Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Jan 10;9:e50631. doi: 10.7554/eLife.50631

Metabolic signature in nucleus accumbens for anti-depressant-like effects of acetyl-L-carnitine

Antoine Cherix 1,, Thomas Larrieu 2,, Jocelyn Grosse 2, João Rodrigues 2, Bruce McEwen 3, Carla Nasca 3, Rolf Gruetter 1, Carmen Sandi 2,
Editors: Harm J Krugers4, Christian Büchel5
PMCID: PMC6970538  PMID: 31922486

Abstract

Emerging evidence suggests that hierarchical status provides vulnerability to develop stress-induced depression. Energy metabolic changes in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) were recently related to hierarchical status and vulnerability to develop depression-like behavior. Acetyl-L-carnitine (LAC), a mitochondria-boosting supplement, has shown promising antidepressant-like effects opening therapeutic opportunities for restoring energy balance in depressed patients. We investigated the metabolic impact in the NAc of antidepressant LAC treatment in chronically-stressed mice using 1H-magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H-MRS). High rank, but not low rank, mice, as assessed with the tube test, showed behavioral vulnerability to stress, supporting a higher susceptibility of high social rank mice to develop depressive-like behaviors. High rank mice also showed reduced levels of several energy-related metabolites in the NAc that were counteracted by LAC treatment. Therefore, we reveal a metabolic signature in the NAc for antidepressant-like effects of LAC in vulnerable mice characterized by restoration of stress-induced neuroenergetics alterations and lipid function.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

Depression is among the leading causes of disability worldwide, which reflects the current lack of understanding of its underlying mechanisms (Friedrich, 2017; Menke, 2018). Metabolic alterations are emerging as key etiological factors for the development of neuropsychiatric disorders, including depression (Pei and Wallace, 2018; Andreazza and Nierenberg, 2018; Kim et al., 2019). The strong reliance of the brain on high energy consumption would make it particularly vulnerable to metabolic alterations (Pei and Wallace, 2018). In addition, chronic stress has a strong capacity to trigger and exacerbate depression (de Kloet et al., 2005; Richter-Levin and Xu, 2018) and impinges metabolic-costly neuronal adaptations in structure and function (Turner and Lloyd, 2004; de Kloet et al., 2005; McEwen et al., 2015). Accordingly, stress-associated depletion of brain's energy resources could lead to impaired neuronal plasticity underlying depression (Morava and Kozicz, 2013; Picard et al., 2018). Mitochondria, by powering the brain with energy production, play a central role in the adaptation and response to stress (Picard et al., 2015), and mitochondrial supplements could provide an efficient means of protecting brain structures that are particularly vulnerable to stress (Parikh et al., 2009).

However, not all individuals are equally affected by stress (Duclot and Kabbaj, 2013; McEwen et al., 2015; Russo et al., 2012); while some individuals show a high vulnerability to develop depression, others endure resilience following stress exposure (Russo et al., 2012; Weger and Sandi, 2018). It remains unclear which factors provide resilience to stress in certain individuals and what are the underlying mechanisms (Larrieu and Sandi, 2018; Ménard et al., 2017). In addition to the great predictive power of high anxiety trait in defining stress vulnerability (Sandi et al., 2008; Castro et al., 2012; for reviews, see Sandi and Richter-Levin (2009); Russo et al., 2012; Weger and Sandi, 2018), epidemiological, clinical and animal work point to a link between social hierarchies and depression (Larrieu and Sandi, 2018).

Recently, in the C57BL/6J inbred mouse strain, we found that high rank animals were more susceptible to display social avoidance following exposure to chronic social defeat stress (CSDS), while low rank mice were not affected (Larrieu et al., 2017). Data from 1H-magnetic resonance spectroscopy [1H-MRS; one of the few non-invasive methods that can provide direct information on brain metabolism in vivo (Duarte et al., 2012)] revealed a relationship between the metabolic profile of the nucleus accumbens [NAc; a hub brain region for the regulation of motivated behaviors (Robbins and Everitt, 1996) implicated in the pathophysiology of depression (Francis and Lobo, 2017)], social rank, and vulnerability to stress. Thus, while under basal conditions low rank showed lower levels of energy-related metabolites than high rank mice, it was only the low rank/resilient group that displayed increased metabolite levels following CSDS (Larrieu et al., 2017). These observations suggested that metabolic targeting may be an optimal treatment intervention and confirmed that NAc is a particularly sensitive structure that might beneficiate from energetic support.

Acetyl-L-carnitine (LAC) has been recently shown to have promising potency to rapidly alleviate depressive-like symptoms in preclinical studies (Bigio et al., 2016; Lau et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2015; Nasca et al., 2013) and in humans, where emerging clinical evidence supports its good tolerability (Wang et al., 2014; Veronese et al., 2018). LAC is an endogenous short-chain acetyl ester of free carnitine involved in the transport of long chain fatty acids into the mitochondria for degradation by beta oxidation thus, contributing to energy metabolism (Ferreira and McKenna, 2017). In addition, LAC can facilitate the removal of oxidative products, provide acetyl groups for protein acetylation, be used as a precursor for acetylcholine, or be incorporated into neurotransmitters such as glutamate, glutamine and GABA (Ferreira and McKenna, 2017). However, it is not known whether LAC treatment can counteract brain metabolic alterations specifically observed in the context of stress-induced depression.

In this study, we investigated the ability of LAC supplementation to protect vulnerable mice against stress induced depressive-like behaviors. As indicated above, socially dominant C57BL/6J mice, were at higher risk of developing depression-like behavior following exposure to CSDS (Larrieu et al., 2017). Here, in order to exclude that the identified vulnerability is a mere reflect of the social stressor used (Larrieu and Sandi, 2018), a first aim of this study was to assess the link between social rank and vulnerability to develop depressive-like behaviors using chronic exposure to a non-social (e.g., physical) stressor. To this end, and given that lipid peroxidation has been shown to be increased by restrain stress in the striatum (Atif et al., 2008), we exposed mice to the well-established 21 day restrain stress protocol (Lau et al., 2017; Nasca et al., 2015). Subsequently, we studied the impact of LAC treatment coinciding with the last week of stress exposure on the concentration of up to 20 metabolites in the NAc using in vivo 1H-MRS at 14 Tesla (Larrieu et al., 2017; Duarte et al., 2012; Mlynárik et al., 2008). We also tested mice for depressive-like behaviors, including motivation to explore social conspecifics and coping responses in the forced swim test (FST) (Nestler et al., 2002). Besides the evident translational potential of 1H-MRS at 14 T in identifying biomarkers, rodent 1H-MRS studies at ultra-high field bridge a potential pathological indicator and its associated molecular signature to physiological mechanisms.

Results

High rank mice are vulnerable to chronic restraint stress

First, we confirmed our initial observations (Larrieu et al., 2017) regarding the behavioral phenotype of high and low rank mice (as assessed by testing the four mice from the same home cage in the social confrontation tube test; SCTT), under basal conditions (i.e., before any stress was applied). Specifically, we report here that high (ranks 1 and 2) and low (ranks 3 and 4) rank mice (Figure 1B and C; time in the tube 1–2 vs 1–4, p<0.001), displayed different profile for anxiety-like behaviors. It was reflected by a higher time spent in the open arms of an elevated plus maze (EPM) (Figure 1D; p<0.05) as well as an increased latency to enter the center of an open field (OF) (Figure 1E, left; p<0.05) in high rank compared to low rank mice. However, the two groups did not show differences in the percent time spent in the center of the OF (Figure 1E, center), indicating that their difference in anxiety-like behaviors depends on the specific threat encountered. Importantly, no difference in locomotor activity was observed, as the distance travelled by both groups in the OF was similar (Figure 1E, right; n.s.).

Figure 1. High rank mice exhibit susceptible behavioral phenotype after 21 days of chronic restraint stress.

Figure 1.

(A) Experimental design of the restraint stress protocol. (B) Summary of nine cages representing the SCTT ranks and winning times as a function of SCTT trials over the 8 days of test. (C) Time spent in tube as a function of rank pairing (F5,35=18.19, p<0.0001, one-way ANOVA; *p<0.05, ***p<0.001, Bonferroni’s test, n = 7 per rank pairing). (D) Anxiety-like behaviors measured as the percent time spent in the open arms of an elevated plus maze after segregation into high rank vs low rank mice (p*<0.05, unpaired t-test, two-tailed, n = 14 per group). (E) Anxiety-related behaviors measured in the open-field, including latency to first enter the center of the arena (*p<0.05, unpaired t-test, two-tailed n = 14 per group) and time in center zone (n.s. unpaired t-test, two-tailed n = 14 per group). Locomotor activity is measured as the distance travelled in the OF (n.s. unpaired t-test, two-tailed n = 14 per group). (F) Social interaction (SI) test measured after chronic restraint stress protocol in high rank vs low rank animals (Social avoidance score: Interaction: F1,21=7.75, p<0.05; rank effect: F1,21=1.18, p>0.05; stress effect: F1,21=7.15, p<0.05, two-way ANOVA; p*<0.05, **p<0.01, Bonferroni’s test, n = 6–7 per group/Time in interaction zone: Interaction: F1,21=12.80, p<0.005; rank effect: F1,21=0.85, p<0.05; stress effect: F1,21=12.21, p<0.005, two-way ANOVA; p*<0.05, **p<0.005, Bonferroni’s test, n = 6–7 per group/Time in corner zone: Interaction: F1,21=3.29, p>0.05; rank effect: F1,21=0.28, p<0.05; stress effect: F1,21=2.14, p>0.05, two-way ANOVA, n = 6–7 per group). Data are displayed as mean ± SEM.

Strikingly, we revealed that the susceptibility to CSDS observed in high rank mice in our previous study (Larrieu et al., 2017) can be generalized to a non-social, chronic restraint stress (CRS) protocol. Indeed, high rank individuals were the ones that, after CRS, showed vulnerability to develop social avoidance towards an unfamiliar mouse in a social interaction test (SI) (Figure 1F). In contrast, low rank mice seemed not being affected by stress exposure (social avoidance score: interaction, F1,21=7.75, p<0.05; time in interaction zone: interaction, F1,21=12.80, p<0.005).

LAC treatment partially abolishes stress-induced behavioral vulnerability in high rank mice

Given the emerging evidence indicating a potential therapeutic efficiency of LAC, the acetylated form of carnitine, in the context of depression (see Introduction), we tested whether LAC treatment could counteract the induction of depressive-like behaviors by CRS in vulnerable (i.e., high rank) mice. In this experiment, mice were exposed to the CRS protocol and received concomitant administration of LAC during the last 7 days of the stress period. Animals exposed to CRS displayed a significant decrease in cumulative body weight gain, regardless of their social rank, that was not counteracted by LAC supplementation (Figure 2B; in high rank: Stress effect F2,10 = 45.0, p<0.0001 and Figure 2—figure supplement 1A; in low rank: Stress effect F2,10 = 20.4, p<0.001). Importantly, whereas stress led to an increase in liquid consumption (+15 ± 12%) in the stressed groups (Figure 2C and Figure 2—figure supplement 1B), there was no difference in liquid consumption between the CRS and CRS + LAC groups (Figure 2C).

Figure 2. High rank mice respond to acetyl-L-carnitine treatment after chronic restraint stress.

(A) Experimental design of the restraint stress protocol and treatment procedure. (B) High rank mice show a reduction of cumulative weight gain during the restraint stress protocol (Interaction: F20,40=11.5, p<0.0001; stress effect: F2,10 = 45.0, p<0.0001, repeated measures two-way ANOVA; ***p<0.001, ****p<0.0001, Bonferroni’s test, n = 6 per group). The start of LAC treatment during CRS protocol is indicated with an arrow (day 14). (C) Daily water intake during the LAC treatment period (given during the last week of the CRS protocol) normalized by total body weight of the four mice per cage (Group effect: F2,4=17.0, *p<0.05; Interaction: F14,28=0.90, p>0.05; time effect: F7,14=1.24, p>0.05, repeated measures two-way ANOVA; n = 3 cages per group). Thus, water intake data represent the cage average value. Liquid consumption during the first days of the CRS protocol is shown in Figure 2—figure supplement 1B (D) Social avoidance scores measured after chronic restraint stress protocol in high rank mice (F2,15=12.08, p<0.01, one-way ANOVA; **p<0.01, Bonferroni’s test, n = 6 per group). (E) Behavioral despair measured with a forced swim test between high rank mice (F2,15=5.31, p<0.05, one-way ANOVA; *p<0.05, Bonferroni’s test, n = 6 per group). (F) Depressive-like behavior measured as a composite z-score component of social avoidance and immobility time between high rank mice (F2,15=10.31, p<0.005, one-way ANOVA; *p<0.05**p<0.005, Bonferroni’s test, n = 6 per group). (G) Locomotor activity measured during the SI test (No target present: F2,15=2.94, p>0.05, one-way ANOVA; *p<0.05, Bonferroni’s test/Target present: F2,15=2.79, p>0.05, one-way ANOVA;/Average: F2,15=3.67, p<0.05, one-way ANOVA; *p<0.05, Bonferroni’s test, n = 6 per group). Effect of LAC on low rank is shown in Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Physiological and behavioral readouts following CRS and LAC treatment in low rank mice.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Low rank mice show a reduction of cumulative weight gain during the restraint stress protocol (Interaction: F40,20=8.17, p<0.0001; stress effect: F2,10 = 20.4, p<0.001, repeated measures two-way ANOVA; n = 6 per group). (B) Daily water intake during the first part of the CRS protocol (day 2–13) normalized by mice total body weight per cage (Group effect: F1,2=1367, p<0.001, repeated measures two-way ANOVA; n = 6 per group). This water intake represents an average value over the cage, including both high rank and low rank mice. (C–D) Depressive-like behavior of subordinate mice is not altered by LAC administration. ANOVA for the SI, FST and composite behavioral z-score for the three groups of low rank mice indicated no significant differences (SI: F2,13=0.55, n.s.; FST: F2,15=1.62, n.s.; Composite behavior: F2,15=0.16, n.s. (C) Social avoidance scores measured after chronic restraint stress protocol in low rank animals (F2,15=0.55, p>0.05, one-way ANOVA; n = 6 per group). (D) Behavioral despair measured with a forced swim test between low rank mice (F2,15=1.62, p>0.05, one-way ANOVA; n = 6 per group). (E) Depressive-like behavior measured as a composite z-score component of social avoidance and immobility time between low rank mice (F2,15=0.16, p>0.05, one-way ANOVA; n = 6 per group).

