Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Apr 1;15(4):e0229662. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0229662

Long-term patterns of gender imbalance in an industry without ability or level of interest differences

Luís A Nunes Amaral 1,2,3,*, João A G Moreira 2, Murielle L Dunand 1, Heliodoro Tejedor Navarro 1, Hyojun Ada Lee 2
Editor: Bing Xue4
PMCID: PMC7112163  PMID: 32236126

Abstract

Female representation has been slowly but steadily increasing in many sectors of society. One sector where one would expect to see gender parity is the movie industry, yet the representation of females in most functions within the U.S. movie industry remain surprisingly low. Here, we study the historical patterns of female representation among actors, directors, and producers in an attempt to gain insights into the possible causes of the lack of gender parity in the industry. Our analyses reveals a remarkable temporal coincidence between the collapse in female representation across all functions and the advent of the Studio System, a period when the major Hollywood studios controlled all aspects of the industry. Female representation among actors, directors, producers and writers dropped to extraordinarily low values during the emergence and consolidation of the Studio System that in some cases have not yet recovered to pre-Studio System levels. In order to explore some possible mechanisms behind these patterns, we investigate the association between the gender balance of actors, writers, directors, and producers and a number of economic indicators, movie industry indicators, and movie characteristics. We find robust, strong, and significant associations which are consistent with an important role for the gender of decision makers on the gender balance of other industry functions. While in no way demonstrating causality, our findings add new perspectives to the discussions of the reasons for female under-representation in fields such as computer science and medicine, that have also experienced dramatic changes in female representation.

Introduction

Gender diversity is increasingly regarded as a desirable condition by educational, business, and governmental organizations. Recent research shows that more gender-balanced groups are better at complex decision-making [1] and that females show less self-interest and are better at complex moral reasoning than males. [2] Indeed, the proportion of women faculty members in many STEM fields has been steadily increasing, [3] as has the number of females in corporate suites and in political office. [4, 5] These trends are significant because the absence of women in leadership positions was reported to have a negative impact on women’s aspirations and advancement and may perpetuate gender biases. [6, 7]

Still, most fields struggle to reach gender parity. A particularly striking example is the computer software industry. [811] While nowadays female representation is remarkably low, there is clear evidence of early contributions by women to computer science. The first recorded computer algorithm was written by Ada Lovelace. During World War II, female mathematicians programmed the computers used for code-breaking at Bletchley Park in Britain, and U.S. female mathematicians programmed the ENIAC and the UNIVAC. The first modern computer language and compiler were developed by Grace Hopper, while another female programmer, Evelyn Berezin, developed the first mainstream word processor and the world’s first computerized airline reservation system. In fact, until the 1970’s coding was seen as a women’s job, and as recently as the mid 1980’s, nearly 40% of computer science graduates were female. [8, 10] Nowadays, that number is only about 17% [9] and there is an ongoing controversy within the industry on whether females have an interest and/or innate ability for coding.

The movie industry, in contrast to the software industry, is a case where some of the mechanisms that are sometimes hypothesized to drive gender differences are unlikely to operate. For example, in spite of the contributions of female coders, most people are better able to recognize the prominent men in the software industry than the prominent women—consider the name recognition of Lovelace, Hopper, and Berezin, versus that of Turing, Gates and Zuckerberg. The situation is quite different for acting. While it took until the 1600’s for women to be allowed on the stage, they quickly proved their singing and acting talent in operas and plays. Already by 1869, only two centuries after they were first allowed on stage in England, John Stuart Mill and Harriet Taylor, would point out that “The only one of the fine arts which women do follow, to any extent, as a profession, and an occupation for life, is the histrionic; and in that they are confessedly equal, if not superior, to men.” [12] Marlene Dietrich, Katharine Hepburn, and Meryl Streep are just as recognizable and easy to name as Douglas Fairbanks, Humphrey Bogart, and Tom Hanks. Moreover, because the time required of most actors to be on set during the filming of a movie is quite brief compared with most other jobs, film acting as a career significantly reduces the possible career impact of maternity leave and childrearing. [13] Yet, there is evidence of significant gender imbalances in U.S.-produced movie casts. [6, 1419]

We study the temporal evolution of female representation in the movie-making teams of over 26 thousand United States (US) produced movies released between 1911 and 2010 (Fig 1; see Methods for details on criteria for inclusion). We find a striking temporal coincidence between the creation and consolidation of the Hollywood Studio System [20, 21] and a drastic reduction in gender balance across several functions in the US movie industry. Additionally, we find that the dearth of female representation among movie producers during the heydays of the Studio System is strongly associated with lower lower levels of female representation among directors, screenwriters, and actors. We also find an intriguing temporal coincidence between the breakup of the Studio System—and the consequent increase in the bargaining power of actors, both male and female—and the increased inclusion of former actors among the producer ranks.

Fig 1. Historical trends of gender imbalance in the U.S. movie industry.

Fig 1

(a) Timeline of 20th century events relevant to the evolution of the U.S. movie industry. Red bars identify major wars with U.S. involvement (chronologically, World War I, World War II, Korean war, and Vietnam war). Light peach bars show the peak of the feminism waves in the U.S. Orange shadings indicate the rates of TV adoption in U.S. households (from unsaturated to saturated: <30%, <60%, <90%). [47] Light blue shadings identify, from less to more saturated, MPPC control, [48] consolidation of the Hollywood Studio System, and Studio System’s heyday. [20] The de Havilland vs. Warner Bros’ 1944 decision freed major stars from life long contracts with as single studio. The U.S. vs. Paramount’s 1948 decision unravelled the vertical integration of the U.S. movie industry. These legal defeats enabled greater negotiating power for movie stars and the ability of independent producing companies to have their movies distributed in a more equitable manner. (b) Number of U.S.-produced movies considered in our study released annually. See S1 Fig for breakdown by movie genre. (c) Concentration of the industry’s output by producing company/studio as measured by the Herfindhal-Hirschman index. Larger values indicate that a large fraction of movies are produced by a small number of studios. We observe an increase of the degree of concentration during the period of control of the industry by MPPC. The degree of concentration reaches a minimum in 1922 but then it grows until 1944. These trends are consistent with current understanding of the consolidation and golden age of the Hollywood Studio System. [20, 48] (d) Temporal dependence of the percentage of females according to movie-making function. The olive solid line in the top panel shows female participation in the U.S workforce in order to provide a comparison to the overall economy. Solid lines indicate average values and the color bands show 95% confidence intervals. The red dotted lines show the highest value of female representation attained prior to 1940, and the red dashed lines show the lowest value attained after 1930, if different from zero. For all movie-making functions there is a similar “U-shape” pattern with an early maximum achieved prior to 1922, a minimum reached by the mid-1940s, and an increase after.

The implications of our findings extend far beyond the movie industry. Indeed, while the study of the historical patterns of female representation among actors and other movie-making functions is a very important field of study on its own, our perspective is that our study is likely to also contribute significant new insights into the reasons why females are under-represented in, for example, STEM fields, without the confounding effect of hypothetic lower female innate abilities or levels of interest for the profession.

Indeed, there is currently a great deal of controversy [22] surrounding the primacy of evolutionary/biological [23, 24] mechanisms versus environmental/cultural mechanisms in explaining observed gender representation imbalances. We pursue here an approach informed by cultural primacy approaches. We believe this to be warranted since studies informed by evolutionary psychology suggest that women have a preference for activities where people and inter-personal interactions are prominent. [25] Such preferences would predict greater female interest for activities such as acting and producing. This hypothesis is supported by a recent survey reporting that high-school aged girls are more interested in careers in the arts, including acting, than males. [26]

If the gender imbalance observed in the movie industry would be due to cultural factors, then one needs to understand the reasons for discrimination. [2732] Note that, in contrast to Fleck and Hanssen, [33] we do no address here age discrimination, the interplay between age and gender, or the reasons why male stars are often cast to play roles younger than their chronological age. Rudman et al. [32] hypothesize that females who achieve prominence may ultimately experience a backlash because of status incongruity. This hypothesis is remarkably consistent with the historical trends we uncover. Moreover, backlash mechanisms for enforcing discrimination—such as the “boys club,” and “locker room” culture [3436]—are quite prominent in the context of the movie industry. The latter, expressed through references to the “casting coach”, has become a trope of the movie industry and is described in many an actress memoir.

Following Becker [37], the question, nonetheless, remains of why would Hollywood’s powers that be, accept the loss of profit arising from discriminating against talent? Even not accounting for those like Hitchcock who remarked that “actors are cattle”, [36] a plausible answer is that it is hard to a priori predict the success of a movie project. [38, 39] Thus, it would have been quite easy to rationalize any losses arising from discriminating against talented females. In fact, even in fields where recognizing excellence earlier is easier, such as sports, racial discrimination was maintained for many decades. [40]

Data

We retrieved release year, title, director, producers, writers, cinematographers, and production companies for 35,281 U.S.-produced movies compiled by the American Film Institute (AFI, https://search.proquest.com/afi/). Because AFI does not keep disambiguated information about industry participants, we also retrieved movie data from the Internet Movie Database (IMDb, https://imdb.com). We obtained release year, title, director, producers, writers, cinematographers, casting directors, and budget for 393,575 movies, and name, gender, birth year, and death year for 1,856,569 industry participants. Using titles, years of release and director names, we matched 25,645 records in the AFI database to movies in IMDb. These are the movies included in our study (see Methods for details).

Results

Century long trends in female representation

The emergence and consolidation of the Hollywood Studio System coincided with a dramatic change in the gender balance of a movie’s cast (Fig 1 and Methods). Between 1922 and 1950, we observe a reduction of nearly 25% in the fraction of females in the cast of the typical movie. Surprisingly, not even the U.S.’s participation in World War II reversed this trend. In order to determine whether this decrease was due to changes in popularity of different movie genres (S1 Fig), we determine female representation in the cast of movies according to genre (Fig 2). While one does find that genres popular during this time period, such as Action, Adventure, and War, have smaller fractions of female cast members [17, 41], the data leaves no doubt that the overall pattern of gender representation over time is replicated across all genres (Fig 2).

Fig 2. Historical trends of gender imbalance in the casts of U.S.-produced movies.

Fig 2

Temporal dependence of the percentage of females actors according to movie genre. Solid lines indicate average values and the color bands show 95% confidence intervals. The red dotted lines highlight the highest value of female representation attained prior to 1940, whereas the red dashed lines highlight the lowest value attained after 1930. For all genres with sufficient number of movies, we find a clear “U-shape” pattern.

The trends we report here for movie casts mirror trends reported earlier for screenwriters, [42] which we confirm (Fig 1d). The fraction of female writers steadily increased up to 1922, the year when industry concentration was lowest. Between 1922 and 1940, the fraction of female writers fell to half of its peak. The falling female representation continued until 1960, and has barely recovered above the 1940s level. [43, 44]

The temporal coincidence between the changes in female representation and the emergence and consolidation of the Studio System is not restricted to actors and writers. For both directors and producers we find similar—but even more extreme—patterns (Fig 1d). By the 1930s, female representation among producers and directors had dropped to essentially zero. This collapse is particularly significant because producers’ decisions are so important. [45, 46]

Predictors of the level of female representation

In order to test for the presence of an association between industry consolidation and female representation, we conduct a multivariate regression of female representation in terms of factors capturing national economic indicators, movie industry indicators, and movie characteristics. Specifically, we consider the model:

Pf(m)=β0f+βpfp(m)+βdfd(m)+βbfb(m)+g{g(m)}βg++δwfWy(m)+δgfGy(m)+δnfNy(m)+δcfCy(m)++ϵy(m)f, (1)

where Pf is the female representation for movie-making function f in movie m released in year y, p(m) and d(m) are the percentage of females in the producing and directing teams, respectively, b(m) is the logarithm of the movie’s budget, {g(m)} is the movie’s set of genres, Wy(m) is the female workforce participation, Gy(m) is the real GDP per capita change, Ny(m) is the number of movies released, Cy(m) is the industry concentration, which we measured using the Herfindhal-Hirschman index, the βs and δs are the coefficients we aim to estimate through regression, and the ϵs are assumed to be Gaussian distributed independent noise terms with zero mean (Fig 3). Because the vast majority of movies have a single director, a single cinematographer, and (prior to 1950) a single producer, we perform logistic regression of the model above for those functions. Naturally, when modeling female representation in directing, we do not include the d(m) term, and when modeling female representation in producing, we do not include the p(m) and d(m) terms.

Fig 3. Multivariate logistic (for director, cinematographer, and producers) and OLS (for actors and writers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

Fig 3

We do not include time dummies in the models for which we show regression results here (see S2S5 Figs for other time periods and for models including time dummies). We plot the base 10 logarithm of the risk ratio for each factor. The risk factor for female workforce participation was calculated for an increase of 10%. The risk factor for change in real GPD per capita was calculated for an increase of 1%. The risk factor for number of movies was calculated for an increase of 300. The risk factor for industry concentration was calculate for a change of 0.03. The risk factor for budget was calculated for an increase by 100 fold. The risk ratio for percentage female producers was calculated for an increase of 50%. This value is one of the four typical values observed, the other three being 0%, 25% and 100%. The risk factor for percentage female directors was calculated for an increase of 100% since typically there is a single director. Risk ratios for which we are not able to reject the null hypothesis at the 0.01 level are shown in grey. Risk ratios significantly larger than 1 are shown in yellow and those significantly smaller than 1 are shown in purple.

For movies released prior to 1950, we lack budget information for the vast majority of movies. Thus, we perform fits of the model separately for time periods pre- and post-1950. For fits addressing the period prior to 1950, we do not include the term b(m) in the model. In order to test the robustness of our model and of the coefficient estimates, we fit the models for several different time periods. Moreover, and because our data extends over a long time period, we repeat all fits with the inclusion of time dummies. Because the large number of time dummies creates convergence issues, especially for the logistic fits, we consider both yearly (S2 and S3 Figs) and 4-year time dummies (S4 and S5 Figs). Re-assuringly, we find that the model fits for the two periods yield remarkably similar coefficient estimates and that the coefficient for the vast majority of the time dummies are not significantly different from zero.

As suggested by the data in Fig 1, for the period prior to 1950, we find strong, consistent, statistically significant associations between female representation across all movie functions and industry concentration. For the post-1950 period, we again we find strong, consistent, statistically significant associations between female representation in casts and industry concentration, but not for other movie functions. For screenwriters, the post-1950 period coefficient would have been statistically significant at the 0.05 level. However, for directors and cinematographers, the fact that we are conducting a logistic regression and the narrower variation range of industry concentration post-1950, mean that we lack statistical power.

Surprisingly, our analyses do not uncover consistent or significant associations between female representation and national economic indicators. However, confirming the expectations, we find strong, consistent, statistically significant associations between female representation in casts and movie genre. Action, Adventure, Crime, Thriller, and Western have lower female representation, whereas Comedy, Drama, and Romance have higher female representation. While some of these associations are also consistently present for screenwriters, the vast majority cannot be observed consistently for directors, cinematographers, or producers.

Confirming expectations, we find a significant negative association between movie budget and female representation. Interestingly, the association is especially strong for director and cinematographer. Remarkably, we also find a strong, consistent, and statistically significant positive association between a female director and female representation in cast and screenwriting teams. Our most important result, however, is the consistent and statistically significant positive association between percentage of female producers and female representation for both periods.

Our regression analyses suggest that females producers and directors contribute to the career advancement of other females in the industry. Our findings thus contrast with survey results [49] suggesting that in many industries females leaders are ‘cogs in the machine’ and unable to effectuate change in the workplace because they are [49] “either not powerful enough to affect the careers of their subordinates, or they have been selected to their managerial positions because they identify with powerful men at the apex of firms”. Our findings also contrast with evidence in science hiring for a preference by both males and females for male applicants. [50]

Changes in producer team characteristics

The pre-1950 period considered in our analyses is particularly interesting because of the dramatic changes in industry concentration. In order to further determine the extent to which the association between industry consolidation and decreased female representation is tied together, we next focus on the period 1918-1930 and calculate female representation for three movie-making functions according to producing company. Specifically, we track the movies produced by the seven companies that would come to dominate the industry during the Studio System: 20th Century, Columbia, Fox, MGM, RKO, Universal, and Warner. We find that prior to 1930, these studios were already producing movies with lower representation of females in acting and producing than the rest of the industry (Fig 4). Thus, it appears quite plausible that the consolidation of the industry centered around the “Big Seven” studios promoted a culture where contributions from females were not welcomed.

Fig 4. Gender balance for four movie-making functions produced by the “Big Seven”—we drop disney from the “Big Eight” because during this period they focused on animation movies—and by the so-called Independents.

Fig 4

We compare female representation for all movies and for movies produced by each group of studios. Values for all movies use the same colors as in Fig 1. Values for movies produced by the “Big Seven” are shown in light yellow, and produced by the Independents in pink. Strikingly, during the first half of the 1920s, the “Big Seven” released movies with lower representation levels of females in both acting and producing.

The status quo of the U.S. movie industry started to change dramatically around 1950 because of the lost legal battles of the 1940s (Fig 1) and the impact of the adoption of television by U.S. households. [47] As a reaction to these changes, the major Hollywood studios focused to a greater extent on big budget movies that made use of technological innovations, such as widescreens and Technicolor, and thus would have a better chance of bringing people to the theaters. [51, 52] The changed focus of the major studios created opportunities for independent competitors [53, 54] and, together with the need for consultants with the desired technical expertise, resulted in a dramatic increase in the number of new producers entering the industry and the size of the producer teams (Fig 5a–5c).

Fig 5. Entry of new producers and directors into the U.S. movie industry.

Fig 5

(a) Number of new producers and new directors entering the industry annually. Except for the 1910s, the number of new producers has systematically exceeded the number of new director entering the industry. (b) Temporal evolution of producing team size, directing team size, and cast size. U.S.-produced movies have maintained the tradition of having a single director. In contrast, producing teams have grown steadily in size, from a mean size of one until the mid-1940s to a mean of eight. Interestingly, cast size has grown in a more discontinuous manner, with three step changes occurring around 1915, 1935, and 1975. (c) Time dependence of the average size of the team of producers for all male and mixed-gender teams. Color bands show 95% confidence intervals. It is visually apparent, that teams that include females are typically larger and have grown faster. (d) Fraction of new producers (directors) who had at least 4 years of prior acting experience at the time of their first producer (director) credit. For directors, that fraction remained approximately constant during the entire period. In contrast, for producers, there are three well-defined peaks centered at 1922, 1948, and 1959. The presence and location of these peaks does not differ if we change the requirement on the duration of the prior acting experience to 6 or 8 years, or if we require a minimum of 5 or 10 prior acting credits.

Fig 5 shows how producer team characteristics changed dramatically around this time. Until 1950, a single producer was associated with a movie. From then on, the average number of producers keeps growing and reaches an average of eight by 2010. This pattern is quite distinct from the more abrupt, almost step-like, changes in cast size, or the unchanged number of directors per movie. The changes in the size of producer teams after 1940 look visually similar to the increases in female representation in other movie-making functions. We thus hypothesize that changes in gender representation may have been, at least partially, driven by movie stars freed from the Studio System and with greater negotiation power who were newly able to make production decisions and to change the status quo.

To test this possibility, we calculate the fraction of new producers entering the industry who had prior acting experience (Fig 5d). The patterns in the data are consistent with our hypothesis. A first peak, centered at 1922, shows that about a quarter of new producers entering the industry had prior acting credits. This peak coincide with peaks in levels of female representation in casts, directing, and producing. A second and third peaks, centered in 1948 and 1959, respectively, again increased rates of new producers with prior acting credits. Suggestively, these peaks coincide almost exactly with “step jumps” in the probability of directors and producers being female (Fig 1d).

Limitations

Our study suffers from three major limitations. First, it does not capture changes in gender representation among starring, as opposed to credited, actors. This limitation is difficult to address in a systematic manner since we do not have a reliable method to determine who within a cast can be classified as a star. This limitation may, however, not be particularly relevant to the detection of systemic issues since stars may be comparatively shielded.

Second, our analysis is restricted to about 26 thousand U.S.-produced movies that are catalogued in the AFI database. Thus, we cannot determine whether similar or distinct trends arose in the movie industry of other countries or in TV scripted shows. However, we believe that at this point it is more important to focus on a less broad case but one for which coverage is comprehensive. [55]

Third, our study can only reveal associations and cannot demonstrate causality. Nonetheless, our analysis support both recent and old accounts of the power held by producers, and the gender bias in awards [56, 57] or collaborations. [58, 59] As demonstrated by the data in Figs 1 and 4, the heydays of the Hollywood Studio System coincided with a disappearance of females from functions with decision-making power. The economic shift in favor of movie stars and the power that shift brought to some actresses enabled them to later play roles as producers and directors coinciding with a virtuous cycle of increased female representation in the industry.

Discussion

While specific to the movie industry, our finding have broad implications that extend far beyond it. Our results are remarkably consistent with the predictions of the status incongruity hypothesis of Rudman et al. [32] Specifically, as an industry grows in importance, there are increasing financial rewards and industry consolidation. This transition may then result in hostility toward successful females and people of color, and create the conditions for a backlash.

In the movie industry, the prominence of the Hollywood studies post World War I, may have made it “unacceptable” to have women and people of color in positions of leadership outside of acting. The hypothetic resulting backlash may then be responsible for the complete diversity collapse observed during the Studio System’s Golden Age especially within producing and directing. The fact that women were the most affected by this transformation, may be the reason why two females—actresses Bette Davis, in the 1930s, and Olivia de Havilland, in the 1940s—were the ones to take legal action against the Studio’s absolute control over actors’ careers.

Our findings can thus be added to the cases of computer science, [8, 9] medicine, [60] and literature, [61] where an increase in the importance of the field was followed by a collapse of diversity. Our findings and the status incongruity hypothesis are also consistent with the loss of prestige of professions, such as elementary school teaching [62] and some medical specialties, [63] that have experienced increases in female representation.

Our study also sheds light on the plausibility of some explanations for why females and other groups are under-represented or even absent from some professions. For some careers, it has been argued that males may be superiorly suited because of greater “physical strength,” greater “mathematical ability”, or some other advantage, or because, unlike females, they do not have to interrupt their careers due to childbearing. In some context, the lack of females has also been attributed to “a shortage of qualified female job candidates.” For the case of motion pictures, none of these arguments is plausible. Surveys show that high-school aged girls are more interested in careers in the arts, including acting, than their male peers. [26] Moreover, there is no credible reason for believing that there are different innate acting abilities and so it cannot be seriously argued that such differences have been the driver of the gender imbalance we report.

Finally, our study provides support for the positive impact of higher female representation at the decision-making level in the movie industry. This is remarkable because a number of studies suggest this is not always the case. [49, 50] For example, some research suggests that in many industries females leaders are ‘cogs in the machine’ and unable to effectuate change in the workplace because they are [49] “either not powerful enough to affect the careers of their subordinates, or they have been selected to their managerial positions because they identify with powerful men at the apex”.

Methods

The hollywood studio system

The birth and expansion of the U.S. movie industry coincided with a period of major historical changes (Fig 1a). Movie making started in the U.S. as soon as the technology became available. The U.S.’s insulation from the effects of the first World War, which greatly hindered the development of the European movie industry, enabled the U.S. movie industry to expand rapidly (Fig 1b). By the 1920s, Hollywood was the dominant player in the global movie industry both in terms of the number of movies being produced and in terms of the profits captured [48].

Prior to 1915, the broad expansion of the U.S. movie industry was hindered by the efforts of the Motion Picture Patents Company (MPPC), which represented the major intellectual property owners. The MPPC activities helped increase industry concentration (Fig 1c). As a result, by 1917, the top 5 (out of 97) companies accounted for 38% of U.S. released movies. Immediately after the MPCC dissolution, the degree of concentration of the industry decreased dramatically—by 1920, the top 5 (out of 112) companies accounted for 28% of releases. This diversification trend, however, was reversed with the consolidation of the industry in Hollywood and the establishment of the Studio System. By 1940, 51% of U.S. movies were released by the top 5 (out of 58) companies. While there is a vigorous discussion on the dates for the establishment and consolidation of the Studio System, our analysis of industry concentration strongly suggests that the Studio System started forming around 1920, and remained stable until the mid-1940s.

Selection criteria for study inclusion

We extracted available information for 35,281 U.S.-produced movies compiled by the American Film Institute (AFI, https://search.proquest.com/afi/) and released between 1910 and 2010. We stored, when available, release year, title, and genre. Additionally, we stored credited and uncredited directors, producers, writers, cinematographers, casting directors, cast members, and production companies for each movie. After excluding movies not in English, we were left with 30,706 movies for consideration (S1 File).

Because the AFI database does not provide disambiguated information on industry participants, we also retrieved data from the IMDb which disambiguates industry participants. Specifically, we retrieved, when available, release year, title, genre, budget, revenue, whether it was a made-for-TV movie, credited and uncredited directors, producers, writers, cinematographers, casting directors, and cast members for 393,575 U.S.-produced movies. We also retrieved the IMDb record for the 1,856,569 industry participants identified in these movies. These participants include humans, but also animals and groups (such as orchestras). For each disambiguated individual record, we stored, when available, name, gender, birth year, and death year. For details on gender assignment, see below. Information on the career of industry participants is available at url https://doi.org/10.21985/n2-ttv2-qz74.

We then used titles, years of release and director names, to match AFI to IMDb records. We excluded from the match Made-for-TV movies, and movies classified as Animation or News. Because release year in AFI can be incorrect—that is, stored year of release does not match information on dates of first screening—we allowed for a ±1 difference in year of release between AFI and IMDb records. This matching process identified 26,082 movies (S2 File). As a test of the accuracy of our process, we manually checked 101 randomly selected movies. We found zero matching errors, 7 cases of different release years, and 3 cases for which AFI did not include director information but IMDb did. Because data for 1910 was dominated by Shorts, we restricted the analysis to movies released in the 100 year period 1911–2010. The reduced our final sample to 25,645 movies.

For the 101 matched movies, we also matched cast lists, credited and all, between AFI and IMDb records (S3 and S4 Files). Matching credited casts is challenging because neither database uses consistent criteria for what makes a cast member credited. For this reason, we looked for consensus in the classifications from the two databases. For the credited (all) casts of the randomly selected 101 movies, we matched 1,161 (2035) actors; 2 (51) actors were listed in AFI but not IMDb, and 55 (831) were listed in IMDb but not AFI. After looking in detail at disagreements, we concluded that IMDb cast lists are at least as accurate as AFI’s, and most times are more detailed.

Assigning gender to individuals

The gender of actors is explicitly mentioned in most of their individual biographical pages, thus we are able to determine their gender. For individuals with other movie-making functions who do not also have acting credits, we used four independent indirect methods for assigning gender and then build a consensus based on the different assignments.

Method 1: We used the method get_gender from the Python package gender-guesser.detector (version 0.4.0) to “guess” the gender using the individual’s given (first) name. [64] The output of this method is one of ‘female,’ ‘mostly female,’ ‘androgynous,’ ‘unknown,’ ‘mostly male,’ or ‘male.’ Method 2: If present, we parsed an individual’s biographical page (http://www.imdb.com/name/person_id/bio) for gender-specific pronouns (he/his/him/himself, or she/her/hers/herself). When the number of (male-) female-specific pronouns exceeded that of (female-) male-specific ones, we assume the individual is a (male) female. Ties are marked as ‘unknown.’ Method 3: We parsed an individual’s biographical page (http://www.imdb.com/name/person_id) for gender-specific pronouns (he/his/him/himself, or she/her/hers/herself). When the number of (male-) female-specific pronouns exceeded that of (female-) male-specific ones, we assume the individual is a (male) female. Ties are marked as ‘unknown.’ Method 4: We scan the webpage http://www.imdb.com/name/person_id for the html tags with id ‘filmo-head-actress’ and ‘filmo-head-actor.’ It assigns the corresponding gender if only one of these searches is successful, otherwise it returns ‘unknown.’

Given the results of the four methods, we make a consensus determination using the following algorithm. Each gender assignment of ‘male’ is worth one point for ‘male’ gender, each assignment of ‘mostly male’ is worth a half-point for ‘male’ gender, and similarly for ‘female.’ All other assignments are worth no points. The gender with the most points is assigned to the individual. In case of a tie, the gender is assigned as ‘unknown.’

Regression analysis

We systematically tested the robustness of our regression analysis. We separate the analysis into two main time periods, pre- and post-1950. This separation is driven by the fact that budget information only becomes available reliably after 1950. For each of these major time period, we repeat the regression fit both with and without time dummies and for different sub-periods. In the main text, we assign a confidence to our coefficient estimates based on the range of regression cases under which we obtain consistent results.

In the Supporting Information, we first show regression models that do not include time dummies. S2 and S3 Figs show results for the periods 1916–1950 and 1951–2010, respectively. Next, we consider regression models that include yearly time dummies. S4 and S5 Figs show results for the periods 1916–1950 and 1951–2010, respectively. Finally, we consider regression models that include 4-year time dummies. S6 and S7 Figs show results for the periods 1916–1950 and 1951–2010, respectively.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. U.S. produced movies considered in our study by genre.

(PDF)

S2 Fig. Multivariate logistic (for director and producers) and OLS (for actors and writers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

We do not include time dummies in our model. We plot the base 10 logarithm of the risk ratio for each factor. The risk factor for female workforce participation was calculated for an increase of 10%. The risk factor for change in real GPD per capita was calculated for an increase of 1%. The risk factor for number of movies was calculated for an increase of 300. The risk factor for industry concentration was calculate for a change of 0.03. The risk factor for budget was calculated for an increase by 100 fold. The risk ratio for percentage female producers was calculated for an increase of 50%. This value is one of the four typical values observed, the other three being 0%, 25% and 100%. The risk factor for percentage female directors was calculated for an increase of 100% since typically there is a single director. Risk ratios for which we are not able to reject the null hypothesis at the 0.01 level are shown in grey. Risk ratios significantly larger than 1 are shown in yellow and those significantly smaller than 1 are shown in purple. If a risk ratio estimate and its confidence bands fall outside the range displayed, we mark this with an arrow. The color of the arrow falls the same convention as the color of the risk ratio estimates.

(PDF)

S3 Fig. Multivariate logistic (for director and cinematographer) and OLS (for actors, writers, and producers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

We do not include time dummies in our model.

(PDF)

S4 Fig. Multivariate logistic (for director) and OLS (for actors and writers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

We consider 1-year time dummies in this case. Note that for directors the maximum number of iterations (35) was exceeded before the convergence criterion was reached. This is due to the time dummies whose estimation uncertainty is enormous and destabilizes the fit. We do not show results for cinematographers because the time dummies make the matrix singular.

(PDF)

S5 Fig. Multivariate logistic (for director and cinematographer) and OLS (for actors and writers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

We consider 1-year time dummies in this case. Note that for directors the maximum number of iterations (35) was exceeded before the convergence criterion was reached. This is due to the time dummies coefficients whose estimation uncertainty is enormous and destabilizes the fit. We do not show results for cinematographers because the time dummies make the matrix singular.

(PDF)

S6 Fig. Multivariate logistic (for director and producers) and OLS (for actors and writers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

We consider 4-year time dummies in this case.

(PDF)

S7 Fig. Multivariate logistic (for director and cinematographer) and OLS (for actors and writers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

We consider 4-year time dummies in this case.

(PDF)

S1 File. Filtered movies from AFI database.

(XLSX)

S2 File. List of movies matched between AFI and IMDb.

(PDF)

S3 File. List of 101 randomly selected movies used to verify the degree of agreement between credited cast lists for AFI and IMDb records of matched movies.

(XLSX)

S4 File. List of 101 randomly selected movies used to verify the degree of agreement between cast lists for AFI and IMDb records of matched movies.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We are in debt to Scott Curtis (Dept. of Radio/Television/Film, Northwestern University, and Communication Program, NU-Qatar) for many insightful comments and suggestions. We are also grateful to Martin Gerlach, Adam Pah, and Thomas Stoeger for stimulating discussions.

Data Availability

The datasets analyzed in this study are available as Supporting Information files and at https://arch.library.northwestern.edu.

Funding Statement

JAGM thanks Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia for grant SFRH-BD-76115-2011, LANA thanks Department of Defense’s Army Research Office for grant 281 W911NF-14-1-0259, the John Templeton Foundation for Award FP053369-A//39147 and the John and Leslie McQuown Gift.

References

  • 1. Woolley AW, Chabris CF, Pentland A, Hashmi N, and Malone TW. Evidence for a collective intelligence factor in the performance of human groups. Science, 330(6004):686–688, 2010. 10.1126/science.1193147 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Bar C and McQueen G. Why women make better directors. Int. J. Bus. Gov. Ethics, 8(1):93–99, 2013. 10.1504/IJBGE.2013.052743 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Duch J, Zeng XHT, Sales-Pardo M, Radicchi F, Otis S, Woodruff TK, et al. The possible role of resource requirements and academic career-choice risk on gender differences in publication rate and impact. PLoS One, 7(12):e51332, 2012. 10.1371/journal.pone.0051332 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Zaichkowsky JL. Women in the board room: One can make a difference. Int. J. Bus. Gov. Ethics, 9(1):91–113, 2014. 10.1504/IJBGE.2014.062774 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Jalalzai F. Shattered, Cracked, or Firmly Intact? Women and the Executive Glass Ceiling Worldwide. Oxford University Press, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Dean D. No human resource is an island: Gendered, racialized access to work as a performer. Gender, Work Organ., 15(2):161–181, 2008. 10.1111/j.1468-0432.2007.00389.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Beaman L, Duflo E, Pande R, and Topalova P. Female leadership raises aspirations and educational attainment for girls: A policy experiment in India. Science, 335(6068):582–586, 2012. 10.1126/science.1212382 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Sydell L. The forgotten female programmers who created modern tech. http://www.npr.org/sections/alltechconsidered/2014/10/06/345799830/, 2014. [Online; accessed 24–April–2017].
  • 9. Margolis J. Unlocking the Clubhouse: Women in Computing. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Thompson C. The secret history of women in coding. https://www.nytimes.com/2019/02/13/magazine/women-coding-computer-programming.html, 2019. [Online; accessed 29–April–2019].
  • 11. Loyalka P, Liu OL, Li G, Chirikov I, Kardanova E, Gu L, et al. Computer science skills across China, India, Russia, and the United States. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 116(14):6732–6736, 2019. 10.1073/pnas.1814646116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Mill JS. The Subjection of Women. Longmans, Green, Reader & Dyer, London, England, 1869. [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Cuddy AJC, Fiske ST, and Glick P. When professionals become mothers, warmth doesn’t cut the ice. J Social Issues, 60(4):701–718, 2004. 10.1111/j.0022-4537.2004.00381.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Bazzini DG, McIntosh WD, Smith SM, Cook S, and Harris C. The aging woman in popular film: Underrepresented, unattractive, unfriendly, and unintelligent. Sex Roles, 36(7–8):531–543, 1997. 10.1007/BF02766689 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Lincoln AE and Allen MP. Double jeopardy in Hollywood: Age and gender in the careers of film actors, 1926–1999. Sociol. Forum, 19(4):611–631, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Lauzen MM and Dozier DM. Maintaining the double standard: Portrayal of age and gender in popular films. Sex Roles, 52(7–8):437–446, 2005. 10.1007/s11199-005-3710-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Smith SL, Choueiti M, and Pieper K. Gender inequality in popular films: Examining on screen portrayals and behind-the-scenes employment patterns in motion pictures released between 2007–2013. Technical report, Annenberg School for Communication and Journalism, University of Southern California, 2014.
  • 18. de Pater IE, Judge TA, and Scott BA. Age, gender, and compensation: A study of Hollywood movie stars. J. Manag. Inq., 23(4):407–420, 2014. 10.1177/1056492613519861 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Smith SL, Choueiti M, and Pieper K. Inclusion in the director’s chair? Gender, race, and age of film directors across 1,000 films from 2007–2016. Technical report, Annenberg School for Communication and Journalism, University of Southern California, 2017.
  • 20. Bordwell D, Staiger J, and Thompson K. Classical Hollywood Cinema: Film Style and Mode of Production to 1960. Columbia University Press, New York, NY, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Crafton D. The Talkies: American Cinema’s Transition to Sound, 1926-1931. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Ketelaar T and Ellis BJ. Are evolutionary explanations unfalsifiable? Evolutionary psychology and the Lakatosian philosophy of science. Psychological Inquiry, 11(1):1–21, 2000. 10.1207/S15327965PLI1101_01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Miller GF. Sexual selection for cultural displays In Dunbar R, Knight C, and Power C, editors, The evolution of culture. An interdisciplinary view, pages 71–91. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Lange BP, Schwarz S, and Euler HA. The sexual nature of human culture. The Evolutionary Review: Art, Science, Culture, 4(1):76–85, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Su R, Rounds J, and Armstrong PI. Men and things, women and people: A meta-analysis of sex differences in interests. Psychological Bulletin, 135:859–884, 2018. 10.1037/a0017364 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Junior Achievement USA. 2017 Teens & Careers Survey. https://janorthflorida.wordpress.com/2017/06/29/2017-teens-careers-survey-by-junior-achievement-usa/, 2017. [Online; accessed 1–April–2019].
  • 27. Kanter RM. Men and Women of the Corporation. Basic Books, New York, NY, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Davidson M and Burke R, editors. Women in Management: Current Research Issues. Paul Chapman, London, UK, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Davidson M and Burke R, editors. Women in Management: Current Research Issues Volume II. Sage Publications Ltd., London, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Eagly AH and Karau SJ. Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109:573–598, 2002. 10.1037/0033-295x.109.3.573 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Lewis P and Simpson R. Kanter revisited: Gender, power and (in)visibility. International Journal of Management Reviews, 14:141–158, 2012. 10.1111/j.1468-2370.2011.00327.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Rudman LA, Moss-Racusin CA, Phelan JF, and Nauts S. Status incongruity and backlash effects: Defending the gender hierarchy motivates prejudice against female leaders. J. Experimental Social Psychol., 48:165–179, 2012. 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.10.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Fleck RK and Hanssen FA. Persistence and change in age-specific gender gaps: Hollywood actors from the silent era onward. International Review of Law and Economics, 48:36–49, 2016. 10.1016/j.irle.2016.08.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Maddock S and Parkin D. Gender cultures: How they affect men and women at work In Davidson M and Burke R, editors, Women in Management: Current Research Issues. Paul Chapman, London, UK, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Simpson R. Have times changed? Career barriers and the token woman manager. British Journal of Management, 8:S121–S130, 1997. 10.1111/1467-8551.8.s1.10 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Lovell A and Krämer P, editors. Screen Acting. Routledge, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Becker GS. The Economics of Discrimination. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, 2nd edition, 1971. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Litman BR. Predicting success of theatrical movies: An empirical study. Journal of Popular Culture, 16(9):159–175, 1983. 10.1111/j.0022-3840.1983.1604_159.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Sharda R and Delen D. Predicting box-office success of motion pictures with neural networks. Expert Systems with Applications, 30:243–254, 2006. 10.1016/j.eswa.2005.07.018 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Kahn LM. Discrimination in professional sports: A survey of the literature. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 44(3):395–418, 1991. 10.2307/2524152 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Wuehr P, Lange BP, and Schwarz S. Tears or fears? Comparing gender stereotypes about movie preferences to actual preferences. Frontiers in Psychology, 8:428, 2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Smith-Doerr L. Flexible organizations, innovation and gender equality: Writing for the US film industry, 1907–27. Ind. Innov., 17(1):5–22, 2010. 10.1080/13662710903573810 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Bielby DD and Bielby WT. Women and men in film: Gender inequality among writers in a culture industry. Gend. Soc., 10(3):248–270, 1996. 10.1177/089124396010003004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Bielby DD. Gender inequality in culture industries: Women and men writers in film and television. Sociol. Trav., 51(2):237–252, 2009. 10.1016/j.soctra.2009.03.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Lauzen MM and Dozier DM. The role of women on screen and behind the scenes in the television and film industries: Review of a program of research. J Communication Inquiry, 23(4):355–373, 1999. 10.1177/0196859999023004004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Cattani G, Ferriani S, Mariani MM, and Mengoli S. Tackling the “Galácticos” effect: Team familiarity and the performance of star-studded projects. Ind. Corp. Chang., 22(6):1629–1662, 2013. 10.1093/icc/dtt001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Steinberg C. TV Facts. New York Facts on File, Inc., 1980.
  • 48. Scott AJ. On Hollywood: The place, the Industry. Princeton University Press, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Maume DJ. Meet the new boss… same as the old boss? Female supervisors and subordinate career prospects. Social Science Research, 40:287–298, 2011. 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2010.05.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Moss-Racusin CA, Dovidio JF, Brescoll VL, Graham MJ, and Handelsman J. Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 109(41):16474–16479, 2012. 10.1073/pnas.1211286109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Garvin DA. Blockbusters: The economics of mass entertainment. J. Cult. Econ., 5:1–20, 1981. 10.1007/BF00189203 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Ravid SA. Information, blockbusters, and stars: A study of the film industry. J. Bus., 72(4):463–492, 1999. 10.1086/209624 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Grugulis I and Stoyanova D. Social capital and networks in film and TV: Jobs for the boys? Organ. Stud., 33(10):1311–1331, 2012. 10.1177/0170840612453525 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Faulkner RR and Anderson AB. Short-term projects and emergent careers: Evidence from Hollywood. Am. J. Sociol., 92(4):879, 1987. 10.1086/228586 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Wasserman M, Mukherjee S, Scott K, Zeng XHT, Radicchi F, and Amaral LAN. Correlations between user voting data, budget, and box office for films in the Internet Movie Database. J. Assoc. Inform. Sci. Technol., 66(4):858–868, 2015. 10.1002/asi.23213 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Carnes M, Geller S, Fine E, Sheridan J, and Handelsman J. NIH Director’s Pioneer Awards: Could the selection process be biased against women? J. Women’s health, 14(8):694–691, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Lincoln AE, Pincus S, Koster JB, and Leboy PS. The Matilda effect in science: Awards and prizes in the us, 1990s and 2000s. Social Studies of Science, 42(2):307–320, 2012. 10.1177/0306312711435830 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Knobloch-Westerwick S, Glynn CJ, and Huge M. The Matilda effect in science communication: An experiment on gender bias in publication quality perceptions and collaboration interest. Science Communication, 35(5):603–625, 2013. 10.1177/1075547012472684 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Zeng XHT, Duch J, Sales-Pardo M, Moreira JAG, Radicchi F, Ribeiro HV, et al. Differences in collaboration patterns across discipline, career stage, and gender. PLoS Biology, 14(11):e1002573, 2016. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002573 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Morantz-Sanchez RM. Sympathy and Science: Women Physicians in American Medicine. Oxford University Press, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Underwood T, Bamman D, and Lee S. The transformation of gender in English-language fiction. Journal of Cultural Analytics, 2018. 10.22148/16.019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Boyle E. The feminization of teaching in America. https://stuff.mit.edu/afs/athena.mit.edu/org/w/wgs/prize/eb04.html, 2004. [Online; accessed 5–May-2017].
  • 63. Davis G and Allison R. Increasing representation, maintaining hierarchy: An assessment of gender and medical specialization. Social Thought and Research, 32:17–45, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Gender Guesser: Guess gender from first name in Python 2 and 3 (version 0.4.0). https://github.com/lead-ratings/gender-guesser, 2016.

Decision Letter 0

Bing Xue

23 Dec 2019

PONE-D-19-30836

Long-term patterns of gender imbalance in an industry without ability or level of interest differences

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Amaral,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Feb 04 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Bing Xue, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements:

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please upload a copy of Figure 1e-f, to which you refer in your text on page 5. If the figure is no longer to be included as part of the submission please remove all reference to it within the text.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: 1) well-written ms

2) very interesting data (data collection, analyses, and presentation are actually really good)

3) Big problem with theoretical background: The authors present numerous gender differences (or, as they call it, „gender imbalances“) and kind of like wonder why they exist. Now, the findings they present are (mostly) the ones that any informed evolutionary psychologist would predict. The authors, of cource, do not need "to like" EP, but, as an influential approach that has produced an abundance of evidence-based research in the past decades within the research of gender, the authors need to, at least, acknowledge the existence of that approach - particularly because it offers a very parsimonious explanation of actual gender differences. Of course, not a single approach can explain everything. So, we need a reconcilation of environmental approaches with biological ones. The authors cite Wuhr et al. In that paper, there is a very well-balanced presentation of different approaches that try to explain gender differences in certain domains, including the media. This is how it should be done. Unfortunately, the authors rely - more or less - on social constructivism instead. Granted, they do seem to have found some cultural factors that influence the studied phenomenon. I am fine with that. And I believe that his paper should be published eventually. However, what they found is only a small part of the whole story. It seems, according to the plethora of available (but not cited…) research, as if there is a very strong effect of binary gender (as predictor) on the production of cultural products (as criterion) with environmental effects (the focus of the paper at hand) being („only“) kind of the moderator variable. Apart from gender, there is a predictable and probably quite strong effect of age as well.

4) Referring to 3) I recommend to the authors to have a look at this literature:

The authors talk about computer software. It might be useful to look at Melzer (2018) and Lange & Schwab (2018).

Generally, the pattern of „gender imbalance“ is a pattern that is found across all sorts of cultural products (nota bene: „cultural“ does not necessarily mean „non-biological“):

Literature: Lange, & Euler (2014); Miller (1999)

Music (Jazz): Miller (1999)

Paintings: Miller (1999)

Religions: Lange et al. (2013)

Scientific discoveries: Kanazawa (2000)

World Records: Lange et al. (2013)

I also recommend: Griskevicius et al. (2006); Hennighausen, & Schwab (2015); Kanazawa (2003); Simonton (e.g., 1975)

Generally, Geoffrey Miller has produced numerous works that might be worthwhile to have a look at. Also, many works by Dean K. Simonton in general are extremely valuable.

I do not recommend to switch to a biological approach. Just be more balanced in terms of how you explain your findings … on - what a coincidence! - gender IMBALANCE.

I am entirely with the auhtors in that we need to do all people justice. So, all imbalances pose a problem. But the same can be applied to scientific theories. The paper at hand is a perfect example of an imbalance in scientific argumentation. It is not all about culture, there are also innate stable differences between people.

The authors should also try to be more differentiated. Comparing patterns of movie actors and actresses with the one for movie producers and directors is not a good one. For most movies, you have at least one female and one male lead. So you necessarily need an actress and an actor. (I have to admit though that some movies only have a male lead, which calls for an explanation.) Contrary, the gender of a director is not pre-determined by such necessities. Also, medicine is not well-suited to be compared to the media. There are many different domains within medicine. One motive within medicine is the nursing aspect (with women, ON AVERAGE, being more prone to). Being a heart surgeon, for instance, is a different story though, as it consists more of technical details (with men, ON AVERAGE, being more prone to; see Su et al., 2009 for gender differences in the preference for people in contrast to the preference for things). Also, just try to think about a different causality here. (I know that you state that you cannot demonstrate causality. But there is clearly an implied causality in the paper at hand.) High-quality research shows that in more egalitarian cultures, gender differences are not decreased but increased – which is exactly the opposite of what social constructivism would predict. Quite obviously (see literature), there is a gender difference in motivation to perform well in numerous domains (and not a gender difference in competence!). So, it is not helping to simply state that „different innate […] abilities“ do not exist. Rather, try to think about innate differences in 1) interests and 2) motivation (to achieve high social status). Individual differences in motivation should not be confused with cultural factors.

Please be also more careful with the use of the term „stereotype“ (see Jussim et al., 2009).

To conclude, please do not get me wrong. This is valuable research that should be published. But first, some quite obvious shortcomings need to be adressed.

Literature cited in this review:

Griskevicius, V., Cialdini, R. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Peacocks, Picasso, and parental investment: The effects of romantic motives on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(1), 63-76.

Hennighausen, C. & Schwab, F. (2015). Evolutionary media psychology and ist epistemological foundation. In T. Breyer (Ed.), Epistemological dimensions of evolutionary psychology (pp. 131-158). New York: Springer.

Jussim, L., Cain, T. R., Crawford, J. T., Harber, K., & Cohen, F. (2009). The unbearable accuracy of stereotypes. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping and discrimination (pp. 199–227). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Kanazawa, S. (2000). Scientific discoveries as cultural displays: A further test of Miller’s courtship model. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 317–321.

Kanazawa, S. (2003). Why productivity fades with age: The crime–genius connection. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(4), 257-272.

Lange, B. P., & Euler, H. A. (2014). Writers have groupies, too: High quality literature production and mating success. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8(1), 20-30. doi:10.1037/h0097246

Lange, B. P., Schwarz, S. & Euler, H. A. (2013). The sexual nature of human culture. The Evolutionary Review: Art, Science, Culture, 4(1), 76-85.

Lange, B. P., & Schwab, F. (2018). Game on: Sex differences in the production and consumption of video games. In J. Breuer, D. Pietschmann, B. Liebold & B. P. Lange (Eds.), Evolutionary psychology and digital games: Digital hunter-gatherers (pp. 193-204). New York, NY: Routledge.

Melzer, A. (2018). Of princesses, paladins, and players: Gender stereotypes in video games. In J. Breuer, D. Pietschmann, B. Liebold & B. P. Lange (Eds.), Evolutionary psychology and digital games: Digital hunter-gatherers (pp. 205-220). New York, NY: Routledge.

Miller, G. F. (1999). Sexual selection for cultural displays. In R. Dunbar, C. Knight, & C. Power (Eds.), The evolution of culture. An interdisciplinary view (pp. 71-91). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

Simonton, D. K. (1975). Age and literary creativity: A cross-cultural and transhistorical survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 6, 259–277.

Su, R., Rounds, J., & Armstrong, P. I. (2009). Men and things, women and people: a meta-analysis of sex differences in interests. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 859–884. doi: 10.1037/a0017364

Reviewer #2: I appreciate the opportunity to review your article, entitled “Long-term patterns of gender imbalance in an industry without ability or level of interest differences.” Indeed, I found your paper interesting and provocative. A clear strength of your paper is, quite obviously, the particularly large dataset. Moreover, I liked your approach to examine the representation of women from different angles (e.g., the representation of women in different roles, such as actors and directors; examining different time periods; the additional analysis on the “Big Seven”). That being said, however, I also had several questions and issues that I would like to raise here. I hope these will be helpful as you further advance this work.

1) I found it somewhat confusing that you started by pointing that “one would expect to see parity,” but then go on to suggest that gender parity is not actually expected (e.g., due to discrimination). Thus, my suggestion is to be more precise here and clarify that certain mechanisms that are sometimes discussed to drive gender differences are unlikely to operate in this context. Thus, this context is interesting because certain mechanisms can be examined in greater isolation.

2a) Page 3, lines 39-40: I agree that it is quite difficult to know “why a SPECIFIC individual discriminates” (emphasis added). However, there is abundant research on the reasons why people discriminate more generally. To give just two examples, both role congruity theory (Eagly & Karau, 2002) and the status incongruity hypothesis (Rudman et al., 2012) help to explain why people engage in discrimination. Going forward, I suggest clarifying this issue. This issue also leads me to raise the following issue (i.e., 2b).

2b) Theoretical depth: I understand that you hinted at different processes in your manuscript (e.g., p. 3, line 41) and, most importantly, I do not want to encourage the practice of “hypothesizing after the results are known” (HARKing; Kerr, 1998). However, I would greatly appreciate more theoretical depth and clear rationales that help to illuminate what was found. Going into this direction could help to strengthen the paper and increase its impact. For instance, it would help to elaborate (in the Discussion section) on the questions of why exactly the consolidation of the studio system as well as the percentage of women producers had such an impact. Certain extant theories (see Issue 2a above) likely help to make sense of the findings.

3) Regression models: As noted in your paper (lines 86-87), “the percentage of females in the producing and directing teams” were predictors. Thus, it is obvious that including these predictors in the models to predict the representation of women as directors and producers is problematic, as this would reflect autocorrelations. I assume this is the reason why the respective estimates for "% female producers" and "% female directors" are not included in certain parts of Figure 3 (and also, for instance, Figure S3). Going forward, please clarify whether or not the model was different when predicting the representation of women as directors and producers specifically.

