Abstract
The ability to execute different responses in an expedient temporal order is central for efficient goal-directed actions and often referred to as multi-component behaviour. However, the underlying neural mechanisms on a cellular level remain unclear. Here we establish a link between neural activity at the cellular level within functional neuroanatomical structures to this form of goal-directed behaviour by analyzing immediate early gene (IEG) expression in an animal model, the pigeon (Columba livia). We focus on the group of zif268 IEGs and ZENK in particular. We show that when birds have to cascade separate task goals, ZENK expression is increased in the avian equivalent of the mammalian prefrontal cortex, i.e. the nidopallium caudolaterale (NCL) as well as in the homologous striatum. Moreover, in the NCL as well as in the medial striatum (MSt), the degree of ZENK expression was highly correlated with the efficiency of multi-component behaviour. The results provide the first link between cellular IEG expression and behavioural outcome in multitasking situations. Moreover, the data suggest that the function of the fronto-striatal circuitry is comparable across species indicating that there is limited flexibility in the implementation of complex cognition such as multi-component behaviour within functional neuroanatomical structures.
Subject terms: Cognitive control, Molecular neuroscience
Introduction
Many everyday activities involve the complex coordination of separate actions in order to generate efficient goal-directed behaviour. For example, driving a car requires us to execute different task goals such as changing gear and accelerating, while simultaneously tracking the paths of other cars in a reasonable temporal sequence. This demonstrates an example of multi-component behaviour that can be summarized as the capacity to execute, cascade and interrupt separate task goals and responses in an adequate chronological order1–5. From a functional neuroanatomical perspective a wide-spread prefrontal cortical1,3–7 and basal ganglia network8,9 has been implicated in this form of behaviour. While the neural correlates of multi-component behaviour are increasingly better understood in humans, their underlying mechanisms on a cellular level still remain unclear and can only be investigated using animal models and appropriate molecular neurobiological methods. Pigeons are a promising model organism, as they are capable of this form of goal-directed behaviour10. The question, however, remains how this behaviour can be realized on a neuronal level, as avian and mammalian brains are not one-to-one comparable11. If the functional neuroanatomical structures being involved in multi-component behaviour are comparable across species, pigeons would allow us to gain unique insights into the evolutionary conserved mechanisms of multi-component behaviour.
Given the involvement of the fronto-striatal network in humans1,3,4,6–9,12,13, it would be expected that the avian equivalents of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the striatum are also involved in multi-component behaviour in pigeons. An analogous structure to the mammalian PFC is the avian nidopallium caudolaterale (NCL), as they share many anatomical14,15, functional16,17, neurochemical18,19 and electrophysiological16,20,21 properties. In contrast to the mammalian PFC, the NCL is located in the caudal telencephalon15 and its genetic profile is different22, which suggests that it is unlikely that the two structures are derived from a common ancestor, but are rather the result of evolutionary convergence23. On the other hand, the avian basal ganglia including the striatum are considered homologues to their mammalian counterparts based on cellular, connectivity and functional data24,25. Based on the analogy of the NCL and the PFC and the homology of the avian and mammalian striatum, we hypothesize that both structures are involved in multi-component behaviour in pigeons. To examine this hypothesis, this study measured brain activity of the above-mentioned brain areas by quantifying immediate early gene (IEG) expression after pigeons engaged in multi-component behaviour.
IEGs are rapidly and transiently activated in response to a wide range of factors modulating cellular transcription levels26–28. IEGs are increasingly expressed in response to local neural activity as induced by electrical, neurobiochemical and behavioural-state changes27,28. Therefore, the analysis of the expression of IEGs in neural tissue can be used as a molecular neurobiological ‘imaging method’ to link neural activity within functional neuroanatomical structures to behaviour. There are several groups of IEGs28, such as c-fos, c-jun and zif268. Regarding the latter, zif268, homologs have been cloned from human (NGFI-A), rodents (egr-1, krox-24) and birds (ZENK)27. In the current study we examine ZENK expression in pigeons (Columba livia) after performing a STOP-CHANGE paradigm.
A STOP-CHANGE paradigm is a way to investigate multi-component behaviour in humans as well as in pigeons4,10. Subjects performing this paradigm are mostly confronted with simple GO trials. However, in some trials (STOP-CHANGE trials) the GO stimulus is followed by a STOP and a CHANGE signal. These signals indicate that the GO response needs to be inhibited and a CHANGE response needs to be executed instead. This paradigm consists of two conditions that vary in the onsets of the STOP and CHANGE stimuli (STOP CHANGE delay; SCD). In the SCD 0 condition, both signals are presented at the same time, whereas in the SCD 300 condition the CHANGE stimulus is presented 300 ms after the STOP signal (Fig. 1C, see methods section for more detail). The complexity of this paradigm can vary considerably depending on whether the to-be-cascaded actions of stopping and changing are signaled at the same time, or are signaled in close succession. When trying to process two different actions at once, restricted response selection capacities29,30 become overstrained and multi-component behaviour unfolds inefficiently as compared to a situation where it is not necessary to process two different actions at once4,31. The efficacy of multi-component behaviour can be quantified by means of a single parameter (see methods section for details). We hypothesize that this efficacy parameter of multi-component is directly correlated with the brain activity in the above-mentioned structures.
Results
Comparison of ZENK expression between the GO, STOP and STOP-CHANGE groups
In a first step we wanted to determine which brain areas were involved in multi-component behaviour in pigeons. Therefore, pigeons were divided in three groups. The GO group only performed GO trials and served as a baseline condition controlling for movement and reward related neural activity during the task (Fig. 1A). The STOP group performed GO and STOP trials and was important to dissociate simple STOP processes from STOP-CHANGE processes (Fig. 1B). The STOP-CHANGE group performed the whole STOP-CHANGE paradigm (see methods for more details) and was the experimental group of this study in which multi-component behaviour was tested (Fig. 1C).
In the final test session, all groups received 400 trials in their particular paradigm and pigeons were perfused 60 minutes after the first trial had been started. With stainings against the immediate early gene ZENK we could quantify the contribution of the NCL, the striatum, the arcopallium and the dorsal portion of the dorsomedial hippocampus (DMd) to the above-mentioned processes. Group differences in the number of IEG-expressing neurons in all areas of interest were analyzed with a repeated measures ANOVA using the within-subject factor “area” and the between subject factor “group”.
We found a main effect of area (F(3,45) = 17.98, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.545) and a main effect of group (F(2,15) = 6.33, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.458). Importantly, there was also an interaction between the factor “group” and “area” (F(6,45) = 3.19, p = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.298). Bonferroni corrected pairwise comparisons revealed that the NCL was significantly more active in the STOP-CHANGE group (1081 cells per mm2 ± 339 SEM) compared to the GO group (172 cells per mm2 ± 45 SEM; p = 0.009; Fig. 2A,F). Furthermore, the ZENK expression in the NCL was significantly higher in the STOP-CHANGE group as compared to the STOP group (292 cells per mm2 ± 92 SEM; p = 0.048; Fig. 2A,F). The ZENK expression pattern in the striatum was very similar to that of the NCL. Bonferroni corrected pairwise comparisons revealed that ZENK expression in the striatum was significantly higher in the STOP-CHANGE group (1599 cells per mm2 ± 370 SEM) compared to the GO group (479 cells per mm2 ± 198 SEM; p = 0.031; Fig. 2B,F). However, ZENK expression in the striatum in the STOP-CHANGE group was not significantly increased as compared to the STOP group (582 cells per mm2 ± 114 SEM; p = 0.177; Fig. 2B,F).
Another area of interest was the arcopallium which is thought to be a functional equivalent to the mammalian pre/motorcortex32. We analyzed this area because it receives motor input from the NCL and should be activated in tasks that require motor feedback15. We found a significant difference between groups within this area. The arcopallium was significantly more active in the STOP-CHANGE group (432 cells per mm2 ± 103 SEM) as compared to the GO group (145 cells per mm2 ± 42 SEM; p = 0.020; Fig. 2C,F). Furthermore, the ZENK expression in the arcopallium was significantly higher in the STOP-CHANGE group compared to the STOP group (97 cells per mm2 ± 17 SEM; p = 0.004; Fig. 2C,F). As the hippocampus is not expected to be involved in STOP-CHANGE processes, its subdivision DMd was analyzed as a control area to ensure that group differences were not the result of varying staining intensities. ZENK expression in DMd was similar between the STOP-CHANGE group (428 cells per mm2 ± 99 SEM) and the GO group (430 cells per mm2 ± 138 SEM; p = 1.000; Fig. 2D,F). Additionally, the activity within DMd was similar between the STOP-CHANGE group and the STOP group (320 cells per mm2 ± 134 SEM; p = 0.787; Fig. 2D,F). In all tested areas, the GO and the STOP group displayed similar patterns of activation (for all comparisons p = 1.000; Fig. 2F).
As already outlined above, the ANOVA also revealed a main effect of area (F(3,45) = 17.980, p < 0.001) indicating that the brain areas significantly differed in their activity. However, this could easily reflect simple differences in neuron densities. To distinguish the relative importance of the NCL and the striatum that can be traced back to STOP-CHANGE processes, the relative increase in ZENK positive cells between all conditions must be considered. Interestingly, the striatum was significantly more active than the NCL in the GO group (NCL: 172 cells per mm2 ± 45 SEM; striatum: 479 cells per mm2 ± 198 SEM; p = 0.047; Fig. 2F) and STOP group (NCL: 292 cells per mm2 ± 92 SEM; striatum: 582 cells per mm2 ± 114 SEM; p = 0.049; Fig. 2F). This was however not the case in the STOP-CHANGE group (NCL: 1081 cells per mm2 ± 339 SEM; striatum: 1599 cells per mm2 ± 370 SEM; p = 0.943; Fig. 2F). The relative increase between the STOP and STOP-CHANGE group in neuronal activity that reflects the CHANGE process was 1.3 times greater for the NCL than for the striatum.
Correlation of ZENK expression in the STOP-CHANGE group with the efficiency of multi-component behavior
Another goal of this study was to determine whether the brain activity as measured with ZENK expression was directly correlated with the efficiency of multi-component behaviour. Therefore, we calculated an individual slope value between the CHANGE (GO2) response times (RTs) in the “SCD 0” and “SCD 300” condition for all pigeons that performed the STOP-CHANGE paradigm (for more details see method section). This slope value indicates whether the task was solved using a parallel processing strategy (slope value closer to 1, less efficient) or a serial processing strategy (slope value closer to 0, more efficient)1,4,31. This slope value was correlated with the number of IEG expressing neurons in all brain areas of interest. For this data analysis the NCL was subdivided into NCL pars lateralis (NCLl; Fig. 3A) and NCL pars medialis (NCLm; Fig. 3B) since both subdivisions have different neuroanatomical target regions. While the NCLm projects to the medial striatum (MSt), the NCLl projects to the arcopallium24. The histological data furthermore suggested to subdivide the striatum into the medial striatum (MSt, Fig. 3C) and the lateral striatum (LSt, Fig. 3D). ZENK expression in the NCLl, the NCLm and the MSt was macroscopically different between pigeons that used a rather serial processing strategy (Fig. 3A–C left) and pigeons that used a rather parallel processing strategy (Fig. 3A–C right). In contrast to this, ZENK expression could not differentiate between the processing strategies in LSt (Fig. 3D, left vs. right).
We found a significant correlation between the number of ZENK-positive neurons in NCLl (r = −0.86; p = 0.028; Fig. 4A), NCLm (r = −0.89, p = 0.016, Fig. 4B) and MSt (r = −0.82, p = 0.047, Fig. 4C) with the slope values of the SCD-RT2 function. Steeper slope values, that indicated a more parallel processing strategy, were associated with a greater brain activation in all three brain areas. However, the number of ZENK-positive neurons in the LSt (r = −0.56, p = 0.250, Fig. 4D), the arcopallium (r = −0.77, p = 0.074) and DMd (r = −0.66, p = 0.150) were not significantly correlated with the slope values of the SCD-RT2 function.
Discussion
In the current study we examined the IEG-correlates of multi-component behaviour. Thus far, previous studies in humans were able to delineate the functional neuroanatomical network and some neurobiological insights into the mechanisms of multi-component behaviour using pharmacological and genotyping approaches. However, in-depth correlates at a cellular level have remained elusive. To this end, we examined IEG expression (i.e. ZENK expression) in an animal model (i.e. pigeons), which have previously been shown to be able to display multi-component behaviour10. Based on the analogy of the NCL and the PFC and the homology of the avian and mammalian striatum, we hypothesized that both structures are involved in multi-component behaviour and would display increased ZENK expression compared to a simple stimulus response task as was conducted in the GO control group and compared to a pure response inhibition task as was conducted in the STOP group. Furthermore, we hypothesized that the brain activity measured in the STOP-CHANGE group within both areas would be correlated with the efficiency of multi-component behaviour. Additionally, we analysed ZENK expression in the arcopallium which is comparable to the mammalian premotor cortex32. Activity within all tested brain areas and groups was symmetrical between both hemispheres. While a lot of studies report behavioural and neuronal asymmetries in the avian literature33–35, it needs to be noted that that the basis of brain asymmetries mostly refers to timing differences between the two hemispheres36. However, the temporal resolution of ZENK is not fast enough to depict such timing differences. Moreover, ZENK is not sensitive enough to visualize certain aspects of lateralization as recently corroborated by an fMRI and ZENK study37. It is therefore still possible that aspects of multi-component behaviour are lateralized that could not be identified with ZENK. In the following, the findings will be summarized and discussed for every area separately.
Similar to several human studies providing evidence that the prefrontal cortex plays an important role1,3–7, we observed ZENK activity in the NCL. ZENK activity was significantly upregulated when birds performed a STOP-CHANGE paradigm, as compared to a condition with simple movement execution (GO group) and as compared to a condition with movement inhibition (STOP group). Furthermore, the ZENK expression in the NCL was significantly correlated with the efficiency of multi-component behaviour. For this correlation analysis, the NCL was subdivided into the NCL pars medialis (NCLm) and the NCL pars lateralis (NCLl). While NCLm is highly connected to MSt, resembling the fronto-striatal network in mammals15,24, NCLl shows strong projections to the arcopallium as a motor output structure15,38. Additionally, NCLl receives massive sensory input from secondary sensory areas and also projects back to these structures15. Both the NCLl and the NCLm revealed strong linear correlations between the number of ZENK-positive cells and the slope of the SCD-RT2 function, which provides an index of the efficacy of multi-component behaviour (see methods section for details). A steeper slope of the SCD-RT2 function has been shown to indicate less efficient multi-component behaviour (parallel processing)2,4,31. Thus, the data show that both subdivisions of the NCL revealed stronger activity when multi-component behaviour was less efficient. While this shows that both NCL parts are involved in multi-component behaviour, it is possible that this is due to different reasons: As the NCLm is part of a network resembling the fronto-striatal network, it is possible that this subdivision is responsible for parts of multi-component behaviour that are governed by prefrontal areas in humans1,3,4,6–9. Activity in the NCLl, however, could be explained by the tasks necessity to use goal-directed movements to solve this particular paradigm, as premotor units have been associated with this area39. Moreover, given the sensory input to NCLl, activity in this area could be a response to stimuli of different modalities involved in this task. In fact, the complexity of sensory integration processes has been shown to modulate neural processes in humans while performing an equivalent task to measure multi-component behaviour6,7,40,41. Furthermore, also in human studies correlations between EEG correlates and the efficiency in multi-component behaviour have been found indicating that stronger amplitudes were associated with less efficient multi-component behavior1,2,4.
While the results obtained from ZENK expression studies are not directly comparable to EEG amplitudes, the explanation for both findings might be similar: As outlined, the slope of the SCD-RT2 function becomes steeper whenever STOP and CHANGE stimuli are processed at the same time (i.e. in parallel). When STOP- and CHANGE-associated task goals are processed in parallel, reaction times increase because these processes must share a limited capacity. Especially the prefrontal cortex which is the mammalian equivalent to the NCL11,23, is subject to simultaneity constraints. The same lateral prefrontal neurons/circuits have been shown to respond to very different stimuli under different task conditions3,42,43. The increased activation as indicated by an increased ZENK-positive cell count might represent an attempt to process different task goals simultaneously. Overall, the ZENK expression data in the NCL indicates that this structure is important for multi-component behaviour in pigeons. As the PFC has been shown to be involved in multi-component behaviour in several human studies1,3–7 this finding illustrates a further functional similarity of the avian NCL to the mammalian PFC. Since the PFC and the NCL are only functionally comparable but differ in their anatomical position15 and genetic profile22, they cannot be considered homologous23. Thus, our data provide further evidence for the idea that the avian NCL and the mammalian PFC are a case of evolutionary convergence and that there is limited flexibility in the implementation of complex cognition such as multi-component behaviour within functional neuroanatomical structures.
Comparable to the results of the NCL, ZENK activity in the striatum was significantly upregulated when birds performed a STOP-CHANGE paradigm, as compared to a condition with simple movement execution (i.e. GO trials). However, ZENK expression in the striatum was not significantly increased as compared to a condition with motor response inhibition (STOP group). Thus, it cannot completely be ruled out that the striatal ZENK activity in the STOP-CHANGE group was the result of STOP processes rather than CHANGE processes. Nevertheless, ZENK activity in the MSt was significantly correlated with the efficiency of multi-component behaviour. This indicates that activity within this area is important for the outcome of multi-component behaviour. In contrast to this, ZENK activity in the LSt was not correlated with the efficiency of multi-component behaviour suggesting subregion-specific differences in the functionality of the avian striatum. Both, the MSt and the LSt, are thought to be homologous to the mammalian striatum (i.e. caudate and putamen) based on similar neurochemistry as well as shared hodological and developmental traits25. Yet, both subdivisions are not completely identical in their cellular composition and circuitry24. While the MSt contains cholinergic and medium-sized aspiny GABAergic interneurons that express neuropeptide Y (NPY) and somatostatin24,44, those cells are fewer or even absent in LSt24,44,45. This suggest that different neuronal computations can be performed within both striatal structures. Furthermore, while the MSt has a strong projection to the substantia nigra and projects only sparsely to the globus pallidus, the LSt shows the reversed pattern with stronger projections to the globus pallidus and only sparse projections to the substantia nigra24,45,46. The functional implication of this has not been investigated yet. However, it is conceivable that structures with different cell types and main targets could vary in their functionality. For example, in mammals, the substantia nigra and the globus pallidus externa belong to different subsystems. While the substantia nigra is part of the direct pathway, the globus pallidus externa is part of the indirect pathway47. Both pathways have been shown to have opposing functions during the movement initiation. While the direct pathway facilitates movements, the indirect pathway suppresses movements48. It is very likely that those two pathways could be differentially involved in multi-component behaviour. However, to make final conclusions about the relative relevance of MSt versus LSt in multi-component behaviour, future mechanistic studies are needed.
The finding that MSt was involved in the outcome of multi-component behaviour is in line with conceptual accounts suggesting that the basal ganglia medium spiny neuron system constitutes an important structure mediating response selection processes49–52. Furthermore, the finding is in line with several human studies showing striatal activation during multi-component behaviour8,9,53. Also in humans, correlations between striatal activity as measured with BOLD activation and the efficiency of multi-component behaviour have been observed, where a lower BOLD activation in the caudate nucleus was associated with inefficient (parallel) processing, and higher BOLD activation in the caudate nucleus was associated with a more efficient (serial) processing mode9.The authors linked this finding to the proposed role of the striatum in producing sequential representations of actions (i.e. action-chunking54). They argued that an increased activation of the striatum is thought to strengthen its role in action-chunking and therefore enforce a rather serial processing of cascaded actions9. At first glance, this correlation seems to be in the opposite direction to our findings in the MSt of pigeons, where a greater activation was associated with less efficient parallel processing. However, it needs to be noted, that results obtained from fMRI and ZENK studies are not directly comparable. BOLD reflects the overall activity within a chosen area at a specific point in time but not on a single cell level, whereas ZENK expression indicates the amount of cells that was recruited during a whole session. A possible explanation for our correlation between a high ZENK expression in MSt and a parallel/less efficient processing strategy might be that when more cells are recruited this leads to more interference and thus more inefficient/parallel processing. This result is in line with models that describe action selection as a function of multiple parallel loops running through the basal ganglia. According to those models, the most active loop dominates the selected response, whereas activity within multiple loops creates interference51. Taken together, the data suggests that similar to humans, striatal structures in pigeons play an important role during multi-component behaviour indicating that there are evolutionary conserved mechanisms of this behaviour.
Another finding of the study was that the ZENK expression in the arcopallium was significantly upregulated when birds performed the STOP-CHANGE paradigm, as compared to a condition with simple movement execution (GO group) and as compared to a condition with movement inhibition (STOP group). However, the activity within the arcopallium was not correlated to the efficiency of multi-component behaviour. Activity within this area might have reflected the task’s necessity to use goal-directed movements as this structure is functionally comparable to the mammalian pre/motorcortex32. Neurons within this area project bilaterally via the tractus occipitomesencephalicus (TOM) to brainstem nuclei to regulate body, head and beak movements55–57. Electrophysiological studies have found that most neurons within the arcopallium are visuomotor neurons that start firing after GO-stimulus presentation and stop firing after the animal has responded. However, those neurons are not responsive to STOP signals36. The finding that the STOP group displayed the lowest ZENK cell count and the STOP-CHANGE group displayed the highest ZENK cell count is well in line with this electrophysiological study. The STOP group encountered 30% STOP trials that probably did not elicit any activity in arcopallial visuomotor neurons. In contrast to this, the STOP-CHANGE group encountered 30% CHANGE trials in which two GO stimuli were presented (GO-stimulus and CHANGE-stimulus) that probably both elicited neuronal activity within different arcopallial visuomotor neurons. Thus, the activity within the arcopallium probably reflected the complexity of visuomotor behaviour that was greater in the STOP-CHANGE group as compared to the STOP and GO group. This idea is further supported by the finding that the ZENK activity within the arcopallium was not significantly correlated with the efficiency in multi-component behaviour suggesting a visuomotor rather than a cognitive mechanism.
To summarize, the current data show that comparable to human studies, the “avian PFC” as well as the MSt are involved in multi-component behaviour, and the activity in both areas is directly correlated to its efficiency indicating a similar function of the fronto-striatal circuitry between species in multi-component behaviour. With this study we furthermore provide a first step towards an appropriate animal model for future mechanistic studies in which neuronal activity can be influenced with methods such as optogenetics to investigate the direct effect of stimulation on the processing mode of multi-component behaviour.
Materials and Methods
Experimental subjects
For this study, N = 18 adult homing pigeons (Columba livia) of undetermined sex were obtained from local breeders. They were individually caged and placed on a 12-hour light-dark cycle. During the time period of training and testing, the birds were maintained at approximately 85% of their free feeding weight. All experiments were performed according to the principles regarding the care and use of animals adopted by the German Animal Welfare Law for the prevention of cruelty to animals as suggested by the European Communities Council Directive of November 24, 1986 (86/609/EEC) and were approved by the animal ethics committee of the Landesamt für Natur, Umwelt und Verbraucherschutz NRW, Germany. All efforts were made to minimize the number of animals used and to minimize their suffering.
Skinner boxes
All experiments were conducted in conventional Skinner boxes (32 cm (w) × 34 cm (d) × 32 cm (h)). All Skinner boxes were equipped with white and red house lights and four transparent, round pecking keys (1.5 cm in diameter). Three keys were located on the front panel and one on the left side panel. A monitor was attached behind the front panel to display color stimuli behind the pecking keys. The initialization key on the side panel was illuminated by a blue LED. Below the keys on the front panel, a feeder was located, where the birds received a food reward consisting of mixed grains when responding correctly within the paradigm. A feeder light was positioned immediately above the feeder and indicated when the feeder was activated and food was available. All programs for this experiment were created using MATLAB and the Biopsychology Toolbox58.
STOP-CHANGE paradigm (STOP-CHANGE group)
For this study, a STOP-CHANGE paradigm was used10 that reflects a direct translation of a human paradigm used in previous studies delineating the functional neuroanatomical and neurobiological correlates of multi-component behaviour in humans1,4,6–9. The paradigm is shown in Fig. 1C. The first phase of training consisted of an autoshaping phase, where the birds learned to associate pecking on the illuminated keys with food reward. After the pigeons had learnt to peck on all the keys, they were trained in a STOP-CHANGE paradigm with a total of 400 trials. Each trial began with the illumination of the blue initialization key on the side panel, along with the presentation of a tone, which indicated the start of a trial. After initialization, a GO stimulus (left green pecking key on the front panel) was presented after a delay of 900 ms. This delay allowed the pigeon to turn towards the front in anticipation of the GO stimulus. In 70% of trials, pecking on the GO stimulus was the correct behaviour, which was rewarded with 2 s of food reward (GO trial). In the other 30% of trials, a STOP signal was presented after the GO stimulus by turning on the red houselight. This signal indicated that the reaction to the GO stimulus had to be inhibited and a reaction to a STOP-CHANGE (SC) stimulus had to be performed. The time between the onset of the GO stimulus and onset of the STOP signal (STOP-SIGNAL delay, SSD) was initially set to 450 ms and adjusted using a staircase procedure31. It was modified so that the probability of successfully interrupted GO responses was 50%. If a pigeon was able to inhibit its reaction to the GO stimulus and subsequently reacted to the SC stimulus, the SSD was shortened by 50 ms for the next trial. If the animal failed to perform both actions, the SSD was prolonged by 50 ms in the next trial. The SC stimulus consisted of a white illumination of either the upper or lower pecking key to the right of the GO key. The SCD between the presentation of the STOP and the SC stimuli was 0 ms in 50% of all STOP-CHANGE trials and 300 ms in the other 50% of trials. If the pigeon correctly pecked on the SC stimulus it received a 2 s food access reward (STOP-CHANGE trial). If it incorrectly pecked on the GO stimulus at any point after the STOP signal appeared, the lights in the box turned off for 5 s. The end of each trial consisted of a 5 s inter-trial interval before the blue initialization key and a tone signaled the start of the next trial (Fig. 1C). An important aspect to consider is that the analysis of IEG in pigeons performing the paradigms reflects activities to CHANGE, STOP and usual GO processes. Therefore, it is necessary to examine pigeons in two ‘control experiments’ in which only GO or GO and STOP processes are required.
GO group
The GO group was trained in the same Skinner boxes and performed the same autoshaping procedure as the STOP-CHANGE group (see Skinner boxes and STOP-CHANGE paradigm). This group was only trained with GO trials and served as a control group for the basic conditions of the paradigm since this condition contains all task-relevant motor executions such as pecking, eating, retrieval of the task and general movement in the Skinner box. ZENK expression patterns going beyond the ZENK expression in this control group can therefore be attributed to STOP or STOP-CHANGE related neuronal activity. The paradigm of the GO group was based on the STOP-CHANGE paradigm described above, but in this case, pigeons were only confronted with GO trials and did not have to perform STOP or STOP-CHANGE actions. Pecking on the green GO key was always rewarded with 2 s of food access (Fig. 1A).
STOP group
The STOP group was trained in the same Skinner boxes and performed the same autoshaping procedure as the STOP-CHANGE group (see Skinner boxes and STOP-CHANGE paradigm). This group was only trained with GO and STOP trials and was important to dissociate simple STOP processes from STOP-CHANGE processes. The paradigm for the STOP group was based on the STOP-CHANGE paradigm described above. The birds in this group, however, received 70% GO and 30% STOP trials. While in GO trials pecking on the green GO stimulus was the rewarded action, in STOP trials the pigeons were rewarded for inhibiting their reaction to the GO stimulus for at least 5 seconds. In this group the STOP signal (red light) appeared always in parallel with the GO stimulus. If the birds incorrectly pecked on the GO stimulus after the STOP signal had appeared, they were punished by turning off the house lights for 5 s (Fig. 1B).
Efficacy estimation of multi-component behaviour
Psychological models suggest that response selection is a capacity-limited process4,5,31,59. As outlined above, the STOP-CHANGE experiment used two different SCD intervals to present the CHANGE signal after the STOP signal. In the SCD 0 condition, STOP and CHANGE stimuli are presented at the same time, whereas in the SCD 300 condition STOP and CHANGE are presented with a 300 ms time gap. Thus, the SCD 0 condition leaves a choice how to process STOP- and CHANGE-associated processes. If the choice is to simultaneously process STOP- and CHANGE-associated task goals (i.e., in parallel), reaction times to the CHANGE stimulus (RT2) increase because these processes must share a limited capacity. However, in the SCD0 condition, it is also possible to choose a strategy in which STOP- and CHANGE-associated task goals are processed in a step-by-step (i.e., serial) manner. In this case, the STOP and CHANGE processes do not have to share a limited capacity when the STOP process is finished before the CHANGE process. This leads to shorter RT2s than the strategy in which STOP- and CHANGE-associated task goals are processed simultaneously. Critically, the SCD 300 condition always enforces a serial processing of the STOP- and CHANGE-related processes because the STOP process has finished when the CHANGE stimulus is presented 300 ms later. If such a serial processing strategy is used in the SCD 0 condition, the RT2s are comparable with those in the SCD 300 condition. The ratio of RT2 differences in the SCD 0 and SCD 300 conditions therefore gives an estimate of the strategy used during multi-component behaviour31.
The value becomes steeper with increasing differences between RT2SCD0 and RT2SCD300. When the STOP process has not finished by the time the CHANGE process is initiated (parallel processing strategy), the slope value becomes steeper, indicating that multi-component behaviour is less efficient. If the STOP process has finished (serial processing strategy), the slope approaches zero, which indicates that multi-component behaviour becomes more efficient31. Therefore, the slope of the SCD-RT2 function is flatter in the case of more efficient processing than in the case of the less efficient processing mode. The mean slope value was individually calculated for each pigeon in the final test session and correlated with the neuronal activity as measured with IEG expression.
Activity assessment/final test session
In the final test session, all groups were trained for 400 trials in their specific paradigm (i.e. GO group, STOP group and STOP-CHANGE group). The immediate early gene ZENK has been linked to long-term memory formation and synaptic plasticity60,61 and was used to visualize the activity in different areas of the pigeon brain. IEGs are a useful tool to visualize brain activity since they have a low basal expression, but a fast induction and degeneration62. The ZENK protein for example can be detected in neurons as fast as 15 minutes after stimulation and reaches its expression peek between 1 and 2 hours after stimulation62. Therefore, the pigeons were sacrificed 60 minutes after the first trial of the final test session had started and when they engaged in a minimum of 80% of the total trials. Intravenous injections of equithesin (0.45 ml per 100 g body weight) were applied into the brachial vein to minimize the time variance in the uptake of the anesthetic which can occur with intramuscular injections. The perfusion started after the heart of the animal stopped beating and eyelid closure reflex was negative.
Perfusion and tissue processing
Pigeons were perfused as previously described elsewhere63. The transcardial perfusion via the ventricle started with 0.9% sodium chloride (NaCl) and was followed by cold (4 °C) 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in 0.12 M phosphate buffer (PB; pH 7.4). After full blood exchange, the brains were removed from the skull and postfixed in 4% PFA with 30% sucrose at 4 °C for 2 hours. Hereafter, the brains were cryoprotected in 30% sucrose solution in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS; pH 7.4) for 24 hours. To simplify slicing, the brains were embedded in 15% gelatin/30% sucrose and were further fixated in 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS for 24 hours. Brains were sectioned in coronal plane in 40 µm-thick slices using a freezing microtome (Leica, Wetzlar, Germany) and stored at 4 °C in PBS with 0.1% sodium azide until further processing.
Immunohistochemistry
For immunohistochemistry against ZENK every tenth slice of all pigeon brains were used. The staining was performed with free floating sections and the ZENK protein was visualized with a DAB (3,3 diaminobenzidinetetrahydrochloride) staining procedure. The DAB-reaction was carried out according to the experimental protocol of the used DAB-Kit (Vector Laboratories, DAB Substrate Kit SK-4100)64. After rinsing (3 × 10 min in PBS), the slices were incubated in 0.3% hydrogen peroxide (H2O2) in distilled water for 30 min to block endogenous peroxidases. Following further rinsing, blocking of unspecific binding sites using 10% normal horse serum (NHS; Vector Laboratories-Vectastain Elite ABC kit) in PBS with 0.3% Triton-X-100 (PBST) was performed for 30 min. In the next step, the slices were incubated with a monoclonal mouse anti-ZENK antibody (1:5000 in PBST, 7B7-A3) at 4 °C over night. The 7B7-A3 antibody was raised in mice against the ZENK peptide from the rock pigeon (Columba livia) and its sensitivity and selectivity for its target was verified in immunblots as well as with histological stainings65. The next day, the slices were rinsed in PBS (3 × 10 min) and incubated with a secondary biotinylated anti-mouse antibody (1:1000 in PBST; Vector Laboratories-Vectastain Elite ABC kit) at room temperature for 1 hour. After further rinsing (3 × 10 min in PBS), the slices were transferred into an avidin-biotin complex (Vector Laboratories-Vectastain Elite ABC kit; 1:100 in PBST). After further rinsing (3 × 10 min), slices were transferred to the DAB solution that consisted of 5 ml distilled water with 2 drops (84 µl) of buffer stock solution, 4 drops (100 µl) of DAB stock solution and 2 drops (80 µl) of nickel solution. Sections were transferred to cell wells, whereby each well contained 1 ml of the working solution. The reaction was started by adding 6 µl H2O2 solution to each well. After 2 min incubation time the slices were transferred into cell wells with PBS and rinsed (2 × 5 min in PBS). Finally, the slices were mounted on gelatin-coated slides, dehydrated in alcohol and coverslipped with depex (Fluka).
Quantification of ZENK activity
For quantitative analysis of ZENK expression, all slices were imaged at 100× magnification using a ZEISS AXIO Imager.M1 with a camera (AxioCam MRm ZEISS 60N-C 2/3″0.63×). The whole slice was imaged bilaterally for all areas of interest. Depending on the rostro-caudal extent of the analyzed brain area, several slices could contribute to one brain region. Therefore, cells were counted in both hemispheres on four consecutive slices in NCLl, NCLm, arcopallium, MSt and LSt at different anterior (A) levels and the arithmetic mean was calculated for the further statistical analysis (planes: NCLm and NCLl: A 5.0–6.5, arcopallium: A 5.5–7.0, MSt A 9.0–10.5 and LSt A 8.0–9.5). As the hippocampus is not expected to be involved in STOP-CHANGE processes, this area was analyzed as a control area to ensure that group differences were not the result of varying staining intensities. Cells were counted in two consecutive slices in the dorsal portion of the dorsomedial hippocampus (DMd) since this subdivision displayed reliable ZENK expression in all tested animals. Cells were counted at A 5.0–6.0 and the arithmetic mean was calculated for the further statistical analysis. ZENK-positive cells were counted automatically with ImageJ (see more in the methods section ImageJ analysis) with the counter being blind to the group of the animal. The whole analysis was performed in all pigeons separately for the two hemispheres to control for hemispheric differences. A repeated measures ANOVA with the within subject factors hemisphere (left, right) and area (NCL, striatum and arcopallium) and the between subject factor group (GO, STOP, CHANGE) revealed that there was no significant difference between the hemispheres in all tested brain areas and groups (F(1,15) = 0.344, p = 0.566, ηp2 = 0.022) (see also Supplementary Fig. 1). A Bayesian analysis to assess the evidence for the null hypothesis (a lack of hemispheric differences)66 revealed a Bayes factor of 3.45. According to Kass and Raftery67, this indicates substantial evidence for the null hypothesis. Therefore, the measurements from both hemispheres were pooled together for the further statistical analyses. Furthermore, for the comparison between the GO, STOP and STOP-CHANGE groups, the data from the MSt and the LSt, and the data from the NCLl and the NCLm were pooled together. The correlation analysis between ZENK activity and the processing mode in the STOP-CHANGE group was however performed for all subdivision separately.
Image analysis
The microscopic images were processed with ImageJ and converted into an 8-bit picture. Mean staining intensities were measured within one section of the HC of all tested pigeons. A One-Way ANOVA revealed that there was no difference in mean staining intensities between all three groups (F(2,15) = 0.022, p = 0.978), indicating that staining intensities were comparable between all animals. Intensely stained neurons within the striatum were taken for the upper threshold and weakly stained neurons within the HC were taken as the lower threshold. Once the threshold was determined it was kept consistent for the analysis of all images. Furthermore, the size (>20 pixel) and roundness (>0.4) of the stained particles was used as a further selection criterion and was adjusted once for all measurements. Since the whole slice had been imaged bilaterally, it was possible to delineate the whole region of interest with anatomically correct borders and count ZENK-positive cells within the whole area. All regions of interest have a different absolute size. Therefore, the size of the delineated area was always measured in ImageJ, so that the counted cells could be standardized to 1 mm2 in the end.
Statistical analysis
We used the Shapiro-Wilk test to test for normal distribution of the data and the Levene’s test to test for the homogeneity of the variance. Both tests indicated that the requirements for parametric tests were violated. Therefore, we performed a logarithmic transformation of the data log10(x) that improved the normality as well as the equality of variances as follows:
Levene’s test before logarithmic transformation: NCL: F(2,15) = 8.261, p = 0.004; striatum: F(2,15) = 2.745, p = 0.096; arcopallium: F(2,15) = 8.905, p = 0.003; DMd: F(2,15) = 0.223, p = 0.803. Levene’s test after logarithmic transformation: NCL: F(2,15) = 0.238, p = 0.791; striatum: F(2,15) = 0.811, p = 0.463; arcopallium: F(2,15) = 0.188, p = 0.830; DMd: F(2,15) = 1.021, p = 0.384. Shapiro-Wilk test before logarithmic transformation: NCL: W(18) = 0.705, p < 0.001; striatum: W(18) = 0.863, p = 0.014; arcopallium: W(18) = 0.775, p = 0.001; DMd: W(18) = 0.835, p = 0.005. Shapiro-Wilk test after logarithmic transformation: NCL: W(18) = 0.967, p = 0.731; striatum: W(18) = 0.955, p = 0.507; arcopallium: W(18) = 0.981, p = 0.956; DMd: W(18) = 0.932, p = 0.213. Group differences in the number of IEG-expressing neurons in all areas of interest were then calculated with a repeated measures ANOVA with a within-subject factor “area” and a between-subject factor “group”. Post-hoc tests were Bonferroni corrected. Correlations between the slope of the SCD-RT2 function and the number of ZENK-positive neurons in all areas were tested with Pearson correlation test; the P-values calculated were Bonferroni corrected. Alpha was set at 0.05 for all analyses. All statistical analyses were performed with the software IBM SPSS Statistics (v. 20).
Ethical statement
All experiments were performed according to the principles regarding the care and use of animals adopted by the German Animal Welfare Law for the prevention of cruelty to animals as suggested by the European Communities Council Directive of November 24, 1986 (86/609/EEC) and were approved by the animal ethics committee of the Landesamt für Natur, Umwelt und Verbraucherschutz NRW, Germany. All efforts were made to minimize the number of animals used and to minimize their suffering.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by grants from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) through Projectnumber 395940726 (SFB 1372, project Neu04), BE4045/20-1, and partly by SFB 940 project B8. We acknowledge support by the DFG Open Access Publication Funds of the Ruhr-Universität Bochum. We want to thank David Keays who provided us with the pigeon specific ZENK antibody.
Author contributions
N.R. collected the data, analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. S.L. collected the data. J.P. helped with data acquisition and analysis and reviewed the manuscript. O.G. reviewed the manuscript, acquired funding and conceived the experiment. C.B. reviewed the manuscript, acquired funding and conceived the experiment.
Data availability
The data will be made available upon request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-019-56998-4.
References
- 1.Beste C, et al. How minimal variations in neuronal cytoskeletal integrity modulate cognitive control. NeuroImage. 2019;185:129–139. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Dippel G, Beste C. A causal role of the right inferior frontal cortex in implementing strategies for multi-component behaviour. Nat. Commun. 2015;6:6587. doi: 10.1038/ncomms7587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Duncan J. The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010;14:172–179. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Mückschel M, Stock A-K, Beste C. Psychophysiological mechanisms of interindividual differences in goal activation modes during action cascading. Cereb. Cortex N. Y. N 1991. 2014;24:2120–2129. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Stock A-K, Arning L, Epplen JT, Beste C. DRD1 and DRD2 genotypes modulate processing modes of goal activation processes during action cascading. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2014;34:5335–5341. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5140-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Stock A-K, Gohil K, Huster RJ, Beste C. On the effects of multimodal information integration in multitasking. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:4927. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-04828-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gohil K, Stock A-K, Beste C. The importance of sensory integration processes for action cascading. Sci. Rep. 2015;5:9485. doi: 10.1038/srep09485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Beste Christian, Saft Carsten. Action selection in a possible model of striatal medium spiny neuron dysfunction: behavioral and EEG data in a patient with benign hereditary chorea. Brain Structure and Function. 2013;220(1):221–228. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0649-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ness V, Beste C. The role of the striatum in goal activation of cascaded actions. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51:2562–2571. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Letzner S, Güntürkün O, Beste C. How birds outperform humans in multi-component behavior. Curr. Biol. CB. 2017;27:R996–R998. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.07.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Güntürkün O, Bugnyar T. Cognition without Cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016;20:291–303. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Yildiz A, et al. Feeling safe in the plane: neural mechanisms underlying superior action control in airplane pilot trainees–a combined EEG/MRS study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2014;35:5040–5051. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Mückschel M, Gohil K, Ziemssen T, Beste C. The norepinephrine system and its relevance for multi-component behavior. NeuroImage. 2017;146:1062–1070. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Durstewitz D, Kröner S, Güntürkün O. The dopaminergic innervation of the avian telencephalon. Prog. Neurobiol. 1999;59:161–195. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00100-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kröner S, Güntürkün O. Afferent and efferent connections of the caudolateral neostriatum in the pigeon (Columba livia): a retro- and anterograde pathway tracing study. J. Comp. Neurol. 1999;407:228–260. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990503)407:2<228::AID-CNE6>3.0.CO;2-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kalenscher T, et al. Neural correlates of a default response in a delayed go/no-go task. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2005;84:521–535. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2005.86-04. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lengersdorf, D., Marks, D., Uengoer, M., Stüttgen, M. C. & Güntürkün, O. Blocking NMDA-receptors in the pigeon’s “prefrontal” caudal nidopallium impairs appetitive extinction learning in a sign-tracking paradigm. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 18.Karakuyu D, Herold C, Güntürkün O, Diekamp B. Differential increase of extracellular dopamine and serotonin in the ‘prefrontal cortex’ and striatum of pigeons during working memory: Monoamine homeostasis in the avian forebrain. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2007;26:2293–2302. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05840.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Waldmann C, Güntürkün O. The dopaminergic innervation of the pigeon caudolateral forebrain: immunocytochemical evidence for a ‘prefrontal cortex’ in birds? Brain Res. 1993;600:225–234. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)91377-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Diekamp B, Kalt T, Güntürkün O. Working memory neurons in pigeons. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2002;22:RC210. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-04-j0002.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lengersdorf D, Pusch R, Güntürkün O, Stüttgen MC. Neurons in the pigeon nidopallium caudolaterale signal the selection and execution of perceptual decisions. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2014;40:3316–3327. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Puelles L, et al. Pallial and subpallial derivatives in the embryonic chick and mouse telencephalon, traced by the expression of the genes Dlx-2, Emx-1, Nkx-2.1, Pax-6, and Tbr-1. J. Comp. Neurol. 2000;424:409–438. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20000828)424:3<409::AID-CNE3>3.0.CO;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Güntürkün O. The avian ‘prefrontal cortex’ and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2005;15:686–693. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kuenzel WJ, Medina L, Csillag A, Perkel DJ, Reiner A. The avian subpallium: New insights into structural and functional subdivisions occupying the lateral subpallial wall and their embryological origins. Brain Res. 2011;1424:67–101. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.09.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Reiner A, Laverghetta AV, Meade CA, Cuthbertson SL, Bottjer SW. An immunohistochemical and pathway tracing study of the striatopallidal organization of area X in the male zebra finch. J. Comp. Neurol. 2004;469:239–261. doi: 10.1002/cne.11012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sheng M, Greenberg ME. The regulation and function of c-fos and other immediate early genes in the nervous system. Neuron. 1990;4:477–485. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(90)90106-P. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Long KD, Salbaum JM. Evolutionary conservation of the immediate-early gene ZENK. Mol. Biol. Evol. 1998;15:284–292. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.molbev.a025925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Loebrich S, Nedivi E. The Function of Activity-Regulated Genes in the Nervous System. Physiol. Rev. 2009;89:1079–1103. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00013.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Meyer DE, Kieras DE. A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. Psychol. Rev. 1997;104:3–65. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.104.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wu C, Liu Y. Queuing network modeling of the psychological refractory period (PRP) Psychol. Rev. 2008;115:913–954. doi: 10.1037/a0013123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Verbruggen F, Schneider DW, Logan GD. How to stop and change a response: the role of goal activation in multitasking. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2008;34:1212–1228. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Shanahan, M., Bingman, V. P., Shimizu, T., Wild, M. & Güntürkün, O. Large-scale network organization in the avian forebrain: a connectivity matrix and theoretical analysis. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 7 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 33.Vallortigara G, Chiandetti C, Sovrano VA. Brain asymmetry (animal): Brain asymmetry. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2011;2:146–157. doi: 10.1002/wcs.100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ. Survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behav. Brain Sci. 2005;28:575–589. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Diekamp B, Regolin L, Güntürkün O, Vallortigara G. A left-sided visuospatial bias in birds. Curr. Biol. 2005;15:R372–R373. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Xiao Q, Güntürkün O. Asymmetrical Commissural Control of the Subdominant Hemisphere in Pigeons. Cell Rep. 2018;25:1171–1180.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Van Ruijssevelt L, et al. fMRI Reveals a Novel Region for Evaluating Acoustic Information for Mate Choice in a Female Songbird. Curr. Biol. 2018;28:711–721.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.01.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Herold C, et al. The receptor architecture of the pigeons’ nidopallium caudolaterale: an avian analogue to the mammalian prefrontal cortex. Brain Struct. Funct. 2011;216:239–254. doi: 10.1007/s00429-011-0301-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Kalt T, Diekamp B, Güntürkün O. Single unit activity during a Go/NoGo task in the “prefrontal cortex” of pigeons. Brain Res. 1999;839:263–278. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(99)01727-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Gohil K, Bluschke A, Roessner V, Stock A-K, Beste C. Sensory processes modulate differences in multi-component behavior and cognitive control between childhood and adulthood. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017;38:4933–4945. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Bluschke A, Gohil K, Petzold M, Roessner V, Beste C. Neural mechanisms underlying successful and deficient multi-component behavior in early adolescent ADHD. NeuroImage Clin. 2018;18:533–542. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.02.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W. & Myers, J. An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behav. Brain Sci. 36 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 43.Duncan J, Owen AM. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:475–483. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01633-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Anderson KD, Reiner A. Distribution and relative abundance of neurons in the pigeon forebrain containing somatostatin, neuropeptide Y, or both. J. Comp. Neurol. 1990;299:261–282. doi: 10.1002/cne.902990302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Person AL, Gale SD, Farries MA, Perkel DJ. Organization of the songbird basal ganglia, including area X. J. Comp. Neurol. 2008;508:840–866. doi: 10.1002/cne.21699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mezey S, Csillag A. Selective striatal connections of midbrain dopaminergic nuclei in the chick (Gallus domesticus) Cell Tissue Res. 2002;308:35–46. doi: 10.1007/s00441-002-0514-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Albin RL, Young AB, Penney JB. The functional anatomy of basal ganglia disorders. Trends Neurosci. 1989;12:366–375. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90074-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Cui G, et al. Concurrent activation of striatal direct and indirect pathways during action initiation. Nature. 2013;494:238–242. doi: 10.1038/nature11846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Plenz D. When inhibition goes incognito: feedback interaction between spiny projection neurons in striatal function. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:436–443. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00196-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bar-Gad I, Morris G, Bergman H. Information processing, dimensionality reduction and reinforcement learning in the basal ganglia. Prog. Neurobiol. 2003;71:439–473. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2003.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Humphries MD, Stewart RD, Gurney KN. A Physiologically Plausible Model of Action Selection and Oscillatory Activity in the Basal Ganglia. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:12921–12942. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3486-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Redgrave P, Vautrelle N, Reynolds JNJ. Functional properties of the basal ganglia’s re-entrant loop architecture: selection and reinforcement. Neuroscience. 2011;198:138–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.07.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Yildiz A, Beste C. Parallel and serial processing in dual-tasking differentially involves mechanisms in the striatum and the lateral prefrontal cortex. Brain Struct. Funct. 2015;220:3131–3142. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0847-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Graybiel AM. The Basal Ganglia and Chunking of Action Repertoires. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 1998;70:119–136. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1998.3843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Dubbeldam JL, Den Boer-Visser AM. Organization of ‘feeding circuits’ in birds: pathways for the control of beak and head movements. Eur. J. Morphol. 1994;32:127–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hellmann B, Güntürkün O, Manns M. Tectal mosaic: organization of the descending tectal projections in comparison to the ascending tectofugal pathway in the pigeon. J. Comp. Neurol. 2004;472:395–410. doi: 10.1002/cne.20056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Wild JM, Arends JJA, Zeigler HP. Telencephalic connections of the trigeminal system in the pigeon (Columba livia): A trigeminal sensorimotor circuit. J. Comp. Neurol. 1985;234:441–464. doi: 10.1002/cne.902340404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Rose J, Otto T, Dittrich L. The Biopsychology-Toolbox: A free, open-source Matlab-toolbox for the control of behavioral experiments. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2008;175:104–107. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2009;33:647–661. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Cole AJ, Saffen DW, Baraban JM, Worley PF. Rapid increase of an immediate early gene messenger RNA in hippocampal neurons by synaptic NMDA receptor activation. Nature. 1989;340:474–476. doi: 10.1038/340474a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Stacho M, Ströckens F, Xiao Q, Güntürkün O. Functional organization of telencephalic visual association fields in pigeons. Behav. Brain Res. 2016;303:93–102. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.01.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Mello CV, Ribeiro S. ZENK protein regulation by song in the brain of songbirds. J. Comp. Neurol. 1998;393:426–438. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19980420)393:4<426::AID-CNE3>3.0.CO;2-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Stacho M, Letzner S, Theiss C, Manns M, Güntürkün O. A GABAergic tecto-tegmento-tectal pathway in pigeons: The indirect tecto-tectal connection. J. Comp. Neurol. 2016;524:2886–2913. doi: 10.1002/cne.23999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Chi, V. & Chandy, K. G. Immunohistochemistry: Paraffin Sections Using the Vectastain ABC Kit from Vector Labs. J. Vis. Exp., 10.3791/308 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 65.Nordmann, G. C. et al. A high sensitivity ZENK monoclonal antibody to map neuronal activity in Aves. Sci. Rep. (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 66.Wagenmakers E-J. A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2007;14:779–804. doi: 10.3758/BF03194105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Kass RE, Raftery AE. Bayes Factors. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1995;90:773–795. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1995.10476572. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data will be made available upon request.