As we showed in our previous and current studies that CSDS and CRS induce social avoidance only in high rank mice, we investigated whether LAC supplementation could attenuate the effects of chronic stress on emotional behavior in this susceptible (i.e., high rank) group. We found that LAC treatment did not prevent the increase in social avoidance induced CRS in a group of 6 high rank mice (Figure 2D; n.s.). Since LAC has been shown to be a mitochondria-boosting supplement, we tested the possibility that LAC would be more efficient in a high energy-demanding test such as the FST. Indeed, LAC treatment was effective to abolish CRS-induced increase in passive coping behaviors in high rank mice in the FST (Figure 2E). Specifically, a significant increase in the immobility time observed in high rank mice following CRS exposure (Figure 2E; F2,15=5.31, p<0.05), was prevented by LAC supplementation (p<0.05). In order to obtain an integrated estimation of how stress and LAC treatments affect emotional behavior more globally, we computed an overall behavioral composite score to integrate deviation from normality considering high rank mice variance in both behavioral tests. Specifically, the use of this composite score allows considering variation from the mean in individuals’ behavior across two different behavioral tests, providing a more robust measurement of individuals’ behavior in tests that are typically used to index mice depressive-like behaviors. ANOVA of these data indicated a significant effect of treatments (Figure 2F; F2,15=10.31, p<0.005); specifically, CRS led to increased depressive-like behaviors (p<0.005) that was restored by LAC (p<0.05). Altogether, our findings support the view that pharmacological enhancement of mitochondrial function by LAC supplementation normalizes behavioral changes in stressed-high rank mice under unescapable adversity.

1H-MRS in NAc reveals stress-responsive metabolites in high rank mice counteracted by LAC treatment

Using in vivo 1H-MRS, we aimed at revealing the NAc neurochemical and metabolite profile in high rank mice following CRS and LAC supplementation. Our 1H-MRS acquisitions led to a spectral signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of 17.5 ± 0.3 with a linewidth of 16 ± 1 Hz after shimming with FAST(EST)MAP. The acquired NAc spectra allowed us to quantify up to 20 metabolites with LCModel (Figure 3). First, we applied an unbiased multivariate factor analysis (FA) that revealed three main factors that accounted for 31%, 12% and 9% of total variance (Figure 4). Individual metabolites with loadings above 0.4 in Factor one included taurine (Tau), glutamate (Glu), phosphocreatine (PCr), N-acetylaspartate (NAA), γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA), creatine (Cr), myo-inositol (Ins), aspartate (Asp), phosphocholine (PCho), glucose (Glc), glutathione (GSH) and ascorbate (Asc) (Figure 5). Metabolites with loading above 0.4 for the two other components were PCho, glycerophosphorylcholine (GPC), and Asc for Factor two, and glutamine (Gln) and lactate (Lac) for Factor three (Figure 6).

Figure 3. The neurochemical profile of the nucleus accumbens measured with in vivo 1H-MRS at 14T.

Figure 3.

Spectrum fitting and neuroanatomical image of the NAc with respective voxel position in mouse brain. Spectrum is decomposed into the total fit, the individual metabolite components of the fit, the residual and the baseline, as a result of LCModel analysis. The fitted neurochemical profile included following metabolites: taurine (Tau), creatine (Cr), phosphocreatine (PCr), glutamate (Glu), γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA), aspartate (Asp), glutamine (Gln), N-acetyl-aspartate (NAA), myo-inositol (Ins), glucose (Glc), ascorbate (Asc), glutathione (GSH), phosphorylcholine (PCho), glycerophosphorylcholine (GPC), lactate (Lac), N-acetylaspartyl-glutamate (NAAG), phosphoethanolamine (PE), alanine (Ala), glycine (Gly), as well as macromolecules (Mac).

Figure 4. Factor analysis identified one main factor that accounts for treatment-related effects in the metabolic profile of nucleus accumbens in high rank mice.

Figure 4.

(A) Metabolites in the nucleus accumbens that load into Factor one, Factor two and Factor three of the factor analysis. The heat map represents the individual loadings of each metabolite into each factor. Factor one represents a linear combination that summarizes neurochemical changes including metabolites with strong contribution (above 0.5: Tau, Cr, PCr, Glc, Glu, GABA, Asp, NAA and Ins.) and moderate (0.4–0.5: GSH and Asc). (B) CRS and LAC treatment in CRS-treated high rank mice impact on Factor one metabolites (F2,13=7.04, p<0.01, one-way ANOVA; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, Fisher LSD test n = 5–6 per group) (C) Factor one correlates with the composite emotional (i.e., depressive-like) behavior in high rank animals (R = −0.58; p<0.05).

Figure 5. Effect of CRS and LAC treatment in CRS-treated mice on the accumbal neurochemical profile of high rank mice for metabolites with strong loading on Factor one.

Metabolites from Factor one with strong loading (above 0.5) include Tau, Cr, PCr, Glc, Glu, GABA, Asp, NAA and Ins. The ratio of PCr/Cr is shown as well. CRS induces a drop in Tau, Glu, PCr, Asp and NAA. Only CRS-induced reductions in Tau and PCr are restored by LAC treatment. The neurochemical profile obtained for low rank mice is reported in Figure 5—figure supplement 1. One-way ANOVA followed by LSD Fisher post-hoc test, *p<0.05, n = 5–6 per group.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Effect of LAC on the accumbal neurochemical profile of low rank mice after CRS.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A) Factor analysis of the metabolic profile of nucleus accumbens of low rank mice after CRS. Factor one represents a linear combination that summarizes neurochemical changes including metabolites with strong (above 0.5: Tau, Cr, PCr, Glc, Glu, GABA, Asp, NAA and Ins.) and moderate (0.4–0.5: GSH and Asc) contribution in low rank mice (F2,15=0.54, p>0.05, one-way ANOVA; Fisher LSD test n = 6 per group). (B) Metabolites from Factor one with strong loading (above 0.5) Tau, Cr, PCr, Glc, Glu, GABA, Asp, NAA and Ins. The ratio of PCr/Cr is shown as well. One-way ANOVA followed by LSD Fisher post-hoc test, n = 5–6 per group. (C) Metabolites with moderate loadings (0.4–0.5) from Factor one and remaining metabolites from Factors two and three included Gln, GSH, Asc, PCho, Lac and GPC. The ratio of GPC/PCho was also reduced upon treatment. One-way ANOVA followed by LSD Fisher post-hoc test, *p<0.05, n = 5–6 per group. Overall, metabolite levels in low rank mice did not show substantial changes, except for Glc that was increased by CRS (p<0.05) and normalized by LAC treatment (p<0.05).

Figure 6. Effect of LAC on the accumbal neurochemical profile of high rank mice after CRS for remaining metabolites and associations between behavior and neurochemistry.

Figure 6.

(A) Metabolites with moderate loadings (0.4–0.5) from Factor one and remaining metabolites from Factors two and three included Gln, GSH, Asc, Lac, PCho and GPC. The ratio of GPC/PCho is also shown. CRS induces a drop in Gln and PCho, which are both restored after LAC treatment. The GPC/PCho ratio is also lowered after LAC administration. One-way ANOVA followed by LSD Fisher post-hoc test, *p<0.05, n = 5–6 per group. (B) Correlation matrix between behavioral components and main metabolic targets of stress in the nucleus accumbens. Behavior included social interaction (SI) test, forced swim test (FST) and a composite behavior including both behaviors (Composite z-score). Each cell includes the Pearson’s correlation coefficient with the associated color scaling. (C) Scatter plot of behavioral despair and accumbal taurine. (D) Scatter plot of depressive-like behavior and accumbal GABA. *p<0.05, n = 16–18 per group.

Only Factor one was able to discriminate for stress and treatment response between high rank/vulnerable mice (Figure 4B; F2,13=7.04; p<0.01). Specifically, in high rank mice, CRS led to a reduction in factor one metabolites as compared to their non-stressed high rank counterparts (p<0.005). Importantly, LAC treatment reversed the effect of CRS on Factor one metabolic profile (p<0.05). Interestingly, we further found a significant negative correlation between the behavioral composite z-score for depressive-like behaviors in high rank mice and their Factor one metabolite levels (Figure 4C; R = −0.58, p<0.05). This finding supports a link between NAc metabolic profile and depression-like behaviors in stress-vulnerable and LAC-treated mice.

Then, we selected the metabolites from Factor one with loadings above 0.4 to perform specific analyses to assess differences between the three groups (i.e., controls, CRS and CRS + LAC) of high rank mice. In metabolites strongly loading in factor one, we found that the observed stress effect was mainly carried by changes in Tau (p<0.05), PCr (p<0.05), Glu (p<0.05), Asp (p<0.05) and NAA (p<0.05), while the stress-reversing effects of LAC were driven by Tau (p<0.05) and PCr (p<0.05) (Figure 5). Among the rest of metabolites that either loaded below 0.5 in Factor one or loaded in the other two factors (Figure 6A), we found that Gln and PCho were similarly reduced by CRS (p<0.05) and reversed by LAC treatment (p<0.05). Notably, the ratio of GPC over PCho, that is the level of degradation product of phospholipids over their precursor, respectively, was reduced by LAC (p<0.05). In further correlational analyses addressed to identify potentially relevant treatment targets, we found two negative correlations, one between immobility time in the FST and taurine levels (R = −0.52, p<0.05) and a second one between the composite behavioral z-score and GABA (R = −0.55, p<0.05) (Figure 6B–D).

Discussion

In this study, using in vivo 1H-MRS at 14 T, we identified a chronic stress-related metabolic signature in the NAc of high rank mice -identified as a group of high vulnerability to develop depressive-like behaviors- that was partially restored by LAC treatment. High rank animals were defined as those that following cohabitation in groups of 4 males, emerged as ranks 1 and 2 in the social confrontation tube test. Our results provide an in vivo metabolic basis for understanding of antidepressant-like properties of LAC and its protective effects against chronic stress.

When exposed to chronic social defeat (i.e., daily exposure and defeat by an aggressive mouse), high rank C57BL/6J mice living in tetrads are at higher risk of developing social avoidance (i.e., a depressive-like behavior) than low rank mice (Larrieu et al., 2017). Our results reinforce the view that social rank in mice predicts vulnerability to stress. In addition, given the non-social character of the CRS protocol, our results argue against the view that their vulnerability would be solely based upon loss of social status (Larrieu and Sandi, 2018). It is important to note that, under basal conditions (i.e., before stress application), higher rank is related to higher anxiety-like behaviors as indicated by their exploration of an elevated plus maze (but note that these animals did not differ from low rank in the time spent in the center of the open field, only in their latency to enter the zone), as high anxiety trait is a well-established risk factor to develop stress-related depressive behaviors (Sandi et al., 2008; Castro et al., 2012; for reviews, see Sandi and Richter-Levin (2009); Russo et al., 2012; Weger and Sandi, 2018). However, a note of caution should be added as previous work suggests that whereas dominant mice tend to display more novelty-related exploratory behavior than less dominant mice, differences in anxiety-related behaviors seem to be less consistent (see Varholick et al., 2018, and references herein). We would also like to emphasize that we find a high reliability and linearity of social rank assessed under our experimental conditions that involve at least 5 weeks of cohabitation prior to carrying out the social confrontation tube test (see also Larrieu et al., 2017). This characterization may determine a different phenotype than studies in which social hierarchy is established within the first 2–3 weeks of cohabitation. Indeed, a recent study showed that during early cohabitation, dominance ranks of mice changed with repeated measurement, but became more stable between the 2nd and 3rd week of testing (Varholick et al., 2018). An additional issue to consider is that, for our analyses, we have grouped ranks 1 and 2 as high rank mice and ranks 3 and 4 as low rank. Although this grouping allows revealing statistical differences in the evaluated variables, each of the ranks in a tetrad home cage hierarchy may in fact lead to idiosyncratic phenotypes. In the future, it would be important to address vulnerability to stress for each specific rank in the colony.

LAC supplementation during the last week of the 3 week CRS protocol was efficient to protect high rank/vulnerable mice from the development of enhanced passive coping responses (i.e., higher floating levels) in the FST. Importantly, LAC levels are markedly reduced in patients with major depressive disorder, particularly in those with treatment-resistant depression and higher reported rates of early life stress in the form of childhood trauma (Nasca et al., 2018). Our results in the FST are in line with previous rodent studies showing the ability of LAC to reduce immobility time in this test (Wang et al., 2015; Bigio et al., 2016; Lau et al., 2017; Pulvirenti et al., 1990) with several small clinical trials in humans reporting effectiveness of LAC treatment in amelioration of depressive symptoms (Martinotti et al., 2011; Pettegrew et al., 2002; Zanardi and Smeraldi, 2006; Wang et al., 2014; Pettegrew et al., 2000). Growing evidence showed the ability of LAC to ameliorate both social interaction and social avoidance at the chronic restraint stress and social defeat stress paradigms in mice with baseline anxiety-like behavior, increased systemic inflammation and decreased hippocampal volume (Lau et al., 2017; Nasca et al., 2019). Thus, the lack of LAC efficiency in reversing stress-induced deficits in social avoidance in mice with different social hierarchy range in the current study pave the way for future research in understading whether the social rank based on the SCTT relate to a mouse' behavior in a light-dark test that probably gets at the same anxiety versus resilient traits. It will be important to study whether social dominance and light dark traits co-occur in the same mice or whether these traits define distinct susceptible phenotypes that may show a differential responssivness to treatments. Moreover, the succesful LAC-induced reversal of increased floating in the FST would fit well with a view that the individual’s NAc metabolic millieu is critically relevant for the animal’s engagement in energetically-costly behaviors (van der Kooij et al., 2018b). Accordingly, depressive-like behaviors which are more fundamentally dependent on resources mobilization would be more likely to be reversed by LAC’s energy support. Restoring normal metabolic function is, thus, more likely to affect behavior in the context set by the FST – that is fighting against an inescapable situation. This view fits well with the particular focus of this study on energy metabolism in the NAc. This postulate is supported by previous studies that showed that pharmacological manipulations leading to either impaired or boosted mitochondrial function in the nucleus accumbens were found to boost energetically-costly high rank behaviors during a social competition test between two male rats (Hollis et al., 2015; van der Kooij et al., 2018a).

Active coping in the FST depends upon dopamine actions in the NAc (Tye et al., 2013; de Kloet and Molendijk, 2016) and LAC has been shown to increase DA release in vivo (Harsing et al., 1992; Tolu et al., 2002) and to prevent a chronic stress-induced decrease in DA output in the NAc shell (Masi et al., 2003). In addition, stress has been shown to induce alterations in the accumbal oxidative stress system (Della et al., 2012; Ignácio et al., 2017), and LAC treatment to exert neuroprotective effects by inhibition of glial activation and oxidative stress in the striatum in an animal model of DAergic neuron damage (Singh et al., 2016). Furthermore, lipid peroxidation has been shown to be increased by immobilization stress in the rat striatum and L-carnitine to reduce associated striatal lipid peroxidation (Méndez-Cuesta et al., 2011). Interestingly, both in rodents (Méndez-Cuesta et al., 2011) and in zebrafish (Marcon et al., 2019), LAC was effective to reverse lipid peroxidation damage in stressed animals while being devoid of effect in controls.

Our in vivo 1H-MRS at 14 T identified key metabolites implicated in the response to chronic restrain stress and LAC treatment in the NAc. Among the nine metabolites loading highly in FA Factor one, five (i.e., taurine, phosphocreatine, glutamate, aspartate and NAA) were reduced by stress in high rank/vulnerable mice and two of them (i.e., taurine and phosphocreatine) reversed by LAC treatment. Taurine is a sulfur containing amino acid, with no involvement in protein synthesis, but with several functions ranging from antioxidant, signaling molecule and osmolyte (Hansen et al., 2006; Yang et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2016; Jamshidzadeh et al., 2017). Brain taurine concentrations have been shown to be reduced by chronic stress (Barbosa Neto et al., 2012) and hyperglycemic conditions (Malone et al., 2008). Although in our study we did not find stress-related changes in animals’ glycemic state following CRS, the repeated stress schedule implemented in our CRS protocol is known to lead to increased blood glucose levels with every daily stress manipulation (e.g., van der Kooij et al., 2018b). It will be important to address whether this is the mechanism that leads to the reduction in accumbal taurine levels observed in our study, as well as exploring how metabolite changes induced by stress and LAC relate to mitochondrial function. Strikingly, in an early study, Sershen et al. (1991) specifically observed a reversal by LAC of ageing-induced reductions in taurine in the striatum, not of any other amino acid in the striatum or of taurine or other amino acids in any other brain region. Importantly, in our study, we also found that NAc taurine levels were the only ones from the measured metabolites that negatively correlated with passive coping responses in the FST, reinforcing the link between levels of this amino acid and energetically-costly coping responses to adversity. In a recent 7T 1H-MRS in humans, we have recently found a negative correlation between trait anxiety and NAc taurine content (Strasser et al., 2019). Given the high link between trait anxiety and vulnerability to depression (Sandi and Richter-Levin, 2009; Weger and Sandi, 2018), our results support the interest in investigating the causal link between NAc taurine and its potential antidepressant actions.

Our finding that LAC increased phosphocreatine levels in high rank/vulnerable stressed mice is consistent with former reports indicating that LAC treatment increases phosphocreatine in the brain (Castro et al., 2012; Smeland et al., 2012; Aureli et al., 1994; Aureli et al., 1990; Hansen et al., 2006). Accordingly, LAC treatment likely improves the capacity of the brain to produce high-energy phosphates, which may be highly beneficial under conditions of disturbed energy metabolism. Several mechanisms have been implicated in the energy-boosting effects of LAC, many of them relating to an increased oxidative capacity of mitochondria through the direct release of oxidable fuel from LAC itself, or indirectly, in avoiding substrate inhibition of pyruvate dehydrogenase (PDH) by excess of AcCoA (Smeland et al., 2012; Broderick et al., 1992; Panchal et al., 2015; Virmani et al., 1995). Our results are in line with the idea of a restored mitochondrial function and support by LAC, visible through the increase in PCr, as well as taurine.

In addition, we observed that LAC restored levels of PCho and the ratio of GPC/PCho, which are disrupted by stress as well in high rank mice. PCho serves as a precursor of phosphatidylcholine (PtdCho), one of the main brain phospholipids, while GPC is its degradation product (Morash et al., 1988). The GPC/PCho ratio is thus considered to reflect the membrane turnover, typically increased in the case of neurodegeneration or excitotoxicity (Nitsch et al., 1992; Kristián and Siesjö, 1998). Increase in GPC can only arise from increased phospholipase activity, which is frequent during excitotoxic events and has been proposed to be a consequence of astrocyte activation (Klein, 2000; Ha et al., 2014). LAC and its deacetylated form L-carnitine (LC) are endogenous metabolites involved mainly in the transport and beta-oxidation of lipids. Exchange of LC with LAC and other acylcarnitines trough carnitine-acylcarnitine translocase (CACT) allows a bidirectional flow from cytoplasm into the inner mitochondrial matrix membrane for lipid oxidation. LAC supplementation could thus have a positive effect on phospholipid metabolism, by restoring normal balance between lipid degradation and synthesis.

A role for astrocytes in the LAC mechanism of action is also suggested by the normalization of stress-induced decreases in Gln content observed in the LAC-treated high rank group. Gln is mostly abundant in astrocytes (typically 80% of total concentration) due to their specific expression of glutamine synthetase (GS), which plays a key role in glutamate recycling at the synapse (Norenberg and Martinez-Hernandez, 1979; Bak et al., 2006). Astrocytic function is fundamental in the resilience to stress and regulation of extrasynaptic glutamate homeostasis (Pellerin and Magistretti, 1994; Nasca et al., 2017). For instance, LAC has shown effective control of astroglial cystine-glutamate exchanger (xCT), which is thought to improve mGlu2 function in hippocampus as a response to stress. A similar transcriptional response could be expected for glial GS, given its established responsiveness to glucocorticoids in stress (Rozovsky et al., 1995; Carter et al., 2013), an effect that could underlie the observed Gln changes. Even though the acetyl moiety of LAC has been shown to be utilized for the build-up of metabolic neurotransmitters synthesized from the TCA cycle (Kuratsune et al., 2002), LAC treatment in our study did not restore the stress-induced decrease of Glu, NAA and Asp observed in the nucleus accumbens of the high rank mice. This would suggest that LAC effects on these metabolites within the NAc may be secondary and part of an indirect and slower process. Nevertheless, as NAA, Glu and Asp measured with MRS reflect mainly neuronal metabolism (Van den Berg et al., 1969; Bhakoo, 2012), we can hypothesize that astrocytes are the first beneficiary of LAC supplementation. Astrocytes are indeed specifically shaped to uptake blood fatty acids, ketone bodies and acetate, and presumably LAC (Blázquez et al., 1998; Valdebenito et al., 2016; Rae et al., 2012).

Altogether, our findings highlight an accumbal metabolic signature for vulnerability to stress and response treatment. By implying an accumbal energy- and membrane metabolism process underlying the behavioral outcome, our study identifies molecular candidates responding in opposite direction to chronic stress and LAC treatment, opening possible mechanistic pathways underlying the anti-depressant-like effect of LAC. In particular, we underscore a strong association between NAc taurine and coping behaviors in an energetically-costly adversity task as well as antidepressant LAC actions. However, it is important to note that our study of LAC effectiveness circumscribed to high rank mice. In the future, it will be important to establish whether LAC treatment would be effective to overcome emotional changes induced by a chronic stress regime capable of affecting low rank individuals and whether LAC treatment could be more effective in the high rank mice because of differential endogenous levels of LAC based on social hierarchy.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type (species)
or resource
Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional
information
Strain, strain background (Mus musculus) Mouse: C57BL/6J Charles River Laboratories Crl:C57BL6/J Male
Chemical compound, drug Acetyl-L-carnitine Sigma Aldrich CAS Number:5080-50-2
Software, algorithm Matlab v.9.6 The MathWorks RRID:SCR_001622
Software, algorithm Observer 11.0 Noldus, Information Technology RRID:SCR_004074
Software, algorithm Ethovision 11.0 XT Noldus, Information Technology RRID:SCR_000441
Software, algorithm Prism 6 GrahpPad RRID:SCR_002798
Software, algorithm LCModel LCModel RRID:SCR_014455
Software, algorithm SPSS version 21 IBM https://www.ibm.com/analytics/fr/fr/technology/spss/

Animals

Six-week-old male C57BL/6J mice were purchased from Charles River Laboratories and, upon arrival, they were housed in groups of four per cage and allowed to acclimate to the animal facility for one week. Mice were weighed at arrival and monitored throughout the experiments. Cages consisted in standard Plexiglass filter-top cages in a temperature (23 ± 1°C) and humidity (40%) controlled environment with normal 12 hr day-light cycle. Animals had ad libitum access to water and standard rodent chow diet. All experiments were performed with the approval of the Cantonal Veterinary Authorities (Vaud, Switzerland) and carried out in accordance with the European Communities Council Directive of 24 November 1986 (86/609EEC).

Experimental design

One week after arrival, mice were tested for their anxiety and locomotor behaviors in an EPM and OF (Figure 1A). After four weeks of cohabitation, a SCTT was used to reveal individual ranks within the home cage tetrad (Larrieu et al., 2017). Subsequently, one group of the animals was subjected to a CRS protocol for 21 days, while the remaining non-stressed animals were submitted to daily handling and body weighting. The impact of chronic stress on behavior was tested in the SI test (CRS day 20). The experiment performed to investigate the ability of LAC treatment on behavioral and metabolic outcomes of CRS, included an additional group treated with LAC from CRS day 15. In addition to the SI test, animals were tested in FST (CRS day 21). Subsequently, 1H-MRS was performed at the end of the protocol, 24 hr after the FST (day 22).

Elevated plus maze test

Animals were placed into a maze made from black PVC with a white floor. The apparatus consisted of an elevated central platform (5 × 5 cm2) at 65 cm from the ground, from which four opposing arms extended. Two of the arms were open (30 × 5 cm2) and lit with 14–15 lx while the two others were closed (30 × 5 × 14 cm3) with reduced light intensity 3–4 lx. Animals were introduced in the maze facing the wall at the end of closed arms and left freely moving for 5 min. The mice were video-recorded from above the arena and tracking analyses performed with the Ethovision 11.0 XT software (Noldus, Information Technology) to determine the time spent in open and closed arms.

Open field test

The OF consisted of a rectangular arena (50 × 50 × 40 cm3) illuminated with dimmed light (30 lx). Mice were introduced near the wall of the arena and allowed to explore for 10 min. Analyses were performed using a tracking software (Ethovision 11.0 XT, Noldus, Information Technology) by drawing a virtual zone (15 × 15 cm2) in the center of the arena defined as the anxiogenic area. Several parameters were analyzed, including the total distance travelled and the time spent in the different zones.

Social confrontation tube test

The SCTT test was performed as previously described (Larrieu et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2011). Mice were housed together in groups of four during 5 weeks prior to the test to allow enough time for the social hierarchy to be stabilized in the home cage. First, each mouse was independently habituated to cross over a plastic tube (Plexiglas of 3 × 30 cm2, diameter x length) on several trials in two consecutive days. Then, evaluation of social rank took place during eight consecutive days. Specifically, two mice were smoothly guided by the tail to get into each side of the tube for a pairwise confrontation. Once the two mice reached the middle of the tube, the tail was released, and the time spent was recorded until one the mice (the most subordinate) retracted out from the tube. The four mice from the same cage were opposed using a round-robin design that led to six face-to-face trials per day. The tube was cleaned with 70% ethanol and dried after each session. The designation of a winner after each of the six possible pairs of confrontations per cage allowed to rank each mouse by its winning times, varying between 0–3. This value was then divided by three and multiplied by 100 to directly obtain the winning percentage. This percentage was then used to determine the index of dominance ranging from 1 (highest winning percentage, that is highest rank) to 4 (lowest winning percentage, that is lowest rank) for each cage. In order to reflect the specificity of the observed dominant and subordinate behavior to the SCTT (Varholick et al., 2018), animals with highest index of dominance (ranks 1 and 2) are referred to as ‘high rank’ herein, whilst animals with lowest index of dominance (ranks 3 and 4) are referred to as ‘low rank’.

Chronic restraint stress

This protocol involved 21 days of chronic retrain stress (CRS) and was adapted from Lau et al. (2017); Nasca et al. (2015). Animals were introduced head first into a 50 ml Falcon tube (11.5 cm in length; diameter of 3 cm) in which the cap was removed. Each restrain tube contained 3 0.4 cm air holes to allow the air to reach the nose of the mouse. Paper was added at the other extremity to adjust the physical constraint to the mouse body size and allowing the tail to reach the open space. The mice were subjected to this restrained environment for two consecutive hours every day for a period of 21 days. Control mice were left undisturbed in their home cage except for handling and body weighting each day for 21 days.

Social avoidance test (social interaction test)

Each animal was introduced into a 40 × 40 × 30 cm white arena containing an unfamiliar old breeder CD1 male mouse (social target) confined in a cylindrical drum with wire mesh placed near one of the arena walls. The test consists in two phases. First, the experimental mouse was allowed to freely explore for 2.5 min the arena when the social target was absent (the arena contained only the drum). Then, the target mouse was introduced in the drum for a 2.5 min interaction session. A social avoidance score was calculated as previously described in Larrieu et al. (2017). The mice were video-recorded from above the arena and tracking analyses performed with the Ethovision 11.0 XT software (Noldus, Information Technology).

Forced swim test

Each animal was introduced into a cylinder (15 cm diameter, 28 cm in height) filled with 5 L 25°C tap water. The level of water was sufficiently high to avoid any contact of the mouse with the bottom of the enclosure and low enough to avoid any possible escape. Animal’s motion was tracked with a camera positioned on top of the setup and recorded for 6 min. Immobility time was quantified using the Observer XT software (Noldus, Information Technology).

Composite behavior

Using MATLAB (Version 9.6, The MathsWorks Inc, Natick, MA), a behavioral composite z-score was calculated by averaging the z-score of the social avoidance score in the SI test and the z-score of the % time spent immobile in the FST. Both z-scores were calculated with the MATLAB function normalize, using the option argument zscore, which divides the difference between each sample and the sample average by the sample’s standard deviation.

Acetyl-L-carnitine treatment

LAC was purchased from (Sigma Aldrich). Mice received LAC in the drinking water at a concentration of 0.3%. The treatment started on day 15 of the 21 day-CRS protocol and continued till the end of the experiment. Control groups received regular tap water available ad libitum. In order to maximize the potential therapeutic effects of LAC on the stress-induced depressive-like behavior of mice (Nasca et al., 2013), a 7 day treatment was preferred over a previously reported 3 day protocol (Lau et al., 2017; Nasca et al., 2017). One bottle per cage of 4 mice was used during LAC treatment. Liquid consumption was monitored every day and analyzed following normalization according to the body weight of the four animals in the cage.

1H-magnetic resonance spectroscopy

In vivo spectroscopy experiments targeting the NAc were performed in anesthetized mice as previously described (Larrieu et al., 2017). Animals were monitored for body temperature (rectal probe and circulating water bath) and respiration (small animal monitor system: SA Instruments Inc, New York, NY, USA) under 1.3–1.5% isoflurane anesthesia mixed with 50% air and 50% O2. Physiological parameters were maintained at 36.5 ± 0.4°C and breathing rate ranged between 70–100 rpm. Animals were scanned in a horizontal 14.1T/26 cm Varian magnet (Agilent Inc, USA) with a homemade 1H surface coil. A set of Fast Spin Echo (FSE) images of the brain was acquired for localizing the Volume of Interest (VOI) of the 1H-MRS scan. Acquisition was done using the spin echo full intensity acquired localized (SPECIAL) sequence (Mlynárik et al., 2006) in the VOI of 1.4 × 4.1 × 1.2 mm3 (TE/TR = 2.8/4000 ms) including the bilateral NAc after field homogeneity adjustment with FAST(EST)MAP (Gruetter and Tkác, 2000). The obtained spectra (20 × 16 averages) were then frequency corrected, summed and quantified using LCModel (Provencher, 2001). Full width at half maximum (FWHM) was used as the output of the LCModel. Concentrations were referenced to the water signal and fitting quality assessed using Cramer-Rao lower bounds errors (CRLB) (Cavassila et al., 2001). A CRLB value below 20% was used as cutoff for high concentration metabolites, while low concentration metabolites were not considered reliable above a CRLB of 50%.

Statistics

All values are represented as mean ± SEM. Results from EPM and OF were analyzed using unpaired Student t-tests. Results from the SCTT were analyzed with one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), with social rank as fixed factor, followed by a Bonferroni corrected post hoc test when appropriate. Behaviors in the SI test and FST were analyzed with a two-way ANOVA, using stress and social rank as fixed factors. In the LAC experiment, behavioral and spectroscopy results were analyzed using one-way ANOVA. Cumulative weight gain was analyzed using a repeated measure two-way ANOVA with time and group as fixed factors. Analyses were followed by Bonferroni post hoc correction when appropriate. Correlations analyses were performed using a Pearson’s correlation coefficient. All statistical tests were performed with GraphPad Prism (GraphPad software, San Diego, CA, USA) using a critical probability of p<0.05. Statistical analyses performed for each experiment are summarized in each figure legend indicating the statistical test used, sample size (‘n’), as well as degree of freedom, F and P values.

Factor analysis

Factor analysis was used as previously described (Larrieu et al., 2017) using IBM SPSS Statistics version 21 to allow statistical tests using the metabolite’s latent variables as dependent variables in NAc. A linear combination of the dependent variables is generated in order to reduce the noise caused by the high number of variables. Missing values were avoided by using mean value imputation before the computation of correlation matrices, to ensure positive definiteness. A total of three factors was chosen for the NAc after analyzing the scree plots, using principal axis factoring. This resulted in a total of variance explained of 52% without rotation and omitting coefficients below 0.4.

Acknowledgements

This project has been supported by grants from the Swiss National Science Foundation [31003A-152614 and −176206; NCCR Synapsy (51NF40-158776 and −185897)], the European Union’s Seventh Framework Program for research, technological development and demonstration under grant agreement no. 603016 (MATRICS), the EPFL-Jebsen Research Program and intramural funding from the EPFL to CS. The funding sources had no additional role in study design, in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data, in the writing of the report or in the decision to submit the paper for publication. This paper reflects only the authors’ views and the European Union is not liable for any use that may be made of the information contained therein.1H-MRS experiments were also supported financially by the Center for Biomedical Imaging (CIBM) of the University of Lausanne (UNIL), University of Geneva (UNIGE), Geneva University Hospital (HUG), Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV), Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (EPFL) and the Leenaards and Louis‐Jeantet Foundations.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Antoine Cherix, Email: antoine.cherix@epfl.ch.

Carmen Sandi, Email: carmen.sandi@epfl.ch.

Harm J Krugers, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands.

Christian Büchel, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Swiss National Science Foundation 31003A-152614 to Carmen Sandi.

  • Swiss National Science Foundation 31003A-176206 to Carmen Sandi.

  • Swiss National Science Foundation NCCR Synapsy 51NF40-158776 to Carmen Sandi.

  • Swiss National Science Foundation NCCR Synapsy 51NF40-185897 to Carmen Sandi.

  • EU Seventh Framework Programme 603016 to Carmen Sandi.

  • École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne Jebsen Research Program to Carmen Sandi.

  • Center for Biomedical Imaging to Rolf Gruetter.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Investigation.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation.

Conceptualization, Supervision.

Conceptualization, Supervision.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Project administration.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experiments were performed with the approval of the Cantonal Veterinary Authorities (Vaud, Switzerland) and carried out in accordance with the European Communities Council Directive of 24 November 1986 (86/609EEC).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.

References

  1. Andreazza AC, Nierenberg AA. Mitochondrial dysfunction: at the core of psychiatric disorders? Biological Psychiatry. 2018;83:718–719. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Atif F, Yousuf S, Agrawal SK. Restraint stress-induced oxidative damage and its amelioration with selenium. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2008;600:59–63. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2008.09.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aureli T, Miccheli A, Ricciolini R, Di Cocco ME, Ramacci MT, Angelucci L, Ghirardi O, Conti F. Aging brain: effect of acetyl-L-carnitine treatment on rat brain energy and phospholipid metabolism. A study by 31P and 1H NMR spectroscopy. Brain Research. 1990;526:108–112. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90255-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aureli T, Miccheli A, Di Cocco ME, Ghirardi O, Giuliani A, Ramacci MT, Conti F. Effect of acetyl-L-carnitine on recovery of brain phosphorus metabolites and lactic acid level during reperfusion after cerebral ischemia in the rat--study by 13P- and 1H-NMR spectroscopy. Brain Research. 1994;643:92–99. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90013-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bak LK, Schousboe A, Waagepetersen HS. The glutamate/GABA-glutamine cycle: aspects of transport, neurotransmitter homeostasis and Ammonia transfer. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2006;98:641–653. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.03913.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barbosa Neto JB, Tiba PA, Faturi CB, de Castro-Neto EF, da Graça Naffah-Mazacoratti M, de Jesus Mari J, de Mello MF, Suchecki D. Stress during development alters anxiety-like behavior and hippocampal neurotransmission in male and female rats. Neuropharmacology. 2012;62:518–526. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bhakoo KK. N-Acetyl-Aspartate (NAA) Metabolism. In: Choi I. Y, Gruetter R, editors. Neural Metabolism in Vivo. Boston: Springer; 2012. pp. 1075–1093. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bigio B, Mathé AA, Sousa VC, Zelli D, Svenningsson P, McEwen BS, Nasca C. Epigenetics and energetics in ventral Hippocampus mediate rapid antidepressant action: implications for treatment resistance. PNAS. 2016;113:7906–7911. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1603111113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Blázquez C, Sánchez C, Velasco G, Guzmán M. Role of carnitine palmitoyltransferase I in the control of ketogenesis in primary cultures of rat astrocytes. Journal of Neurochemistry. 1998;71:1597–1606. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1998.71041597.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Broderick TL, Arthur QH, Lopaschuk Gary D. Carnitine stimulation of glucose oxidation in the fatty. The Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1992;267:3758–3763. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carter BS, Hamilton DE, Thompson RC. Acute and chronic glucocorticoid treatments regulate astrocyte-enriched mRNAs in multiple brain regions in vivo. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2013;7:139. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Castro JE, Diessler S, Varea E, Márquez C, Larsen MH, Cordero MI, Sandi C. Personality traits in rats predict vulnerability and resilience to developing stress-induced depression-like behaviors, HPA Axis hyper-reactivity and brain changes in pERK1/2 activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1209–1223. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cavassila S, Deval S, Huegen C, van Ormondt D, Graveron-Demilly D. Cramér-Rao bounds: an evaluation tool for quantitation. NMR in Biomedicine. 2001;14:278–283. doi: 10.1002/nbm.701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. de Kloet ER, Joëls M, Holsboer F. Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005;6:463–475. doi: 10.1038/nrn1683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. de Kloet ER, Molendijk ML. Coping with the forced swim stressor: towards understanding an adaptive mechanism. Neural Plasticity. 2016;2016:1–13. doi: 10.1155/2016/6503162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Della FP, Abelaira HM, Réus GZ, Antunes AR, Dos Santos MA, Zappelinni G, Steckert AV, Vuolo F, Galant LS, Dal-Pizzol F, Kapczinski F, Quevedo J. Tianeptine exerts neuroprotective effects in the brain tissue of rats exposed to the chronic stress model. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2012;103:395–402. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2012.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Duarte JM, Lei H, Mlynárik V, Gruetter R. The neurochemical profile quantified by in vivo 1H NMR spectroscopy. NeuroImage. 2012;61:342–362. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Duclot F, Kabbaj M. Individual differences in novelty seeking predict subsequent vulnerability to social defeat through a differential epigenetic regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor expression. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:11048–11060. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0199-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ferreira GC, McKenna MC. L-Carnitine and Acetyl-L-carnitine roles and neuroprotection in developing brain. Neurochemical Research. 2017;42:1661–1675. doi: 10.1007/s11064-017-2288-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Francis TC, Lobo MK. Emerging role for nucleus accumbens medium spiny neuron subtypes in depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2017;81:645–653. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Friedrich MJ. Depression is the leading cause of disability around the world. Jama. 2017;317:1517. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.3826. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gruetter R, Tkác I. Field mapping without reference scan using asymmetric echo-planar techniques. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2000;43:319–323. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1522-2594(200002)43:2&#x0003c;319::AID-MRM22&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ha JS, Dho SH, Youm TH, Kwon KS, Park SS. Astrocytic phospholipase A2 contributes to neuronal glutamate toxicity. Brain Research. 2014;1590:97–106. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hansen SH, Andersen ML, Birkedal H, Cornett C, Wibrand F. The important role of taurine in oxidative metabolism. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology. 2006;583:129–135. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-33504-9_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Harsing LG, Sershen H, Toth E, Hashim A, Ramacci MT, Lajtha A. Acetyl-L-carnitine releases dopamine in rat corpus striatum: an in vivo microdialysis study. European Journal of Pharmacology. 1992;218:117–121. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(92)90154-V. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hollis F, van der Kooij MA, Zanoletti O, Lozano L, Cantó C, Sandi C. Mitochondrial function in the brain links anxiety with social subordination. PNAS. 2015;112:15486–15491. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1512653112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ignácio ZM, Réus GZ, Abelaira HM, de Moura AB, de Souza TG, Matos D, Goldim MP, Mathias K, Garbossa L, Petronilho F, Quevedo J. Acute and chronic treatment with quetiapine induces antidepressant-like behavior and exerts antioxidant effects in the rat brain. Metabolic Brain Disease. 2017;32:1195–1208. doi: 10.1007/s11011-017-0028-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jamshidzadeh A, Heidari R, Abasvali M, Zarei M, Ommati MM, Abdoli N, Khodaei F, Yeganeh Y, Jafari F, Zarei A, Latifpour Z, Mardani E, Azarpira N, Asadi B, Najibi A. Taurine treatment preserves brain and liver mitochondrial function in a rat model of fulminant hepatic failure and hyperammonemia. Biomedicine & Pharmacotherapy. 2017;86:514–520. doi: 10.1016/j.biopha.2016.11.095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kim Y, Vadodaria KC, Lenkei Z, Kato T, Gage FH, Marchetto MC, Santos R. Mitochondria, metabolism, and redox mechanisms in psychiatric disorders. Antioxidants & Redox Signaling. 2019;31:275–317. doi: 10.1089/ars.2018.7606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Klein J. Membrane breakdown in acute and chronic neurodegeneration: focus on choline-containing phospholipids. Journal of Neural Transmission. 2000;107:1027–1063. doi: 10.1007/s007020070051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kristián T, Siesjö BK. Calcium in ischemic cell death. Stroke. 1998;29:705–718. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.29.3.705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kuratsune H, Yamaguti K, Lindh G, Evengård B, Hagberg G, Matsumura K, Iwase M, Onoe H, Takahashi M, Machii T, Kanakura Y, Kitani T, Långström B, Watanabe Y. Brain regions involved in fatigue sensation: reduced acetylcarnitine uptake into the brain. NeuroImage. 2002;17:1256–1265. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Larrieu T, Cherix A, Duque A, Rodrigues J, Lei H, Gruetter R, Sandi C. Hierarchical status predicts behavioral vulnerability and nucleus accumbens metabolic profile following chronic social defeat stress. Current Biology. 2017;27:2202–2210. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Larrieu T, Sandi C. Stress‐induced depression: is social rank a predictive risk factor? BioEssays. 2018;40:1800012. doi: 10.1002/bies.201800012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lau T, Bigio B, Zelli D, McEwen BS, Nasca C. Stress-induced structural plasticity of medial amygdala stellate neurons and rapid prevention by a candidate antidepressant. Molecular Psychiatry. 2017;22:227–234. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Malone JI, Hanna S, Saporta S, Mervis RF, Park CR, Chong L, Diamond DM. Hyperglycemia not hypoglycemia alters neuronal dendrites and impairs spatial memory. Pediatric Diabetes. 2008;9:531–539. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5448.2008.00431.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Marcon M, Mocelin R, de Oliveira DL, da Rosa Araujo AS, Herrmann AP, Piato A. Acetyl-L-carnitine as a putative candidate for the treatment of stress-related psychiatric disorders: novel evidence from a zebrafish model. Neuropharmacology. 2019;150:145–152. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Martinotti G, Andreoli S, Reina D, Di Nicola M, Ortolani I, Tedeschi D, Fanella F, Pozzi G, Iannoni E, D'Iddio S, Prof LJ. Acetyl-l-Carnitine in the treatment of anhedonia, melancholic and negative symptoms in alcohol dependent subjects. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2011;35:953–958. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Masi F, Leggio B, Nanni G, Scheggi S, De Montis MG, Tagliamonte A, Grappi S, Gambarana C. Effects of long-term acetyl-L-carnitine administration in rats--II: protection against the disrupting effect of stress on the acquisition of appetitive behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:683–693. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. McEwen BS, Bowles NP, Gray JD, Hill MN, Hunter RG, Karatsoreos IN, Nasca C. Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:1353–1363. doi: 10.1038/nn.4086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Ménard C, Pfau ML, Hodes GE, Russo SJ. Immune and neuroendocrine mechanisms of stress vulnerability and resilience. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42:62–80. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Méndez-Cuesta LA, Márquez-Valadez B, Pérez-De la Cruz V, Maldonado PD, Santana RA, Escobar-Briones C, Galván-Arzate S, Carrillo-Mora P, Santamaría A. Early changes in oxidative stress markers in a rat model of acute stress: effect of l-carnitine on the striatum. Basic & Clinical Pharmacology & Toxicology. 2011;109:123–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-7843.2011.00691.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Menke A. Precision pharmacotherapy: psychiatry's future direction in preventing, diagnosing, and treating mental disorders. Pharmacogenomics and Personalized Medicine. 2018;11:211–222. doi: 10.2147/PGPM.S146110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mlynárik V, Gambarota G, Frenkel H, Gruetter R. Localized short-echo-time proton MR spectroscopy with full signal-intensity acquisition. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2006;56:965–970. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Mlynárik V, Cudalbu C, Xin L, Gruetter R. 1h NMR spectroscopy of rat brain in vivo at 14.1Tesla: improvements in quantification of the neurochemical profile. Journal of Magnetic Resonance. 2008;194:163–168. doi: 10.1016/j.jmr.2008.06.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Morash SC, Cook HW, Spence MW. Phosphatidylcholine metabolism in cultured cells: catabolism via glycerophosphocholine. Biochimica Et Biophysica Acta (BBA) - Lipids and Lipid Metabolism. 1988;961:194–202. doi: 10.1016/0005-2760(88)90114-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Morava E, Kozicz T. Mitochondria and the economy of stress (mal)adaptation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;37:668–680. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Nasca C, Xenos D, Barone Y, Caruso A, Scaccianoce S, Matrisciano F, Battaglia G, Mathé AA, Pittaluga A, Lionetto L, Simmaco M, Nicoletti F. L-acetylcarnitine causes rapid antidepressant effects through the epigenetic induction of mGlu2 receptors. PNAS. 2013;110:4804–4809. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216100110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Nasca C, Bigio B, Zelli D, Nicoletti F, McEwen BS. Mind the gap: glucocorticoids modulate hippocampal glutamate tone underlying individual differences in stress susceptibility. Molecular Psychiatry. 2015;20:755–763. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Nasca C, Bigio B, Zelli D, de Angelis P, Lau T, Okamoto M, Soya H, Ni J, Brichta L, Greengard P, Neve RL, Lee FS, McEwen BS. Role of the astroglial glutamate exchanger xCT in ventral Hippocampus in resilience to stress. Neuron. 2017;96:402–413. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Nasca C, Bigio B, Lee FS, Young SP, Kautz MM, Albright A, Beasley J, Millington DS, Mathé AA, Kocsis JH, Murrough JW, McEwen BS, Rasgon N. Acetyl-l-carnitine deficiency in patients with major depressive disorder. PNAS. 2018;115:8627–8632. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1801609115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Nasca C, Menard C, Hodes G, Bigio B, Pena C, Lorsch Z, Zelli D, Ferris A, Kana V, Purushothaman I, Dobbin J, Nassim M, DeAngelis P, Merad M, Rasgon N, Meaney M, Nestler EJ, McEwen BS, Russo SJ. Multidimensional predictors of susceptibility and resilience to social defeat stress. Biological Psychiatry. 2019;86:483–491. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.06.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Nestler EJ, Barrot M, DiLeone RJ, Eisch AJ, Gold SJ, Monteggia LM. Neurobiology of depression. Neuron. 2002;34:13–25. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00653-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Nitsch RM, Blusztajn JK, Pittas AG, Slack BE, Growdon JH, Wurtman RJ. Evidence for a membrane defect in alzheimer disease brain. PNAS. 1992;89:1671–1675. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.5.1671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Norenberg MD, Martinez-Hernandez A. Fine structural localization of glutamine synthetase in astrocytes of rat brain. Brain Research. 1979;161:303–310. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90071-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Panchal SK, Poudyal H, Ward LC, Waanders J, Brown L. Modulation of tissue fatty acids by L-carnitine attenuates metabolic syndrome in diet-induced obese rats. Food & Function. 2015;6:2496–2506. doi: 10.1039/C5FO00480B. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Parikh S, Saneto R, Falk MJ, Anselm I, Cohen BH, Haas R, Medicine Society TM. A modern approach to the treatment of mitochondrial disease. Current Treatment Options in Neurology. 2009;11:414–430. doi: 10.1007/s11940-009-0046-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Pei L, Wallace DC. Mitochondrial etiology of neuropsychiatric disorders. Biological Psychiatry. 2018;83:722–730. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.11.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Pellerin L, Magistretti PJ. Glutamate uptake into astrocytes stimulates aerobic glycolysis: a mechanism coupling neuronal activity to glucose utilization. PNAS. 1994;91:10625–10629. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.22.10625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Pettegrew JW, Levine J, McClure RJ. Acetyl-L-carnitine physical-chemical, metabolic, and therapeutic properties: relevance for its mode of action in Alzheimer's disease and geriatric depression. Molecular Psychiatry. 2000;5:616–632. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pettegrew JW, Levine J, Gershon S, Stanley JA, Servan-Schreiber D, Panchalingam K, McClure RJ. 31P-MRS study of acetyl-L-carnitine treatment in geriatric depression: preliminary results. Bipolar Disorders. 2002;4:61–66. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-5618.2002.01180.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Picard M, McManus MJ, Gray JD, Nasca C, Moffat C, Kopinski PK, Seifert EL, McEwen BS, Wallace DC. Mitochondrial functions modulate neuroendocrine, metabolic, inflammatory, and transcriptional responses to acute psychological stress. PNAS. 2015;112:E6614–E6623. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515733112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Picard M, McEwen BS, Epel ES, Sandi C. An energetic view of stress: focus on mitochondria. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2018;49:72–85. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2018.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Provencher SW. Automatic quantitation of localized in vivo 1H spectra with LCModel. NMR in Biomedicine. 2001;14:260–264. doi: 10.1002/nbm.698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Pulvirenti G, Valerio C, Spadaro F, D'Agata V, Freni V, Nardo L, Drago F. Acetylcarnitine reduces the immobility of rats in a despair test (constrained swim) Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1990;54:110–114. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(90)91296-N. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Rae C, Fekete AD, Kashem MA, Nasrallah FA, Bröer S. Metabolism, compartmentation, transport and production of acetate in the cortical brain tissue slice. Neurochemical Research. 2012;37:2541–2553. doi: 10.1007/s11064-012-0847-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Richter-Levin G, Xu L. How could stress lead to major depressive disorder? IBRO Reports. 2018;4:38–43. doi: 10.1016/j.ibror.2018.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Neurobehavioural mechanisms of reward and motivation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1996;6:228–236. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80077-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Rozovsky I, Laping NJ, Krohn K, Teter B, O'Callaghan JP, Finch CE. Transcriptional regulation of glial fibrillary acidic protein by corticosterone in rat astrocytes in vitro is influenced by the duration of time in culture and by astrocyte-neuron interactions. Endocrinology. 1995;136:2066–2073. doi: 10.1210/endo.136.5.7720656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Russo SJ, Murrough JW, Han MH, Charney DS, Nestler EJ. Neurobiology of resilience. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:1475–1484. doi: 10.1038/nn.3234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Sandi C, Cordero MI, Ugolini A, Varea E, Caberlotto L, Large CH. Chronic stress-induced alterations in amygdala responsiveness and behavior--modulation by trait anxiety and corticotropin-releasing factor systems. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:1836–1848. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06451.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Sandi C, Richter-Levin G. From high anxiety trait to depression: a neurocognitive hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences. 2009;32:312–320. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Sershen H, Harsing LG, Banay-Schwartz M, Hashim A, Ramacci MT, Lajtha A. Effect of acetyl-L-carnitine on the dopaminergic system in aging brain. Journal of Neuroscience Research. 1991;30:555–559. doi: 10.1002/jnr.490300313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Singh S, Mishra A, Shukla S. ALCAR exerts neuroprotective and Pro-Neurogenic effects by inhibition of glial activation and oxidative stress via activation of the wnt/β-Catenin signaling in parkinsonian rats. Molecular Neurobiology. 2016;53:4286–4301. doi: 10.1007/s12035-015-9361-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Smeland OB, Meisingset TW, Borges K, Sonnewald U. Chronic acetyl-L-carnitine alters brain energy metabolism and increases noradrenaline and serotonin content in healthy mice. Neurochemistry International. 2012;61:100–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2012.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Strasser A, Xin L, Gruetter R, Sandi C. Nucleus accumbens neurochemistry in human anxiety: a 7 T 1H-MRS study. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;29:365–375. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Tolu P, Masi F, Leggio B, Scheggi S, Tagliamonte A, De Montis MG, Gambarana C. Effects of long-term acetyl-L-carnitine administration in rats: I. increased dopamine output in Mesocorticolimbic Areas and protection toward acute stress exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:410–420. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00306-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Turner RJ, Lloyd DA. Stress burden and the lifetime incidence of psychiatric disorder in young adults: racial and ethnic contrasts. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61:481–488. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.5.481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Tye KM, Mirzabekov JJ, Warden MR, Ferenczi EA, Tsai HC, Finkelstein J, Kim SY, Adhikari A, Thompson KR, Andalman AS, Gunaydin LA, Witten IB, Deisseroth K. Dopamine neurons modulate neural encoding and expression of depression-related behaviour. Nature. 2013;493:537–541. doi: 10.1038/nature11740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Valdebenito R, Ruminot I, Garrido-Gerter P, Fernández-Moncada I, Forero-Quintero L, Alegría K, Becker HM, Deitmer JW, Barros LF. Targeting of astrocytic glucose metabolism by beta-hydroxybutyrate. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism. 2016;36:1813–1822. doi: 10.1177/0271678X15613955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Van den Berg CJ, Krzalić L, Mela P, Waelsch H. Compartmentation of glutamate metabolism in brain. evidence for the existence of two different tricarboxylic acid cycles in brain. Biochemical Journal. 1969;113:281–290. doi: 10.1042/bj1130281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. van der Kooij MA, Jene T, Treccani G, Miederer I, Hasch A, Voelxen N, Walenta S, Müller MB. Chronic social stress-induced hyperglycemia in mice couples individual stress susceptibility to impaired spatial memory. PNAS. 2018a;115:E10187–E10196. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1804412115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. van der Kooij MA, Hollis F, Lozano L, Zalachoras I, Abad S, Zanoletti O, Grosse J, Guillot de Suduiraut I, Canto C, Sandi C. Diazepam actions in the VTA enhance social dominance and mitochondrial function in the nucleus accumbens by activation of dopamine D1 receptors. Molecular Psychiatry. 2018b;23:569–578. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Varholick JA, Bailoo JD, Palme R, Würbel H. Phenotypic variability between social dominance ranks in laboratory mice. Scientific Reports. 2018;8:6593. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24624-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Veronese N, Stubbs B, Solmi M, Ajnakina O, Carvalho AF, Maggi S. Acetyl-L-Carnitine supplementation and the treatment of depressive symptoms. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2018;80:154–159. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Virmani MA, Biselli R, Spadoni A, Rossi S, Corsico N, Calvani M, Fattorossi A, De Simone C, Arrigoni-Martelli E. Protective actions of L-carnitine and acetyl-L-carnitine on the neurotoxicity evoked by mitochondrial uncoupling or inhibitors. Pharmacological Research. 1995;32:383–389. doi: 10.1016/S1043-6618(05)80044-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wang F, Zhu J, Zhu H, Zhang Q, Lin Z, Hu H. Bidirectional control of social hierarchy by synaptic efficacy in medial prefrontal cortex. Science. 2011;334:693–697. doi: 10.1126/science.1209951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Wang S-M, Han C, Lee S-J, Patkar AA, Masand PS, Pae C-U. A review of current evidence for acetyl-l-carnitine in the treatment of depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2014;53:30–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Wang W, Lu Y, Xue Z, Li C, Wang C, Zhao X, Zhang J, Wei X, Chen X, Cui W, Wang Q, Zhou W. Rapid-acting antidepressant-like effects of acetyl-l-carnitine mediated by PI3K/AKT/BDNF/VGF signaling pathway in mice. Neuroscience. 2015;285:281–291. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.11.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Wang Q, Fan W, Cai Y, Wu Q, Mo L, Huang Z, Huang H. Protective effects of taurine in traumatic brain injury via mitochondria and cerebral blood flow. Amino Acids. 2016;48:2169–2177. doi: 10.1007/s00726-016-2244-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Weger M, Sandi C. High anxiety trait: a vulnerable phenotype for stress-induced depression. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2018;87:27–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Yang Y, Zhang Y, Liu X, Zuo J, Wang K, Liu W, Ge J. Exogenous taurine attenuates mitochondrial oxidative stress and endoplasmic reticulum stress in rat cardiomyocytes. Acta Biochimica Et Biophysica Sinica. 2013;45:359–367. doi: 10.1093/abbs/gmt034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Zanardi R, Smeraldi E. A double-blind, randomised, controlled clinical trial of acetyl-L-carnitine vs. amisulpride in the treatment of dysthymia. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;16:281–287. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2005.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Harm J Krugers1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

Depression is a common mental disorder that is a major cause of disability worldwide. This emphasizes the need to get a better understanding of the underlying mechanisms. While prolonged stress-exposure is an important risk factor for depression, not all individuals are vulnerable to develop depression after stress-exposure. Here, the authors tested in mice the relationship between social rank and the vulnerability to develop depressive like behaviour after exposure to chronic stress. The authors report in mice that high rank animals are more vulnerable to chronic stress-exposure in comparison to low rank animals. In these more sensitive mice, the levels of several energy-related metabolites in the nucleus accumbens were reduced. Interestingly, Acetyl L-carnitine – a mitochondrial-boosting supplement – administered via the drinking water was able to partially reduce behavioural effects and brain metabolic alterations after chronic stress and in high rank mice. This work illustrates a role for energy metabolism in the vulnerability for developing chronic-stress evoked depressive like behaviour.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Metabolic signature in nucleus accumbens for anti-depressant-like effects of acetyl-L-carnitine" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Christian Büchel as the Senior Editor.

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

In this manuscript, the authors report that dominant, but not subordinate, mice exhibit a vulnerable phenotype after chronic stress exposure. Dominant animals also expressed reduced levels of energy-related metabolites in the nucleus accumbens. Alterations in metabolic signature and behaviour were counteracted by treatment with Acetyl-L-Carnitine. While the reviewers generally find the study interesting, well-conducted and find the manuscript well written, they also raise a number of issues.

In particular, the reviewers ask you to consider statistics, details on experimental procedures and potential motor effects of Acteyl-L-Carnitine (Reviewer 1); Discuss and consider the definition of dominant/subordinate (Reviewer 2) and the forced swim test (Reviewer 3). Additional data might be helpful to address some of the reviewers’ issues.

Please address the issues raised by the reviewers in a point-by-point reply.

Reviewer #1:

In this paper, the authors test the hypothesis that chronic stress vulnerability and consequential development of depressive-like symptoms is linked to energy metabolic changes in the nucleus accumbens, which can be reversed by LAC supplementation. The authors based their hypothesis on previous results involving the NAc in a protocol to induce depressive-like symptoms based on chronic social defeat and preclinical studies using LAC to alleviate depressive-like symptoms. To test their hypothesis, they used a variant of the chronic stress protocol using restrain stress for 21 days, and administered LAC via water supplementation the last 7 days of the protocol. To assess the impact of both the stress and the treatment in depressive-like symptoms, they used a social avoidance and a forced swimming test, and they analyzed up to 20 metabolites using in vivo H-MRS related to mitochondrial energy metabolism, neurotransmission and lipid peroxidation.

The study is interesting, well conducted and well written. There are however a few considerations to be taken into account to improve the quality of the manuscript and the completeness of the data to support the conclusions.

1) Effects of LAC treatment on depressive-like behaviors in dominant and subordinate mice. Although the sample size of the data is small (n = 6-7), the experiments were able to replicate the predictability of the social status based on the anxiety phenotype and the vulnerability of the dominant mice to chronic-stress induced depressive-like deficits like social avoidance (Figure 1). Interestingly, they failed to see the basal difference in latency to immobility in the forced swimming test between dominant and subordinate male mice reported by previous published data (Horii et al., 2017). However, the trend between the control, CRS and CRS+LAC conditions is the same between dominant and subordinate males, as the reduction in the% of immobility appears to be the same between the CRS and the CRS+LAC dominant and subordinate groups. If the authors wish to report conclusions of their effect of the LAC treatment across dominant and subordinate mice (as they do in their Results and Discussion sections), they should carry on a two-way ANOVA with post-hoc analysis with social status and group condition as the variables in all of their behavioral tests (social avoidance, FST and composite behavior) (Figure 2).

2) Moreover, seeing that LAC supplementation appears to decrease% cumulative weight gain and% of immobility of the stressed mice, one could argue that the effect of LAC could be due to an increase in locomotor activity of the mice which, on one hand could be considered a treatment for depressed locomotor activity but on the other hand could also limit the therapeutic potential of the mitochondrial boost supplementation. While Lau and colleagues (2017) didn't see effects of LAC in immobility in non-stressed mice and no effect should be expected in the experiments presented in this article, I would suggest the authors to provide a graph with the total distance travelled in the FST as a supplementary figure to discard the motor effects of LAC or an extra open field test post treatment.

3) In terms of the LAC dose administration, the authors report to have used drinking water supplementation with a concentration of 0.3%. It is not clear, however, if at the time of administration, the animals were singled or grouped housed (in groups of 4 as during the cohabitation). If possible, a water intake report should be presented in order to report a clear dosage of the supplement per animal.

4) Changes in NAc metabolites in response to chronic stress and LAC supplementation

The use of in vivo H-MRS allows to explore multiple metabolites and neurochemical compounds at the same time in dominant and subordinate males, which englobe processes known to be affected by LAC like mitochondrial beta-oxidation, protein acetylation, lipid peroxidation and neurotransmitter composition. According to their results, chronic stress in dominant mice affects different metabolites related to mitochondrial transport and glutamatergic neurotransmission.

Interestingly, they saw a decrease in taurine levels in the CRS dominant group that was restored to control levels by LAC supplementation (Figure 5). While the authors discuss the negative correlation between NAc taurine levels and traits like anxiety and immobility, they don't discuss the surprising nature of their results. Taurine is a mitochondrial transport protein that is implicated in oxidative stress, and typically increased in situations that require high mitochondrial activity like chronic restrain stress and decreased by hyperglycemia. However, in these results, CRS decreases taurine levels without changes in glucose levels. Furthermore, this only is seen in dominant males. A deeper discussion of these results is recommended to accentuate the importance of the findings regarding the changes in taurine.

5) Regarding the main effects of LAC in correcting the defects in mitochondrial-related metabolites, an extra experiment measuring complex I-II activity in the NAc (following the experiments by the corresponding author in Hollis et al., 2015) would be appropriate to support the conclusions.

Reviewer #2:

Cherix and coworkers investigated the impact of acetyl-L-carnitine treatment on stress induced depression-like behavior in mice characterized according to the rank assessed in a tube test. The experiments are conducted to a high technical standard and the manuscript is very well written. Although the overall findings are interesting and novel (and this study would be of interest for the audience of eLife), there is one major and other specific concerns that should be addressed:

1) Even after reading several times this manuscript (and the previous Current Biology 2017 paper), I still struggle with the nomenclature used, i.e. dominant and subordinate, and how the individual rank is assessed. Below several concerns on this issue, and an overall recommendation to use a different nomenclature and avoid interpreting the data in favor of social status differences.

– I’m still unclear of what the Figure 1C is actually measuring, and how the index of dominance (Figure 1B) was calculated. The Materials and methods states "the time spent was recorded until one of the mouse (the most subordinate) retracted out from the tube". This definition assumes a priori that a mouse rapidly retracting from the tube is subordinate, and does not take into account what the other mouse in the test tube is actually doing (isn`t the purpose of this test to evaluate concomitantly the behavior of 2 mice to assess their relative rank?). I provide below several alternative (speculative) interpretations: i) high time spent in the tube implies extended social affiliation between 2 mice, and not social confrontation; ii) low time implies high fear of the tube per se, independently from the partner presence; iii) high time implies enhanced fear response manifested as freezing/immobility, again independently from the social interaction.

– The conclusion that the rank in the tube test is equivalent to social rank has not been confirmed with behavioral observations of agonistic interaction in the home cage. Mice were housed for 5 weeks prior to the tube test to "allow enough time for the social hierarchy to be stabilized". However, there is no record of the agonistic interactions and the social rank in the home cage. How do they know that the hierarchy in the home cage is reflected in the rank in the tube test? The experience with B6 is that very often the hierarchy is not as linear as the test tube rank will suggest.

– The way "dominant" and "subordinate" are defined trough the manuscript is methodologically concerning (comparing ranks 1+2 to ranks 3+4 is equivalent to a median split of the population; a questionable analytical approach) and biologically unclear (what`s the relevance of grouping 1/2 and 3/4 for the sociobiology of mice, which are known to be highly despotic? Classic paper https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/36/2/299/960816).

– Authors should also consider and discuss the results of this study (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5920077/) that used a similar tube test protocol (as well as their review of the relevant literature).

– Overall, I strongly suggest to avoid the definition of "dominant" and "subordinate" in the manuscript and to analyze their very nice data according to a more neutral "high rank" and "low rank" in the tube test (or even better to compare the 4 ranks individually, e.g. alpha, beta, etc) and try to determine, or at least speculate, of what that rank really means. As they and others have demonstrated, there is some very interesting biology associated with the rank assessed in this test. It clearly reflects an individual trait (similar to trait anxiety measured in the EPF for example?) that may or may not be related to social status, or social rank. Indeed, there is a vast literature showing that when social rank is assessed using behavioral observations of social interaction, subordinate mice manifested higher anxiety and depression-like behavior when compared to dominant mice, a finding which is opposite when ranks is assessed in the tube test (Figure 1).

2) Figure 2. LAC treatment significantly improved stress-induced immobility (albeit the% change is modest) but had no effect in the social interaction test. Therefore, authors should be very careful in using strong statements on the "anti-depressant" effect of this drug.

3) I see little added value in the use of the composite behavioral score of just 2 tests, out of which only one show a significant difference between groups. Essentially the score will be driven by the FST. Unless authors will be able to provide a strong rationale for its significance, this score should be removed.

4) Figure 1F; please include actual total time in the interaction zone and the corners.

5) Figure 1E; please include time in the center, time in the outer ring or corners etc. The significance of latency to enter the center as the most important measure of anxiety is questionable.

Reviewer #3:

The authors examined the potential antidepressant effects of Acetyl-L-Carnitine in dominant and subordinate mice exposed to chronic restraint stress. Dominant and subordinate mice were classified based on their hierarchical status. Following chronic stress, dominant mice show vulnerability in social interaction and in the forced swim test. Treatment with carnitine reversed the deficits seen in the Porsolt swim test but not social avoidance. The authors then used MRS and identified in the NAc some metabolites that are decreased by stress and rescued by carnitine. Some of these metabolites are related to mitochondrial function. Despite being correlational, this discovery approach has definitely some merits as it has identified some targets in the NAc that could be followed on with functional studies, in the hope that they play a major role in depression. While we liked this work, we still have a few major concerns that the authors need to address:

1) The authors are relying too much on the forced swim test to demonstrate that L-Carnitine is an antidepressant drug. As the authors likely know, this behavioral test has received a lot of criticism lately and is definitely not a test for depression. Adding more behavioral tests -may be tests that require energy and coping- is necessary to strengthen this manuscript.

2) The EPM data shown in Figure 1: Animals in this test spend 4-8% of their time in the open arms. This is an extremely low time that will not allow the experimenters to appreciate the effects of anxiogenic treatments.

3) OF data shown in Figure 1: why only latency to enter the center is reported? Usually, it is the time spent in the center of the OF that is reported

4) Why rank 2 was considered dominant? If that is the case, rank 3 is also dominant over rank 4, right? Did the authors think about examining just the extremes (rank 1 vs. rank4)?

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Metabolic signature in ccumbens for anti-depressant-like effects of acetyl-L-carnitine" for further consideration by eLife. Your revised article has been evaluated by Christian Büchel (Senior Editor) and a Reviewing Editor.

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed before acceptance, as outlined below.

1) While the authors changed the nomenclature from dom/sub to high/low rank in the Result section/figures, the interpretation of what that rank reflects is unchanged in the Abstract, Introduction and large part of the Discussion. As currently written, the role of status/rank in the observed phenotype is confusing. If authors consider social rank a trait (genetically encoded?) rather than a state, they should be consistent through the manuscript.

2) Authors did not address a main concern: high and low rank groups simply reflect a median split of the population. A visual inspection of Figure 1 clarifies that ranks 2 and 3 are very similar, essentially indistinguishable from each other. Because the N is low, they could not analyze each rank separately, and rank 1 vs rank 4 are not statistically different (Author response images).

3) The authors did not provide any justification for the use of the composite behavioral score (except for convenience of having a second measure of "global" depression-like behavior).

4) Finally, the evidence in favor of high rank mice being characterized by high anxiety is limited to only 1 behavioral test, the EPM.

I would like to ask the authors to address these comments by thoroughly discussing the strengths/limits of the ranking dominance classifications.

eLife. 2020 Jan 10;9:e50631. doi: 10.7554/eLife.50631.sa2

Author response


Reviewer #1:

[…] 1) Effects of LAC treatment on depressive-like behaviors in dominant and subordinate mice. Although the sample size of the data is small (n = 6-7), the experiments were able to replicate the predictability of the social status based on the anxiety phenotype and the vulnerability of the dominant mice to chronic-stress induced depressive-like deficits like social avoidance (Figure 1). Interestingly, they failed to see the basal difference in latency to immobility in the forced swimming test between dominant and subordinate male mice reported by previous published data (Horii et al., 2017). However, the trend between the control, CRS and CRS+LAC conditions is the same between dominant and subordinate males, as the reduction in the% of immobility appears to be the same between the CRS and the CRS+LAC dominant and subordinate groups. If the authors wish to report conclusions of their effect of the LAC treatment across dominant and subordinate mice (as they do in their results and Discussion sections), they should carry on a two-way ANOVA with post-hoc analysis with social status and group condition as the variables in all of their behavioral tests (social avoidance, FST and composite behavior) (Figure 2).

We convey with the reviewer that in order to claim a differential effect between high rank and low rank mice, regarding the efficacy of the LAC treatment, the observation of an interaction in the behavioral tests is required. As the interaction effect is not significant, we have followed our established goal for the study which was/is to investigate whether LAC treatment may prevent the emerging of depression-like behaviors and metabolic dysregulation in the NAc induced by CRS.

Accordingly, we have made this aim more explicit in the text and specified in the Results section as well that our focus in the LAC experiment was to examine results in high rank mice. All the results from low rank animals in the LAC experiment have been moved to the figure supplements (i.e., eLife ‘child’ figures; Figure 5—figure supplement 1). All previous reference to the comparison of LAC treatment effects in high vs low rank animals have now been deleted from the text.

2) Moreover, seeing that LAC supplementation appears to decrease% cumulative weight gain and% of immobility of the stressed mice, one could argue that the effect of LAC could be due to an increase in locomotor activity of the mice which, on one hand could be considered a treatment for depressed locomotor activity but on the other hand could also limit the therapeutic potential of the mitochondrial boost supplementation. While Lau and colleagues (2017) didn't see effects of LAC in immobility in non-stressed mice and no effect should be expected in the experiments presented in this article, I would suggest the authors to provide a graph with the total distance travelled in the FST as a supplementary figure to discard the motor effects of LAC or an extra open field test post treatment.

In order to address the reviewer’s concern and to evaluate potential effects of the treatments on general animal locomotion, we have measured the distance travelled by mice in the Social Interaction test (measurement in the FST gives similar -though inverse- results as the ones provided in Figure 2E). Locomotor activity was increased by CRS and CRS+LAC did not significantly differ in locomotion from CRS ones, though they tended to move less when the target animal was not present (Figure 2G). If anything, LAC-treated mice tended to move slightly less than CRS and non-LAC treated mice, supporting the idea that the reduced floating induced by LAC treatment in CRS-treated mice in the FST is not explained by a general increase in locomotor activity induced by the treatment.

3) In terms of the LAC dose administration, the authors report to have used drinking water supplementation with a concentration of 0.3%. It is not clear, however, if at the time of administration, the animals were singled or grouped housed (in groups of 4 as during the cohabitation). If possible, a water intake report should be presented in order to report a clear dosage of the supplement per animal.

We have now clarified this point in the Materials and methods section, as follows:

“One bottle per cage of 4 mice was used during LAC treatment. Liquid consumption was monitored every day and analyzed following normalization according to the body weight of the 4 animals in the cage.”

The water intake report has been described in the Result section, as follows:

“Animals exposed to CRS displayed a significant decrease in cumulative body weight gain, regardless of their social rank, that was not counteracted by LAC supplementation (Figure 2B; in high rank: Stress effect F2,10=45.0, p<.0001 and Figure 2—figure supplement 1A; in low rank: Stress effect F2,10= 20.4, p<.001). Importantly, whereas stress led to an increase in liquid consumption (+15 ± 12%) in the stressed groups (Figure 2C and Figure 2—figure supplement 1B), there was no difference in liquid consumption between the CRS and CRS+LAC groups (Figure 2C)”.

With statistics described in the figure legend, as follows:

“Figure 2 (C) Daily water intake during the LAC treatment period (given during the last week of the CRS protocol) normalized by total body weight of the 4 mice per cage (Group effect: F2,4=17.0, *P<.05; Interaction: F14,28=0.90, P>.05; time effect: F7,14=1.24, P>.05, repeated measures two-way ANOVA; n=3 cages per group). Thus, water intake data represents the cage average value.”

4) Changes in NAc metabolites in response to chronic stress and LAC supplementation

The use of in vivo H-MRS allows to explore multiple metabolites and neurochemical compounds at the same time in dominant and subordinate males, which englobe processes known to be affected by LAC like mitochondrial betation, protein acetylation, lipid peroxidation and neurotransmitter composition. According to their results, chronic stress in dominant mice affects different metabolites related to mitochondrial transport and glutamatergic neurotransmission.

Interestingly, they saw a decrease in taurine levels in the CRS dominant group that was restored to control levels by LAC supplementation (Figure 5). While the authors discuss the negative correlation between NAc taurine levels and traits like anxiety and immobility, they don't discuss the surprising nature of their results. Taurine is a mitochondrial transport protein that is implicated in oxidative stress, and typically increased in situations that require high mitochondrial activity like chronic restrain stress and decreased by hyperglycemia. However, in these results, CRS decreases taurine levels without changes in glucose levels. Furthermore, this only is seen in dominant males. A deeper discussion of these results is recommended to accentuate the importance of the findings regarding the changes in taurine.

We thank the reviewer for bringing up this mechanistic perspective. Indeed, stress-induced hyperglycemia during daily stress exposure may have contributed to the observed changes in NAc taurine levels. Accordingly, the lower levels of NAc taurine found in stressed high rank mice have been now further addressed with respect to hyperglycemia, by including the following paragraph in the Discussion section:

“Taurine is a sulfur containing amino acid, with no involvement in protein synthesis, but with several functions ranging from antioxidant, signaling molecule and osmolyte (Hansen et al., 2006; Yang et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2016; Jamshidzadeh et al., 2017). Brain taurine concentrations have been shown to be reduced by chronic stress (Barbosa Neto et al., 2012) and hyperglycemic conditions (Malone et al., 2008). […] It will be important to address whether this is the mechanism that leads to the reduction in accumbal taurine levels observed in our study.”

As to the effects of chronic stress in mitochondrial function, given that there are brain region specific differences typically found in the literature, we would prefer not to include this aspect, until its careful consideration in future studies, for clarity of the message and discussion.

5) Regarding the main effects of LAC in correcting the defects in mitochondrial-related metabolites, an extra experiment measuring complex I-II activity in the NAc (following the experiments by the corresponding author in Hollis et al., 2015) would be appropriate to support the conclusions.

Although the measurement of mitochondrial respiration for complexes I and II as in Hollis et al., 2015, could provide further insights on whether mitochondrial function is specifically affected by the treatment, and we agree of interest on its own, we would like to address these and several other potential mechanisms related to mitochondria and ER, and their interactions in the context of stress and nutritional supplements, such as LAC, in future studies that go in-depth into mechanistic understanding. At this point, and given the lengthy investment that such an experiment would require, we have opted by focusing on the metabolic characterization of metabolites that can be quantified with 1H-MRS, in order to progress on potential endpoints and biomarkers that can be followed up as well in human studies. Therefore, and hoping that it is ok with the reviewers and editors, and due to the actual lack of time to perform such lengthy experiment for the requested revision, we have addressed now this point by including the following comment in the Discussion section:

“It will be important to address whether this is the mechanism that leads to the reduction in accumbal taurine levels observed in our study, as well as exploring how metabolite changes induced by stress and LAC relate to mitochondrial function.”

Reviewer #2:

[…] 1) Even after reading several times this manuscript (and the previous Current Biology 2017 paper), I still struggle with the nomenclature used, i.e. dominant and subordinate, and how the individual rank is assessed. Below several concerns on this issue, and an overall recommendation to use a different nomenclature and avoid interpreting the data in favor of social status differences.

– I’m still unclear of what the Figure 1C is actually measuring, and how the index of dominance (Figure 1B) was calculated. […] I provide below several alternative (speculative) interpretations: i) high time spent in the tube implies extended social affiliation between 2 mice, and not social confrontation; ii) low time implies high fear of the tube per se, independently from the partner presence; iii) high time implies enhanced fear response manifested as freezing/immobility, again independently from the social interaction.

– The conclusion that the rank in the tube test is equivalent to social rank has not been confirmed with behavioral observations of agonistic interaction in the home cage. Mice were housed for 5 weeks prior to the tube test to "allow enough time for the social hierarchy to be stabilized". However, there is no record of the agonistic interactions and the social rank in the home cage. How do they know that the hierarchy in the home cage is reflected in the rank in the tube test? The experience with B6 is that very often the hierarchy is not as linear as the test tube rank will suggest.

– The way "dominant" and "subordinate" are defined trough the manuscript is methodologically concerning (comparing ranks 1+2 to ranks 3+4 is equivalent to a median split of the population; a questionable analytical approach) and biologically unclear (what`s the relevance of grouping 1/2 and 3/4 for the sociobiology of mice, which are known to be highly despotic? Classic paper https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article/36/2/299/960816).

– Authors should also consider and discuss the results of this study (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5920077/) that used a similar tube test protocol (as well as their review of the relevant literature).

– Overall, I strongly suggest to avoid the definition of "dominant" and "subordinate" in the manuscript and to analyze their very nice data according to a more neutral "high rank" and "low rank" in the tube test (or even better to compare the 4 ranks individually, e.g. alpha, beta, etc) and try to determine, or at least speculate, of what that rank really means. As they and others have demonstrated, there is some very interesting biology associated with the rank assessed in this test. It clearly reflects an individual trait (similar to trait anxiety measured in the EPF for example?) that may or may not be related to social status, or social rank. Indeed, there is a vast literature showing that when social rank is assessed using behavioral observations of social interaction, subordinate mice manifested higher anxiety and depression-like behavior when compared to dominant mice, a finding which is opposite when ranks is assessed in the tube test (Figure 1).

There are several points in reviewer 2’s comments:

1) How the index of social dominance is measured. In order to explain this better, we have now added the following sentences to the corresponding part of the Materials and methods section:

“The designation of a winner after each of the 6 possible pairs of confrontations per cage allowed to rank each mouse by its winning times, varying between 0 – 3. […] In order to reflect the specificity of the observed dominant and subordinate behavior to the SCTT (Varholick et al., 2018), animals with highest index of dominance (ranks 1 and 2) are referred to as “high rank” herein, whilst animals with lowest index of dominance (ranks 3 and 4) are referred to as “low rank”.”

2) The validity of the Social Confrontation Tube Test to index individuals’ social hierarchy in the homecage. Although potential different explanations of the mice behavior in this test suggested by the reviewer are certainly valid, our experimental conditions have been chosen to minimize ambiguity in the interpretation of the data. Note that, as indicated by the reviewer, mice were housed together for at least 5 weeks before performing the tube test. This is highly important, as also indicated in one of the references provided by the reviewer (see above: Varholick JA, Bailoo JD, Palme R and Würbel H, Sci. Rep. 2018, Phenotypic variability between Social Dominance Ranks in laboratory mice – doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24624-4), when they tested mice hierarchy dynamics throughout time from first establishment of the homecage colony, they “found that dominance ranks of most mice changed with time, but were most stable between the 2nd and 3rd week of testing”.

In our previous study (Larrieu et al., 2017) we also verified that the measured hierarchy was stable over time and established as well that it followed a linear pattern (see Author response image 1).

Author response image 1. Hierarchical Rank Using a Social Confrontation Tube Test.

Author response image 1.

(A) Illustration of the general timeline of the study. (B) Example of one cage representing the tube test ranks and winning times as a function of tube test trials. (C) Summary for nine cages over the 6-day test trials. (D) Time spent in the tube (s) as a function of the rank pairing (F5,48 = 9.78, p < 0.001, one-way ANOVA; ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗∗∗p < 0.001, Bonferroni’s test, n = 9 per rank pairing). (E) Dominance score after agonistic behaviors in the homecage (t28 = 2.30, ∗p < 0.05, unpaired t test, two-tailed n = 15 per group). (F) 2 × 2 contingency table for correlation between agonistic behaviors and tube test ranks (Fisher’s exact test, two-tailed, p = 0.050). (G) Left: picture representing typical urine marks profile of dominant and subordinate mice revealed by a UV light source. Right: 2 × 2 contingency table for correlation between urine marking test and tube test ranks (Fisher’s exact test, two-tailed, p = 0.026, n = 26 pairs). From Larrieu et al., 2017, Curr Biol..

In addition, our choice for this experimental approach is based on a careful consideration of the validation of this test in the literature – see, for example, the following list of articles published on the methodological aspects or revision of the validity of the test throughout published data:

Fan Z, Zhu H, Zhou T, Wang S, Wu Y, Hu H. Using the tube test to measure social hierarchy in mice. Nat Protoc. 2019 Mar;14(3):819-831. doi: 10.1038/s41596-018-0116-4. PMID: 30770887.