4) I appreciate your robustness checks. Yet, please explain why you examined “time” by including a number of dummy variables, rather than by including time as a continuous variable.

5) Page 7, line 107: I would like to know what leads you to reason that low statistical power is the reason for the absence of the predicted effects. The sample size generally appears quite large.

Once again, I hope that these comments are helpful, as you are clearly onto something interesting.

References:

Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573-598.

Kerr, N. L. (1998). HARKing: Hypothesizing after the results are known. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 196-217.

Rudman, L. A., Moss-Racusin, C. A., Phelan, J. E., & Nauts, S. (2012). Status incongruity and backlash effects: Defending the gender hierarchy motivates prejudice against female leaders. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 165-179.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Apr 1;15(4):e0229662. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0229662.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


24 Jan 2020

See cover letter for response to reviewers and editors

Decision Letter 1

Bing Xue

7 Feb 2020

PONE-D-19-30836R1

Long-term patterns of gender imbalance in an industry without ability or level of interest differences

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Amaral,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Mar 23 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Bing Xue, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors have reworked their MS considerably. However, sadly, they have mostly ignored (or misunderstood) what I was criticising. They have added Miller (1999) and Lange et al. (2013) – two of many works I had recommended. That is basically good. However, althought they cite some of the suggested works now, R1 of the MS leaves many doubts if they understood what those works are asserting (based on empirical data of course). To make it more concrete, they state (p. 3): „Indeed, there is currently a great deal of controversy surrounding the primacy of evolutionary/biological mechanisms versus environmental/cultural mechanisms in explaining observed gender representation imbalances.“ Correct, nothing to criticise here. They go on: „Even though we pursue here an approach informed by cultural primacy approaches, we believe that our study is able to sidestep the controversy….“ Ok too, although this is were an informed biosocial researcher should get nervous. And indeed, the full sentence goes as follows: „Even though we pursue here an approach informed by cultural primacy approaches, we believe that our study is able to sidestep the controversy because the movie industry provides a case study in which evolutionary mechanisms actually would lead to the expectation that women would be over-represented. Specifically, studies informed by evolutionary psychology, suggest that women have a preference for activities where people and inter-personal interactions are prominent.“ First, how can you sidestep a controvery, while in the very same sentence diving fully into the very same controversy? Secondly and more importantly, the statement is utterly wrong. No informed evolutionary psychologist would predict that! The reference they use for this awkward statement is (no wonder) a non-evolutionary one (Lewis P. and Simpson R. Kanter revisited: Gender, power and (in)visibility. International Journal of Management Reviews, 14:141{158, 2012.). They are so many good references for this topic, and I have invested so much time in my first review mentioning many of them to the authors. This is really disappoiting! I am investing so much time to try to help the authors to end up having a sound piece of science. And this is what happened. �

As I have already explained in my first review: When it comes to gender differences (on average, of course) in the context of „social“ occupations, you need to be more differentiated. The motivation of an average men in the context of „social“ is to strive for social status, that is to climb the social ladder in order to get an upper spot in the social hierarchy. This pattern is cross-culturally universal and also in terms of time very stable. So the most parsimonious explanation for this would be to assume that this pattern is partially due to evolutionary selection pressures. How exactly? Men can benefit from high social status in terms of so-called fitness benefits over-proportionally compared to women of high social status. On the contrary, „social“ in the context of occupations that women are, on average, more interested in is kind of synonymous with nurturing and caring. The authors themselves state: „women have a preference for activities where people and inter-personal interactions are prominent“.

I won’t go into more detail, because (1) I have explained this in my first review already and (2) I have suggested so many works that explain it in detail and support the assertion by strony empirical evidence.

I leave it to the authors to change this part of their paper in order to make it correct or not.

Reviewer #2: I appreciate the opportunity to review a revised version of your manuscript. You have addressed all of my comments thoroughly: My methodological questions were clarified. Moreover, I greatly appreciate the greater theoretical depth of this revision. This way, the potential impact of your paper might be even stronger.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 2

Bing Xue

12 Feb 2020

Long-term patterns of gender imbalance in an industry without ability or level of interest differences

PONE-D-19-30836R2

Dear Dr. Amaral,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

With kind regards,

Bing Xue, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Bing Xue

28 Feb 2020

PONE-D-19-30836R2

Long-term patterns of gender imbalance in an industry without ability or level of interest differences

Dear Dr. Amaral:

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

For any other questions or concerns, please email plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE.

With kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Professor Bing Xue

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. U.S. produced movies considered in our study by genre.

    (PDF)

    S2 Fig. Multivariate logistic (for director and producers) and OLS (for actors and writers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

    We do not include time dummies in our model. We plot the base 10 logarithm of the risk ratio for each factor. The risk factor for female workforce participation was calculated for an increase of 10%. The risk factor for change in real GPD per capita was calculated for an increase of 1%. The risk factor for number of movies was calculated for an increase of 300. The risk factor for industry concentration was calculate for a change of 0.03. The risk factor for budget was calculated for an increase by 100 fold. The risk ratio for percentage female producers was calculated for an increase of 50%. This value is one of the four typical values observed, the other three being 0%, 25% and 100%. The risk factor for percentage female directors was calculated for an increase of 100% since typically there is a single director. Risk ratios for which we are not able to reject the null hypothesis at the 0.01 level are shown in grey. Risk ratios significantly larger than 1 are shown in yellow and those significantly smaller than 1 are shown in purple. If a risk ratio estimate and its confidence bands fall outside the range displayed, we mark this with an arrow. The color of the arrow falls the same convention as the color of the risk ratio estimates.

    (PDF)

    S3 Fig. Multivariate logistic (for director and cinematographer) and OLS (for actors, writers, and producers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

    We do not include time dummies in our model.

    (PDF)

    S4 Fig. Multivariate logistic (for director) and OLS (for actors and writers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

    We consider 1-year time dummies in this case. Note that for directors the maximum number of iterations (35) was exceeded before the convergence criterion was reached. This is due to the time dummies whose estimation uncertainty is enormous and destabilizes the fit. We do not show results for cinematographers because the time dummies make the matrix singular.

    (PDF)

    S5 Fig. Multivariate logistic (for director and cinematographer) and OLS (for actors and writers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

    We consider 1-year time dummies in this case. Note that for directors the maximum number of iterations (35) was exceeded before the convergence criterion was reached. This is due to the time dummies coefficients whose estimation uncertainty is enormous and destabilizes the fit. We do not show results for cinematographers because the time dummies make the matrix singular.

    (PDF)

    S6 Fig. Multivariate logistic (for director and producers) and OLS (for actors and writers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

    We consider 4-year time dummies in this case.

    (PDF)

    S7 Fig. Multivariate logistic (for director and cinematographer) and OLS (for actors and writers) regressions models of female representation reveal associations with percentage female producers and genre.

    We consider 4-year time dummies in this case.

    (PDF)

    S1 File. Filtered movies from AFI database.

    (XLSX)

    S2 File. List of movies matched between AFI and IMDb.

    (PDF)

    S3 File. List of 101 randomly selected movies used to verify the degree of agreement between credited cast lists for AFI and IMDb records of matched movies.

    (XLSX)

    S4 File. List of 101 randomly selected movies used to verify the degree of agreement between cast lists for AFI and IMDb records of matched movies.

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response_to_reviewers.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    The datasets analyzed in this study are available as Supporting Information files and at https://arch.library.northwestern.edu.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES