Skip to main content
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
. 2020 Mar 31;14:45. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00045

Adolescent Ethanol Exposure: Anxiety-Like Behavioral Alterations, Ethanol Intake, and Sensitivity

Trevor T Towner 1,*, Elena I Varlinskaya 1
PMCID: PMC7136472  PMID: 32296315

Abstract

Adolescence is a developmental period associated with rapid age-specific physiological, neural, and hormonal changes. Behaviorally, human adolescents are characterized by age-typical increases in novelty-seeking and risk-taking, including the frequent initiation of alcohol and drug use. Alcohol use typically begins during early adolescence, and older adolescents often report high levels of alcohol consumption, commonly referred to as high-intensity drinking. Early-onset and heavy drinking during adolescence are associated with an increased risk of developing alcohol use disorders later in life. Yet, long-term behavioral consequences of adolescent alcohol use that might contribute to excessive drinking in adulthood are still not well understood. Recent animal research, however, using different exposure regimens and routes of ethanol administration, has made substantial progress in identifying the consequences of adolescent ethanol exposure that last into adulthood. Alterations associated with adolescent ethanol exposure include increases in anxiety-like behavior, impulsivity, risk-taking, and ethanol intake, although the observed alterations differ as a function of exposure regimens and routes of ethanol administration. Rodent studies have also shown that adolescent ethanol exposure produces alterations in sensitivity to ethanol, with these alterations reminiscent of adolescent-typical ethanol responsiveness. The goal of this mini-review article is to summarize the current state of animal research, focusing on the long-term consequences related to adolescent ethanol exposure, with a special emphasis on the behavioral alterations and changes to ethanol sensitivity that can foster high levels of drinking in adulthood.

Keywords: adolescence, alcohol, adolescent ethanol exposure, anxiety, ethanol intake, ethanol sensitivity

Introduction

Initiation of alcohol use is commonly reported during early (11–15 years of age) adolescence (Faden, 2006; Masten et al., 2009; Morean et al., 2018), and this early initiation is frequently associated with the development of alcohol abuse/dependence later in life (Kuntsche et al., 2016). Several researchers have reported that adolescents who begin drinking at or before the age of 14 are at an elevated risk of becoming alcohol-dependent compared to those who initiate alcohol use at the age of 19 or later (DeWit et al., 2000; Ehlers et al., 2006; Dawson et al., 2008). Likewise, a fast progression from first drink to the first intoxication is a strong predictor and indicator of binge and high-intensity drinking among adolescents (Morean et al., 2014, 2018; Kuntsche et al., 2016; Patrick et al., 2019).

The developing adolescent brain is thought to be particularly vulnerable to alcohol (Olsson et al., 2016; Silveri et al., 2016; Spear, 2018). Thus, early initiation of alcohol use together with high levels of drinking during adolescence (Patrick et al., 2013) can potentially disrupt maturational changes occurring in the brain (Blakemore, 2012; Mills et al., 2014). Therefore, investigations of the consequences of ethanol exposure on the adolescent brain and behavior are critical for understanding the relationship between adolescent drinking and the development of alcohol use disorders later in life.

Rodent models of adolescence allow researchers to determine the consequences of adolescent ethanol exposure that may contribute to the development of alcohol use disorders in adulthood. Behaviorally, consequences of adolescent alcohol exposure include reductions in cognitive flexibility, as well as increases in risk-taking and anxiety, which are associated with multiple neural alterations reviewed in several recent publications (Pascual et al., 2009; Crews et al., 2016, 2019; Spear, 2018). The focus of this mini-review article is on the specific consequences of adolescent alcohol exposure that might foster high levels of ethanol intake later in life and, therefore, become risk factors for the development of alcohol use disorders.

Adolescent Ethanol Exposure: Impact on Ethanol Intake in Adulthood

Different animal models of ethanol exposure have been used to determine whether adolescent experience with ethanol influences subsequent intake. Voluntary ethanol consumption in laboratory rodents is commonly assessed using a two-bottle choice (2BC) paradigm, in which animals are given free access to water and ethanol, with this paradigm also used for adolescent ethanol exposure. Intake levels in the 2BC paradigm generally do not produce blood ethanol concentrations (BECs) in the binge range (80 mg/dl and higher), but rather in the low-to-moderate range. The original 2BC paradigm has been modified to increase ethanol consumption, via models such as the drinking in the dark (DID) paradigm (reviewed in Thiele and Navarro, 2014) and scheduled high-alcohol consumption (SHAC) procedure (Finn et al., 2005). These modified voluntary consumption paradigms implement a restricted time of access to ethanol that allows for elevated ethanol intake resulting in higher BECs, similar to those achieved by forced exposure. Indeed, studies focused on ethanol exposures that produce BECs well into the binge range (100–250 mg/dl) use forced ethanol administration via intragastric gavage (IG), intraperitoneal injections (IP), and ethanol vapor inhalation (VI). Findings from studies evaluating the effects of adolescent ethanol exposure on later ethanol intake are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Effects of adolescent ethanol exposure on ethanol intake.

Exposure
Strain Sex Route and Dose Pattern Timing and Duration (days) Test Results Reference
Sprague–Dawley rats Male IP 3.0 g/kg Intermittent P30–43 (14) P45–58 (14) 2BC Operant SA Increased intake after P30–43 exposure Alaux-Cantin et al. (2013)
Male IP 4.0 g/kg Intermittent P24–33 (10) P69–78 (10) 2BC No change in intake Broadwater et al. (2011)
Male Female 1 bottle with 10% EtOH in SS Intermittent P28–42 (15) 30-min access to 10% sweet EtOH Increased intake of a familiar solution during initial sessions Broadwater et al. (2013)
Male Single bottle with fade on to 20% EtOH Continuous P35–250 (215) P75–290 (215) Continuous single bottle access Increased intake for adolescent-onset animals Fernandez et al. (2016)
Male Female IG 1.5, 3.0, or 5.0 g/kg Intermittent P28–45 (18) 2BC Increased intake Maldonado-Devincci et al. (2010)
Male IP 2.0 g/kg Intermittent P28–41 (14) 2BC Increased intake Pandey et al. (2015)
Male IP 2.0 g/kg Intermittent P28–41 (14) 2BC Increased intake Sakharkar et al. (2019)
Male VI Consecutive P30–39 (10) Free choice operant No change in intake Slawecki and Betancourt (2002)
Male Female IG 3.5 g/kg Intermittent P25–45 (21) Social drinking No change in intake Varlinskaya et al. (2017)
Male 2BC 10% EtOH Continuous P27–90 (64) P70–90 (20) 2BC No change in intake Vetter et al. (2007)
not stated 2BC 5% EtOH sweetened Variable P22–50 (29) Operant SA No change in intake Williams et al. (2018)
Wistar rats Male 2BC 20% EtOH Intermittent P26–59 (34) P92–125 (34) Operant SA Increased SA regardless of exposure age Amodeo et al. (2017)
Male Female 2BC 20% EtOH + VI Variable P22–67 (46) 2BC Increased intake Amodeo et al. (2018)
Male 1 bottle 5% EtOH Intermittent P40–90 (51) 2BC Increased intake Blomeyer et al. (2013)
Male 2BC 10% EtOH + VI Variable P29–100 (72) 2BC Increased intake Criado and Ehlers (2013)
Female 3BC 5 and 20% EtOH Continuous P31–87 (56) P71–127 (56) 3BC Adult onset consumed more ethanol than adolescent onset Fullgrabe et al. (2007)
Male Female 4BC 5, 10, and 20% EtOH Continuous P19–28 (10) P28–37 (10) P90–99 (10) 4BC No difference in intake García-Burgos et al. (2009)
Male 1 bottle of 5% sweetened EtOH Operant SA IP 2 g/kg Variable P27–39 (13) P28–42 (14) Single bottle 30-min access or operant SA No difference in adult intake following voluntary ethanol consumption; decrease in adult intake following adolescent ethanol exposure via injection Gilpin et al. (2012)
Male 1 bottle 8% EtOH Consecutive during the dark phase (12 h/day) P51–58 (7) 2BC Increased intake Milivojevic and Covault (2013)
Male IP 3.0 g/kg Intermittent P25–38 (14) 2BC (24 h and limited access) Increased intake Pascual et al. (2009)
Female 2BC 15% EtOH Continuous P30–60 (30) Operant SA Increased acquisition Rodd-Henricks et al. (2002)
Male 3BC 5 and 20% EtOH Continuous P31–71 (40) 3BC Adult-onset consumed more ethanol than adolescent-onset Siegmund et al. (2005)
Female 2BC 15% EtOH Continuous P30–60 (30) Operant SA Increased acquisition Toalston et al. (2015)
Male Traverse runway to have free access to varying % + yoked controls Consecutive P29–54 (25) 2BC Adolescents that navigated runway to earn ethanol reward had increased intake in adulthood compared to yoked controls Walker and Ehlers (2009)
Long-Evans rats Male VI Intermittent P28–42 (14) Operant SA Increased intake Gass et al. (2014)
Male 2BC 20% EtOH Intermittent P23–56 (24) 2BC No changes in intake Moaddab et al. (2017)
Male VI Intermittent P28–44 (16) 2BC Operant SA No changes in intake Nentwig et al. (2019)
Male Female 1 bottle 5% or 10% EtOH Continuous P21–70 (50) 2BC Male mice increased intake; Female mice decreased intake Siciliano and Smith (2001)
C57 mice Male 2BC 15% EtOH+ VI Variable P30–57 (28) P70–97 (28) 2BC Increased intake but no effect of age of exposure Carrara-Nascimento et al. (2013)
Male Female SHAC 5% EtOH Limited intermittent P28–49 (21) P56–77 (21) 2BC No changes in intake Cozzoli et al. (2014)
Male 2BC 10% EtOH Continuous P28–49 (21) P42–63 (21) 2BC No changes in intake Hefner and Holmes (2007)
Male 2BC 10% EtOH Continuous P24–112 (88) P56–112 (56) 2BC Increased intake with adolescent onset of ethanol consumption Ho et al. (1989)
Female VI Consecutive P28–56 (28) P56–84 (28) 2BC No changes in intake Jury et al. (2017)
Male 2BC 15% EtOH+ VI Variable P28–70 (42) P56–98 (42) 2BC No change in intake for adolescent exposed; adult CIE exposed increased intake Jury et al. (2017)
Male 4BC-DID 5, 10, 20, and 40% EtOH Consecutive limited 2-h access P28–41 (14) P56–69 (14) DID Increased intake following adolescent ethanol exposure Lee et al. (2017)
Male Female DID 20% EtOH Consecutive limited 2 h access P28–42 (14) DID Increased intake Moore et al. (2010)
Male Female SHAC 5% EtOH Limited intermittent P26–47 (21) P58–79 (21) DID 2BC No difference in intake during DID; increased intake for both sexes during the 2BC following adolescent exposure Strong et al. (2010)
BALB Male Female 2BC 10% EtOH; 1 bottle 10% EtOH; Gradual 0.5–10% EtOH Continuous P35–84 (49) 2BC BALB/cByJ increased intake regardless of method BALB/cJ increased intake following gradual concentration change Blizard et al. (2004)
Male Female IG 2.2 g/kg Consecutive P22–25 (4) DID Increased intake Jacobsen et al. (2018)
HS/Ibg (HAP2 or WSC1) Male Female 2BC 10% EtOH Continuous P28–42 (14) P60–74 (14) 2BC Increased intake over initial test days but no effect of age of exposure O’Tousa et al. (2013)
Male Female 2BC 6% EtOH Continuous P28–84 (56) P70–126 (56) 2BC Increased intake but no effect of age of exposure Tambour et al. (2008)

EtOH, Ethanol; 2BC, Two bottle choice; 3BC, Three bottle choice; 4BC, Four bottle choice; IP, Intraperitoneal; IG, Intragastric Gavage; VI, Vapor Inhalation; SS, Super sac; DID, Drinking in the Dark; SHAC, Scheduled High Alcohol Consumption; SA, Self-Administration. *Testing took place in adolescence for the adolescent exposed group. The bold text in the table represents results in which increases in ethanol consumption were found.

More than half of the studies outlined in Table 1 demonstrate that adolescent ethanol exposure results in persistent increases in ethanol intake in adulthood, findings across multiple models of exposure and intake assessment methods. For example, although Blomeyer et al. (2013) exposed male, Wistar rats via a 2BC paradigm during mid-adolescence, and Pandey et al. (2015) administered ethanol IP (2 g/kg) in male, Sprague–Dawley rats, both studies demonstrated higher intakes in ethanol-exposed animals relative to their age-matched controls. Similarly, Gass et al. (2014) reported increased ethanol intake in adulthood following adolescent VI exposure. Some studies have also shown increased intake in adulthood following a combination of voluntary (2BC) and forced (VI) ethanol exposures (Criado and Ehlers, 2013; Amodeo et al., 2018). However, 19 of the studies reviewed found either no effects of adolescent ethanol exposure, more pronounced effects of adult exposure on ethanol intake, or an equivalent response following adolescent and adult exposures (see Table 1).

Several factors potentially contribute to these inconsistent findings, with exposure duration and exposure timing representing two important variables that vary drastically between the different studies. Throughout the reviewed literature, exposure duration ranged from 4 days (Jacobsen et al., 2018) to as long as 8 weeks (Fullgrabe et al., 2007) during adolescence and in some cases well into adulthood (Fernandez et al., 2016). The results of the only study that directly addressed the issue of exposure timing within the adolescent period on ethanol intake later in life demonstrated that early adolescent males (postnatal days 30–43) are more vulnerable to ethanol-exposure-related increases in ethanol intake than their more mature (postnatal days 45–58) adolescent counterparts (Alaux-Cantin et al., 2013).

Only a third of the studies (5 out of 16) that used voluntary ethanol consumption, such as 2BC or similar models of adolescent exposure, demonstrated increased ethanol intake later in life. Very few studies reported BEC data during the voluntary consumption exposure phase, and those that did generally observed low BECs that ranged from 0 to 100 mg/dl with averages around 25–35 mg/dl (Gilpin et al., 2012; Broadwater et al., 2013; Amodeo et al., 2017; however see O’Tousa et al., 2013). However, findings of studies implementing voluntary consumption in which higher BECs (80–200 mg/dl) were achieved, commonly from DID and SHAC procedures, generally supported elevated intake levels in adulthood. Of the studies that report increased ethanol intake in adulthood, forced adolescent exposure appears to more effectively increase ethanol intake than voluntary exposure models. Indeed, more than half (11 out of 19) of the studies that employed forced exposure paradigms reported exposure-related increases in ethanol intake. Together, these findings suggest that to enhance ethanol intake later in life, adolescent exposure models, both voluntary and forced, should produce BECs well into the binge range.

Many studies that reported increases in ethanol intake following adolescent ethanol exposure did not include other age groups for comparison. Of the studies that included adolescents and adults, previous exposure to ethanol tended to increased ethanol intake later in life regardless of exposure timing (Hefner and Holmes, 2007; Tambour et al., 2008; Strong et al., 2010; Carrara-Nascimento et al., 2013; O’Tousa et al., 2013; Amodeo et al., 2017) or following adult exposure only (Fullgrabe et al., 2007; Jury et al., 2017), suggesting that exposure-related increases in ethanol consumption may not be specific to adolescent exposure. Therefore, it is still not clear whether adolescents are more vulnerable than adults to ethanol-exposure-related increases in ethanol intake.

The studies conducted to date indicate that many factors contribute to the effects of adolescent ethanol exposure on ethanol intake later in life, including exposure regiments (continuous vs. intermittent), exposure duration, exposure mode (voluntary vs. forced), exposure levels, BECs achieved, and strain. Given their respective effects and contributions, all these factors should be considered in future studies.

Ethanol Sensitivity Following Adolescent Ethanol Exposure

In general, adolescent laboratory rodents are less responsive than their adult counterparts to adverse effects of ethanol that may curb ethanol intake. These adverse effects of ethanol include social inhibition (Varlinskaya and Spear, 2002), sedation (Moy et al., 1998; Silveri and Spear, 1998; Draski et al., 2001), motor impairment (White et al., 2002; Ramirez and Spear, 2010), and aversion (Vetter-O’Hagen et al., 2009; Anderson et al., 2010; Schramm-Sapyta et al., 2014; Saalfield and Spear, 2015, 2019). In contrast, adolescent rats are uniquely responsive to social facilitation induced by low doses of ethanol (Varlinskaya and Spear, 2002, 2007, 2015; Trezza et al., 2009; Willey et al., 2009), with some evidence also suggesting higher responsiveness to the rewarding effects of ethanol during adolescence than in adulthood (Pautassi et al., 2008). Adolescent ethanol exposure produces alterations in responsiveness to ethanol that resemble these adolescent-typical ethanol sensitivities. This retention of adolescent-typical responding to ethanol has been termed as the “lock-in” effect of adolescent ethanol exposure (reviewed in Spear and Swartzwelder, 2014). The “locking in” of adolescent-typical responding to ethanol effects may play a substantial role in increased ethanol intake in adulthood following adolescent exposure.

Effects of chronic adolescent exposure to ethanol on ethanol-induced sedation indexed via the loss of the righting reflex (LORR) have been assessed in laboratory rodents. For instance, adult male rats exposed to ethanol during adolescence (P30–48, 1, 2, 3 or 4 g/kg ethanol, IP) and challenged with a hypnotic ethanol dose regained their righting reflex more rapidly than did their non-exposed counterparts (Matthews et al., 2008), with these alterations evident only following high exposure doses of ethanol (3 and 4 g/kg). These results were also replicated by the same group (Matthews et al., 2017) and others using mice (Jury et al., 2017). However, similar decreases in LORR duration were evident following adult exposure as well (Jury et al., 2017). These findings suggest that adolescent ethanol exposure results in relative insensitivity to ethanol-induced sedation, although the development of metabolic tolerance to ethanol cannot be ruled out. Indeed, the development of metabolic tolerance has been reported following adolescent ethanol exposure (Silvers et al., 2003). When adolescent and adult male rats were repeatedly exposed to an ethanol dose of 4 g/kg and challenged with the same dose 24 h after the last exposure, adult rats, but not their adolescent counterparts, demonstrated chronic tolerance to the sedative effects of ethanol that appeared to be metabolic, but not functional (Broadwater et al., 2011). Evidence of decreased sensitivity to ethanol-induced sedation associated with adolescent ethanol exposure came from the study of Quoilin et al. (2012): female Swiss mice exposed to ethanol during adolescence regained the righting reflex at higher BECs than controls.

Adult animals exposed to ethanol during adolescence become relatively insensitive to the aversive effects of ethanol assessed via ethanol-induced conditioned taste aversion (CTA). Diaz-Granados and Graham (2007) exposed adolescent male mice to ethanol vapor either continuously or intermittently and found attenuated CTA to ethanol later in life, with intermittent exposure producing greater attenuation and adult exposure not producing similar effects. Saalfield and Spear (2015), assessing the impact of ethanol exposure (4.0 g/kg, IG) during early (P25–45) and late (P45–65) adolescence on ethanol-induced CTA in male rats, found that both adolescent exposures resulted in decreased sensitivity to the aversive effects of ethanol. Alaux-Cantin et al. (2013) also found that male rats exposed to ethanol during early adolescence (3 g/kg, IP, P30–43) demonstrated attenuated ethanol-induced CTA in adulthood. The reductions in sensitivity to ethanol CTA following adolescent ethanol exposure appear to be sex-specific, with only male Long-Evans rats, but not females, demonstrating an attenuated CTA in adulthood following adolescent exposure (Sherrill et al., 2011).

Adolescent ethanol exposure (P25–45, IG, 4 g/kg) of Sprague–Dawley male rats resulted in precipitation of adolescent-typical responding to acute ethanol challenge with social facilitation (i.e., ethanol-induced increases in peer-directed social behavior) when these males were tested in adulthood (Varlinskaya et al., 2014). Enhanced sensitivity to ethanol reinforcement indexed via a significant leftward shift in the dose-response curve for ethanol self-administration into the posterior ventral tegmental area following adolescent ethanol exposure (4 g/kg IG, P28–48) was also evident in adult male and female Wistar rats, as well as in alcohol-preferring (P) male rats (Hauser et al., 2019). Carrara-Nascimento et al. (2014) showed that adult male Swiss mice exposed to ethanol during adolescence displayed a robust CPP to 2.0 g/kg ethanol, whereas adult exposure decreased sensitivity to the reinforcing properties of ethanol. Similarly, BALB/c adult mice demonstrated enhanced sensitivity to ethanol-induced CPP following only four exposures to ethanol given during the juvenile period on P22–25 (Jacobsen et al., 2018).

Taken together, the experimental findings demonstrate that exposure to ethanol during adolescence changes sensitivity to many ethanol effects later in life, decreasing sensitivity to adverse effects of ethanol and making adult laboratory rodents more sensitive to stimulatory and rewarding properties of ethanol. This pattern of sensitivity to the adverse and desired ethanol effects, reminiscent of that typically shown by adolescent rodents, may allow adult animals to ingest higher amounts of ethanol without experiencing negative consequences.

Adolescent Alcohol Exposure: Anxiety-Like Behavioral Alterations

Adolescents and young adults who engage in problematic drinking often drink for enhancement of positive emotional states or alleviation of negative affective states (Ham and Hope, 2003; Kuntsche et al., 2006). The association between negative reinforcement and alcohol use has been shown to become stronger in individuals with alcohol use disorder, with no changes evident in the association between positive reinforcement and alcohol consumption (Cho et al., 2019). Indeed, available research suggests relatively strong associations between adolescent alcohol use and increased prevalence of anxiety and depression disorders in adulthood (Rohde et al., 2001; Jeanblanc, 2015). In older adults, alcohol use disorder is frequently comorbid with depression and anxiety (Vorspan et al., 2015; Wiener et al., 2018). Therefore, the assessment of affective behavioral alterations in animal models of adolescent alcohol exposure seems utterly important.

Increases in anxiety-like behavior have been reported using different models of adolescent ethanol exposure. For instance, adult male Sprague–Dawley rats exposed to 2 g/kg ethanol (Kokare et al., 2017; Kyzar et al., 2017, 2019; Sakharkar et al., 2019) or 4 g/kg ethanol (Van Skike et al., 2015) given IP during adolescence, as well as male Long-Evans rats exposed IG to a 1.5 g/kg ethanol dose (Loxton and Canales, 2017), demonstrated elevated levels of anxiety-like behavior when tested on the elevated plus-maze (EPM). Our recent findings indicated that IG ethanol exposure of Sprague–Dawley males and females during early/mid-adolescence (P25–45) results in enhanced anxiety-like behavior on the EPM in adulthood regardless of sex (Varlinskaya et al., 2019). However, only males demonstrated enhanced anxiety-like behavior on the EPM following late-adolescent/emerging-adulthood exposure (P45–65), suggesting that the effects of ethanol exposure are sex- and exposure-timing dependent.

However, Torcaso et al. (2017), exposing male Wistar rats IG to ethanol (3 g/kg, P37–44) reported no behavioral changes on the EPM, whereas other researchers have demonstrated that adolescent ethanol exposure resulted in decreases of anxiety-like behavior on the EPM (Gilpin et al., 2012; Gass et al., 2014). For example, Long-Evans male rats were intermittently exposed to ethanol via VI during early-mid adolescence (P28–P42), and this exposure regimen resulted in decreased anxiety, as indexed by increased open arm behavior evident in adult rats (Gass et al., 2014). Similarly, adolescent (P28–P42) ethanol exposure of male Wistar rats via self-administration increased percent open arm time on the EPM when these males were tested in adulthood (Gilpin et al., 2012). These inconsistent results are likely associated with procedural differences such as rat strain, route of ethanol administration, phase of light/dark cycle during testing, and pre-test manipulations (see Hogg, 1996; Carobrez and Bertoglio, 2005). For example, the two studies that reported decreased anxiety-like behavior on the EPM tested animals in low-light conditions during the dark part of the light/dark cycle, conditions that may reduce anxiety-like behavior (Gilpin et al., 2012; Gass et al., 2014). It is possible that the observed increases in open arm entries and/or open arm time reflect disinhibition but not decreases in anxiety-like behavior, since the characteristics of the test situation determine whether anxiety or disinhibition are manifested in the EPM (Ennaceur, 2014).

Anxiety-like alterations associated with adolescent ethanol exposure were reported for other tests of anxiety as well. Experiments using the light/dark box have shown that adolescent ethanol exposure increases time spent in the dark portion of the apparatus and decreases entries into the light side in male Sprague–Dawley rats (Pandey et al., 2015; Vetreno et al., 2016; Sakharkar et al., 2019). Anxiety-like behavior in the open-field (Coleman et al., 2014; Yan et al., 2015) and marble-burying test (Lee et al., 2017) have also been enhanced following adolescent ethanol exposure of Sprague–Dawley males (Yan et al., 2015) and male C57BL/6J mice (Coleman et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2017). When tested in adulthood, male Sprague–Dawley rats exposed to ethanol during early-mid adolescence demonstrated social anxiety-like behavioral alterations indexed via decreases in social investigation and social preference (Varlinskaya et al., 2014), with no changes in social behavior evident following late adolescent ethanol exposure.

Increased anxiety-like behavior following adolescent ethanol exposure may also contribute to increases in ethanol intake due to the anxiolytic properties of ethanol. Although links between anxiety and alcohol consumption have been commonly reported in humans (Vorspan et al., 2015) and laboratory rodents (Pelloux et al., 2015), it remains to be investigated whether animals that demonstrate increases in anxiety following adolescent ethanol exposure drink more ethanol for its negatively reinforcing, anxiolytic effects.

Conclusions

Although adolescent alcohol exposure is associated with behavioral alterations and changes in ethanol sensitivity, it is still not clear whether these alterations contribute to increases in ethanol intake. Considering that very few studies assessing changes in ethanol intake following adolescent ethanol exposure included both sexes, the question of whether responding to ethanol exposure during adolescence differs in males and females remains unanswered. Furthermore, among the studies that assessed changes in ethanol intake, only a limited number included both adolescent and adult ethanol exposure conditions (see Table 1), producing mixed results and not allowing to conclude that enhanced ethanol intake in adulthood is specific to adolescent ethanol exposure. The impact of ethanol exposure timing within the adolescent period (i.e., during early vs. late adolescence) on ethanol intake and sensitivity is still not well understood, and this important issue should also be addressed in future studies (Spear, 2015). The relative insensitivity of adolescents to the acute effects of ethanol is related in part to age differences in compensatory responses, including acute tolerance, that serve to counteract ethanol-induced impairment. Therefore, it is important to investigate whether adolescent ethanol exposure decreases sensitivity to the adverse effects of acute ethanol by enhancing the development of acute tolerance. More studies are needed for a better understanding of the consequences of alcohol exposure during adolescence that might contribute to heavy drinking later in life and put individuals at risk for the development of alcohol use disorders.

Author Contributions

TT and EV contributed equally.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Funding. The work presented in this manuscript was funded by grants U01 AA019972 (Neurobiology of Adolescent Drinking in Adulthood Consortium—NADIA Project), P50 AA017823 (Developmental Exposure Alcohol Research Center), and T32 AA025606 (Development and Neuroadaptation in Alcohol and Addictions—DNAA Project).

References

  1. Alaux-Cantin S., Warnault V., Legastelois R., Botia B., Pierrefiche O., Vilpoux C., et al. (2013). Alcohol intoxications during adolescence increase motivation for alcohol in adult rats and induce neuroadaptations in the nucleus accumbens. Neuropharmacology 67, 521–531. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amodeo L. R., Kneiber D., Wills D. N., Ehlers C. L. (2017). Alcohol drinking during adolescence increases consumptive responses to alcohol in adulthood in Wistar rats. Alcohol 59, 43–51. 10.1016/j.alcohol.2016.12.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amodeo L. R., Wills D. N., Sanchez-Alavez M., Nguyen W., Conti B., Ehlers C. L. (2018). Intermittent voluntary ethanol consumption combined with ethanol vapor exposure during adolescence increases drinking and alters other behaviors in adulthood in female and male rats. Alcohol 73, 57–66. 10.1016/j.alcohol.2018.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anderson R. I., Varlinskaya E. I., Spear L. P. (2010). Ethanol-induced conditioned taste aversion in male Sprague-Dawley rats: impact of age and stress. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 34, 2106–2115. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01307.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blakemore S. J. (2012). Development of the social brain in adolescence. J. R. Soc. Med. 105, 111–116. 10.1258/jrsm.2011.110221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Blizard D. A., Vandenbergh D. J., Jefferson A. L., Chatlos C. D., Vogler G. P., McClearn G. E. (2004). Effects of periadolescent ethanol exposure on alcohol preference in two BALB substrains. Alcohol 34, 177–185. 10.1016/j.alcohol.2004.08.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blomeyer D., Friemel C. M., Buchmann A. F., Banaschewski T., Laucht M., Schneider M. (2013). Impact of pubertal stage at first drink on adult drinking behavior. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 37, 1804–1811. 10.1111/acer.12154 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Broadwater M., Varlinskaya E. I., Spear L. P. (2011). Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure in early adolescent and adult male rats: effects on tolerance, social behavior, and ethanol intake. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 35, 1392–1403. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01474.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Broadwater M., Varlinskaya E. I., Spear L. P. (2013). Effects of voluntary access to sweetened ethanol during adolescence on intake in adulthood. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 37, 1048–1055. 10.1111/acer.12049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Carobrez A. P., Bertoglio L. J. (2005). Ethological and temporal analyses of anxiety-like behavior: the elevated plus-maze model 20 years on. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 1193–1205. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carrara-Nascimento P. F., Lopez M. F., Becker H. C., Olive M. F., Camarini R. (2013). Similar ethanol drinking in adolescent and adult C57BL/6J mice after chronic ethanol exposure and withdrawal. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 37, 961–968. 10.1111/acer.12056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carrara-Nascimento P. F., Olive M. F., Camarini R. (2014). Ethanol pre-exposure during adolescence or adulthood increases ethanol intake but ethanol-induced conditioned place preference is enhanced only when pre-exposure occurs in adolescence. Dev. Psychobiol. 56, 36–48. 10.1002/dev.21089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cho S. B., Su J., Kuo S. I., Bucholz K. K., Chan G., Edenberg H. J., et al. (2019). Positive and negative reinforcement are differentially associated with alcohol consumption as a function of alcohol dependence. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 33, 58–68. 10.1037/adb0000436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Coleman L. G., Jr., Liu W., Oguz I., Styner M., Crews F. T. (2014). Adolescent binge ethanol treatment alters adult brain regional volumes, cortical extracellular matrix protein and behavioral flexibility. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 116, 142–151. 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.11.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cozzoli D. K., Strong-Kaufman M. N., Tanchuck M. A., Hashimoto J. G., Wiren K. M., Finn D. A. (2014). The effect of mGluR5 antagonism during binge drinking on subsequent ethanol intake in C57BL/6J mice: sex- and age-induced differences. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 38, 730–738. 10.1111/acer.12292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Crews F. T., Robinson D. L., Chandler L. J., Ehlers C. L., Mulholland P. J., Pandey S. C., et al. (2019). Mechanisms of persistent neurobiological changes following adolescent alcohol exposure: NADIA consortium findings. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 43, 1806–1822. 10.1111/acer.14154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Crews F. T., Vetreno R. P., Broadwater M. A., Robinson D. L. (2016). Adolescent alcohol exposure persistently impacts adult neurobiology and behavior. Pharmacol. Rev. 68, 1074–1109. 10.1124/pr.115.012138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Criado J. R., Ehlers C. L. (2013). Effects of adolescent onset voluntary drinking followed by ethanol vapor exposure on subsequent ethanol consumption during protracted withdrawal in adult Wistar rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 103, 622–630. 10.1016/j.pbb.2012.10.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dawson D. A., Goldstein R. B., Chou S. P., Ruan W. J., Grant B. F. (2008). Age at first drink and the first incidence of adult-onset DSM-IV alcohol use disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 32, 2149–2160. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00806.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. DeWit D. J., Adlaf E. M., Offord D. R., Ogborne A. C. (2000). Age at first alcohol use: a risk factor for the development of alcohol disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 745–750. 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.745 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Diaz-Granados J. L., Graham D. L. (2007). The effects of continuous and intermittent ethanol exposure in adolesence on the aversive properties of ethanol during adulthood. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 31, 2020–2027. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00534.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Draski L. J., Bice P. J., Deitrich R. A. (2001). Developmental alterations of ethanol sensitivity in selectively bred high and low alcohol sensitive rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 70, 387–396. 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00621-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ehlers C. L., Slutske W. S., Gilder D. A., Lau P., Wilhelmsen K. C. (2006). Age at first intoxication and alcohol use disorders in Southwest California Indians. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 30, 1856–1865. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00222.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Ennaceur A. (2014). Tests of unconditioned anxiety—pitfalls and disappointments. Physiol. Behav. 135, 55–71. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.05.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Faden V. B. (2006). Trends in initiation of alcohol use in the United States 1975 to 2003. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 30, 1011–1022. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00115.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fernandez G. M., Stewart W. N., Savage L. M. (2016). Chronic drinking during adolescence predisposes the adult rat for continued heavy drinking: Neurotrophin and behavioral adaptation after long-term, continuous ethanol exposure. PLoS One 11:e0149987. 10.1371/journal.pone.0149987 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Finn D. A., Belknap J. K., Cronise K., Yoneyama N., Murillo A., Crabbe J. C. (2005). A procedure to produce high alcohol intake in mice. Psychopharmacology 178, 471–480. 10.1007/s00213-004-2039-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fullgrabe M. W., Vengeliene V., Spanagel R. (2007). Influence of age at drinking onset on the alcohol deprivation effect and stress-induced drinking in female rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 86, 320–326. 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. García-Burgos D., González F., Manrique T., Gallo M. (2009). Patterns of ethanol intake in preadolescent, adolescent, and adult Wistar rats under acquisition, maintenance, and relapse-like conditions. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 33, 722–728. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00889.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gass J. T., Glen W. B., Jr., McGonigal J. T., Trantham-Davidson H., Lopez M. F., Randall P. K., et al. (2014). Adolescent alcohol exposure reduces behavioral flexibility, promotes disinhibition, and increases resistance to extinction of ethanol self-administration in adulthood. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 2570–2583. 10.1038/npp.2014.109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gilpin N. W., Karanikas C. A., Richardson H. N. (2012). Adolescent binge drinking leads to changes in alcohol drinking, anxiety, and amygdalar corticotropin releasing factor cells in adulthood in male rats. PLoS One 7:e31466. 10.1371/journal.pone.0031466 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ham L. S., Hope D. A. (2003). College students and problematic drinking: a review of the literature. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 23, 719–759. 10.1016/s0272-7358(03)00071-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hauser S. R., Knight C. P., Truitt W. A., Waeiss R. A., Holt I. S., Carvajal G. B., et al. (2019). Adolescent intermittent ethanol increases the sensitivity to the reinforcing properties of ethanol and the expression of select cholinergic and dopaminergic genes within the posterior ventral tegmental area. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 43, 1937–1948. 10.1111/acer.14150 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hefner K., Holmes A. (2007). An investigation of the behavioral actions of ethanol across adolescence in mice. Psychopharmacology 191, 311–322. 10.1007/s00213-006-0646-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ho A., Chin A. J., Dole V. P. (1989). Early experience and the consumption of alcohol by adult C57BL/6J mice. Alcohol 6, 511–515. 10.1016/0741-8329(89)90060-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hogg S. (1996). A review of the validity and variability of the elevated plus-maze as an animal model of anxiety. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 54, 21–30. 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02126-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Jacobsen J. H. W., Buisman-Pijlman F. T., Mustafa S., Rice K. C., Hutchinson M. R. (2018). Antagonising TLR4-TRIF signalling before or after a low-dose alcohol binge during adolescence prevents alcohol drinking but not seeking behaviour in adulthood. Neuropharmacology 128, 460–473. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2017.09.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jeanblanc J. M. (2015). Comorbidity between psychiatric diseases and alcohol use disorders: impact of adolescent alcohol consumption. Curr. Addict. Rep. 2, 239–301. 10.1007/s40429-015-0076-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Jury N. J., Pollack G. A., Ward M. J., Bezek J. L., Ng A. J., Pinard C. R., et al. (2017). Chronic ethanol during adolescence impacts corticolimbic dendritic spines and behavior. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 41, 1298–1308. 10.1111/acer.13422 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kokare D. M., Kyzar E. J., Zhang H., Sakharkar A. J., Pandey S. C. (2017). Adolescent alcohol exposure-induced changes in α-melanocyte stimulating hormone and neuropeptide Y pathways via histone acetylation in the brain during adulthood. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20, 758–768. 10.1093/ijnp/pyx041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kuntsche E., Knibbe R., Gmel G., Engels R. (2006). Who drinks and why? A review of socio-demographic, personality, and contextual issues behind the drinking motives in young people. Addict. Behav. 31, 1844–1857. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.12.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kuntsche E., Rossow I., Engels R., Kuntsche S. (2016). Is ‘age at first drink’ a useful concept in alcohol research and prevention? We doubt that. Addiction 111, 957–965. 10.1111/add.12980 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kyzar E. J., Zhang H., Pandey S. C. (2019). Adolescent alcohol exposure epigenetically suppresses amygdala arc enhancer RNA expression to confer adult anxiety susceptibility. Biol. Psychiatry 85, 904–914. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.12.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kyzar E. J., Zhang H., Sakharkar A. J., Pandey S. C. (2017). Adolescent alcohol exposure alters lysine demethylase 1 (LSD1) expression and histone methylation in the amygdala during adulthood. Addict. Biol. 22, 1191–1204. 10.1111/adb.12404 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lee K. M., Coehlo M. A., Solton N. R., Szumlinski K. K. (2017). Negative affect and excessive alcohol intake incubate during protracted withdrawal from binge-drinking in adolescent, but not adult, mice. Front. Psychol. 8:1128. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Loxton D., Canales J. J. (2017). Long-term cognitive, emotional and neurogenic alterations induced by alcohol and methamphetamine exposure in adolescent rats. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 1–8. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2016.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Maldonado-Devincci A. M., Alipour K. K., Michael L. A., Kirstein C. L. (2010). Repeated binge ethanol administration during adolescence enhances voluntary sweetened ethanol intake in young adulthood in male and female rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 96, 476–487. 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.07.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Masten A. S., Faden V. B., Zucker R. A., Spear L. P. (2009). A developmental perspective on underage alcohol use. Alcohol Res. Health 32, 3–15. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Matthews D. B., Novier A., Diaz-Granados J. L., Van Skike C. E., Ornelas L., Mittleman G. (2017). Impact of adolescent alcohol use across the lifespan: long-lasting tolerance to high-dose alcohol coupled with potentiated spatial memory impairments to moderate-dose alcohol. Alcohol 61, 33–42. 10.1016/j.alcohol.2017.01.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Matthews D. B., Tinsley K. L., Diaz-Granados J. L., Tokunaga S., Silvers J. M. (2008). Chronic intermittent exposure to ethanol during adolescence produces tolerance to the hypnotic effects of ethanol in male rats: a dose-dependent analysis. Alcohol 42, 617–621. 10.1016/j.alcohol.2008.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Milivojevic V., Covault J. (2013). Alcohol exposure during late adolescence increases drinking in adult Wistar rats, an effect that is not reduced by finasteride. Alcohol Alcohol. 48, 28–38. 10.1093/alcalc/ags105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mills K. L., Lalonde F., Clasen L. S., Giedd J. N., Blakemore S. J. (2014). Developmental changes in the structure of the social brain in late childhood and adolescence. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 123–131. 10.1093/scan/nss113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Moaddab M., Mangone E., Ray M. H., McDannald M. A. (2017). Adolescent alcohol drinking renders adult drinking BLA-dependent: BLA hyper-activity as contributor to comorbid alcohol use disorder and anxiety disorders. Brain Sci. 7:E151. 10.3390/brainsci7110151 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Moore E. M., Mariani J. N., Linsenbardt D. N., Melon L. C., Boehm S. L., II. (2010). Adolescent C57BL/6J (but not DBA/2J) mice consume greater amounts of limited-access ethanol compared to adults and display continued elevated ethanol intake into adulthood. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 34, 734–742. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01143.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Morean M. E., Kong G., Camenga D. R., Cavallo D. A., Connell C., Krishnan-Sarin S. (2014). First drink to first drunk: age of onset and delay to intoxication are associated with adolescent alcohol use and binge drinking. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 38, 2615–2621. 10.1111/acer.12526 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Morean M. E., L’Insalata A., Butler E. R., McKee A., Krishnan-Sarin S. (2018). Age at drinking onset, age at first intoxication and delay to first intoxication: assessing the concurrent validity of measures of drinking initiation with alcohol use and related problems. Addict. Behav. 79, 195–200. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.12.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Moy S. S., Duncan G. E., Knapp D. J., Breese G. R. (1998). Sensitivity to ethanol across development in rats: comparison to [3H]zolpidem binding. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 22, 1485–1492. 10.1097/00000374-199810000-00018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Nentwig T. B., Starr E. M., Chandler L. J., Glover E. J. (2019). Absence of compulsive drinking phenotype in adult male rats exposed to ethanol in a binge-like pattern during adolescence. Alcohol 79, 93–103. 10.1016/j.alcohol.2019.01.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Olsson C. A., Romaniuk H., Salinger J., Staiger P. K., Bonomo Y., Hulbert C., et al. (2016). Drinking patterns of adolescents who develop alcohol use disorders: results from the victorian adolescent health cohort study. BMJ Open 6:e010455. 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-010455 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. O’Tousa D. S., Matson L. M., Grahame N. J. (2013). Effects of intoxicating free-choice alcohol consumption during adolescence on drinking and impulsivity during adulthood in selectively bred high-alcohol preferring mice. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 37, 141–149. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01857.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pandey S. C., Sakharkar A. J., Tang L., Zhang H. (2015). Potential role of adolescent alcohol exposure-induced amygdaloid histone modifications in anxiety and alcohol intake during adulthood. Neurobiol. Dis. 82, 607–619. 10.1016/j.nbd.2015.03.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Pascual M., Boix J., Felipo V., Guerri C. (2009). Repeated alcohol administration during adolescence causes changes in the mesolimbic dopaminergic and glutamatergic systems and promotes alcohol intake in the adult rat. J. Neurochem. 108, 920–931. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05835.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Patrick M. E., Evans-Polce R., Terry-McElrath Y. M. (2019). Faster escalation from first drink to first intoxication as a risk factor for binge and high-intensity drinking among adolescents. Addict. Behav. 92, 199–202. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.01.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Patrick M. E., Schulenberg J. E., Martz M. E., Maggs J. L., O’Malley P. M., Johnston L. D. (2013). Extreme binge drinking among 12th-grade students in the United States: prevalence and predictors. JAMA Pediatr. 167, 1019–1025. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.2392 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Pautassi R. M., Myers M., Spear L. P., Molina J. C., Spear N. E. (2008). Adolescent but not adult rats exhibit ethanol-mediated appetitive second-order conditioning. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 32, 2016–2027. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00789.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Pelloux Y., Costentin J., Duterte-Boucher D. (2015). Differential involvement of anxiety and novelty preference levels on oral ethanol consumption in rats. Psychopharmacology 232, 2711–2721. 10.1007/s00213-015-3910-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Quoilin C., Didone V., Tirelli E., Quertemont E. (2012). Chronic ethanol exposure during adolescence alters the behavioral responsiveness to ethanol in adult mice. Behav. Brain Res. 229, 1–9. 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.12.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Ramirez R. L., Spear L. P. (2010). Ontogeny of ethanol-induced motor impairment following acute ethanol: assessment via the negative geotaxis reflex in adolescent and adult rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 95, 242–248. 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.01.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Rodd-Henricks Z. A., Bell R. L., Kuc K. A., Murphy J. M., McBride W. J., Lumeng L., et al. (2002). Effects of ethanol exposure on subsequent acquisition and extinction of ethanol self-administration and expression of alcohol-seeking behavior in adult alcohol-preferring (P) rats: I. Periadolescent exposure. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 26, 1632–1641. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2002.tb02465.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rohde P., Lewinsohn P. M., Kahler C. W., Seeley J. R., Brown R. A. (2001). Natural course of alcohol use disorders from adolescence to young adulthood. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40, 83–90. 10.1097/00004583-200101000-00020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Saalfield J., Spear L. (2015). Consequences of repeated ethanol exposure during early or late adolescence on conditioned taste aversions in rats. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 174–182. 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.01.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Saalfield J., Spear L. (2019). Fos activation patterns related to acute ethanol and conditioned taste aversion in adolescent and adult rats. Alcohol 78, 57–68. 10.1016/j.alcohol.2019.02.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Sakharkar A. J., Kyzar E. J., Gavin D. P., Zhang H., Chen Y., Krishnan H. R., et al. (2019). Altered amygdala DNA methylation mechanisms after adolescent alcohol exposure contribute to adult anxiety and alcohol drinking. Neuropharmacology 157:107679. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.107679 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Schramm-Sapyta N. L., Francis R., MacDonald A., Keistler C., O’Neill L., Kuhn C. M. (2014). Effect of sex on ethanol consumption and conditioned taste aversion in adolescent and adult rats. Psychopharmacology 231, 1831–1839. 10.1007/s00213-013-3319-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Sherrill L. K., Berthold C., Koss W. A., Juraska J. M., Gulley J. M. (2011). Sex differences in the effects of ethanol pre-exposure during adolescence on ethanol-induced conditioned taste aversion in adult rats. Behav. Brain Res. 225, 104–109. 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.07.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Siciliano D., Smith R. F. (2001). Periadolescent alcohol alters adult behavioral characteristics in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 74, 637–643. 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00623-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Siegmund S., Vengeliene V., Singer M. V., Spanagel R. (2005). Influence of age at drinking onset on long-term ethanol self-administration with deprivation and stress phases. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 29, 1139–1145. 10.1097/01.alc.0000171928.40418.46 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Silveri M. M., Dager A. D., Cohen-Gilbert J. E., Sneider J. T. (2016). Neurobiological signatures associated with alcohol and drug use in the human adolescent brain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 70, 244–259. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Silveri M. M., Spear L. P. (1998). Decreased sensitivity to the hypnotic effects of ethanol early in ontogeny. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 22, 670–676. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb04310.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Silvers J. M., Tokunaga S., Mittleman G., Matthews D. B. (2003). Chronic intermittent injections of high-dose ethanol during adolescence produce metabolic, hypnotic, and cognitive tolerance in rats. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 27, 1606–1612. 10.1097/01.alc.0000090141.66526.22 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Slawecki C. J., Betancourt M. (2002). Effects of adolescent ethanol exposure on ethanol consumption in adult rats. Alcohol 26, 23–30. 10.1016/s0741-8329(01)00192-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Spear L. P. (2015). Adolescent alcohol exposure: are there separable vulnerable periods within adolescence? Physiol. Behav. 148, 122–130. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.01.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Spear L. P. (2018). Effects of adolescent alcohol consumption on the brain and behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 197–214. 10.1038/nrn.2018.10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Spear L. P., Swartzwelder H. S. (2014). Adolescent alcohol exposure and persistence of adolescent-typical phenotypes into adulthood: a mini-review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 45, 1–8. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.04.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Strong M. N., Yoneyama N., Fretwell A. M., Snelling C., Tanchuck M. A., Finn D. A. (2010). “Binge” drinking experience in adolescent mice shows sex differences and elevated ethanol intake in adulthood. Horm. Behav. 58, 82–90. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.10.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Tambour S., Brown L. L., Crabbe J. C. (2008). Gender and age at drinking onset affect voluntary alcohol consumption but neither the alcohol deprivation effect nor the response to stress in mice. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 32, 2100–2106. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00798.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Thiele T. E., Navarro M. (2014). “Drinking in the dark” (DID) procedures: a model of binge-like ethanol drinking in non-dependent mice. Alcohol 48, 235–241. 10.1016/j.alcohol.2013.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Toalston J. E., Deehan G. A., Jr., Hauser S. R., Engleman E. A., Bell R. L., Murphy J. M., et al. (2015). The reinforcing properties of ethanol are quantitatively enhanced in adulthood by peri-adolescent ethanol, but not saccharin, consumption in female alcohol-preferring (P) rats. Alcohol 49, 513–518. 10.1016/j.alcohol.2015.04.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Torcaso A., Asimes A., Meagher M., Pak T. R. (2017). Adolescent binge alcohol exposure increases risk assessment behaviors in male Wistar rats after exposure to an acute psychological stressor in adulthood. Psychoneuroendocrinology 76, 154–161. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.11.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Trezza V., Baarendse P. J., Vanderschuren L. J. (2009). Prosocial effects of nicotine and ethanol in adolescent rats through partially dissociable neurobehavioral mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 34, 2560–2573. 10.1038/npp.2009.85 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Van Skike C. E., Diaz-Granados J. L., Matthews D. B. (2015). Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure produces persistent anxiety in adolescent and adult rats. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 39, 262–271. 10.1111/acer.12617 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Varlinskaya E. I., Hosová D., Towner T., Werner D. F., Spear L. P. (2019). Effects of chronic intermittent ethanol exposure during early and late adolescence on anxiety-like behaviors and behavioral flexibility in adulthood. Behav. Brain Res. 378:112292. 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Varlinskaya E. I., Kim E. U., Spear L. P. (2017). Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure during adolescence: effects on stress-induced social alterations and social drinking in adulthood. Brain Res. 1654, 145–156. 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.03.050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Varlinskaya E. I., Spear L. P. (2002). Acute effects of ethanol on social behavior of adolescent and adult rats: role of familiarity of the test situation. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 26, 1502–1511. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2002.tb02449.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Varlinskaya E. I., Spear L. P. (2007). Chronic tolerance to the social consequences of ethanol in adolescent and adult Sprague-Dawley rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 29, 23–30. 10.1016/j.ntt.2006.08.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Varlinskaya E. I., Spear L. P. (2015). Social consequences of ethanol: impact of age, stress, and prior history of ethanol exposure. Physiol. Behav. 148, 145–150. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Varlinskaya E. I., Truxell E., Spear L. P. (2014). Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure during adolescence: effects on social behavior and ethanol sensitivity in adulthood. Alcohol 48, 433–444. 10.1016/j.alcohol.2014.01.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Vetreno R. P., Yaxley R., Paniagua B., Crews F. T. (2016). Diffusion tensor imaging reveals adolescent binge ethanol-induced brain structural integrity alterations in adult rats that correlate with behavioral dysfunction. Addict. Biol. 21, 939–953. 10.1111/adb.12232 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Vetter C. S., Doremus-Fitzwater T. L., Spear L. P. (2007). Time course of elevated ethanol intake in adolescent relative to adult rats under continuous, voluntary-access conditions. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 31, 1159–1168. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00417.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Vetter-O’Hagen C., Varlinskaya E., Spear L. (2009). Sex differences in ethanol intake and sensitivity to aversive effects during adolescence and adulthood. Alcohol Alcohol. 44, 547–554. 10.1093/alcalc/agp048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Vorspan F., Mehtelli W., Dupuy G., Bloch V., Lepine J. P. (2015). Anxiety and substance use disorders: co-occurrence and clinical issues. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 17:4. 10.1007/s11920-014-0544-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Walker B. M., Ehlers C. L. (2009). Age-related differences in the blood alcohol levels of Wistar rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 91, 560–565. 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.09.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. White A. M., Truesdale M. C., Bae J. G., Ahmad S., Wilson W. A., Best P. J., et al. (2002). Differential effects of ethanol on motor coordination in adolescent and adult rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 73, 673–677. 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00860-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Wiener C. D., Moreira F. P., Zago A., Souza L. M., Branco J. C., Oliveira J. F., et al. (2018). Mood disorder, anxiety, and suicide risk among subjects with alcohol abuse and/or dependence: a population-based study. Braz. J. Psychiatry 40, 1–5. 10.1590/1516-4446-2016-2170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Willey A. R., Varlinskaya E. I., Spear L. P. (2009). Social interactions and 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in adolescent and adult rats. Behav. Brain Res. 202, 122–129. 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Williams K. L., Nickel M. M., Bielak J. T. (2018). Oral binge-like ethanol pre-exposure during juvenile/adolescent period attenuates ethanol-induced conditioned place aversion in rats. Alcohol Alcohol. 53, 518–525. 10.1093/alcalc/agy040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Yan W., Kang J., Zhang G., Li S., Kang Y., Wang L., et al. (2015). The effects of gonadectomy and binge-like ethanol exposure during adolescence on open field behaviour in adult male rats. Neurosci. Lett. 604, 52–57. 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.07.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES