Abstract
Introduction
We examined the effect of visual optimizations on warning text recall.
Methods
We used Amazon’s Mechanical Turk to recruit 1854 young adult (18–34 years) electronic cigarette (e-cigarette) users or susceptible nonusers. We conducted a between-subjects 3 × 2 × 2 experiment to examine the influence of color (black text on white background [BW] vs. black on yellow [BY] vs. yellow on black [YB]), shape (rectangle vs. novel), and signal word (presence vs. absence of the word “warning”). We randomized participants to view one of 12 warnings on a fictional e-cigarette advertisement. We coded open-ended recall responses into three categories: (1) recalled nothing, (2) recalled something, (3) recalled the concept. We examined main effects on warning text recall using multinomial regression. We examined differences in attention, perceived message effectiveness, and appeal.
Results
Those exposed to BW or BY warnings were more likely than those exposed to YB to recall something (AOR = 1.6, AOR = 1.5, respectively) or the concept (OR = 1.4, BW). Those exposed to novel shape (44.7% novel vs. 37.9% rectangle; p = .003) or color (44.5% BY vs. 41.9% YB vs. 37.5% BW; p = .04) warnings were more likely to report attention to the warning. In aided recall, those exposed to the signal word were more likely than those not exposed to select the correct response (64.0% vs. 31.3%; p < .0001). We did not find differences for message effectiveness or appeal.
Conclusions
Visual optimizations such as color may influence warning text recall and should be considered for new warnings. Research should continue exploring variations for advertisement warnings to maximize attention to warning text.
Implications
This study examines the impact of visual optimizations on recall of the US Food and Drug Administration-mandated e-cigarette advertisement warning text. We found that color might influence warning text recall, but we did not find effects for shape or signal word. It is possible the newly mandated e-cigarette advertisement warnings, which are required to occupy at least 20% of the advertisement, are currently novel enough to attract attention. Future research should examine optimizations following implementation of the new advertisement warnings.
Introduction
The US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) educates the public about the risk of tobacco products through warnings on tobacco packaging and advertisements.1 Warnings have been on cigarette advertisements since the 1970s, and have remained visually similar. With the standard Surgeon General’s warning design in place for decades, individuals attune to the familiar rectangle shape, with black text on a white background (BW), and ignore the message content, directing attention toward the more visually appealing portion of the advertisement.2,3
As of August 2018, electronic cigarette (e-cigarette) advertisements are required to include a warning.4 The FDA-mandated warning label for e-cigarette advertisements is a rectangle with either a white background with black text and border or a black background with white text and border, placed on the upper 20% of the advertisement, with the following text: “WARNING: This product contains nicotine. Nicotine is an addictive chemical.” The mandated warning is substantially larger than previous cigarette advertisement warnings and may be more effective at attracting attention.
However, e-cigarette companies have already begun to distract consumers from the FDA-mandated warning for e-cigarettes. Blu, a popular e-cigarette brand, released an advertisement campaign in 2017, which included a large rectangle at the top of the advertisement that contained text such as “IMPORTANT: Contains flavor” and “IMPORTANT: No ashtrays needed.”5 It is possible e-cigarette consumers will overlook the FDA-mandated warnings because they are conditioned to think it is part of the advertisement.
Changing the design of advertisement warnings may increase their effectiveness. Studies of cigarettes and smokeless tobacco consistently show that novel color, placement, and size, increase attention to and recall of advertisement warnings.2,3,6,7 Color has been shown to increase warning readability and risk perceptions.8,9 Visual communication research has shown that yellow increases harm perceptions and is more effective at attracting attention compared to other colors and black and white.8–10 In addition, tobacco industry research suggests yellow is most effective for capturing consumer attention and signaling danger.11 However, it is unclear whether color must be in the background to attract attention, or whether it can be within the text. Thus, it is important to test both variations. To date, only one e-cigarette advertisement experiment has tested color, finding that changing the background color to red increased warning attention among young adults.12
The format or shape of the warning may also influence its impact. Visual design theories and warnings research suggest several shapes are potentially more effective than the mandated rectangle for increasing attention and warning recall.13,14 For example, Riley et al.14, tested 19 shapes for warnings and identified an inverted triangle as most preferable, whereas Pieters et al.13, found visually complex features (eg, sharp angles and asymmetrical design) increased attention.
Furthermore, text-based cues within the warning may influence consumers’ attention to and interpretation of the warning. Warnings research not specific to tobacco identified differences in meaning conveyed with various signal words (ie, the word that precedes warning text) such as “caution,” “warning,” or “danger.” Some research suggests that the word “warning” conveys serious injury whereas “caution” implies less severe injuries.15 Some suggest a signal word is necessary for clarity and understanding, whereas others posit a signal word encourages individuals to overlook the remaining text.2,10,15,16 However, no research has addressed the impact of eliminating the signal word.
