Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Mar 27;9:e53060. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53060

Optimization of energy state transition trajectory supports the development of executive function during youth

Zaixu Cui 1, Jennifer Stiso 2, Graham L Baum 1, Jason Z Kim 2, David R Roalf 1, Richard F Betzel 3, Shi Gu 4, Zhixin Lu 2, Cedric H Xia 1, Xiaosong He 2, Rastko Ciric 1, Desmond J Oathes 1, Tyler M Moore 1, Russell T Shinohara 5, Kosha Ruparel 1, Christos Davatzikos 2,6, Fabio Pasqualetti 7, Raquel E Gur 1, Ruben C Gur 1, Danielle S Bassett 2,6,8,9,10,, Theodore D Satterthwaite 1,†,
Editors: Thomas Yeo11, Timothy E Behrens12
PMCID: PMC7162657  PMID: 32216874

Abstract

Executive function develops during adolescence, yet it remains unknown how structural brain networks mature to facilitate activation of the fronto-parietal system, which is critical for executive function. In a sample of 946 human youths (ages 8-23y) who completed diffusion imaging, we capitalized upon recent advances in linear dynamical network control theory to calculate the energetic cost necessary to activate the fronto-parietal system through the control of multiple brain regions given existing structural network topology. We found that the energy required to activate the fronto-parietal system declined with development, and the pattern of regional energetic cost predicts unseen individuals’ brain maturity. Finally, energetic requirements of the cingulate cortex were negatively correlated with executive performance, and partially mediated the development of executive performance with age. Our results reveal a mechanism by which structural networks develop during adolescence to reduce the theoretical energetic costs of transitions to activation states necessary for executive function.

Research organism: Human

eLife digest

Adolescents are known for taking risks, from driving too fast to experimenting with drugs and alcohol. Such behaviors tend to decrease as individuals move into adulthood. Most people in their mid-twenties have greater self-control than they did as teenagers. They are also often better at planning, sustaining attention, and inhibiting impulsive behaviors. These skills, which are known as executive functions, develop over the course of adolescence.

Executive functions rely upon a series of brain regions distributed across the frontal lobe and the lobe that sits just behind it, the parietal lobe. Fiber tracts connect these regions to form a fronto-parietal network. These fiber tracts are also referred to as white matter due to the whitish fatty material that surrounds and insulates them.

Cui et al. now show that changes in white matter networks have implications for teen behavior. Almost 950 healthy young people aged between 8 and 23 years underwent a type of brain scan called diffusion-weighted imaging that visualizes white matter. The scans revealed that white matter networks in the frontal and parietal lobes mature over adolescence. This makes it easier for individuals to activate their fronto-parietal networks by decreasing the amount of energy required. Cui et al. show that a computer model can predict the maturity of a person's brain based on the energy needed to activate their fronto-parietal networks.

These changes help explain why executive functions improve during adolescence. This in turn explains why behaviors such as risk-taking tend to decrease with age. That said, adults with various psychiatric disorders, such as ADHD and psychosis, often show impaired executive functions. In the future, it may be possible to reduce these impairments by applying magnetic fields to the scalp to reduce the activity of specific brain regions. The techniques used in the current study could help reveal which brain regions to target with this approach.

Introduction

Executive function is essential for a wide range of cognitive tasks, and is strongly associated with both overall intelligence (Arffa, 2007) and academic performance (Best et al., 2011). Executive function undergoes protracted maturation during adolescence (Best and Miller, 2010; Gur et al., 2012), and its development is linked to the expansion of the cognitive and behavioral repertoire. Notably, executive deficits are linked to both increased morbidity associated with risk-taking behaviors (Romer et al., 2009) as well as a wide range of neuropsychiatric disorders (Shanmugan et al., 2016), such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and psychosis (Barkley, 1997; Wolf et al., 2015).

Prior studies have consistently established that executive function relies on activity in a distributed network of fronto-parietal regions, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, cingulate cortex, superior parietal cortex, and frontopolar cortex (Alvarez and Emory, 2006; Mansouri et al., 2017; Niendam et al., 2012; Rottschy et al., 2012; Satterthwaite et al., 2013). Notably, both functional (Fair et al., 2007; Grayson and Fair, 2017; Gu et al., 2015; Power et al., 2010) and structural (Baum et al., 2017; Hagmann et al., 2010; Huang et al., 2015) connectivity among these regions undergoes active remodeling during adolescence, with increased connectivity among executive regions, and diminished connectivity between executive regions and other systems such as the default mode network. As structural white matter networks are known to constrain both intrinsic connectivity and patterns of task-related activation (Hermundstad et al., 2013; Honey et al., 2009), it is possible that white matter networks develop during adolescence to facilitate dynamic transitions to fronto-parietal system activation states with lower theoretical energetic cost. However, research that seeks to relate developing white matter networks to the functional dynamics of the fronto-parietal executive system remains sparse.

Network control theory provides a powerful framework to address this gap in our knowledge. Previous work has shown that the brain becomes more theoretically controllable to all possible brain states (on average) through the control of individual brain regions during adolescence (Tang et al., 2017). Network control theory has the potential to provide novel insights regarding mechanisms needed to transition to executive states, as executive function exerts top-down control on other brain systems in a manner akin to control points in a dynamic network (Gu et al., 2015; Tang et al., 2017). Capitalizing on recent developments in network control theory (Betzel et al., 2016; Gu et al., 2017; Stiso et al., 2019), here we examine how the developing brain structural network supports the transition to a specific state necessary for executive function through the distributed control of multiple brain regions. Specifically, this new framework allows one to integrate information regarding network topology and patterns of brain activation within one mathematical model, in order to specify how theoretical neural dynamics are constrained by the structural connectome (Tang and Bassett, 2018). Such models assume that the activation state of the brain at a given time is a linear function of the previous state, the underlying white matter network, and any additional control energy injected into the system (Betzel et al., 2016Gu et al., 2017). From this paradigm, one can calculate the optimal energy cost to move the brain from one state to another given a structural network topology (Betzel et al., 2016; Gu et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2018). In the present work, we apply this new technique to a large sample of youth. Specifically, we investigated how the energetic cost of transitions to a fronto-parietal system activation state necessary for the executive function changes in response to the maturation of structural brain network. We hypothesized that maturation of structural brain networks would allow for the target activation state of the fronto-parietal executive system to be reached at a lower energetic cost.

To test this hypothesis, we capitalized on a large sample of youth (8–23 years) who completed neuroimaging as part of the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort (PNC) (Satterthwaite et al., 2014). We examined how white matter networks (estimated using diffusion imaging) support the transition to a fronto-parietal system activation state. As described below, we demonstrate that the energy required to reach this state declines with age, especially within the fronto-parietal control network. Furthermore, we find that the whole-brain control energy pattern contains sufficient information to predict individuals’ brain maturity across development. Finally, participants with better performance on executive tasks require less energetic cost in the bilateral cingulate cortex to reach this activation target, and the energetic cost of this region mediates the development of executive performance with age. Notably, these results could not be explained by individual differences in general network control properties, and were not present in alternative activation target states. Together, these results suggest that structural brain networks become optimized in development to minimize the energetic costs of transitions to activation states necessary for executive function through the distributed control of multiple brain regions.

Results

Network topology constrains the transition to a fronto-parietal activation state

In this study, we included 946 youths aged 8–23 years who were imaged as part of the PNC (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Structural white matter networks were reconstructed for each participant from diffusion imaging data using probabilistic tractography and a standard parcellation of 232 regions. Capitalizing on recent advances in network control theory, we modeled how structural networks facilitate state transitions from an initial baseline state to the target state. In the initial state, all regions had an activity magnitude of 0. In the target state, regions in the fronto-parietal system had activity magnitude of 1, with all other regions having an activity magnitude of 0. Specifically, we defined the trajectory of a neural system to be the temporal path that the system traverses through diverse states, where the item state was defined as the vector of neurophysiological activity across brain regions at a single time point. Based on each participant’s unique network topology, we estimated the regional energetic cost required for the brain to transition from the baseline to the fronto-parietal activation target state (Betzel et al., 2016; Gu et al., 2017; Stiso et al., 2019; Figure 1—figure supplement 2 and Figure 1a). Formally, this estimation was operationalized as a multi-point network control optimization problem, where we aimed to identify the optimal trajectory between baseline and the fronto-parietal activation target state that minimizes both the energetic cost and the distance between the final state and the target state.

Figure 1. Schematic of the network control approach and the estimation of control energy.

(a) From a baseline state, we calculated the control energy required to reach a fronto-parietal activation target state. This transition was calculated for each subject based on their structural brain network, which was estimated using diffusion imaging and probabilistic tractography. (b) The average energetic costs to reach the fronto-parietal activation target state varied by cognitive system, with the largest energetic costs being present in the fronto-parietal control network and the ventral attention network. (c) The regional control energy required to reach the fronto-parietal activation target. (d) The control energy cost of a transition to the fronto-parietal activation target state was significantly lower in real brain networks than in null model networks where the strength and degree distribution were preserved.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Sample construction.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

The cross-sectional sample of the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort (PNC) has 1601 participants in total. 340 subjects were excluded owing to clinical factors, such as medical disorders. Then, 312 subjects were excluded because of low quality of T1 or diffusion data, incomplete diffusion data, lacking of field map. Finally, three subjects were excluded due to incomplete image coverage during brain parcellation. The final sample consisted of the remaining 946 subjects.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Functional brain networks defined by Yeo et al. (2011).

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Each parcel was mapped to one of these networks.
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Relationship between trajectory distance and control energy.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(a) The activation profiles of all 27 brain regions of the fronto-parietal system during an optimal trajectory from the baseline state to the final state. We define the final state to be a vector in which elements corresponding to the 27 regions of the fronto-parietal system are set to 1, and all other elements are set to 0. The activity magnitudes vary by region and by time. The trajectory here is the average of trajectory over all subjects. (b) For each subject, the Euclidean distance from the current state x(t) to the target state x(T) decreases with time. The final distance to the target state was 0, indicating that all subjects reached the target state. Each line represents a different subject. (c) For each subject, the total control energy cost of all brain regions at a particular time point increases with time. Each line represents a different subject. (d) We observe a tight correlation between the total trajectory distance and the total control energy across subjects (r = 0.97, p<2 × 10−16).

Results of this linear dynamical model indicate that the trajectory distance (i.e., the distance between current and target states) decreases with time until the desired target state is reached (Figure 1—figure supplement 3a and b). For each network node, we calculated the control energy cost, which provides an indication of where energy must be injected into the network to achieve the transition to the target state. Consistent with a recent methodological study (Karrer et al., 2019), and several recent empirical studies (Betzel et al., 2016; Gu et al., 2017; Stiso et al., 2019), the trajectory distance (Figure 1—figure supplement 3b) was inversely related to the time-dependent energy cost within subject (Figure 1—figure supplement 3c). We calculated the trajectory distance at each time point, which was defined as the Euclidean distance between the current brain state and the target brain state. A small distance suggests that the current vector of brain activity is similar to the target vector of brain activity. Across all subjects, we found the total trajectory distance of all time points was positively correlated with total control energy of all time points (r = 0.97, p<2 × 10−16, Figure 1—figure supplement 3d), suggesting that subjects whose state transition trajectory is long require more energy input to reach the target state. Prior literature has demonstrated that control energy cost is lower in human brain than in the brains of Drosophila and mouse to support diverse network dynamics (Kim et al., 2018), is related to network topology (Betzel et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2018) and reflects the magnitude of focal electrocorticography stimulation required to drive the brain to a target memory state in patients with medically refractory epilepsy (Stiso et al., 2019). Accordingly, here we used the control energy as a metric to summarize the optimal trajectory. We calculated the mean control energy of each system; the highest control energy was observed in systems involved in executive function (Figure 1b and c), including the fronto-parietal and ventral attention/cingulo-opercular systems (see Figure 1—figure supplement 2; Yeo et al., 2011).

Based on recent evidence that network control properties depend appreciably on the topological structure of the network (Kim et al., 2018; Wu-Yan et al., 2017), we next sought to demonstrate that the topological structure of brain networks facilitates this transition. We therefore compared the energetic cost of this transition in empirical brain networks to the energetic cost observed in null model networks. Specifically, we randomly permuted (100 times per participant) the placement of edge weights, while preserving the network degree and strength distribution. The mean whole brain energetic cost of the null networks was significantly higher (p<2 × 10−16) than that of the empirical networks (Figure 1d), indicating that structural brain networks are topologically optimized to reduce the energetic costs of the transition to a fronto-parietal activation state.

Energetic costs of the transition to a fronto-parietal activation state decline with development

Having shown that the topology of structural brain networks facilitates transitions to a fronto-parietal activation state, we next investigated how the energetic costs of this transition evolve in youth. We hypothesized that the energy required to make this transition would decline as networks were remodeled in development. Prior studies have demonstrated that the developmental changes of both brain structure and function could be either linear (Hagmann et al., 2010; Wierenga et al., 2016) or non-linear (Grayson and Fair, 2017; Mills et al., 2016; Vandekar et al., 2015). Therefore, we used generalized additive models (GAM) with penalized splines, which allowed us to rigorously characterize both linear and nonlinear effects while avoiding over-fitting. Age associations with control energy were examined at multiple scales, including the level of the whole brain, cognitive systems, and individual nodes. For all analyses, we included sex, handedness, in-scanner head motion, total brain volume, and total network strength as covariates. These analyses revealed that the whole-brain average energetic cost of the transition to the fronto-parietal activation state declined with age (Z = −5.12, p=3.06 × 10−7, Partial r = −0.17, 95% confidence interval (CI) = [−0.23,–0.10]; Figure 2a). Notably, analyses of cognitive systems indicated that age effects were heterogeneously distributed (Figure 2b), with the largest declines in control energy occurring in fronto-parietal (Z = −5.30, PFDR = 4.54 × 10−7, Partial r = −0.17, CI = [−0.23,–0.11]; Figure 2c), visual (Z = −4.25, PFDR = 5.71 × 10−5, Partial r = −0.14, CI = [−0.20,–0.08]), and motor (Z = −3.20, PFDR = 2.70 × 10−3, Partial r = −0.09, CI = [−0.15,–0.03]) systems. In contrast, energetic costs within the limbic (Z = 8.69, PFDR <2 × 10−16, Partial r = 0.29, CI = [0.23, 0.35]) and default mode (Z = 2.86, PFDR = 5.66 × 10−3, Partial r = 0.10, CI = [0.04, 0.17]) systems increased with age (see Figure 2—figure supplement 1). These system-level results aligned with analyses of individual network nodes; we found that the control energy of 49 regions decreased significantly with age (PFDR <0.05), including regions in the fronto-parietal control, visual, and motor systems. Furthermore, the control energy significantly increased with development in 30 regions (PFDR <0.05), which were mainly situated in limbic and default mode systems (Figure 2d).

Figure 2. Control energy evolves with age in youth.

(a) The mean whole-brain control energy required to reach the fronto-parietal activation target declines with age. (b) Control energy declines significantly with age in the fronto-parietal, visual, motor and subcortical systems. In contrast, control energy increased in the ventral attention, default mode and limbic systems. For each system with a significant association, the effect size is reported (in each bar) as the partial correlation between system-level control energy and age while controlling for the covariates. There is one outlier in the scatter plot of ventral attention system (Figure 2—figure supplement 1c) and the age-related changes of control energy was not significant (p=0.11) in this system after removing the outlier. (c) The control energy of the fronto-parietal system declines significantly with age. (d) The age effect of control energy for each brain region. The color of the contour of each brain region represents the cognitive system for each region (see Figure 1—figure supplement 2). In the scatterplots shown in panels (a and c), data points represent each subject (n = 946), the bold line indicates the best fit from a general additive model, and the shaded envelope denotes the 95% confidence interval. It should be noted that Z value was derived from the general additive model, which captures both linear and nonlinear relationships; the partial correlation reflects only linear relationships. VS: visual; MT: motor; DA: dorsal attention; VA: ventral attention; LM: limbic; FP: fronto-parietal; DM: default mode; SC: subcortical.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Scatter plots of significant age effects of control energy at the system scale.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

The control energy of (a) visual, (b) motor, and (f) subcortical systems decline significantly with age, while that of (c) ventral attention, (d) limbic and (e) default mode systems increase significantly with age. There is one outlier in the scatter plot of ventral attention system and the age-related changes of control energy was not significant (p=0.11) in this system after removing the outlier. Data points represent each subject (n = 946), the bold line indicates the best fit from a general additive model, and the shaded envelope denotes the 95% confidence interval. There is one outlier in the scatter plot of ventral attention system (panel c) and the age-related changes of control energy was not significant (p=0.11) in this system after removing the outlier.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Specificity and sensitivity analyses provide convergent results.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

The effect size (i.e., partial correlation r) of the age effect of control energy at the whole-brain level and in the fronto-parietal system (a) with 100 different initial states, in which the activation value of regions in fronto-parietal follow the Gaussian distribution with a mean value of 0 and standard deviation of 0.1, and (b) with 100 different target states, in which the activation value of regions in fronto-parietal system follow the Gaussian distribution with a mean value of 1 and standard deviation of 0.1. (c) The distribution of the age effect of average control energy of whole-brain and fronto-parietal systems when using null model networks, which preserve the degree and strength distribution. The null networks were created by brain connectivity toolbox (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). The red arrow indicates the actual age effect estimated using the data from the real brain network. (d), The whole-brain control energy cost to activate the fronto-parietal system when constraining the whole brain was highly significantly correlated with the energy cost when only the fronto-parietal system was constrained (r = 0.94, p<2 × 10−16). (e), Left: the energy required to reach a motor activation state was significantly higher for null networks than real networks. Right: the whole brain average control energy did not change over the age range studied.
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Age effects at the whole brain, cognitive system, and nodal levels remain after controlling for the (a) modal controllability and (b) network modularity.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

For each system with a significant association, the effect size is reported (in each bar) as the partial correlation between system-level control energy and age while controlling for the covariates. It should be noted that Z values reflect both linear and nonlinear relationships with age, while effect size is reported using a partial correlation, which reflects only linear relationships. VS: visual; MT: motor; DA: dorsal attention; VA: ventral attention; LM: limbic; FP: fronto-parietal; DM: default mode; SC: subcortical.
Figure 2—figure supplement 4. Convergent results from a target state defined by a working memory task that recruits the fronto-parietal system.

Figure 2—figure supplement 4.

(a) Alternative target state, defined by the 2-back >0-back contrast on the fractal n-back working memory task (see Satterthwaite et al., 2013). (b) As in the main results, the control energy cost to reach this alternative target state was significantly lower using data from real networks than null networks. (c) Using this alternative target state, the control energy cost was highest in the fronto-parietal system. (d) As in the main results, the mean whole-brain control energy declines with age. (e) Similarly, the control energy of the fronto-parietal system declines significantly with age. (f) Nodal analyses of associations between age and control energy provide convergent results.

Having found associations between age and control energy, we next conducted a series of eight additional analyses. First, we found that our results held true for a range of baseline initial states and a range of fronto-parietal activation target states. When 100 different initial baseline states were evaluated, we found that in all cases both the whole brain and the fronto-parietal system showed a significant decline in control energy with age (Figure 2—figure supplement 2a). Similarly, when 100 different target states of fronto-parietal activation were evaluated, we found that in all cases both the whole-brain and the fronto-parietal system showed a significant decline in control energy with age (Figure 2—figure supplement 2b).

Second, we evaluated whether age effects could be due to non-topological network properties by evaluating the presence of age effects in null networks where degree and strength distributions were preserved. We found that the significance level of age effects in null networks were smaller than those observed in the real network (p<0.01, 100 permutations), suggesting that the empirically measured developmental effects were indeed driven by changes in the network topology (Figure 2—figure supplement 2c). Third, it should be noted that we only constrained the state of regions in the fronto-parietal system. Therefore, the distance travelled by these off-target regions outside the fronto-parietal system were not included in our cost function for calculating optimal control energy. This choice also serves to ensure that our calculation of control energy is largely robust to both the initial and target states of other regions. To demonstrate the robustness of our results to our definition of the matrix S, we calculated the control energy cost using the same initial and target states as in the main analyses but constraining the whole brain. Results showed that there is a high correlation (r = 0.94, p<2 × 10−16) between the whole-brain control energy cost when constraining the whole brain and that when constraining the fronto-parietal system only (Figure 2—figure supplement 2d).

Fourth, we assessed whether the structural network optimized the transition to an a priori motor system activation target (Figure 1—figure supplement 2; Yeo et al., 2011). Results indicated that the mean whole brain energetic cost of the null networks was significantly higher (p<2 × 10−16) than that of the empirical networks (Figure 2—figure supplement 2e left), suggesting that the lower energetic cost of activtivating thefronto-parietal system was not unique, but was present when activating other systems as well. We further evaluated the age effects of control energy cost to activate the motor system. As the age range of 8–23 years is a critical period in the development of executive function rather than motor function, we expected weaker age effects in the motor system. We found that the whole-brain control energy required to transition to the motor system activation did not significantly change over the age range studied (Z = 1.48, p=0.14, Partial r = 0.05, CI = [−0.02, 0.11]; Figure 2—figure supplement 2e right).

Fifth, we evaluated whether our developmental results could be explained by modal controllability. Modal controllability reflects the extent to which all dynamic modes of a system will change in response to small changes at a single node (Gu et al., 2015). If an individual has high modal controllability, it suggests that the underlying brain structural network was optimized to support efficient state transitions to diverse states. In line with this intuition, modal controllability increases with development in youth as flexible switching between patterns of brain activity becomes more common (Tang et al., 2017). Controlling for modal controllability did not alter our results (Figure 2—figure supplement 3a). Specifically, while controlling for modal controllability, average control energy of the whole-brain and fronto-parietal system both significantly declined with age (whole-brain: Z = −6.00, p=2.09 × 10−9, Partial r = −0.19, CI = [−0.25,–0.13]; fronto-parietal: Z = −9.95, PFDR <2 × 10−16, Partial r = −0.32, CI = [−0.37,–0.26]).

Sixth, because the modularity of brain networks evolves with age, one could ask whether that evolution impacts the observed assocations with control energy (Baum et al., 2017; Hagmann et al., 2010; Huang et al., 2015). However, we found that results remained consistent after controlling for network modularity in all analyses (Figure 2—figure supplement 3b). For example, average control energy of the whole brain and of the fronto-parietal system both significantly declined with age after controlling for network modularity (whole-brain: Z = −3.95, p=7.73 × 10−5, Partial r = −0.13, CI = [−0.19,–0.07]; fronto-parietal: Z = −4.31, PFDR = 6.46 × 10−5, Partial r = −0.14, CI = [−0.20,–0.08]). We further assessed whether the increasing segregation of fronto-parietal system during youth (Baum et al., 2017) could explain the age effect of control energy. Results remained consistent after controlling for the average participation coefficient within the fronto-parietal system when examining age-related differences in the average control energy of the fronto-parietal system (Z = −4.64, p=3.51 × 10−6, Partial r = −0.15, CI = [−0.21,–0.09]).

Seventh, we assessed whether connectivity within the fronto-parietal system or between the fronto-parietal and other systems could explain observed associations between age and control energy. Specifically, we calculated the sum of all the connections within fronto-parietal system and also the sum of all the connections between the fronto-parietal system and other systems. While controlling for within fronto-parietal connectivity strength, the control energy in the fronto-parietal system still significantly declined with development (Z = −3.53, p=0.0004, Partial r = −0.12, CI = [−0.18,–0.06]). Similarly, while controlling for the connectivity strength between the fronto-parietal system and other systems, the control energy in the fronto-parietal system still significantly declined with development (Z = −4.88, p=1.06 × 10−6, Partial r = −0.16, CI = [−0.22,–0.10]).

Finally, in our main analyses, we specified the target state as regions within the fronto-parietal system, with each region having a magnitude of 1. As a final step, we also considered a biologically recorded target state defined as the average activation pattern elicited by an n-back working memory task that reliably recruits the fronto-parietal system (Figure 2—figure supplement 4a). Using this alternative target state, we found that the control energy cost of the real network was significantly lower than null networks (Figure 2—figure supplement 4b). As in the main analyses, the control energy cost was highest in the fronto-parietal system (Figure 2—figure supplement 4c). Similarly, the whole-brain average control energy cost (Z = −7.59, p=3.26 × 10−14, Partial r = −0.25, CI = [−0.30,–0.18]; Figure 2—figure supplement 4d) and average control energy in the fronto-parietal system (Z = −5.26, PFDR = 2.92 × 10−7, Partial r = −0.17, CI = [−0.23,–0.11]; Figure 2—figure supplement 4e) both significantly declined with age. Nodal analyses provided convergent results, revealing that the control energy in nodes within the fronto-parietal system significantly declined with age (Figure 2—figure supplement 4f).

Patterns of control energy can predict brain maturity

Having established that the control energy required to reach the fronto-parietal activation state changes with age on a regional and system-level basis using mass-univariate analysis, we next evaluated the developmental changes of control energy using multivariate pattern analysis. Multivariate pattern analysis complements mass-univariate analysis, as mass-univariate analysis investigates each feature (i.e., control energy of one brain region) in isolation. In contrast, multivariate pattern analyses are sensitive to the spatially distributed pattern of features (Davatzikos, 2004; Haynes, 2015; Haynes and Rees, 2006; Norman et al., 2006). To provide an integrated view of this high-dimensional data, we used multivariate pattern analysis to determine whether spatially distributed patterns of control energy could accurately predict participant age. Specifically, we applied ridge regression with nested two-fold cross validation (2F-CV, see Figure 3—figure supplement 1) to identify an individual participant’s age in an unbiased fashion using the multivariate pattern of regional control energy. Specifically, we divided all subjects into two subsets based on age, with the first subset used as a training set and the second subset used as a testing set. Within the training set, we used inner 2F-CV to select an optimal regularization parameter (λ). Then, we trained a model using the training data and predicted the brain maturity (i.e., ‘brain age’) of participants in the testing set (Dosenbach et al., 2010; Franke et al., 2010). The significance of the model was evaluated using permutation testing, where the correspondence between a subject’s control energy features and their age was permuted at random. This analysis revealed that the multivariate pattern of control energy could predict an unseen individual’s age (Figure 3a and Figure 3—figure supplement 2a and b): the correlation between the predicted ‘brain age’ and chronological age was 0.63 (p < 0.001) after controlling for the covariates, and the mean absolute error (MAE) was 2.16 years (p < 0.001). For completeness, we also repeated this procedure while reversing the training and test sets, which yielded very similar results (partial r = 0.58, p < 0.001; MAE = 2.27, p < 0.001; Figure 3a and Figure 3—figure supplement 2c and d). We further examined model weights at the level of individual network nodes. The regions that contributed the most to the prediction of brain maturity aligned with mass-univariate analyses, and included the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, the cingulate cortex, superior parietal cortex, and lateral temporal cortex (Figure 3b). In order to ensure that our initial split of the data was representative, we repeated this analysis with 100 random splits, which returned highly consistent results (mean partial r = 0.61, mean MAE = 2.21 years).

Figure 3. The whole-brain control energy pattern contains sufficient information to predict brain maturity in unseen individuals.

(a) The predicted brain maturity index was significantly related to the chronological age in a multivariate ridge regression model that used 2-fold cross validation (2F-CV) with nested parameter tuning. The complete sample of of subjects was divided into two subsets according to age rank. The blue color represents the best-fit line between the actual score of the first subset of subjects and their scores predicted by the model trained using the second subset of subjects. The green color represents the best-fit line between the actual score of the second subset of subjects and their scores predicted by the model trained using the first subset of subjects. (b) Regions with the highest contribution to the multivariate model aligned with mass-univariate analyses and included frontal, parietal, and temporal regions. We displayed the 79 regions with the highest contribution, to facilitate comparisons with mass-univariate analyses (where there were 79 regions with significant age effects).

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Schematic overview of one outer loop of the nested 2-fold cross-validation (2F-CV) prediction framework.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

All subjects were divided into 2 halves according to age rank, with the first half used as a training set and the second half used as a testing set. Each feature was linearly scaled between zero and one across the training dataset, and the scaling parameters were also applied to scale the testing dataset. An inner 2F-CV was applied within training set to select the optimal λ parameter. Based on the optimal λ, we trained a model using all subjects in the training set, and then used that model to predict the age of all subjects in the testing set.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. The histograms of the permutation distribution of the (a) correlation r and (b) MAE with the first subset used as a training set and the second subset used as a testing set, and (c) correlation r and (d) MAE with the first subset used as the testing set and the second subset used as training set.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

The red arrow represents the actual prediction accuracy (i.e., r or MAE). The actual correlation r was significantly higher than expected by chance (p<0.001) and the actual MAE was significantly lower than expected by chance (p<0.001).

Participants with higher executive function need less energy to activate the fronto-parietal network

Lastly, we investigated the cognitive implications of individual differences in control energy. Specifically, we expected that participants with higher executive performance on a standardized cognitive battery would require reduced control energy to activate the fronto-parietal system. In order to ensure that associations were present above and beyond the observed developmental effects, we controlled for linear and nonlinear effects of age in addition to the other covariates described above. While we did not find effects at the whole-brain or systems level, two regions survived after FDR correction at nodal level. Specifically, reduced control energy within two regions in the fronto-parietal control system -- the left and right middle cingulate cortex -- was associated with higher executive function (Left: Z = −3.65, PFDR = 0.032, Partial r = −0.13, CI = [−0.19–0.06]; Right: Z = −4.49, PFDR = 0.002, Partial r = −0.15, CI = [−0.21–0.08]; Figure 4a and b).

Figure 4. Reduced control energy in both the a, left and b, right mid-cingulate cortex was associated with higher executive performance.

Figure 4.

Of all brain regions examined, only the left and right mid-cingulate cortex survived FDR correction. Data points represent each subject (n = 944), the bold line indicates the best linear fit, and the shaded envelope denotes the 95% confidence interval.The yellow color indicates that the two regions belong to the fronto-parietal system (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). The control energy of both (c) left and (d) right mid-cingulate cortex partially mediates the improvement of executive function with age. Significance of mediation effect was assessed using bootstrapped confidence intervals.

Given that control energy reflects the topology of diffusion network (Kim et al., 2018) and prior study showed that diffusion network properties mediated the age-related development of executive function (Baum et al., 2017), we conducted mediation analyses to investigate the extent to which control energy accounted for the association between age and executive function. Using a bootstrapped mediation analysis while adjusting for the covariates described above (See Materials and methods), we found that control energy in both the left (β = 0.03, p=0.001, 95% confidence interval = [0.01, 0.04]; Figure 4c) and right middle cingulate cortex (β = 0.03, p<0.001, 95% confidence interval = [0.02, 0.05]; Figure 4d) mediated the development of executive function with age.

Discussion

Using a large sample of youths (8–23 years) and a generative model of brain network function, we demonstrated that the control energy theoretically required to transition to a fronto-parietal activation state declines with age in youth. Furthermore, the multivariate pattern of the whole-brain control energy predicted the brain maturity of unseen individual participants. These results could not be explained by general network control property and were not observed in analyses stipulating alternative activation targets. Finally, we found that individuals who had higher executive function required lower control energy in the bilateral middle cingulate cortex to activate the fronto-parietal system, and the control energy of this region partially mediated the development of executive performance with age. These results suggest that maturation of structural brain networks may facilitate transitions to fronto-parietal activation states that support executive function.

While prior work has consistently demonstrated similarities in the configuration of structural and functional connectivity (Hagmann et al., 2010; Honey et al., 2009; Mollink et al., 2019), these studies did not evaluate how brain structural networks constrain functional dynamics (Avena-Koenigsberger et al., 2017). Recently, using network control methods, several studies have modeled the structural network and brain activation in one framework to describe how the structural connectome may theoretically constrain dynamic transitions between two activation states (Betzel et al., 2016; Gu et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2018; Stiso et al., 2019). By extending this framework in a large population, we found that the theoretical energetic cost of brain state transition from a baseline to a fronto-parietal activation state was lower in real brain networks compared to null networks that preserved basic properties such as degree and strength distribution. This result was consistent with prior studies showing that human structural brain networks exhibit non-random topological properties, such as both high clustering and segregated modules (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009; Gong et al., 2009b; Hagmann et al., 2008). This non-random topological organization could support the energetically efficient activation of functional systems (Avena-Koenigsberger et al., 2017), and we specifically demonstrate that it supports the efficient control of transitions to the precise activation states required for executive function.

Structural connectivity remodels during youth, with increasing segregation (Huang et al., 2015), and greater integration (Hagmann et al., 2010). Together, these impose a stronger constraint on functional dynamics (Hagmann et al., 2010). However, prior work had not explored how the development of structural connectivity supports the emergence of functional activation relevant to executive function. Our results indicate that the control energy theoretically required to transition to the fronto-parietal activation state declines with development, and suggest that the topological organization of structural connectivity supports more energetically efficient signal transformation to activate the fronto-parietal network. Consistent with our results, prior work has demonstrated that metabolism costs declined in youth at both resting-state (Jog et al., 2016; Takahashi et al., 1999) and during working memory tasks (Jog et al., 2016).

Examination of individual cognitive systems revealed that this decline in whole-brain energy was driven by reduced energetic costs within the fronto-parietal system. In particular, substantial negative associations between age and control energy were observed in lateral prefrontal cortex and middle cingulate cortex, which are responsible for preparation, execution, monitoring and switching of tasks (i.e., working memory, attention, inhibitory control, etc.) (Alvarez and Emory, 2006; Apps et al., 2013; Niendam et al., 2012; Rottschy et al., 2012). Such reduced regional energetic cost suggests that structural brain networks may mature to allow for neural events that occur in these regions to impact the broad activation state of the entire network more efficiently (Baum et al., 2017; Hagmann et al., 2010), and more easily drive the brain towards the fronto-parietal activation state associated with demanding executive tasks (Satterthwaite et al., 2013; Thomason et al., 2009).

In contrast, the energetic cost of regions within the limbic and default mode systems increased with age. This localization of costs suggests that these regions become less able to move the brain to a fronto-parietal activation state as development progresses. This result was consistent with previous studies using both structural and functional connectivity data, which have shown that the fronto-parietal system becomes more segregated in development from other systems in association cortex (Grayson and Fair, 2017; He et al., 2019; Huang et al., 2015; Lee and Telzer, 2016; Sherman et al., 2014), including the default mode (Sherman et al., 2014) and limbic (Lee and Telzer, 2016) systems. This maturation may potentially allow for functional specialization and a reduction of interference.

It should be noted that in prior work we demonstrated that another network control property – modal controllability – increased with age (Tang et al., 2017). However, modal controllability quantifies the general controllability necessary to reach all possible states, and therefore is not sufficient to answer questions about specific patterns of activity. Given the importance of executive function in youth to academic achievement (Best et al., 2011), risk taking behaviors (Romer et al., 2009), and psychopathology (Shanmugan et al., 2016), here we sought to understand how structural networks develop to facilitate the transition to fronto-parietal activation states that are necessary for executive function. We therefore leveraged recent advances in network control theory that allow for examining the controllability of a specific biologically meaningful target state and using multi-point control – rather than examining the general controllability and using the single point control methods employed previously (Tang et al., 2017). We have previously demonstrated the validity of this new control framework using mathematics, animal data, brain stimulation data, and human brain imaging data (Betzel et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2018; Stiso et al., 2019). As the brain is constantly receiving input from multiple sensory modalities and top down cognitive processes, we believe that multi-point control is substantially more biologically plausible for understanding executive function than single-point control frameworks.

Using this framework, we found that the energetic costs to transition to a specific fronto-parietal activation target state decline with age, and models trained using control energy accurately encode brain maturity in unseen data. Importantly, we demonstrate that our current results are not simply a result of increasing single-point modal controllability: when modal controllability was included as a model covariate, our results remained unchanged. Critically, the increase of general controllability (i.e., modal controllability) with development does not provide information regarding control of the transition to a specific brain state transition. While the brain becomes more controllable on average as shown in Tang et al. (2017), the control energy required to arrive a specific activation state could decrease, not change, or even possibly increase. Consistent with this intuition, we demonstrated that the control energy cost to activate the motor system did not significantly change during development. The result accords with prior evidence indicating that motor development precedes executive development, and is largely complete by late childhood or early adolescence (Andersen, 2003; Gogtay et al., 2004) and suggests that the observed developmental changes in control energy may be specific for transitions to activation states recruiting higher-order cognitive systems, which undergo protracted maturation.

Additionally, we found null networks that preserved degree and strength distribution did not represent similar developmental changes of control energy, suggesting our results were driven by topological structure of the brain network. Our results suggest that neither the overall modularity of the structural network nor the segregation of the fronto-parietal system, which both mature during youth (Baum et al., 2017), could explain the association between age and control energy. This result was consistent with our recent study showing that the modularity can be positively, non-monotonically, and non-significantly related to control energy depending on the architecture of the structural adjacency matrix (Patankar et al., 2020). We found that connectivity strength within the fronto-parietal system partially contributed to reduced energy requirements for activating the fronto-parietal areas in development. The energy required for a state transition depends upon a system’s response to an energetic perturbation. The simplest such perturbation engenders an impulse response (Karrer et al., 2020). In Srivastava et al., 2020, we show that the impulse response of the system is formally related to the network communicability, suggesting that paths of all lengths -- not just direct connections -- contribute to the control energy. However, the precise relationship between network topology and control energy is unknown. In prior theoretical work, we made some progress in linking the underlying graph architecture to the observed control energy (Kim et al., 2018). Specifically, given we have the ability to control a subset of nodes in the network, we showed that the more strongly and more diversely the control nodes are connected to the non-control nodes, the less energy is required to control the network on average. We provided an analytical derivations of the expressions relating a network’s minimum control energy to its connectivity between control and non-control nodes, as well as offering an intuitive geometric representation to visualize this relationship, and rules for modifying edges to alter control energy in a predictable manner (Kim et al., 2018). However, this theory is not applicable to our current study because the theory requires that only a set of brain regions be used for control, whereas here we set the control set to be the entire brain. More formal theoretical work is necessary to identify the topological properties that explain control energy.

Our main results regarding brain development (as well as the supplementary analyses described above) used a mass-univariate analysis approach, where the association between the control energy of each region was modeled separately. Complementary analyses sought to identify distributed multivariate patterns of control energy, which could be used to predict the brain maturity of unseen individuals. Such an approach is similar to prior studies that have used structural (Franke et al., 2010), functional (Dosenbach et al., 2010), or diffusion (Erus et al., 2015) based imaging to predict brain development. Here, we used a rigorous split half validation framework with nested parameter tuning. We found that the complex pattern of control energy could be used to predict individual brain maturity. The feature weights from this multivariate model were generally consistent with findings from mass-univariate analyses, underscoring the robustness of these results to the methodological approach. In this context, control energy could have potential to determine whether individuals display either precocity or delay in specific dynamic aspects of brain maturation, which may be relevant to studying developmental disorders and neuropsychiatric syndromes (Dosenbach et al., 2010; Erus et al., 2015).

Furthermore, while controlling for age, we observed a significant negative correlation between control energy of both the bilateral middle cingulate cortex and executive function performance. The middle cingulate is a component of the fronto-parietal control system (Fair et al., 2007; Yeo et al., 2011) and is critical for executive tasks such as performance monitoring, error detection, and task switching (Apps et al., 2013; Vogt, 2016). This result suggests that individuals with better executive function may be able to transition to the fronto-parietal activation state more easily. This result is consistent with prior studies showing that executive function training improves efficiency in activating the executive network (Kozasa et al., 2012; Rueda et al., 2012), and that subjects with higher cognitive performance have lower brain cerebral metabolism cost in resting-state (Bastin et al., 2012) and have improved metabolic control (Ryan et al., 2006). Moreover, the decline of control energy of the bilateral middle cingulate cortex partially mediated the observed improvement of executive function with age. This result is consistent with prior literature suggesting that the optimization of the structural network is associated with better executive function (Baum et al., 2017; Wen et al., 2011), as the decline of control energy cost reflects the optimization of the structural brain network (Kim et al., 2018). However, it should be noted that this mediation effect was small, as the direct effect was much larger than the indirect effect.

While brain network dynamics are known to be nonlinear, we used a linearization model here. It should be acknowledged that this linearization constrains the model’s predictive power to short time-scales and to states in the immediate vicinity of the operating point. Nonetheless, it has been consistently demonstrated that linearization offers fundamental insights into nonlinear dynamics. For example, Honey et al. (2009) shows that predictions of function from structure can be obtained with both linear and nonlinear models. Second, if the linearized system is locally controllable along a specific trajectory in state space, then the original nonlinear system is also controllable along the same trajectory (Coron, 2007; Yan et al., 2017). Finally, linear controllers are often used to control nonlinear systems through gain scheduling in flight and process control (Leith and Leithead, 2000). Accordingly, while the linear control trajectories in our work suffer from consequences due to linearization, they also serve as a natural and informative prior for future work in principled neural stimulation and nonlinear control when more is known about the exact nature of the brain’s nonlinearity.

Several limitations should be noted. First, all data presented here were cross-sectional, which precludes inference regarding within-individual developmental effects. Ongoing follow-up of the PNC will yield informative longitudinal data, as will other large-scale studies such as the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development Study. Second, it should be noted that probabilistic tractography methods remain limited in their ability to fully resolve the complex white matter architecture of the human brain. However, these methods are currently considered state-of-the-art, and may be superior to tensor-based tractography in resolving crossing fibers (Behrens et al., 2007). Third, it should be noted that motion artifact is a major potential confound for any study of brain development, and prior studies by our group and others have shown that motion artifact can bias estimates of tractography and confound developmental inference (Baum et al., 2018). However, to limit the impact of this confound, we conducted rigorous quality assurance and included in-scanner motion as a covariate in all analyses. Fourth, it should be noted that most univariate effect sizes at system level reported in our work were small. However, prior work has consistently demonstrated that small samples systematically inflate the apparent effect size (Yarkoni, 2009), whereas large samples (such as this one, n = 946) provide a much more accurate estimate of the true effect size. Furthermore, in contrast to the small univariate effects observed at system level, some univariate effects at nodal level were at medium size and results from the multivariate analyses yielded large effect sizes in unseen data. Finally, the differences between the developmental effects with the motor system target state and that with the fronto-parietal system target state could be due to the differences in size or in spatial congruence between the two target states. However, the non-significant developmental effect with motor target state is consistent with the evidence that motor development is largely complete by late childhood or early adolescence (Andersen, 2003; Gogtay et al., 2004).

These potential limitations notwithstanding, we demonstrated that the topological structure of white matter networks is optimized during development to facilitate transitions to a fronto-parietal activation state. Moving forward, this framework may be useful for understanding the developmental substrates of executive dysfunction in diverse psychiatric disorders including psychosis and ADHD. Improved knowledge regarding both normal network development and abnormalities associated with psychopathology is a prerequisite for developing individualized interventions to alter disease trajectories and improve patient outcomes. In the future, advances in non-invasive neuromodulatory therapies may allow for targeted stimulation of specific brain regions that are optimally situated within the brain’s control architecture to facilitate transitions to specific target states (Medaglia et al., 2018). Such advances could potentially aid in the treatment of the wide range of neuropsychiatric disorders marked by executive dysfunction (Braun et al., 2018).

Materials and methods

Participants

All subjects or their parent/guardian provided informed consent, and minors provided assent. The Institutional Review Boards of both Penn and CHOP approved study procedures. Overall, 1601 participants were enrolled (Satterthwaite et al., 2014). However, 340 subjects were excluded owing to clinical factors including medical disorders that could affect brain function, current use of psychoactive medications, prior inpatient psychiatric treatment, or an incidentally encountered structural brain abnormality. Among the 1261 subjects eligible for inclusion, 54 subjects were excluded for a low quality T1-weighted image or errors in the FreeSurfer reconstruction. Of the remaining 1207 subjects with a usable T1 image, 128 subjects were excluded because of the lack of a complete diffusion scan. Of the 1079 subjects with complete diffusion data, 110 subjects failed quality assurance as part a rigorous quality assurance protocol for diffusion MRI (Roalf et al., 2016). Additionally, 20 subjects were excluded because they had no field map for distortion correction. Finally, of the remaining 949 subjects, three subjects were excluded due to incomplete image coverage during brain parcellation, yielding a final sample of 946 participants (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

Cognitive assessment

The Penn computerized neurocognitive battery (Penn CNB) was administered to all participants during a separate session from neuroimaging. The CNB consists of 14 tests adapted from tasks applied in functional neuroimaging to evaluate a broad range of cognitive domains (Gur et al., 2012). These domains include executive control (abstraction and mental flexibility, attention, working memory), episodic memory (verbal, facial, spatial), complex cognition (verbal reasoning, nonverbal reasoning, spatial processing), social cognition (emotion identification, emotion differentiation, age differentiation) and sensorimotor and motor speed. Accuracy and speed for each test were z-transformed and summarized into an efficiency score. A factor analysis was used to summarize these efficiency scores into four factors (Moore et al., 2015), including executive function, complex reasoning, memory, and social cognition. Here, we focused on the executive function factor score. Of the sample of 946 participants with complete imaging data that passed quality assurance, two participants had incomplete cognitive data. Accordingly, 944 participants were used in the analysis examining the association between cognition and control energy.

Image acquisition

As previously described (Satterthwaite et al., 2014), all MRI scans were acquired on the same 3T Siemens Tim Trio whole-body scanner and 32-channel head coil at the Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania.

Structural MRI

Prior to dMRI acquisitions, a 5 min magnetization-prepared, rapid acquisition gradient-echo T1-weighted (MPRAGE) image (TR = 1810 ms; TE = 3.51 ms; FOV = 180 × 240 mm2, matrix = 192 × 256, effective voxel resolution = 0.9 × 0.9×1 mm3) was acquired. This high-resolution structural image was used for tissue segmentation and parcellating gray matter into anatomically defined regions in native space.

Diffusion MRI

Diffusion MRI scans were acquired using a twice-refocused spin-echo (TRSE) single-shot echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR = 8100 ms; TE = 82 ms; FOV = 240 mm2/240 mm2; Matrix = RL:128, AP:128; Slices: 70, in-plane resolution (x and y) 1.875 mm2; slice thickness = 2 mm, gap = 0; flip angle = 90/180/180; volumes = 71; GRAPPA factor = 3; bandwidth = 2170 Hz/pixel; PE direction = AP). This sequence used a four-lobed diffusion encoding gradient scheme combined with a 90-180-180 spin-echo sequence designed to minimize eddy-current artifacts. For dMRI acquisition, a 64-direction set was divided into two independent 32-direction imaging runs in order to ensure that the scan duration was more tolerable for young subjects. Each 32-direction sub-set was chosen to be maximally independent such that they separately sampled the surface of a sphere (Jones et al., 2002). The complete sequence consisted of 64 diffusion-weighted directions with b = 1000 s/mm2 and 7 interspersed scans where b = 0 s/mm2. The total duration of dMRI scans was approximately 11 min. The imaging volume was prescribed in axial orientation covering the entire cerebrum with the topmost slice just superior to the apex of the brain (Satterthwaite et al., 2014).

N-back task fMRI

Blood oxygen level-dependent fMRI was acquired using a whole-brain, single-shot, multislice, gradient-echo echoplanar sequence with the following parameters: 231 volumes; TR = 300 ms; TE = 32 ms; flip angle = 90; FOV = 192 × 192 mm; matrix = 64 × 64; 46 slices; slice thickness/gap = 3/0 mm; effective voxel resolution = 3 × 3×3 mm.

Field map

In addition, a B0 field map was derived for application of distortion correction procedures, using following the double-echo, gradient-recalled echo (GRE) sequence: TR = 1000 ms; TE1 = 2.69 ms; TE2 = 5.27 ms; 44 slices; slice thickness/gap = 4/0 mm; FOV = 240 mm; effective voxel resolution = 3.8 × 3.8×4 mm.

Scanning procedure

Before scanning, to acclimate subjects to the MRI environment, a mock scanning session where subjects practiced the task was conducted using a decommissioned MRI scanner and head coil. Mock scanning was accompanied by acoustic recordings of the noise produced by gradient coils for each scanning pulse sequence. During these sessions, feedback regarding head movement was provided using the MoTrack motion tracking system (Psychology Software Tools). Motion feedback was given only during the mock scanning session. To further minimize motion, before data acquisition, subjects’ heads were stabilized in the head coil using one foam pad over each ear and a third over the top of the head.

Image processing

Structural image processing and network node definition

The structural image was processed using FreeSurfer (version 5.3) (Fischl, 2012), and cortical and subcortical gray matter was parcellated in native structural space according to the Lausanne atlas (Cammoun et al., 2012), which includes whole-brain sub-divisions of the Desikan-Killany anatomical atlas (Desikan et al., 2006) at multiple spatial scales. The acquired 233-region gray matter parcellation of each subject was dilated by 2 mm and then masked by the boundary of each subject’s white matter segmentation (Baum et al., 2018). Once defined for each subject, the structural parcellation atlas was co-registered to the first b = 0 vol of each subject’s diffusion image using boundary-based registration (Greve and Fischl, 2009). These parcels were then used as nodes for brain network construction. The left lateral occipital parcel was missing in 18 subjects and therefore was removed from analyses, yielding 232 brain regions that were present in all participants.

Diffusion image pre-processing

FSL was used for diffusion data processing (Jenkinson et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2004). The two consecutive 32-direction acquisitions were merged into a single 64-direction time series. In-scanner head motion and the total network strength were used as covariates in this study. Specifically, in-scanner head motion was measured by the mean relative volume-to-volume displacement between the higher SNR b = 0 images (n = 7), which summarizes the total translation and rotation in 3-dimensional Euclidean space (Baum et al., 2018; Roalf et al., 2016). A mask in subject diffusion space was defined by registering a binary mask of a standard fractional anisotropy (FA) map (FMRIB58 FA) to each subject’s dMRI reference image (mean b = 0) using FSL FLIRT. This mask was provided as input to FSL eddy in addition to the non-brain extracted dMRI image. Eddy currents and subject motion were estimated and corrected using the FSL eddy tool (version 5.0.5: [Andersson and Sotiropoulos, 2016]). This procedure uses a Gaussian Process to simultaneously model the effects of eddy currents and head motion on diffusion-weighted volumes, resampling the data only once. Diffusion gradient vectors were also rotated to adjust for subject motion estimated by eddy (Leemans and Jones, 2009). After the field map was estimated, distortion correction was then applied to dMRI images using FSL’s FUGUE utility.

Probabilistic tractography and network construction

We first fitted a ball-and-sticks diffusion model for each subject’s dMRI data with FSL bedpostx, which uses Markov chain Monte Carlo sampling to build distributions on principal fiber orientation and diffusion parameters at each voxel (Behrens et al., 2007). Probabilistic tractography was run using FSL probtrackx, which repetitively samples voxel-wise fiber orientation distributions to model the spatial trajectory and strength of anatomical connectivity between specified seed and target regions (Behrens et al., 2007).

Each cortical and subcortical region defined along the gray-white boundary was selected as a seed region, and its connectivity strength to each of the other 231 regions was calculated using probabilistic tractography. At each seed voxel, 1000 samples were initiated. We used the default tracking parameters (a step-length of 0.5 mm, 2000 steps maximum, curvature threshold of 0.02). To increase the biological plausibility of white matter pathways reconstructed with probabilistic tractography, streamlines were terminated if they traveled through the pial surface, and discarded if they traversed cerebro-spinal fluid (CSF) in ventricles or re-entered the seed region (Baum et al., 2018). The connection probability from the seed voxel i to another voxel j was defined by the number of fibers passing through voxel j divided by the total number of fibers that were not rejected by exclusion criteria sampled from voxel i. For a seed cortical region, 1,000 × n fibers were sampled (1000 fibers per voxel), where n is the number of voxels in this region. The number of fibers passing through a given region divided by 1,000 × n is calculated as the connectivity probability from the seed region to this given region. Therefore, a 232*232 connection probability matrix was created for each subject. Notably, the probability from region i to region j is not necessarily equivalent to the one from region j to region i due to the dependence of tractography on the seeding location. Thus, we defined the unidirectional connectivity probability Pij between region i and region j by averaging these two probabilities (Baum et al., 2018; Gong et al., 2009a).

Defining a priori network modules

Each of the 232 nodes in our network was assigned to a standard set of 7 functional systems originally defined by Yeo et al. (2011) in a whole-brain clustering analysis. To make this assignment, we calculated the purity index for the 7-system parcellation and brain regions from the Lausanne 232 parcellation atlas as in prior work (Baum et al., 2017). This measure quantifies the maximum overlap of cortical Lausanne labels and functional systems defined by Yeo et al. (2011). Each cortical Lausanne label was assigned to a functional system by calculating the non-zero mode of all voxels in each brain region (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Subcortical regions were assigned to an eighth, subcortical module.

Control analysis

We investigated how a structural brain network composed of white matter fiber tracts constrains the brain in transitioning from a baseline state (i.e., 1 × 232 zero vector) to a fronto-parietal activation state, which was defined as regions in the fronto-parietal system that had activity magnitude equal to one while other regions had activity magnitude equal to 0. According to previous studies (Betzel et al., 2016; Gu et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2018; Stiso et al., 2019), we employed a simplified noise-free linear continuous-time and time-invariant network model:

x˙(t)=Ax(t)+Bu(t) (1)

Here, x(t) is a 1 × N vector that represents the brain state at a given time, where N is the number of ROIs (N = 232). The initial sate x(0) is a 1 × 232 zero vector, and the target state xT is a 1 × 232 vector of fronto-parietal activation. The matrix A encodes the connection probability weighted network, where A has been scaled by its largest eigenvalue and had the identity matrix subtracted to assure that it is stable (Betzel et al., 2016; Gu et al., 2017; Karrer et al., 2020; Stiso et al., 2019). The matrix B is a N × N input matrix that identifies the nodes in the control set. Here, B is an identity matrix because all 232 regions in the whole brain were control nodes. The input u(t) denotes the control energy injected for each node at a given time.

This work aims to model the control process necessary to activate the fronto-parietal system, which is critical to executive function. We set the baseline state to zero, because we sought to model the contrast in activation between an executive task and the resting state. This comparison is motivated by a long history of task fMRI experiments that explicitly contrast executive tasks to the resting state, resulting in robust activation of the fronto-parietal cortex (Cohen et al., 1997; Forsyth et al., 2014; Nagel et al., 2009; Ragland et al., 2002; Rowe et al., 2000). We set the values of regions in the fronto-parietal system to one to represent the fact that these regions were activated.

We were interested in a control task where the system transitions from initial state x(0) to target state xT with minimum-energy input, which is an optimal control problem. We first defined a cost function as the weighted sum of the energy cost of the transition and the integrated squared distance between the transition states and the target state.

minu0T(xTx(t))TS(xTx(t))+ρu(t)Tu(t)dt,s.t. x˙(t)=Ax(t)+Bu(t), x(0)= x0, and x(T)=xT, (2)

where xT is the target state, (xT-x(t))T(xT-x(t)) is the distance between the state at time t and the target state xT, T is a free parameter that defines the finite amount of time given to reach the target state, and ρ is a free parameter that weights the energy constraint. Because the time of each step was defined as 0.001, there were 1,000 steps from initial to target state if we set T=1. S is 0-1 diagonal matrix of size N×N that selects only the nodes that we wish to control. Here, we only constrain the activity of the fronto-parietal system. Specially, (xT-x(t))TS(xT-x(t)) constrains the trajectories of all nodes in fronto-parietal system by preventing the system from traveling too far from the target state, and utTu(t) constrains the amount of energy used to reach the target state.

To compute an optimal u* that induces a transition from the initial state x(0) to the target state xT, we define a Hamiltonian as:

H(p,x,u,t)=xT-xTS(xT-x)+ρuTu+p(Ax+Bu) (3)

From the Pontryagin minimum principle (Boltyanskii et al., 1960), if u* is a solution to the minimization problem with corresponding trajectory x*, then there exists p* such that:

Hx=2S(xTx)+ATp=p˙, (4)
Hu=2ρu+ BTp=0. (5)

From Equation (5) and Equation (1), we derive that

u=12ρBTp, (6)
x˙=Ax12ρBBTp. (7)

Then, we rewrite Equations (4) and (7) as

[x˙p˙]=[A12ρBBT2SAT][xp]+[02S]xT, (8)

We denote:

A~=[A12ρBBT2SAT],
x~=[xp],
b~=[02S]xT,

Then, Equation (8) can be reduced as:

x~˙=A~x~+b~,

Which can be solved as:

x~(t)=eA~tx~(0)+A~1(eA~tI)b~. (9)

Then, by fixing t = T, we rewrote Equation (9) as

x~(T)=eA~Tx~(0)+A~1(eA~TI)b~. (10)

Let

c=A~1(eA~TI)b~,
eAT~=[E11E12E21E22].

We can then rewrite Equation (10) as:

[x(T)p(T)]=[E11E12E21E22][x(0)p(0)]+[c1c2],

from which we can obtain

x*T=E11x*0+E12p*0+c1,

which can be rearranged to

p*0=E12-1x*T-E11x*0-c1.

Now that we have obtained p*(0), we can use it and x(0) to solve for x~ via forward integration according to Equation (9). To solve for u*, we take p* from our solution of x~ and plug it into Equation (6).

To quantify differences in trajectories, and the ease of controlling the system, we calculated a single measure of energy for every trajectory. Particularly, the energy of each control node i was defined as:

Ei=t=0Tui*t2.

Comparison to null model network

In order to determine whether the topology of brain networks specifically facilitated transitions to the fronto-parietal activation target state, we compared the energetic cost to that of null model networks. Specifically, for each participant we constructed 100 null model networks where the degree and strength distribution was preserved (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). We compared the control energy cost of the transition to the fronto-parietal activation target state estimated from the empirical networks to the average energy cost estimated in these null networks using a paired t-test.

Statistical analyses of developmental and cognition effects

Prior studies demonstrated that the developmental changes of brain structure and function could be either linear (Hagmann et al., 2010; Wierenga et al., 2016) or non-linear (Grayson and Fair, 2017; Mills et al., 2016; Vandekar et al., 2015). Accordingly, for our developmental analyses we used generalized additive models (GAMs) in order to simultaneously model linear and nonlinear relationships with age using penalized splines (Wood, 2004). We evaluated associations between control energy and age at multiple resolutions, including the whole brain, cognitive systems, and network nodes. Similarly, we evaluated associations between control energy and executive performance while controlling for age. For all models, we included sex, handedness, total brain volume, total network strength, and in-scanner head motion during the diffusion scan as model covariates. Multiple comparisons were accounted for using the False Discovery Rate (q < 0.05). For developmental effect of control energy, the GAM model was:

  • Energy = spline(Age) + Sex + Handedness + Motion + TBV + Network Strength + intercept.

  • For the associations between control energy and executive performance, the GAM model was:

  • Energy = Executive Performance + spline(Age) + Sex + Handedness + Motion + TBV + Network Strength + intercept.

We used the gam command in the R package ‘mgcv’ to implement the model. The spline term estimates a nonparametric smooth function for age-related differences in control energy, which can include linear or nonlinear effects depending on the structure of the data. Restricted maximum likelihood is used to penalize non-linearity in order to prevent overfitting (Wood, 2004).

Furthermore, for regions that displayed the associations between control energy and both age and cognition, we evaluated whether regional control energy might mediate the relationship between age and executive function. Specifically, we regressed out the effects of nuisance covariates (i.e., sex, handedness, total brain volume, total network strength, and in-scanner head motion) on the independent (X, age), dependent (Y, executive efficiency) and mediating (M, control energy) variables using a linear model. The resultant normalized residuals were used in our mediation analysis. We then evaluated the significance of the indirect effect using bootstrapped confidence intervals within the R package lavaan. Then, we examined: 1) path c: the total effect of age on executive performance; 2) path a: the relationship between age and the control energy; 3) path b: the relationship between control energy and executive performance; and 4) path c’: the age effect of executive function controlling for the mediator/control energy. The mediation/indirect effect a*b is the effect size of the relationship between age and executive performance that was reduced after controlling for the mediator/control energy. For each path, we calculated the beta coefficient, which reflected the changes of the outcome for every one-unit change in the predictor. A bootstrap analysis (i.e., resampled 10,000 times) was implemented to estimate the confidence intervals for the indirect effect.

Prediction of brain maturity from the pattern of control energy

As a complement to the mass-univariate analyses described above, we also sought to predict individual brain maturity using the multivariate pattern of control energy (Dosenbach et al., 2010; Erus et al., 2015; Franke et al., 2012). We used ridge regression with nested two-fold cross validation (2F-CV).

Ridge regression

A linear regression model was adopted to predict brain maturity using the pattern of whole-brain control energy. The linear model can be formalized as follows:

yi=j=1pβjxi,j+β0,

where yi is the age of the ith individual, p is the number of features, xi,j is the value of the jth feature of the ith subject, and βj is the regression coefficient.

To avoid over-fitting and to improve the prediction accuracy, we applied ridge regression (Cui and Gong, 2018; Hoerl and Kennard, 1970; Siegel et al., 2016), which used an L2 penalty during model fitting. The objective function is:

min βi=1N(f(xi)-yi)2+λj=1p||βj||2.

This technique shrinks the regression coefficients, resulting in better generalizability for predicting unseen samples. In this algorithm, a regularization parameter λ is used to control the trade-off between the prediction error of the training data and L2-norm regularization, i.e., a trade-off of penalties between the training error and model complexity. A large λ corresponds to a greater penalty on model complexity, and a small λ represents a greater penalty on training error. Compared with the traditional ordinary least squares regression, ridge regression is less impacted by multicollinearity and can avoid over-fitting (Cui and Gong, 2018).

Prediction framework

See Figur 3—figure supplement 1 for the schematic overview of the prediction framework. Specifically, we applied a nested 2-fold cross validation (2F-CV), with outer 2F-CV estimating the generalizability of the model and the inner 2F-CV determining the optimal parameter λ for the ridge regression model.

Outer 2F-CV

In the outer 2F-CV, all subjects were divided into 2 subsets. Specifically, we sorted the subjects according to their age and then assigned the individuals with an odd rank to subset 1 and the individuals with an even rank to subset 2 (Cui and Gong, 2018; Cui et al., 2018). We first used subset 1 as a training set, and we used subset 2 as a testing set. Each feature was linearly scaled between zero and one across the training dataset, and the scaling parameters were also applied to scale the testing dataset (Cui and Gong, 2018; Cui et al., 2018). We applied an inner 2-fold cross validation (2F-CV) within training set to select the optimal λ parameter. Based on the optimal λ, we trained a model using all subjects in the training set, and then used that model to predict the age of all subjects in the testing set. Analogously, we used subset 2 as a training set and subset 1 as a testing set, and repeated the above procedure. Across the testing subjects for each fold, the correlation and mean absolute error (MAE) between the predicted and actual age was used to quantify the prediction accuracy. Here, we used the scikit-learn library to implement ridge regression (http://scikit-learn.org) (Pedregosa et al., 2011).

Inner 2F-CV

Within each loop of the outer 2F-CV, we applied inner 2F-CVs to determine the optimal λ. Specially, the training set for each loop of the outer 2F-CV was further partitioned into 2 subsets according to their rank of the age, as like the outer loop (i.e., subjects with odd rank in subset 1 and subjects with even rank in subset 2). One subset was selected to train the model under a given λ in the range [2−10, 2−9,..., 24, 25] (i.e., 16 values in total) (Cui and Gong, 2018), and the remaining subset was used to test the model. This procedure was repeated 2 times such that each subset was used once as the testing dataset, resulting in 2 inner 2F-CV loops in total. For each inner 2F-CV loop, the correlation r between the actual and predicted age and the mean absolute error (MAE) were calculated for each λ, and averaged over each fold. The sum of the mean correlation r and reciprocal of the mean MAE was defined as the inner prediction accuracy, and the λ with the highest inner prediction accuracy was chosen as the optimal λ (Cui and Gong, 2018; Cui et al., 2018). Of note, the mean correlation r and the reciprocal of the mean MAE cannot be summed directly, because the scales of the raw values of these two measures are quite different. Therefore, we normalized the mean correlation r and the reciprocal of the mean MAE across all values and then summed the resultant normalized values.

Evaluation of generalizability

The correlation and mean absolute error (MAE) between the predicted ‘brain age’ and chronological age was used to quantify the degree to which the model captured the development trajectory of the brain. Particularly, when we calculated the correlation, we controlled sex, handedness, total brain volume, total network strength, and in-scanner head motion.

Interpreting the model

In a linear prediction model such as ridge regression, one weight/regression coefficient was assigned for each feature/brain region. We trained a prediction model using all the samples and acquired a weight vector w. According to Haufe et al. (2014); Waskom and Wagner (2017), we left multiplied the model weight vector w by the data covariance matrix ∑X, which was formulized as a = ∑X· w. The transformed weight vector a was a distributed pattern quantifying the contribution of each brain region in the multivariate ridge prediction. The absolute value of the transformed weight represents the importance of the corresponding feature in a prediction (Haufe et al., 2014; Mourão-Miranda et al., 2005).

Randomly split 2F-CV

In the above prediction analysis, we split subjects into two halves according to their age rank. For completeness, we also split the subjects randomly into two halves for both outer 2F-CV and inner 2F-CV, and calculated the mean partial correlation r and MAE across two folds. Because the split is random, we repeated this procedure 100 times and averaged the partial correlation and MAE across the 100 times to acquire the final prediction accuracy.

Specificity and sensitivity analysis

We conducted several additional supplementary analyses to assess the sensitivity and specificity of our results. First, in order to evaluate the robustness of our results to variation in target states, we additionally generated 100 new initial states and 100 new target states with noise added. In this distribution of initial states, the activation value of regions in the fronto-parietal system is Gaussian with a mean value of 0 and a standard deviation of 0.1, while in the distribution of target states, the activation value of regions in the fronto-parietal system is Gaussian with a mean value of 1 and a standard deviation of 0.1. Second, to ensure the observed associations with age were driven by the topological structure of real brain networks, we tested whether age effects existed using null networks that preserved the degree and strength distribution. We created 100 null networks and calculated the one-tailed P value for effect size of whole-brain and fronto-parietal system, which was the portion of null networks that showed a lower negative effect size value than the actual value for the real network. Third, we assessed whether the structural network also contributed to other cognitive functions as well by comparing the control energy cost required to reach a motor activation state for real networks and null networks. We further evaluated the age effects of control energy cost needed to activate the motor system.

Fourth, the present work explored a specific transition of the brain from a baseline state to a state of fronto-parietal activation by enacting multi-point control. In contrast, modal controllability quantifies the difficulties of transitioning to all possible states via single-node control (Gu et al., 2015). Modal controllability identifies brain areas that can push the brain into difficult-to-reach states; our prior work has shown that modal controllability increases with age in youth (Tang et al., 2017). Accordingly, it is important to establish whether our present results were driven by developmental changes in modal controllability. As in Tang et al. (2017), before calculating controllability, we scaled the matrix by 1+ξ0, where ξ0 is the largest eigenvalue value of the matrix. Next, we conducted sensitivity analyses where we controlled for modal controllability by including it as a covariate in the regression equation at each resolution of analysis (e.g., whole brain, functional system, network nodes). Specifically, we controlled for nodal modal controllability in nodal analysis of control energy, controlled for the average modal controllability of each system for system-level analysis, and controlled for the whole-brain average modal controllability for whole-brain analysis.

Fifth, one might expect that the modular organization of the brain’s structural network could potentially change the control energy cost of brain state transitions (Avena-Koenigsberger et al., 2017). Prior work has reported age-related increases in brain network modularity during youth (Baum et al., 2017; Hagmann et al., 2010; Huang et al., 2015). Here, we evaluated if observed developmental associations with control energy might be driven by changes in network modularity. We calculated network modularity quality (Q) using the community structure defined by the functional atlas (Yeo et al., 2011) as in Baum et al. (2017). For comparability with analyses of control energy, we scaled the matrix by the maximum eigenvalue before calculating Q. However, for this specific analysis, we did not subtract the identity matrix because it would lead to (uninterpretable) negative values of Q. We controlled for Q by including it as a model covariate in sensitivity analyses, which were conducted at all resolutions (whole brain, functional systems, and network nodes). Further, we evaluated the possibility that the segregation of the fronto-parietal system during youth (Baum et al., 2017) could explain the age effect of control energy. We calculated the average participation coefficient of the fronto-parietal system, and evaluated if developmental associations with control energy in the fronto-parietal system remained while controlling for the average participation coefficient in this system alongside with other covariates.

Finally, we evaluated an alternative, biologically recorded target state that was defined using the activation pattern from a working memory task that reliably recruits the fronto-parietal network and executive system. Specifically, the target state was defined as the average participants’ 2-back > 0-back contrast from a fractal n-back working memory task (Ragland et al., 2002); task design and image processing was as previously detailed (Satterthwaite et al., 2013). For each participant, we calculated the control energy cost to transition from the baseline (zero) state to this target activation state defined by the average pattern of activation recruited by the working memory task. As in the main analyses, we compared the control energy cost from real brain networks and that from null networks. Furthermore, we calculated the developmental association between control energy and age at multiple scales, including the whole brain, each cognitive system, and for each network node (with covariates as prior).

Acknowledgements

Thanks to Chad Jackson, in memoriam. This study was supported by grants from National Institute of Mental Health: R21MH106799 (DSB and TDS) and R01MH113550 (TDS and DSB). Additional support was provided by R01MH107703 (TDS), RF1MH116920 (DJO, TDS and DSB), R01MH112847 (RTS and TDS), R01MH107235 (RCG), and R01EB022573 (CD), K01MH102609 (DRR), R01NS085211 (RTS), and the Penn-CHOP Lifespan Brain Institute. The PNC was supported by MH089983 and MH089924. Additionally, DSB acknowledges support from the John D and Catherine T MacArthur Foundation, the Alfred P Sloan Foundation, the ISI Foundation, the Paul Allen Foundation, the Army Research Laboratory (W911NF-10-2-0022), the Army Research Office (Bassett-W911NF-14-1-0679, Grafton-W911NF-16-1-0474, DCISTW911NF-17-2-0181), the Office of Naval Research, National Institute of Health (2-R01-DC-009209–11, 1R01HD086888-01, R01 – MH112847, R01-MH107235), National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R01 NS099348), and National Science Foundation (BCS-1441502, BCS-1430087, NSF PHY-1554488 and BCS- 1631550). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of any of the funding agencies.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Theodore D Satterthwaite, Email: sattertt@pennmedicine.upenn.edu.

Thomas Yeo, National University of Singapore, Singapore.

Timothy E Behrens, University of Oxford, United Kingdom.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institute of Mental Health R21MH106799 to Danielle S Bassett, Theodore D Satterthwaite.

  • National Institute of Mental Health R01MH113550 to Theodore D Satterthwaite, Danielle S Bassett.

  • National Institute of Mental Health R01MH107703 to Theodore D Satterthwaite.

  • National Institute of Mental Health RF1MH116920 to Desmond J Oathes.

  • National Institutes of Health R01MH112847 to Russell T Shinohara, Theodore D Satterthwaite.

  • National Institutes of Health R01MH107235 to Ruben C Gur.

  • National Institutes of Health R01EB022573 to Christos Davatzikos.

  • National Institutes of Health K01MH102609 to David R Roalf.

  • National Institutes of Health R01NS085211 to Russell T Shinohara.

  • John D and Catherine T MacArthur Foundation to Danielle S Bassett.

  • Alfred P. Sloan Foundation to Danielle S Bassett.

  • ISI Foundation to Danielle S Bassett.

  • Paul G. Allen Family Foundation to Danielle S Bassett.

  • Army Research Laboratory W911NF-10-2-0022 to Danielle S Bassett.

  • Army Research Office Bassett-W911NF-14-1-0679 to Danielle S Bassett.

  • Army Research Office Grafton-W911NF-16-1-0474 to Danielle S Bassett.

  • Army Research Office DCISTW911NF-17-2-0181 to Danielle S Bassett.

  • Office of Naval Research to Danielle S Bassett.

  • National Institutes of Health 2-R01-DC-009209–11 to Danielle S Bassett.

  • National Institutes of Health 1R01HD086888-01 to Danielle S Bassett.

  • National Institutes of Health R01-MH112847 to Danielle S Bassett.

  • National Institutes of Health R01-MH107235 to Danielle S Bassett.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke R01 NS099348 to Danielle S Bassett.

  • National Science Foundation BCS-1441502 to Danielle S Bassett.

  • National Science Foundation BCS-1430087 to Danielle S Bassett.

  • National Science Foundation NSF PHY-1554488 to Danielle S Bassett.

  • National Science Foundation BCS-1631550 to Danielle S Bassett.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

has received legal consulting and advisory board income from Genentech/Roche.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Project administration.

Validation, Methodology.

Data curation, Software.

Methodology.

Data curation.

Methodology.

Methodology.

Methodology.

Visualization.

Methodology.

Data curation.

Resources.

Data curation.

Resources.

Resources.

Resources.

Methodology.

Resources.

Resources.

Conceptualization, Resources, Software, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration.

Ethics

Human subjects: All subjects or their parent/guardian provided informed consent, and minors provided assent. The Institutional Review Boards of both Penn and CHOP approved study procedures (IRB-approved protocol number 810336).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

The PNC data is publicly available in the Database of Genotypes and Phenotypes: accession number: phs000607.v3.p2; https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/projects/gap/cgi-bin/study.cgi?study_id=phs000607.v3.p2. All analysis code is available here: https://github.com/ZaixuCui/pncControlEnergy (copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/pncControlEnergy), with detailed explanation in https://github.com/ZaixuCui/pncControlEnergy/wiki.

The following previously published dataset was used:

Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, Loughead J, Prabhakaran K, Calkins ME, Hopson R, Jackson C, Keefe J, Riley M, Mentch FD, Sleiman P, Verma R, Davatzikos C, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE. 2014. Neuroimaging of the Philadelphia neurodevelopmental cohort. NCBI. phs000607.v3.p2

References

  1. Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology Review. 2006;16:17–42. doi: 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andersen SL. Trajectories of brain development: point of vulnerability or window of opportunity? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2003;27:3–18. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(03)00005-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andersson JLR, Sotiropoulos SN. An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. NeuroImage. 2016;125:1063–1078. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Apps MA, Lockwood PL, Balsters JH. The role of the midcingulate cortex in monitoring others' decisions. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2013;7:251. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Arffa S. The relationship of intelligence to executive function and non-executive function measures in a sample of average, above average, and gifted youth. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2007;22:969–978. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Avena-Koenigsberger A, Misic B, Sporns O. Communication dynamics in complex brain networks. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2017;19:17–33. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;121:65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bastin C, Yakushev I, Bahri MA, Fellgiebel A, Eustache F, Landeau B, Scheurich A, Feyers D, Collette F, Chételat G, Salmon E. Cognitive reserve impacts on inter-individual variability in resting-state cerebral metabolism in normal aging. NeuroImage. 2012;63:713–722. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Baum GL, Ciric R, Roalf DR, Betzel RF, Moore TM, Shinohara RT, Kahn AE, Vandekar SN, Rupert PE, Quarmley M, Cook PA, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, Gur RE, Gur RC, Bassett DS, Satterthwaite TD. Modular segregation of structural brain networks supports the development of executive function in youth. Current Biology. 2017;27:1561–1572. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Baum GL, Roalf DR, Cook PA, Ciric R, Rosen AFG, Xia C, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, Verma R, Tunç B, Gur RC, Gur RE, Bassett DS, Satterthwaite TD. The impact of in-scanner head motion on structural connectivity derived from diffusion MRI. NeuroImage. 2018;173:275–286. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Behrens TE, Berg HJ, Jbabdi S, Rushworth MF, Woolrich MW. Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: what can we gain? NeuroImage. 2007;34:144–155. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Best JR, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences. 2011;21:327–336. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Best JR, Miller PH. A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development. 2010;81:1641–1660. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Betzel RF, Gu S, Medaglia JD, Pasqualetti F, Bassett DS. Optimally controlling the human connectome: the role of network topology. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:30770. doi: 10.1038/srep30770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Boltyanskii VGe, Gamkrelidze RVy, Pontryagin LS. The Theory of Optimal Processes I. the Maximum Principle. TRW SPACE TECHNOLOGY LABS LOS ANGELES CALIF; 1960. [Google Scholar]
  16. Braun U, Schaefer A, Betzel RF, Tost H, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Bassett DS. From maps to Multi-dimensional network mechanisms of mental disorders. Neuron. 2018;97:14–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Meskaldji D, Thiran JP, Sporns O, Do KQ, Maeder P, Meuli R, Hagmann P. Mapping the human connectome at multiple scales with diffusion spectrum MRI. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2012;203:386–397. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.09.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cohen JD, Perlstein WM, Braver TS, Nystrom LE, Noll DC, Jonides J, Smith EE. Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature. 1997;386:604–608. doi: 10.1038/386604a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Coron J-M. Control and Nonlinearity. American Mathematical Soc; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  21. Cui Z, Su M, Li L, Shu H, Gong G. Individualized prediction of reading comprehension ability using gray matter volume. Cerebral Cortex. 2018;28:1656–1672. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cui Z, Gong G. The effect of machine learning regression algorithms and sample size on individualized behavioral prediction with functional connectivity features. NeuroImage. 2018;178:622–637. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Davatzikos C. Why voxel-based morphometric analysis should be used with great caution when characterizing group differences. NeuroImage. 2004;23:17–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT, Albert MS, Killiany RJ. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dosenbach NU, Nardos B, Cohen AL, Fair DA, Power JD, Church JA, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Vogel AC, Lessov-Schlaggar CN, Barnes KA, Dubis JW, Feczko E, Coalson RS, Pruett JR, Barch DM, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science. 2010;329:1358–1361. doi: 10.1126/science.1194144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Erus G, Battapady H, Satterthwaite TD, Hakonarson H, Gur RE, Davatzikos C, Gur RC. Imaging patterns of brain development and their relationship to cognition. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25:1676–1684. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fair DA, Dosenbach NU, Church JA, Cohen AL, Brahmbhatt S, Miezin FM, Barch DM, Raichle ME, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration. PNAS. 2007;104:13507–13512. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705843104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fischl B. FreeSurfer. NeuroImage. 2012;62:774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Forsyth JK, McEwen SC, Gee DG, Bearden CE, Addington J, Goodyear B, Cadenhead KS, Mirzakhanian H, Cornblatt BA, Olvet DM, Mathalon DH, McGlashan TH, Perkins DO, Belger A, Seidman LJ, Thermenos HW, Tsuang MT, van Erp TG, Walker EF, Hamann S, Woods SW, Qiu M, Cannon TD. Reliability of functional magnetic resonance imaging activation during working memory in a multi-site study: analysis from the north american prodrome longitudinal study. NeuroImage. 2014;97:41–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Franke K, Ziegler G, Klöppel S, Gaser C. Estimating the age of healthy subjects from T1-weighted MRI scans using kernel methods: Exploring the influence of various parameters. NeuroImage. 2010;50:883–892. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Franke K, Luders E, May A, Wilke M, Gaser C. Brain maturation: predicting individual BrainAGE in children and adolescents using structural MRI. NeuroImage. 2012;63:1305–1312. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, Nugent TF, Herman DH, Clasen LS, Toga AW, Rapoport JL, Thompson PM. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. PNAS. 2004;101:8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gong G, Rosa-Neto P, Carbonell F, Chen ZJ, He Y, Evans AC. Age- and gender-related differences in the cortical anatomical network. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009a;29:15684–15693. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2308-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gong G, He Y, Concha L, Lebel C, Gross DW, Evans AC, Beaulieu C. Mapping anatomical connectivity patterns of human cerebral cortex using in vivo diffusion tensor imaging tractography. Cerebral Cortex. 2009b;19:524–536. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Grayson DS, Fair DA. Development of large-scale functional networks from birth to adulthood: a guide to the neuroimaging literature. NeuroImage. 2017;160:15–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Greve DN, Fischl B. Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. NeuroImage. 2009;48:63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Gu S, Satterthwaite TD, Medaglia JD, Yang M, Gur RE, Gur RC, Bassett DS. Emergence of system roles in normative neurodevelopment. PNAS. 2015;112:13681–13686. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1502829112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gu S, Betzel RF, Mattar MG, Cieslak M, Delio PR, Grafton ST, Pasqualetti F, Bassett DS. Optimal trajectories of brain state transitions. NeuroImage. 2017;148:305–317. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gur RC, Richard J, Calkins ME, Chiavacci R, Hansen JA, Bilker WB, Loughead J, Connolly JJ, Qiu H, Mentch FD, Abou-Sleiman PM, Hakonarson H, Gur RE. Age group and sex differences in performance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in children age 8-21. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:251–265. doi: 10.1037/a0026712. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hagmann P, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Meuli R, Honey CJ, Wedeen VJ, Sporns O. Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLOS Biology. 2008;6:e159. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hagmann P, Sporns O, Madan N, Cammoun L, Pienaar R, Wedeen VJ, Meuli R, Thiran JP, Grant PE. White matter maturation reshapes structural connectivity in the late developing human brain. PNAS. 2010;107:19067–19072. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009073107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Haufe S, Meinecke F, Görgen K, Dähne S, Haynes JD, Blankertz B, Bießmann F. On the interpretation of weight vectors of linear models in multivariate neuroimaging. NeuroImage. 2014;87:96–110. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Haynes J-D. A Primer on Pattern-Based Approaches to fMRI: Principles, Pitfalls, and Perspectives. Neuron. 2015;87:257–270. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Haynes JD, Rees G. Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7:523–534. doi: 10.1038/nrn1931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. He W, Sowman PF, Brock J, Etchell AC, Stam CJ, Hillebrand A. Increased segregation of functional networks in developing brains. NeuroImage. 2019;200:607–620. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hermundstad AM, Bassett DS, Brown KS, Aminoff EM, Clewett D, Freeman S, Frithsen A, Johnson A, Tipper CM, Miller MB, Grafton ST, Carlson JM. Structural foundations of resting-state and task-based functional connectivity in the human brain. PNAS. 2013;110:6169–6174. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1219562110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hoerl AE, Kennard RW. Ridge regression: biased estimation for nonorthogonal problems. Technometrics. 1970;12:55–67. doi: 10.1080/00401706.1970.10488634. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Honey CJ, Sporns O, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Thiran JP, Meuli R, Hagmann P. Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity from structural connectivity. PNAS. 2009;106:2035–2040. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811168106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Huang H, Shu N, Mishra V, Jeon T, Chalak L, Wang ZJ, Rollins N, Gong G, Cheng H, Peng Y, Dong Q, He Y. Development of human brain structural networks through infancy and childhood. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25:1389–1404. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL. NeuroImage. 2012;62:782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Jog MA, Yan L, Kilroy E, Krasileva K, Jann K, LeClair H, Elashoff D, Wang DJJ. Developmental trajectories of cerebral blood flow and oxidative metabolism at Baseline and during working memory tasks. NeuroImage. 2016;134:587–596. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Jones DK, Griffin LD, Alexander DC, Catani M, Horsfield MA, Howard R, Williams SC. Spatial normalization and averaging of diffusion tensor MRI data sets. NeuroImage. 2002;17:592–617. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Karrer TM, Kim JZ, Stiso J, Kahn AE, Pasqualetti F, Habel U, Bassett DS. A practical guide to methodological considerations in the controllability of structural brain networks. arXiv. 2019 doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/ab6e8b. https://arxiv.org/abs/1908.03514 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  54. Karrer TM, Kim JZ, Stiso J, Kahn AE, Pasqualetti F, Habel U, Bassett DS. A practical guide to methodological considerations in the controllability of structural brain networks. Journal of Neural Engineering. 2020;17:026031. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/ab6e8b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kim JZ, Soffer JM, Kahn AE, Vettel JM, Pasqualetti F, Bassett DS. Role of graph architecture in controlling dynamical networks with applications to neural systems. Nature Physics. 2018;14:91–98. doi: 10.1038/nphys4268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kozasa EH, Sato JR, Lacerda SS, Barreiros MA, Radvany J, Russell TA, Sanches LG, Mello LE, Amaro E. Meditation training increases brain efficiency in an attention task. NeuroImage. 2012;59:745–749. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lee TH, Telzer EH. Negative functional coupling between the right fronto-parietal and limbic resting state networks predicts increased self-control and later substance use onset in adolescence. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2016;20:35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Leemans A, Jones DK. The B-matrix must be rotated when correcting for subject motion in DTI data. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2009;61:1336–1349. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Leith DJ, Leithead WE. Survey of gain-scheduling analysis and design. International Journal of Control. 2000;73:1001–1025. doi: 10.1080/002071700411304. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Mansouri FA, Koechlin E, Rosa MGP, Buckley MJ. Managing competing goals - a key role for the frontopolar cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2017;18:645–657. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Medaglia JD, Harvey DY, White N, Kelkar A, Zimmerman J, Bassett DS, Hamilton RH. Network controllability in the inferior frontal gyrus relates to controlled language variability and susceptibility to TMS. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:6399–6410. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0092-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Mills KL, Goddings AL, Herting MM, Meuwese R, Blakemore SJ, Crone EA, Dahl RE, Güroğlu B, Raznahan A, Sowell ER, Tamnes CK. Structural brain development between childhood and adulthood: convergence across four longitudinal samples. NeuroImage. 2016;141:273–281. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Mollink J, Smith SM, Elliott LT, Kleinnijenhuis M, Hiemstra M, Alfaro-Almagro F, Marchini J, van Cappellen van Walsum AM, Jbabdi S, Miller KL. The spatial correspondence and genetic influence of interhemispheric connectivity with white matter microstructure. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:809–819. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0379-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Moore TM, Reise SP, Gur RE, Hakonarson H, Gur RC. Psychometric properties of the penn computerized neurocognitive battery. Neuropsychology. 2015;29:235–246. doi: 10.1037/neu0000093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Mourão-Miranda J, Bokde AL, Born C, Hampel H, Stetter M. Classifying brain states and determining the discriminating activation patterns: support vector machine on functional MRI data. NeuroImage. 2005;28:980–995. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Nagel IE, Preuschhof C, Li SC, Nyberg L, Bäckman L, Lindenberger U, Heekeren HR. Performance level modulates adult age differences in brain activation during spatial working memory. PNAS. 2009;106:22552–22557. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908238106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Niendam TA, Laird AR, Ray KL, Dean YM, Glahn DC, Carter CS. Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2012;12:241–268. doi: 10.3758/s13415-011-0083-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Norman KA, Polyn SM, Detre GJ, Haxby JV. Beyond mind-reading: multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10:424–430. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Patankar SP, Kim JZ, Pasqualetti F, Bassett DS. Binary mesoscale architecture does not explain controllability of structural brain networks. arXiv. 2020 https://arxiv.org/abs/2002.06514
  70. Pedregosa F, Varoquaux G, Gramfort A, Michel V, Thirion B, Grisel O, Blondel M, Prettenhofer P, Weiss R, Dubourg V. Scikit-learn: machine learning in Python. Journal of Machine Learning Research : JMLR. 2011;12:2825–2830. [Google Scholar]
  71. Power JD, Fair DA, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. The development of human functional brain networks. Neuron. 2010;67:735–748. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Ragland JD, Turetsky BI, Gur RC, Gunning-Dixon F, Turner T, Schroeder L, Chan R, Gur RE. Working memory for complex figures: an fMRI comparison of letter and fractal n-back tasks. Neuropsychology. 2002;16:370–379. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.16.3.370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Roalf DR, Quarmley M, Elliott MA, Satterthwaite TD, Vandekar SN, Ruparel K, Gennatas ED, Calkins ME, Moore TM, Hopson R, Prabhakaran K, Jackson CT, Verma R, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE. The impact of quality assurance assessment on diffusion tensor imaging outcomes in a large-scale population-based cohort. NeuroImage. 2016;125:903–919. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Romer D, Betancourt L, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, Farah M, Hurt H. Executive cognitive functions and impulsivity as correlates of risk taking and problem behavior in preadolescents. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2916–2926. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rottschy C, Langner R, Dogan I, Reetz K, Laird AR, Schulz JB, Fox PT, Eickhoff SB. Modelling neural correlates of working memory: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. NeuroImage. 2012;60:830–846. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Rowe JB, Toni I, Josephs O, Frackowiak RS, Passingham RE. The prefrontal cortex: response selection or maintenance within working memory? Science. 2000;288:1656–1660. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5471.1656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. NeuroImage. 2010;52:1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Rueda MR, Checa P, Cómbita LM. Enhanced efficiency of the executive attention network after training in preschool children: immediate changes and effects after two months. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2012;2 Suppl 1:S192–S204. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Ryan CM, Freed MI, Rood JA, Cobitz AR, Waterhouse BR, Strachan MW. Improving metabolic control leads to better working memory in adults with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2006;29:345–351. doi: 10.2337/diacare.29.02.06.dc05-1626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Erus G, Ruparel K, Elliott MA, Gennatas ED, Hopson R, Jackson C, Prabhakaran K, Bilker WB, Calkins ME, Loughead J, Smith A, Roalf DR, Hakonarson H, Verma R, Davatzikos C, Gur RC, Gur RE. Functional maturation of the executive system during adolescence. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:16249–16261. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2345-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, Loughead J, Prabhakaran K, Calkins ME, Hopson R, Jackson C, Keefe J, Riley M, Mentch FD, Sleiman P, Verma R, Davatzikos C, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE. Neuroimaging of the Philadelphia neurodevelopmental cohort. NeuroImage. 2014;86:544–553. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Shanmugan S, Wolf DH, Calkins ME, Moore TM, Ruparel K, Hopson RD, Vandekar SN, Roalf DR, Elliott MA, Jackson C, Gennatas ED, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Shinohara RT, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE, Satterthwaite TD. Common and dissociable mechanisms of executive system dysfunction across psychiatric disorders in youth. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2016;173:517–526. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15060725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Sherman LE, Rudie JD, Pfeifer JH, Masten CL, McNealy K, Dapretto M. Development of the default mode and central executive networks across early adolescence: a longitudinal study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2014;10:148–159. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Siegel JS, Ramsey LE, Snyder AZ, Metcalf NV, Chacko RV, Weinberger K, Baldassarre A, Hacker CD, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Disruptions of network connectivity predict impairment in multiple behavioral domains after stroke. PNAS. 2016;113:E4367–E4376. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1521083113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage. 2004;23 Suppl 1:S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Srivastava P, Nozari E, Kim JZ, Ju H, Zhou D, Becker C, Pasqualetti F, Bassett DS. Models of communication and control for brain networks: distinctions, convergence, and future outlook. arXiv. 2020 doi: 10.1162/netn_a_00158. https://arxiv.org/abs/2002.07029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  87. Stiso J, Khambhati AN, Menara T, Kahn AE, Stein JM, Das SR, Gorniak R, Tracy J, Litt B, Davis KA, Pasqualetti F, Lucas TH, Bassett DS. White matter network architecture guides direct electrical stimulation through optimal state transitions. Cell Reports. 2019;28:2554–2566. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Takahashi T, Shirane R, Sato S, Yoshimoto T. Developmental changes of cerebral blood flow and oxygen metabolism in children. AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 1999;20:917–922. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Tang E, Giusti C, Baum GL, Gu S, Pollock E, Kahn AE, Roalf DR, Moore TM, Ruparel K, Gur RC, Gur RE, Satterthwaite TD, Bassett DS. Developmental increases in white matter network controllability support a growing diversity of brain dynamics. Nature Communications. 2017;8:1252. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01254-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Tang E, Bassett DS. Colloquium : Control of dynamics in brain networks. Reviews of Modern Physics. 2018;90:031003. doi: 10.1103/RevModPhys.90.031003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Thomason ME, Race E, Burrows B, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD. Development of spatial and verbal working memory capacity in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:316–332. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Vandekar SN, Shinohara RT, Raznahan A, Roalf DR, Ross M, DeLeo N, Ruparel K, Verma R, Wolf DH, Gur RC, Gur RE, Satterthwaite TD. Topologically dissociable patterns of development of the human cerebral cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:599–609. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3628-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Vogt BA. Midcingulate cortex: structure, connections, homologies, functions and diseases. Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy. 2016;74:28–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2016.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Waskom ML, Wagner AD. Distributed representation of context by intrinsic subnetworks in prefrontal cortex. PNAS. 2017;114:2030–2035. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1615269114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Wen W, Zhu W, He Y, Kochan NA, Reppermund S, Slavin MJ, Brodaty H, Crawford J, Xia A, Sachdev P. Discrete neuroanatomical networks are associated with specific cognitive abilities in old age. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:1204–1212. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4085-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Wierenga LM, van den Heuvel MP, van Dijk S, Rijks Y, de Reus MA, Durston S. The development of brain network architecture. Human Brain Mapping. 2016;37:717–729. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Wolf DH, Satterthwaite TD, Calkins ME, Ruparel K, Elliott MA, Hopson RD, Jackson CT, Prabhakaran K, Bilker WB, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE. Functional neuroimaging abnormalities in youth with psychosis spectrum symptoms. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:456–465. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.3169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Wood SN. Stable and efficient multiple smoothing parameter estimation for generalized additive models. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 2004;99:673–686. doi: 10.1198/016214504000000980. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Wu-Yan E, Betzel RF, Tang E, Gu S, Pasqualetti F, Bassett DS. Benchmarking measures of network controllability on canonical graph models. Journal of Nonlinear Science. 2017;1:39. doi: 10.1007/s00332-018-9448-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  100. Yan G, Vértes PE, Towlson EK, Chew YL, Walker DS, Schafer WR, Barabási AL. Network control principles predict neuron function in the Caenorhabditis elegans connectome. Nature. 2017;550:519–523. doi: 10.1038/nature24056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Yarkoni T. Big correlations in little studies: inflated fMRI correlations reflect low statistical Power-Commentary on vul et al. (2009) Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2009;4:294–298. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01127.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Roffman JL, Smoller JW, Zöllei L, Polimeni JR, Fischl B, Liu H, Buckner RL. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106:1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Thomas Yeo1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

In this study, Cui and colleagues utilized linear control theory to compute the amount of "energy" necessary for a brain to transition from a baseline state to a frontoparietal control state. They showed that minimum control energy necessary for the transition is lower for older participants and is maximal in the frontoparietal regions. The patterns of control energy can be used to predict participants' age. Furthermore, control energy of cingulate cortex is negatively correlated with executive function performance even after controlling for age. This works highlights a potential mechanism by which executive function develops. The work is technically excellent and the findings are likely to be of broad interest to the community. Moreover, the authors demonstrate the effect from multiple angles. We are also very impressed by the authors' response to the reviewers' comments.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Optimization of energy state transition trajectory supports the development of executive function during youth" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by Timothy Behrens as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

In this study, Cui and colleagues utilized linear control theory to compute the amount of "energy" necessary for a brain to transition from a baseline state (zero activity everywhere) to a frontoparietal control state (activity of ones in frontoparietal regions and zeros everywhere else). They showed that minimum control energy necessary for the transition is lower for older participants and is maximal in the frontoparietal regions. The patterns of control energy can be used to predict participants' age. Furthermore, control energy of cingulate cortex is negatively correlated with executive function performance even after controlling for age. This works highlights a potential mechanism by which executive function develops. The manuscript follows logically from the authors' previous work and convincingly demonstrates that network-mediated control of executive areas is correlated with age. The work is technically excellent and the findings are likely to be of broad interest to the community. Moreover, the authors demonstrate the effect from multiple angles.

Essential revisions:

1) For a life sciences journal, the manuscript is quite technical and uses a lot of jargon such as modal controllability that is not well defined and might not be understandable to a life science readership. It would be useful to better explain the biological basis of terms such as the control trajectory distance.

2) We suggest toning down language throughout the paper. Associations of r=0.17 can hardly be called strong, particularly given that this is cross-sectional sample. The extended discussion of electrical stimulation and neurofeedback is peripheral to the current results, given that these modalities were not investigated. I suggest keeping the discussion specific to the current results and providing some basic discussion of biological or network mechanisms that could potentially underpin the relation between control energy and age.

3) While network control theory is a novel way to capture the relation between brain network development, it would be useful to understand the basic network properties that change over age and therefore underpin the relation between age and control energy. As they currently stand, the control theory results are quite abstract and do not appear to provide insight into specific network-related or biological mechanisms that enable lower-cost transitions.

a) The computation of control energy is fully characterized by the network connectivity matrix. Therefore, any relation between age and control energy should be able to be traced back to relations between age and other simpler network properties of the connectivity matrix. It would be informative to evaluate whether the modular structure or connectivity strength relates to age, which could provide a simpler characterization of the control energy result that is closer to the underlying biology.

b) One possibility is that the FP network may show greater modularization with age, together with an overall increase in network integration. Indeed, the authors have previously suggested increased segregation of the FPN in this dataset (Baum et al. 2017). The fact that control energy is greater in the randomized null might be consistent with this notion. This kind of insight would be useful to link the control energy results and underlying network-level mechanisms.

c) A related possibility is that the results might simply be re-capitulating the fact that younger participants have weaker long range connections. First, it seems obvious that the best way to "activate" the frontoparietal regions would be to inject energy into the target (frontoparietal) regions, so it's not surprising that frontoparietal regions required the most control energy (Figure 1C). For older participants, this energy might be lower because frontoparietal regions are now more strongly connected to other regions, so the overall control energy (and frontoparietal control energy) can be reduced by "distributing" the control energy budget to other regions.

4) The authors should explicitly mention the definition of the final state and initial states in the Results section. While this is explained in the Materials and methods section, this should really be in the Results section because this is quite important.

5) The authors justified their choice of initial and final state as follows "We set the baseline state to zero, because we sought to model the contrast in activation between an executive task and the resting state. This comparison is motivated by a long history of task fMRI experiments that explicitly contrast executive tasks to the resting state, resulting in robust activation of the fronto-parietal cortex (Cohen et al., 1997; Forsyth et al., 2014; Nagel et al., 2009; Ragland et al., 2002; Rowe et al., 2000). We set the values of regions in the fronto-parietal system to 1 to represent the fact that these regions were activated." We agree with the rationale, but based on the rationale, it seems that a better initial state should be the fMRI activity pattern averaged across all time points during a resting-fMRI scan and the final state should be the fMRI activity pattern averaged across all time points during an executive function task.

6) Equation 2: "S is 0-1 diagonal matrix of size N x N that selects only the nodes that we wish to control. Here, we only constrain the activity of the fronto-parietal system." – Can the authors clarify this statement? Is S the identity matrix? We thought that all nodes are targeted since the target state comprises 1 for the frontoparietal nodes and 0 for other nodes. But the authors now seem to imply they only constrain the activity of the frontoparietal nodes.

7) "When model weights were examined at the level of individual network nodes, the regions that most contributed to the prediction of brain maturity aligned with univariate analyses, and included the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, the cingulate cortex, superior parietal cortex, and lateral temporal cortex" – It is well-known that model weights should not be interpreted without filtering (Haufe et al., 2014). Given the authors are using ridge regression, Haufe's approach is perfect for this situation.

8) The authors need to provide more details about how they controlled for linear and nonlinear effects of age in their analysis (e.g., correlation between control energy and executive performance). The authors mentioned generalized additive models and penalized splines, but the details were nowhere close to being sufficient.

9) The authors need to provide more details about the mediation analysis (e.g., assumptions). How do we interpret the betas in Figure 4C and D? Total effect is 0.67, while mediation effect is 0.03 and direct effect is 0.64. Doesn't this mean that the analysis is suggesting that the indirect effect is quite small relative to the direct effect? If so, this should be discussed.

10) The age prediction analysis requires motivation and better integration with the rest of the paper. It is likely that simpler features such as the tractography connectivity strengths or basic network properties would also yield good predictive models of age. Therefore, the reason for using a more complex feature space is unclear, unless it can be demonstrated that control energy can outperform the accuracy of more basic features, or reveals a specific mechanism that characterizes network development.

11) The relation identified between control energy and age appears to be linear based on Figure 2 and thus the reasons for fitting penalized splines to characterize nonlinear associations could be better described. Are the lines shown in Figure 2 representative of the splines? The claims regarding the specificity of the FP network require clarification. The null model corresponding to the rewired graph does not appear to have been evaluated for the other canonical networks. To establish specificity of the FP, the null model should also be considered for the other networks. Based on this global null model alone, we would suggest that it is hard to claim that the brain is explicitly wired to optimize transition cost to the FP activation state. Wiring organization is also likely to contribute to other cognitive functions as well.

12) In addition, it would be interesting to know whether the node-level measures are related to degree and/or strength. The authors use the two statistics as covariates in the predictive model, but do not show whether they are related to control energy directly.

eLife. 2020 Mar 27;9:e53060. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53060.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) For a life sciences journal, the manuscript is quite technical and uses a lot of jargon such as modal controllability that is not well defined and might not be understandable to a life science readership. It would be useful to better explain the biological basis of terms such as the control trajectory distance.

We appreciate this comment, and agree. In the revised manuscript, we now explicitly define these terms, including control trajectory, state, trajectory distance, and modal controllability (Results section):

“Specifically, we defined the trajectory of a neural system to be the temporal path that the system traverses through diverse states, where the item state was defined as the vector of neurophysiological activity across brain regions at a single time point.”

“We calculated the trajectory distance at each time point, which was defined as the Euclidean distance between the current brain state and the target brain state. A small distance suggests that the current vector of brain activity is similar to the target vector of brain activity.”

“Fifth, we evaluated whether our developmental results could be explained by modal controllability. Modal controllability reflects the extent to which all dynamic modes of a system will change in response to small changes at a single node (Gu et al., 2015). If an individual has high modal controllability, it suggests that the underlying brain structural brain network was optimized to support efficient state transitions to diverse states. In line with this intuition, modal controllability increases with development in youth as flexible switching between patterns of brain activity becomes more common (Tang et al., 2017).”

2) We suggest toning down language throughout the paper. Associations of r=0.17 can hardly be called strong, particularly given that this is cross-sectional sample. The extended discussion of electrical stimulation and neurofeedback is peripheral to the current results, given that these modalities were not investigated. I suggest keeping the discussion specific to the current results and providing some basic discussion of biological or network mechanisms that could potentially underpin the relation between control energy and age.

We agree, and are happy to incorporate this feedback. We have moderated the language throughout the paper, removing any references to “strong associations” or “high accuracy.”

Furthermore, we have removed the discussion about electrical stimulation and neurofeedback. Instead, as suggested, we have expanded our discussion regarding the network mechanisms underlying the development of control energy (Discussion section):

“Our results suggest that neither the overall modularity of the structural network nor the segregation of the fronto-parietal system, which both mature during youth (Baum et al., 2017), could explain the association between age and control energy. […] In Srivastava et al., (2020), we show that the impulse response of the system is formally related to the network communicability, suggesting that paths of all lengths – not just direct connections – contribute to the control energy.”

3) While network control theory is a novel way to capture the relation between brain network development, it would be useful to understand the basic network properties that change over age and therefore underpin the relation between age and control energy. As they currently stand, the control theory results are quite abstract and do not appear to provide insight into specific network-related or biological mechanisms that enable lower-cost transitions.

This is a useful comment, and we are happy to address each specific point below.

a) The computation of control energy is fully characterized by the network connectivity matrix. Therefore, any relation between age and control energy should be able to be traced back to relations between age and other simpler network properties of the connectivity matrix. It would be informative to evaluate whether the modular structure or connectivity strength relates to age, which could provide a simpler characterization of the control energy result that is closer to the underlying biology.

This is a good point. Before calculating the control energy, to ensure stability, the matrix was been scaled by its largest eigenvalue and the identity matrix was subtracted as in prior work (Betzel et al., 2016; Gu et al., 2017; Karrer et al., 2020; Stiso et al., 2019). After scaling, the total connectivity strength weakly increased with age (Z = 2.22, P = 0.03, Partial r = 0.07, CI = [0.01, 0.14]). In all subsequent analyses we controlled for total network strength of this scaled network, including both mass-univariate analyses at multiple levels and multivariate pattern analysis. Accordingly, the strength of the network that was used to calculate the control energy could not explain the developmental decline of control energy observed.

Beyond network strength, our previous work has demonstrated that network modularity is related to age during youth (Baum et al., 2017). Accordingly, we calculated the network modularity quality (Q) using the community structure defined by the functional atlas (Yeo et al., 2011). As prior, we found that Q significantly increased with development (Z = 5.09, P = 3.62 × 10-7, Partial r = 0.17, CI = [0.10, 0.23]) while controlling for sex, handedness, motion, total brain volume, and total network strength. To determine whether network modularity Q explained our observed results, we calculated the developmental effects of control energy while also controlling for modularity. Results were generally consistent with our main results (Figure 2—figure supplement 3B). For example, average control energy of the whole brain and of the fronto-parietal system still significantly declined with age (whole-brain: Z = -3.95, P = 7.73 × 10-5, Partial r = -0.13, CI = [-0.19, -0.07]; fronto-parietal: Z = -4.31, PFDR = 6.46 × 10-5, Partial r = -0.14, CI = [-0.20, -0.08]). These analyses suggest that network modularity does not explain the developmental decline of control energy in our results. We now include these analyses in the revised Materials and methods section and Results section.

Materials and methods section:

“Fifth, one might expect that the modular organization of the brain’s structural network could potentially change the control energy cost of brain state transitions (Avena-Koenigsberger et al., 2017). […] We controlled for Q by including it as a model covariate in sensitivity analyses, which were conducted at all resolutions (whole brain, functional systems, and network nodes).”

Results section:

“Sixth, because the modularity of brain networks evolves with age, one could ask whether that evolution impacts the observed assocations with control energy (Baum et al., 2017; Hagmann et al., 2010; Huang et al., 2015). However, we found that results remained consistent after controlling for network modularity in all analyses (Figure 2—figure supplement 3B). For example, average control energy of the whole brain and of the fronto-parietal system both significantly declined with age after controlling for network modularity (whole-brain: Z = -3.95, P = 7.73 × 10-5, Partial r = -0.13, CI = [-0.19, -0.07]; fronto-parietal: Z = -4.31, PFDR = 6.46 × 10-5, Partial r = -0.14, CI = [-0.20, -0.08]).”

These results are also highlighted in revised Figure 2—figure supplement 3B.

b) One possibility is that the FP network may show greater modularization with age, together with an overall increase in network integration. Indeed, the authors have previously suggested increased segregation of the FPN in this dataset (Baum et al. 2017). The fact that control energy is greater in the randomized null might be consistent with this notion. This kind of insight would be useful to link the control energy results and underlying network-level mechanisms.

This is a great suggestion. Above, we examined whether overall network modularity could explain our control energy results. To more specifically test the potential impact of fronto-parietal system segregation, we calculated the participation coefficient of each brain region in a manner consistent with our previous work (Baum et al., 2017), and averaged the participation coefficient of brain regions within the fronto-parietal system. Then, we evaluated the developmental changes of average control energy in the fronto-parietal system while controlling for average participation coefficient in the fronto-parietal system and other covariates. Results reveal that average control energy in the fronto-parietal system still significantly decreased with age (Z = -4.64, P = 3.51 × 10-6, Partial r = -0.15, CI = [-0.21, -0.09]), suggesting that increased segregation of the fronto-parietal system did not account for age associations with control energy. We have updated the Materials and methods section and Results section in the revised manuscript to reflect these new analyses:

Materials and methods section:

“Further, we evaluated the possibility that the segregation of the fronto-parietal system during youth (Baum et al., 2017) could explain the age effect of control energy. We calculated the average participation coefficient of the fronto-parietal system, and evaluated if developmental associations with control energy in the fronto-parietal system remained while controlling for the average participation coefficient in this system alongside with other covariates.”

Results section:

“We further assessed whether the increasing segregation of fronto-parietal system during youth (Baum et al., 2017) could explain the age effect of control energy. Results remained consistent after controlling for the average participation coefficient within the fronto-parietal system when examining age-related differences in control energy (Z = -4.64, P = 3.51 × 10-6, Partial r = -0.15, CI = [-0.21, -0.09]).”

c) A related possibility is that the results might simply be re-capitulating the fact that younger participants have weaker long range connections. First, it seems obvious that the best way to "activate" the frontoparietal regions would be to inject energy into the target (frontoparietal) regions, so it's not surprising that frontoparietal regions required the most control energy (Figure 1C). For older participants, this energy might be lower because frontoparietal regions are now more strongly connected to other regions, so the overall control energy (and frontoparietal control energy) can be reduced by "distributing" the control energy budget to other regions.

We appreciate this comment, and are happy to address this point. In order to examine connections both within the fronto-parietal system and between the fronto-parietal system and other systems, we calculated the sum of all the connections within the fronto-parietal system, as well as the sum of all the connections between the fronto-parietal system and other systems. Controlling for sex, handedness, motion, total brain volume and total network strength, we found that the within fronto-parietal system strength significantly increased (Z = 4.17, P = 3.05 × 10-5, Partial r = 0.14, CI = [0.07, 0.20]) during youth. Moreover, the total network strength between the fronto-parietal system and other systems also changed with age (Z = 3.27, P = 0.001, Partial r = 0.08, CI = [0.01, 0.14]).

Next, we assessed whether these effects could explain the observed developmental association with control energy. While controlling for within fronto-parietal system connectivity strength, the control energy in the fronto-parietal system still significantly declined with development (Z = -3.53, P = 0.0004, Partial r = -0.12, CI = [-0.18, -0.06]). Similarly, while controlling for the connectivity strength between fronto-parietal system and other systems, the control energy in the fronto-parietal system still significantly declined with development (Z = -4.88, P = 1.06 × 10-6, Partial r = -0.16, CI = [-0.22, -0.10]). These results suggest that connectivity either within the fronto-parietal system or between the fronto-parietal system and other systems does not explain age-related differences in control energy. We have added these results to the revised manuscript (Results section).

Results section:

“Seventh, we assessed whether connectivity within the fronto-parietal system or between the fronto-parietal and other systems could explain observed associations between age and control energy. […] Similarly, while controlling for the connectivity strength between the fronto-parietal system and other systems, the control energy in the fronto-parietal system still significantly declined with development (Z = -4.88, P = 1.06 × 10-6, Partial r = -0.16, CI = [-0.22, -0.10]).”

4) The authors should explicitly mention the definition of the final state and initial states in the Results section. While this is explained in the Materials and methods section, this should really be in the Results section because this is quite important.

We thank the reviewer for raising this point. We have added the definition of the initial and final states to the revised Results section.

Results section:

“Capitalizing on recent advances in network control theory, we modeled how structural networks facilitate state transitions from an initial baseline state to the target state. In the initial state, all regions had an activity magnitude of 0. In the target state, regions in the fronto-parietal system had activity magnitude of 1, with all other regions having an activity magnitude of 0.”

5) The authors justified their choice of initial and final state as follows "We set the baseline state to zero, because we sought to model the contrast in activation between an executive task and the resting state. This comparison is motivated by a long history of task fMRI experiments that explicitly contrast executive tasks to the resting state, resulting in robust activation of the fronto-parietal cortex (Cohen et al., 1997; Forsyth et al., 2014; Nagel et al., 2009; Ragland et al., 2002; Rowe et al., 2000). We set the values of regions in the fronto-parietal system to 1 to represent the fact that these regions were activated." We agree with the rationale, but based on the rationale, it seems that a better initial state should be the fMRI activity pattern averaged across all time points during a resting-fMRI scan and the final state should be the fMRI activity pattern averaged across all time points during an executive function task.

The reviewer raises a good point that the empirical BOLD signal could be used as an initial state. We fully acknowledge that these initial and target states are theoretical, and not obtained directly from imaging data. However, the choice of a simplified state allows us to isolate the energy required to activate the frontoparietal system without confounding effects of activity spread from other systems. Nonetheless, it is important that these theoretical states have biological plausibility. Therefore, as suggested, we first averaged the resting-state fMRI time series across all time points for several subjects and found the average value was close to 0 (e.g., around 7.63×10-8) for all brain regions. This indicated to us that we could retain theoretical simplicity without sacrificing too much biological plausibility. As such, we retained the baseline state as zero in all brain regions. However, in an effort to evaluate a more biologically plausible target state, we also evaluated a target state specified by the 2-back > 0-back task contrast on a fractal n-back working memory task, which reliably recruits the fronto-parietal network and distributed executive system (Ragland et al., 2002; Satterthwaite et al., 2013). As before, we calculated the control energy cost for each participant to transition from the baseline state to this new target state, and evaluated the associations of control energy and age. Consistent with our main results, the control energy cost for reaching this activation-based target state using real network data was significantly lower than that from a null network (see new Figure 2—figure supplement 4B). Similarly, control energy cost remained highest in frontoparietal system (Figure 2—figure supplement 4C). Critically, both the whole-brain control energy cost (Z = -7.59, P = 3.26 × 10-14, Partial r = -0.25, CI = [-0.30, -0.18]; Figure 2—figure supplement 4d) and control energy in the fronto-parietal system (Z = -5.26, PFDR = 2.92 × 10-7, Partial r = -0.17, CI = [-0.23, -0.11]; Figure 2—figure supplement 4E) both significantly declined with age. Nodal-level analyses provided convergent results, with control energy in fronto-parietal system nodes decreasing with development (Figure 2—figure supplement 4F). The Materials and methods section and Results section have been revised to reflect these new analyses:

Materials and methods section:

“Finally, we evaluated an alternative, biologically recorded target state that was defined using the activation pattern from a working memory task that reliably recruits the fronto-parietal network and executive system. […] Furthermore, we calculated the developmental association between control energy and age at multiple scales, including the whole brain, each cognitive system, and for each network node (with covariates as prior).”

Results section:

“Finally, in our main analyses, we specified the target state as regions within the fronto-parietal system, with each region having a magnitude of 1. […] Nodal analyses provided convergent results, revealing that the control energy in nodes within the fronto-parietal system significantly declined with age (Figure 2—figure supplement 4F).”

6) Equation 2: "S is 0-1 diagonal matrix of size N x N that selects only the nodes that we wish to control. Here, we only constrain the activity of the fronto-parietal system." – Can the authors clarify this statement? Is S the identity matrix? We thought that all nodes are targeted since the target state comprises 1 for the frontoparietal nodes and 0 for other nodes. But the authors now seem to imply they only constrain the activity of the frontoparietal nodes.

We are happy to clarify. The diagonal matrix S selects the nodes that will have a state penalty in the cost function. In our analyses, the diagonal matrix S was not an identity matrix, and only nodes in the fronto-parietal system had a magnitude of 1. This means there was a penalty associated with large state deviations only for the frontoparietal system. Our work aimed to examine how structural networks facilitate the selective activation of the fronto-parietal system. Therefore, we specifically care about the energy required to activate the fronto-parietal system without large deviations from 1 (with 1 being the target state value for frontoparietal regions). This choice also makes our results robust to other choices of state values, as evinced by the fact that both the initial and the target state values of these off-target regions did not impact the calculation of control energy. In the revised manuscript, we have clarified this point in the revised Results section:

“Third, it should be noted that we only constrained the state of regions in the fronto-parietal system. Therefore, the distance travelled by these off-target regions outside the fronto-parietal system were not included in our cost function for calculating optimal control energy. This choice also serves to ensure that our calculation of control energy is largely robust to both the initial and target states of other regions.”

While our choice of S has clear theoretical motivation, the reviewer is correct that it is a still a parameter whose impact on the results could be explored. To demonstrate the robustness of our results to our choice of S, in the revised manuscript we now include a sensitivity analysis that sets S as the identity matrix and constrains the activation of all nodes. Results showed there was a high correlation (r = 0.94, p < 2 × 10-16) between the whole-brain control energy cost when constraining the fronto-parietal system and the whole-brain control energy cost when constraining the whole brain (Figure 2—figure supplement 2D). This new result has been included in the revised manuscript:

“To demonstrate the robustness of our results to our definition of the matrix S, we calculated the control energy cost using the same initial and target states as in the main analyses but constraining the whole brain. Results showed that there is a high correlation (r = 0.94, p < 2 × 10-16) between the whole-brain control energy cost when constraining the whole brain and that when constraining the fronto-parietal system only (Figure 2—figure supplement 2D).”

7) "When model weights were examined at the level of individual network nodes, the regions that most contributed to the prediction of brain maturity aligned with univariate analyses, and included the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, the cingulate cortex, superior parietal cortex, and lateral temporal cortex" – It is well-known that model weights should not be interpreted without filtering (Haufe et al., 2014). Given the authors are using ridge regression, Haufe's approach is perfect for this situation.

We appreciate this excellent suggestion. In the revised manuscript, we used the suggested method (Haufe et al., 2014) to interpret the weights of the ridge regression model. Specifically, we multiplied the model weight vector w by the data covariance matrix ∑X (Haufe et al., 2014; Waskom and Wagner, 2017), which was formulized as a = ∑Xw. The transformed weight vector a was a distributed pattern quantifying the contribution of each brain region in multivariate ridge prediction. Results were consistent with mass-univariate analysis, which demonstrated that the brain regions contributing the most to the prediction included the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, the cingulate cortex, superior parietal cortex, and lateral temporal cortex (Figure 3B). We have described this new approach in the revised Materials and methods section:

“Interpreting the model: In a linear prediction model such as ridge regression, one weight/regression coefficient was assigned for each feature/brain region. […] The absolute value of the transformed weight represents the importance of the corresponding feature in a prediction (Haufe et al., 2014; Mourao-Miranda et al., 2005).”

We have also updated the Results section:

“We further examined model weights at the level of individual network nodes. The regions that contributed the most to the prediction of brain maturity aligned with mass-univariate analyses, and included the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, the cingulate cortex, superior parietal cortex, and lateral temporal cortex (Figure 3B).”

8) The authors need to provide more details about how they controlled for linear and nonlinear effects of age in their analysis (e.g., correlation between control energy and executive performance). The authors mentioned generalized additive models and penalized splines, but the details were nowhere close to being sufficient.

We are happy to elaborate. We used the following generalized additive model (GAM) to estimate the associations between control energy cost and executive performance.

Energy = Executive Performance + spline(Age) + Sex + Handedness + Motion + TBV + Network Strength + intercept.

This modeling approach fits penalized splines to flexibly model nonlinearities in the relationship between age and measures of interest, without being bound to specific linear or polynomial functions. The spline age term estimates a nonparametric smooth function for age-related associations with control energy, which can include linear or nonlinear effects depending on the structure of the data. Restricted maximum likelihood is used to penalize non-linearity in order to avoid over-fitting (Wood, 2004). In our data, the association between control energy and age was linear for the fronto-parietal system (Figure 2C), but we observed a non-linear association in the limbic system (Figure 2 —figure supplement 1D). Therefore, in the above model, GAMs controlled for both linear or non-linear age effects as dictated by the specific data. We have clarified these details in revised Materials and methods section:

“For the associations between control energy and executive performance, the GAM model was:

Energy = Executive Performance + spline(Age) + Sex + Handedness + Motion + TBV + Network Strength + intercept.

We used the gam command in the R package ‘mgcv’ to implement the model. The spline term estimates a nonparametric smooth function for age-related differences in control energy, which can include linear or nonlinear effects depending on the structure of the data. Restricted maximum likelihood is used to penalize non-linearity in order to prevent overfitting (Wood, 2004).”

9) The authors need to provide more details about the mediation analysis (e.g., assumptions). How do we interpret the betas in Figure 4C and D? Total effect is 0.67, while mediation effect is 0.03 and direct effect is 0.64. Doesn't this mean that the analysis is suggesting that the indirect effect is quite small relative to the direct effect? If so, this should be discussed.

We appreciate this comment, and we have added further details clarifying the mediation analysis in the revised Materials and methods section:

“Furthermore, for regions that displayed the associations between control energy and both age and cognition, we evaluated whether regional control energy might mediate the relationship between age and executive function. […] A bootstrap analysis (i.e., resampled 10,000 times) was implemented to estimate the confidence intervals for the indirect effect.”

We agree with the reviewer that the indirect effect was quite small relative to the direct effect. As suggested, in the revised manuscript, we now emphasize the small indirect effect size (Discussion section):

“Moreover, the decline of control energy of the bilateral middle cingulate cortex partially mediated the observed improvement of executive function with age. This result is consistent with prior literature suggesting that the optimization of the structural network is associated with better executive function (Baum et al., 2017; Wen et al., 2011), as the decline of control energy cost reflects the optimization of the structural brain network (Kim et al., 2018). However, it should be noted that this mediation effect was small, as the direct effect was much larger than the indirect effect.”

10) The age prediction analysis requires motivation and better integration with the rest of the paper. It is likely that simpler features such as the tractography connectivity strengths or basic network properties would also yield good predictive models of age. Therefore, the reason for using a more complex feature space is unclear, unless it can be demonstrated that control energy can outperform the accuracy of more basic features, or reveals a specific mechanism that characterizes network development.

We are happy to clarify this point. Multivariate pattern analyses complement mass-univariate approaches, as mass-univariate analyses investigates each brain region in isolation and multivariate pattern analyses are sensitive to the spatially distributed pattern of features (Davatzikos, 2004; Haynes, 2015; Haynes and Rees, 2006; Norman et al., 2006). We included a multivariate pattern analysis to explore whether the results were consistent with our mass-univariate analyses, and also to examine the total predictive power of the complex pattern of control energy. It is important to note that we do not emphasize the relative predictive power of these control energy features compared to simpler features; this analysis is intended to provide an integrated view of the high-dimensional data. Results were consistent with the mass-univariate analysis, which underscores the robustness of these findings to the methodological approach. In the revised manuscript, we have clarified the motivation for the multivariate pattern analysis (Results section):

“Having established that the control energy required to reach the fronto-parietal activation state changes with age on a regional and system-level basis using mass-univariate analysis, we next evaluated the developmental changes of control energy using multivariate pattern analysis. […] Specifically, we applied ridge regression with nested two-fold cross validation (2F-CV, see Figure 3—figure supplement 1) to identify an individual participant’s age in an unbiased fashion using the multivariate pattern of regional-level control energy.

11) The relation identified between control energy and age appears to be linear based on Figure 2 and thus the reasons for fitting penalized splines to characterize nonlinear associations could be better described. Are the lines shown in Figure 2 representative of the splines?

We thank the reviewer for their comment. Because prior studies demonstrated that the developmental changes of brain structure and functions could be either linear (Hagmann et al., 2010; Wierenga et al., 2016) or non-linear (Grayson and Fair, 2017; Mills et al., 2016; Vandekar et al., 2015), we used generalized additive models (GAMs) to fit penalized splines, which characterized both linear and nonlinear associations between age and control energy cost. The GAM formula used to model the effect of development on energy was:

Energy = spline(Age) + Sex + Handedness + Motion + TBV + Network Strength + intercept.

We used the gam command in the R package ‘mgcv’ to implement the model. Penalized splines allow the model to capture both linear and non-linear relationships with age, while penalizing overfitting using relative maximum likelihood. For example, while the plot shown in Figure 2A suggested the relationship between whole-brain average control energy and age was linear. However, associations between age and average control energy in limbic system were non-linear (see new Figure 2—figure supplement 1D).

In revised manuscript, we now elaborate regarding the motivation for using penalized splines (Results section):

“Prior studies have demonstrated that the developmental changes of both brain structure and function could be either linear (Hagmann et al., 2010; Wierenga et al., 2016) or non-linear (Grayson and Fair, 2017; Mills et al., 2016; Vandekar et al., 2015). Therefore, we used generalized additive models (GAM) with penalized splines, which allowed us to rigorously characterize both linear and nonlinear effects while avoiding over-fitting.”

We also added the details about the implementation of GAM in the revised Materials and methods section.

“For developmental effect of control energy, the GAM model was:

Energy = spline(Age) + Sex + Handedness + Motion + TBV + Network Strength + intercept.

For the associations between control energy and executive performance, the GAM model was:

Energy = Executive Performance + spline(Age) + Sex + Handedness + Motion + TBV + Network Strength + intercept.

We used the gam command in the R package ‘mgcv’ to implement the model. The spline term estimates a nonparametric smooth function for age-related differences in control energy, which can include linear or nonlinear effects depending on the structure of the data. Restricted maximum likelihood is used to penalize non-linearity in order to prevent overfitting (Wood, 2004).”

The claims regarding the specificity of the FP network require clarification. The null model corresponding to the rewired graph does not appear to have been evaluated for the other canonical networks. To establish specificity of the FP, the null model should also be considered for the other networks. Based on this global null model alone, we would suggest that it is hard to claim that the brain is explicitly wired to optimize transition cost to the FP activation state. Wiring organization is also likely to contribute to other cognitive functions as well.

This is a valuable comment. To address the reviewer’s point, we also evaluated the null model for a motor target state. Results indicated that the mean whole brain energetic cost of the null networks was also significantly higher (p < 2 × 2-16) than that of the empirical networks (see new Figure 2—figure supplement 2E left). As the reviewer suggested, this suggests that the lower energetic cost of the fronto-parietal system compared to null networks was not unique but was present in other systems as well. We have added this result in the revised manuscript.

Materials and methods section:

“Third, we assessed whether the structural network also contributed to other cognitive functions as well by comparing the control energy cost required to reach a motor activation state for real networks and null networks.”

Results section:

“Fourth, we assessed whether the structural network optimized the transition to an a priori motor system activation target (Figure 1—figure supplement 2) (Yeo et al., 2011). Results indicated that the mean whole brain energetic cost of the null networks was significantly higher (p < 2 × 10-16) than that of the empirical networks (Figure 2—figure supplement 2E left), suggesting that the lower energetic cost of the fronto-parietal system was not unique, but was present in other systems as well.”

Based on these results, we have removed statements regarding specificity of the fronto-parietal network in the Materials and methods section, Results section and Discussion section.

However, it should be noted that when developmental associations with control energy cost were evaluated for the motor system, we found that the whole-brain control energy required to transition to the motor system activation did not significantly change over the age range studied (Z = 1.48, P = 0.14, Partial r = 0.05, CI = [-0.02 0.11]; Figure 2—figure supplement 2E right). This result is consistent with extensive literature (Andersen, 2003; Gogtay et al., 2004) demonstrating more protracted neurodevelopment of executive versus motor systems.

12) In addition, it would be interesting to know whether the node-level measures are related to degree and/or strength. The authors use the two statistics as covariates in the predictive model, but do not show whether they are related to control energy directly.

This is a good point. As suggested, we tested the association between the total network strength and whole-brain control energy, while controlling for a spline of age, sex, handedness, motion, and total brain volume. Results suggested the association between total network strength and control energy was not significant (Z = 0.79, P = 0.43, Partial r = 0.03, CI = [-0.04, 0.09]). It should be noted that we have scaled the network matrix and we have controlled for the total network strength of this scaled network. It should be noted that the energy required for a state transition depends upon a system’s response to an energetic perturbation. The simplest such perturbation engenders an impulse response (Karrer et al., 2020). In Srivastava et al., (2020), we show that the impulse response of the system is formally related to the network communicability, suggesting that paths of all lengths – not just direct connections as quantified in degree or strength – contribute to control energy.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, Loughead J, Prabhakaran K, Calkins ME, Hopson R, Jackson C, Keefe J, Riley M, Mentch FD, Sleiman P, Verma R, Davatzikos C, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE. 2014. Neuroimaging of the Philadelphia neurodevelopmental cohort. NCBI. phs000607.v3.p2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    The PNC data is publicly available in the Database of Genotypes and Phenotypes: accession number: phs000607.v3.p2; https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/projects/gap/cgi-bin/study.cgi?study_id=phs000607.v3.p2. All analysis code is available here: https://github.com/ZaixuCui/pncControlEnergy (copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/pncControlEnergy), with detailed explanation in https://github.com/ZaixuCui/pncControlEnergy/wiki.

    The following previously published dataset was used:

    Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, Loughead J, Prabhakaran K, Calkins ME, Hopson R, Jackson C, Keefe J, Riley M, Mentch FD, Sleiman P, Verma R, Davatzikos C, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE. 2014. Neuroimaging of the Philadelphia neurodevelopmental cohort. NCBI. phs000607.v3.p2


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES