpful for the lever‐pull movement after 12 day training in PLP‐tg mice. A subset of the PLP‐tg mice that had performed the task with the stimulation throughout training Days 1–12 performed the task without the stimulation on Days 13 and 14. The mean success rate over Days 11 and 12 was not different from that over Days 13 and 14 (Figure 8h). This result suggests that the synchronous input of thalamocortical axons was critical to form the M1 neuronal activity required for skill acquisition, and that after training this M1 activity might be induced without the assistance of the synchronous input of thalamocortical axons.
4. DISCUSSION
This study demonstrates that impaired myelination increases axonal conduction variability in long‐range axons, thereby leading to a greater spread in the timing of synaptic transmission and postsynaptic activity, and that this abnormality can be the origin of other abnormalities of the neural circuits, such as weak task‐related activity and high basal activity. The weak task‐related activity is presumably caused by a decrease in the coincident long‐range synaptic inputs required for induction of strong burst firing in postsynaptic neurons (Figure 9). The abnormally high basal activity observed in PLP‐tg mice may be caused by occasionally occurring synchronous events of temporally‐spread synaptic transmission from many distant areas. These abnormalities would weaken the propagation of information and STDP within cortical networks (Moldakarimov, Bazhenov, & Sejnowski, 2015; Pajevic et al., 2014) and may deprive neurons of the ability to regulate activity‐dependent myelination in respect to white matter plasticity (Hines et al., 2015; Mensch et al., 2015; Wake et al., 2011; Wake et al., 2015). During development, myelination changes the CV of thalamocortical pathways to increase synchronicity of the cortical activity, which may facilitate cortical information processing (Kimura & Itami, 2009; Salami et al., 2003). It should be clarified in the future whether and to what extent the CVs of thalamocortical axons become increasingly synchronized, and their synapses or downstream synapses show STDP during learning processes.
Abnormally high basal activity in the cerebral cortex is also a feature in Alzheimer's disease (Busche et al., 2008) and Fragile X syndrome (Goncalves et al., 2013) mouse models. PLP‐tg mice at 2 months of age show behavioral abnormalities related to schizophrenia (Tanaka et al., 2009). Taken together, abnormally high basal activity may be a common phenotype in neurological and psychiatric disorders. Even in WT mice, the low basal activity on a given day tended to be correlated with the increase in the success rate on that day. In WT mice, the basal activity was lower than in PLP‐tg mice and varied daily, and therefore this effect may not be distinguished over training sessions. In the mouse models of Alzheimer disease (Busche et al., 2008) and Fragile X syndrome (Goncalves et al., 2013), high basal activity was also detected in the anesthetized state, whereas the basal activity was similar between anesthetized WT and PLP‐tg mice. In contrast to the former mouse models, PLP‐tg mice did not show any apparent defect in inhibition. Thus, the underlying mechanism was probably different. If the abnormal basal activity generally precedes the behavioral deficits, it may be a preclinical phenotype.
The motor impairment in PLP‐tg mice was not clearly detected in the early training stage. However, during the early training stage, the success rate of initial 30 min period was higher compared to that of the last 30 min period in PLP‐tg mice than in WT mice. This suggests that the improvement in the task performance in an early session was barely retained by the time of the next session. Motor learning can be divided into two stages: associative learning in the early stage and skill learning in the late stage (Balleine & O'Doherty, 2010; Doyon & Benali, 2005; Makino, Hwang, Hedrick, & Komiyama, 2016). In the late training stage, which may correspond to the skill learning stage, the coordinated movement of forelimb and other body parts may be acquired by long‐range signal transmission including sensory inputs from the periphery and cerebellum to the cerebral cortex through the thalamus (Bosch‐Bouju et al., 2013; Shadmehr & Krakauer, 2008). In such a case, the synchronous input of myelinated long‐range axons connecting distant areas, and information processes with a high signal‐to‐noise ratio, may be important (Gibson et al., 2014; McKenzie et al., 2014; Xiao et al., 2016). In contrast to our results, the effect of impaired myelin regulation was detected from the beginning of motor training in other studies (McKenzie et al., 2014; Xiao et al., 2016). These assessed motor learning ability using a running wheel with irregularly spaced rungs, in which the mice had to coordinate many parts of the body to avoid missing their step, even in the first session. This task may have a much greater requirement for synchronous long‐range axonal transmission than the task we used, and the effect of impaired myelination may be apparent and detectable from the first to the final session.