We sought to determine whether visual optimizations would increase warning recall. Recall is an important precursor to emotional and cognitive reactions, and changes in knowledge, beliefs, intentions, and behavior.17 Our primary aim was to determine whether variations in color (mandated black text on white background vs. black text on yellow background [BY] vs. yellow text on black background [YB]), shape (mandated rectangle vs. novel shape), or signal word (present vs. absent) influence open-ended recall. Our secondary aims were to examine whether optimizations influenced aided recall, attention, warning appeal, advertisement appeal, and product appeal. We hypothesized those who viewed optimized warnings (BY, YB, novel shape, or absent signal word) would have greater recall, greater attention, greater warning appeal, reduced advertisement appeal, and reduced product appeal than those within the FDA-mandated conditions. We focused exclusively on young adults, who are increasingly likely to use e-cigarettes and report high exposure to e-cigarette advertisements.18 Further, receptivity to e-cigarette advertising in young adults is associated with later cigarette smoking,18 and evidence indicates e-cigarette advertisements have targeted this age group.19 Thus, this is an important population for understanding the implications.
Methods
Sample
Between April and May 2018, we used Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) to recruit young adults (18–34 years) who reported ever using e-cigarettes or were susceptible to e-cigarette use (defined in the measures section). MTurk (www.mturk.com) is an online marketplace commonly used for data collection in social science and tobacco control research.20,21 MTurk samples yield high-quality data,21,22 are demographically more diverse than typical college populations often used to obtain young adult samples,21 and produce experimental results similar to those of nationally representative studies.20,23 After reviewing a description of the study, interested MTurk workers were directed to Qualtrics to complete informed consent and a screener survey. Interested MTurk workers were screened to ensure they (1) were between ages 18 and 34 years, and (2) either used e-cigarettes or were susceptible to using e-cigarettes. To increase data quality, we restricted participation to MTurk workers with high approval ratings (ie, >85%), and included attention checks.22 Participants received approximately $1.00 via MTurk for completing the survey.
Procedure
We conducted a between-subjects 3 × 2 × 2 factorial experiment to examine the influence of color (mandated BW vs. BW vs. YB), shape (mandated rectangle vs. novel shape), and signal word (presence vs. absence of the word “warning”) on warning text recall.
Preexperiment
To create warnings for the main experiment, in February 2018, we conducted a pretest to select the novel shape and color. To test seven warning shapes and three shades of yellow, we surveyed 285 young adults who reported ever using e-cigarettes or susceptibility to e-cigarette use via MTurk. We randomized participants to view one of seven warning shapes, developed using visual communication theory principles, on our fictional e-cigarette advertisement. We did not find significant differences for warning text recall across the seven shapes. At the end of the survey, we presented all seven warnings to participants and asked which best attracted their attention, a potential predictor of recall (Supplementary Material).6,17 The warning with a triangle and exclamation point was selected as the most attention getting (44%). To determine the shade of yellow, we exposed participants to three shades of yellow (true yellow, dark yellow, and light yellow), for both BY and YB and asked about readability and attention for each. True yellow (cyan: 0, magenta: 0, yellow: 100, black: 0) was selected in both BY and YB as most attention getting (77%) and easiest to read (55%; Supplementary Material). Those who completed the pretest were not eligible to complete the full survey.
Main Experiment
All warnings in the main experiment were shown on a fictional e-cigarette brand advertisement created by our team to mimic existing e-cigarette brands and advertisements (Supplementary Material). We used the same advertisement across all experimental conditions. To reduce the likelihood participants would become aware the study was on e-cigarettes, we created two fictional decoy advertisements (ie, soft drink and cough syrup), each of which contained a warning visually similar to the FDA-mandated warning. We randomized participants to one of 12 warning conditions for our e-cigarette advertisement, each containing the FDA-mandated nicotine warning text (Figure 1). Participants were shown this advertisement in a random order with the two decoy advertisements. After viewing each of the three advertisements, participants responded to items about the advertisement (ad appeal, product appeal, likelihood of purchase). After viewing all three advertisements, we displayed the e-cigarette advertisement again, with the warning text covered, and measured open-ended recall. We then showed the e-cigarette advertisement again with the warning exposed, and asked participants to respond to items about the e-cigarette advertisement warning (perceived message effectiveness, brand trustworthiness). Finally, we asked participants about their attention to the e-cigarette advertisement.
Measures
Recall
Our primary outcome was warning text recall. Participants were shown the advertisement with the warning masked in red and asked to respond to the following open-ended item: “The red area at the top of this advertisement contained a text warning. Please type the text you remember, as accurately as possible, in the space below.”
Following open-ended recall, we asked participants to select from a list which warning text they saw on the e-cigarette advertisement (aided recall). Response options were Warning: This product contains nicotine. Nicotine is an addictive chemical (correct for those in the “signal word” conditions); This product contains nicotine. Nicotine is an addictive chemical. (correct for those in the “no signal word” conditions); E-cigarette use while pregnant can harm you and your baby (incorrect); Warning: E-cigarettes contain nicotine, an addictive chemical (incorrect); and None of the above (incorrect). For analyses, we dichotomized responses into correct or incorrect.
Attention
We asked participants to select the area on the advertisement that best attracted their attention. Using the hot spot feature in Qualtrics, we identified, a priori, seven locations on the advertisement (warning, woman holding device, vapor imagery, ad slogan, brand logo, e-cigarette device, or elsewhere). Participants selected an area by clicking on the advertisement displayed on the screen. For analyses, we dichotomized responses into selecting the warning (yes) or not (no).
Warning Perceptions
We asked participants items specific to the advertisement warning, including perceived message effectiveness24 (makes e-cigarette or vaping use seem unpleasant to me; makes me concerned about the health effects of e-cigarette use or vaping; discourages me from wanting to use e-cigarettes or other vaping devices) and whether the warning increased the trustworthiness of the ad.25 Each item included 5-point Likert response options ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5).
Advertisement and Product Appeal
To ensure warnings did not have unintended effects, we assessed reactions to the advertisement with items for advertisement appeal (How appealing is this advertisement to you?), product appeal (How appealing is this product to you?), and likelihood of purchasing the product (How likely are you to buy this product?).26 Each item included 5-point Likert response options.
E-Cigarette Use
We assessed e-cigarette use as part of the screener for participant inclusion. Susceptible users were defined as those who answered “definitely yes,” “probably yes,” or “probably no” to any of the following five items shown to predict cigarette smoking experimentation: (1) Do you think that you will use e-cigarettes or other vaping devices soon?; (2) Do you think that in the future you might experiment with e-cigarettes or other vaping devices?; (3) At any time during the next year do you think you will use e-cigarettes or other vaping devices?; (4) If your best friend were to offer you an e-cigarette or other vaping device, would you use it?; or (5) Have you ever been curious about using e-cigarettes or other vaping devices?27,28 Ever e-cigarette users reported ever trying e-cigarettes or other vaping devices, even one or two puffs, but did not report past 30-day use. Current users reported past 30-day use of e-cigarettes or other vaping devices.
Demographic Variables
We measured age (continuous variable), sex (male or female), race (white alone, black alone, or other), ethnicity (Hispanic or not Hispanic), income (<$50 000 or ≥$50 000), and sexual orientation (heterosexual, LGB+) as potential covariates.
Analyses
Descriptive statistics for demographic and design factors are presented. Multinomial logistic regression models were used to examine the main effects of color, shape, and signal word on a three-category warning text recall response detailed below. We first examined effects of advertisement display order, which were not statistically significant. We then fit models adjusted for age, gender, race, sexual orientation, income, and e-cigarette use status. Next, we tested interactions between the three design factors. Finally, we examined differences in perceived message effectiveness and attention by color, shape, and marker word using analysis of variance and pairwise z-tests of least square means. For aided recall, we performed chi-square tests of independence.
Coding
On the basis of previous research on cigarette advertisement warnings,29,30 we coded responses to the open-ended warning recall item using the following three categories: (1) recalled nothing correct from the warning, (2) recalled something from the warning (eg, this contains nicotine), (3) recalled the warning concept (eg, linked the product to nicotine and addiction) or the exact text. Participants’ responses that included partial warning information (eg, the words nicotine and/or addiction), but not the complete warning concept (eg, product has nicotine which is addictive), were coded as 2. Responses coded as 2 included both factually correct (eg, tobacco is addictive) and incorrect (eg, nicotine causes cancer) statements. Responses that contained the warning text verbatim, with grammatical differences, or with the full concept were coded as 3. Thus, a higher score indicates greater recall. Two coders independently coded all responses (κ = 0.87), with all discrepancies decided upon by a third coder or resolved by the full study team.
Results
Sample Characteristics
Between April and May 2018, 5485 participants completed the screener survey. Of those, 2080 (37.9%) were eligible to complete the full survey. After deleting surveys with duplicate MTurk IDs (n = 19, 0.9%), or incorrect attention checks (n = 206, 9.9%), our final sample was 1854. Participants were 52.7% male, 76.0% white, 14.1% Hispanic, 81.9% heterosexual, and 54.4% with household income greater than $50 000 (Table 1). Most (74.8%) reported ever using e-cigarettes. Of those, 26.0% reported past 30-day e-cigarette use, the majority of whom reported daily or almost daily (46.9%) or weekly (33.0%) use. Approximately 40% of the sample reported other tobacco use, which is common among e-cigarette users.31 In sensitivity analyses, recall, condition, and outcomes did not differ among those who used other tobacco products compared to those who did not use other products. Therefore, this was not included in additional analyses.
Table 1.
Sample characteristics (N = 1854) | |
---|---|
Age (n = 1854) | 27.7 (4.1) |
Sex (n = 1852) | |
Male | 976 (52.7%) |
Female | 876 (47.3%) |
Race (n = 1845) | |
White | 1403 (76.0%) |
Black | 189 (10.2%) |
Other | 253 (13.7%) |
Ethnicity (n = 1842) | |
Not Hispanic | 1583 (85.9%) |
Hispanic | 259 (14.1%) |
Income (n = 1839) | |
<$50 000 | 1000 (54.4%) |
≥$50 000 | 839 (45.6%) |
Sexual orientation (n = 1839) | |
Heterosexual | 1507 (81.9%) |
LGB+ | 332 (18.1%) |
E-cigarette user status (n = 1854) | |
Susceptible never user | 467 (25.2%) |
Ever user | 905 (48.8%) |
Current user | 482 (26.0%) |
Recall
Most respondents recalled the concept (43.2%; coded as 3) or recalled something (37.5%; coded as 2) from the warning text, though 19.3% recalled nothing correct (coded as 1; Table 2). We found main effects for color but not shape or signal word (Table 3). Participants exposed to BW or BY were more likely than those exposed to YB to recall something (coded as 2) from the warning (AOR = 1.6, 95% CI = 1.2 to 2.2; AOR = 1.5, 95% CI =1.1 to 2.1). In addition, those exposed to BW were more likely than those exposed to YB to recall the correct concept compared to recalling nothing (OR = 1.4, 95% CI = 1.0 to 1.9); this finding was no longer significant in adjusted models. We also found an interaction effect for signal word on color. Specifically, participants exposed to BW or BY were more likely than those exposed to YB to recall something (coded as 2) from the warning when the signal word was present (AOR = 2.5, 95% CI = 1.6 to 3.9; AOR = 2.2, 95% CI = 1.4 to 3.4), but not when it was absent (AOR = 1.0, 95% CI = 0.6 to 1.6 and AOR = 1.0, 95% CI = 0.6 to 1.6).
Table 2.
Recall score 1 n (%) or mean (SD) |
Recall score 2 n (%) or mean (SD) |
Recall score 3 n (%) or mean (SD) |
|
---|---|---|---|
Full sample | 358 (19.3%) | 695 (37.5%) | 801 (43.2%) |
Color | |||
Black on white | 102 (16.6%) | 238 (38.7%) | 275 (44.7%) |
Black on yellow | 117 (18.8%) | 254 (40.8%) | 252 (40.4%) |
Yellow on black | 139 (22.6%) | 203 (33.0%) | 274 (44.5%) |
Shape | |||
Rectangle | 170 (18.4%) | 351 (38.0%) | 402 (43.6%) |
Novel | 188 (20.2%) | 344 (36.9%) | 399 (42.9%) |
Signal word | |||
Present | 184 (19.7%) | 344 (36.9%) | 404 (43.3%) |
Absent | 174 (18.9%) | 351 (38.1%) | 397 (43.1%) |
Age | 27.9 (4.0) | 27.7 (4.1) | 27.6 (4.1) |
Sex | |||
Male | 197 (20.2%) | 360 (36.9%) | 419 (42.9%) |
Female | 161 (18.4%) | 333 (38.0%) | 382 (43.6%) |
Race | |||
White | 253 (18.0%) | 527 (37.6%) | 623 (44.4%) |
Black | 55 (29.1%) | 69 (36.5%) | 65 (34.4%) |
Other | 47 (18.6%) | 95 (37.4%) | 111 (43.9%) |
Ethnicity | |||
Not Hispanic | 291 (18.4%) | 601 (38.0%) | 691 (43.7%) |
Hispanic | 66 (25.5%) | 86 (33.2%) | 107 (41.3%) |
Income | |||
<$50,000 | 184 (18.4%) | 365 (36.5%) | 451 (45.1%) |
≥$50,000 | 171 (20.4%) | 326 (38.9%) | 342 (40.8%) |
Sexual orientation | |||
Heterosexual | 295 (19.6%) | 572 (38.0%) | 640 (42.5%) |
LGB+ | 61 (18.4%) | 114 (34.3%) | 157 (47.3%) |
E-cigarette user status | |||
Susceptible never user | 97 (20.8%) | 145 (31.0%) | 225 (48.2%) |
Ever user | 163 (18.0%) | 360 (39.8%) | 382 (42.2%) |
Current user | 98 (20.3%) | 190 (39.4%) | 194 (40.2%) |
Recall score 1 = recalled nothing correct from the warning; recall score 2 = recalled something from the warning; recall score 3 = recalled the warning concept.
Table 3.
Unadjusted odds for recall score 2 vs. 1 OR (95% CI) |
Adjusted odds for recall score 2 vs. 1 AOR (95% CI) |
Unadjusted odds for recall score 3 vs. 1 OR (95% CI) |
Adjusted odds for recall score 3 vs. 1 AOR (95% CI) |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
Color | ||||
BW vs. BYa | 1.07 (0.78 to 1.48) | 1.07 (0.78 to 1.48) | 1.25 (0.91 to 1.72) | 1.26 (0.92 to 1.74) |
BW vs. YBa | 1.59 (1.16 to 2.19) | 1.61 (1.17 to 2.23) | 1.36 (1.01 to 1.85) | 1.35 (0.99 to 1.84) |
BY vs. YBa | 1.48 (1.09 to 2.02) | 1.50 (1.10 to 2.06) | 1.09 (0.81 to 1.47) | 1.07 (0.79 to 1.45) |
Shape | ||||
Rectangle vs. novela | 1.12 (0.87 to 1.45) | 1.14 (0.87 to 1.47) | 1.11 (0.86 to 1.43) | 1.15 (0.89 to 1.49) |
Signal word | ||||
Present vs. absenta | 0.93 (0.72 to 1.20) | 0.90 (0.69 to 1.17) | 0.96 (0.75 to 1.24) | 0.94 (0.73 to 1.21) |
Bold indicates p < .05. Adjusted analyses control for age, sex, race, income, sexual orientation, and e-cigarette user status. AOR = adjusted odds ratio; BW = black text on white background; BY = black text on yellow background; CI = confidence interval; OR = odds ratio; YB = yellow text on black background.
aReference group.
While coding the open-ended recall responses, we identified several unanticipated responses. Several (n = 312, 16.8%) participants identified a health effect not specified within the warning text within their response, including 146 (7.9%) who used the word “cancer” (eg, nicotine causes cancer); 147 participants (7.9%) who replaced “this product” with other tobacco-related terminology, including “e-cigarettes” or “smoking” or “vaping”; and 32 (1.7%) who included the word “additive.” These unanticipated responses did not vary by color, shape, or signal word.
For aided recall, those exposed to rectangle warnings were more likely than those exposed to novel shape warnings to correctly identify the warning text (50.5% vs. 45.0%; p = .017). Those exposed to warnings with the signal word were more likely than those exposed to warnings without the signal word to correctly identify the warning text (64.0% vs. 31.3%; p < .0001). Notably, 77.2% of those in the no signal word condition incorrectly selected the response option: “WARNING: This product contains nicotine. Nicotine is an addictive chemical.” We did not find differences in aided recall by color conditions (48.9% BW vs. 48.3% BY vs. 45.9% YB; p = .54).
Attention
When asked to select the area of the advertisement which most attracted their attention, those exposed to BY warnings were more likely than those exposed to other warning color conditions to select the warning as attracting their attention (44.5% of BY vs. 41.9% of YB vs. 37.5% of BW; p = .04). In addition, those exposed to novel shape warnings were more likely than those exposed to rectangle warnings to select the warning as attracting attention (44.7% novel vs. 37.9% rectangle; p = .003). We did not find differences in reported attention to the warning between those exposed to the signal word and those not exposed to the signal word (40.5% vs. 42.2%, p = .43).
Warning and Advertisement Perceptions
The warning styles did not impact perceived message effectiveness of the warning. Overall, the warnings were all perceived as moderately effective (M = 3.5, SD = 1.3). Across variations in color, shape, and signal word, we did not find significant differences in ad appeal (M = 2.9, SD = 1.3), product appeal (M = 2.8, SD = 1.3), likelihood of purchasing the product (M = 2.3, SD = 1.3), or brand trustworthiness (M = 3.3, SD = 1.2).
Discussion
E-cigarette marketing targets and reaches millions of young adults annually.32,33 Exposure to e-cigarette advertising influences perceptions and increases the likelihood for experimentation and use.18,34 FDA-mandated warnings on e-cigarette advertisement may help educate young adults about the risks of e-cigarette use, however, the warnings must be attended to and remembered to have an impact. We modified the FDA-mandated e-cigarette advertisement warning in three areas: color, shape, and signal word. Color of the warning significantly influenced recall of the warning text. Specifically, among those exposed to the signal word, those exposed to the black text on white or yellow backgrounds had greater warning text recall than those exposed to the yellow on black warnings. Although our study suggests warning color plays a role in recall, we could not tease out the unique influence of having a signal word before the warning.
More than 40% of participants correctly recalled the concept of the FDA-mandated warning text—the product has nicotine, and nicotine is addictive—when shown on an e-cigarette advertisement designed to appeal to young adults. An additional one-third of participants were able to recall at least some of the warning. These findings indicate that most participants were attending to, reading, and perhaps understanding the warning, regardless of the warning format. Warning recall rates in this study were slightly higher than those previously reported for cigarette advertisements,30,35 possibly because of the novelty of the warning and its context. The warnings in this study were shown at the newly mandated larger size (20%) and placement (at the top), which may have contributed to higher recall.36
Although many were able to recall the warning wholly or in part, we had several introductions of new information within our open-ended recall. Some respondents generalized the text “this product” to other tobacco or vape products, some generalized the warning to other health effects such as cancer, and some included the word additive either in addition to or instead of the word addictive. Extending or generalizing the warning text to other tobacco products could mean that users view a warning for one tobacco product and make assumptions that it applies to other tobacco products, unable to distinguish between harms for one tobacco product versus another. This suggests it may be important to examine warnings that use specific terms such as “e-cigarettes” rather than “this product” to reduce generalizing across tobacco products.
In addition, more than 16% of participants included health effects not listed within the warning text (eg, cancer, pregnancy complications) when openly recalling the advertisement warning. Past research has identified misperceptions surrounding nicotine (eg, it is the nicotine in cigarettes that cause cancer).37–40 It is possible participants heuristically recalled previously held misperceptions when trying to remember the warning; these responses were perhaps the most mentally accessible harms.41,42 Following this same logic, participants may have transferred to other health claims because of their familiarity with other warnings (eg, cigarette warnings about pregnancy or general health harm). An alternate explanation for the not-listed, but recalled, health effects is that they were the result of negative health “halos”—when consumers transfer salient claims to general holistic impressions or beliefs about unrelated attributes or effects (eg, claim of addiction translated into cancer risk).43–45
Moreover, there were a number of instances of participants using “additive” when openly recalling the warning. We are not aware of other literature that mentions additive instead of or in addition to addictive (eg, addictive additive) when participants recall tobacco warnings or anti-tobacco advertising. If even a small portion of users and susceptible nonusers misinterpret this warning to mean “additive,” instead of nicotine being addictive, this has serious implications, as this is the only FDA-mandated warning for multiple tobacco products. Future research should explore whether this is an isolated finding or whether these were simply misspellings.
We found some differences among those exposed to the signal word compared to those not exposed to the signal word for aided recall. Regardless of whether the signal word was present, participants selected the text with the word “warning.” This may suggest that the word “warning” is implied when attending to the warning text, even if it is not present. More research is necessary to distill this finding, including testing other signal words such as “caution” or “danger”; however, the implications for removing the signal word on tobacco warnings may lead to additional space for important warning content.
Advertisement attention varied in ways we expected: reported attention to the warning was more common among those exposed to BY warnings than those exposed to BW warnings, and among those exposed to novel shape warnings—with the addition of a commonly used signal icon to indicated danger (triangle with exclamation)—compared to rectangle warnings without the icon. Following hierarchy of effects models, increasing attention is the first step for influencing perceptions and behavior necessary for increasing public health.46,47 Thus, changing the background color of warnings shows promise as a technique to increase the likelihood the public will attend to important risk information, though it might not be the specific color, rather the presence of color that garners attention through implied risk.9 Specific to e-cigarette advertisement warnings, we found yellow to increase attention; Mays et al.6, found a similar increase in attention using a red background. Although a promising minority of participants (>37%) within our sample reported the warning attracted their attention, most devoted their focus to other areas of the advertisement (eg, female, vapor). However, even a slight increase in attention (a second or two) can, over time, have a large impact on consumer perceptions and behavior.48,49
We did not find significant differences for perceived message effectiveness for the warning. In general, warnings were perceived as moderately effective. In addition, ad appeal, product appeal, likelihood of purchasing, and brand trustworthiness did not vary by warning conditions. These initial findings may indicate warning variations do not affect brand or advertisement appeal, which could potentially limit legal arguments from the tobacco industry that the warnings are infringing on brand communication.
This study is not without limitations. Warning recall measured after a single exposure does not replicate the “real world,” where young adults would be repeatedly exposed to this same warning on a variety of advertisements and marketing materials. In addition, although novel insights were uncovered in the open-ended recall responses, it is important to note these unanticipated responses may not be actual effects (eg, halo effects), and instead a result of not recalling the warning text and simply writing something to complete the survey item. Finally, we did not examine participants’ perceptions regarding nicotine. Future studies may explore how nicotine perceptions influence recall and attitudes.
Future research may consider testing warnings on additional advertisements. Our study had one e-cigarette advertisement; it is possible the warning optimizations would have different results on a different advertisement background. Future research may also consider examining recall after the mandated warnings are in place; although blu e-cigarettes and smokeless tobacco products have had larger sized warnings or statements on advertisements for an extended period of time, overall, the larger warning size is still relatively novel. It is possible the optimizations tested here would increase effectiveness as consumers become more accustomed to larger warnings on advertisements. Finally, using more objective measures for attention, such as eye tracking, may reveal different patterns of results with important implications for exposure and downstream effects of the warnings. Given the single exposure, future studies should investigate whether repeated exposure results in greater attention and higher recall. Notwithstanding, these findings are encouraging as potential ways to optimize warnings for their public health impact.
Funding
Research reported in this publication was supported by grant number P50-CA180907 from the National Cancer Institute and the US Food and Drug Administration Center for Tobacco Products. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health or the FDA.
Declaration of Interests
There are no conflicts of interest to report for this study.
Supplementary Material
References
- 1. Family Smoking Prevention and Tobacco Control Act; 2009.https://www.fda.gov/tobacco-products/rules-regulations-and-guidance/family-smoking-prevention-and-tobacco-control-act-table-contents. Accessed August 18, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Popper ET, Murray KB. Communication effectiveness and format effects on in-ad disclosure of health warnings. J Public Policy Mark. 1989;8(1):109–123. [Google Scholar]
- 3. Fischer PM, Krugman DM, Fletcher JE, Fox RJ, Rojas TH. An evaluation of health warnings in cigarette advertisements using standard market research methods: what does it mean to warn? Tob Control. 1993;2(4):279–285. [Google Scholar]
- 4. Food and Drug Administration, HHS. Deeming tobacco products to be subject to the federal food, drug, and cosmetic act, as amended by the family smoking prevention and tobacco control act; restrictions on the sale and distribution of tobacco products and required warning statements for tobacco products final rule. Fed Regist. 2016;81(90):28973–29106. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Rutgers School of Public Health. Trinkets & Trash: Artifacts of the Tobacco Epidemic https://www.trinketsandtrash.org/detail.php?artifactid=12286&page=1. Accessed August 13, 2018.
- 6. Mays D, Villanti AC, Niaura RS, Lindblom EN, Strasser AA. The effects of varying electronic cigarette warning label design features on attention, recall, and product perceptions among young adults. Health Commun. 2019;34(3):317–324. doi:10.1080/10410236.2017.1372050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Truitt L, Hamilton WL, Johnston PR, et al. . Recall of health warnings in smokeless tobacco ads. Tob Control. 2002;11 (suppl 2):ii59–ii63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Kline PB, Braun CC, Peterson N, Silver NC. The impact of color on warnings research. Proc Hum Factors Ergon Soc Annu Meet. 1993;37(14):940–944. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Braun CC, Mine PB, Clayton Silver N. The influence of color on warning label perceptions. Int J Ind Ergon. 1995;15(3):179–187. [Google Scholar]
- 10. Roditis ML, Halpern-Felsher BL, Lempert LK, Glantz SA, Popova L, Cataldo JK.. FDA’s Proposed Warning Statements are Weak and Ineffective both in Form and content and Should be Replaced with Effective Messages. June 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 11. Lempert LK, Glantz SA. Implications of tobacco industry research on packaging colors for designing health warning labels. Nicotine Tob Res. 2016;18(9):1910–1914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Mays D, Smith C, Johnson AC, Tercyak KP, Niaura RS. An experimental study of the effects of electronic cigarette warnings on young adult nonsmokers’ perceptions and behavioral intentions. Tob Induc Dis. 2016;14:17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Pieters R, Wedel M, Batra R. The stopping power of advertising: measures and effects of visual complexity. J Mark. 2010;74(5):48–60. [Google Scholar]
- 14. Riley MW, Cochran DJ, Ballard JL. An investigation of preferred shapes for warning labels. Hum Factors J Hum Factors Ergon Soc. 1982;24(6):737–742. [Google Scholar]
- 15. Wogalter MS, Jarrard SW, Simpson SN. Influencing of warning label signal words on perceived hazard level. Hum Factors. 1994;36(3):547–556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Fischhoff B, Brewer NT, Downs JS, eds. Communicating Risks and Benefits: An Evidence-Based User’s Guide. Silver Spring, MA: Food and Drug Administration (FDA), US Department of Health and Human Services; 2011. https://www.fda.gov/aboutfda/reportsmanualsforms/reports/ucm268078.htm. Accessed August 26, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 17. Noar SM, Hall MG, Francis DB, Ribisl KM, Pepper JK, Brewer NT. Pictorial cigarette pack warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. Tob Control. 2016;25(3):341–354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Pierce JP, Sargent JD, Portnoy DB, et al. . Association between receptivity to tobacco advertising and progression to tobacco use in youth and young adults in the PATH study. JAMA Pediatr. 2018;172(5):444–451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Padon AA, Maloney EK, Cappella JN. Youth-Targeted E-cigarette marketing in the US. Tob Regul Sci. 2017;3(1):95–101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Kraemer JD, Strasser AA, Lindblom EN, Niaura RS, Mays D. Crowdsourced data collection for public health: a comparison with nationally representative, population tobacco use data. Prev Med. 2017;102:93–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: a new source of inexpensive, yet high-Quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011;6(1):3–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Hauser DJ, Schwarz N. Attentive Turkers: MTurk participants perform better on online attention checks than do subject pool participants. Behav Res Methods. 2016;48(1):400–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Jeong M, Zhang D, Morgan JC, et al. . Similarities and differences in tobacco control research findings from convenience and probability samples. Ann Behav Med. 2019;53(5):476–485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Lazard AJ, Schmidt A, Vu H, et al. . Icons for health effects of cigarette smoke: a test of semiotic type. J Behav Med. 2017;40(4):641–650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Steinhart Y, Carmon Z, Trope Y. Warnings of adverse side effects can backfire over time. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(9):1842–1847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Stark E, Kim A, Miller C, Borgida E.. Effects of including a graphic warning label in advertisements for reduced-exposure products: implications for persuasion and policy1. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2008;38(2):281–293. [Google Scholar]
- 27. Nodora J, Hartman SJ, Strong DR, et al. . Curiosity predicts smoking experimentation independent of susceptibility in a US national sample. Addict Behav. 2014;39(12):1695–1700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Strong DR, Hartman SJ, Nodora J, et al. . Predictive validity of the expanded susceptibility to smoke index. Nicotine Tob Res. 2015;17(7):862–869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Crespo A, Cabestrero R, Grzib G, Quiros P.. Visual attention to health warnings in tobacco advertisements: an eye-tracking research between smokers and non-smokers. Stud Psychol. 2007;49(1):39–51. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Strasser AA, Tang KZ, Romer D, Jepson C, Cappella JN. Graphic warning labels in cigarette advertisements: recall and viewing patterns. Am J Prev Med. 2012;43(1):41–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. King JL, Reboussin D, Cornacchione Ross J, Wiseman KD, Wagoner KG, Sutfin EL. Polytobacco use among a nationally representative sample of adolescent and young adult e-cigarette users. J Adolesc Health. 2018;63(4):407–412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Kim AE, Lee YO, Shafer P, Nonnemaker J, Makarenko O. Adult smokers’ receptivity to a television advert for electronic nicotine delivery systems. Tob Control. 2015;24(2):132–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Grana RA, Ling PM. “Smoking revolution”: a content analysis of electronic cigarette retail websites. Am J Prev Med. 2014;46(4):395–403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Villanti AC, Rath JM, Williams VF, et al. . Impact of exposure to electronic cigarette advertising on susceptibility and trial of electronic cigarettes and cigarettes in US young adults: a Randomized Controlled Trial. Nicotine Tob Res. 2016;18(5):1331–1339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Fischer PM, Richards JW Jr, Berman EJ, Krugman DM. Recall and eye tracking study of adolescents viewing tobacco advertisements. JAMA. 1989;261(1):84–89. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Evans AT, Peters E, Keller-Hamilton B, et al. . Warning size affects what adolescents recall from tobacco advertisements. Tob Regul Sci. 2018;4(3):79–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Cummings KM, Hyland A, Giovino GA, Hastrup JL, Bauer JE, Bansal MA. Are smokers adequately informed about the health risks of smoking and medicinal nicotine? Nicotine Tob Res. 2004;6(suppl 3):S333–S340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Johnson SE, Coleman B, Tessman GK, Dickinson DM. Unpacking smokers’ beliefs about addiction and nicotine: a qualitative study. Psychol Addict Behav. 2017;31(7):744–750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Pacek LR, Rass O, Johnson MW. Knowledge about nicotine among HIV-positive smokers: implications for tobacco regulatory science policy. Addict Behav. 2017;65:81–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Patel D, Peiper N, Rodu B. Perceptions of the health risks related to cigarettes and nicotine among university faculty. Addict Res Theory. 2013;21(2):154–159. [Google Scholar]
- 41. Shah AK, Oppenheimer DM. Heuristics made easy: an effort-reduction framework. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(2):207–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Tversky A, Kahneman D. Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognit Psychol. 1973;5(2):207–232. [Google Scholar]
- 43. Baig SA, Byron MJ, Lazard AJ, Brewer NT. “Organic” “natural,” and “additive-Free” cigarettes: comparing the effects of advertising claims and disclaimers on perceptions of harm. Nicotine Tob Res. 2019;21(7):933–939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Apaolaza V, Hartmann P, Echebarria C, Barrutia JM. Organic label’s halo effect on sensory and hedonic experience of wine: a pilot study. J Sens Stud. 2017;32(1):e12243. [Google Scholar]
- 45. Williams P. Consumer understanding and use of health claims for foods. Nutr Rev. 2005;63(7):256–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Ray M. Marketing communication and the hierarchy-of-effects. In: Clarke P, ed. New Models of Communication Research. Vol 2 Beverly Hills: SAGE. [Google Scholar]
- 47. Greenwald AG, Leavitt C. Audience involvement in advertising: four levels. J Consum Res. 1984;11(1):581–592. [Google Scholar]
- 48. Zajonc RB. Mere exposure: a gateway to the subliminal. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2001;10(6):224–228. [Google Scholar]
- 49. Unger JB, Cruz TB, Schuster D, Flora JA, Johnson CA. Measuring exposure to pro- and anti-tobacco marketing among adolescents: intercorrelations among measures and associations with smoking status. J Health Commun. 2001;6(1):11–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.