Abstract
We aimed to examine the impact of claims, fruit images, and health warnings on consumers’ perceptions of fruit-flavored drinks with added sugar (i.e., “fruit drinks”). We conducted three 2×2×2 randomized experiments with online convenience samples of U.S. adults (Study 1 n=2,139 in 2018, current e-cigarette users and smokers; Study 2 n=670 in 2018, current e-cigarette users; Study 3 n=1,001 in 2019, general sample). Participants viewed a fruit drink that differed in the presence of a “100% Vitamin C” nutrition claim, a fruit image, or a health warning. On average across the three studies, consumers who saw a claim on a fruit drink believed that the drink was more healthful than those who did not see the claim (mean average differential effect (ADE)=.66, p<.001); they were also more interested in consuming the drink (mean ADE=.38, p=.001). The health warning decreased perceived product healthfulness (mean ADE=−.65, p<.001) and consumption interest (mean ADE=−.49, p<.001). The fruit image had no effect on perceived product healthfulness (mean ADE=.03, p=.81) or purchase intentions (mean ADE=−.04, p=.77). In Study 1 and Study 2, there were no interactions between claims, images, or warnings (all p>.05). In Study 3, the “100% Vitamin C” nutrition claim only increased perceived product healthfulness when the drink did not also have a health warning (interaction p<.05). These findings suggest that 100% Vitamin C claims increase the appeal of fruit drinks, whereas health warnings decrease the appeal. Together, these studies support policies to restrict marketing and require health warnings on sugar-sweetened beverage packaging.
Keywords: Fruit drinks, sugar-sweetened beverages, health warnings, warning labels, front of package labels, claims, nutrition claims, marketing, product packaging
INTRODUCTION
Sugar-sweetened beverage (SSB) consumption is a major driver of the obesity epidemic among both children and adults (1, 2). Fruit-flavored drinks with added sugar (hereinafter referred to as “fruit drinks”) are the most popular SSB besides soda among adults and are by far the most consumed SSB among children (3). Thus, strategies to reduce fruit drink intake could reduce overall SSB consumption, and subsequently could help to prevent obesity and its associated health consequences.
One promising policy strategy for reducing SSB intake is restricting marketing cues, including nutrient content claims and images, on SSB packaging. Nutrient content claims (i.e., text statements characterizing the level of a nutrient such as “low fat”) are a highly prevalent marketing claim used on beverage packages, and are regulated by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) (4). A 2014 report found that fruit drink packages in the U.S. contain an average of 4.3 different claims (5). Nutrient content claims may lead consumers to mistakenly infer that a product is healthful, regardless of its overall nutritional profile (i.e., the “health halo effect”), and can subsequently increase intentions to purchase the product (6–10). More research is needed to elucidate the impact of claims on interest in fruit drinks in particular, given that consumers already incorrectly believe that fruit drinks are healthier than other types of SSBs like soda (11, 12).
In addition to nutrient content claims, images of fruits are another common marketing element on food and fruit drink packaging (13, 14), even when these products do not contain fruit or are not 100% fruit juice. A large body of research has shown how cartoon characters on product packaging increase consumer preferences and intake (15, 16), but less is known about other features such as fruit images.
A second promising strategy for reducing SSB intake is requiring health warnings on the front of SSB packages. Lawmakers in five U.S. states have proposed laws requiring health warnings to appear on SSBs, and FDA has demonstrated an interest in stronger nutrition labels by commissioning two reviews of front-of-package labels (17, 18). Several online studies have found that SSB health warnings affect key antecedents to behavior change, such as visual attention (19, 20) and intentions to purchase SSBs (11, 21–24). One recent randomized trial found that health warnings placed on SSB containers reduced actual SSB purchases (25). However, it remains unknown whether SSB health warnings interact with marketing elements. Tobacco research suggests that removing marketing elements, including claims and imagery, from cigarette packages may heighten the impact of health warnings (26–28), suggesting that the effectiveness of health warnings could be reduced by marketing elements. It is also possible that health warnings could weaken the effect of marketing elements or that the effects of warnings and marketing elements might “cancel out” one another. These questions merit further exploration in the context of SSB packing.
The current studies aimed to fill key gaps in the literature by studying two marketing cues (i.e., nutrient content claims and fruit images) used on fruit drinks and their possible interaction with health warnings. Specifically, in three experiment replications, we examined the impact of front-of-package nutrient content claims, fruit images, and health warnings on perceived product healthfulness and interest in consuming fruit drinks, two constructs theorized to predict behavior change (29, 30). Based on prior research (6–11, 21, 23–25, 31–33), we predicted that nutrient content claims and fruit images would lead to greater perceived product healthfulness and consumption interest whereas health warnings would lead to lower perceived product healthfulness and consumption interest. We also predicted that the effect of nutrient content claims and fruit images would weaken the health warnings.
METHODS
Participants
For Study 1, in August 2018, we recruited a convenience sample of U.S. adults, ages 18 or older to participate in an online experiment. Online convenience samples can yield highly generalizable findings for experiments (34). Participants were recruited for an e-cigarette study to examine vaping prevention (35). Thus, participants were eligible if they were current e-cigarette users (defined as currently vaping every day or some days), current smokers (defined as having smoked at least 100 cigarettes and now smoking every day or some days), or dual users (defined as both current smokers and current e-cigarette users). Recruitment occurred through Amazon’s Prime Panels, a platform with access to over 20 million participants for behavioral research. For Study 2, we recruited a convenience sample of U.S. adults in December 2018 through Amazon’s Prime Panels to participate in an online experiment. The main purpose of this e-cigarette study was to develop a new measure about e-cigarette use, so participants were eligible if they were current e-cigarette users (as defined in Study 1). For Study 3, we recruited a convenience sample of U.S. adults (including 65% non-tobacco users) using Amazon Mechanical Turk in December 2019. The main purpose of this study was to replicate the current experiment with a general sample, so the study was open to all adults ages 18 and older residing in the U.S.
Procedures
The procedures were identical for all three studies. We conducted a between-subjects 2×2×2 experiment, representing eight different conditions (Figure 1). The three experimental factors were nutrient content claim (no claim vs. “100% Vitamin C,” a prevalent claim on SSBs, particularly fruit drinks (5)), image of fruit (no image vs. image of oranges), and health warning (no warning vs. “WARNING: Beverages with added sugar contribute to tooth decay, diabetes, and obesity.”). We selected this warning because previous studies have shown that it increases perceived message effectiveness (24) and reduces SSB purchases (25) compared to control labels. The experimental factors were displayed on an image of an orange fruit drink, created by a professional designer. To control for established brand preferences, the image displayed the logo of a Japanese food company (i.e., “Nissin”) that has the realism of an actual brand but would be unfamiliar to most U.S. consumers (36).
Figure 1.
Stimuli used in Studies 1, 2, and 3
After providing informed consent, participants in the three studies completed an online survey primarily about e-cigarettes. Participants viewed a prompt that read: “On the next page, you’ll see a sweetened fruit drink (not 100% juice). Then you’ll answer some questions.” Participants were then randomly assigned to view one of the eight experimental conditions and answered survey items. Participants in Study 1 and Study 2 received incentives in cash, gift cards, or reward points from Amazon’s Prime Panels. Participants in Study 3 received an incentive of $1.40. The University of North Carolina institutional review board approved these studies. Prior to data collection, we pre-registered Study 1 on AsPredicted.org: http://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=543ti3. Studies 2 and 3 were replications of Study 1, with identical predictions and procedures, so we did not pre-register those studies.
Measures
The measures were identical across the three studies. We assessed perceived product healthfulness with an item adapted from a previous study (22) that read: “How healthy or unhealthy would it be for you to drink this product every day?” Response options were on a seven-point scale ranging from “unhealthy” (coded as 1) to “healthy” (coded as 7). We measured consumption interest using an item adapted from a prior study (11) that read: “How interested would you be in drinking this product in the next 30 days, if it were available?” Response options were on a seven-point scale ranging from “not at all interested” (coded as 1) to “extremely interested” (coded as 7). Participants then answered standard demographic items such as age, race, and ethnicity. We assessed frequency of fruit drink consumption with an item adapted from a standardized beverage intake questionnaire (37) that read: “How often did you drink sweetened fruit drinks (not 100% juice) like lemonade, fruit punch, Capri Sun, or Oceanspray juice cocktail in the past 30 days?”
Data analysis
Analyses used Stata/SE version 14.1 with two-tailed tests and a critical alpha of 0.05. The analytic samples included participants with complete data on both outcomes (n=2,139 for Study 1, n=670 for Study 2, n=1,006 for Study 3). The analytic procedures were identical for the three studies. We first examined whether randomization created equivalent groups using chi-squared tests for categorical variables and t-tests for continuous variables. In all three studies, sexual orientation was not equally distributed across the eight conditions (p=0.03 for Study 1, p=0.003 for Study 2, p=0.02 for Study 3) and low-income status was not equally distributed in Study 3 (p=0.02). Unadjusted analyses revealed an identical pattern of findings compared to the adjusted findings, so we report only the unadjusted findings per the CONSORT guidelines for randomized trials (38).
We ran two linear regression models for each study, one with perceived product healthfulness as the outcome and one with consumption interest as the outcome. The initial models included indicators for the three experimental factors and all interactions between these three factors. The final models for all studies retained only the significant interactions. Due to skewness of the residuals of the outcomes (most Shapiro-Wilk tests (39) of residuals indicated significant skewness), we conducted sensitivity analysis with ordinal regression models; the pattern of findings was identical so we report linear models for ease of interpretation. We report average differential effects of each experimental factor on the outcomes as generated by the final models. We probed interactions by calculating means at different levels of the moderating factors. To synthesize the findings across the three studies, we estimated average effects by taking the unweighted mean of the point estimates for each study. We then obtained a standard error and t-statistic for each of these average effects. Finally, to put the effects into a common metric, we calculated a standardized effect size (Cohen’s d) with 95% confidence intervals.
RESULTS
Most (81%) Study 1 participants were White, 61% had not completed college, nearly half (47%) were low income, and over half (56%) consumed fruit drinks at least once a week (Table 1). Similarly, 76% of Study 2 participants were White, 61% had not completed college, 48% were low income, and 63% consumed fruit drinks at least once a week. Study 3 participants were 77% White, 31% had not completed college, 35% were low income, and about half (46%) consumed fruit drinks at least once a week.
Table 1.
Participant characteristics
Study 1 (n=2,139) | Study 2 (n=670) | Study 3 (n=1,006) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | % | n | % | n | % | |
Age in years | ||||||
Mean (SD) | 43 | (15) | 37 | (13) | 39 | (12) |
Range | [18–85] | [18–82] | [20–76] | |||
Gender | ||||||
Female | 1,187 | 55.5 | 343 | 51.2 | 458 | 45.9 |
Male | 952 | 44.5 | 327 | 48.8 | 537 | 53.8 |
Gay, lesbian, or bisexual | 207 | 9.7 | 80 | 11.9 | 120 | 12.1 |
Hispanic | 211 | 9.9 | 65 | 9.9 | 138 | 13.7 |
Race | ||||||
American Indian or Alaskan Native | 41 | 2.0 | 6 | 0.9 | 7 | 0.7 |
Asian | 79 | 3.8 | 33 | 5.1 | 62 | 6.3 |
Black or African American | 216 | 10.3 | 80 | 12.3 | 137 | 13.8 |
Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander | 10 | 0.5 | 1 | 0.2 | 4 | 0.4 |
White | 1,703 | 80.8 | 492 | 75.5 | 760 | 76.6 |
Other | 58 | 2.8 | 40 | 6.1 | 22 | 2.2 |
Education | ||||||
High school graduate or less | 650 | 30.4 | 189 | 28.2 | 113 | 11.3 |
Some college | 662 | 31.0 | 222 | 33.1 | 192 | 19.2 |
College graduate or associates degree | 682 | 32.0 | 218 | 32.5 | 549 | 55.0 |
Graduate degree | 142 | 6.6 | 41 | 6.1 | 145 | 14.5 |
Household income, annual | ||||||
$0-$24,999 | 562 | 26.3 | 173 | 25.8 | 158 | 15.7 |
$25,000–$49,999 | 697 | 32.6 | 213 | 31.8 | 325 | 32.3 |
$50,000–$74,999 | 427 | 20.0 | 123 | 18.4 | 250 | 24.9 |
$75,000+ | 453 | 21.2 | 161 | 24.0 | 273 | 27.1 |
Low income1 | 1,102 | 47.4 | 320 | 48.1 | 349 | 35.0 |
1+ child ages 0–18 in household | -- | -- | -- | -- | 427 | 42.5 |
Tobacco use | ||||||
Current e-cigarette user only | 432 | 20.2 | 302 | 45.1 | 81 | 8.1 |
Current smoker only | 833 | 38.9 | 0 | 0.0 | 135 | 13.5 |
Dual e-cigarette user and smoker | 874 | 40.9 | 368 | 54.9 | 130 | 13.0 |
Not current e-cigarette user or smoker | 0 | 0.0 | 0 | 0.0 | 653 | 65.4 |
Frequency of fruit drink consumption | ||||||
Never | 444 | 20.8 | 120 | 17.9 | 315 | 31.3 |
<1 time per week | 504 | 23.6 | 131 | 19.6 | 226 | 22.5 |
1–6 times per week | 936 | 43.8 | 325 | 48.5 | 359 | 35.7 |
1+ times per day | 255 | 11.9 | 94 | 14.0 | 106 | 10.5 |
Note. Missing demographic data range from 0% to 2.7%. -- = not assessed.
<200% of 2018 federal poverty level
Participants exposed to the “100% Vitamin C” claim rated the fruit drink as more healthful than those who did not see the claim in all three studies (mean average differential effect (ADE)=.66, all p<.001). On average across the three studies, the “100% Vitamin C” claim led to higher consumption intentions (mean ADE=.38, p=.001), despite differences among studies. In Study 1 and Study 2, participants who viewed the “100% Vitamin C” claim were more interested in consuming the fruit drink (Study 1 ADE=.49, p<.001; Study 2 ADE=43, p=.006), but the effect was not significant at the 0.05 level in Study 3 (ADE=.21, p=.09).
On average across the three studies, the fruit image did not change perceived product healthfulness (mean ADE=.03, p=.81) or purchase intentions (mean ADE=−.04, p=.77). In Studies 1 and 2, the fruit image had no effect on perceived product healthfulness (Study 1 ADE=−.03, p=.67; Study 2 ADE=−.15, p=.29) or purchase intentions (Study 1 ADE=−.10, p=.27; Study 2 ADE=−.22, p=.16). In Study 3, however, the fruit image led to greater perceived product healthfulness (Study 3 ADE=.26, p=.014) but not consumption interest ADE=.21, p=.09).
In all three studies, health warnings led to lower perceived healthfulness of the fruit drink (mean ADE=−.65, all p<.001). Health warnings also led to lower interest in consuming the fruit drink in all three studies (mean ADE=−.49, all p<.05).
In Study 1 and Study 2, there were no interactions between claims, images, or warnings (all p>.05). In Study 3, there was an interaction between the “100% Vitamin C” claim and the health warning on perceived product healthfulness of fruit drink (interaction p=.009), such that “100% Vitamin C” only increased perceived product healthfulness without a health warning (ADE of claim without warning=.79, p<.001; ADE of claim with warning=.21, p=.10).
DISCUSSION
In three experiments with U.S. adults (two studies with current tobacco users and one with a majority of non-users of tobacco), participants who saw a “100% Vitamin C” claim on a fruit drink believed that the fruit drink was more healthful than those who did not see the claim. Participants who saw the claim were also more interested in consuming the fruit drink. These findings build on prior research that has found that health-related and nutrient content claims make food and beverages seem healthier and more appealing (6–10, 32). The observed findings are particularly concerning given high rates of consumption of fruit drinks in the U.S., particularly Black and Latino youth (40, 41). In light of these findings and prior research, FDA should be concerned that nutrient content claims can cause consumers to mistakenly think SSBs are healthy. If future research continues to show similar types of misconceptions among consumers, policymakers should consider taking regulatory action to prevent claims from causing consumer deception. One option for regulation could include requiring a disclosure statement on fruit drinks. However, many studies have shown that disclosures are generally ineffective and do not correct misperceptions (42–44). A likely more effective regulatory action would be banning the use of specific misleading claims (similar to a 2010 ban of the use of misleading terms such “light” on cigarette packaging).
In all three experiments, participants who saw a health warning on a fruit drink believed that the fruit drink was less healthful than those who did not see the warning. Likewise, participants in all three studies who saw the health warning were less interested in consuming the fruit drink. These findings build on a growing body of research demonstrating the promise of front-of-package warnings on SSBs (11, 21, 23–25), and provide additional data to policymakers in the U.S. and globally considering SSB warning policies. Based on prior tobacco studies (26–28), we expected that the effect of health warnings would be weakened by claims and fruit images. We generally did not observe these hypothesized interactions. However, in Study 3, we found that the impact of the claim on perceived product healthfulness disappeared in the presence of a health warning. In other words, in this study, health warnings did appear to weaken the effect of the claim on perceptions of the fruit drink’s healthfulness. Additional research will clarify the extent to which health warnings, including pictorial warnings which are more effective than text warnings (45–47), might reduce the effects of claims.
The presence of a fruit image on a fruit drink did not change perceptions of product healthfulness or consumption interest. The one exception to this overall pattern of null findings was that the fruit image increased perceived product healthfulness among a combined sample of smokers and non-smokers in Study 3. This overall pattern of null findings stands in contrast with qualitative studies that found that fruit images on product packaging can lead parents (48) and children (49) to believe that products are more healthful. Our findings build on one prior study that concluded that including a fruit image on granola bar packaging did not change perceived product healthfulness (50). One possible explanation for our null findings could be that selected image of oranges did not attract attention given that it matched the background color of the product. Previous research has also found that color alone influences perceptions of sweetness, taste, and quality (51–53), so the orange fruit image and the orange-colored background may have been conveying redundant information to consumers. Future studies should consider testing images of multiple types of fruit as well as images with varied levels of realism (e.g., cartoonlike images of fruit).
Strengths of this study include the exact replication of stimuli and measures in three samples and the use of randomized factorial experiments. We largely observed the same pattern of findings across the three studies, but observed some differences noted above. The differences across studies could be due either to random chance or due to differences in the samples. One limitation is that two of the studies (but not the third) involved convenience samples of solely tobacco product users, meaning the generalizability of the findings to other populations remains to be established. However, online convenience samples tend to provide valid results for experiments, accurately estimating the impact of manipulated variables (54–56). Future studies with representative samples could extend the generalizability of these findings and could also help to better understand individual factors that influence perceptions of fruit drinks. Moreover, the sample includes only adults so the effects on children remains unknown. We only tested one type of drink with a mock brand; examining drinks that are not known to have high Vitamin C content, or real brands, may have yielded different results. Finally, the studies used a brief exposure to the study stimuli on a computer screen and assessed non-behavioral outcomes using self-report.
Conclusions
Our studies found that a “100% Vitamin C” claim increased consumers’ beliefs that sugar-sweetened fruit drinks are healthful, providing evidence to policymakers considering restricting marketing elements on SSB packaging. We also found that the presence of a health warning lowered consumers’ beliefs that sugar-sweetened fruit drinks are healthful and reduced their interest in the drinks, supporting health warnings as a policy for reducing SSB consumption. Future studies should examine the effects of claims, imagery, and warnings in naturalistic settings using objective outcomes such as purchasing or consumption behavior.
Supplementary Material
Figure 2.
Interaction between “100% Vitamin C” claim and health warning on perceived product healthfulness of fruit drink, Study 3 (n=1,006)
Table 2.
Effects of “100% Vitamin C” claim, fruit image, and health warning on perceived product healthfulness and consumption interest, in Study 1 (n=2,149), Study 2 (n=670), Study 3 (n=1,006), and overall (n=3,825)
Perceived product healthfulness | Consumption interest | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ADE | SE | p | ADE | SE | p | |
“100% Vitamin C” claim | ||||||
Study 1 | .75 | .08 | <.001 | .49 | .09 | <.001 |
Study 2 | .72 | .14 | <.001 | .43 | .16 | .006 |
Study 3 | .51 | .10 | <.001 | .21 | .12 | .09 |
Average | .66 | .11 | <.001 | .38 | .13 | .001 |
Fruit image | ||||||
Study 1 | −.03 | .08 | .67 | −.10 | .09 | .27 |
Study 2 | −.15 | .14 | .29 | −.22 | .16 | .16 |
Study 3 | .26 | .10 | .014 | .21 | .12 | .09 |
Average | .03 | .11 | .81 | −.04 | .13 | .77 |
Health warning | ||||||
Study 1 | −.94 | .08 | <.001 | −.60 | .09 | <.001 |
Study 2 | −.57 | .14 | <.001 | −.53 | .16 | .001 |
Study 3 | −.44 | .10 | <.001 | −.35 | .12 | .005 |
Average | −.65 | .11 | <.001 | −.49 | .13 | <.001 |
Note. ADE=average differential effect from linear regression models. SE=standard error. Bold indicates statistically significant findings at p<.05 level.
HIGHLIGHTS.
Sugar-sweetened fruit drinks are unhealthy but commonly consumed by children.
In our study, nutrition claims made people think fruit drinks were healthy.
Health warnings helped people understand that fruit drinks were unhealthy.
Acknowledgments
FUNDING
Research reported in this publication was supported by grant number P50CA180907 from the National Cancer Institute, FDA Center for Tobacco Products (CTP), and NIH grant to the Carolina Population Center, grant numbers P2C HD050924 and T32 HD007168. T32-CA057726 from the National Cancer Institute of the National Institutes of Health and K01HL147713 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute of the National Institutes of Health supported Marissa Hall’s time writing the paper. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH or the Food and Drug Administration.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest statement: The authors have no conflicts to declare.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
REFERENCES
- 1.Malik VS, Popkin BM, Bray GA, Despres JP, Hu FB. Sugar-sweetened beverages,obesity, type 2 diabetes mellitus, and cardiovascular disease risk. Circulation 2010;121(11):1356–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Morenga LT, Mallard S, Mann J. Dietary sugars and body weight: systematic review and meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials and cohort studies. BMJ 2013;346:e7492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kay MC, Welker EB, Jacquier EF, Story MT. Beverage Consumption Patterns among Infants and Young Children (0(−)47.9 Months): Data from the Feeding Infants and Toddlers Study, 2016. Nutrients 2018;10(7). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Taillie LS, Ng SW, Xue Y, Busey E, Harding M. No Fat, No Sugar, No Salt . . . No Problem? Prevalence of “Low-Content” Nutrient Claims and Their Associations with the Nutritional Profile of Food and Beverage Purchases in the United States. J Acad Nutr Diet 2017;117(9):1366–1374.e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Harris J, Schwartz M, LoDolce M, Munsell C, Fleming-Milici F, Elsey J, et al. Sugary drink FACTS 2014: Some progress but much room for improvement in marketing to youth. Rudd Center for Food Policy and Obesity: New Haven, CT: 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Roe B, Levy AS, Derby BM. The impact of health claims on consumer search and product evaluation outcomes: results from FDA experimental data. J Public Policy Mark 1999:89–105. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Choi H, Yoo K, Hyun Baek T, Reid LN, Macias W. Presence and effects of health and nutrition-related (HNR) claims with benefit-seeking and risk-avoidance appeals in female-orientated magazine food advertisements. Int J Advert 2013;32(4):587–616. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Schuldt JP, Schwarz N. The” organic” path to obesity? Organic claims influence calorie judgments and exercise recommendations. Judgm Decis Mak 2010;5(3):144. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kaur A, Scarborough P, Rayner M. A systematic review, and meta-analyses, of the impact of health-related claims on dietary choices. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2017;14(1):93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Talati Z, Pettigrew S, Neal B, Dixon H, Hughes C, Kelly B, et al. Consumers’ responses to health claims in the context of other on-pack nutrition information: a systematic review. Nutr Rev 2017;75(4):260–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Moran AJ, Roberto CA. Health warning labels correct parents’ misperceptions about sugary drink options. Am J Prev Med 2018;55(2):e19–e27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Munsell CR, Harris JL, Sarda V, Schwartz MB. Parents’ beliefs about the healthfulness of sugary drink options: opportunities to address misperceptions. Public Health Nutr 2016;19(1):46–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Simmonds G, Woods AT, Spence C. ‘Show me the goods’: Assessing the effectiveness of transparent packaging vs. product imagery on product evaluation. Food Quality and Preference 2018;63:18–27. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Machiels CJ, Karnal N. See how tasty it is? Effects of symbolic cues on product evaluation and taste. Food quality and preference 2016;52:195–202. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kraak VI, Story M. Influence of food companies’ brand mascots and entertainment companies’ cartoon media characters on children’s diet and health: a systematic review and research needs. Obes Rev 2015;16(2):107–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ogle AD, Graham DJ, Lucas-Thompson RG, Roberto CA . Influence of Cartoon Media Characters on Children’s Attention to and Preference for Food and Beverage Products. J Acad Nutr Diet 2017;117(2):265–270.e2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Institute of Medicine. Examination of front-of-package nutrition rating systems and symbols: Phase 1 report. Washington, DC: National Academies of Science; 2010. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Institute of Medicine. Front-of-package nutrition rating systems and symbols: Promoting healthier choices. Washington, DC: National Academies of Science; 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Popova L, Nonnemaker J, Taylor N, Bradfield B, Kim A. Warning Labels on Sugar-sweetened Beverages: An Eye Tracking Approach. Am J Health Behav 2019;43(2):406–419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Tortora G, Machin L, Ares G. Influence of nutritional warnings and other label features on consumers’ choice: Results from an eye-tracking study. Food Res Int 2019;119:605–611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.VanEpps EM, Roberto CA. The influence of sugar-sweetened beverage warnings: A randomized trial of adolescents’ choices and beliefs. Am J Prev Med 2016;51(5):664–672. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bollard T, Maubach N, Walker N, Ni Mhurchu C. Effects of plain packaging, warning labels, and taxes on young people’s predicted sugar-sweetened beverage preferences: an experimental study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2016;13(1):95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Roberto CA, Wong D, Musicus A, Hammond D. The influence of sugar-sweetened beverage health warning labels on parents’ choices. Pediatrics 2016;137(2):e20153185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Grummon AH, Hall MG, Taillie LS, Brewer NT. How should sugar-sweetened beverage health warnings be designed? A randomized experiment. Preventive Medicine In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Grummon AG, Taillie LS, Golden SD, Hall MG, Ranney LM, Brewer NT. Sugar-sweetened beverage health warnings and purchases: A randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Preventive Medicine In Press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Moodie C, Stead M, Bauld L, McNeill A, Angus K, Hinds K, et al. Plain tobacco packaging: a systematic review. 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Stead M, Moodie C, Angus K, Bauld L, McNeill A, Thomas J, et al. Is consumer response to plain/standardised tobacco packaging consistent with framework convention on tobacco control guidelines? A systematic review of quantitative studies. PLoS One 2013;8(10):e75919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.McNeill A, Gravely S, Hitchman SC, Bauld L, Hammond D, Hartmann-Boyce J. Tobacco packaging design for reducing tobacco use. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2017;4:Cd011244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ajzen I The theory of planned behavior. Organizational behavior and human decision processes 1991;50(2):179–211. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rosenstock IM. Historical origins of the health belief model. Health Education &Behavior 1974;2(4):328–335. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Acton RB, Hammond D. Do manufacturer ‘nutrient claims’ influence the efficacy of mandated front-of-package labels? Public Health Nutr 2018;21(18):3354–3359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Fernan C, Schuldt JP, Niederdeppe J. Health Halo Effects from Product Titles and Nutrient Content Claims in the Context of “Protein” Bars. Health Commun 2018;33(12):1425–1433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Berhaupt-Glickstein A, Hooker NH, Hallman WK. Qualified Health Claim Language affects Purchase Intentions for Green Tea Products in the United States. Nutrients 2019;11(4). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Jeong M, Zhang D, Morgan JC, Ross JC, Osman A, Boynton MH, et al. Similarities and Differences in Tobacco Control Research Findings From Convenience and Probability Samples. Ann Behav Med 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Brewer NT, Jeong M, Hall MG, Baig SA, Mendel JR, Lazard AJ, et al. The impact of e-cigarette health warnings on motivation to vape and smoke. Under Review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Lazard AJ, Mackert MS, Bock MA, Love B, Dudo A, Atkinson L. Visual assertions:Effects of photo manipulation and dual processing for food advertisements. Visual Communication Quarterly 2018;25(1):16–30. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hedrick VE, Savla J, Comber DL, Flack KD, Estabrooks PA, Nsiah-Kumi PA, et al. Development of a brief questionnaire to assess habitual beverage intake (BEVQ-15): sugar-sweetened beverages and total beverage energy intake. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics 2012;112(6):840–849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Schulz KF, Altman DG, Moher D. CONSORT 2010 Statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMC Med 2010;8:18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Shapiro SS, Wilk MB. An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples).Biometrika 1965;52(3/4):591–611. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Kay MC, Welker EB, Jacquier EF, Story MT. Beverage consumption patterns among infants and young children (0 – 47.9 months): Data from the Feeding Infants and Toddlers Study, 2016. Nutrients 2018;10(7). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Mendez MA, Miles DR, Poti JM, Sotres-Alvarez D, Popkin BM. Persistent disparities over time in the distribution of sugar-sweetened beverage intake among children in the United States. Am J Clin Nutr 2019;109(1):79–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Baig SA, Byron MJ, Lazard AJ, Brewer NT. “Organic,” “natural,” and “additive-free” cigarettes: Comparing the effects of advertising claims and disclaimers on perceptions of harm. Nicotine Tob Res 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Green KC, Armstrong JS. Evidence on the effects of mandatory disclaimers in advertising. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing 2012;31(2):293–304. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kesselheim AS, Connolly J, Rogers J, Avorn J. Mandatory disclaimers on dietary supplements do not reliably communicate the intended issues. Health Aff (Millwood) 2015;34(3):438–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Donnelly GE, Zatz LY, Svirsky D, John LK. The effect of graphic warnings on sugary drink purchasing. Psychol Sci 2018;29(8):1321–1333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Brewer NT, Hall MG, Noar SM, Parada H Jr, Stein-Seroussi A, Bach LE, et al. Effect of pictorial cigarette pack warnings on changes in smoking behavior: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern Med 2016;176(7):905–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Noar SM, Hall MG, Francis DB, Ribisl KM, Pepper JK, Brewer NT. Pictorial cigarette pack warnings: A meta-analysis of experimental studies. Tobacco Control 2016;25(3):341–354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Abrams KM, Evans C, Duff BR. Ignorance is bliss. How parents of preschool children make sense of front-of-package visuals and claims on food. Appetite 2015;87:20–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Letona P, Chacon V, Roberto C, Barnoya J. A qualitative study of children’s snack food packaging perceptions and preferences. BMC public health 2014;14(1):1274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Centurion M, Machin L, Ares G. Relative Impact of Nutritional Warnings and Other Label Features on Cereal Bar Healthfulness Evaluations. J Nutr Educ Behav 2019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Zellner DA, Durlach P. Effect of color on expected and experienced refreshment, intensity, and liking of beverages. Am J Psychol 2003;116(4):633–47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Clydesdale FM. Color as a factor in food choice. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 1993;33(1):83–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Spence C, Levitan CA, Shankar MU, Zampini M. Does food color influence taste and flavor perception in humans? Chemosensory Perception 2010;3(1):68–84. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Jeong M, Zhang D, Morgan JC, Cornacchione J, Osman A, Boynton MH, et al. Similarities and differences in tobacco control research findings from convenience and probability samples. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Berinsky AJ, Huber GA, Lenz GS. Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk. Polit Anal 2012;20(3):351–368. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Weinberg JD, Freese J, McElhattan D. Comparing data characteristics and results of an online factorial survey between a population-based and a crowdsource-recruited sample. Sociol Sci 2014;1:292–310. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.