We demonstrated that motor learning could be restored by optogenetically stimulating a population of thalamocortical axons and thus overcoming asynchrony in long‐range synaptic inputs. However, the extent of the rescue appeared partial because the late training stage success rate in this experiment (Figure 8c) was lower than that in the normal training schedule in WT mice (Figure 1c). The reason why the restoration was partial may be that other long‐range axons such as the pyramidal tracts were involved in the motor learning. In addition, the duration of the optogenetic assistance was limited to the lever‐pull movement, so the effect of the high frequency of basal activity remained in the late training stage. It is perhaps surprising that facilitating the synchronization of only one myelinated pathway was effective in restoring learning. This suggests that the synchronicity of the spike‐time arrival in the thalamocortical pathway has a great influence on learning. In addition, activation of thalamic cell bodies also contributes to improvement of motor learning. Inputs from basal ganglia and cerebellum mediating motor thalamus are crucial for motor learning (Bosch‐Bouju et al., 2013; Tanaka et al., 2018). Therefore, optogenetic stimulation of thalamic cell bodies with lever pulling might restore the success rate in PLP‐tg mice by either or both of increase in thalamic activity itself and/or partial compensation of dispersed inputs from basal ganglia and cerebellum to motor nucleus of thalamus. Furthermore, a subset of PLP‐tg mice (10 of 16 PLP‐tg mice) with the optogenetic assistance during training Days 1–12 performed the task with a similar success rate without the stimulation on Days 13 and 14. One interpretation of this result might be that cortical neurons in the descending pathway of the thalamocortical inputs are able to maintain the partially rescued performance once it has formed. Another interpretation of this result might be that an antidromic activity in thalamo‐cortical axons induced by optogenetic stimulation contributes to activity‐dependent myelination that promotes synchrony of spike time arrival. In psychiatric diseases such as schizophrenia, subtle impairment of myelin regulation and thalamocortical dysrhythmia are observed (Hakak et al., 2001; Nave & Ehrenreich, 2014; Schulman et al., 2011). In the much larger human brain, myelin regulation of cortico‐cortical long‐range axons such as transcallosal cortical projections is also crucial (Nave, 2010), and is necessary for cognition and learning (Nave & Ehrenreich, 2014; Whitford, Ford, Mathalon, Kubicki, & Shenton, 2012). If a population of cortically projecting axons or a subset of cortical neurons can be synchronously stimulated with transcranial magnetic stimulation during relevant behaviors and sensory inputs (Frantseva et al., 2008; Thabit et al., 2010; Zeller et al., 2010), it might be possible to improve cognitive and behavioral abnormalities in the early stages of diseases featuring impaired myelination.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors have no financial conflict of interest.
AUTHORS CONTRIBUTIONS
D.K., H.W., N.M., J.N., and M.M. designed the research. D.K., H.W., P.R.L., Y.T., R. O., Y. T. and S. S. performed the experiments with advice from Y.H.T., Y.M., A.J.M., N.T., and K.I. R.H. developed the task apparatus, and Y.R.T. developed the scripts for motion correction and ROI extraction. D.K. and H.W. analyzed the data. D.K., H.W., R.D.F., and M.M. wrote the manuscript with comments from all other authors.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank V. Jayaraman, R. Kerr, D. Kim, L. Looger, and K. Svoboda of the GENIE Project at Janelia Farm Research Campus (HHMI) for providing rAAV2/1‐CAG‐flex‐GCaMP6f and K. Deisseroth at Stanforde University for providing rAAV2/1‐Syn‐ChR2 (H134R)‐EYFP. rAAV2/1‐Syn‐GCaMP3 was synthesized and purified by Takashi Okada (Nippon Medical School, Japan) with the AAV2/1 helper plasmid (J.M. Wilson, University of Pennsylvania). This work was supported by Grants‐in‐Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas (15H01300, 16H01346, 17H05747, 19H04753, 19H05219, and 25110732 to H.W.; 22115005 and 17H06309 to M.M.); by Grants‐in‐Aid for Young Scientists (A) (26710004 to H.W.); Grant‐in‐Aid for Scientific Research (B) (18H02598 to H.W.); by Grants‐in‐Aid for Scientific Research (23300148 to M.M.) from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology (MEXT), Japan; by AMED (JP17dm0107053 to M.M.) and by JST CREST Grant Number JPMJCR1755, Japan.
Kato D, Wake H, Lee PR, et al. Motor learning requires myelination to reduce asynchrony and spontaneity in neural activity. Glia. 2020;68:193–210. 10.1002/glia.23713
Funding information Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development, Grant/Award Number: JP17dm0107053; Japan Science and Technology Agency, Grant/Award Number: JPMJCR1755; Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, Grant/Award Numbers: 18H02598, Grants‐in‐Aid for Scientific Research, Grants‐in‐Aid for Scientific Research on Innovativ, Grants‐in‐Aid for Young Scientists (A)
Contributor Information
Hiroaki Wake, Email: hirowake@med.kobe-u.ac.jp.
Masanori Matsuzaki, Email: mzakim@m.u-tokyo.ac.jp.
REFERENCES
- Amlien, I. K. , & Fjell, A. M. (2014). Diffusion tensor imaging of white matter degeneration in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neuroscience, 276, 206–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Back, S. A. , Kroenke, C. D. , Sherman, L. S. , Lawrence, G. , Gong, X. , Taber, E. N. , … Montine, T. J. (2011). White matter lesions defined by diffusion tensor imaging in older adults. Annals of Neurology, 70, 465–476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine, B. W. , & O'Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control: Corticostriatal determinants of goal‐directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 48–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bando, Y. , Takakusaki, K. , Ito, S. , Terayama, R. , Kashiwayanagi, M. , & Yoshida, S. (2008). Differential changes in axonal conduction following CNS demyelination in two mouse models. European Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 1731–1742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barres, B. A. , & Raff, M. C. (1993). Proliferation of oligodendrocyte precursor cells depends on electrical activity in axons. Nature, 361, 258–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett, I. J. , & Madden, D. J. (2014). Disconnected aging: Cerebral white matter integrity and age‐related differences in cognition. Neuroscience, 276, 187–205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bosch‐Bouju, C. , Hyland, B. I. , & Parr‐Brownlie, L. C. (2013). Motor thalamus integration of cortical, cerebellar and basal ganglia information: Implications for normal and parkinsonian conditions. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 7, 163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Busche, M. A. , Eichhoff, G. , Adelsberger, H. , Abramowski, D. , Wiederhold, K. H. , Haass, C. , … Garaschuk, O. (2008). Clusters of hyperactive neurons near amyloid plaques in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Science, 321, 1686–1689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castro‐Alamancos, M. A. (2013). The motor cortex: A network tuned to 7‐14 Hz. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, 7, 21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chehrehasa, F. , Meedeniya, A. C. , Dwyer, P. , Abrahamsen, G. , & Mackay‐Sim, A. (2009). EdU, a new thymidine analogue for labelling proliferating cells in the nervous system. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 177, 122–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen, T. W. , Wardill, T. J. , Sun, Y. , Pulver, S. R. , Renninger, S. L. , Baohan, A. , … Kim, D. S. (2013). Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature, 499, 295–300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciocchi, S. , Passecker, J. , Malagon‐Vina, H. , Mikus, N. , & Klausberger, T. (2015). Brain computation. Selective information routing by ventral hippocampal CA1 projection neurons. Science, 348, 560–563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa, R. M. , Cohen, D. , & Nicolelis, M. A. (2004). Differential corticostriatal plasticity during fast and slow motor skill learning in mice. Current Biology, 14, 1124–1134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doyon, J. , & Benali, H. (2005). Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 161–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emery, B. (2010). Regulation of oligodendrocyte differentiation and myelination. Science, 330, 779–782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eto, K. , Wake, H. , Watanabe, M. , Ishibashi, H. , Noda, M. , Yanagawa, Y. , & Nabekura, J. (2011). Inter‐regional contribution of enhanced activity of the primary somatosensory cortex to the anterior cingulate cortex accelerates chronic pain behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 7631–7636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman, D. E. (2012). The spike‐timing dependence of plasticity. Neuron, 75, 556–571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fields, R. D. (2008). White matter in learning, cognition and psychiatric disorders. Trends in Neurosciences, 31, 361–370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frantseva, M. V. , Fitzgerald, P. B. , Chen, R. , Moller, B. , Daigle, M. , & Daskalakis, Z. J. (2008). Evidence for impaired long‐term potentiation in schizophrenia and its relationship to motor skill learning. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 990–996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson, E. M. , Purger, D. , Mount, C. W. , Goldstein, A. K. , Lin, G. L. , Wood, L. S. , … Monje, M. (2014). Neuronal activity promotes oligodendrogenesis and adaptive myelination in the mammalian brain. Science, 344, 1252304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goncalves, J. T. , Anstey, J. E. , Golshani, P. , & Portera‐Cailliau, C. (2013). Circuit level defects in the developing neocortex of fragile X mice. Nature Neuroscience, 16, 903–909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hakak, Y. , Walker, J. R. , Li, C. , Wong, W. H. , Davis, K. L. , Buxbaum, J. D. , … Fienberg, A. A. (2001). Genome‐wide expression analysis reveals dysregulation of myelination‐related genes in chronic schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 4746–4751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hines, J. H. , Ravanelli, A. M. , Schwindt, R. , Scott, E. K. , & Appel, B. (2015). Neuronal activity biases axon selection for myelination in vivo. Nature Neuroscience, 18, 683–689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hira, R. , Ohkubo, F. , Ozawa, K. , Isomura, Y. , Kitamura, K. , Kano, M. , … Matsuzaki, M. (2013). Spatiotemporal dynamics of functional clusters of neurons in the mouse motor cortex during a voluntary movement. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1377–1390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hooks, B. M. , Mao, T. , Gutnisky, D. A. , Yamawaki, N. , Svoboda, K. , & Shepherd, G. M. (2013). Organization of cortical and thalamic input to pyramidal neurons in mouse motor cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 748–760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huber, D. , Gutnisky, D. A. , Peron, S. , O'Connor, D. H. , Wiegert, J. S. , Tian, L. , … Svoboda, K. (2012). Multiple dynamic representations in the motor cortex during sensorimotor learning. Nature, 484, 473–478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kagawa, T. , Ikenaka, K. , Inoue, Y. , Kuriyama, S. , Tsujii, T. , Nakao, J. , … Mikoshiba, K. (1994). Glial cell degeneration and hypomyelination caused by overexpression of myelin proteolipid protein gene. Neuron, 13, 427–442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimura, F. , & Itami, C. (2009). Myelination and isochronicity in neural networks. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, 3, 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee, P. R. , Cohen, J. E. , Tendi, E. A. , Farrer, R. , DE Vries, G. H. , Becker, K. G. & Fields, R. D. (2004). Transcriptional profiling in an MPNST‐derived cell line and normal human Schwann cells. Neuron Glia Biology, 1, 135–147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li, Q. , Brus‐Ramer, M. , Martin, J. H. , & McDonald, J. W. (2010). Electrical stimulation of the medullary pyramid promotes proliferation and differentiation of oligodendrocyte progenitor cells in the corticospinal tract of the adult rat. Neuroscience Letters, 479, 128–133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu, J. , Dietz, K. , DeLoyht, J. M. , Pedre, X. , Kelkar, D. , Kaur, J. , … Casaccia, P. (2012). Impaired adult myelination in the prefrontal cortex of socially isolated mice. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 1621–1623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makino, H. , Hwang, E. J. , Hedrick, N. G. , & Komiyama, T. (2016). Circuit mechanisms of sensorimotor learning. Neuron, 92, 705–721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makinodan, M. , Rosen, K. M. , Ito, S. , & Corfas, G. (2012). A critical period for social experience‐dependent oligodendrocyte maturation and myelination. Science, 337, 1357–1360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martini, R. , & Schachner, M. (1997). Molecular bases of myelin formation as revealed by investigations on mice deficient in glial cell surface molecules. Glia, 19, 298–310. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masamizu, Y. , Tanaka, Y. R. , Tanaka, Y. H. , Hira, R. , Ohkubo, F. , Kitamura, K. , … Matsuzaki, M. (2014). Two distinct layer‐specific dynamics of cortical ensembles during learning of a motor task. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 987–994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKenzie, I. A. , Ohayon, D. , Li, H. , de Faria, J. P. , Emery, B. , Tohyama, K. , & Richardson, W. D. (2014). Motor skill learning requires active central myelination. Science, 346, 318–322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mensch, S. , Baraban, M. , Almeida, R. , Czopka, T. , Ausborn, J. , El Manira, A. , & Lyons, D. A. (2015). Synaptic vesicle release regulates myelin sheath number of individual oligodendrocytes in vivo. Nature Neuroscience, 18, 628–630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miall, R. C. , Price, S. , Mason, R. , Passingham, R. E. , Winter, J. L. , & Stein, J. F. (1998). Microstimulation of movements from cerebellar‐receiving, but not pallidal‐receiving areas of the macaque thalamus under ketamine anaesthesia. Experimental Brain Research, 123, 387–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moldakarimov, S. , Bazhenov, M. , & Sejnowski, T. J. (2015). Feedback stabilizes propagation of synchronous spiking in cortical neural networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112, 2545–2550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nave, K. A. (2010). Myelination and support of axonal integrity by glia. Nature, 468, 244–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nave, K. A. , & Ehrenreich, H. (2014). Myelination and oligodendrocyte functions in psychiatric diseases. JAMA Psychiatry, 71, 582–584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Connor, D. H. , Peron, S. P. , Huber, D. , & Svoboda, K. (2010). Neural activity in barrel cortex underlying vibrissa‐based object localization in mice. Neuron, 67, 1048–1061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pajevic, S. , Basser, P. J. , & Fields, R. D. (2014). Role of myelin plasticity in oscillations and synchrony of neuronal activity. Neuroscience, 276, 135–147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters, A. J. , Chen, S. X. , & Komiyama, T. (2014). Emergence of reproducible spatiotemporal activity during motor learning. Nature, 510, 263–267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roelfsema, P. R. , Engel, A. K. , Konig, P. , & Singer, W. (1997). Visuomotor integration is associated with zero time‐lag synchronization among cortical areas. Nature, 385, 157–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salami, M. , Itami, C. , Tsumoto, T. , & Kimura, F. (2003). Change of conduction velocity by regional myelination yields constant latency irrespective of distance between thalamus and cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 6174–6179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sampaio‐Baptista, C. , Khrapitchev, A. A. , Foxley, S. , Schlagheck, T. , Scholz, J. , Jbabdi, S. , … Johansen‐Berg, H. (2013). Motor skill learning induces changes in white matter microstructure and myelination. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 19499–19503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saugier‐Veber, P. , Munnich, A. , Bonneau, D. , Rozet, J. M. , Le Merrer, M. , Gil, R. , & Boespflug‐Tanguy, O. (1994). X‐linked spastic paraplegia and Pelizaeus‐Merzbacher disease are allelic disorders at the proteolipid protein locus. Nature Genetics, 6, 257–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scholz, J. , Klein, M. C. , Behrens, T. E. , & Johansen‐Berg, H. (2009). Training induces changes in white‐matter architecture. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 1370–1371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulman, J. J. , Cancro, R. , Lowe, S. , Lu, F. , Walton, K. D. , & Llinas, R. R. (2011). Imaging of thalamocortical dysrhythmia in neuropsychiatry. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadmehr, R. , & Krakauer, J. W. (2008). A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Experimental Brain Research, 185, 359–381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimizu, T. , Tanaka, K. F. , Takebayashi, H. , Higashi, M. , Wisesmith, W. , Ono, K. , … Ikenaka, K. (2013). Olig2‐lineage cells preferentially differentiate into oligodendrocytes but their processes degenerate at the chronic demyelinating stage of proteolipid protein‐overexpressing mouse. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 91, 178–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shine, H. D. , Readhead, C. , Popko, B. , Hood, L. , & Sidman, R. L. (1992). Morphometric analysis of normal, mutant, and transgenic CNS: Correlation of myelin basic protein expression to myelinogenesis. Journal of Neurochemistry, 58, 342–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanahira, C. , Higo, S. , Watanabe, K. , Tomioka, R. , Ebihara, S. , Kaneko, T. , & Tamamaki, N. (2009). Parvalbumin neurons in the forebrain as revealed by parvalbumin‐Cre transgenic mice. Neuroscience Research, 63, 213–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka, H. , Ikenaka, K. , & Isa, T. (2006). Electrophysiological abnormalities precede apparent histological demyelination in the central nervous system of mice overexpressing proteolipid protein. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 84, 1206–1216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka, H. , Ma, J. , Tanaka, K. F. , Takao, K. , Komada, M. , Tanda, K. , … Ikenaka, K. (2009). Mice with altered myelin proteolipid protein gene expression display cognitive deficits accompanied by abnormal neuron‐glia interactions and decreased conduction velocities. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 8363–8371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka, Y. H. , Tanaka, Y. R. , Kondo, M. , Terada, S. I. , Kawaguchi, Y. , & Matsuzaki, M. (2018). Thalamocortical axonal activity in motor cortex exhibits layer‐specific dynamics during motor learning. Neuron, 100, 244–258.e212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tennant, K. A. , Taylor, S. L. , White, E. R. , & Brown, C. E. (2017). Optogenetic rewiring of thalamocortical circuits to restore function in the stroke injured brain. Nature Communications, 8, 15879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thabit, M. N. , Ueki, Y. , Koganemaru, S. , Fawi, G. , Fukuyama, H. , & Mima, T. (2010). Movement‐related cortical stimulation can induce human motor plasticity. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 11529–11536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tian, L. , Hires, S. A. , Mao, T. , Huber, D. , Chiappe, M. E. , Chalasani, S. H. , … Looger, L. L. (2009). Imaging neural activity in worms, flies and mice with improved GCaMP calcium indicators. Nature Methods, 6, 875–881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veniero, D. , Ponzo, V. , & Koch, G. (2013). Paired associative stimulation enforces the communication between interconnected areas. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 13773–13783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wake, H. , Lee, P. R. , & Fields, R. D. (2011). Control of local protein synthesis and initial events in myelination by action potentials. Science, 333, 1647–1651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wake, H. , Ortiz, F. C. , Woo, D. H. , Lee, P. R. , Angulo, M. C. , & Fields, R. D. (2015). Nonsynaptic junctions on myelinating glia promote preferential myelination of electrically active axons. Nature Communications, 6, 7844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitford, T. J. , Ford, J. M. , Mathalon, D. H. , Kubicki, M. , & Shenton, M. E. (2012). Schizophrenia, myelination, and delayed corollary discharges: A hypothesis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38, 486–494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiao, L. , Ohayon, D. , McKenzie, I. A. , Sinclair‐Wilson, A. , Wright, J. L. , Fudge, A. D. , … Richardson, W. D. (2016). Rapid production of new oligodendrocytes is required in the earliest stages of motor‐skill learning. Nature Neuroscience, 19, 1210–1217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin, H. H. , Mulcare, S. P. , Hilario, M. R. , Clouse, E. , Holloway, T. , Davis, M. I. , … Costa, R. M. (2009). Dynamic reorganization of striatal circuits during the acquisition and consolidation of a skill. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 333–341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zatorre, R. J. , Fields, R. D. , & Johansen‐Berg, H. (2012). Plasticity in gray and white: Neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 528–536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeller, D. , aufm Kampe, K. , Biller, A. , Stefan, K. , Gentner, R. , Schutz, A. , … Classen, J. (2010). Rapid‐onset central motor plasticity in multiple sclerosis. Neurology, 74, 728–735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, W. , Peterson, M. , Beyer, B. , Frankel, W. N. , & Zhang, Z. W. (2014). Loss of MeCP2 from forebrain excitatory neurons leads to cortical hyperexcitation and seizures. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 2754–2763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.