Wang F, Kessels HW, Hu H. The mouse that roared: neural mechanisms of social hierarchy. Trends Neurosci. 2014 Nov;37(11):674-82. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.07.005. Review. PMID: 25160682

3) The cross-validation of the results from the tube test with other confrontational tests. As done in (Wang et al., 2014), we also previously validated that co-habitation in tetrads in the homecage for corresponding changes in dominance behavior in other tests (e.g., agonistic/fighting behaviors and urine marking test) – see Author response image 1. In addition, the warmspot test was also recently used to validate the outcome of the tube test rank order outcome (see Zhou et al., 2017).

See Author response image 1 with the cross-validation of our current experimental conditions (at least 5 weeks of cohabitation in the homecage), as published in our study (Larrieu et al., 2017). When computing a dominance score including several submissive or offensive parameters (flight, avoidance, freezing, submissive posture; chasing attack including bite, upright and side offensive posture), we have shown that high rank (1-2) mice have a significantly higher dominance score as compared to low rank (3-4).

4) The use of the terms ‘dominant’ and ‘subordinate’. As suggested by the reviewer, we have now modified all former references to these terms and substitute them, as suggested, by ‘high rank’ and ‘low rank’. Clarifications in the Materials and methods have been done as follows:

“In order to reflect the specificity of the observed dominant and subordinate behavior to the SCTT (Varholick et al., 2018), animals with highest index of dominance (ranks 1 and 2) are referred to as “high rank” herein, whilst animals with lowest index of dominance (ranks 3 and 4) are referred to as “low rank”.”

5) In addition, following the reviewer recommendation to discuss the study by Varholick et al., 2018, we have now added the following paragraph to the Discussion:

“However, a note of caution should be added as previous work suggests that whereas dominant mice tend to display more novelty-related exploratory behavior than less dominant mice, differences in anxiety-related behaviors seem to be less consistent (see Varholick et al., 2018, and references herein). We would also like to emphasize that we find a high reliability and linearity of social rank assessed under our experimental conditions that involve at least 5 weeks of cohabitation prior to carrying out the social confrontation tube test (see also Larrieu et al., 2017). This characterization may determine a different phenotype than studies in which social hierarchy is established within the first 2-3 weeks of cohabitation. Indeed, a recent study showed that during early cohabitation, dominance ranks of mice changed with repeated measurement, but became more stable between the 2nd and 3rd week of testing (Varholick et al., 2018).”

6) Finally, the reviewer asked how the results would look if we concentrated in a comparison between ranks 1 and 4. Comparison between rank 1 and 4, presented in Author response image 2, showing a similar trend as when comparing rank 1-2 with rank 3-4. Nevertheless, due to the strong reduction of animals per group using this approach (N=3), statistical interpretation needs to be cautious. As such, we have decided not to include these results in the manuscript. If the reviewers would like to see these results in the manuscript, we would of course reconsider our proposal.

Author response image 2. Behavioral and metabolic comparisons between Rank 1 vs Rank 4.

Author response image 2.

(A) Comparison of trait-anxiety parameters measured with an elevated plus maze when only the highest rank (R1) and lowest rank (R4) are compared. Student’s t-test, n=6-7 per group. (B) Comparison of trait-anxiety parameters measured with an open field when only the highest rank (R1) and lowest rank (R4) are compared. Student’s t-test, n=3 per group. (C) Depressive-like behavior measured as a composite z-score component of social avoidance and immobility time in animals of Rank 1 (F2,6=2.95, P>.05, one-way ANOVA; n=3 per group). (D) Depressive-like behavior measured as a composite z-score component of social avoidance and immobility time in animals of Rank 4 (F2,6=0.07, P>.05, one-way ANOVA; n=3 per group). (E) Accumbal neurochemistry in Rank 1 mice. One-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s test, n=3 per group. (F) Accumbal neurochemistry in Rank 4 mice. One-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s test, n=3 per group.

2) Figure 2. LAC treatment significantly improved stress-induced immobility (albeit the% change is modest) but had no effect in the social interaction test. Therefore, authors should be very careful in using strong statements on the "anti-depressant" effect of this drug.

We have now indeed softened the text according to the reviewer’s comments. Note, however, the we also found differences in the composite z-score for the two behavioral tests that indicates that, overall, CRS increases alterations in these emotional tests and LAC reverses it.

3) I see little added value in the use of the composite behavioral score of just 2 tests, out of which only one show a significant difference between groups. Essentially the score will be driven by the FST. Unless authors will be able to provide a strong rationale for its significance, this score should be removed.

Although we appreciate the reviewer’s comment, the composite score allows obtaining a more global figure of how individual animals are affected when tested a the two behavioral tests used. Therefore, this measurement has a higher significant value than regarding individual tests separately and, thus, unless this represents a major obstacle in the reviewer’s view, we would prefer to keep it in the article.

4) Figure 1F; please include actual total time in the interaction zone and the corners.

The time in interaction zone and in the corners has now been added in Figure 1F.

“Figure 1: (F) […] Time in interaction zone: Interaction: F1,21=12.80, P<.005; rank effect: F1,21=0.85, P<.05; stress effect: F1,21=12.21, P<.005, two-way ANOVA; p*<.05, **p<.005, Bonferroni’s test, n=6-7 per group / Time in corner zone: Interaction: F1,21=3.29, P>.05; rank effect: F1,21=0.28, P<.05; stress effect: F1,21=2.14, P>.05, two-way ANOVA, n=6-7 per group).”

5) Figure 1E; please include time in the center, time in the outer ring or corners etc. The significance of latency to enter the center as the most important measure of anxiety is questionable.

We have included the time in center as well as the distance travelled for the OF in the Figure 1E. These results have been described in the Results section and in the legend of Figure 1, as follows:

“However, the two groups did not show differences in the percent time spent in the center of the OF (Figure 1E, center), indicating that their difference in anxiety-like behaviors depends on the specific threat encountered. Importantly, no difference in locomotor activity was observed, as the distance travelled by both groups in the OF was similar (Figure 1E, right; n.s.).”

“Figure 1: (E) Anxiety-related behaviors measured in the open-field, including latency to first enter the center of the arena (*p<.05, unpaired t-test, two-tailed n = 14 per group) and time in center zone (n.s. unpaired t-test, two-tailed n = 14 per group). Locomotor activity is measured as the distance travelled in the OF (n.s. unpaired t-test, two-tailed n = 14 per group).”

Reviewer #3:

The authors examined the potential antidepressant effects of Acetyl-L-Carnitine in dominant and subordinate mice exposed to chronic restraint stress. Dominant and subordinate mice were classified based on their hierarchical status. Following chronic stress, dominant mice show vulnerability in social interaction and in the forced swim test. Treatment with carnitine reversed the deficits seen in the Porsolt swim test but not social avoidance. The authors then used MRS and identified in the NAc some metabolites that are decreased by stress and rescued by carnitine. Some of these metabolites are related to mitochondrial function. Despite being correlational, this discovery approach has definitely some merits as it has identified some targets in the NAc that could be followed on with functional studies, in the hope that they play a major role in depression. While we liked this work, we still have a few major concerns that the authors need to address:

1) The authors are relying too much on the forced swim test to demonstrate that L-Carnitine is an antidepressant drug. As the authors likely know, this behavioral test has received a lot of criticism lately and is definitely not a test for depression. Adding more behavioral tests -may be tests that require energy and coping- is necessary to strengthen this manuscript.

We thank the reviewer for this comment and, in fact, we use this test given its capability to distinguish between active and passive coping responses to a stressful situation. We take the point and will be aiming at increasing the battery of tests in our future studies.

2) The EPM data shown in Figure 1: Animals in this test spend 4-8% of their time in the open arms. This is an extremely low time that will not allow the experimenters to appreciate the effects of anxiogenic treatments.

Thanks for this comment. Given to normal inter-experiment variation (at least in our hands), percent time in open arms in this study was slightly (though non-significantly different) lower than results in other studies in the lab (see, e.g., our Larrieu et al., 2017, Curr Biol. Paper). As the purpose of this test in our study was to characterize basal anxiety-like behaviors in high and low rank animals, this point is not problematic regarding potential treatments, but we thank the reviewer for pointing out the issue and will aim at modifying the experimental conditions to make the test less anxiogenic whenever we aim at studying how a particular treatment may modify anxiety-like behaviors in mice.

3) OF data shown in Figure 1: why only latency to enter the center is reported? Usually, it is the time spent in the center of the OF that is reported

Thanks for this comment, we have now included these results in Figure 1E and reported the new data in the Results section.

4) Why rank 2 was considered dominant? If that is the case, rank 3 is also dominant over rank 4, right? Did the authors think about examining just the extremes (rank 1 vs. rank4)?

As mentioned earlier, given the low number of animals in our study, a comparison between only rank 1 vs. rank 4 was challenging. We have tested whether a significant difference existed between rank 1 and rank 4 in the different parameters of the basal anxiety measurements (OF and EPM) and post-CRS (depressive-like behavior and accumbal metabolites). While the tendency was similar as when including rank 1-2 vs. rank 3-4, the statistical test (Student’s t-test or one-way ANOVA with post-hoc) did not pass the threshold of significance, due to the reduced statistical power, but are reassuring that our grouping of animals in high and low rank animals reflect the changes observed in ranks 1 vs 4, as follows:

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed before acceptance, as outlined below.

1) While the authors changed the nomenclature from dom/sub to high/low rank in the Result section/figures, the interpretation of what that rank reflects is unchanged in the Abstract, Introduction and large part of the Discussion. As currently written, the role of status/rank in the observed phenotype is confusing. If authors consider social rank a trait (genetically encoded?) rather than a state, they should be consistent through the manuscript.

We have modified this in the text:

Abstract: we include now the following sentence and wording:

“High rank, but not low rank, mice, as assessed with the tube test,….”

Results: we include now the following wording:

the behavioral phenotype of high and low rank mice “(as assessed by testing the 4 mice from the same home cage in the social confrontation tube test),”

We have also added “high (ranks 1 and 2) and low (ranks 3 and 4) rank mice”

In the Discussion we have now included the following sentence:

“High rank animals were defined as those that following cohabitation in groups of 4 males, emerged as ranks 1 and 2 in the social confrontation tube test.”

2) Authors did not address a main concern: high and low rank groups simply reflect a median split of the population. A visual inspection of Figure 1 clarifies that ranks 2 and 3 are very similar, essentially indistinguishable from each other. Because the N is low, they could not analyze each rank separately, and rank 1 vs rank 4 are not statistically different (Author response images).

We would like to indicate that the figures that we included in the revision R1 rebuttal are supportive of differences in results between rank 1 and rank 4. It should be noted that in our report, we applied two-tailed statistics; as the comparisons were made following specific predictions, one-tailed statistics for Rank 1 animals in those results give trends very close to significance (i.e., p<0.056, P<0.075, p<0.08; p<0.05; p<0.1; p<0.12; p<0.056; p<0.056) while they are clearly not significant for Rank 4 mice, further supporting the reported results.

In order to address this issue from the reviewer more explicitly, we have now added the following note of caution to the Discussion section:

“An additional issue to consider is that, for our analyses, we have grouped ranks 1 and 2 as high rank mice and ranks 3 and 4 as low rank. Although this grouping allows revealing statistical differences in the evaluated variables, each of the ranks in a tetrad home cage hierarchy may in fact lead to idiosyncratic phenotypes. In the future, it would be important to address vulnerability to stress for each specific rank in the colony.”

3) The authors did not provide any justification for the use of the composite behavioral score (except for convenience of having a second measure of "global" depression-like behavior).

In fact, in our rebuttal to R1, we provided an explanation for the use of the composite behavior, as follows:

“Although we appreciate the reviewer’s comment, the composite score allows obtaining a more global figure of how individual animals are affected when tested across the two behavioral tests used. Therefore, this measurement has a higher significant value than regarding individual tests separately and, thus, unless this represents a major obstacle in the reviewer’s view, we would prefer to keep it in the article.”

In the manuscript we had also included the following explanation:

“In order to obtain an integrated estimation of how stress and LAC treatments affect emotional behavior more globally, we computed an overall behavioral composite to integrate deviation from normality considering high rank mice variance in both behavioral tests.”

Perhaps this was not clear enough and, therefore, we have now added the following additional explanation to the text:

“Specifically, the use of this composite score allows considering variation from the mean in individuals’ behavior across two different behavioral tests, providing a more robust measurement of individuals’ behavior in tests that are typically used to index mice depressive-like behaviors.”

In other words, by computing animal’s behavior (or their deviation from normality/mean) across behavioral tests, we allow revealing consistent changes across tests, which in fact is a different measurement that computing the different behaviors separately; something that do not allow revealing whether the same animals were similarly or differently affected across tests; this composite behavior allows a more global picture of individuals’ deviation in global behavioral testing.

We hope this additional explanation in the manuscript and here in this rebuttal R2 helps us making a strong case for why we believe that this is an important additional measurement to add to the manuscript.

4) Finally, the evidence in favor of high rank mice being characterized by high anxiety is limited to only 1 behavioral test, the EPM.

Yes, this is indicated in the text, already from our revision in R1, as follows: “indicating that their difference in anxiety-like behaviors depends on the specific threat encountered”.

In order to make this point now clearer, we have added the following explanation in the Discussion:

“….higher rank is related to higher anxiety-like behaviors as indicated by their exploration of an elevated plus maze (but note that these animals did not differ from low rank in the time spent in the center of the open field, only in their latency to enter the zone)”

I would like to ask the authors to address these comments by thoroughly discussing the strengths/limits of the ranking dominance classifications.

We have tried now to make all those revisions more explicit and hope the manuscript is now in a satisfactory form (see specific explanations above).

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES