Abstract
Patient stratification is critical for the sensitivity of clinical trials at early stages of neurodegenerative disorders. In Huntington’s disease (HD), genetic tests make cognitive, motor and brain imaging measurements possible before symptom manifestation (pre-HD). We evaluated pre-HD stratification models based on single visit resting-state functional MRI (rs-fMRI) data that assess observed longitudinal motor and cognitive change rates from the multisite Track-On HD cohort (74 pre-HD, 79 control participants). We computed longitudinal performance change on 10 tasks (including visits from the preceding TRACK-HD study when available), as well as functional connectivity density (FCD) maps in single rs-fMRI visits, which showed high test-retest reliability. We assigned pre-HD subjects to subgroups of fast, intermediate, and slow change along single tasks or combinations of them, correcting for expectations based on aging; and trained FCD-based classifiers to distinguish fast- from slow-progressing individuals. For robustness, models were validated across imaging sites. Stratification models distinguished fast- from slow-changing participants and provided continuous assessments of decline applicable to the whole pre-HD population, relying on previously-neglected white matter functional signals. These results suggest novel correlates of early deterioration and a robust stratification strategy where a single MRI measurement provides an estimate of multiple ongoing longitudinal changes.
Subject terms: Machine learning, Huntington's disease
Introduction
The detection of treatment effects in clinical trials is partly limited by the individual differences in the population under study. In neurodegenerative disorders, enrichment strategies to decrease sample heterogeneity and identify subjects likely to respond to treatment are important for the efficiency and success of clinical trials, especially for early interventions1. Given the high dimensionality of neurological data, computational approaches including machine learning techniques appear particularly promising for detecting individual differences in clinical populations by integrating distributed information, providing stratification of subjects for predictive or enrolment purposes2–4.
Huntington’s disease (HD) is a neurological disorder caused by an expansion in the cytosine-adenine-guanine (CAG) trinucleotide repeat region in the huntingtin (HTT) gene. The earliest symptoms are related to changes in mood and cognitive ability, followed by lack of motor coordination, unsteady gait, and chorea, ultimately leading to dementia and death5. If we take into consideration that clinical damage associated with the disease is potentially irreversible, early intervention is imperative. For this to happen, trials with patients at early stages should be tailored towards individuals who will develop a steep deterioration, since testing experimental early treatments on stable individuals can produce misleading results6.
The availability of genetic tests has enabled large neuroimaging studies, such as PREDICT-HD7 and TRACK-HD8,9, which have followed patients and controls over several years, even before motor-based diagnosis. These cohorts provide opportunities for moving beyond descriptive statistical observations toward assessments that provide predictions at an individual level4. (Note that in the context of predictive modelling, “prediction” refers to the ability to make assessments on previously-unencountered individuals, not necessarily forecasts of future events). Cross-validated strategies are a step toward that goal, where different sets of subjects are used for training a model and for testing its individual predictions10. In that sense, it is critical that the signals distinguishing individuals are reliable across successive measurements that are repeated on time scales that are short with respect to the expected rate of change11, but also that learned patterns are shown to be robust, e.g. valid across sub-cohorts from different imaging sites12.
In neuroimaging, structural correlates of HD such as striatal atrophy have received most attention13. Resting state functional MRI (rs-fMRI) is a more recent development14 and, since then, most analyses have been restricted to specific subnetworks or considered few regions, or seeds. However, functional brain networks are scale-free15, i.e. their degree distributions follow power laws spanning several orders of magnitude. This phenomenology can only be observed on networks with a large number of nodes such as voxel-level resolution networks. In addition, functional signals and networks of neurobiological significance have been recognized recently in white matter16–19. Therefore, previously-overlooked correlates of disease progression may be found by analyses designed to reflect these major aspects of functional networks.
Track-On HD20 is an extension of the TRACK-HD study that included yearly rs-fMRI data for up to 3 years in more than 200 pre-manifest HD (pre-HD, i.e. before diagnosis based on motor symptoms) subjects and healthy controls. Images are accompanied by a wealth of cognitive and motor measures, most also acquired during preceding TRACK-HD visits, in combination spanning up to 7 yearly measurements. Such a rich dataset allows for the estimation of subtle longitudinal changes in cognition and motor control, allowing subjects to be compared to their own performance baselines, and enabling analyses of population heterogeneity for a stage at which systematic brain differences from controls are mild21. In addition, because performance on different tasks might rely on the same affected neural systems, it is possible to identify combinations of measures for which longitudinal changes covary, potentially boosting behavioural signal-to-noise and isolating common brain structures responsible for cognitive decline.
Here, we evaluate the potential of single rs-fMRI scans for stratification of pre-HD individuals with respect to cognitive/motor decline, which has never been tested. We built functional connectivity density (FCD) maps (Fig. 1a) from a Track-On HD cohort of 153 subjects (74 pre-HD, 79 healthy controls) at the voxel-level. We first established test-retest reliability of multivariate FCD measures. We then trained cross-validated predictive models (Fig. 1b) to segregate pre-HD subpopulations according to cognitive/motor longitudinal trajectories (Fig. 1c) along multiple dimensions. To establish robustness of predictive power, we used a leave-one-site-out cross-validation (LOSO-CV) approach. The resulting models suggest a stratification strategy that considers disease progression in terms of longitudinal rates of change along different cognitive dimensions, and where a single MRI scan provides such information, otherwise measurable over years.
Methods
Participants
We included 153 participants from the four Track-On HD study sites in this analysis, belonging to one of two groups: 74 individuals without manifest HD (before diagnosis by a physician based on motor symptoms) but carrying the mutant HTT gene and 79 roughly age- and sex-matched controls (see Table 1 for demographics).
Table 1.
Controls | Pre-HD | |
---|---|---|
N | 79 | 74 |
Sex (Female) | 30 | 34 |
Age (mean ± STD) | 49 ± 11 | 42 ± 10 |
CAG | — | 43 ± 2 |
CAP | — | 48 ± 8 |
Expected years to disease onset | — | 11 ± 4 (minimum: 5) |
CAG: number of CAG repeats in the HTT gene. CAP: CAG-age-product66, a measure of disease burden given by the product of a subject’s number of excess CAG repeats and their age. Here we use the formula CAP = 100 × AGE × [(CAG − 35.5) ÷ 627], i.e. normalized so that score at disease onset (diagnosis based on motor symptoms) is approximately 100.
Exclusion criteria for analysis were as follows: manifest disease at any point before MRI acquisitions, age below 18 or above 65 (unless previously part of the TRACK-HD study), history of major psychiatric, neurological, or medical disorders, or severe head injury. These criteria are part of the Track-On HD study20. In addition, we excluded left-handed individuals, as known rs-fMRI lateralization differences in motor regions22 are outside the focus of this analysis, as well as those with fewer than two visits with rs-fMRI scans. Furthermore, we excluded subjects based on scan motion or unsatisfactory registration to standard template (see below, and Supplementary Table S1 for detailed numbers).
The Track-On HD study was approved by local ethics committees (Leiden site: Medical Ethical Commission of Leids Universtais Medisch Centrum; Vancouver site: Clinical Research Ethics Board at the University of British Columbia; London site: National Research Ethics Service of the National Health Service (NHS) London; Paris site: Comité de Protection des Personnes Ile De France VI, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital) and all participants gave written informed consent according to the Declaration of Helsinki. All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.
Cognitive/motor measures
On each visit, participants (both controls and pre-HD patients) were administered several motor and cognitive tests. For consistency with previous studies20, the following tests were considered: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), self-paced tapping, grip-force variability, map search, Stroop test, spot change test, cancelation test, mental rotation, indirect circle tracing, and counting backwards. Detailed description of tasks and their outcome measurements is provided elsewhere20 (see Supplementary Information for a brief summary). Tests were administered on each yearly visit in most cases (up to 3 in Track-On HD). Several tests were also included in the immediately preceding visits from the previous TRACK-HD study8, in which case up to seven time points were available (Fig. 1c, distribution of visits for each task in Supplementary Fig. S1).
To quantify longitudinal cognitive change for asymptomatic HD patients, we computed the slope of longitudinal performance change for each task. The underlying change is not necessarily linear; however, slopes are used as the simplest measure of subtle decline given the few time points per subject and measurement noise. All sessions in which a given subject was administered a test were included in the slope estimation, including those preceding fMRI acquisitions.
For consistency, we defined the sign of all task measures such that a negative longitudinal slope would indicate decline.
As we were interested in cognitive decline beyond the effects of healthy aging sex or age, we removed variance associated with these covariates. Linear effects of sex and age were estimated and removed using data from healthy controls.
Robust principal components for cognitive/motor measurements
Different tasks involve overlapping skills and brain circuits. Therefore, their longitudinal slopes share with each other a portion of inter-subject variability (i.e., they covary). To find measures of degeneration along different effective dimensions (that is, reflecting decoupled aspects deterioration), we computed decorrelated combinations of slopes via Principal Component Analysis (PCA), based on their covariance matrix. To reduce sensitivity to the presence of outliers, we used Minimum Covariance Determinant23, a robust method to estimate the covariance matrix. Units of the slopes of different tasks are different, so we also normalized slopes of each task by its interquartile range to produce a dimensionless quantity that could be combined with the other tasks.
We performed the PCA procedure twice: 1) First, both pre-HD subjects and controls were included, and the slopes were corrected (see below) for effects of normal aging and sex estimated from healthy subjects (using the age at the time of acquisition of the first functional image). This allowed a visualization of pre-HD and control trajectories, to confirm longitudinal differences were observable. 2) A second PCA included only slopes from pre-HD subjects, to best capture heterogeneity within this population. These are the principal components (PCs) we used for stratifying the pre-HD population (see “Discretization of cognitive/motor trajectories” below, and Supplementary Fig. S2 for a flowchart).
MRI data Acquisition
3T MRI data from 2 different scanner systems (Siemens Tim Trio and Philips Achieva) distributed across four acquisition sites were acquired, as described elsewhere8,20,24, including multiple design measures before and during acquisition to maximize data homogeneity across sites20.
In brief, T1-weighted image volumes were acquired using a 3D MPRAGE acquisition sequence with the following imaging parameters: TR = 2200 ms (Siemens)/7.7 ms (Philips), TE = 2.2 ms (S)/3.5 ms(P), FA = 10° (S)/8° (P), FOV = 28 cm (S)/24 cm (P), matrix size 256 × 256(S)/224 × 224(P), 208(S)/164(P) sagittal slices to cover the entire brain with a slice thickness of 1 mm (no gap 1 mm isotropic voxels).
For rs-fMRI, whole brain volumes were acquired with a TR of 3 s using a T2*-weighted echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence with the following parameters: TE 30 ms, FOV 212 mm, flip angle 80°, 48 slices in ascending order (thickness: 2.8 mm, gap: 0.5 mm, in plane resolution: 3.3 × 3.3 mm) and bandwidth of 1906 Hz per Px. 165 time points were acquired for 8:20 minutes. All data passed visual inspections centralized by IXICO.
MRI data Processing
fMRI pre-processing used FSL25 parallelized for large-scale computation using Nipype26. The first 5 time points of the fMRI time series were discarded to exclude unsteady acquisitions. Slice-timing correction was applied followed by rigid realignment to the volume in the middle of the series (motion-correction). Subjects with more than 10 frames with relative displacement (as computed by FSL’s mcflirt command) above 0.6 mm were excluded (4 subjects, Supplementary Table S1). Physiological (respiration, heart rate, etc.) as well as motion-associated noise was corrected using the tCompCor method27 that identifies a noise-dominated mask and removes its associated temporal variance. We deliberately avoided imposing an anatomical white matter (WM) mask to preserve recently recognized WM signals of neural origin16,18,19,28,29. On inspection, noise masks isolated cerebrospinal fluid and head edges. Finally, the time series of each voxel was band-pass filtered (0.01–0.16 Hz) to remove drifts and high-frequency noise (see Supplementary Fig. S3 for a flowchart).
In-session anatomical volumes underwent brain extraction (BET) followed by non-linear normalization to MNI space (FNIRT). All EPI time series were then rigidly co-registered to the in-session anatomical volume and non-linearly normalized to MNI standard space using the same transformation, preserving original voxel size (3.3 mm isotropic).
Acceptability of the concatenated registrations was verified as in other large sample studies30. Session-specific average volumes for each participant and a group average template were computed. Participants whose time-averaged EPI volume had a correlation with the template that deviated more than 3 standard deviations from the population were excluded from the analyses upon visual re-inspection.
Feature extraction
We computed voxel-level functional networks (Fig. 1a) as follows: (1) pair-wise Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated between voxels; (2) correlations above 0.7 constituted links, as in previous studies31.
For each voxel, we computed the log-degree, defined as log(degree + 1), where degree is defined as the total number of links. Each resulting log-degree map was normalized by the median value across all voxels as baseline reference. We refer to the resulting maps as FCD maps (see Supplementary Fig. S4 for a flowchart).
Univariate comparisons (described below) rely on local signals, so FCD maps were locally enhanced by Gaussian smoothing (FWHM = 2.5 voxels). For multivariate classifications, which pool signals together, no smoothing was used.
The above procedure was repeated to compute separate FCD maps from each individual at each visit.
Multivariate classifications
Using FCD maps, multivariate classification between 2 groups (pre-HD vs controls, or fast vs slow-decline) was performed using logistic regression with elastic net regularization, using a LOSO-CV scheme (Fig. 1b, see flowchart in Supplementary Fig. S5), to ensure robustness of predictions across imaging sites. Elastic net regularization is a penalty term that automatically performs variable selection, reducing overfitting. Unlike other variable selection terms such as LASSO, elastic net can select more features than the number of training samples (critical when the number of features is much larger than available samples), and is more stable when correlated features are present. Logistic regression was chosen over other linear classifiers due to good optimization convergence when combined with elastic net penalty using a SAGA optimizer32 on similar datasets33, but model performance is similar to a standard support vector machine. Optimal value of regularization parameters was determined within each training set via internal 5-fold cross-validation (i.e., nested cross-validation).
For classifications of pre-HD vs controls, FCD maps were linearly corrected for healthy aging and sex during model training, using weights learned from controls in the training set.
The metric of evaluation of binary classifiers was the area under the receiving operator characteristic (ROC) curve (AUC). This metric has the advantage that it is a measure of a continuous output, independent of the choice of a decision threshold, and robust to imbalanced classes. We computed the AUC for each test site and averaged them. For robustness of the estimation to outliers, we repeated the computation with bootstrap resampling (random resampling with replacement, 105 resamples) of the test subjects, and reported the median value. In the case of stratification models, it should be noted that the AUC tests whether a pattern of ongoing decline is successfully learned, but not its application to the general pre-HD population. For that we performed a second validation (see “Validation of continuous classifier output for pre-HD stratification” below).
All predictive modelling was implemented using scikit-learn34 and Lightning (http://contrib.scikit-learn.org/lightning/index.html) libraries in Python. Plots of results on the brain (and brain schematics used in figures) were generated using Nilearn35.
Univariate correlations of FCD features with cognitive/motor decline
For tasks slopes and their PCs, we computed voxelwise Spearman correlations with FCD, which were converted to a t-statistic from which statistical significance was calculated, and multiple comparison correction was applied (false discovery rate, FDR, see below).
Discretization of cognitive/motor trajectories
Exact values of task performance and their slopes are invariably noisy (see “Estimation of noise in cognitive slopes” in Supplementary Information for a quantification), but they can be used to isolate coarse subgroups of subjects, which has proven useful for longitudinal change in other disorders36. We defined three subgroups for each single task or PC projection: fast, intermediate, and slow, which corresponded respectively to subjects below, in-between, or above half a robust estimate of the standard deviation of decline away from the median. The robust estimate of the standard deviation was defined as the standard deviation of a normal distribution with the same interquartile range as the observed distribution of decline, making it insensitive to outliers. This threshold was chosen because, while being the same for all tasks, it divided the pre-HD population approximately into terciles. We performed cross-validated classifications between the extreme subgroups (fast and slow) on the basis of FCD maps (stratification models). Subjects with a slope or PC projection in-between these extreme ranges were not included in the classification of the two subgroups. This was the case for roughly one third of the subjects, the exact number depending on the PC or task (Supplementary Table S5). The subgroups used for training/testing the models did not differ in the number of visits used for computing the slopes (Supplementary Table S4).
Validation of continuous classifier output for pre-HD stratification
We propose that brain signatures based on extreme subgroups could be used to provide information about ongoing decline in any subject of the population, including those in the intermediate decline group. As a validation of this proposed application, we computed the Spearman rank correlation between the continuous output of the stratification models (signed distance to the decision hyperplane) and cognitive decline rates using all subjects from each test site. Spearman correlations computed within each of the four test sites were Fisher z-transformed, averaged over sites, and then inverse-transformed back to r-space, as is common practice when pooling together correlation coefficients37.
Integration with expectations from genetics
To quantify the variance in cognitive decline rates explained by CAG repeats, we computed the Spearman correlation between them. We also studied whether combining CAG repeats as a feature with FCD maps could improve predictive power in stratification models.
FCD test-retest reliability
We studied the consistency of FCD maps within individuals in two different ways.
First, we studied the correlation between baseline and follow-up FCD maps, in terms of their power to identify individuals (“fingerprinting”), similar to others11. For the baseline FCD map of each individual, we computed its Pearson correlation (across voxels) to the follow-up FCD map of each individual, and ranked these correlations (153 correlations). We stored the rank of the correlation between the same subject’s baseline and follow-up scan, a measure of how similar the two were relative to similarity to other subjects. The resulting distribution of 153 stored same-subject ranks was compared to chance levels.
Second, using LOSO-CV, we tested the within-subject consistency of predictions of a multivariate classifier trained to distinguish a neurologically relevant signature: that of pre-HD subjects vs. controls. We computed a transition matrix with the probability of a follow-up classification outcome given a baseline classification outcome, and compared the observation to chance levels (see “Statistical Significance Analyses” below).
Removal of local atrophy effects by voxel-based morphometry (VBM)
As a control for the effects of local atrophy, we used an estimation of grey matter concentration from voxel-based morphometry, as proposed by Oakes and colleagues38. The ‘optimized’ VBM protocol39 of the FMRIB Software Library (FSL)25 was used to evaluate grey matter patterns, as detailed in a previous study on the TRACK-HD cohort40. For each subject a T1-weighted image was used along with masks estimated from concurrent T2-weighted volumes to do brain extraction of the raw T1-weighted volume. The resulting images were then segmented into 3 tissue partial volumes (grey matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid concentrations) that represent the probability of each voxel belonging to a given tissue. Grey matter concentration (GMC) volumes of each subject/visit were nonlinearly normalized to the MNI standard space using FSL’s FNIRT routine (without normalization by the Jacobian determinants). The resulting GMC images were down-sampled to the resolution of FCD maps using linear interpolation to preserve their range. For each voxel the linear effect of GMC on FCD across subjects was estimated (i.e., a voxelwise covariate). For each subject at each voxel, the expected FCD based on the local GMC was subtracted from the observed FCD. The residuals constituted local-atrophy-controlled FCD maps. We used this atrophy-controlled FCD maps in multivariate detection of pre-HD (vs controls), and stratification models, for comparison of the resulting predictive power, and their associated weight maps. In addition, we evaluated full-brain GMC maps in terms of their multivariate predictive power to differentiate pre-HD vs control subjects, and stratification of the population with a LOSO-CV scheme, identically as done with FCD maps.
Removal of site variability
To minimize potential effects of imaging site on learned FCD patterns, we removed the associated variability from FCD maps using ComBat41, a technique originally proposed to remove batch effects in gene expression data and recently shown to outperform several others for harmonization of maps from different MRI modalities42,43. For each voxel, ComBat estimates a site-specific mean value and site-specific scaling factor. Importantly, while ComBat allows for the specification of biological variates of interest (subject group or subgroup, age, sex, etc.) to best preserve such variance, we chose not to include them in the model in order to guarantee information about classification labels would not leak into the corrected FCD maps. We used a Python implementation of the original method (https://github.com/brentp/combat.py).
Removal of motion associations
Rigid motion correction and motion artefact removal with tCompCorr had been applied to the fMRI time series. However, no method is able to completely mitigate motion-related artefacts44, so we additionally controlled in stratification models for the effects of motion by removal of motion variability across subjects in each feature, as proposed by others45. The mean framewise displacement was used as a quantifier of head motion44, as provided by FSL’s mcflirt, which calculates the average voxel displacement between successive time points in terms of rigid transformations over a spherical, brain-sized region of interest. To remove motion artefacts while preserving the neurological signals potentially correlated with motion in pre-HD subjects, the linear weight of motion on each feature was learned from healthy controls, who had a motion distribution very similar to the pre-HD population (see Results, “Robustness of FCD patterns to non-functional covariates”). We detrended FCD features with respect to head motion in the pre-HD population using these weights, and compared the resulting stratification model and FCD patterns to those without this additional motion control.
Statistical significance analyses
All voxel-wise univariate statistical tests for between-group comparisons were analysed with non-parametric Mann–Whitney U tests. We controlled for multiple comparisons by estimating a false discovery rate (FDR) through the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure46.
Null distributions for assessing significance of cross-validated classification results were computed via permutation of test subject labels (105 randomizations, implemented in Python) within each site. Similarly, null distributions for the Spearman correlations between decline rates and distance to the decision hyperplane were obtained via permutation of test subject labels (105 randomizations) within each site.
For test-retest reliability analyses (transition matrix), we computed a null distribution of within-subject consistency of classifier predictions: classifier predictions of each subject’s baseline FCD were compared with a distribution of follow-up visit predictions of randomized subjects that belonged to the same group (group-preserving permutations of the second sample, 105 permutations). This ensured the null distribution samples contained the same proportion of each possible prediction for each group and visit as in the observed case, while only the within-subject correspondence between baseline and follow-up classification varied.
Results
Using data from the Track-On HD cohort (Table 1), we first evaluated the test-retest reliability of full-brain FCD maps (46667 voxels) and then evaluated models that stratify the pre-HD population by longitudinal cognitive decline based on single-visit FCD maps.
Test-retest reliability of FCD maps
We validated FCD maps in terms of within-subject test-retest reliability. First, we evaluated the within-subject similarity of baseline and follow-up FCD maps, irrespective of a neurological signature of interest. For, this we evaluated the ability to identify (“fingerprint”) individuals on follow-up visits based on the correlations of follow-up and baseline FCD maps (Methods, “FCD test-retest reliability”). More that 67% of the baseline maps had their top correlation with the follow-up map from the same subject (Fig. 2a), two orders of magnitude above chance expectations (1 in 153, i.e. 0.65%, red line in Fig. 2a). Of the remaining cases, a large fraction had their same-subject correlation on the top rankings. This result suggests FCD maps reliably capture full-brain individual differences.
We then evaluated the reliability of FCD maps for reflecting a neurologically meaningful difference, that of pre-HD vs controls. Classification based on presence or absence of mutated HTT gene is ideal to study within-subject reliability: genetic labels have virtually no associated noise, and are strictly stable across visits. Moreover, because functional differences are mild in gene-carriers far from symptom onset21, the sensitivity to test-retest fluctuations is expected to be higher than for strong neurological signatures. We trained binary classifiers using sex- and age-corrected FCD maps validated using LOSO-CV to ensure robustness to site differences. The resulting AUC (0.64, p = 0.00082, permutation test, Fig. 2b) was compatible with mild but detectable signatures of pre-HD. Subjects classified correctly with the first FCD sample were highly likely to be classified correctly also with the second sample (95% consistency for pre-HD, p < 10−5; 76% consistency for controls, p = 0.0007 permutation test; Fig. 2c), indicating high consistency for correct classifications. Moreover, the whole transition matrix was dominated by the diagonal, so consistency of errors was also well above chance levels (63% for controls, p = 0.0007; 69% for pre-HD, p < 10−5, compare to chance matrix in Fig. 2c), suggesting misclassifications were driven by reliable FCD heterogeneity and not random noise. Together, the observed consistency suggests high test-retest reliability of multivariate FCD patterns from visits separated up to 2 years, and motivates the study of individual variation within the disease, which is consistently observed but not captured by group differences.
To visualize the FCD signatures of early degeneration in pre-HD, we first computed a univariate voxel-wise group comparison. Significant differences (Mann-Whitney U test, FDR corrected, q < 0.05) were localized in the globus pallidus and its interface with the putamen (Fig. 2d). Similar but more extensive results were obtained by inspection of the multivariate classification weights. A map of mean classifier weights (Fig. 2e) revealed the clusters were bilaterally symmetric. This suggests functional hyper-connectivity of the globus pallidus is a novel signature of pre-HD.
Analysis of cognitive/motor longitudinal decline
To quantify cognitive/motor decline, we computed longitudinal slopes for 10 tasks (see Methods, Supplementary Information for task description). Steeper decline slopes of pre-HD subjects vs controls were observed for the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT, p = 0.009, Mann-Whitney U test), the Stroop test (p = 0.017, Mann-Whitney U test), indirect circle trace (p = 0.032, Mann-Whitney U test), and paced tapping (p = 0.014, Mann-Whitney U test) tasks (see Supplementary Table S2 for all tasks). Associations with healthy aging in controls were not significant (Supplementary Table S3). In pre-HD, longitudinal slopes were not significantly correlated with CAG repeats (Supplementary Table S3), but there were associations with CAP score for the counting backwards task (Spearman’s rho = −0.30, p = 0.01, t-test) and the Stroop test (Spearman’s rho = −0.25, p = 0.03, see Supplementary Table S3 for all tasks). It should be noted that age and CAG repeats were strongly anticorrelated in this population (Spearman’s rho = −0.63, p = 2*10−9, t-test). These observations indicate detectable longitudinal cognitive decline in pre-HD, but show weak associations with genetic burden.
To isolate decorrelated components of decline, we performed PCA on the task slopes (Fig. 3a, see Methods). This first PCA was performed on age-corrected slopes of both pre-HD and controls (Fig. 3b). Strong longitudinal group differences became apparent along PC1 (p = 2.2*10−6, Mann–Whitney U test), consisting of a weighted sum of the most reliable cognitive tasks. Some pre-HD trajectories overlapped with controls (in the sense that those subjects were “stable”), but the asymmetry became prominent in ranges away from the mean, suggesting task slopes and their PCs are sensitive to pre-HD decline.
To study heterogeneity of decline within pre-HD alone, computed a second PCA using only subjects from this group (Fig. 3c). The top 5 PCs each captured more than or around 10% of the variance (i.e., the effective equivalent of 1 task out of 10), and the bottom 5 were disregarded.
Stratification of pre-HD by longitudinal cognitive/motor decline rates using single-visit FCD
Single-task slopes (corrected for age and sex) and PCs were used for stratification models of the pre-HD population. For each such measure of decline rate, subgroups of fast, intermediate and slow-decline (or stable) subjects were defined (see Methods, flowchart in Supplementary Fig. S2, and Supplementary Table S5 for subgroup subject numbers). Slow and fast subgroups were used to train binary cross-validated classifier models using single-visit FCD maps as predictors (see Methods, flowchart in Supplementary Fig. S5). To establish robustness to site differences, we used a LOSO-CV scheme. First, we validated models in terms of their ability to effectively distinguish subjects from these extreme subgroups in test sites, excluding the intermediate subgroup. Then we studied the usefulness of the distance to the decision hyperplane as a proxy for the continuous value of cognitive decline. This key validation used test site subjects from all three subgroups. Below we summarize the results of this approach (complete results in Tables 2 and 3).
Table 2.
Task | AUC | p-value | Spearman’s rho | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
SDMT | 0.73 | 0.0023 | 0.41 | 0.00005 |
Cancelation | 0.73 | 0.002 | 0.27 | 0.005 |
Paced tap | 0.70 | 0.008 | 0.05 | 0.2977 |
Grip variability | 0.61 | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.26453 |
Stroop | 0.59 | 0.136 | 0.17 | 0.04877 |
Count backwards | 0.54 | 0.314 | 0.08 | 0.19927 |
Spot change | 0.52 | 0.396 | 0.01 | 0.4807 |
Mental rotation | 0.51 | 0.439 | −0.01 | 0.54327 |
Map search | 0.47 | 0.665 | −0.01 | 0.52993 |
Indirect circle trace | 0.36 | 0.97 | −0.14 | 0.93302 |
Summary of AUC for classifications between fast-declining and stable subjects (extreme subgroups, subject numbers in Supplementary Table S5) using single tasks (permutation test). The Spearman rank correlation corresponds to the distance between the decision hyperplane and cognitive slopes using all pre-HD subjects from the test sites (within-site permutation test, see Methods).
Table 3.
Task | AUC | p-value | Spearman’s rho | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
PC 1 | 0.67 | 0.013 | 0.18 | 0.04045 |
PC 2 | 0.59 | 0.11 | 0.06 | 0.2563 |
PC 3 | 0.56 | 0.23 | 0.09 | 0.19351 |
PC 4 | 0.73 | 0.0008 | 0.29 | 0.00306 |
PC 5 | 0.65 | 0.02 | 0.16 | 0.06194 |
Summary of AUC for classifications between fast-declining and stable subjects using principal components. The Spearman rank correlation corresponds to the distance between the decision hyperplane and cognitive slopes using all subjects from the test sites (within-site permutation test, see Methods).
Binary classification of subjects from the extreme subgroups based on cognitive slopes from single tasks was possible for several tasks: SDMT, cancelation task, and paced tapping. The strongest results were observed for the SDMT task (Fig. 4). Visual inspection of the ROC curve (Fig. 4a, AUC: 0.73, p = 0.0023, permutation test) shows the predictive power resided in the correct identification of cases with highest confidence (vertical and horizontal segments near the extremes). The result suggests robust information in FCD maps about ongoing cognitive decline.
That validation tested whether brain patterns of ongoing decline could be learned, but not its application to the larger pre-HD population. However, the binary classifier can provide information in a setting where a patient might not belong to either subgroup but to the intermediate range not considered during training. This is because linear classifiers provide a continuous output: the (signed) distance to the decision hyperplane (Fig. 4b). To validate the usefulness of this measure in the whole pre-HD population, we computed on each test site the Spearman rank correlation between the distance to the decision hyperplane and the cognitive slope using all pre-HD subjects, and taking an average across sites (see Methods, “Validation of continuous classifier output for pre-HD stratification”). The average correlation was significant for the SDMT (Spearman’s rho = 0.41, p = 0.00005, permutation test) and the cancelation tasks (Spearman’s rho = 0.27, p = 0.00464, permutation test). This is illustrated for the SDMT task in Fig. 4c. We note that the out-of-sample correlations that can be achieved are limited by noise in cognitive slopes. As a reference, an optimistic estimate for the SDMT task (see Supplementary Information) indicates a ceiling for correlations at around 0.6. The results suggest the continuous output of the classification models can be informative for stratification by longitudinal decline of subjects from the whole pre-HD population.
What neurological signals underlie these predictions? We inspected the voxel-level correlations between FCD measures and SDMT slopes (Fig. 4d), which revealed patterns of association between SDMT decline and WM functional connectivity. These included clusters around the basal ganglia (Fig. 4d, top row), but mostly consisted of increased FCD on bilaterally-symmetrical WM extensions (Fig. 4d, center row), and also reduced FCD on cortical regions, notably left temporal cortex (Fig. 4d, bottom row). These observations reveal the spatial extent of signals related to early cognitive decline, and suggest WM as a source of functional signals associated with it in pre-HD.
The finding that stratification was possible for decline along more than one individual task could indicate the existence of multiple distinct decline signals, or simply one shared signal that underlies change along the individual tasks. We used the PCs of cognitive slopes to investigate whether stratification was possible along components of decline that were uncorrelated from each other, keeping in mind that because PCs are orthogonal by design they combine tasks with different signs, limiting interpretability. Stratification was possible, using the same LOSO-CV scheme, for three PCs (1, 4, and 5), in which models successfully distinguished extreme subgroups of decline (Table 3). The first component combined most tasks with positive signs, providing a summary of global decline. While the others are harder to interpret, we note PC 4 combined SDMT, cancelation, and self-paced tapping with the same sign, which were the tasks that showed best individual results. This suggests stratification along individual tasks might not be a consequence of a single dominant direction of decline, and instead stratification along multiple measures survives in decorrelated decline components.
Robustness of FCD patterns to non-functional covariates
The goal of our analyses was not only to establish the predictive power of FCD maps but also to interpret them as reflecting functional signals. Therefore, even if predictions have been validated across imaging sites, we also verified the robustness of the identified patterns when controlling for non-functional sources of variation. In particular, we trained the main multivariate classification algorithms presented above (pre-HD vs. controls and stratification by SDMT decline) after correcting FCD features by a local atrophy measure (grey matter concentration as estimated by voxel-based morphometry, see Methods), removing variance attributable to scanning sites (even if there were no group differences on any of them, as assessed by chi-squared tests, see also Methods for details on harmonization), and skipping FCP map normalization by its median. Detection of pre-HD signatures (vs. controls) with these alternative FCD maps had similar predictive power (AUC: 0.64, p = 0.0005, permutation test), and the classifier weights showed a similar cluster in the globus pallidus (Supplementary Fig. S6). Performance of stratification by SDMT change was again comparable to the original model (AUC: 0.69, p = 0.007 permutation test; Spearman’s rho = 0.37, p = 0.00025, permutation test) and the voxel weights of the classifier revealed similar clusters (Supplementary Fig. S7) on white matter, while grey matter (GM) clusters on the temporal lobe were no longer observed, indicating perhaps concurrent GM atrophy. We also evaluated full-brain GMC maps (including WM voxels and with the same resolution as FCD maps), in terms of multivariate detection of pre-HD change (AUC 0.52, p = 0.378), and stratification by SDMT slopes (AUC 0.53, p = 0.39; Spearman’s r: 0.09, p = 0.24), showing low predictive power. Taken together, these observations suggest that the predictive patterns found are unlikely to be a reflection of local atrophy, site, or effects of normalization choice.
We also considered effects of in-scanner motion, as quantified by the mean framewise displacement (Methods, “Removal of motion associations”). Both groups had very similar motion distributions (pre-HD vs controls, p = 0.77, Mann-Whitney U-test, Supplementary Fig. S7). We computed motion associations with cognitive decline rates (Supplementary Table S7), which was significant for the SDMT (Spearman’s rho = −0.32, p = 0.006) and “spot the change” tasks (Spearman’s rho = 0.28, p = 0.02). Head motion association was not sufficient (e.g. “spot the change” task) nor necessary (e.g. cancelation task) for stratification results. We performed direct controls for FCD features in SDMT stratification (see Methods, “Removal of motion associations”). Both model performance and brain patterns were robust to this control (AUC: 0.73, p = 0.0009, permutation test; Spearman’s rho = 0.35, p = 0.00031, permutation test; Supplementary Fig. S9).
Finally, we considered whether addition of CAG repeats to the FCD features improved the stratification results. Results with these combined features were almost identical (Supplementary Table S8).
Discussion
We investigated the reliability and stratification power of FCD in pre-HD using a cross-validated multivariate approach. FCD patterns showed high within-subject reliability. We found that longitudinal slopes provided a sensitive measure of early cognitive decline and FCD features could be used to stratify the population across multiple components of change. Together, these results highlight the potential of FCD as a tool with valuable applications.
Relative to structural markers, the literature of resting state measurements in HD is relatively recent14. Cross-sectional correlations of resting-state fMRI with clinical measures were reported on predefined components47,48. With that approach, cross-sectional group differences were observed49 within the TRACK-HD study but longitudinal group-differences were not50. The Track-On HD extension allowed for the investigation of putative compensation mechanisms20 and relationships with the anatomical connectome24. A valuable step toward predictive applications has been a recent study combining structural and resting-state markers in 19 premanifest patients used to predict transition to the manifest state51. Our goal was to further push the field from descriptive brain mapping toward predictive models that directly detect the population heterogeneity unexplained by genetic disease load, before manifestation.
Key for the usefulness of fMRI as a tool is verifying its robustness across known sources of variability such as sites and individuals. Site heterogeneity has been suggested to be a major challenge for robustness of models, and of LOSO-CV has been proposed as a methodology to ensure generalizability12. This was the motivation behind our choice. However, it is possible that the efforts made to minimize site discrepancies during acquisition in Track-ON20 make site differences unrepresentative of those found in other potential practical applications. In terms of within-subject consistency, concerns have been raised regarding the reliability of fMRI for biomarkers to be applied at the individual level52. The use of multivariate models avoid the important consistency issues of single links: models that rely on global patterns have been shown to be robust enough to enable individual fingerprinting11. This allowed us to extend assessments from other studies of test-retest reliability for rs-fMRI in pre-HD53 to the individual level. We found that FCD maps contained a highly reproducible signature that identified individuals. This extends robustness to a new feature type, FCD, but also in time, since the scans considered were separated by one or two years. More important neurologically, we found that the mild pre-HD signatures are highly reproducible within-subjects (0.95 consistency in pre-HD subjects), highlighting the importance of sorting out the reproducible heterogeneity.
The single clearest signature of pre-HD in FCD was bilateral hyper-connectivity centred on the globus pallidus and its interface with the putamen. This is striking because the globus pallidus is not prominent in markers of volume change compared to the putamen. Anomalous FCD of the globus pallidus in the past was perhaps previously unobserved because of the use of predefined components47 or networks20 that excluded this region. It was unperturbed when correcting by GMC. In fact, the same models using full-brain GMC maps did not significantly distinguish the groups. We note that GMC could distinguish groups with 70% accuracy in the larger TRACK-HD cohort40 in models that included only grey matter voxels, higher resolution voxels, and a different cross-validation scheme. Even then, the globus pallidus did not contribute to that discrimination. Interestingly also, the pallidus pattern seemed to be a generic signature of pre-HD, but did not appear to be relevant for the cognitive decline stratifications within the pre-HD population.
In contrast, the recurrent finding in our stratification by cognitive decline is the hyper-connectivity of white matter (WM). Although until recently disregarded as noise, there is no evidence against the detection of physiological BOLD signals in WM54. On the contrary, several studies have found reproducible WM activations in tasks requiring interhemispheric integration55–58, and several features that speak to their physiological origin: they depend on diffusion anisotropy and fibre orientation59, on activity of connected cortical areas18,19,29, and alertness28. In resting state fMRI, highly reproducible WM networks have been reported and are associated with combinations of known tracts18. Of particular interest, WM activations were reported in SDMT specifically60, the task where we found the largest WM associations. WM signatures have started only recently to be exploited as potential markers of brain disease16. This is the first demonstration of functional WM signals in HD. Future studies should focus on the mechanisms of white matter functional signals in neural degeneration.
We found SDMT to be more strongly associated with FCD features than any other measure, either individual tasks or PCs. SDMT has long been recognized as one of the best measures of early deterioration in HD5,61. In this dataset, it is the task that showed strongest group differences in longitudinal decline. It is possible that this sensitivity of the task to tracking progression, in combination with reliance on white matter integrity62,63, explains its advantage over the other tasks considered. PCs were meant to isolate different dimensions of cognitive change in an unsupervised way. In addition, they are orthogonal by design, and as a result of this constraint can mix tasks sensitive to disease progression with those that are not. These properties of the method could explain why a high-sensitivity task could have a stratification advantage over PCs.
The interpretation of the current results should not be considered separately from the limitations of this study. First, the cohort was restricted to premanifest participants, which limited our conclusions to the premanifest period and likely introduced a selection bias, as suggested by the strong anticorrelation between age and CAG repeats. Similarly, inclusion of only right-handed subjects, common in the neuroscientific literature, can limit clinical applications. In addition, cross-validated analyses could have overoptimistic results, particularly if care is not taken to exclude the possibility of “double-dipping”, as in the choice of model hyperparameters64. While we made efforts to avoid such instances using nested-cross-validation65, conclusions should be validated on a separate, independent cohort. In terms of cognitive/motor changes, available data included visits that preceded rs-fMRI acquisition for the sake of maximizing power to detect changes. Because of that, one should not interpret our models as evidence of the ability to predict future decline but rather to detect ongoing cognitive change; and prediction in this context refers to the ability to make assessments for individuals not considered during model training. Similarly, AUC-based validations were based on binary labels that did not include the full pre-HD population and should not be taken as evidence of usefulness in more general clinical contexts. Correlations with continuous decline using the full population (our second validation) should be considered as effect sizes for that scenario. Finally, the signatures found by our models are partly a consequence of the type of model chosen. Multiple model types are possible, with different complexities. However, this is an issue for every analysis effort, even univariate comparisons, which assume a type of model implicitly. We used a linear model for its simplicity of interpretation.
Early progression of HD is usually modelled in terms of the CAP score5,66 and predictive models have used this, sometimes combined with cognitive61 and brain measurements51,67, for the prediction of the age of diagnosis or conversion68,69. Part of the value of our approach lies in its acceptance that degeneration trajectories are heterogeneous, placing individuals in a multidimensional space. Accordingly, we focused on the prediction of longitudinal change as opposed to estimating proximity to a given behavioural threshold, as schematized in Fig. 5. Such multidimensional estimates could not only inform patient selection in clinical trials, but potentially be used for individualized quantification of treatment effect sizes and expected patient evolution from ongoing decline, increasing sensitivity.
In conclusion, we found robust evidence for the usefulness of FCD as a stratification tool. This is critical because subjects showing signs of decline are unlikely to slow their course in the absence intervention. In this context, FCD provides a new window into the evaluation, in a single visit, of ongoing change, an important type of assessment for which few alternatives exist.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
Data used in this work was generously provided by the participants in the TRACK-HD study and made available by Dr. Sarah Tabrizi, Principal Investigator, University College London. The authors would like to thank Sara Berger for comments on early versions of the manuscript, Rachel Ostrand for proof-reading the manuscript, Pouya Bashivan for technical computer scripts, two anonymous reviewers whose suggestions shaped the manuscript substantially, and CHDI Foundation for financial support.
Author contributions
P.P., E.C., I.R., J.H.W., A.W., C.S. and G.A.C. conceived and designed the study. P.P. and E.C. analysed the data. P.P. wrote the manuscript. P.P., E.C., D.P. and G.A.C. edited the manuscript.
Data availability
For the privacy of gene carriers and control participants who generously provided data for the Track-On HD study, the dataset is not posted online for direct download. The original unprocessed TRACK-IDS-2015-10-R2.1 dataset will be made available upon request after appropriate data use agreements are signed. Please direct inquiries to info@chdifoundation.org with the words [IBM-CHDI Resting State fMRI Study] in the subject line.
Code availability
All scripts used for generating the results of this study are publicly available as a Github repository (https://github.com/polosecki/ton_rfmri_repo) for reproducibility.
Competing interests
PP, EC, IR, and GAC disclose that they are employed by IBM Research, which is the research branch of IBM Corporation, and GAC owns stock in IBM corporation.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-58074-8.
References
- 1.Holland D, McEvoy LK, Desikan RS, Dale A. M.Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Enrichment and stratification for predementia Alzheimer disease clinical trials. PLoS One. 2012;7:e47739. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Wang X-J, Krystal JH. Computational psychiatry. Neuron. 2014;84:638–654. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Huys QJM, Maia TV, Frank MJ. Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:404–413. doi: 10.1038/nn.4238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Polosecki P, et al. Computational psychiatry: Advancing predictive modeling of neurodegeneration with neuroimaging of Huntington’s disease. IBM J. Res. Dev. 2017;61:4:1–4:10. doi: 10.1147/JRD.2017.2648700. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ross CA, et al. Huntington disease: natural history, biomarkers and prospects for therapeutics. Nature Reviews. Neurol. 2014;10:204–216. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2014.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Shoulson I, et al. A controlled clinical trial of baclofen as protective therapy in early Huntington’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 1989;25:252–259. doi: 10.1002/ana.410250308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Paulsen JS, et al. Detection of Huntington’s disease decades before diagnosis: the Predict-HD study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 2008;79:874–880. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.128728. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Tabrizi SJ, et al. Biological and clinical manifestations of Huntington’s disease in the longitudinal TRACK-HD study: cross-sectional analysis of baseline data. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8:791–801. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70170-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Tabrizi SJ, et al. Predictors of phenotypic progression and disease onset in premanifest and early-stage Huntington’s disease in the TRACK-HD study: analysis of 36-month observational data. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12:637–649. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70088-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gabrieli JDE, Ghosh SS, Whitfield-Gabrieli S. Prediction as a Humanitarian and Pragmatic Contribution from Human Cognitive Neuroscience. Neuron. 2015;85:11–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pannunzi M, et al. Resting-state fMRI correlations: From link-wise unreliability to whole brain stability. NeuroImage. 2017;157:250–262. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Abraham A, et al. Deriving reproducible biomarkers from multi-site resting-state data: An Autism-based example. NeuroImage. 2017;147:736–745. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Georgiou-Karistianis N, Scahill R, Tabrizi SJ, Squitieri F, Aylward E. Structural MRI in Huntington’s disease and recommendations for its potential use in clinical trials. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013;37:480–490. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Unschuld PG, et al. Impaired cortico-striatal functional connectivity in prodromal Huntington’s Disease. Neurosci. Lett. 2012;514:204–209. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.02.095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Eguíluz VM, Chialvo DR, Cecchi GA, Baliki M, Apkarian AV. Scale-Free Brain Functional Networks. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2005;94:018102. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.94.018102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Chen X, et al. Extraction of dynamic functional connectivity from brain grey matter and white matter for MCI classification. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017;38:5019–5034. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ding Z, et al. Detection of synchronous brain activity in white matter tracts at rest and under functional loading. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;8(2017):11567–600. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1711567115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Peer M, Nitzan M, Bick AS, Levin N, Arzy S. Evidence for functional networks within the human brain’s white matter. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:6394–6407. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3872-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wu X, et al. Functional connectivity and activity of white matter in somatosensory pathways under tactile stimulations. NeuroImage. 2017;152:371–380. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Klöppel S, et al. Compensation in Preclinical Huntington’s Disease: Evidence From the Track-On HD Study. EBioMedicine. 2015;2:1420–1429. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2015.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gorges M, et al. Intact sensory-motor network structure and function in far from onset premanifest Huntington’s disease. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:43841. doi: 10.1038/srep43841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Pool E-M, Rehme AK, Eickhoff SB, Fink GR, Grefkes C. Functional resting-state connectivity of the human motor network: Differences between right- and left-handers. NeuroImage. 2015;109:298–306. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hubert M, Debruyne M. Minimum covariance determinant. Wiley Interdiscip. Reviews: Computational Stat. 2010;2:36–43. doi: 10.1002/wics.61. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.McColgan P, et al. White matter predicts functional connectivity in premanifest Huntington’s disease. Ann. Clin. Transl. Neurol. 2017;4:106–118. doi: 10.1002/acn3.384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL. NeuroImage. 2012;62:782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gorgolewski K, et al. Nipype: a flexible, lightweight and extensible neuroimaging data processing framework in python. Front. Neuroinform. 2011;5:13. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2011.00013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage. 2007;37:90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wu T-L, et al. Effects of anesthesia on resting state BOLD signals in white matter of non-human primates. Magnetic Reson. Imaging. 2016;34:1235–1241. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2016.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Marussich, L., Lu, K.-H., Wen, H. & Liu, Z. Mapping White-Matter Functional Organization at Rest and during Naturalistic Visual Perception. NeuroImage, 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.005 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 30.Allen EA, et al. Tracking whole-brain connectivity dynamics in the resting state. Cereb. Cortex. 2014;24:663–676. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Rish I, et al. Schizophrenia as a Network Disease: Disruption of Emergent Brain Function in Patients with Auditory Hallucinations. PLoS One. 2013;8:e50625. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Defazio, A., Bach, F. & Lacoste-Julien, S. SAGA: A fast incremental gradient method with support for non-strongly convex composite objectives. In 1646–1654 (2014).
- 33.Gheiratmand M, et al. Learning stable and predictive network-based patterns of schizophrenia and its clinical symptoms. Nat. Publ. Group. 2017;3:22. doi: 10.1038/s41537-017-0022-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Pedregosa F, et al. Scikit-learn: Machine learning in Python. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 2011;12:2825–2830. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Abraham A, et al. Machine learning for neuroimaging with scikit-learn. Front. Neuroinform. 2014;8:14. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2014.00014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Baliki MN, et al. Corticostriatal functional connectivity predicts transition to chronic back pain. Nat. Neurosci. 2012;15:1117–1119. doi: 10.1038/nn.3153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Liegeois R, et al. Resting brain dynamics at different timescales capture distinct aspects of human behavior. Nat. Comms. 2019;10:2317. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-10317-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Oakes TR, et al. Integrating VBM into the General Linear Model with voxelwise anatomical covariates. NeuroImage. 2007;34:500–508. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Good CD, et al. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage. 2001;14:21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Castro E, et al. Baseline multimodal information predicts future motor impairment in premanifest Huntington’s disease. NeuroImage: Clin. 2018;19:443–453. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Johnson WE, Li C, Rabinovic A. Adjusting batch effects in microarray expression data using empirical Bayes methods. Biostatistics. 2007;8:118–127. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/kxj037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Fortin J-P, et al. Harmonization of cortical thickness measurements across scanners and sites. NeuroImage. 2018;167:104–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Fortin J-P, et al. Harmonization of multi-site diffusion tensor imaging data. NeuroImage. 2017;161:149–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Parkes L, Fulcher B, Yücel M, Fornito A. An evaluation of the efficacy, reliability, and sensitivity of motion correction strategies for resting-state functional MRI. NeuroImage. 2018;171:415–436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Nalci A, Luo W, Liu TT. Nuisance effects in inter-scan functional connectivity estimates before and after nuisance regression. NeuroImage. 2019;202:116005. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.07.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing on JSTOR. Journal of the royal statistical society Series B, 10.2307/2346101 (1995).
- 47.Poudel GR, et al. Abnormal synchrony of resting state networks in premanifest and symptomatic Huntington disease: the IMAGE-HD study. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2014;39:87–96. doi: 10.1503/jpn.120226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Wolf RC, et al. Abnormal resting-state connectivity of motor and cognitive networks in early manifest Huntington’s disease. Psychol. Med. 2014;44:3341–3356. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Dumas EM, et al. Reduced functional brain connectivity prior to and after disease onset in Huntington’s disease. NeuroImage: Clin. 2013;2:377–384. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Odish OFF, et al. Longitudinal resting state fMRI analysis in healthy controls and premanifest Huntington’s disease gene carriers: a three-year follow-up study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015;36:110–119. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Mason SL, et al. Predicting clinical diagnosis in Huntington’s disease: An imaging polymarker. Ann. Neurol. 2018;83:532–543. doi: 10.1002/ana.25171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Nord CL, Gray A, Charpentier CJ, Robinson OJ, Roiser JP. Unreliability of putative fMRI biomarkers during emotional face processing. NeuroImage. 2017;156:119–127. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Seibert TM, Majid DSA, Aron AR, Corey-Bloom J, Brewer JB. Stability of resting fMRI interregional correlations analyzed in subject-native space: A one-year longitudinal study in healthy adults and premanifest Huntington’s disease. NeuroImage. 2012;59:2452–2463. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Gawryluk JR, Mazerolle EL, D’Arcy RCN. Does functional MRI detect activation in white matter? A review of emerging evidence, issues, and future directions. Front. Neurosci. 2014;8:145. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Tettamanti M, et al. Interhemispheric transmission of visuomotor information in humans: fMRI evidence. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;88:1051–1058. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.2.1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Omura K, et al. Different mechanisms involved in interhemispheric transfer of visuomotor information. Neuroreport. 2004;15:2707–2711. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200404290-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Weber B, et al. Attention and Interhemispheric Transfer: A Behavioral and fMRI Study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005;17:113–123. doi: 10.1162/0898929052880002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Gawryluk JR, Brewer KD, Beyea SD, D’Arcy RCN. Optimizing the detection of white matter fMRI using asymmetric spin echo spiral. NeuroImage. 2009;45:83–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Hernández-Torres E, et al. Anisotropic cerebral vascular architecture causes orientation dependency in cerebral blood flow and volume measured with dynamic susceptibility contrast magnetic resonance imaging. J. Cereb. Blood Flow. Metab. 2016;37:1108–1119. doi: 10.1177/0271678X16653134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Gawryluk JR, Mazerolle EL, Beyea SD, D’Arcy RCN. Functional MRI activation in white matter during the Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:589. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Long JD, et al. Validation of a prognostic index for Huntington’s disease. Mov. Disord. 2016;32:256–263. doi: 10.1002/mds.26838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Felmingham KL, Baguley IJ, Green AM. Effects of diffuse axonal injury on speed of information processing following severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology. 2004;18:564–571. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Hoffmann S, Tittgemeyer M, Cramon von DY. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2007;20:275–280. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32810c8e87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Varma S, Simon R. Bias in error estimation when using cross-validation for model selection. BMC Bioinforma. 2006;7:91. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-7-91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Krstajic D, Buturovic LJ, Leahy DE, Thomas S. Cross-validation pitfalls when selecting and assessing regression and classification models. J. Cheminform. 2014;6:10. doi: 10.1186/1758-2946-6-10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Zhang Y, et al. Indexing disease progression at study entry with individuals at-risk for Huntington disease. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2011;156B:751–763. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Long JD, Paulsen JS. Multivariate prediction of motor diagnosis in Huntington’s disease: 12 years of PREDICT‐HD. Mov. Disord. 2015;30:1664–1672. doi: 10.1002/mds.26364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Langbehn DR, Hayden M, Paulsen JS, Group TP-HIOTHS. CAG-Repeat Length and the Age of Onset in Huntington Disease (HD): A Review and Validation Study of Statistical Approaches. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2010;153B:397–n/a. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Penney JB, Vonsattel JP, MacDonald ME, Gusella JF, Myers RH. CAG repeat number governs the development rate of pathology in Huntington’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 1997;41:689–692. doi: 10.1002/ana.410410521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
For the privacy of gene carriers and control participants who generously provided data for the Track-On HD study, the dataset is not posted online for direct download. The original unprocessed TRACK-IDS-2015-10-R2.1 dataset will be made available upon request after appropriate data use agreements are signed. Please direct inquiries to info@chdifoundation.org with the words [IBM-CHDI Resting State fMRI Study] in the subject line.
All scripts used for generating the results of this study are publicly available as a Github repository (https://github.com/polosecki/ton_rfmri_repo) for reproducibility.