Abstract
Background
The present paper presents a fundamentally novel approach to model individual differences of persons with the same biologically heterogeneous mental disorder. Unlike prevalent case-control analyses, that assume a clear distinction between patient and control groups and thereby introducing the concept of an ‘average patient’, we describe each patient's biology individually, gaining insights into the different facets that characterize persistent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
Methods
Using a normative modeling approach, we mapped inter-individual differences in reference to normative structural brain changes across the lifespan to examine the degree to which case-control analyses disguise differences between individuals.
Results
At the level of the individual, deviations from the normative model were frequent in persistent ADHD. However, the overlap of more than 2% between participants with ADHD was only observed in few brain loci. On average, participants with ADHD showed significantly reduced gray matter in the cerebellum and hippocampus compared to healthy individuals. While the case-control differences were in line with the literature on ADHD, individuals with ADHD only marginally reflected these group differences.
Conclusions
Case-control comparisons, disguise inter-individual differences in brain biology in individuals with persistent ADHD. The present results show that the ‘average ADHD patient’ has limited informative value, providing the first evidence for the necessity to explore different biological facets of ADHD at the level of the individual and practical means to achieve this end.
Key words: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, heterogeneity, normative modeling, precision medicine
Introduction
Two patients suffering from the same mental disorder may show differences in symptom expression, behavior, and pathophysiology. Case-control research paradigms ignore these sources of heterogeneity; they assume that each diagnostic group is a distinct entity. A key goal in many such studies is to identify biological markers that are reliable indicators of disease state. However, markers identified through this approach generally explain only a small part of the variance linked to mental disorders (Schmaal et al., 2016; Hibar et al., 2017; Hoogman et al., 2017). Therefore, the case-control paradigm has been challenged in recent years. For example, large international initiatives aim to bridge the gap between a psychiatric diagnosis and its underlying biology through the integration of information across multiple dimensions (Insel, 2009; Insel et al., 2010; Schumann et al., 2014), yielding subgroups of patients stratified based on behavior (Fair et al., 2013; Mostert et al., 2015) or biological functioning (Marquand et al., 2016b). While such stratification approaches may produce more homogeneous diagnostic groups, these approaches still do not fully inform on how patients differ from one another in terms of the underlying biology. Therefore, inter-individual differences become a novel research focus (Foulkes and Blakemore, 2018; Seghier and Price, 2018).
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a prevalent and impairing neurodevelopmental disorder, which persists into adulthood in a substantial part of the patients (Simon et al., 2009). Reliable group differences between healthy individuals and those with ADHD have been established for various biological readouts (Bush et al., 2005; Seidman et al., 2005; Valera et al., 2007; Cortese and Castellanos, 2012; van Ewijk et al., 2012; van Rooij et al., 2015; Wolfers et al., 2016). These include neuroimaging-based brain readouts, where differences in gray matter volume, white matter volume, as well as functional brain readouts (Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012; Onnink et al., 2014; Faraone et al., 2015; Greven et al., 2015; Wolfers et al., 2015b, 2017; Francx et al., 2016; Norman et al., 2016) have been reported. However, these differences are mostly of small to medium effect size and have not readily translated into individualized predictions (Wolfers et al., 2015a). In line with this observation, evidence accumulated in the last decades points towards ADHD being characterized by a high degree of heterogeneity (Faraone et al., 2015): More specifically, individuals with ADHD can differ from each other in their symptom profiles (clinical heterogeneity), their exposure to environmental stressors (environmental heterogeneity), and the underlying biology of their disorder (biological heterogeneity). This complexity, and the rather exclusive research focus on a categorical diagnosis, has hindered progress towards a better understanding of ADHD (Burmeister et al., 2008; Sullivan et al., 2012). Moreover, the developmental character of ADHD has been shown in numerous studies, and differences in brain development and aging have been observed across the lifespan (Shaw et al., 2007; Greven et al., 2015; Hoogman et al., 2017). Therefore, the importance of modeling ADHD across the lifespan has become increasingly apparent (Shaw et al., 2006; Hoogman et al., 2017). For example, individually different growth trajectories of different brain regions may be an important aspect of this complex phenotype (Shaw et al., 2006, 2007).
In this study, we aimed to quantify and map the brain structural heterogeneity in adults with persistent ADHD, at the level of the individual patient. We employed a normative modeling approach for this purpose, which provides a perspective that is fundamentally different from the classic case-control approach. A normative model can be understood as a statistical model that maps demographic, behavioral, or any other variable to -for example- a quantitative brain read-out (Marquand et al., 2016a), whilst providing estimates of centiles of variation within the population. Then, the individual can be placed within the normative range, allowing for the characterization of differences between individual patients in relation to the healthy range. In this way, we (i) chart the heterogeneity in abnormalities of brain structure at the level of the individual with ADHD, and (ii) investigate the degree of spatial overlap in terms of deviations from the normative model to provide concrete estimates for disorder heterogeneity. Based on previous case-control comparisons (e.g. Onnink et al., 2014; Faraone et al., 2015; Greven et al., 2015; Wolfers et al., 2015b, 2017; Francx et al., 2016), which introduced the notion of the ‘average ADHD patient’, we expected participants with ADHD to show on average larger negative deviations from the normative brain ageing model than healthy individuals. More importantly, we anticipated that the individual local deviance from the normative model would differ substantially between individuals, suggesting that previous group-level distinctions provide an incomplete picture of the neurobiological abnormalities in ADHD and disguise extreme inter-individual differences between individuals with ADHD.
Methods
Participants
We selected adult participants with persistent ADHD and healthy individuals from the Dutch cohort of the International Multicenter persistent ADHD CollaboraTion (IMpACT; Hoogman et al., 2011; Mostert et al., 2015), based on data availability for structural MRI images. Participants with persistent ADHD were recruited from the Department of Psychiatry of the Radboud University Medical Center and through advertisements. In this recruitment process, the participants with persistent ADHD were matched for gender, age, and estimated intelligence to a healthy individual population. All participants underwent psychiatric assessments, neuropsychological testing, and neuroimaging. The diagnostic interview for persistent ADHD (DIVA; Sandra Kooij et al., 2008) was conducted to confirm the diagnosis of ADHD in adulthood. This interview focuses on the 18 DSM-IV symptoms of ADHD and uses realistic examples to thoroughly investigate whether a symptom is currently present or was already present in childhood (Sandra Kooij et al., 2008). In all participants in the ADHD cohort, a childhood history of ADHD symptoms was established, and persistent ADHD was diagnosed. The ADHD Rating Scale-IV was filled in by each participant to report current symptoms of attention and hyperactivity/impulsivity (Pappas, 2006). To assess comorbidities, the structured clinical interviews (SCID-I and SCID-II) for DSM-IV were administered (van Groenestijn et al., 1999; Weertman et al., 2003; Lobbestael et al., 2011). The inclusion criteria for participants with ADHD were: (i) DSM-IV-TR criteria for ADHD met in childhood as well as in adulthood, (ii) no psychosis, (iii) no substance use disorder, (iv) full-scale intelligence estimate >70 (prorated from Block Design and Vocabulary of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (Wechsler, 2012), (v) no neurological disorders, (vi) no obvious sensorimotor disabilities, (vii) no medication use other than psychostimulants or atomoxetine. Additional inclusion criteria for healthy individuals were: (viii) no current neurological or mental disorder according to DIVA, SCID-I, or SCID-II, (ix) no first-degree relatives with ADHD or other major mental disorders. All participants were Dutch and of European Caucasian ancestry. This study was approved by the regional ethics committee (Centrale Commissie Mensgebonden Onderzoek: CMO Regio Arnhem – Nijmegen; Protocol number III.04.0403). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.
MRI acquisition
Whole brain imaging was performed using a 1.5 T scanner (Magnetom Avanto, Siemens Medical Systems) with a standard 8-channel head coil. A high-resolution T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) anatomic scan was obtained from each participant, in which the inversion time (TI) was chosen to provide optimal gray matter–white matter T1 contrast [repetition time (TR) 2730 ms, echo time (TE) 2.95 ms, TI 1000 ms, flip angle 7°, field of view (FOV) 256 × 256 × 176 mm3, voxel size 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm3]. The T1 images served as a basis for the extraction of gray and white matter volumes.
Estimation of gray and white matter volume
Prior to gray matter volume estimation, all participants’ T1 images were rigidly aligned using statistical parametric mapping version 12 (SPM-12). Subsequently, images were segmented, normalized, and bias field–corrected using ‘new segment’ from SPM12 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm; Ashburner and Friston, 2000, 2005) yielding images containing gray and white matter segments. We then used DARTEL (Ashburner, 2007) to create a study-specific gray matter template to which all segmented images were normalized. Subsequently, all gray matter volumes were smoothed with an 8-mm full width half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel, and the normative model was estimated.
Normative modeling
The normative modeling method employed here is described in the supplemental methods (Marquand et al., 2016a). Briefly, normative models were estimated using Gaussian process regression (Rasmussen and Williams, 2006), a Bayesian non-parametric interpolation method that yields coherent measures of predictive confidence in addition to point estimates. This is important, as we used this uncertainty measure to quantify both the centiles of variation within the cohort and the deviation of each patient from the group mean at each specific brain locus. In this way, we were able to statistically quantify deviations from the normative model with regional specificity, by computing a Z-score for each voxel, reflecting the difference between the predicted volume and the true volume normalized by the uncertainty of the prediction (Marquand et al., 2016a). Thus, we quantified extreme positive and negative deviations (reflecting increased or decreased volume, respectively) from the normative model using a reasonable threshold for the resulting z-statistic. In the present study, we estimated normative brain changes across the adult lifespan represented in our study (Fig. 1) using Gaussian process regression to predict regional gray and white matter volumes across the brain from age and sex. The normative range for this model in healthy individuals was estimated using 10-fold cross-validation, then we applied the model trained on all healthy individuals to participants with ADHD.
Fig. 1.
In (a), the estimation of the normative model in healthy individuals is depicted using age and gender as covariates. In (b), the characterization of the normative model is shown. We see that the normative model changes with age and that, from age 20 to 70 years, gray matter is predominantly decreasing; this is true for both females and males and more strongly observed in frontal brain regions. Blue colors indicate a decrease, red colors an increase. In (c), we depict the application of the normative model to persistent ADHD. In (d), we present the steps that were taken to characterize the deviations from the normative model.
First, we assessed group-level deviations from the normative model. For this, individual gray and white matter deviation maps were fed into PALM (Permutation Analysis of Linear Model; Winkler et al., 2015), which allowed for permutation-based inference. We estimated mean group-level deviations from the normative model in healthy individuals and in patients with ADHD. PALM creates a map of z-values for each of these groups. We thresholded these group maps using Z = ± 2.6, to assist comparisons with the individual maps of deviation described below. Further, we report the contrasts for participants with persistent ADHD and healthy individuals corrected for false discovery rate (FDR) at the 5% inference level using threshold-free cluster enhancement.
Next, the individual maps of deviation were thresholded at |Z| > 2.6. These maps reflect the deviation from the normative model at the individual level. Note that the use of a fixed statistical threshold across participants allows for a simplified comparison between participants in terms of numbers of extreme deviation from the normative model, even when the overall distribution of deviations of a participant is shifted. We also repeated the analyses correcting for multiple comparisons at the individual participant level using the Benjamini and Hochberg procedure (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). This did not change our conclusions. Extreme positive deviations were defined as all voxels with a value higher than Z > 2.6, while extreme negative deviations are defined as a value below the Z < −2.6. All extreme deviations were combined into scores representing the percentage of extreme positive and extreme negative deviations for each participant. We tested for associations between diagnosis and those scores using a non-parametric χ2 test in a general linear model. We corrected for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni-Holm method (Holm, 1979). We created individualized maps of extreme deviations and calculated the voxel-wise overlap between individuals from the same groups. In a final analysis, we tested for associations between the percentage of extremely deviating voxels and age, symptom scores, and comorbidity. We corrected for the number of correlations (8) and modality using the Bonferroni-Holm method (Holm, 1979). All analyses were performed in python3.6 (www.python.org).
Results
Participants
Table 1 shows the demographics of the study population. We included 153 adults with ADHD and 146 healthy adults. About the same proportion of individuals in both groups were male (43.8% first and 41.2% second group, respectively). The average age of participants was 35 years in both groups with an age distribution that was very similar (online Supplementary Fig. S1). Individuals with persistent ADHD showed higher scores than healthy individuals for hyperactivity-impulsivity (5.45 v. 0.63; t test: p < 0.01) as well as inattention (7.27 v. 0.55; t test: p < 0.01).
Table 1.
Demographics and clinical characteristics
Healthy individuals | Attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disordera | |
---|---|---|
Demographics | ||
Total (N) | 146 | 153 |
Males (%) | 43.8% | 41.2% |
Age (years) (mean ± std) | 35.43 ± 12.01 | 35.05 ± 10.81 |
Education (years ± std) | 5.19 ± 0.808 | 4.78 ± 0.811 |
Estimated intelligenceb (mean ± std) | 109.94 ± 14.53 | 107.45 ± 15.08 |
Symptoms | ||
Hyperactivity/impulsivityc (mean ± std) | 0.63 ± 1.12 | 5.45 ± 2.46 |
Inattentiond (mean ± std) | 0.55 ± 1.21 | 7.27 ± 1.74 |
Comorbiditiese (mean ± std) | 0.01 ± 0.117 | 0.20 ± 0.436 |
Stimulant medication | Current = 0.0% Past = 0.0% No medication = 100.0% |
Current = 11.8% Past = 76.4% No medication = 11.8% |
ADHD diagnosis was based on a structured Diagnostic Interview for ADHD in Adults (DIVA; Sandra Kooij et al., 2008).
Estimated intelligence was based on the block-design and vocabulary subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III; Wechsler, 2012).
DIVA hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms in adults.
DIVA inattention symptoms in adults.
Normative model
Figure 1a, c and d show a high-level visual summary of the analysis procedure. Figure 1b depicts a spatial representation of the voxel-wise normative model. This model was characterized by global gray matter decreases from age 20 to 70 years, with the largest decreases primarily in frontal and cerebellar regions, which is in line with the typical decline of gray matter volume over age (Ziegler et al., 2012; Farokhian et al., 2017). This was true for females and males, which we modeled separately due to the presence of sex effects in ADHD (Martin et al., 2018). In contrast, the normative model for the white matter was characterized by both decreases and increases across adulthood. More specifically, parietal and temporal brain regions showed an increase with age, areas in frontal and in particular thalamic regions showed decreased, in both sexes. This is in line with earlier reports on healthy aging (Farokhian et al., 2017). In online Supplementary Fig. S2, we depict the mean deviation of the normative model across all ages separately for females and males.
Characterization of mean deviations from the normative model
Figure 2a shows the mean deviations from the normative model in the gray matter for healthy individuals and those with ADHD. Individuals with ADHD and healthy individuals differed significantly after correction for multiple comparisons in their mean deviations from the normative model in the cerebellum, temporal brain regions, and the hippocampus. Participants with ADHD on average showed larger mean negative deviations in those regions. Looking at Z-score maps thresholded at ±2.6, this pattern was confirmed, and additional regions showing negative mean deviations were observed in the anterior cingulate, insula, and frontal cortex (Fig. 2b). No differences in mean deviations between patients and controls were observed in white matter (online supplementary Fig. S3a), although some positive and negative mean deviations exceeded the z-score threshold of ±2.6 in patients: for instance, temporal brain regions showed positive deviations, while frontal and parietal regions showed negative deviations (online Supplementary Fig. S3b).
Fig. 2.
In (a), the contrast between persistent ADHD and healthy individuals is depicted corrected at a false discovery rate of 5%. Cerebellar regions, temporal regions, and the hippocampus deviate significantly in gray matter. In (b), the group-level mean deviations of participants with persistent ADHD and healthy individuals are depicted (|Z| < 2.6) and compared with the overlap maps of extreme negative deviations (Z < −2.6). In summary, while we reproduce prominent group-level differences between healthy individuals and participants with persistent ADHD, we observe that extreme negative deviations are hardly present in more than 2% of the individuals with persistent ADHD in those brain regions.
Association of extreme deviations from the normative model with persistent ADHD
An analysis of the total percentage of extreme negative deviations in gray matter across the groups showed that participants with persistent ADHD differed significantly from healthy individuals (Wald χ2(1) = 23.64, pcorr. < 0.001). This effect was driven by a larger percentage of negative deviations in participants with persistent ADHD (0.48%; 95% confidence interval 0.30–0.66%) than in healthy individuals (0.28%; 95% confidence interval 0.24–0.34%). In white matter, significant differences in the percentage of extreme negative deviations were observed between groups as well (Wald χ2(1) = 18.02, pcorr. < 0.001); again, a significantly higher proportion of negative deviations was seen in participants with persistent ADHD (0.41%; 95% confidence interval 0.24–0.57%) than in healthy individuals (0.24%; 95% confidence interval 0.17–0.31%). No differences between groups were observed in positive deviations on measures in gray and white matter (online Supplementary Table S1). As only the percentage of extreme negative deviations were significant between individuals with persistent ADHD and healthy participants we focus our characterizations of those deviations on extreme negative deviations in gray matter, we report the other extreme deviations in different modalities in the supplement but report the main outcomes in the section below as well.
Characterization of extreme negative deviations from the normative model
Participants with ADHD showed overlap in local gray matter negative deviations in more than 2% of patients primarily in the cerebellum, hippocampus, and basal ganglia; less overlap in negative deviations was observed in healthy individuals (Fig. 2b). In white matter, we also observed greater overlap in participants with ADHD than in healthy individuals, again involving regions around the hippocampus and the basal ganglia (online supplementary Fig. S3b). A scattered pattern of positive deviations was seen in the (online supplementary Fig. S4) overlap maps for participants with ADHD as well as for healthy individuals in both gray and white matter. The overlap maps of the extreme negative deviations partly resembled the pattern observed in the mean deviation analyses of cases and controls (Fig. 2b), also when detecting extreme deviation based on the FDR (online Supplementary Fig. S5). Further, nine out of the ten most negatively deviating patients showed extreme values in the cerebellum (Fig. 3), although in non-overlapping areas. Generally, deviations in both positive and negative directions were unique for each participant with ADHD in gray and white matter, when looking at the patterns of individual deviations, with limited overlap (online Supplementary Fig. S6). The extreme negative deviations were associated with age in participants with ADHD (β-weight = 0.198, p = 0.014), but not symptom scores, stimulant medication, or comorbidity, before correction for multiple comparisons (online Supplementary Table S1); for the extreme positive deviations, we did not find any associations that were even nominally significant.
Fig. 3.
The individual extreme negative deviations from the normative model in gray matter are depicted for participants with persistent ADHD. Below the corresponding overlap map in gray matter is depicted. In summary, individual extreme deviations show a very unique pattern across participants with persistent ADHD.
Discussion
We mapped the biological heterogeneity of persistent ADHD in reference to normative brain aging across the adult lifespan, based on voxel-based morphometry derived brain measures. In participants with ADHD, we observed robust mean deviations in gray matter from the normative model in the cerebellum, temporal regions, and the hippocampus. However, at the individual level, we found that few brain loci showed extreme negative deviations in more than 2% of the participants with ADHD, providing a measure for the (substantial) inter-individual variation between adults with persistent ADHD.
Case-control comparisons show small to medium effect sizes of (gray matter) alterations in adult ADHD patients (Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012; Ziegler et al., 2012; Hoogman et al., 2017). Here, we show that some of these differences between participants with ADHD and healthy individuals in normative gray matter deviations are consistent with these earlier case-control findings. Note that our approach differs as we modeled the healthy range prior to computing group-level differences on the basis of deviations from normative aging. That said, mean normative differences in hippocampus and temporal region overlap with regions that have earlier been identified in children with ADHD (Hoogman et al., 2017). In addition, we observed mean normative deviations in the cerebellum; a decreased gray matter was seen in individuals with ADHD across the adult lifespan. The cerebellum is of increasing interest in ADHD (Berquin et al., 1998): for example, in case-control studies, those with ADHD have shown a decreased size of the cerebellum (Carmona et al., 2005; Ivanov et al., 2014), which may be linked to timing problems that are present across many individuals with this disorder (Aase and Sagvolden, 2005). We do not observe a robust difference in the prefrontal cortex or basal ganglia, regions that have often been implicated in (childhood) ADHD (Faraone and Biederman, 1998; Frodl and Skokauskas, 2012). However, when reducing thresholding in the group-level maps (|Z| > 2.6), these regions do present reductions in gray matter volume also in the current study (Fig. 2).
Whilst the group-level results based on normative deviations described above are largely in line with existing ADHD literature and point to the cerebellum as an important structure in persistent ADHD, we additionally observe a large biological heterogeneity at the level of the brain. Specifically, we found that only a few individual brain loci showed extreme negative deviations in more than 2% of the participants with ADHD, providing quantitative evidence of the biological heterogeneity of persistent ADHD (Faraone et al., 2015) and showing that inter-individual differences at the level of brain structure are a hallmark for this phenotype. This is consistent with conceptual developments such as the Research Domain Criteria (Insel et al., 2010), which emphasize the importance of moving beyond simple group comparisons in psychiatry towards multilevel, high-dimensional descriptions of individual patients. Our finding that patients with persistent ADHD differ substantially on an individual level speaks against the concept of the ‘average ADHD patient’ and suggests that it does not sufficiently reflect the degree of inter-individual variation that characterizes this disorder. This may explain why case-control studies, which dominate research on ADHD and mental disorders in general, have shown small group differences between patients and healthy individuals (Franke et al., 2009; Hamshere et al., 2013; Onnink et al., 2014; Faraone et al., 2015; Greven et al., 2015; Wolfers et al., 2015b, 2017; Francx et al., 2016). We expect a high degree of inter-individual differences for other biological readouts (e.g. functional measures) but quantifying the degree and mapping the nature of such heterogeneity is an important topic of future research.
Voxel-based morphometry studies are fundamentally reductionist, comparing group differences on the voxel by voxel level, making strong assumptions on (i) a single locus contributing to a disorder and (ii) group homogeneity. This approach has been extended by pattern classification studies, which consider multiple voxels at once and show that using structural MRI the predictions of ADHD range from about 60% to up to about 90% accuracy indicating a high variability between studies (Bansal et al., 2012; Igual et al., 2012; The ADHD 200 consortium, 2012; Lim et al., 2013; Peng et al., 2013; Johnston et al., 2014; Wolfers et al., 2015a). A prime example, the ADHD-200 competition, in which ADHD was predicted on the basis of different brain read-outs, showed predictions that did not exceed 60% accuracy (The ADHD 200 consortium, 2012). These outcomes were replicated in follow-up research, summarized in different reviews and studies using all kinds of brain imaging readouts (Sabuncu and Konukoglu, 2014; Wolfers et al., 2015a, 2016, 2017). Here, we used mass-univariate predictions, similar to voxel-based morphometry. However, unlike this approach we did not assume homogenous groups of individuals with ADHD and healthy participants. While this assumption is fundamental in voxel-based morphometry, it is also essential for pattern classification approaches. The present results question this assumption.
The present results allow for a novel interpretation of earlier large-scale pattern recognition studies in ADHD, which often showed relatively low accuracy in discriminating ADHD cases from controls (Wolfers et al., 2015a). In larger studies, the predictive accuracy for ADHD is reduced relative to smaller studies, which is counterintuitive to the premises of general machine learning, where an increase in sample size usually improves learning from data (Hastie et al., 2009). This conundrum can be understood in the context of the present results, as larger, more representative samples capture more of the biological as well as procedural heterogeneity (e.g. due to different scanners sites) of this disorder. Therefore, a larger sample will provide a better estimate of the variation between individuals. This increases the difficulty to find a common decision function across participants with ADHD in pattern classification analyses. Note, however, that larger studies also deal, to a greater extent with for instance acquisition inhomogeneities across different scanners, which might affect predictions negatively, while smaller studies may be more carefully controllable, or just by chance select a more homogenous subgroup.
We are confident that the present results and the main conclusions are replicable in follow-up studies. However, a few limitations require a discussion. First, we had to use 10-fold cross-validation in healthy individuals and out of sample predictions in individuals with persistent ADHD, as our healthy sample was too small to split it into two. This potentially introduces a small bias. Second, we did not find associations of symptom scores with the percentage of deviation from the normative model. However, the measures we used to assess symptoms rely on self-report, which is generally noisier than measures from diagnostic interviews. Finally, our sample did not allow to inspect the effect of comorbidities and other potentially confounding factors on the obtained results as the comorbidities were inconsistent across individuals. In future studies, we envision that normative models are built on the basis of large population samples. These population-based normative models can subsequently be applied to cohorts that sample ADHD using the same inclusion criteria for healthy individuals as for those with a disorder. In this way normative modeling is complementary to classical case-control comparisons as it allows for the investigation of individual differences. Here, we show that an approach relying on case-control differences is not sufficient to understand ADHD and its biological heterogeneity.
In conclusion, while our group level effects are largely in line with existing literature on ADHD, our approach also shows that the disorder is a much more biologically heterogeneous on the individual level than previously anticipated. We thus need to move towards descriptions of biology for the individual patient to improve our understanding of ADHD. The present results provide the first quantitative estimate of the degree of biological heterogeneity, in terms of spatial overlap of an individual's extreme gray and white matter deviations, linked to ADHD. In this way, we provide valuable information to improve the nosology and characterization of the different facets of persistent ADHD.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank all the participants that took part in this study. This study makes use of the data of the Dutch node of the IMpACT cohort, which was supported by a grant from the Brain & Cognition Excellence Program and a Vici grant (to Barbara Franke) from NWO (grant numbers 433-09-229 and 016-130-669), by the Netherlands Brain Foundation [grant number, 15F07(2)27], and BBMRI-NL (grant CP2010-33). Andre Marquand gratefully acknowledges support from the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO) via a Vernieuwingsimpuls VIDI fellowship (grant number 016.156.415). Christian F Beckmann received funding from a personal VIDI NWO grant (864-12-003) as well as the Wellcome Trust UK Strategic Award (098369/Z/12/Z). The research leading to these results also received funding from the European Community's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) under grant agreements no. 602450 (IMAGEMEND), no. 602805 (Aggressotype), and no. 278948 (TACTICS). In addition, the project received funding from the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under grant agreements no. 643051 (Marie Sklodowska-Curie program; MiND) and no. 667302 (CoCA).
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000084.
Author ORCIDs
Thomas Wolfers, 0000-0002-7693-0621
Christian F. Beckmann, 0000-0002-3373-3193
Martine Hoogman, 0000-0002-1261-7628
Conflict of interest
Barbara Franke has received educational speaking fees from Shire and Medice. Jan K. Buitelaar has been a consultant to/member of advisory board of and/or speaker for Janssen Cilag BV, Eli Lilly, Medice, Roche, and Servier. He is not an employee of any of these companies. He is not a stock shareholder of any of these companies. He has no other financial or material support, including expert testimony, patents, or royalties. Christian F Beckmann is director and shareholder of SBGneuro Ltd. The other authors report no potential conflicts of interest.
Ethical standards
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008 and further that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional guides on the care and use of laboratory animals.
References
- Aase H and Sagvolden T (2005) Moment-to-moment dynamics of ADHD behaviour. Behavioral and Brain Functions 1, 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashburner J (2007) A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. NeuroImage 38, 95–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashburner J and Friston KJ (2000) Voxel-based morphometry - The methods. NeuroImage 11, 805–821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashburner J and Friston KJ (2005) Unified segmentation. NeuroImage 26, 839–851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bansal R, Staib LH, Laine AF, Hao X, Xu D, Liu J, Weissman M, Peterson BS and Zhan W (2012) Anatomical brain images alone can accurately diagnose chronic neuropsychiatric illnesses. PLoS ONE 7, 1–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y and Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society 57, 289–300. [Google Scholar]
- Berquin PC, Giedd JN, Jacobsen LK, Hamburger SD, Krain AL, Rapoport JL and Castellanos FX (1998) Cerebellum in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a morphometric MRI study. Neurology 50, 1087–1093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burmeister M, McInnis MG and Zöllner S (2008) Psychiatric genetics: progress amid controversy. Nature Reviews Genetics 9, 527–540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bush G, Valera EM and Seidman LJ (2005) Functional neuroimaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review and suggested future directions. Biological Psychiatry 57, 1273–1284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmona S, Vilarroya O, Bielsa A, Trèmols V, Soliva JC, Rovira M, Tomàs J, Raheb C, Gispert JD, Batlle S and Bulbena A (2005) Global and regional gray matter reductions in ADHD: a voxel-based morphometric study. Neuroscience Letters 389, 88–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cortese S and Castellanos FX (2012) Neuroimaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: current neuroscience-informed perspectives for clinicians. Current Psychiatry Reports 14, 568–578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fair DA, Nigg JT, Iyer S, Bathula D, Mills KL, Dosenbach NUF, Schlaggar BL, Mennes M, Gutman D, Bangaru S, Buitelaar JK, Dickstein DP, Di Martino A, Kennedy DN, Kelly C, Luna B, Schweitzer JB, Velanova K, Wang Y-F, Mostofsky S, Castellanos FX and Milham MP (2013) Distinct neural signatures detected for ADHD subtypes after controlling for micro-movements in resting state functional connectivity MRI data. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 6, 80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faraone SV and Biederman J (1998) Neurobiology of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 44, 951–958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faraone SV, Asherson P, Banaschewski T, Biederman J, Buitelaar JK, Ramos-Quiroga JA, Rohde LA, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Tannock R and Franke B (2015) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Reviews Disease Primers 1, 15020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farokhian F, Yang C, Beheshti I, Matsuda H and Wu S (2017) Age-Related gray and white matter changes in normal adult brains. Aging and Disease 8, 899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foulkes L and Blakemore SJ (2018) Studying individual differences in human adolescent brain development. Springer US Nature Neuroscience 21, 1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francx W, Llera A, Mennes M, Zwiers MP, Faraone SV, Oosterlaan J, Heslenfeld D, Hoekstra PJ, Hartman CA, Franke B, Buitelaar JK and Beckmann CF (2016) Integrated analysis of gray and white matter alterations in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical 11, 357–367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franke B, Neale BM and Faraone SV (2009) Genome-wide association studies in ADHD. Human Genetics 126, 13–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frodl T and Skokauskas N (2012) Meta-analysis of structural MRI studies in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder indicates treatment effects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 125, 114–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greven CU, Bralten J, Mennes M, O'Dwyer L, Van Hulzen KJE, Rommelse N, Schweren LJS, Hoekstra PJ, Hartman CA, Heslenfeld D, Oosterlaan J, Faraone SV, Franke B, Zwiers MP, Arias-Vasquez A and Buitelaar JK (2015) Developmentally stable whole-brain volume reductions and developmentally sensitive caudate and putamen volume alterations in those with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 490–499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamshere ML, Langley K, Martin J, Agha SS, Stergiakouli E, Anney RJL, Buitelaar J, Faraone SV, Lesch KP, Neale BM, Franke B, Sonuga-Barke E, Asherson P, Merwood A, Kuntsi J, Medland SE, Ripke S, Steinhausen HC, Freitag C, Reif A, Renner TJ, Romanos M, Romanos J, Warnke A, Meyer J, Palmason H, Vasquez AA, Lambregts-Rommelse N, Roeyers H, Biederman J, Doyle AE, Hakonarson H, Rothenberger A, Banaschewski T, Oades RD, McGough JJ, Kent L, Williams N, Owen MJ, Holmans P, O'Donovan MC and Thapar A (2013) High loading of polygenic risk for ADHD in children with comorbid aggression. American Journal of Psychiatry 170, 909–916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hastie T, Tibshirani R and Friedman J (2009) The Elements of Statistical Learning. 2nd Edn. vol. 27 Standford: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Hibar DP, Westlye LT, Doan NT, Jahanshad N, Cheung JW, Ching CRKK, Versace A, Bilderbeck AC, Uhlmann A, Mwangi B, Krämer B, Overs B, Hartberg CB, Abe C, Dima D, Grotegerd D, Sprooten E, Ben E, Jimenez E, Howells FM, Delvecchio G, Temmingh H, Starke J, Almeida JRCC, Goikolea JM, Houenou J, Beard LM, Rauer L, Abramovic L, Bonnin M, Ponteduro MF, Keil M, Rive MM, Yao N, Yalin N, Najt P, Rosa PG, Redlich R, Trost S, Hagenaars S, Fears SC, Alonso-Lana S, van Erp TGMM, Nickson T, Chaim-Avancini TM, Meier TB, Elvsashagen T, Haukvik UK, Lee WH, Schene AH, Lloyd AJ, Young AH, Nugent A, Dale AM, Pfennig A, McIntosh AM, Lafer B, Baune BT, Ekman CJ, Zarate CA, Bearden CE, Henry C, Simhandl C, McDonald C, Bourne C, Stein DJ, Wolf DH, Cannon DM, Glahn DC, Veltman DJ, Pomarol-Clotet E, Vieta E, Canales-Rodriguez EJ, Nery FG, Duran FLSS, Busatto GF, Roberts G, Pearlson GD, Goodwin GM, Kugel H, Whalley HC, Ruhe HG, Soares JC, Fullerton JM, Rybakowski JK, Savitz J, Chaim KT, Fatjó-Vilas M, Soeiro-de-Souza MG, Boks MP, Zanetti MV, Otaduy MCGG, Schaufelberger MS, Alda M, Ingvar M, Phillips ML, Kempton MJ, Bauer M, Landén M, Lawrence NS, van Haren NEM, Horn HR, Freimer NB, Gruber O, Schofield PR, Mitchell PB, Kahn RS, Lenroot R, Machado-Vieira R, Ophoff RA, Sarró S, Frangou S, Satterthwaite TD, Hajek T, Dannlowski U, Malt UF, Arolt V, Gattaz WF, Drevets WC, Caseras X, Agartz I, Thompson PM and Andreassen OA (2017) Cortical abnormalities in bipolar disorder: an MRI analysis of 6503 individuals from the ENIGMA bipolar disorder working group. Molecular Psychiatry 23, 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holm S (1979) A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics 6, 65–70. [Google Scholar]
- Hoogman M, Aarts E, Zwiers M, Slaats-Willemse D, Naber M, Onnink M, Cools R, Kan C, Buitelaar J and Franke B (2011) Nitric oxide synthase genotype modulation of impulsivity and ventral striatal activity in adult ADHD patients and healthy comparison subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 1099–1106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoogman M, Bralten J, Hibar DP, Mennes M, Zwiers MP, Schweren LSJ, van Hulzen KJE, Medland SE, Shumskaya E, Jahanshad N, de Zeeuw P, Szekely E, Sudre G, Wolfers T, Onnink AMH, Dammers JT, Mostert JC, Vives-Gilabert Y, Kohls G, Oberwelland E, Seitz J, Schulte-Rüther M, Ambrosino S, Doyle AE, Høvik MF, Dramsdahl M, Tamm L, van Erp TGM, Dale A, Schork A, Conzelmann A, Zierhut K, Baur R, McCarthy H, Yoncheva YN, Cubillo A, Chantiluke K, Mehta MA, Paloyelis Y, Hohmann S, Baumeister S, Bramati I, Mattos P, Tovar-Moll F, Douglas P, Banaschewski T, Brandeis D, Kuntsi J, Asherson P, Rubia K, Kelly C, Di Martino A, Milham MP, Castellanos FX, Frodl T, Zentis M, Lesch KP, Reif A, Pauli P, Jernigan TL, Haavik J, Plessen KJ, Lundervold AJ, Hugdahl K, Seidman LJ, Biederman J, Rommelse N, Heslenfeld DJ, Hartman CA, Hoekstra PJ, Oosterlaan J, von Polier G, Konrad K, Vilarroya O, Ramos-Quiroga JA, Soliva JC, Durston S, Buitelaar JK, Faraone SV, Shaw P, Thompson PM and Franke B (2017) Subcortical brain volume differences in participants with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adults: a cross-sectional mega-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry 4, 310–319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Igual L, Soliva JC, Escalera S, Gimeno R, Vilarroya O and Radeva P (2012) Automatic brain caudate nuclei segmentation and classification in diagnostic of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Computerized Medical Imaging and Graphics 36, 591–600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel TR (2009) Disruptive insights in psychiatry: transforming a clinical discipline. Journal of Clinical Investigation 119, 700–705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K, Sanislow C and Wang P (2010) Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 748–751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivanov I, Murrough JW, Bansal R, Hao X and Peterson BS (2014) Cerebellar morphology and the effects of stimulant medications in youths with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 718–726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston BA, Mwangi B, Matthews K, Coghill D, Konrad K and Steele JD (2014) Brainstem abnormalities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder support high accuracy individual diagnostic classification. Human Brain Mapping 35, 5179–5189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim L, Marquand A, Cubillo AA, Smith AB, Chantiluke K, Simmons A, Mehta M and Rubia K (2013) Disorder-Specific predictive classification of adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) relative to autism using structural magnetic resonance imaging. PLoS ONE 8, e63660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobbestael J, Leurgans M and Arntz A (2011) Inter-rater reliability of the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders (SCID I) and axis II disorders (SCID II). Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy 18, 75–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marquand AF, Rezek I, Buitelaar J and Beckmann CF (2016a) Understanding heterogeneity in clinical cohorts using normative models: beyond case-control studies. Biological Psychiatry 80, 552–561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marquand AF, Wolfers T, Mennes M, Buitelaar J and Beckmann CF (2016b) Beyond lumping and splitting: a review of computational approaches for stratifying psychiatric disorders. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging 1, 433–447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin J, Walters RK, Demontis D, Mattheisen M, Lee SH, Robinson E, Brikell I, Ghirardi L, Larsson H, Lichtenstein P, Eriksson N, Werge T, Mortensen PB, Pedersen MG, Mors O, Nordentoft M, Hougaard DM, Bybjerg-Grauholm J, Wray NR, Franke B, Faraone SV, O'Donovan MC, Thapar A, Børglum AD and Neale BM (2018) A genetic investigation of sex bias in the prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 83, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mostert JC, Hoogman M, Onnink AMH, van Rooij D, von Rhein D, van Hulzen KJE, Dammers J, Kan CC, Buitelaar JK, Norris DG and Franke B (2015) Similar subgroups based on cognitive performance parse heterogeneity in adults with ADHD and healthy controls. Journal of Attention Disorders 22, 281–292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norman LJ, Carlisi C, Lukito S, Hart H, Mataix-Cols D, Radua J and Rubia K (2016) Structural and functional brain abnormalities in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparative meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 815–825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Onnink AMH, Zwiers MP, Hoogman M, Mostert JC, Kan CC, Buitelaar J and Franke B (2014) Brain alterations in adult ADHD: effects of gender, treatment and comorbid depression. European Neuropsychopharmacology 24, 397–409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pappas D (2006) Test reviews: aDHD rating scale-IV: checklists, norms, and clinical interpretation. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 24, 172–178. [Google Scholar]
- Peng X, Lin P, Zhang T and Wang J (2013) Extreme learning machine-based classification of ADHD using brain structural MRI data. PLoS ONE 8, 2–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rasmussen CE and Williams CKI (2006) Model selection and adaptation of hyperparameters. Gaussian Processes for Machine Learning (Adaptive Computation and Machine Learning series) 4, 105–128. [Google Scholar]
- Sabuncu MR and Konukoglu E (2014) Clinical prediction from structural brain MRI scans: a large-scale empirical study. Neuroinformatics 13, 31–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandra Kooij JJ, Marije Boonstra A, Swinkels SHN, Bekker EM, De Noord I and Buitelaar JK (2008) Reliability, validity, and utility of instruments for self-report and informant report concerning symptoms of ADHD in adult patients. Journal of Attention Disorders 11, 445–458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmaal L, Veltman DJ, van Erp TGM, Sämann PG, Frodl T, Jahanshad N, Loehrer E, Tiemeier H, Hofman A, Niessen WJ, Vernooij MW, Ikram MA, Wittfeld K, Grabe HJ, Block A, Hegenscheid K, Völzke H, Hoehn D, Czisch M, Lagopoulos J, Hatton SN, Hickie IB, Goya-Maldonado R, Krämer B, Gruber O, Couvy-Duchesne B, Rentería ME, Strike LT, Mills NT, de Zubicaray GI, McMahon KL, Medland SE, Martin NG, Gillespie NA, Wright MJ, Hall GB, MacQueen GM, Frey EM, Carballedo A, van Velzen LS, van Tol MJ, van der Wee NJ, Veer IM, Walter H, Schnell K, Schramm E, Normann C, Schoepf D, Konrad C, Zurowski B, Nickson T, McIntosh AM, Papmeyer M, Whalley HC, Sussmann JE, Godlewska BR, Cowen PJ, Fischer FH, Rose M, Penninx BWJH, Thompson PM and Hibar DP (2016) Subcortical brain alterations in major depressive disorder: findings from the ENIGMA Major depressive disorder working group. Molecular Psychiatry 21, 806–812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schumann G, Binder EB, Holte A, de Kloet ER, Oedegaard KJ, Robbins TW, Walker-Tilley TR, Bitter I, Brown VJ, Buitelaar J, Ciccocioppo R, Cools R, Escera C, Fleischhacker W, Flor H, Frith CD, Heinz A, Johnsen E, Kirschbaum C, Klingberg T, Lesch KP, Lewis S, Maier W, Mann K, Martinot JL, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Müller CP, Müller WE, Nutt DJ, Persico A, Perugi G, Pessiglione M, Preuss UW, Roiser JP, Rossini PM, Rybakowski JK, Sandi C, Stephan KE, Undurraga J, Vieta E, van der Wee N, Wykes T, Haro JM and Wittchen HU (2014) Stratified medicine for mental disorders. European Neuropsychopharmacology 24, 5–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seghier ML and Price CJ (2018) Interpreting and utilising intersubject variability in brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22, 517–530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seidman LJ, Valera EM and Makris N (2005) Structural brain imaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 57, 1263–1272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw P, Shaw P, Lerch J, Lerch J, Greenstein D, Greenstein D, Sharp W, Sharp W, Clasen L, Clasen L, Evans A, Evans A, Giedd J, Giedd J, Castellanos FX, Castellanos FX, Rapoport J and Rapoport J (2006) Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 63, 540–549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, Blumenthal J, Lerch JP, Greenstein D, Clasen L, Evans A, Giedd J and Rapoport JL (2007) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104, 19649–19654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon V, Czobor P, Balint S, Meszaros A and Bitter I (2009) Prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry 194, 204–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan PF, Daly MJ and O'Donovan M (2012) Genetic architectures of psychiatric disorders: the emerging picture and its implications. Nature Reviews Genetics 13, 537–551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- The ADHD 200 consortium (2012) The ADHD-200 consortium: a model to advance the translational potential of neuroimaging in clinical neuroscience. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 6, 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valera EM, Faraone SV, Murray KE and Seidman LJ (2007) Meta-Analysis of structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 61, 1361–1369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Ewijk H, Heslenfeld DJ, Zwiers MP, Buitelaar JK and Oosterlaan J (2012) Diffusion tensor imaging in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36, 1093–1106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Groenestijn MAC, Akkerhuis GW, Kupka RW, Schneider N and Nolen WA (1999) Gestructureerd Klinisch Interview Voor de Vaststelling van DSM-IV As 1 Stoornissen [Dutch Translation of the SCID-I/P]. Lisse, the Netherlands: Zwets & Zeitlinger. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. [Google Scholar]
- van Rooij D, Hartman CA, Mennes M, Oosterlaan J, Franke B, Rommelse N, Heslenfeld D, Faraone SV, Buitelaar JK and Hoekstra PJ (2015) Altered neural connectivity during response inhibition in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings. NeuroImage: Clinical 7, 325–335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D (2012) Wais-III Nederlandstalige Bewerking. Technische Handleiding. London: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Weertman A, Arntz A, Dreessen L, van Velzen C and Vertommen S (2003) Short-interval test-retest interrater reliability of the Dutch version of the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV personality disorders (SCID-II). Journal of Personality Disorders 17, 562–567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkler AM, Webster MA, Vidaurre D, Nichols TE and Smith SM (2015) Multi-level block permutation. NeuroImage 123, 253–268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfers T, Buitelaar JK, Beckmann CF, Franke B and Marquand AF (2015a) From estimating activation locality to predicting disorder: a review of pattern recognition for neuroimaging-based psychiatric diagnostics. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 57, 328–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfers T, Onnink AMH, Zwiers MP, Arias-Vasquez A, Hoogman M, Mostert JC, Kan CC, Slaats-Willemse D, Buitelaar JK and Franke B (2015b) Lower white matter microstructure in the superior longitudinal fasciculus is associated with increased response time variability in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 40, 344–351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfers T, van Rooij D, Oosterlaan J, Heslenfeld D, Hartman CA, Hoekstra PJ, Beckmann CF, Franke B, Buitelaar JK and Marquand AF (2016) Quantifying patterns of brain activity: distinguishing unaffected siblings from participants with ADHD and healthy individuals. NeuroImage: Clinical 12, 227–233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfers T, Llera A, Onnink AMH, Dammers J, Hoogman M, Zwiers MP, Buitelaar JK, Franke B, Marquand AF and Beckmann CF (2017) Refinement by integration: aggregated effects of multimodal imaging markers on adult ADHD. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 42, 386–394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ziegler G, Dahnke R, Lutz J, Yotter RA and Gaser C (2012) Brain structural trajectories over the adult lifespan. Human Brain Mapping 33, 2377–2389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]