Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Mar 13;9:e53129. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53129

Supramammillary nucleus synchronizes with dentate gyrus to regulate spatial memory retrieval through glutamate release

Yadong Li 1,2, Hechen Bao 1,2,, Yanjia Luo 1,2,, Cherasse Yoan 3, Heather Anne Sullivan 4, Luis Quintanilla 1,2,5, Ian Wickersham 4, Michael Lazarus 3, Yen-Yu Ian Shih 6, Juan Song 1,2,
Editors: Laura L Colgin7, Hiroshi Ito8
PMCID: PMC7069722  PMID: 32167473

Abstract

The supramammillary nucleus (SuM) provides substantial innervation to the dentate gyrus (DG). It remains unknown how the SuM and DG coordinate their activities at the circuit level to regulate spatial memory. Additionally, SuM co-releases GABA and glutamate to the DG, but the relative role of GABA versus glutamate in regulating spatial memory remains unknown. Here we report that SuM-DG Ca2+ activities are highly correlated during spatial memory retrieval as compared to the moderate correlation during memory encoding when mice are performing a location discrimination task. Supporting this evidence, we demonstrate that the activity of SuM neurons or SuM-DG projections is required for spatial memory retrieval. Furthermore, we show that SuM glutamate transmission is necessary for both spatial memory retrieval and highly-correlated SuM-DG activities during spatial memory retrieval. Our studies identify a long-range SuM-DG circuit linking two highly correlated subcortical regions to regulate spatial memory retrieval through SuM glutamate release.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

The hippocampus mediates certain forms of learning and memory, such as spatial information processing and pattern separation. The dentate gyrus (DG) is the first input region of the hippocampus, in which DG granule cells (GCs) receive major excitatory inputs from the entorhinal cortex (EC) and send excitatory outputs to CA3 through mossy fibers (Bao and Song, 2018; Knierim and Neunuebel, 2016; Treves et al., 2008; Vivar and van Praag, 2013). GCs are the principal neurons in the DG that exhibit sparse firing, thus establishing sparse GC-CA3 connectivity to contribute to spatial information processing (Knierim and Neunuebel, 2016). Besides the major cortical inputs from EC, the DG also receives inputs from subcortical regions, such as the medial septum and the supramammillary nucleus (SuM) of the hypothalamus (Leranth and Hajszan, 2007). It has been well established that these subcortical regions play critical roles in regulating hippocampal theta rhythm, which is widely known to be important for spatial information processing (Kocsis and Vertes, 1997; Pan and McNaughton, 2004). Therefore, it is critical to understand how the DG integrates synaptic inputs from the subcortical regions to modulate hippocampal-dependent learning and memory process.

In this study, we focus on the SuM inputs to the DG, based on the dense projections from SuM to DG GCs (Berger et al., 2001; Pedersen et al., 2017; Soussi et al., 2010). Both SuM and DG have been implicated in hippocampal-dependent spatial memory (Ito et al., 2018; Vertes, 2015), but how SuM and DG coordinate their activities during distinct stages of the memory process and ultimately regulate this process is unknown. In addition, the precise neural circuit that regulates the spatial memory process remains to be determined. Furthermore, recent studies demonstrated that SuM-DG axonal terminals co-release glutamate and GABA to the DG (Hashimotodani et al., 2018; Root et al., 2018). However, the relative role of GABA versus glutamate released from SuM neurons in regulating the spatial memory process remains to be established.

Here we applied an in vivo multi-fiber photometry system to simultaneously record the Ca2+ activity from both the SuM and DG. We demonstrated that activities of SuM and DG neurons become significantly higher and correlated during the spatial memory retrieval than those during the spatial memory encoding in a hippocampal-dependent behavioral test. Furthermore, we employed circuit-based approaches and showed that the activity of both SuM neurons and SuM-DG projections are required for spatial memory retrieval. Finally, we used the viral-mediated genetic knockdown approach and showed that glutamate (but not GABA) release from SuM is necessary for both spatial memory retrieval and highly correlated SuM-DG Ca2+ activities during spatial memory retrieval.

Results

Ca2+ activities of SuM and DG are increased and highly correlated during spatial memory retrieval

While long-range anatomical connections enable multiregional interactions (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009), such interactions must be dynamic to cope with changing behavioral demands (Ito et al., 2018). Therefore, we sought to examine the inter-regional correlation between the SuM and the DG neuronal activity at baseline and during distinct phases of the spatial memory process. We first assessed whether neuronal activity in the SuM is functionally correlated with that in the DG at baseline. For this purpose, we simultaneously recorded spontaneous Ca2+ dynamics from SuM neurons and DG GCs labeled with CaMKII-GCaMP6f in freely moving mice using a customized in vivo fiber photometry recording system (Inoue et al., 2019; Meng et al., 2018; Figure 1A–B, Figure 1—figure supplement 1A–C). Correlation analysis based on frequency spectrums showed that the Ca2+ events from the SuM and DG neurons are correlated in the range of 0.1–0.5 Hz (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D–I). Therefore, we extracted the Ca2+ signals from this frequency range, and found that the Ca2+ events from these two brain regions were moderately correlated (R = 0.4, p<0.001) at baseline when mice were placed at their home cages (Figure 1D–E).

Figure 1. Ca2+ activities of the SuM and DG at home cage and during the spatial memory test.

(A) Experimental diagram of the DG and SuM calcium recording at the home cage. (B) Coronal sections showing photometry fiber traces and CaMKII-GCaMP/tdTomato expressed neurons in the DG (left) and SuM (right). Scale bar = 100 µm. (C) A sample of normalized ΔF/F of GCaMP6f signals from the DG and SuM. The traces of GCaMP activity were moderately correlated with R = 0.31, p < 0.0001 at home cage. (D) Sample of GCaMP6f traces of the DG and SuM in 0.1–0.5 Hz spectrum. The traces of GCaMP signals were moderately correlated with R = 0.48, p < 0.0001. (E) Mean correlation of SuM and DG calcium activity in 0.1–0.5 Hz spectrum. n = 10 mice, R = 0.40, p<0.0001. (F) Experimental illustration of in vivo fiber photometry recordings during the NPR test. After 3 days of habituation, mice were recorded for calcium activity during distinct phases (spatial memory encoding versus retrieval) of the NPR test. (G) Time spent exploring the objects during the NPR test. In the test phase, time spent exploring the novel-location object (B) was significantly increased (n = 8 mice, paired t-test, t1,7 = 6.843, **p < 0.01). No difference was found during familiarization (n = 8 mice, paired t-test, t1,7 = 1.593, p = 0.16). (H, L) Scaled color plot of DG and SuM calcium activity while exploring object A (old location) and object B (novel location) during familiarization (H) and test (L) phase, respectively. n = 49, 62, 39, 59 trails. (I, M) Averaged DG and SuM calcium activities during familiarization (I) and test (M). Semi-transparent borders indicate ± SEM. (J, N) Cumulative DG and SuM calcium activities in the NPR test. Time points when mice started to explore the objects were defined as 0 s. Calcium activity was aligned within a 2 s time window at each phase: pre-exploring (−3 ~ −1 s), exploring (−1 ~ 1 s) and post-exploring (1 ~ 3 s). Cumulative activity = ∆F/F × Time (2 s). Calcium activity in both the DG and SuM increased when mice were exploring objects/locations during familiarization (J) and test (N). (K, O) Correlation of the SuM and DG calcium activity during familiarization (K) and test (O). Calcium activity of the SuM and DG during exploration was highly correlated (R = 0.9292, p < 0.0001) during spatial memory retrieval, as compared to a moderate correlation (R = 0.5438, p<0.0108) during spatial memory encoding. (P, Q) Cumulative activity of the DG (P) and SuM (Q) was increased during test, compared to familiarization. DG: Unpaired t-test, t207 = 2.009, *p=0.0459. SuM: Unpaired t-test, t207 = 1.975, *p = 0.0496.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Experimental protocol of fiber photometry recording.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Schematic illustration of the photometry system used for measuring red and green fluorescent sensors in the SuM and DG. CaMKII-GCaMP6f and CAG-tdTomato were delivered to the left DG and SuM. (B) Co-expression of GCaMP6f and tdTomato in DG GCs. Scale bar = 20 µm. (C) Mixed emission spectrum of GCaMP6f and tdTomato. (D) Unmixing coefficients of GCaMP6f and tdTomato by fitting signals to standard emission curves. TdTomato signals were further used as motion correction during recording. (E) Sample traces of simultaneous recording from the SuM and DG post unmixing and normalized process. Those data were used for activity analysis. (F) The color code sample frequency of SuM and DG calcium signals were mainly appeared at 0.1–0.5 Hz. (G) The calcium signals > 0.5 Hz were lower than 0.01 dB/Hz. Due to the low calcium signal associated with >0.5 Hz, we selected 0.1–0.5 Hz frequency range to analyze correlation of calcium activities from the SuM and DG. (H) Heatmap of the correlation coefficient in SuM and DG GCaMP frequency spectrum. (I) Correlation of SuM and DG GCaMP signals in 0.1–0.5 Hz range during home cage recording.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Low correlation of GCaMP activity in EC and DG at home cage and during the NPR test.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(A) Experimental diagram of the DG and EC calcium recording. (B) Coronal sections showing photometry fiber traces and CaMKII-GCaMP/tdTomato expressing neurons in the DG (left) and EC (right). Scale bar = 100 µm. (C–D) Samples (C) and correlation (D) of calcium activities of the DG and EC from mice at home cage. n = 4, R = 0.22, p > 0.05. Correlation of SuM and DG calcium activities in 0.1–0.5 Hz range were analyzed. (E) Scaled color plot of DG and EC GCaMP activities in NPR test period, n = 41 trails. (F) Average of DG and EC calcium activities in the test period. Transparent borders indicate ± SEM. During the exploring period, both DG and EC activities were increased. (G) Cumulative activity of the DG and EC during the exploring period (−1 ~ 1 s) in the NPR test. (H) Correlation of averaged DG and SuM calcium activity during the NPR test period. R = 0.21, p > 0.05.

To assess the involvement of the SuM and DG during spatial memory process, we employed an in vivo multi-fiber photometry system and recorded the Ca2+ dynamics in SuM neurons and DG GCs during a hippocampal-dependent spatial memory task, novel place recognition (NPR) test (Leger et al., 2013; Sawangjit et al., 2018; Figure 1F). During the NPR test, mice were familiarized with the relative locations of two objects (spatial memory encoding phase) followed by a test phase to identify an object placed in a novel location (spatial memory retrieval phase). Consistent with previous reports (Sawangjit et al., 2018), we found that during spatial memory encoding (familiarization phase), mice spent equal amount of time exploring both objects; while during spatial memory retrieval (test phase), mice spent significantly more time exploring the object associated with the new location than that associated with the old location (Figure 1G). Interestingly, during memory encoding, when mice were investigating objects at their relative locations, Ca2+ activities in the SuM and DG were moderately increased from the baseline (Figure 1H–J) and were moderately correlated (Figure 1K, R = 0.54, p = 0.01). Strikingly, during spatial memory retrieval when mice were trying to recall the objects/locations, the Ca2+ activities in the SuM and DG were significantly increased from the baseline (Figure 1L–N), and Ca2+ events were highly correlated (Figure 1O, R = 0.92, p<0.0001). Higher correlation of the Ca2+ activities between the SuM and DG during spatial memory retrieval appeared to be due to synchronized increase of SuM and DG activity, which was not observed during memory encoding (Figure 1K,O). While Ca2+ activities in the SuM and DG were increased during both spatial memory encoding and retrieval, they were significantly higher in both SuM and DG during memory retrieval than encoding (Figure 1P–Q).

To further explore whether increased correlation of the SuM and DG activity during spatial memory retrieval is unique to these two brain regions, we simultaneously recorded Ca2+ dynamics from DG GCs and glutamatergic neurons in layer II of the lateral EC when mice were at their home cage or performing the NPR test (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). EC is known to be highly involved in spatial memory and layer II neurons form synapses with DG GCs via the perforant pathway (Montchal et al., 2019; Reagh et al., 2018; Roy et al., 2017; Valeeva et al., 2019). Interestingly, despite that EC and DG are highly connected anatomically, their Ca2+ activities exhibit low correlation when mice were at their home cage (Figure 1—figure supplement 2C–D, R = 0.22, p > 0.05). Importantly, although both the DG and EC activities were increased during spatial memory retrieval in the NPR test, the correlation of their Ca2+ activities was not significantly altered (Figure 1—figure supplement 2E–H, R = 0.21, p>0.05). These results indicate that the high correlation of DG and EC activities may not be related to spatial memory retrieval.

Taken together, these data suggest that activities in the SuM and DG and the level of correlation between these subcortical regions are dependent on distinct phases of the spatial memory process. Specifically, activities of the SuM and DG become significantly higher and correlated during spatial memory retrieval than those during spatial memory encoding.

SuM activity is required for regulating spatial memory retrieval

Having identified that the activities of both SuM neurons and DG GCs are increased during spatial memory retrieval (Figure 1L–N), we next sought to address whether SuM activity is required for modulating the activity of DG GCs and spatial memory retrieval. It has been suggested that there are two distinct pathways from the SuM to DG with distinct neurotransmitter systems. One pathway originates from the lateral SuM (SuML) expressing markers for both GABA (Vgat: vesicular GABA transporter) and glutamate (Vglut2: vesicular glutamate transporter 2) (Pedersen et al., 2017; Soussi et al., 2010). The other pathway originates from the medial SuM (SuMM) mainly expressing VGLUT2 (Vertes, 2015). In the current study, we mainly targeted SuML based on our rabies-based monosynaptic retrograde tracing (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A-G). Specifically, our rabies tracing revealed that major inputs from SuM to DG GCs are located in the SuML (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A-E), and nearly 80% of DG-projecting SuML neurons are positive for GABA (Figure 2—figure supplement 1F-G). By contrast, the input neurons labeled in the SuMM is sparse. These data support SuML inputs as the major inputs to DG GCs. Therefore, we specifically target SuML by injecting AAVs to the SuML using Vgat-Cre mice. By doing this, we are able to preferentially manipulate the activity of Vgat+/Vglut2+ neurons in the SuML. Supporting the retrograde tracing data, our anterograde tracing of SuML Vgat+ (SuMLVgat) neurons confirmed that SuMLVgat neurons send dense projections to the granule cell layer of the DG (Figure 2—figure supplement 1H-J).

To address the role of SuML neurons in regulating dentate GC activities, we took a chemogenetic approach by delivering AAVs expressing excitatory DREADDs (AAV-DIO-hM3Dq) or inhibitory DREADDs (AAV-DIO-hM4Di) to the SuML of Vgat-Cre mice to activate or inhibit the SuMLVgat neurons, respectively, through CNO administration (Figure 2A). Interestingly, we found that activation or inhibition of SuMLVgat neurons increases or decreases the density of c-Fos+ GCs, respectively (Figure 2B–E). To validate the c-Fos data, we recorded Ca2+ dynamics of DG GCs labeled with GCaMP6f upon chemogenetic activation of SuML neurons using in vivo fiber photometry (Figure 2F). Consistent with increased density of cFos+ GCs, we found increased Ca2+ activity in DG GCs upon chemogenetic activation of SuML neurons (Figure 2G–I). These results suggest that the SuMLVgat activity is required for modulating GC activity through the excitatory glutamate component.

Figure 2. SuM activity is required for spatial memory retrieval.

(A) Experimental scheme. Coronal sections showing mCherry expression in SuMLVgat neurons in Vgat-Cre mice. Scale bar = 100 µm. (B) Sample images showing the c-Fos expression in SuMLVgat neurons 1 hr after CNO injection from AAV-DIO-hM3Dq- and AAV-DIO-hM4Di- injected mice. Scale bar = 100 µm. (C) Quantification of the density of c-Fos+/mCherry+ SuM neurons. CNO 1 mg/kg induced higher density of c-Fos+/hM3Dq-mCherry+ cells, but lower density of c-Fos+/hM4Di-mCherry+ cells. (n = 5–6 mice, one-way ANOVA followed by PLSD post hoc test, **p < 0.01). (D) Sample images showing c-Fos expression in the DG 1 hr after CNO injection from AAV-DIO-hM3Dq- and AAV-DIO-hM4Di- injected mice. Scale bar = 100 µm. (E) Quantification of c-Fos expression in DG GCs. C-Fos+ GCs were increased in hM3Dq-mice, but decreased in hM4Di-mice 1 hr after CNO injection. GCL: granule cell layer. (n = 5–6 mice, one-way ANOVA followed by PLSD post hoc test, **p < 0.01). (F) Diagram of in vivo photometry recording. AAV-CaMKII-GCaMP6f was injected in the left DG, and AAV-CaMKII-GCaMP6f mixed with CaMKII-hM3Dq was injected in the left SuM. Optic fibers were implanted above the DG and SuM, respectively. (G) Typical GCaMP6f traces (5 min) of baseline (upper) and 40 mins after 1 mg/kg CNO injection (below) from the DG and SuM. (H) The peak ∆F/F of the DG calcium signal increased after CNO injection. (n = 5 mice, paired t-test, t = 6.126, *p < 0.05). (I) The peak of ∆F/F of the SuM calcium signal increased after CNO injection. (n = 5 mice, paired t-test, t = 9.037, *p < 0.05). (J) Diagram of the NPR and NOR tests. CNO (1 mg/kg) was administrated 1 hr before these tests. (K) Activation or inhibition of SuMVgat neurons increased or decreased the discrimination ratio in the NPR test, respectively. (Unpaired t-test, t21 = 2.244, *p = 0.0358 in hM3Dq-mice, t18 = 2.238, *p = 0.0381 in hM4Di-mice). (L) Activation or inhibition of SuMVgat neurons did not change the discrimination ratio in the NOR test. (Unpaired t-test, t21 = 1.869, p > 0.05 in hM3Dq-mice, t18 = 0.9197, p > 0.05 in hM4Di-mice).

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Cell-type-specific anterograde and retrograde tracing for SuMVgat-DG connections.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Diagram of rabies-based monosynaptic synaptic tracing of DG GCs. (B) Experimental timeline of retrograde tracing. On day 1, AAV-CaMKII-Cre, Cre-dependent TVA-GFP and RG virus were injected to the left DG. On day 14, Rabies-mCherry was injected at the same site and mice were perfused on day 21. (C) Fluorescence images showing starter cells with mCherry expressions in the DG. Scale bar = 100 µm. (D) Amplified starter cells in the DG. Zoomed-in view of injection site showing that starter neurons (yellow, arrows indicated) were infected with AAV helper virus (green) and RV (red). Scale bar = 20 µm. (E) Representative coronal section showing retrogradely RV-labeled cell bodies (red) were in the SuM, with dense mCherry+ cells in the lateral SuM (SuML) and sparse mCherry+ cells in the medial SuM (SuMM). Scale bar = 100 µm. (F) RV-labeled SuML neurons were positive for GABA staining (white arrows indicated). Scale bar = 20 µm. (G) Percentage of retrograde labeling mCherry+ cells co-localized with GABA staining in the SuML. (H) Diagram of anterograde tracing. AAV5-DIO-eYFP was delivered to the SuML in Vgat-Cre mice. (I–J) Fluorescence images showed SuML projections located in the DG and CA2. Scale bar = 200 µm.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Locomotor activity after activity manipulation of SuMVgat neurons.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

Locomotor activity in the open field (5 mins, same for both NPR and NOR tests) was recorded 1 hr after CNO administration in mice injected with AAVs expressing mCherry-, hM3Dq- or hM4Di. (A-B) Typical locomotor traces (A) of mCherry- or hM3Dq- mice after CNO injection, and locomotor activity was not changed (B), compared to mCherry control mice (unpaired t-test, t16 = 0.1325, p=0.8963). (C-D) Typical locomotor traces (C) of mCherry- or hM4Di- mice after CNO injection, showed locomotor activity was not changed (D), compared to mCherry control mice (unpaired t-test, t16 = 0.2374, p = 0.8177). (E, G) Heatmap of representative locomotor traces in the NPR test during chemogenetic activation of SuM neurons (E) or optogenetic activation of SuM-DG projections (G), respectively. (F, H) The average running speed during the NPR test during chemogenetic activation of SuM neurons (F) or optogenetic activation of SuM-DG projections (H), respectively. p > 0.05 by unpaired t-test.
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Stimulation of SuMM neurons did not significantly increase spatial memory.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

(A) Schematic diagram of the viral delivery. AAV5-CaMKII-mCherry (150 nL) was injected to the SuMM. (B) The representative image showing mCherry expression preferentially in the SuMM. (C) Chemogenetic activation of SuMM neurons did not significantly alter the discrimination ratio in the NPR test.

To address the functional role of SuML neurons in regulating spatial memory, we bi-directionally manipulate the activity of the SuMLVgat neurons using chemogenetic approaches. Specifically, we administered CNO by intraperitoneal injection one hour before the test phase of the NPR task (Figure 2J). Interestingly, we found that activation or inhibition of SuMLVgat neurons increased or decreased the discrimination ratio in the hippocampus-dependent NPR test, respectively (Figure 2K). In contrast, activation or inhibition of SuMLVgat neurons did not affect the discrimination ratio in the novel-object recognition (NOR) test, which is a non-spatial memory test (Cohen et al., 2013; Oliveira et al., 2010; Sawangjit et al., 2018; Winters et al., 2004; Winters et al., 2008; Figure 2L). Importantly, CNO administration did not alter the locomotor activity of the mice in the open field test (Figure 2—figure supplement 2A-D) or average running speed of the mice during the NPR test (Figure 2—figure supplement 2E-F). These results suggest that the activity of SuMLVgat neurons is required for regulating spatial memory retrieval.

To further address whether SuMM neurons impact spatial memory retrieval, we attempted to selectively target SuMM neurons by injecting a reduced volume of AAV5-CaMKII-hM3Dq-mCherry (150 nL) to the SuMM (Figure 2—figure supplement 3A-B). As a result, we found that chemogenetic activation of SuMM neurons does not significantly increase the discrimination ratio in the NPR test (Figure 2—figure supplement 3C). These results indicate that SuML neurons, but not SuMM neurons, regulate spatial memory retrieval during the NPR test.

SuM-DG circuit activity is required for spatial memory retrieval (but not encoding)

Next, we sought to address whether SuML-DG circuit activity is required for regulating spatial memory retrieval. We took an optogenetic approach to precisely manipulate the activity of SuMLVgat-DG projections during spatial memory encoding or retrieval. First, we sought to address whether the SuML-DG circuit activity is sufficient to regulate spatial memory retrieval by delivering AAV-DIO-ChR2-mCherry to the SuML of Vgat-Cre mice to activate SuMLVgat-DG projections (Figure 3A). Our slice electrophysiology confirmed the efficacy of ChR2 expression in the SuMLVgat-DG projections for inducing monosynaptic responses in dentate GCs (Figure 3B–G). Importantly, optogenetic activation of the SuMLVgat-DG projections induced both glutamatergic excitatory postsynaptic currents (oEPSCs, holding at −60 mV) and GABAergic inhibitory postsynaptic currents (oIPSCs, holding at +5 mV) in 70.6% of recorded cells (Figure 3C–D). These data support the co-release of GABA and glutamate from SuMLVgat neurons to DG GCs. Interestingly, we found that the mean amplitude of oEPSCs is significantly higher than oIPSCs (Figure 3E–F). In addition, 17.8% of recorded cells exhibited solo oEPSCs, and no recorded cells exhibited solo oIPSCs (Figure 3G). These data support a dominant effect of glutamatergic transmission on GCs. Supporting this notion, we found a significant increase in the density of cFos+ GCs upon optogenetic activation of SuMLVgat-DG projections in vivo (Figure 3H–J). Furthermore, optogenetic activation of SuMLVgat-DG projections specifically during spatial memory encoding had no significant effects on the discrimination ratio of the mice in the NPR test (Figure 3K). In contrast, optogenetic activation of SuMLVgat-DG projections specifically during spatial memory retrieval significantly increased the discrimination ratio of the mice in the NPR test (Figure 3L). As a control parameter, we measured the running speed of the mice during optogenetic stimulation of SuMLVgat-DG projections, and did not find significant alterations (Figure 2—figure supplement 1G–H), suggesting that the general activity of the mice does not contribute to the increased discrimination ratio observed in the NPR test. These results suggest that optogenetic activation of SuMLVgat-DG projections during spatial memory retrieval is sufficient to improve memory performance during the NPR test.

Figure 3. Activity of SuMVgat-DG projections is required for spatial memory retrieval.

(A) Schematic diagram of the experimental design. Cre-dependent ChR2-mCherry was injected in the SuM of Vgat-Cre mice. Terminals of the infected SuM GABAergic neurons were photo-stimulated, and responses were recorded in DG GCs. (B) Representative image of a connected biocytin-filled DG GC (green) showing the typical morphology of GCs that was located in the dense ChR2-mCherry positive fibers (red). Scale bar: 50 μm. ML: molecular layer. GCL: granule cell layer. (C–D) Representative traces showing light-evoked EPSCs (C, Vh = −60 mV, near the reversal potential of IPSCs to isolate EPSCs) and light-evoked IPSCs (D, Vh = +5 mV, near the reversal potential of EPSCs to isolate IPSCs) from a DG GC. When clamping neurons at −60 mV, the inward current was predominantly glutamatergic (blocked by APV+NBQX). In contrast, when holding at +5 mV, the outward current was primarily GABAergic (blocked by BIC). (E–F) Quantification of the latency (E) and amplitude (F) of light-evoked EPSCs and IPSCs recorded from GCs. The mean latency was less than 5 ms in EPSCs and IPSCs indicating that SuM neurons directly connected to GCs. (G) Bar chart showing connectivity rate of GCs for optogenetic stimulation of the SuMVgat-DG projections. (H) Experimental scheme of in vivo optogenetics. (I) Sample images of c-Fos expression in the DG after1 h opto-activation of SuMVgat-DG projections in Vgat-Cre mice. (J) Quantification of c-Fos in DG granule cell layers. Photo-stimulation of the SuMVgat-DG projections increased c-Fos expression in GCs. (Unpaired t-test, t10 = 2.440, *p = 0.045). (K) Opto-activation (10 Hz/5 ms duration, 473 nm blue light for 5 min) of the SuMVgat-DG projection during the encoding phase did not affect the discrimination ratio in the NPR test. (Unpaired t-test, t17 = 0.2867, p = 0.7778). (L) Opto-activation of the SuMVgat-DG projections during retrieval phase increased the discrimination ratio in the NPR test (Unpaired t-test, t17 = 4.974, **p = 0.0001). (M) Opto-inhibition (561 nm green light was continuously given for 5 min) of the SuMVgat-DG projections during the encoding phase did not affect the discrimination ratio in the NPR test. (Unpaired t-test, t14 = 0.2829, p = 0.7851). (N) Opto-inhibition of the SuMVgat-DG projections during retrieval phase decreased the discrimination ratio in the NPR test (Unpaired t-test, t14 = 3.657, **p = 0.0033).

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Functional expression of Arch in SuMVgat neurons.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Left, schematic diagram of optogenetic inhibition of SuMVgat neurons in the acute brain slices; Right, Live differential interference contrast (DIC) and the fluorescence image from whole-cell electrophysiology recording showing an Arch positive cell with the microelectrode above the membrane surface in the SuM. Scale bar, 5 µm. (B) Representative images showing Arch-EYFP projections in the DG (left) and SuM (right) of a Vgat-Cre mouse. Note that dashed lines showed the fiber track above the DG used for in vivo optogenetic inhibition. (C) Sample trace under voltage-clamp mode showing that a 5 s green light application induced a robust outward current in a SuM EYFP+ cell. (D) Quantification of the magnitude of outward currents in Arch-expressing SuM Vgat+ neurons following optogenetic inhibition (n = 8 cells from two mice). (E) Sample trace under the current-clamp mode showing that a 5 s green light application induced a hyperpolarization in a SuM EYFP+ cell. (F) Quantification of the magnitude of hyperpolarization in Arch-expressing SuM Vgat+ neurons upon optogenetic inhibition (n = 6 cells from two mice). Data represent mean ± SEM.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Optogenetic activation of SuMVgat-CA2 projections did not change the discrimination ratio in the NPR test.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(A) Diagram of optogenetic activation of SuMVgat-CA2 projections. AAV5-DIO-ChR2-mCherry was injected into the SuML and bilateral fibers were implanted above the CA2 in Vgat-Cre mice. (B) Coronal sections showing the optic fiber track and mCherry terminals in the DG and CA2. Scale bar = 100 µm. (C) Optogenetic activation of SuMVgat-CA2 projections did not alter the discrimination ratio in the NPR test. (n = 8 mice in each group, p>0.05 by unpaired t-test).

To address whether the activity of the SuML-DG circuit is necessary to regulate spatial memory retrieval, we delivered AAV-DIO-Arch to the SuML of Vgat-Cre mice to optogenetically inhibit the activity of SuMLVgat-DG projections (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). As a result, we found that optogenetic inhibition of SuMLVgat-DG projections during memory retrieval significantly decreased the discrimination ratio in the NPR test (Figure 3N). In contrast, optogenetic inhibition of SuMLVgat-DG projections during memory encoding did not alter the discrimination ratio (Figure 3M–N). Together, these results suggest that the activity of the SuML-DG circuit during the spatial memory retrieval (but not encoding) phase is both sufficient and necessary for regulating spatial memory performance.

It is worth noting that our anterograde tracing also revealed that SuML projections send collaterals to other hippocampal structures, such as CA2 (Figure 2—figure supplement 1J). To address whether the SuML-CA2 pathway is involved in spatial memory retrieval during the NPR test, we injected AAV-DIO-ChR2 to the SuML in Vgat-Cre mice and bilaterally implanted optic fibers above CA2. As a result, we found that optogenetic activation of SuMLVgat-CA2 projections did not significantly alter the discrimination ratio in the NPR test (Figure 3—figure supplement 2), indicating that SuMLVgat-CA2 projections are not sufficient to regulate spatial memory retrieval during the NPR test.

SuM glutamate transmission is necessary for spatial memory retrieval

Recent studies showed that SuML neurons co-release GABA and glutamate (Hashimotodani et al., 2018; Root et al., 2018), but the relative role of GABA versus glutamate in regulating the spatial memory process has yet to be determined. To probe the relative role of GABA or glutamate release from SuML neurons in regulating spatial memory retrieval, we delivered AAVs expressing validated short-hairpin RNA (shRNA) against Vglut2 (shVglut2 mice) (Valencia Garcia et al., 2017) or Vgat (shVgat mice) (Garcia et al., 2018) into the SuML to reduce glutamate or GABA release from SuML neurons, respectively (Figure 4A). We validated the efficiency of Vglut2 or Vgat knockdown in SuML-DG terminals by both immunohistology and slice electrophysiology (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Specifically, our immunohistology using antibodies against Vglut2 or Vgat demonstrated reduced Vglut2+ or Vgat+ SuML-DG terminals in shVglut2 or shVgat mice, respectively, as compared to those in the control mice injected with control AAVs for shVglut2 or shVgat (shControl mice) (Figure 4B,D, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Our slice recordings of the GCs in shVglut2 injected mice showed a significant decrease only in the mean amplitude of oEPSCs, but not oIPSC, respectively (Figure 4F–G). The latency of oIPSCs or oEPSCs in shVgat and shVglut2 mice was not significantly different from the shControl mice (Figure 4H–I), suggesting that knocking down glutamate component does not alter the nature of monosynaptic connections between SuMLVgat neurons and DG GCs. Strikingly, knocking down the glutamate component in SuML neurons led to 38% of GCs exhibiting solo oIPSCs in shVglut2 mice, as compared to the control mice with no GCs having solo oIPSCs (Figure 4J), suggesting that reducing glutamate release from SuML neurons may exert more inhibition on DG GCs. Supporting this, we found a decreased density of c-Fos+ GCs in shVglut2 mice after optogenetic activation of the SuML-DG projections (Figure 4K–L). In contrast, knocking down the GABA component in SuML neurons led to a significant reduction in the mean amplitude of oIPSCs upon optogenetic stimulation of SuMLVgat-DG projections (Figure 4G), but did not alter the distribution of GCs exhibiting solo oEPSCs or dual oEPSCs and oIPSCs (Figure 4J). The density of c-Fos+ GCs in shVgat mice after optogenetic activation of the SuML-DG projections did not significantly change as compared to shControl mice (Figure 4K–L). Together, these results validated the knockdown efficiency of the shVglut2 and shVgat expression in the SuML-DG circuit using GC activity as a readout. Based on these results, it appeared that knocking down the glutamate component in SuML has a more profound effect on GC activity than knocking down the GABA component.

Figure 4. SuM glutamate transmission modulates dentate granule cell activity.

(A) Schematic diagram of the experimental design. Cre-dependent AAV-ChR2-mCherry mixed with AAV-shVgat or AAV-shVglut2 was injected in the SuM of Vgat-Cre mice. Terminals of the infected SuM GABAergic neurons were photo-stimulated, and responses were recorded in DG GCs. (B–E) Immunohistology and electrophysiology data showing the efficiency of genetic Vglut2 or Vgat knockdown. With DIO-ChR2-mCherry/CMV-shVglut2 (B) or DIO-ChR2-mCherry/DIO-shVgat (D) expressed in SuMVgat neurons, Vgat (B) or Vglut2 (D) expression was virtually absent in DG mCherry+ terminals emanating from SuMVgat neurons. Scale bars = 10 µm. Representative traces of optical-evoked EPSCs (oEPSC) at a holding potential of −60 mV (C, blue traces), and optical-evoked IPSCs (oIPSC) at a holding potential of +5 mV (E, green traces) recorded from a ChR2+shVglut2 and a ChR2+shVgat mouse, respectively. (F–G) Amplitude of oEPSC (F) and oIPSC (G) in GCs in control, shVgat, and shVglut2 mice. The mean amplitude of oEPSC and oIPSC was decreased in ChR2+shVglut2 and ChR2+shVgat mice, respectively. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. One-way ANOVA followed by PLSD post hoc test. (H–I) Latency of oEPSC (H) and oIPSC (I) in GCs in control, shVgat, and shVglut2 mice. Knocking down Vglut2 or Vgat did not affect latencies of oEPSC and oIPSC in GCs. (J) Pie charts showing the proportion of GCs exhibiting both oEPSC and oIPSC, oEPSC only, or oIPSC only in control, shVgat, and shVglut2 mice. (K–L) C-Fos expression in the DG following light stimulation from ChR2, ChR2+shVglut2 and ChR2+shVgat mice. **p < 0.01. One-way ANOVA followed by PLSD post hoc test.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Genetic knockdown of Vgat and Vglut2 in SuMVgat neurons.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Immunohistochemical staining of Vgat or Vglut2 in DG synaptic terminals emanating from SuMVgat neurons. After shVgat and shVglut2 expression in the SuM, Vgat and Vglut2 expression from SuMVgat-DG projections was virtually absent in DG mCherry+ terminals (white arrow heads). Scale bar = 20 µm.

Using these validated AAV shRNA constructs, we first measured the discrimination ratio of the behaving mice in the NPR and NOR tests without optogenetic stimulation of SuMLVgat-DG projections (Figure 5A). We found that shVglut2 (but not shVgat) mice, exhibited a significant decrease in the discrimination ratio in the NPR test (Figure 5B–C). In contrast, we found no significant change in the discrimination ratio in the NOR test in both shVglut2 and shVgat mice (Figure 5D-E). These results suggest that glutamate transmission from SuML neurons is required for hippocampal-dependent spatial memory retrieval. Next, we examined the effect of optogenetic stimulation of SuMLVgat-DG projections on the discrimination ratio of the behaving mice during the NPR test (Figure 5F). Similarly, we found that shVglut2 (but not shVgat) mice exhibit a significant reduction of the discrimination ratio (Figure 5G–H). These results support a critical role for glutamate (but not GABA) transmission from SuML neurons in regulating spatial memory retrieval.

Figure 5. SuM glutamate transmission is necessary for spatial memory retrieval.

Figure 5.

(A) Schematic diagram of genetic knockdown of Vgat or Vglut2 in SuM neurons. (B–C) Knocking down Vglut2 (B) but not Vgat (C) decreased discrimination ratio in the NPR test. (B, unpaired t-test, t14 = 3.662, **p = 0.0028; C, unpaired t-test, t14 = 0.000, p > 0.05). (D–E) Knocking down Vgat (D) or Vglut2 (E) did not alter the discrimination ratio in the NOR test, as compared to the control mice (D, n = 8 mice, unpaired t-test, t14 = 0.3378, p > 0.05; E, n = 8 mice, unpaired t-test, t14 = 0.066, p > 0.05). (F) Diagram of the experimental paradigm with the combination of in vivo optogenetic activation of SuM-DG projections and genetic knockdown of Vgat or Vglut2. (G–H) Knocking down Vglut2 (G), but not Vgat (H) impaired spatial memory retrieval upon optogenetic activation of SuM-DG projections (G, unpaired t-test, t20 = 3.888, **p = 0.0009; H, unpaired t-test, t16 = 1.935, p = 0.8327), as compared to the control mice injected with AAVs expressing shControl.

SuM glutamate transmission is necessary for the high correlation of SuM and DG Ca2+ activities during spatial memory retrieval

To further investigate the role of SuML glutamate transmission in regulating the Ca2+ activities of the SuM and DG and the correlation between them, we performed in vivo fiber photometry recordings in shVgut2 mice in their home cage and during the NPR test (Figure 6A–B). Interestingly, knocking down shVglut2 did not alter the DG and SuML activity in their home cage (Figure 6C). However, shVglut2 mice exhibited a significant reduction in the correlation of the Ca2+ activities between SuM and DG as compared to the control mice (Figure 6D–F). Furthermore, shVglut2 mice exhibited significantly reduced DG Ca2+ activity during spatial memory retrieval in the NPR test (Figure 6G–I). In contrast, no significant alteration in the SuM Ca2+ activity was observed in shVglut2 mice during spatial memory retrieval (Figure 6I). These data suggest that SuML glutamate transmission is necessary for regulating the activity level of the DG during spatial memory retrieval. Moreover, we measured the correlation of SuM and DG activities during spatial memory retrieval during the NPR test and found a significant reduction in the correlation of Ca2+ activities between the SuM and DG in shVglut2 mice (Figure 6J). Together, these results indicate that SuML glutamate release is necessary for regulating the DG activity and establishing the high correlation of the activities between SuM and DG during spatial memory retrieval.

Figure 6. SuM glutamate transmission is necessary for regulating DG activity and establishing high SuM-DG synchrony during spatial memory retrieval.

Figure 6.

(A) Experimental diagram of the DG and SuM Ca2+ recording under expressing shVglut2 in the SuM. (B) Coronal sections showing fiber photometry traces and CaMKII-GCaMP/tdTomato expressed neurons in the DG (left) and SuML (right). Scale bar = 100 µm. (C) Ca2+ activity of the DG and SuM at home cage was not changed in shVglut2 mice. (D–E) Sample of GCaMP6f traces of the DG and SuM at home cage in control (D) and shVglut2 mice (E). (F) Correlation of the SuM and DG Ca2+ activity at home cage. n = 8–11 mice, p = 0.05 by unpaired t-test after Fisher transform. (G) Scaled color plot of the DG and SuM Ca2+ activity from control and shVglut2 mice in the NPR test period, respectively. (H) Averaged DG and SuM Ca2+ activities in the NPR test period. Semi-transparent borders indicate ± SEM. During the exploring period, Ca2+ activities of both the DG and SuM were increased in control mice, but only the SuM Ca2+ activity was increased in shVglut2 mice. (I) Cumulative activities of the DG and SuM in the NPR test period. The Ca2+ activity in the DG, but not the SuM, was decreased in shVglut2 mice. Unpaired t-test, **p < 0.01. (J) Correlation of the SuM and DG Ca2+ activity in the NPR test period. R = 0.7391, p < 0.0001 in control mice; R = 0.2713, p = 0.2341 in shVglut2 mice.

Discussion

Our studies bridge several gaps in the current understanding of the role of the SuM in regulating spatial memory. First, the SuM sends dense projections to the DG, but how the SuM and DG coordinate their activities during distinct phases of the spatial memory process remains unknown. Using an in vivo multi-fiber photometry system to simultaneously record the Ca2+ activities of both the SuM and DG, we provide the first evidence that activities of the SuM and DG become significantly higher and correlated during spatial memory retrieval than those during the encoding phase (Figure 7A). Such high-level interregional synchronization was not observed during spatial memory encoding, suggesting that interregional synchrony is highly dependent on distinct behavioral states. Importantly, selective knockdown of SuM glutamate transmission disrupts the correlation of the Ca2+ activities between the SuM and DG during spatial memory retrieval (Figure 7B). Together, these findings suggest that synchronized SuML-DG activity maybe essential for spatial memory retrieval. Second, previous studies implicated a role for the SuM in spatial learning and memory (Hashimotodani et al., 2018; Ito et al., 2018; Root et al., 2018), but the precise neural circuits that regulate such hippocampal-dependent spatial behavior remains unknown. Using circuit-based chemogenetic and optogenetic approaches to specifically manipulate the activity of SuML neurons and SuML-DG projections, we identified that the activity of both SuML neurons and SuMLVgat-DG projections are required for spatial memory retrieval (Figure 7C). Third, it has been well established that SuML neurons co-release GABA and glutamate to the DG, but the relative role of GABA versus glutamate transmission from SuML neurons in regulating spatial memory process remains to be determined. Using a genetic approach to selectively knockdown GABA or glutamate release from SuML neurons, we identified that SuML glutamate (but not GABA) release from SuML neurons plays a critical role in regulating spatial memory retrieval (Figure 7D).

Figure 7. Summary model of a long-range SuM-DG circuit linking two highly correlated subcortical regions to regulate spatial memory retrieval through SuM glutamate tranmission.

Figure 7.

(A) Correlation of the SuM and DG Ca2+ activity is increased during spatial memory retrieval. (B) SuM glutamate transmission is necessary for the high correlation of the SuM and DG activities. (C) The activity of SuM neurons or SuM-DG projections is required for regulating spatial memory retrieval. (D) SuM glutamate transmission is necessary for spatial memory retrieval.

Our study revealed significantly higher correlation levels between the SuML and DG during spatial memory retrieval than during memory encoding. Moreover, the activities from both the SuML and DG increased significantly more during memory retrieval than those during memory encoding, as compared to the baseline activity. These results suggest that the combination of high SuML-DG synchrony and high activity in these brain regions maybe required to promote memory retrieval (Frankland et al., 2019). In contrast to the significant increase in activity and correlation between the SuML and DG during spatial memory retrieval, the activity and correlation between the SuML and DG only slightly increased during spatial memory encoding. These results suggest that SuML and DG activity and correlation may play a relatively minor role in regulating memory encoding. Supporting this, our optogenetic experiments showed that the activity of the SuMLVgat-DG pathway during spatial memory retrieval (but not encoding) is both sufficient and necessary in regulating spatial memory performance during the NPR test. We speculate that other major inputs to DG maybe more heavily involved in regulating memory encoding. Indeed, the cholinergic inputs from the medial septum and diagonal band have been shown to be critical for spatial memory encoding (Barry et al., 2012; Hasselmo, 2006).

In our study, we observed that the Ca2+ events from the SuML and DG neurons are highly correlated in the range of 0.1–0.5 Hz (Figure 4—figure supplement 1H). It remains to be determined whether the slow 0.1–0.5 Hz Ca2+ activity is associated with the hippocampal theta rhythm known to be critical for spatial learning and memory. This is mainly due to the technical limitations associated with currently available Ca2+ indicators that do not fully recapitulate the spiking patterns of the neurons (Kwan, 2008; Looger et al., 2003). Ongoing efforts towards developing fast Ca2+ indicators that can recapitulate the actual firing pattern of neurons will allow the field to address the correlation of the power spectrum between Ca2+ and electrical signals.

It was recently shown that optogenetic activation of SuML-DG projections excites dentate interneurons (IN), and both monosynaptic and polysynaptic GABAergic inputs have been identified in dentate GCs (Hashimotodani et al., 2018). Despite that GCs receive both GABAergic and glutamatergic inputs, we found that stimulation of SuML neurons or SuMLVgat-DG projections promotes memory retrieval. Furthermore, we demonstrated that inhibiting the activity of SuML neurons and SuML-DG projections or reducing glutamate release from SuML neurons leads to impaired spatial memory retrieval. These results suggest that monosynaptic SuML glutamatergic inputs to GCs play a dominant role in regulating memory retrieval processes. It is worth mentioning that our results are based on the NPR test during which encoding and retrieval phases are relatively short (5 min at each phase). It is possible that the monosynaptic SuML GABAergic inputs or polysynaptic SuML-IN mediated GABAergic inputs play a role in regulating long-term memory or other episodic memory processes, such as contextual memory.

Together, our findings provide novel information on a novel long-range circuit involving two highly synchronized subcortical brain regions in regulating distinct phases of the spatial memory process. Accumulating evidence has suggested that long-range anatomical connections enable multiregional interactions, and such interactions dynamically cope with changing behavioral demands (Ito et al., 2018). Neuronal correlation/synchrony has been thought to be critical for behavior-dependent functional coupling of neural circuits through spike-time coordination (Ito et al., 2018). Supporting this view, Ito et al. recorded the local field potentials of SuM and CA1 and found a significant increase of SuM-CA1 coherence in the theta frequency band during the T-maze test, suggesting that the correlation of SuM and CA1 theta rhythm is increased during spatial memory (Ito et al., 2018). Aligning with this idea, our data suggest that highly synchronized SuM-DG activity is critical for spatial memory retrieval. A recent study showed that SuM inputs to DG GCs are weak, therefore, they are not sufficient to drive action potentials in GCs by themselves; instead, they can facilitate GC firing elicited by stimulating the perforant pathway (PP) projected from EC (Hashimotodani et al., 2018). Therefore, the SuM can potentially modulate the cortical inputs to the DG and influence DG information processing during the spatial memory process. The high SuM-DG synchrony may play a critical role in ensuring the spike-timing precision of the modulatory action of the SuM on PP-GC synapses during the spatial memory process. Ample studies have shown that aberrant neuronal synchronization is associated with Alzheimer’s disease, which may in part contribute to the cognitive deficits seen in this disease (Palop and Mucke, 2016). It has been well established that EC and hippocampus are impaired during early AD (Harris et al., 2010; Hyman et al., 1986), therefore, our findings may guide novel therapeutic strategies to treat AD-associated memory deficits by targeting SuM inputs to enhance SuM-DG synchrony and modulate EC inputs onto GCs.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type
(species) or resource
Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional
information
Strain, strain background (Mus musculus, C57BL6/J) Slc32a1tm2(cre)Lowl/J, (Bl6) Jackson laboratory JAX: 016962 All sexes used
Strain, strain background (Mus musculus, C57BL6/J) Mouse: C57BL/6J Jackson laboratory RRID: IMSR_JAX:000664 All sexes used
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) AAV5-EF1α-FLEX-TVA-mCherry Massachusetts Institute of Technology Dr. Ian R. Wickersham
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) AAV-CA-FLEX-RG Massachusetts Institute of Technology Dr. Ian R. Wickersham
Strain, strain background (rabies virus) RABV-SAD∆G-mCherry Massachusetts Institute of Technology Dr. Ian R. Wickersham
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) AAV5-EF1α-DIO-eYFP UNC Vector Core
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) AAV5-EF1α-DIO-mCherry UNC Vector Core
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) AAV5-EF1a-DIO-hChR2(H134R)-mCherry UNC Vector Core
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) AAV5-EF1a-DIO-Arch 3.0-eYFP UNC Vector Core
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) AAV5-hSyn-DIO-hM3Dq-mCherry addgene #50474-AAV5
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) AAV5-hSyn-DIO-hM4Di-mCherry addgene #50475-AAV5
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) AAV5-CaMKII-GCaMP6f UNC Vector Core
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) AAV2-CaMKII-tdTomato UNC Vector Core
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) AAV5-CaMKII-hM3Dq-mCherry UNC Vector Core
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) AAV8-hsyn-DIO-shVgat-mCherry University of Tsukuba Dr. Michael Lazarus
Strain, strain background (adeno-associated virus) CMV-shVglut2-mCherry University of Tsukuba Dr. Michael Lazarus
Antibody Donkey polyclonal
Anti- c-Fos
Millipore Cat# 3168266; RRID: ABE457 1:1 k
Antibody Rabbit polyclonal Anti- GABA Sigma Cat# A2052-100UL 1:1 k
Antibody Goat polyclonal Anti- GPF Rockland Cat# 600-101-215; RRID: AB_218182 1:1 k
Antibody Rabbit polyclonal Anti-Vgat Synaptic Systems Cat#131002, RR:131003/32 1:1 k
Antibody Guinea pig Polyclonal Anti-Vglut2 Millipore Cat# 3101508; RRID: AB2251-I 1:1 k
Chemical compound, drug Clozapine-N-oxide (CNO) Sigma Cat# C0832-5MG
Software, algorithm ImageJ (Fiji) http://fiji.sc/
Software, algorithm FV3000 Olympus
Software, algorithm Ocean view Ocean Optics
Software, algorithm GraphPad Prism GraphPad Software Prism 8
Software, algorithm Matlab MathWorks R2014b
Software, algorithm EthoVision XT Noldus

Animals

C57BL/6J wild-type mice and Vgat-Cre (backcrossed with BL6, Slc32a1tm2(cre)Lowl/J) mice were obtained from the Jackson laboratory. Male and female mice at 8–12 weeks of age were used for experiments. Mice were group housed of 3–5 mice in each cage and had access to food and water ad libitum and were maintained at a constant temperature of 22–24°C, humidity of 40–60%, a 12 hr light/dark cycle (100 Lux, light on at 07:00) and under veterinary supervision. Animals subjected to surgical procedures were moved to a satellite housing facility for recovery with the same light-dark cycle. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and with the approval of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

Stereotaxic surgery

C57BL/6J wild-type and Vgat-Cre mice were anesthetized under 1.5% isoflurane in oxygen at 0.8 LPM flow rate. Virus was injected by microsyringe (Hamilton, 33GA) and microinjection pump (Harvard Apparatus), at a rate of 30–50 nl/min with the following coordinates: virus were injected bilaterally (optogenetics, chemogenetics, slice recordings, and knockdowns) or unilaterally (tracing and photometry) into the SuML (anteroposterior (AP)_−2.4 mm, mediolateral (ML)_±0.6 mm, dorsoventral (DV) _−4.85 mm), SuML (anteroposterior (AP)_−2.4 mm, mediolateral (ML)_±0.0 mm, dorsoventral (DV) _−4.7 mm) or DG (AP_ −2.0 mm, ML_±1.4 mm, DV_−2.0 mm). A total of 150–250 nl of virus was delivered to each site, and the needle was left in the site for at least 10 min to permit diffusion. All coordinates were based on values from 'The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates' (Paxinos and Franklin, 2019).

For RV based retrograde tracing, mice were injected with 200 nl AAV5-EF1α-FLEX-TVA-mCherry and AAV-CA-FLEX-RG (provided by Dr. Ian Wickersham) at a ratio of 1:1 into the left DG at AP_−2.0 mm, ML_+1.4 mm, DV_−2.0 mm. After 2 weeks, the same animals had the second injection of 250 nl pseudo-typed RV RABV-SAD∆G-mCherry (provided by Dr. Ian Wickersham) using the same coordinates. Animals were then transferred to a quarantined cubicle for special housing and monitoring. Seven days post rabies injection, animals were perfused and brain tissues were collected.

For anterograde tracing, mice were injected with 150 nl of AAV5-EF1α-DIO-eYFP (UNC Vector Core) into the SuML and were perfused and brain tissues were collected at 2 weeks after virus injection.

For in vivo optogenetics, mice were bilaterally injected with 250 nl of AAV5-EF1α-DIO-hChR2 (H134R)-mCherry, AAV5- EF1α DIO-Arch3.0-eYFP or AAV5-EF1α-DIO-mCherry/AAV5-EF1α-DIO-YFP (UNC Vector Core) with or without 250 nl of AAV8-DIO-shVgat-mCherry (Garcia et al., 2018)/CMV-shVglut2-mCherry (Valencia Garcia et al., 2017) (From Dr. Michael Lazarus, University of Tsukuba) into the SuM and bilaterally implanted with optical fibers (Newdoon Inc, O.D.: 1.25 mm, core: 200 µm, NA: 0.37) above the DG at AP_−2.0 mm, ML_±1.4 mm, DV_−1.7 mm. After 2–3 weeks of recovery, mice were used for in vivo behavior testes.

For chemogenetic manipulation, mice were bilaterally injected with 250 nl of AAV5-EF1α-DIO-hM3Dq-mCherry, AAV5-hSyn-DIO-hM4Di-mCherry (add gene) or AAV5-EF1α-DIO-mCherry into the SuML (coordinates AP_−2.4 mm, ML_+0.6 mm, DV_−4.85 mm), respectively.

For in vivo photometry recordings, mice were unilaterally injected with 250 nl of AAV5-CaMKII-GCaMP6f mixed with 50 nl AAV2-CaMKII-tdTomato into the DG at these coordinates AP_−2.0 mm, ML_+1.4 mm, DV_−1.9 mm and 250 nl of AAV5-CaMKII-GCaMP6f, 250 nl of AAV2-CaMKII-hM3Dq-mCherry, mixed with 50 nl of AAV2-CaMKII-tdTomato to the SuM at AP_−2.4 mm, ML_+0.6 mm, DV_−4.8 mm. Optical fibers (Newdoon Inc, O.D.: 1.25 mm, core: 200 µm, NA: 0.37) were implanted above the DG (AP_−2.0 mm, ML_+1.4 mm, DV_−1.8 mm) and above the SuM at (AP_−2.4 mm, ML_+0.6 mm, DV_−4.7 mm), respectively.

For slice recording, AAV5-EF1α-DIO-hChR2 (H134R)-mCherry, AAV8-DIO-shVgat-mCherry and/or CMV-shVglut2-mCherry were bilaterally injected to the SuM of Vgat-Cre mice for different experiments.

Behavioral tests

Habituation and memory tasks (Leger et al., 2013; Meng et al., 2018; Sawangjit et al., 2018). Mice were handled daily for five consecutive days for 5 min. From day 1 to day 3, mice were placed into an empty open field environment (45 cm × 45 cm × 45 cm constructed of grey PVC) for 5 min. After the habituation phase, the encoding phase of the memory task started on day 4. The encoding phases were identical for the novel position recognition (NPR) task and the novel object recognition (NOR) task and comprised a 5 min interval during which the mice were allowed to explore two identical objects in the open field. After the encoding phase, mice spent 6 hr to allow memory consolidation. To test retrieval in the NOR task, one of the two objects from the encoding phase was replaced by a novel object. To test retrieval in the NPR task, one of the two objects from the encoding phase was moved to a different location. At each test, mice had 5 min to explore the arena. Noise-generator provided a masking 40 dB background noise. Through the open upper side of the arena, the mouse could perceive distal cues. Objects for exploration were were glass cylinders or cube shaped wood (height: 4 cm; base diameter: 1.5 cm), and were stuck to the arena floor to prevent the mouse from moving them. Objects were positioned at least 5 cm equidistant from the walls and at least 25 cm between each other to ensure that corner perferences did not bias exploration times. The mice showed no preference for objects in the encoding phase and the locations of objects were randomized across mice during the encoding and retrieval phases.

For DREADD experiments, CNO at 1 mg/kg was i.p. injected 1 hr before retrieval tests. For optogenetic activation experiments, mice were connected to the optic cable for five mins habituation sessions for 3 days. A 10 Hz, 5 ms duration, 473 nm blue light stimulation paradigm was given for 5 min during the familiarization phase (spatial memory encoding) or test phases (spatial memory retrieval). For optogenetic inhibition experiments, 561 nm green light was continuously given for 5 min. All mice were only used once in each test. Laser power was adjusted to a final optical fiber output of 7 ~ 10 mW. Bilateral patch cables were connected to a rotary commutator (Doric) to avoid twisting. After each phase, the apparatus and objects were cleaned with water containing 70% ethanol to remove any scent from the previous animal.

Analysis of memory performance (Sawangjit et al., 2018). Exploration behavior was monitored by a video camera and manually analyzed by an experienced researcher blinded to mouse groups. Object exploration was considered whenever the mouse sniffed the object or touched the object while looking at it (when the distance between the nose and the object was less than 1 cm). Climbing onto the object (unless the mouse sniffed the object it had climbed on) did not qualify as exploration. For each task, times were converted into a discrimination ratio according to the general formula: (time at novel − time at old)/(time at old + time at novel), where ‘novel’ on the NOR task refers to the novel object and on the NPR task refers to the novel position object. A value of zero indicates no exploration preference, whereas a positive value indicates preferential exploration of the novel configuration, thus indicating memory of the familiar configuration. Any mouse that did not reach the criterion or did not explore the objects at all (explored both objects less than three times or did not explore one objects at all) was excluded from the analysis. Running speed was analyzed by EthoVision XT (Noldus, Netherlands).

Open field test (OFT) was used to evaluate locomotion after activation and inhibition of SuM neurons. The OFT apparatus was the same box used in the NPR test. Mice were gently placed in the center of the field 1 hr after CNO injection and the movement was recorded for 5 min with a video tracking system. The arena was cleaned with a 70% ethanol solution before and after each test. All behavior experiments were performed in a dim light room (~60 lux) with 40 dB background noise.

Fiber photometry system

The multi-fiber photometry system was used as previously described (Luo et al., 2018; Meng et al., 2018). Briefly, the system consisted of a 488 nm excitation laser, a fluorescence cube and a spectrometer (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). The 488 nm laser beams first launched into the fluorescence cube, then launched into the optical fibers. The GCaMP and tdTomato emission fluorescence collected from the fiber probe traveled back to the spectrometer. Only animals with strong GCaMP and tdTomato expression were included in the study (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B). Spectral data was acquired by OceanView software (Ocean Optics, Inc, Figure 1—figure supplement 1C) at 10 Hz and was synchronized to a 20 Hz video recording system to acquire the animal behavior.

The in vivo recordings were carried out in an open-top home cage (21.6 × 17.8 × 12.7 cm) or in the open field, for the NPR in the 30 Lux red light environment. Laser power was adjusted to a final optical fiber output of 30 mW. Photometry data were exported to MATLAB R2014b for analysis. Coefficients of GCaMP6f and tdTomato were unmixed by a customized script by fitting spectrum signals to standard emission curves. GCaMP6f signals were normalized by tdTomato signals for motion correction. (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D). The fluorescence bleaching was corrected by a 0.1 Hz high-pass filter. Photometry signals (∆F/F) were derived by calculating (F–F0)/F0, where F0 is the median of the fluorescence signal (Figure 1—figure supplement 1E). For the home cage analysis, we recorded data for 5 min per mouse and calculated the ∆F/F for further analyze the correlation of SuM and DG signals in raw data (Figure 1C). Preprocessed GCaMP6 signal from SuM and DG were analyzed by Morlet wavelet with customized MATLAB scripts and segmented into frequency bands between 0.05–2 Hz (Figure 1—figure supplement 1F–H). We selected the correlation of SuM and DG signals in the low frequency spectrum 0.1–0.5 Hz (Figure 1D–E, Figure 1—figure supplement 1I) because the main frequency band was at 0.1–0.5 Hz (Figure 1—figure supplement 1F–G). We also found the calcium activity of the SuM and DG to highly correlate at this frequency band (Figure 1—figure supplement 1H) Correlation matrices across various frequencies were measured by Pearson's Linear Correlation Coefficient function ‘corr’ in MATLAB. To analyze GCaMP signals in NPR behaviors, we defined the exploration start time as 0 s (based on the video) and aligned ∆F/F to a ± 3 s window around the exploration point and further calculated GCaMP activity. GCaMP activity was further divided into a 2 s window for three periods as pre-exploring (−3 ~ −1 s), exploring (−1 ~ 1 s) and post-exploring (1 ~ 3 s) for cumulative activity analysis. Cumulative activity = ∆F/F × Time. Correlation of average SuM and DG activities in raw data was analyzed by Pearson's Linear Correlation Coefficient function ‘corr’ in MATLAB. To analyze the GCaMP signals in the DREADD experiments, the average of the top 20 peaks of ∆F/F during 5 min of baseline and 40 min after CNO treatment were calculated (Luo et al., 2018). The customized spectral linear unmixing algorithm scripts and analysis codes written in MATLAB are available on Github (https://github.com/Yadlee/SuM-DG-calcium-activity-correlation-analysisLi, 2020; copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/SuM-DG-calcium-activity-correlation-analysis).

In vitro electrophysiology

The in vitro electrophysiology experiments were performed as previously described (Song et al., 2012; Yeh et al., 2018). 4–6 weeks after AAV-ChR2 and shVgat/shVglut2 injections into the SuM, Vgat-Cre mice were anesthetized with 5% isoflurane in oxygen and transcardially perfused with ice-cold NMDG-based aCSF (N-methyl-D-glucamine) saturated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 and containing (in mM): 92 NMDG, 30 NaHCO3, 25 glucose, 20 HEPES, 10 MgSO4, 5 sodium ascorbate, 3 sodium pyruvate, 2.5 KCl, 2 thiourea, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 0.5 CaCl2 (pH 7.3, 305–315 mOsm). Brains were rapidly removed, and acute coronal slices (280 μm) containing the SuM or hippocampus were cut using a vibratome (VT1200, Leica, Germany). Slices were warmed to 34.5°C for 8 min and then maintained in a holding chamber containing HEPES aCSF (in mM): 92 NaCl, 30 NaHCO3, 25 glucose, 20 HEPES, 5 sodium ascorbate, 3 sodium pyruvate, 2.5 KCl, 2 thiourea, 2 MgSO4, 2 CaCl2, 1.25 NaH2PO4 (pH 7.3, 305–315 mOsm) at room temperature for at least 1 hr before recording.

Electrophysiological recordings were conducted in the whole-cell configuration using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Axon Instruments). Signals were filtered at 1 kHz and sampled at 10 kHz using the Digidata 1440A (Axon Instruments), data acquisition was performed using pClamp 10.3 (Axon Instruments). For circuit mapping experiments, patch-clamp recordings were made from granule cells in the dentate gyrus of the dorsal hippocampus using a Cs-based intracellular solution (containing in mM: 127.5 CH3O3SCs, 7.5 CsCl, 2.5 MgCl2, 0.6 EGTA, 10 HEPES, 4 ATP-Na2, 0.4 GTP-Na3, 10 phosphocreatine-Na (pH 7.25, 290 mOsm). Under this condition, where the reversal potential of Cl- is nearly −60 mV, it is possible to detect outward GABAergic (at +5 mV) and inward glutamatergic currents (at −60 mV). Functional synaptic connectivity to SuM neurons was determined by optogenetic stimulation of CHR2+ terminals in the DG brain sections. The 473 nm LED (Thorlabs, Canada), at 3–5 mWs, provided optical stimulation through the 40X objective. Light-evoked currents in granule cells were obtained by applying 5 ms, 10 Hz light pulses for 2 s every 20 s. Light-evoked postsynaptic current amplitudes were calculated at the peak of the first response after light pulses. Latencies were calculated as the delays from the onset of light pulses to the onset of oEPSCs or oIPSCs. To block GABAergic or glutamatergic components, 50 μM d-(−)−2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (d-APV), 10 μM 6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (NBQX), or 20 μM bicuculline (BIC) were added to block NMDA, AMPA, and GABAA receptors, respectively. Chemicals were obtained from Sigma or Tocris. Series resistance (Rs) was monitored throughout all experiments and cells with Rs changes over 20% were discarded. To label the recorded cells, 0.2% biocytin was included in the recording pipette. After whole-cell recordings, slices were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 12 hr, then washed thoroughly with PBS. Slices were incubated with streptavidin conjugated to Alexa 488 (Life technology, USA) and DAPI (Life technology, USA) for 6 hr at room temperature, sections were then washed and mounted on glass slides using Bio-Rad Fluoroguard Anti-fade Reagent mounting media, and visualized under a confocal microscope (FV-3000, Olympus, Japan).

Immunohistochemistry

For c-Fos staining, mice were perfused 2 hr after the injection of 1 mg/kg CNO or after 8 hr of opto-stimulation at 10 Hz, 5 ms latency in a 5 min ON/5 min OFF protocol. Mice were anesthetized with 4% isoflurane and perfused with 25 ml room temperature PBS followed by 25 ml ice-cold 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PB. Brains were collected and placed in 4% PFA for 4 hr and switched to 30% sucrose for 2–3 days until they were fully submerged. Brains were sectioned on a microtome at a thickness of 40 μm, and stored in an anti-freeze solution at −20°C for further usage.

For floating section staining (Bao et al., 2017), brain sections were washed twice with PBS, transferred to 1 mg/ml sodium borohydride in PBS for 20 mins, followed by permeabilization with 0.3% Triton-100 PBS for 20 min. After washing with PBS for 5 min and blocking with 3% fresh donkey serum in PBS for 30 min, slices were incubated overnight at 4°C in PBS containing 0.03% Triton-X (PBST), primary antibodies. Primary antibodies against c-Fos (1:3000; Anti-Rabbit; Millipore, USA) GABA (1:500, Anti-Rabbit, Sigma), GFP (1:1000, Anti-Goat, Rockland), Vgat (1:500, Anti-Rabbit, Synaptic Systems) and Vglut2 (1:500, Anti-Guinea pig, Millipore) were used. Sections were incubated in the primary antibody overnight or 48 hr (c-Fos) at 4 °C on the shaker. On the second day, brain sections were washed with PBS and then incubated with secondary antibody at room temperature for 2 hr.

Imaging and cell quantification

Confocal images were acquired by Olympus FLUOVIEW3000 confocal microscopy, under 20x or 60x Oil objectives (NA1.30), XY-resolution 0.4975 mm/pixel, Z-resolution 0.5 or 1.0 µm/slice. Tiled images were stitched after acquisition using the Olympus FluoView imaging software. Brightness and contrast were adjusted with ImageJ. Whole-brain input screening images images for retrograde tracing experiments were acquired and stitched by UNC Translational Pathology Laboratory. For c-Fos and GFP quantification, whole tiled DG and SuM images were acquired and quantified manually through z-stacks using image J (FIJI). For each mouse, five spaced sections from anterior to posterior were stained, and total number of cells were manually counted and were normalized to the DG volume (calculated in ImageJ). Slide identities were blinded with tape during imaging and quantification. Percent of c-Fos+/mCherry+ cells were calculated in SuM-DREADD experiments (Crowther et al., 2018).

Statistical analyses

Data are reported and presented as the mean ± SEM. Animals or data points were not excluded unless stated, and normality tests were applied. Both NPR/NOR tests and cell counting were performed blinded to the conditions of the experiments. Discrimination ratios were also compared with chance level performance (zero) using one-sample t-tests. To evaluate the discrimination ratios for different groups, we used unpaired t-tests. An unpaired t-test was used after Fisher transform to compare R value between different groups. When applicable, statistical tests were paired or unpaired t-test and one-way or two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), followed by PLSD post hoc test. Testing was always performed two-tailed with α = 0.05. The sample size was represented by ‘n’ and reported in the figure legends. ‘n.s’ indicates no significant difference (p > 0.05). Statistical analyses were performed in GraphPad Prism8.

Acknowledgements

We thank members of the Song lab for comments and discussions. We also thank Bentley Midkiff in the UNC Translational Pathology Laboratory (TPL) for the technical assistance with the quantification of rabies-based retrograde tracing. Confocal microscopy was performed at the UNC Neuroscience Microscopy Core Facility with the technical assistance from Dr. Michelle S Itano, The Neuroscience Microscopy Core was supported in part by funding from the NIH-NINDS Neuroscience Center Support Grant P30 NS045892 and the NIH-NICHD Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center Support Grant U54 HD079124. This work was supported by grants awarded to JS from NIH (R01MH111773, R01MH122692, R21AG058160, R21NS104530), and Alzheimer’s Association.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Juan Song, Email: juansong@email.unc.edu.

Laura L Colgin, University of Texas at Austin, United States.

Hiroshi Ito, Max Planck Institute for Brain Research, Germany.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institutes of Health MH111773 to Luis Quintanilla, Juan Song.

  • National Institutes of Health AG058160 to Juan Song.

  • National Institutes of Health NS104530 to Juan Song.

  • National Institutes of Health MH122692 to Juan Song.

  • Alzheimer's Association to Juan Song.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Wrote the paper, Designed all experiments, Carried out all aspects of experiments and collected the data, Assisted with preparing the manuscript.

Assisted with in vivo photometry recording and data analysis, Assisted with preparing the manuscript.

Carried out all aspects of in vitro slice electrophysiology and data analysis.

Prepared the AAVs with genetic knockdown of Vgat or Vglut2.

Prepared the rabies virus and AAVs for monosynaptic retrograde tracing experiment.

Assisted with the in vivo photometry recording and data analysis.

Prepared the rabies virus and AAVs for monosynaptic retrograde tracing experiment.

Prepared the AAVs with genetic knockdown of Vgat or Vglut2.

Assisted with in vivo photometry recording and data aalysis.

Wrote the paper, Designed all experiments, Supervised the project.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.

References

  1. Bao H, Asrican B, Li W, Gu B, Wen Z, Lim SA, Haniff I, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K, Philpot B, Song J. Long-Range GABAergic inputs regulate neural stem cell quiescence and control adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Cell Stem Cell. 2017;21:604–617. doi: 10.1016/j.stem.2017.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bao H, Song J. Treating brain disorders by targeting adult neural stem cells. Trends in Molecular Medicine. 2018;24:991–1006. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2018.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barry C, Heys JG, Hasselmo ME. Possible role of acetylcholine in regulating spatial novelty effects on theta rhythm and grid cells. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2012;6:5. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Berger B, Esclapez M, Alvarez C, Meyer G, Catala M. Human and monkey fetal brain development of the supramammillary-hippocampal projections: a system involved in the regulation of theta activity. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2001;429:515–529. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20010122)429:4&#x0003c;515::AID-CNE1&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cohen SJ, Munchow AH, Rios LM, Zhang G, Asgeirsdóttir HN, Stackman RW. The rodent Hippocampus is essential for nonspatial object memory. Current Biology. 2013;23:1685–1690. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Crowther AJ, Lim SA, Asrican B, Albright BH, Wooten J, Yeh CY, Bao H, Cerri DH, Hu J, Ian Shih YY, Asokan A, Song J. An Adeno-Associated Virus-Based toolkit for preferential targeting and manipulating quiescent neural stem cells in the adult Hippocampus. Stem Cell Reports. 2018;10:1146–1159. doi: 10.1016/j.stemcr.2018.01.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Frankland PW, Josselyn SA, Köhler S. The neurobiological foundation of memory retrieval. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:1576–1585. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0493-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Garcia SV, Brischoux F, Clement O, Libourel PA, Arthaud S, Lazarus M, Luppi PH, Fort P. Ventromedial medulla inhibitory neuron inactivation induces REM sleep without atonia and REM sleep behavior disorder. Nature Communications. 2018;9:1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02761-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Harris JA, Devidze N, Verret L, Ho K, Halabisky B, Thwin MT, Kim D, Hamto P, Lo I, Yu GQ, Palop JJ, Masliah E, Mucke L. Transsynaptic progression of amyloid-β-induced neuronal dysfunction within the entorhinal-hippocampal network. Neuron. 2010;68:428–441. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.10.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Hashimotodani Y, Karube F, Yanagawa Y, Fujiyama F, Kano M. Supramammillary nucleus afferents to the dentate gyrus Co-release glutamate and GABA and potentiate granule cell output. Cell Reports. 2018;25:2704–2715. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.11.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hasselmo ME. The role of acetylcholine in learning and memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2006;16:710–715. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hyman BT, Van Hoesen GW, Kromer LJ, Damasio AR. Perforant pathway changes and the memory impairment of alzheimer's disease. Annals of Neurology. 1986;20:472–481. doi: 10.1002/ana.410200406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Inoue M, Takeuchi A, Manita S, Horigane SI, Sakamoto M, Kawakami R, Yamaguchi K, Otomo K, Yokoyama H, Kim R, Yokoyama T, Takemoto-Kimura S, Abe M, Okamura M, Kondo Y, Quirin S, Ramakrishnan C, Imamura T, Sakimura K, Nemoto T, Kano M, Fujii H, Deisseroth K, Kitamura K, Bito H. Rational engineering of XCaMPs, a multicolor GECI suite for in vivo Imaging of Complex Brain Circuit Dynamics. Cell. 2019;177:1346–1360. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ito HT, Moser EI, Moser MB. Supramammillary nucleus modulates Spike-Time coordination in the Prefrontal-Thalamo-Hippocampal circuit during navigation. Neuron. 2018;99:576–587. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Knierim JJ, Neunuebel JP. Tracking the flow of hippocampal computation: pattern separation, pattern completion, and attractor dynamics. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2016;129:38–49. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kocsis B, Vertes RP. Phase relations of rhythmic neuronal firing in the supramammillary nucleus and mammillary body to the hippocampal theta activity in urethane anesthetized rats. Hippocampus. 1997;7:204–214. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1997)7:2&#x0003c;204::AID-HIPO7&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-M. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kwan AC. What can population calcium imaging tell us about neural circuits? Journal of Neurophysiology. 2008;100:2977–2980. doi: 10.1152/jn.91037.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Leger M, Quiedeville A, Bouet V, Haelewyn B, Boulouard M, Schumann-Bard P, Freret T. Object recognition test in mice. Nature Protocols. 2013;8:2531–2537. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2013.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Leranth C, Hajszan T. Extrinsic afferent systems to the dentate gyrus. Progress in Brain Research. 2007;163:63–84. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)63004-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Li Y. SuM-DG-Calcium-Activity-Correlation-Analysis. ac80979GitHub. 2020 https://github.com/Yadlee/SuM-DG-calcium-activity-correlation-analysis
  22. Looger LL, Dwyer MA, Smith JJ, Hellinga HW. Computational design of receptor and sensor proteins with novel functions. Nature. 2003;423:185–190. doi: 10.1038/nature01556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Luo YJ, Li YD, Wang L, Yang SR, Yuan XS, Wang J, Cherasse Y, Lazarus M, Chen JF, Qu WM, Huang ZL. Nucleus accumbens controls wakefulness by a subpopulation of neurons expressing dopamine D1 receptors. Nature Communications. 2018;9:1576. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03889-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Meng C, Zhou J, Papaneri A, Peddada T, Xu K, Cui G. Spectrally resolved fiber photometry for Multi-component analysis of brain circuits. Neuron. 2018;98:707–717. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Montchal ME, Reagh ZM, Yassa MA. Precise temporal memories are supported by the lateral entorhinal cortex in humans. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:284–288. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0303-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Oliveira AM, Hawk JD, Abel T, Havekes R. Post-training reversible inactivation of the Hippocampus enhances novel object recognition memory. Learning & Memory. 2010;17:155–160. doi: 10.1101/lm.1625310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Palop JJ, Mucke L. Network abnormalities and interneuron dysfunction in alzheimer disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2016;17:777–792. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Pan WX, McNaughton N. The supramammillary area: its organization, functions and relationship to the Hippocampus. Progress in Neurobiology. 2004;74:127–166. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Paxinos G, Franklin KB. Paxinos and Franklin's the Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. 5th Edition. Vol. 375. Academic press; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  30. Pedersen NP, Ferrari L, Venner A, Wang JL, Abbott SBG, Vujovic N, Arrigoni E, Saper CB, Fuller PM. Supramammillary glutamate neurons are a key node of the arousal system. Nature Communications. 2017;8:1405. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01004-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Reagh ZM, Noche JA, Tustison NJ, Delisle D, Murray EA, Yassa MA. Functional imbalance of anterolateral entorhinal cortex and hippocampal dentate/CA3 Underlies Age-Related Object Pattern Separation Deficits. Neuron. 2018;97:1187–1198. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Root DH, Zhang S, Barker DJ, Miranda-Barrientos J, Liu B, Wang HL, Morales M. Selective brain distribution and distinctive synaptic architecture of dual Glutamatergic-GABAergic neurons. Cell Reports. 2018;23:3465–3479. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.05.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Roy DS, Kitamura T, Okuyama T, Ogawa SK, Sun C, Obata Y, Yoshiki A, Tonegawa S. Distinct neural circuits for the formation and retrieval of episodic memories. Cell. 2017;170:1000–1012. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.07.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sawangjit A, Oyanedel CN, Niethard N, Salazar C, Born J, Inostroza M. The Hippocampus is crucial for forming non-hippocampal long-term memory during sleep. Nature. 2018;564:109–113. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0716-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Song J, Zhong C, Bonaguidi MA, Sun GJ, Hsu D, Gu Y, Meletis K, Huang ZJ, Ge S, Enikolopov G, Deisseroth K, Luscher B, Christian KM, Ming GL, Song H. Neuronal circuitry mechanism regulating adult quiescent neural stem-cell fate decision. Nature. 2012;489:150–154. doi: 10.1038/nature11306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Soussi R, Zhang N, Tahtakran S, Houser CR, Esclapez M. Heterogeneity of the supramammillary-hippocampal pathways: evidence for a unique GABAergic neurotransmitter phenotype and regional differences. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;32:771–785. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07329.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Treves A, Tashiro A, Witter MP, Moser EI. What is the mammalian dentate gyrus good for? Neuroscience. 2008;154:1155–1172. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.04.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Valeeva G, Janackova S, Nasretdinov A, Rychkova V, Makarov R, Holmes GL, Khazipov R, Lenck-Santini PP. Emergence of coordinated activity in the developing Entorhinal-Hippocampal network. Cerebral Cortex. 2019;29:906–920. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Valencia Garcia S, Libourel PA, Lazarus M, Grassi D, Luppi PH, Fort P. Genetic inactivation of glutamate neurons in the rat sublaterodorsal tegmental nucleus recapitulates REM sleep behaviour disorder. Brain. 2017;140:414–428. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Vertes RP. Major diencephalic inputs to the Hippocampus: supramammillary nucleus and nucleus reuniens. Circuitry and function. Progress in Brain Research. 2015;219:121–144. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Vivar C, van Praag H. Functional circuits of new neurons in the dentate gyrus. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2013;7:15. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Winters BD, Forwood SE, Cowell RA, Saksida LM, Bussey TJ. Double dissociation between the effects of peri-postrhinal cortex and hippocampal lesions on tests of object recognition and spatial memory: heterogeneity of function within the temporal lobe. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:5901–5908. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1346-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Winters BD, Saksida LM, Bussey TJ. Object recognition memory: neurobiological mechanisms of encoding, consolidation and retrieval. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32:1055–1070. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Yeh CY, Asrican B, Moss J, Quintanilla LJ, He T, Mao X, Cassé F, Gebara E, Bao H, Lu W, Toni N, Song J. Mossy cells control adult neural stem cell quiescence and maintenance through a dynamic balance between direct and indirect pathways. Neuron. 2018;99:493–510. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Hiroshi Ito1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

The study demonstrates the key role of the projection from the supramammillary nucleus to the dentate gyrus in spatial memory, and the authors have addressed the major concerns of the reviewers and strengthened the primary findings.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Supramammillary nucleus synchronizes with dentate gyrus to regulate spatial memory retrieval through glutamate release" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by Laura Colgin as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

Li and colleagues report findings that shed light on the role of the projection from the supramammillary nucleus to the dentate gyrus in spatial memory. The manuscript includes two major observations; 1) calcium signals measured by fiber photometry are correlated between SuM and DG in the range of frequency from 0.1 to 0.5 Hz during memory retrieval of object locations. 2) the manipulation of the SuM activity by the DREADDs, optogenetics, or shRNAs resulted in impairment of memory about object locations. Based on these observations, the authors conclude that glutamatergic inputs from SuM to DG are responsible for the correlated and elevated activity in DG, which supports retrieval of spatial memory.

All reviewers agreed that the methods are comprehensive and the main conclusions are well supported by the results and analyses in general. However, the reviewers raised several concerns and suggestions for further analyses and clarification to support the main conclusions. The essential revisions suggested by the reviewers are listed below.

Essential revisions:

1) It is imperative to include actual histological sections from DG and SUM showing the locations of the various probes (or AAV constructs) to verify locations in respective sites. For instance, in this regard, the authors are obviously aware that there are two populations of SUM neurons projecting to DG: GABA/GLUT fibers of the lateral SUM and only GLUT+ cells/fibers from the medial SUM (Soussi et al., 2010). Did they attempt to selectively activate (or inhibit) these two systems? If done selectively for the medial GLUT system, this could further support their claim of a SUM GLUT involvement in retrieval. In addition, it seems that some of their manipulations (e.g., DREADDs) could have differentially affected one or the other system and if more the medial than the lateral system, this could be the basis for the greater GLUT behavioral effect?

2) Although the DREADD-induced SUM activation or inhibition differentially affected the DG (c-fos labeled cells and other), this does not necessarily mean that this connection (SUM-DG) was responsible for the DREADD-induced behavioral effects they obtained on the NPR test. The SUM projects to other sites that could affect this behavior, notably to the medial septum and CA2 of the hippocampus. Similarly, optogenetic activation of SUM Vgat fibers obviously affects the DG – is this the only site, possibly also CA2? Also, the large differences in discrimination ratios with optogenetic stimulation for retrieval vs. encoding are not apparent when examining Figures 3K and L. These points should be clarified.

3) The SuM-DG synchrony at 0.1-0.5 Hz may be related to a particular behavior, as oscillatory activity in the brain largely depends on the animal's behavioral state, especially running speed in rodents (e.g. Omar and Mehta, 2012; Long et al., 2014). One possibility of the observed higher synchrony between SUM and DG may thus be due to lower running speed, for example, by spending more time to check the objects. The authors should thus examine if the animal's running speed affects the degree of synchrony.

4) While several manipulations of neural activity were performed by using DREADDs, optogenetics, and shRNAs, their influence on animals' behaviors is not investigated carefully, except for open field experiments in Figure 2–figure supplement 2. The authors should check if these manipulations affect the general activity of the animals during the NPR tests, such as running speed or time spent at each object as in Figure 1H.

5) Several analyses and statistics of fiber photometry experiments are not clearly described. For example, in Figure 1D, how were correlation coefficients calculated? Is this correlation of power or phase? (If this is a plot of phase-phase correlations, it cannot have high correlations between different frequencies.) In Figure 1E and F, only single R value and P value are described in the text, and it is not clear which statistical test was implemented, how many animals were used, and how correlation values were acquired from individual animals and trials.

6) It is not clear why the authors only measured "peaks" of ΔF/F for DG and SuM in Figure 2H. Couldn't the authors find any differences in average ΔF/F signals? Or does CNO influence only sporadic synchronous activity in the regions, which may be reflected as peaks? In a similar plot in Figure 6C, the authors instead plotted "Calcium activity" (not clear what this means), and the authors should provide the rationale of these plots. Also, as the authors claim the importance of correlative activity via direct SuM-DG projections, additional correlation analysis between SuM and DG signals would be necessary to exclude a possibility of indirect influence of SuM to DG. Furthermore, did the authors observe any time lag between the two signals from SUM and DG, which could suggest the direction of influence?

eLife. 2020 Mar 13;9:e53129. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53129.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) It is imperative to include actual histological sections from DG and SUM showing the locations of the various probes (or AAV constructs) to verify locations in respective sites. For instance, in this regard, the authors are obviously aware that there are two populations of SUM neurons projecting to DG: GABA/GLUT fibers of the lateral SUM and only GLUT+ cells/fibers from the medial SUM (Soussi et al., 2010). Did they attempt to selectively activate (or inhibit) these two systems? If done selectively for the medial GLUT system, this could further support their claim of a SUM GLUT involvement in retrieval. In addition, it seems that some of their manipulations (e.g., DREADDs) could have differentially affected one or the other system and if more the medial than the lateral system, this could be the basis for the greater GLUT behavioral effect?

This is an important point raised by the reviewer. Indeed, as the reviewer mentioned, it has been suggested that there are two distinct pathways from the SuM to DG with distinct neurotransmitter systems. One pathway originates from the lateral SuM (SuML) expressing markers for both GABA and glutamate(Pedersen et al., 2017; Soussi et al., 2010). The other pathway originates from the medial SuM (SuMM) mainly expressing VGLUT2 (Vertes, 2015). In the current study, we mainly targeted SuML based on our rabies-based monosynaptic retrograde tracing (Figure 2–figure supplement 1). Specifically, our rabies tracing revealed that major inputs from SuM to DG GCs are located in the SuML, and nearly 80% of DG-projecting SuML neurons are positive for GABA. By contrast, the input neurons labeled in the SuMM is sparse. These data support SuML inputs as the major inputs to DG GCs. Therefore, we specifically target SuML by injecting AAVs to the lateral SuM using VGAT-Cre mice. By doing this, we preferentially manipulated the activity of Vgat+ Vglut2+ neurons in the SuML and demonstrated that SuM glutamate transmission is necessary for spatial memory retrieval during the NPR test. We have included the images from both DG and SuM in multiple places throughout the manuscript (Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 6, Figure 2–figure supplement 1, Figure 2–figure supplement 3, Figure 3–figure supplement 1), as well as fiber placement in relation to those brain regions in Figure 1 and Figure 6. For scientific rigor and reproducibility, we have excluded the animals with off-target injection and fiber implantation from our analysis after post-hoc examination of the injection/implantation sites. To clarify our targeting of lateral SuM, we have modified the terms from "SuM" to "SuML" throughout the manuscript.

To further address whether SuMM neurons impact spatial memory retrieval, we attempted to selectively target SuMM neurons by injecting a reduced volume of AAV5-CaMKII-hM3Dq-mCherry (150 µL) to SuMM (Figure 2–figure supplement 3A). As a result, we found that chemogenetic activation of SuMM neurons does not significantly increase the discrimination ratio in the NPR test (Figure 2–figure supplement 3C). These results indicate that SuML neurons, but not SuMM neurons, regulate spatial memory retrieval in the NPR test. We have modified the text accordingly to reflect this added component (subsection "SuM activity is required for regulating spatial memory retrieval", last paragraph, and Figure 2–figure supplement 3).

2) Although the DREADD-induced SUM activation or inhibition differentially affected the DG (c-fos labeled cells and other), this does not necessarily mean that this connection (SUM-DG) was responsible for the DREADD-induced behavioral effects they obtained on the NPR test. The SUM projects to other sites that could affect this behavior, notably to the medial septum and CA2 of the hippocampus. Similarly, optogenetic activation of SUM Vgat fibers obviously affects the DG – is this the only site, possibly also CA2? Also, the large differences in discrimination ratios with optogenetic stimulation for retrieval vs. encoding are not apparent when examining Figures 3K and 3L. These points should be clarified.

We agree with the reviewer that DREADD-mediated activation or inhibition of the SuM neurons does not necessarily mean that the SuM-DG projections are responsible for the behavioral effects. This is why we performed optogenetic experiment to specifically manipulate the SuM-DG projections (Figure 3). By doing so, we showed that optogenetic activation of SuM-DG projections during spatial memory retrieval is sufficient to improve memory performance during the NPR test. During revision, we added optogenetic inhibition experiment by inhibiting the activity of the SuM-DG projections to further support the requirement of SuM-DG pathway during memory retrieval (Figure 3M-N). Specifically, we found that optogenetic inhibition of SuMVgat-DG projections during memory retrieval significantly decreased the discrimination ratio in the NPR test (Figure 3N). In contrast, optogenetic inhibition of SuMVgat-DG projections during memory encoding did not alter the discrimination ratio (Figure 3M-N). Together with the results from optogenetic activation of the SuMVgat-DG pathway, we provide bi-directional evidence that the activity of SuMVgat-DG pathway during memory retrieval is both sufficient and necessary in regulating spatial memory retrieval during the NPR test. Please see the second paragraph of the subsection "SuM-DG circuit activity is required for spatial memory retrieval (but not encoding)" and Figure 3M-N for added data.

As the reviewer pointed out, our anterograde tracing also revealed SuM projections to other hippocampal structures, such as CA2 (Figure 2–figure supplement 1A-C). To address whether SuM-CA2 pathway is involved in spatial memory retrieval during the NPR test, we injected AAV5-DIO-ChR2 to the SuM in Vgat-Cre mice and bilaterally implanted optic fibers above CA2. As a result, we found that optogenetic activation of SuMVgat-CA2 projection did not significantly alter the discrimination ratio in the NPR test (Figure 3–figure supplement 2), indicating that SuMVgat-CA2 projections are not sufficient to regulate spatial memory retrieval during the NPR test. Please see the last paragraph of the aforementioned subsection and Figure 3–figure supplement 2 for added data.

To address the reviewer's comments on the discrimination ratio when the SuM-DG circuit was stimulated during spatial memory retrieval versus encoding, it is worth mentioning that Figure 3K and L are from two separate groups of mice with their own littermate controls with slightly different baselines. Our results showed that optogenetic activation of the SuM-DG pathway during spatial memory retrieval (but not encoding) is sufficient to improve memory performance (Figure 3K-L). Furthermore, we obtained additional support from optogenetic inhibition experiment, and demonstrated that the discrimination ratio is significantly reduced when the SuM-DG circuit is inhibited during spatial memory retrieval, as compared to that when the SuM-DG circuit is inhibited during spatial memory encoding (Figure 3M-N).

3) The SuM-DG synchrony at 0.1-0.5 Hz may be related to a particular behavior, as oscillatory activity in the brain largely depends on the animal's behavioral state, especially running speed in rodents (e.g. Omar and Mehta, 2012; Long et al., 2014). One possibility of the observed higher synchrony between SUM and DG may thus be due to lower running speed, for example, by spending more time to check the objects. The authors should thus examine if the animal's running speed affects the degree of synchrony.

This is a good point raised by the reviewer. We believe that the high correlation of the calcium activity between SuM and DG is not due to the low running speed based on the following evidence. (1) During both memory encoding and retrieval in the NPR test, mice exhibited very low running speed because they have to slow down or even stop in order to explore the objects, and this time point was assigned as "0" in the plots of Figure 1J and N. However, the correlation of the SuM and DG calcium activity was only slightly increased during the encoding phase (Figure 1J), while it significantly increased during the retrieval phase (Figure 1N), suggesting that the low running speed is independent of the high SuM-DG calcium correlation. (2) During memory retrieval, mice spent less time in exploring the object A (old location) than object B (new location). However, the correlation between the SuM and DG calcium activity was similar, thus suggesting that increased SuM-DG calcium correlation during memory retrieval is independent of the time mice spent in exploring the objects/locations. (3) An additional supporting evidence comes from the measurement of the correlation between the EC and DG calcium activity (Figure 1–figure supplement 2). In this experiment, we demonstrated that the correlation between the EC and DG calcium activity is significantly lower than that between the SuM and DG during spatial memory retrieval in the NPR test, suggesting that low running speed does not necessarily lead to the high correlation of two anatomically highly connected brain regions, such as EC and DG.

4) While several manipulations of neural activity were performed by using DREADDs, optogenetics, and shRNAs, their influence on animals' behaviors is not investigated carefully, except for open field experiments in the Figure 2–figure supplement 2. The authors should check if these manipulations affect the general activity of the animals during the NPR tests, such as running speed or time spent at each object as in Figure 1H.

We agree with the reviewer on this point. As suggested by the reviewer, here we provide the measurements of the running speed from chemogenetic and optogenetic activation experiments (Figure 2—figure supplement 2E-H). Our results showed chemogenetic activation of SuMLVgat neurons or optogenetic activation of SuMLVgat-DG projections did not increase the average running speed of the mice in the NPR test. These results suggest that the general activity of the mice does not contribute to the increased discrimination ratio observed in the NPR test.

5) Several analyses and statistics of fiber photometry experiments are not clearly described. For example, in Figure 1D, how were correlation coefficients calculated? Is this correlation of power or phase? (If this is a plot of phase-phase correlations, it cannot have high correlations between different frequencies.) In Figure 1E and F, only single R value and P value are described in the text, and it is not clear which statistical test was implemented, how many animals were used, and how correlation values were acquired from individual animals and trials.

In Figure 1D, the heatmap matrix showed the power correlation of different frequency bands. Specifically, preprocessed GCaMP6f signals from SuM and DG were analyzed by Morlet wavelet with customized MATLAB scripts and segmented into frequency bands between 0.05-2 Hz. Frequency range were selected based on power spectral density (Figure 1–figure supplement 1E-H). Then, correlation matrix across various frequencies were measured by Pearson's Linear Correlation Coefficient function "corr" in MATLAB.

In Figure 1E and F, 10 mice were included. The correlation values (R) were acquired from the 5 min traces of SuM and DG calcium activities (ΔF/F). Same correlation function "corr" was applied and returned the correlation coefficient along with probability value testing two-tailed null hypothesis. In Figure 1E, both R and P value was from one single animal corresponding to the sample trace shown. In Figure 1F, correlation of SuM and DG were measured individually from 10 mice and described in mean value in the text. The P value represents the statistical significance of each R value. Combined P value was measured by Fisher's P-value in MATLAB.

The information mentioned above has been incorporated into the Materials and methods section.

6) It is not clear why the authors only measured "peaks" of ΔF/F for DG and SuM in Figure 2H. Couldn't the authors find any differences in average ΔF/F signals? Or does CNO influence only sporadic synchronous activity in the regions, which may be reflected as peaks? In a similar plot in Figure 6C, the authors instead plotted "Calcium activity" (not clear what this means), and the authors should provide the rationale of these plots. Also, as the authors claim the importance of correlative activity via direct SuM-DG projections, additional correlation analysis between SuM and DG signals would be necessary to exclude a possibility of indirect influence of SuM to DG. Furthermore, did the authors observe any time lag between the two signals from SUM and DG, which could suggest the direction of influence?

The average ΔF/F signals were not significantly changed in 5-min home cage recording. The photometry signal (ΔF/F) was calculated from (F–F0)/F0, where F0 is the median of the fluorescence signal. There is no significant difference between different groups in averaged activity. Therefore, we adjusted the threshold for event detection to 3 x standard deviation (3SD), and only ΔF/F signals over the threshold are considered as "neuronal activity". In Figure 6C, the "calcium activity" indicates "neuronal activity (percentage of ΔF/F above threshold)". To clarify it, we have modified the label in Figure 6C.

We compared the calcium activity of mCherry control and hM3Dq mice by sorting the highest 20 peaks of 5-min ΔF/F traces recorded in the home cage and calculated mean ΔF/F of the 20 peaks as a dot in the Figure 2H and 2I. We found that after CNO activation of SuM neurons, the peaks of ΔF/F traces were increased in hM3Dq mice, indicating that the calcium activity was increased. These analyses are based on the method of our previous study (Luo et al., 2018) and are adapted from several previous publications (Beier et al., 2017; Calipari et al., 2016).

The time lag between SuM and DG was measured by cross correlation "xcorr" function in MATLAB (reviewer only Figure 2). Specifically, during the encoding phase, the SuM appears to respond to the object/location 0.5 seconds ahead of the DG. During the retrieval phase, the time lag between the SuM and DG is 0. These data are consistent with the Pearson's correlation in Figure 1L and P, thus supporting the leading role of SuM during memory encoding of the NPR test. However, it is worth mentioning the technical limitations in the current study. For fiber photometry recording, the acquisition rate of GCaMP6 signals by spectrometer was 10 Hz (Meng et al., 2018), which may miss some of the calcium events during the interval and underestimate the lag time. Future studies using in vivo multi-channel electrophysiological recording may help validate the lag time between these two brain regions.

Author response image 1. The time lag between the SuM and DG calcium activities.

Author response image 1.

(A) Scaled color plot of the SuM and DG calcium activity. (B) Averaged DG and SuM calcium activities. (C) Cross correlation of the DG and SuM calcium activities during the exploring phase (2 s). The highest correlation appeared at -0.5 and -0.6 s during familiarization, indicating that there is a 0.5-0.6 s time lag between the DG and SuM calcium activities in memory encoding. In contrast, during spatial memory retrieval, the highest correlation appeared at 0 s, indicating no time lag between the DG and SuM calcium activities in memory retrieval.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES