Abstract
Background
Enuresis (bedwetting) affects up to 20% of five‐year‐olds and can have considerable social, emotional and psychological effects. Treatments include alarms (activated by urination), behavioural interventions and drugs.
Objectives
To assess the effects of enuresis alarms for treating enuresis in children.
Search methods
We searched the Cochrane Incontinence Specialised Register, which contains trials identified from the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), MEDLINE, MEDLINE In‐Process, MEDLINE Epub Ahead of Print, ClinicalTrials.gov, WHO ICTRP, and handsearching of journals and conference proceedings (searched 25 June 2018), and reference lists of relevant articles.
Selection criteria
We included randomised or quasi‐randomised trials of enuresis alarms or alarms combined with another intervention for treating nocturnal enuresis in children between 5 and 16 years old.
Data collection and analysis
Two review authors independently assessed risk of bias and extracted data.
Main results
We included 74 trials (5983 children).
At treatment completion, alarms may reduce the number of wet nights a week compared to control or no treatment (mean difference (MD) −2.68, 95% confidence interval (CI) −4.59 to −0.78; 4 trials, 127 children; low‐quality evidence). Low‐quality evidence suggests more children may achieve complete response (14 consecutive dry nights) with alarms compared to control or no treatment (RR 7.23, 95% CI 1.40 to 37.33; 18 trials, 827 children) and that more children may remain dry post‐treatment (RR 9.67, 95% CI 4.74 to 19.76; 10 trials, 366 children; low‐quality evidence).
At treatment completion, we are uncertain whether there is any difference between alarms and placebo drugs in the number of wet nights a week (MD −0.96, 95% CI −2.32 to 0.41; 1 trial, 47 children; very low‐quality evidence). Alarms may result in more children achieving complete response than with placebo drugs (RR 1.59, 95% CI 1.16 to 2.17; 2 trials, 181 children; low‐quality evidence). No trials comparing alarms to placebo reported the number of children remaining dry post‐treatment.
Compared with control alarms, code‐word alarms probably slightly increase the number of children achieving complete response at treatment completion (RR 1.11, 95% CI 0.97 to 1.27; 1 trial, 353 children; moderate‐quality evidence) but there is probably little to no difference in the number of children remaining dry post‐treatment (RR 0.91, 95% CI 0.79 to 1.05; moderate‐quality evidence). Very low‐quality evidence means we are uncertain if there are any differences in effectiveness between the other different types of alarm.
At treatment completion, alarms may reduce the number of wet nights a week compared with behavioural interventions (waking, bladder training, dry‐bed training, and star chart plus rewards) (MD ‐0.81, 95% CI ‐2.01 to 0.38; low‐quality evidence) and may increase the number of children achieving complete response (RR 1.77, 95% CI 0.98 to 3.19; low‐quality evidence) and may slightly increase the number of children remaining dry post‐treatment (RR 1.39, 95% CI 0.81 to 2.41; low‐quality evidence).
The evidence relating to alarms compared with desmopressin in the number of wet nights a week (MD −0.64, 95% CI −1.77 to 0.49; 4 trials, 285 children) and the number of children achieving complete response at treatment completion (RR 1.12, 95% CI 0.93 to 1.36; 12 trials, 1168 children) is low‐quality, spanning possible harms and possible benefits. Alarms probably slightly increase the number of children remaining dry post‐treatment compared with desmopressin (RR 1.30, 95% CI 0.92 to 1.84; 5 trials, 565 children; moderate‐quality evidence).
At treatment completion, we are uncertain if there is any difference between alarms and tricyclics in the number of wet nights a week, the number of children achieving complete response or the number of children remaining dry post‐treatment, because the quality of evidence is very low.
Due to very low‐quality evidence we are uncertain about any differences in effectiveness between alarms and cognitive behavioural therapy, psychotherapy, hypnotherapy and restricted diet.
Alarm plus desmopressin may reduce the number of wet nights a week compared with desmopressin monotherapy (MD −0.88, 95% CI −0.38 to −1.38; 2 trials, 156 children; low‐quality evidence). Alarm plus desmopressin may increase the number of children achieving complete response (RR 1.32, 95% CI 1.08 to 1.62; 5 trials, 359 children; low‐quality evidence) and the number of children remaining dry post‐treatment (RR 2.33, 95% CI 1.26 to 4.29; 2 trials, 161 children; low‐quality evidence) compared with desmopressin alone.
Alarm plus dry‐bed training may increase the number of children achieving a complete response compared to dry‐bed training alone (RR 3.79, 95% CI 1.85 to 7.77; 1 trial, 80 children; low‐quality evidence). It is unclear if there is any difference in the number of children remaining dry post‐treatment because of the wide confidence interval (RR 0.56, 95% CI 0.15 to 2.12; low‐quality evidence). Due to very low‐quality evidence, we are uncertain about any differences in effectiveness between alarm plus bladder training versus bladder training alone.
Of the 74 included trials, 17 reported one or more adverse events, nine reported no adverse events and 48 did not mention adverse events. Adverse events attributed to alarms included failure to wake the child, ringing without urination, waking others, causing discomfort, frightening the child and being too difficult to use. Adverse events of comparator interventions included nose bleeds, headaches and abdominal pain.
There is probably a slight increase in adverse events between code‐word alarm and standard alarm (RR 1.34, 95% CI 0.75 to 2.38; moderate‐quality evidence), although we are uncertain because of the wide confidence interval. Alarms probably reduce the number of children experiencing adverse events compared with desmopressin (RR 0.38, 95% CI 0.20 to 0.71; 5 trials, 565 children; moderate‐quality evidence). Very low‐quality evidence means we cannot be certain whether the adverse event rate for alarms is lower than for other treatments.
Authors' conclusions
Alarm therapy may be more effective than no treatment in reducing enuresis in children. We are uncertain if alarm therapy is more effective than desmopressin but there is probably a lower risk of adverse events with alarms than with desmopressin. Despite the large number of trials included in this review, further adequately‐powered trials with robust randomisation are still needed to determine the full effect of alarm therapy.
Plain language summary
Alarm interventions for nocturnal enuresis (bedwetting) in children
Review question
Is alarm training an effective treatment for bedwetting in children?
Background
Bedwetting is common in childhood and affects up to 20% of five‐year‐olds. Although children may spontaneously recover from this, bedwetting can have significant social, emotional and psychological effects on the child and their family. Treatments for bedwetting include alarms (activated by urination), behavioural interventions and drugs.
How up‐to‐date is this review?
The evidence is current up to 25 June 2018.
Study characteristics
This review includes 74 trials involving 5983 children.
Study funding sources
Three of the studies included in the review were funded or supported by a pharmaceutical company.
Key results
Alarms may be better than no treatment in terms of reducing the number of wet nights a week and increasing the number of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights. Alarms may increase the number of children remaining dry after stopping treatment, compared to no treatment.
We are uncertain if there is any difference between alarms and behavioural interventions (such as bladder training), or between alarms and desmopressin medication in terms of reducing the number of wet nights a week, increasing the number of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights and increasing the number of children remaining dry after stopping treatment.
We are uncertain whether alarms reduce the number of wet nights more than placebo drugs, other types of alarms, tricyclics medication, or various treatments such as hypnotherapy or restricted diet.
Alarm plus desmopressin may reduce the number of wet nights a week compared with desmopressin alone. It may increase the number of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights and increase the number of children remaining dry after stopping treatment. Alarm plus dry‐bed training may increase the number of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights compared to dry‐bed training alone, but we are uncertain if the response is maintained after stopping treatment.
Due to very low‐quality evidence, we are uncertain about any differences in effectiveness between alarm plus bladder training versus bladder training alone.
Alarm therapy probably reduces the number of children experiencing adverse events compared with desmopressin. We think there is probably a slight increase in adverse events between code‐word alarms and standard alarms but until we have more evidence from more children we cannot be certain about this. We are uncertain about the safety of alarm therapy compared to other treatments, because the quality of evidence is very low.
Quality of the evidence
The quality of the evidence means that, in general, the level of certainty in our conclusions is low.
Authors' conclusions
Alarm therapy may be more effective than no treatment, but due to the low‐quality evidence we do not know how much alarm therapy is more effective than none at all. Adding alarm therapy to desmopressin may be more effective than desmopressin alone. Despite the large number of trials we identified, the low certainty of the evidence means that we cannot tell from these studies whether or not alarm therapy is better than most other therapies for reducing bedwetting in children. We cannot be sure about its effects until there are better‐designed trials.
Summary of findings
Background
This is one of seven Cochrane Reviews of interventions for enuresis (bedwetting). The others focus on desmopressin (Glazener 2002), tricyclics (Caldwell 2016), drugs other than desmopressin or tricyclics (Deshpande 2012), simple behavioural interventions (Caldwell 2013), complex behavioural and educational interventions (Glazener 2004) and complementary and miscellaneous therapy (Huang 2011).
Description of the condition
Enuresis (or nocturnal enuresis) is the involuntary loss of urine at night, in the absence of organic disease, at an age when a child could reasonably be expected to be dry (by consensus, at a developmental age of five years) (APA 2013; Austin 2014; WHO 1992).
The International Children's Continence Society (ICCS) promotes the use of standardised nomenclature for describing paediatric lower urinary tract function, particularly in research, so that studies can be compared (Austin 2014). With improved understanding about the pathophysiology of bedwetting, enuresis is now subclassified as either monosymptomatic (bedwetting only) or non‐monosymptomatic (bedwetting with lower urinary tract symptoms in the presence or absence of daytime urinary incontinence). This subclassification is important, as the pathophysiology and treatment for both are different. However, many older studies did not distinguish between these groups.
The ICCS has also defined treatment outcomes in terms of change in frequency of wetting compared with baseline frequency (non‐response is less than a 50% reduction in frequency of wetting, partial response is 50% to 99% reduction and complete response is 100% reduction), relapse (more than one symptom recurrence a month after cessation of treatment) and duration of treatment success (initial success is a complete response at the end of treatment, continued success is a complete response for six months after cessation of treatment, and complete success is a complete response for two years after cessation of treatment) (Austin 2014). Unfortunately, most older studies usually did not assess these outcomes or were unclear about their definition of success.
Prevalences and causes
Enuresis is common in children. Estimating the true prevalence of enuresis is difficult because of the variability of diagnostic criteria used in different studies, with very few studies using ICCS definitions (Austin 2014; De Jonge 1973; Krantz 1994). The generally quoted prevalence rates are 15% to 20% of five‐year‐olds, 7% of seven‐year‐olds, 5% of 10‐year‐olds, 2% to 3% of teenagers and 0.5% to 2% of adults, with an annual spontaneous remission rate of about 14% until adolescence (Blackwell 1989; Forsythe 1974; Rutter 1973). The prevalence of enuresis is higher amongst children in residential care and is more persistent in children with severe enuresis (wetting more than three nights a week) (Morgan 1970; Yeung 2006).
Butler 2000 proposed the conceptual model that enuresis occurs when there is a defective arousal response during sleep to the sensation of a full bladder, a lack of inhibition of bladder emptying during sleep, a low capacity or overactive bladder or excessive nocturnal urine production. Other known risk factors for enuresis include:
genetic influences (APA 1980; Bakwin 1971; Bakwin 1973; Eiberg 1995);
delay in attaining bladder control (Jarvelin 1989; Koff 1995);
physiological factors (Djurhuus 1992; Norgaard 1993), for example constipation, faecal incontinence, daytime urinary incontinence and caffeine consumption (Blackwell 1989; Loening‐Baucke 1997; McGrath 2008; O'Regan 1986; Sureshkumar 2009);
sleep apnoea and upper airway obstructive symptoms (Maizels 1993); and
psychological factors (Devlin 1991; Moffatt 1989; Rutter 1973; Shaffer 1977).
These factors are thought to impact on sleep arousal, bladder contraction and overnight urine production.
Description of the intervention
Enuresis alarms
An enuresis alarm is an alarm system activated by micturition. The first enuresis alarms were pads or mats which contained an electrical circuit and were placed on the bed under the child (Mowrer 1938). Urine coming into contact with the pad or mat would complete the circuit, causing a bell to ring (bell and pad alarm). Historically, some bell and pad alarms gave an electric stimulus or shock to the child's skin. More recently, body‐worn alarms were developed where the sensor is placed in the child's underwear (instead of the pad or mat). A code‐word alarm has also been developed which works by delivering a pre‐recorded, personalised message when the child wets the bed; the child is encouraged to remember the word the following morning.
Overlearning
An overlearning procedure may be initiated after successful alarm treatment to reinforce alarm training. The child is given extra drinks at bedtime so that they learn to wake to void with a full bladder. Alarm training is then continued until 14 consecutive dry nights are once again achieved (Blackwell 1989).
How the intervention might work
Enuresis alarms work by conditioning the child to rouse when they wet at night, in response to being woken by the alarm signal when wetting occurs. They learn eventually either to rouse in response to bladder distension and void in the toilet before wetting, or to inhibit voiding by contracting their urethral sphincter if their bladder is not full (Forsythe 1989; Mowrer 1938).
Why it is important to do this review
Although enuresis in itself is pathologically benign and has a high rate of spontaneous remission, it can affect children's quality of life and self‐esteem, as well as impact on their social, emotional and psychological well‐being (Deshpande 2011; Hagglof 1998). More worryingly, enuresis may persist into adulthood, with significant long‐term effects (Fitzwater 1992; Yeung 2004). Enuresis affects the whole family and children with enuresis may experience parental disapproval, sibling teasing and be at an increased risk of emotional and physical abuse (Warzak 1992). Consequently, it is important to identify effective interventions for treating this common condition, as effective treatment can improve self‐esteem and quality of life (Hagglof 1998).
It is important to assess the effectiveness of alarm training both as monotherapy and in combination with other treatments, as it is a non‐pharmacological intervention and therefore has a potentially lower risk of side effects compared with pharmacological interventions.
Objectives
To assess the effects of enuresis alarms for treating enuresis in children.
Methods
Criteria for considering studies for this review
Types of studies
We included randomised controlled trials (RCTs) or quasi‐randomised trials, including cross‐over trials and cluster RCTs, of alarm training versus any comparable treatments for enuresis. Quasi‐randomised trials use methods of allocating participants to a treatment that is not strictly random, for example by alternate assignment or by replacing dropouts with the next participant.
Types of participants
We included trials of children (as defined by the trialists, but usually from 5 to 16 years of age) with enuresis.
We excluded studies of adults with enuresis, or where participants only had daytime urinary incontinence or an organic cause for their enuresis. We only included studies of children with daytime urinary incontinence if the primary focus of the study was enuresis. As it was often difficult to differentiate between monosymptomatic and non‐monosymptomatic enuresis, we included both types, with subgroup analyses of children with monosymptomatic enuresis if possible.
Types of interventions
We assessed any trial that used an enuresis alarm in at least one arm of the study for this review. Enuresis alarms included standard alarms (a bell and pad alarm that immediately emits a continuous single sound on wetting), or alarms using body‐worn sensors, or alarms of various loudness or that triggered a vibration, light or electric shock. Alarm signals could also be twin‐signal, intermittent or delayed. Alarms could wake the child or the parents. Alarm training could also be combined with another therapy.
We made the following comparisons:
Alarms versus control (non‐functioning) alarms or no treatment;
Alarms versus placebo drug;
Alarms versus other types of alarm;
Alarms versus other behavioural interventions;
Alarms versus drugs;
Alarms versus miscellaneous treatments; and
Alarms combined with another intervention versus other interventions.
We analysed alarm versus control or no treatment separately from alarm versus placebo drug, because placebo could have a different effect from control alarms or no treatment at all.
We identified studies comparing alarms combined with another intervention versus alarm monotherapy, or versus other treatment combinations, but we did not include them in the analysis because they do not directly help to answer the question about the effectiveness of alarm therapy. These comparisons are covered in Appendix 1 and Appendix 2 respectively.
Types of outcome measures
We based the outcomes considered in this review on those used in previous reviews.
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Proportion of children with adverse events
Where trials did not specifically report the number of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights, we interpreted the number of children achieving full response as the number achieving 14 consecutive dry nights.
Main outcomes for 'Summary of findings' tables
We included the following outcomes in the 'Summary of findings' tables (Guyatt 2008).
Mean number of wet nights a week;
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment;
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment);
Proportion of children with adverse events.
Search methods for identification of studies
We did not impose any language or other limitations on any of the searches described below.
Electronic searches
This review drew on the search strategy developed for Cochrane Incontinence. We identified relevant trials from the Cochrane Incontinence Specialised Register. For more details of the search methods used to build the Specialised Register, please see the Group's webpages where details of the Register's development (from inception) and the most recent searches performed to populate the Register can be found. To summarise, the Register contains trials identified from the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), MEDLINE, MEDLINE In‐Process, MEDLINE Epub Ahead of Print, ClinicalTrials.gov, WHO ICTRP, UK Clinical Research Network Portfolio and handsearching of journals and conference proceedings. Many of the trials in the Cochrane Incontinence Specialised Register are also contained in CENTRAL.
The terms used to search the Cochrane Incontinence Specialised Register are given in Appendix 3.
Date of the most recent search of the Register for this review: 25 June 2018.
Searching other resources
The review authors searched the reference lists of relevant articles.
Data collection and analysis
We assessed the studies for this review using the methods described in the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011).
Selection of studies
At least two review authors checked the titles and, where possible, abstracts of all studies located by the searches to identify those likely to be evaluations of the effects of alarm training for enuresis. We then obtained full papers and assessed them to identify studies that met the inclusion criteria. We settled any differences by discussion with a third review author.
Data extraction and management
We extracted data using a standard form and we processed included trial data as described in the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011) and contacted study authors for missing information where appropriate.
We converted results to the mean and standard deviation of the number of wet nights a week or the proportion of children achieving a complete response (defined as 14 consecutive dry nights) at the end of the treatment period, or the mean and standard deviation of the number of wet nights a week, or the proportion of children dry at follow‐up after stopping treatment (defined as children who remained dry after stopping treatment). When a mean value was reported with no standard deviation, we entered the data into 'Additional Tables'.
Assessment of risk of bias in included studies
We assessed risk of bias in included studies with Cochrane's 'Risk of bias' tool (Higgins 2017), using the following domains: random sequence generation; allocation concealment; blinding of study participants; blinding of study personnel; blinding of outcome assessors; incomplete outcome data addressed; selective reporting; and other potential bias (e.g. whether there was a washout period in cross‐over trials, and whether appropriate statistical techniques were used).
Where studies did not include the following, we considered them to be at high risk of reporting bias:
whether children with daytime wetting or organic causes of enuresis were specifically excluded;
whether there were baseline differences between groups;
whether adverse events were mentioned;
whether full data sets were given.
Measures of treatment effect
Where possible, we intended to calculate standardised effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals (CIs), mean differences (MDs) where outcomes were continuous variables and risk ratios (RRs) where they were binary. We weighted the mean differences by the inverse of the variance and gave them as differences in number of wet nights a week. Negative values indicated fewer wet nights in the alarm group.
Unit of analysis issues
The unit of allocation was individual participants.
We indicated cross‐over trials by using the symbol '#' after the study ID. For cross‐over trials, we planned to calculate data from paired comparisons to provide information for generic inverse variance analysis but none of the trials provided suitable data. Instead, we chose to tabulate the data as if the data were parallel groups. This approach leads to wider confidence intervals. Hence, statistically significant results using a parallel‐group approach would be significant if correctly analysed as cross‐over studies, but non‐significant results might hide a significant difference.
Dealing with missing data
When intention‐to‐treat analysis was not mentioned in the paper or if some participants were not analysed in the group to which they were randomised, we looked for sufficient information to restore them to the correct group. If participants could not be analysed in their allocated groups, we noted this in the 'Risk of bias' table and in the Risk of bias in included studies section.
Where there were missing outcome data in the included studies, the primary analysis used the number of participants with available data as the denominator (i.e. 'available case' analysis). We also attempted to contact the authors. If there was evidence of differential dropouts from the groups that may have been caused by adverse events or ineffectiveness of the interventions, we stated this in the 'Risk of bias' table. We obtained standard deviations, where not explicitly mentioned, from standard errors, CIs, t values or P values that related to the differences between means in two groups, according to section 7.7.3.3 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011).
Assessment of heterogeneity
We further investigated differences between trials when we noted statistically significant heterogeneity at the 10% probability level or using the I2 statistic (Higgins 2003), or if heterogeneity appeared obvious from visual inspection of forest plots. If there was no obvious reason for the heterogeneity, or it persisted despite the removal of outlying trials, we used a random‐effects model for pooling trial results.
Assessment of reporting biases
We investigated the possibility of publication bias and related biases, and produced funnel plots for outcomes measured in 10 or more studies.
Data synthesis
We performed meta‐analysis according to recommendations of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions if there were sufficient trials for each comparison (Higgins 2011). We used a fixed‐effect model for analysis. If there was substantial heterogeneity, suggesting that treatment effects differed between trials, we used a random‐effects model to produce an overall summary estimate.
Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity
Where data were available, we conducted subgroup analysis of our two primary outcomes to investigate whether alarms have different effects in children with monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis (MNE) compared with children with non‐monosymptomatic enuresis (NMNE). We interpreted subgroup analyses using the Chi2 statistic, P value and the interaction test I2 value.
Sensitivity analysis
We planned to do sensitivity analyses by removing trials with unclear or high risk of bias. However, there were so few trials at low risk of bias that it would not have given meaningful results.
Summary of findings and assessment of the certainty of the evidence
We prepared 'Summary of findings' tables for our main comparisons and presented the results for the four outcomes prespecified for this purpose in the Types of outcome measures.
We adopted the GRADE approach to assess the quality of evidence related to these outcomes (Guyatt 2008). The four levels of evidence certainty are 'high', 'moderate', 'low' or 'very low'. We considered the following factors for assessing the quality of evidence: limitations in the study design, inconsistency of results, indirectness of evidence, imprecision and publication bias.
Results
Description of studies
Results of the search
The literature search produced 493 abstracts and titles for review. We assessed 182 full‐text articles and identified 87 reports for the 74 included studies. In three cases, two different trials were reported within the same paper (Bollard 1981a; Bollard 1981b; Butler 1990a; Butler 1990b; Geffken 1986a; Geffken 1986b). In another, three trials were reported within a single paper (Lovibond 1964a; Lovibond 1964b; Lovibond 1964c).
We identified 92 reports for 89 excluded studies, and two reports of one ongoing study (NCT03389412).
The flow of literature through this assessment process is shown in the PRISMA flowchart (see Figure 1).
1.
PRISMA study flow diagram
Included studies
Design
This review includes 74 randomised or quasi‐randomised trials of alarm training versus any other treatment for enuresis; 40 were parallel‐group studies and 34 were multi‐arm studies. Twenty‐six trials evaluated the effect of enuresis alarms combined with another intervention.
There were two cross‐over studies (Kwak 2010#; Sukhai 1989#). There was also a study where participants were offered the alternate treatment if the first treatment was not effective (Wille 1986).
Sample size
The sample size of studies ranged from 14 to 353 participants (mean 81 participants). There were 30 studies with 50 or fewer participants, 22 studies with 51 to 99 participants, and 22 studies with 100 or more participants.
Setting
Most of the trials were conducted in outpatient settings. Fourteen trials were based in the community (Danquah 1975; Hoseinzadeh 1997), including schools (Kolvin 1972 Turner 1970), community health clinics (Bryant 2003; Nawaz 2002; Ronen 1995; Sacks 1974), from an alarm rental agency (Van Londen 1993), or recruited through the media (Azrin 1974; Azrin 1978; Baker 1969; Fournier 1987; Houts 1986). Some trials used multiple settings (Lovibond 1964a; Lovibond 1964b; Lovibond 1964c; Lynch 1984; Sukhai 1989#; Wagner 1982). Four trials were based in institutional settings (Jehu 1977; Kennedy 1968, Sloop 1973; Wagner 1985). No settings were provided in four trials (Hojsgaard 1979; Ozden 2008; Shakiba 2001; Wright 1974).
Participants
This review included 5983 children. Most trials included children aged between 5 and 16 years. Four trials included older participants: Bollard 1982a included boys up to 17 years of age; McKendry 1975; Scholander 1968 up to 17; and Sloop 1973 up to 18. Seven trials included a few children under the age of five years (Azrin 1974; Azrin 1978; Jehu 1977; Taylor 1975; Wille 1986; Wright 1974; Young 1972). One paper reported two trials (Geffken 1986a; Geffken 1986b) which considered different populations of participants split according to large and small baseline functional bladder capacity.
Duration
The duration of alarm training varied between studies, ranging from two weeks to six months. Most trials incorporated a period of follow‐up, but 22 trials did not report any follow‐up of participants after the end of treatment (Azrin 1978; Azrin 1978; Baker 1969; Butler 1988; Caceres 1982; Caldwell 2015; Elinder 1985; Fagundes 2017; Fournier 1987; Hojsgaard 1979; Kennedy 1968; Lynch 1984; McKendry 1975; Moffatt 1987; Motavalli 1994; Rodriguez 2001; Sacks 1974; Scholander 1968; Tobias 2001; Tuncel 2014; Werry 1965; Wright 1974).
Interventions
Most alarms used in trials were bell and pad (Mowrer) alarms. Some studies used body‐worn alarms with the sensor in the child's underwear (Butler 1990a; Butler 1990b). The enuresis alarm usually activated an audio stimulus such as a bell or buzzer, but other stimuli included vibration and electric shock (Crosby or Uristop alarm) (Lovibond 1964a; Netley 1984; Tobias 2001). Some studies compared variability in the alarm stimulus, such as loudness (Finley 1977), a single versus twin stimulus (Lovibond 1964a; Lovibond 1964c; Turner 1970), immediate versus delayed triggering of the stimulus after wetting (Lynch 1984; Wagner 1985), continuous versus intermittent reinforcement of alarm training (Finley 1973; Taylor 1975), and a stimulus that woke the child only versus the parents as well (Azrin 1974). Other studies compared whether the child was rewarded for being dry, for correct behaviour, or not rewarded at all (Caldwell 2015; Van Londen 1993). There were four studies of overlearning, which is aimed at reducing relapse rates (Houts 1986; Moffatt 1987; Taylor 1975; Young 1972).
Twenty trials compared an enuresis alarm with no treatment or control: 18 with no treatment or wait list (Baker 1969; Bennett 1985; Bollard 1981a; Bollard 1981b; Hojsgaard 1979; Houts 1986; Jehu 1977; Lynch 1984; Moffatt 1987; Nawaz 2002; Ronen 1995; Sacks 1974; Shakiba 2001; Sloop 1973; Wagner 1982; Wagner 1985; Werry 1965; Wright 1974), one with a control (nonfunctioning) alarm (Elinder 1985), and one with a control alarm that rang in the parents' room 20 minutes later (Finley 1973). In standard enuresis alarm training, a sound is immediately triggered when the child wets the bed. Standard alarm training is usually supervised by parents. Non‐standard forms of alarm training that were compared with no treatment included alarms that delivered an electric shock to the child's skin (Elinder 1985; Hojsgaard 1979), had a delayed trigger (Lynch 1984; Wagner 1985), or sounded intermittently (Finley 1973).
Five studies compared alarm to placebo drug (Fournier 1987; Kolvin 1972; Longstaffe 2000; Turner 1970; Wright 1974).
Thirteen studies compared different types of alarms:
bell and pad versus body‐worn alarms (Butler 1990a)
continuous versus intermittent alarms (Finley 1973; Taylor 1975)
immediate versus delayed alarms (Lynch 1984; Wagner 1985)
single‐signal versus twin‐signal alarms (Lovibond 1964b; Lovibond 1964c; Turner 1970)
bell and pad versus electric shock alarms (Lovibond 1964a)
loud versus quiet alarms (Finley 1973; Finley 1977)
audio versus vibration alarms (Tobias 2001)
body‐worn alarm versus code‐word body‐worn alarm (Caldwell 2015)
Ten studies compared alarm training to other behavioural interventions:
waking to void (Azrin 1978; Baker 1969; Fournier 1987; Lovibond 1964b; Turner 1970)
bladder training (Bennett 1985; Bryant 2003)
dry‐bed training (a complex behavioural intervention involving bedtime fluids, hourly waking and cleanliness training) (Bollard 1981b; Caceres 1982)
star chart and rewards (Ronen 1995)
Twenty‐four trials reported on 28 comparisons that compared alarm training to placebo drugs or active drug treatment (including Fournier 1987, Kolvin 1972, Longstaffe 2000 and Wright 1974, which had multiple comparisons):
desmopressin (Ahmed 2013; Bak 2007; Evans 2011; Fagundes 2017; Faraj 1999; Kwak 2010#; Longstaffe 2000; Ng 2005; Onol 2015; Tuncel 2014; Tuygun 2007; Vogt 2010; Wille 1986)
tricyclics (imipramine: Fournier 1987; Hoseinzadeh 1997; Kolvin 1972; McKendry 1975; Motavalli 1994; Netley 1984; Wagner 1982; clomipramine: Motavalli 1994; amitriptyline: Danquah 1975)
combined amphetamine, ephedrine and atropine sulphate (Wright 1974)
placebo drug (Fournier 1987; Kolvin 1972; Longstaffe 2000; Turner 1970; Wright 1974)
Six studies compared alarms to other treatments not classified elsewhere:
cognitive behavioural therapy (Ronen 1995)
psychotherapy (Sacks 1974; Werry 1965)
hypnotherapy (Seabrook 2005)
shaming (traditional Ghanian method for treating enuresis which consisted of the enuretic child being carried from home by a singing mob and being thrown into the lagoon) (Danquah 1975)
restricted diet (McKendry 1975)
Nine studies investigated alarms combined with another treatment versus the other treatment alone:
alarm plus bladder training versus bladder training (Bennett 1985; Bryant 2003)
alarm plus dry‐bed training versus dry‐bed training (Bollard 1981b)
alarm plus desmopressin versus desmopressin (Ahmed 2013; Bak 2007; Fagundes 2017; Ma 2007; Ng 2005)
alarm plus imipramine versus imipramine (Fournier 1987)
Twenty‐two trials reported on 25 comparisons that investigated alarm monotherapy compared with the following combinations of treatments (including Houts 1986, Naitoh 2005 and Taylor 1975, which had multiple comparisons):
alarm plus overlearning (Taylor 1975; Young 1972)
alarm plus dry‐bed training (Bollard 1981b; Nawaz 2002)
alarm plus bladder training (Bennett 1985; Bryant 2003; Fielding 1980; Geffken 1986a; Geffken 1986b; Houts 1986)
alarm plus bladder training plus overlearning (Houts 1986)
alarm plus desmopressin (Ahmed 2013; Bak 2007; Bradbury 1995; Gibb 2004; Leebeek 2001; Naitoh 2005; Ng 2005; Ozden 2008; Rodriguez 2001)
alarm plus imipramine (Fournier 1987; Naitoh 2005)
alarm plus methedrine (Kennedy 1968)
alarm plus reward (Van Londen 1993)
alarm plus pretreatment (Taylor 1975)
As shown in Table 9, 14 trials investigated alarms combined with another treatment compared to no treatment, placebo, one single treatment or combinations of treatment (Bennett 1985; Bollard 1981b; Fournier 1987; Gibb 2004; Houts 1986; Leebeek 2001; Ma 2007; Naitoh 2005; Nawaz 2002; Scholander 1968; Sukhai 1989#; Taylor 1975; Titawee 2000; Van Londen 1993).
1. Studies investigating combination therapies.
Treatment combination: alarm plus other | Comparator | Study |
Alarm plus bladder training | No treatment | Bennett 1985; Houts 1986; Nawaz 2002 |
Alarm plus dry‐bed training | Bollard 1981b | |
Alarm plus bladder training plus overlearning | Houts 1986 | |
Alarm plus imipramine | Placebo | Fournier 1987 |
Alarm plus nortriptyline | Scholander 1968 | |
Alarm plus behavioural treatment | Desmopressin | Ma 2007 |
Alarm plus behavioural therapy and desmopressin | Ma 2007 | |
Alarm plus imipramine | Random waking | Fournier 1987 |
Alarm plus dry‐bed training | Dry‐bed training | Bollard 1981b |
Alarm plus desmopressin | Alarm plus placebo | Gibb 2004; Leebeek 2001; Sukhai 1989# |
Alarm plus imipramine | Fournier 1987 | |
Alarm plus nortriptyline | Scholander 1968 | |
Alarm plus behavioural treatment and desmopressin | Alarm plus behavioural treatment | Ma 2007 |
Alarm plus desmopressin | Alarm plus imipramine | Naitoh 2005 |
Alarm plus behavioural therapy and desmopressin | Alarm plus behavioural therapy | Ma 2007 |
Alarm plus behavioural therapy | Fluid restriction and lifting plus behavioural therapy | Titawee 2000 |
Alarm plus immediate reward | Alarm plus delayed reward | Van Londen 1993 |
Alarm plus pretreatment A | Alarm plus pretreatment B | Taylor 1975 |
Types of outcomes
Two studies did not report our primary outcomes (Azrin 1978; Hoseinzadeh 1997). Forty‐eight studies did not mention adverse events.
Excluded studies
We excluded 89 studies from this review. Thirty‐eight studies were not randomised controlled trials and 41 studies did not use an enuresis alarm. We excluded five studies because they either involved adults (Azrin 1973; Collins 1975; Crisp 1984; Hanson 1988), or children with daytime wetting (Halliday 1987). We excluded the other five studies for methodological reasons: two were not intervention studies (Juul 2011; Mahler 2012); one was a diagnostic test study (Elmissiry 2013); one switched children between treatment groups (McConaghy 1969); and one, which was included in a previous version of this review, added amphetamine to the alarm group, depending on treatment response (Forrester 1964).
Details are given in the Characteristics of excluded studies.
Ongoing studies
There were two reports of one ongoing study (NCT03389412). Details are given in the Characteristics of ongoing studies.
Risk of bias in included studies
There was a high or unclear risk of bias across all domains in most trials due to insufficient information. In particular, most studies did not provide sufficient details about methods of randomisation and allocation concealment, and most studies did not blind participants or outcome assessors (see Figure 2 and Figure 3).
2.
Risk of bias graph: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item presented as percentages across all included studies.
3.
Risk of bias summary: review authors' judgements about each risk of bias item for each included study.
Allocation
Random sequence generation
Ten trials used an appropriate method for random sequence generation (Ahmed 2013; Azrin 1974; Bryant 2003; Caldwell 2015; Evans 2011; Faraj 1999; Kwak 2010#; Longstaffe 2000; Seabrook 2005; Tobias 2001).
Twelve trials were at high risk of selection bias due to quasi‐randomised methods of sequence generation. Studies used alternate allocation (Butler 1990a; Butler 1990b; Kennedy 1968; Wagner 1985), or sequential allocation (Ronen 1995; Taylor 1975; Van Londen 1993), randomised participants in pairs (Sloop 1973), allocated the intervention based on previous treatment (Naitoh 2005), initially used a system of random numbers but later abandoned randomisation to ensure balanced groups (Werry 1965), and replaced children who dropped out by the next child referred to the clinic (Bennett 1985; Turner 1970).
The remaining trials did not provide sufficient information about the method used to randomly allocate children to the treatment group.
Allocation concealment
Five trials achieved adequate concealment of group allocation by using a remote computer (Azrin 1974; Bryant 2003; Caldwell 2015; Evans 2011; Longstaffe 2000).
Thirteen trials had inadequate allocation concealment and were at high risk of selection bias (Bennett 1985; Butler 1990a; Butler 1990b; Hoseinzadeh 1997; Naitoh 2005; Ronen 1995; Sloop 1973; Taylor 1975; Turner 1970; Van Londen 1993; Wagner 1985; Werry 1965), including one study where participants who did not accept their allocated treatment were assigned to the control group (Ma 2007).
The remaining trials did not provide sufficient details of the method used to conceal treatment allocation after randomisation.
Blinding
In trials where an enuresis alarm was compared with a dissimilar intervention, it was usually difficult to blind the participants and outcome assessors (who we assume are the parents because they determine and record the child's treatment outcome). Since it is uncertain if lack of blinding in this situation could have an impact on the outcome, most studies had an unclear risk of performance and detection bias. Six studies adequately blinded participants and outcome assessors: one trial compared a code‐word alarm with a control alarm (Caldwell 2015); four trials compared alarms combined with medication versus alarms combined with placebo (Gibb 2004; Leebeek 2001; Scholander 1968; Sukhai 1989#); and one trial compared two alarms of different noise intensity (Finley 1977). In trials using placebo, blinding of some but not all arms of the study was possible (Fournier 1987; Turner 1970; Van Hoeck 2008). One study stated that research personnel were blinded (Titawee 2000).
Incomplete outcome data
Thirty‐one studies were at low risk of attrition bias. In these studies, data were reported for each main outcome and the rate of attrition was less than 10%, with reasons for attrition and exclusions from analysis given which were thought to be unrelated to treatment response (Ahmed 2013; Bollard 1981a; Bollard 1981b; Caceres 1982; Caldwell 2015; Gibb 2004; Houts 1986; Jehu 1977; Kennedy 1968; Kwak 2010#; Leebeek 2001; Longstaffe 2000; Lovibond 1964a; Lovibond 1964b; Lovibond 1964c; Lynch 1984; Ma 2007; Moffatt 1987; Nawaz 2002; Sacks 1974; Scholander 1968; Shakiba 2001; Sukhai 1989#; Titawee 2000; Tuncel 2014; Tuygun 2007; Van Hoeck 2008; Van Londen 1993; Wagner 1985; Wille 1986; Wright 1974). In 10 trials, the risk of attrition bias was unclear or attrition was not mentioned (Azrin 1978; Bollard 1982a; Finley 1973; Finley 1977; Hojsgaard 1979; Hoseinzadeh 1997; Motavalli 1994; Naitoh 2005; Seabrook 2005; Sloop 1973). The other trials had high risk of attrition bias, because the reasons for withdrawal were not provided, or over 10% of participants were not accounted for in the results.
Only seven trials reported no withdrawals (Ma 2007; Nawaz 2002; Scholander 1968; Sukhai 1989#; Tuncel 2014; Tuygun 2007; Wagner 1985). In studies with known withdrawals, analyses were conducted according to the reported results, which usually excluded dropouts. However, in two studies, dropouts were clearly related to the treatment group and treated as failures (Bollard 1981b; McKendry 1975). In another study sponsored by a pharmaceutical company, although no reasons for withdrawal were provided, there were different rates of withdrawal between treatment groups and withdrawals were reported and analysed as failures (Leebeek 2001).
Children withdrew after enrolment due to being ineligible for the trial after baseline assessment, receiving the wrong intervention, experiencing family disruption, failing to attend for monitoring or follow‐up, and not adhering to treatment due to difficulty with the treatment, including reluctance to use the electric shock alarms. Sometimes withdrawals were replaced by other participants, which added bias to the reported outcomes (Taylor 1975; Turner 1970).
Follow‐up after cessation of treatment
Fifty‐two studies followed up participants after cessation of treatment or provided data about relapse, but only 35 had useful data (Bennett 1985; Bollard 1981a; Bollard 1981b; Bradbury 1995; Butler 1990a; Caldwell 2015; Danquah 1975; Evans 2011; Fielding 1980; Finley 1973; Finley 1977; Gibb 2004; Hoseinzadeh 1997; Houts 1986; Jehu 1977; Kolvin 1972; Leebeek 2001; Lovibond 1964a; Lovibond 1964b; Lovibond 1964c; Ma 2007; Nawaz 2002; Ng 2005; Ozden 2008; Ronen 1995; Scholander 1968; Seabrook 2005; Shakiba 2001; Taylor 1975; Titawee 2000; Van Hoeck 2008; Van Londen 1993; Wagner 1982; Wagner 1985; Young 1972). Three of the studies with follow‐up data monitored the participants for less than three months (Gibb 2004; Shakiba 2001; Wagner 1982). We were uncertain whether participants were followed up after completion of treatment in one study, because a full translation was not available (Hojsgaard 1979).
Thirteen trials had no follow‐up after stopping treatment (Azrin 1974; Azrin 1978; Baker 1969; Bollard 1982a; Butler 1988; Lynch 1984; Moffatt 1987; Motavalli 1994; Naitoh 2005; Rodriguez 2001; Sloop 1973; Tobias 2001; Werry 1965). In six trials, follow‐up data did not reflect true treatment response after stopping treatment because participants were offered the alternative treatment if they failed to attain dryness (Caceres 1982; Faraj 1999; Kwak 2010#; Longstaffe 2000; Turner 1970; Wille 1986). We did not use follow‐up data in the cross‐over trial of alarm training combined with desmopressin or placebo, given that data were provided after use of both treatment combinations (Sukhai 1989#). However, in cross‐over trials that provided data about relapse, we used the proportion remaining dry after stopping treatment in the first phase of the trial only (Kwak 2010#; Sukhai 1989#).
Selective reporting
We assessed selective reporting bias in studies by examining whether systematic baseline measurements of enuresis were made before starting treatment, organic causes of enuresis or daytime urinary incontinence were excluded, adverse events were reported, full data sets were provided, and all prespecified outcomes were reported in studies that provided study protocols. Nine studies had low reporting bias (Ahmed 2013; Bradbury 1995; Caldwell 2015; Evans 2011; Gibb 2004; Kwak 2010#; Nawaz 2002; Ng 2005; Sukhai 1989#), while 12 studies had unclear reporting bias (Azrin 1974; Butler 1988; Butler 1990a; Butler 1990b; Hoseinzadeh 1997; Motavalli 1994; Netley 1984; Scholander 1968; Wagner 1982; Wagner 1985; Werry 1965; Young 1972). The remaining studies had high risk of reporting bias.
Baseline wetting
In 22 trials, baseline wetting was not recorded before beginning the trial (Azrin 1978; Bak 2007; Baker 1969; Bennett 1985; Bollard 1981a; Bollard 1981b; Finley 1973; Finley 1977; Hojsgaard 1979; Hoseinzadeh 1997; Kennedy 1968; Lovibond 1964a; Lovibond 1964b; Lovibond 1964c; McKendry 1975; Rodriguez 2001; Sacks 1974; Shakiba 2001; Titawee 2000; Tobias 2001; Van Hoeck 2008; Van Londen 1993).
Exclusion of organic causes or daytime wetting
Most trials mentioned the exclusion of children with known organic causes of wetting, but many did not specify the exclusion of children with daytime urinary incontinence. Only 26 trials reported both organic causes of wetting and daytime urinary incontinence as exclusion criteria and hence could be considered as addressing monosymptomatic enuresis (Ahmed 2013; Azrin 1978; Butler 1990b; Elinder 1985; Evans 2011; Fagundes 2017; Faraj 1999; Fielding 1980; Finley 1973; Finley 1977; Kwak 2010#; Leebeek 2001; Longstaffe 2000; Naitoh 2005; Nawaz 2002; Ng 2005; Onol 2015; Ozden 2008; Rodriguez 2001; Seabrook 2005; Sukhai 1989#; Tobias 2001; Tuygun 2007; Vogt 2010; Wagner 1982; Wille 1986).
Reporting of adverse events
Seventeen trials reported one or more adverse events (Baker 1969; Caldwell 2015; Danquah 1975; Elinder 1985; Evans 2011; Fournier 1987; Gibb 2004; Jehu 1977; Kwak 2010#; Lovibond 1964a; McKendry 1975; Moffatt 1987; Scholander 1968; Shakiba 2001; Tobias 2001; Turner 1970; Wille 1986), while nine studies reported that there were no adverse events noted (Ahmed 2013; Bradbury 1995; Leebeek 2001; Ma 2007; Ng 2005; Sukhai 1989#; Titawee 2000; Tuygun 2007; Van Hoeck 2008). The remaining trials did not mention adverse events.
Adverse events attributed to alarm training included the alarm failing to wake the child, ringing when the child had not wet the bed, waking others and disrupting the family, causing discomfort, frightening the child, and being too difficult or complicated to use. Alarms delivering electric shocks to the skin were clearly unacceptable because they frightened children and sometimes caused skin burns. These alarms included the Mozes detector that delivered an electric shock to the abdominal skin (McKendry 1975; Netley 1984), the Uristop that delivered an electric impulse to the groin (Elinder 1985; Hojsgaard 1979), and the Crosby Dri‐Nite that delivered an electric shock (Lovibond 1964a). These electric shock alarms are no longer in use. Side effects of comparator interventions included nose bleeds, headaches and abdominal pain.
Statistical reporting
Twenty‐three trials reported continuous data but did not provide measures of dispersion, such as standard deviations (Azrin 1974; Azrin 1978; Bak 2007; Baker 1969; Bollard 1981a; Bollard 1981b; Bollard 1982a; Bryant 2003; Butler 1988; Butler 1990a; Butler 1990b; Danquah 1975; Evans 2011; Fielding 1980; Finley 1973; Fournier 1987; Jehu 1977; Kolvin 1972; Leebeek 2001; Naitoh 2005; Wagner 1982; Wagner 1985; Wright 1974). We report their data in Table 10 and Table 11.
2. Mean number of wet nights a week at end of treatment (no SDs).
Trial | Intervention | Mean wet nights a week | Comparator | Mean wet nights a week |
Alarm versus control/placebo | ||||
Baker 1969 | alarm | 1.8 (n = 10) | control | 5.9 (n = 10) |
Bollard 1981b | alarm | 0.6 (n = 20) | control | 4.4 (n = 20) |
Finley 1973 | Intermittent alarm | 0.2 (n = 10) | control | 8 (n = 10) |
Jehu 1977 | alarm | 0.3 (n = 19) | control | 5.3 (n = 20) |
Wagner 1982 | pad‐and‐bell/buzzer | 0.58 (n = 12) | control | 4.26 (n = 12) |
Wagner 1985 | delayed alarm | 0.38 (n = 13) | control | 5.1 (n = 13) |
Fournier 1987 | alarm | 2.5 (n = 8) | placebo | 5 (n = 8) |
Kolvin 1972 | alarm | 2.3 (n = 32) | placebo | 2.7 (n = 27) |
Wright 1974 | alarm | 1.7 (n = 10) | placebo | 3.5 (n = 5) |
Alarm versus other types of alarm | ||||
Butler 1990a | bell and pad alarm | 1.2 (n = 17) | body alarm | 1 (n = 18) |
Finley 1973 | alarm | 1 (n = 18) | intermittent alarm | 0.6 (n = 10) |
Wagner 1985 | immediate alarm | 0.38 (n =1 3) | delayed alarm | 1.45 (n = 13) |
Alarm versus other behavioural interventions | ||||
Baker 1969 | alarm | 1.8 (n = 10) | waking | 3.1 (n = 10) |
Fournier 1987 | alarm | 2.5 (n = 8) | waking | 3.3 (n = 8) |
Bryant 2003 | alarm | 3.0 (n = 34) | bladder training | 4.0 (n = 37) |
Azrin 1978 | alarm | 5.32 (n = 27) | dry‐bed training (no alarm) | 1.05 (n = 28) |
Bollard 1981b | alarm | 0.6 (n = 20) | dry‐bed training (no alarm) | 3.8 (n = 20) |
Alarm versus drugs | ||||
Evans 2011 | alarm | 5.0 (n = 59) | desmopressin | 4.28 (n = 192) |
Fournier 1987 | alarm | 2.5 (n = 8) | imipramine | 1.9 (n = 8) |
Kolvin 1972 | alarm | 2.3 (n = 32) | imipramine | 2.3 (n = 35) |
Wagner 1985 | alarm | 0.58 (n = 12) | imipramine | 2.75 (n = 12) |
Danquah 1975 | alarm | 3.2 (n = 10) | amitriptyline | 4 (n = 10) |
Wright 1974 | alarm | 1.7 (n = 10) | amphetamine sulphate/Enetrol (ephedrine + atropine) | 4.1 (n = 8) |
Alarm versus miscellaneous treatments | ||||
Danquah 1975 | alarm | 3.2 (n = 10) | shaming | 5.6 (n = 10) |
Alarm plus other intervention versus other intervention | ||||
Bryant 2003 | alarm plus bladder training | 4.3 (n = 45) | bladder training | 4.0 (n = 37) |
Fournier 1987 | alarm plus imipramine | 1 (n = 8) | imipramine | 1.9 (n = 8) |
Alarm plus other intervention versus alarm monotherapy | ||||
Bryant 2003 | alarm plus bladder training | 4.3 (n = 45) | alarm | 4.0 (n = 37) |
Fielding 1980 | alarm plus bladder training | 1.5 (n = 16) | alarm | 0.6 (n = 17) |
Bollard 1981b | alarm plus dry bed training | 0 (n = 60) | alarm | 0.6 (n = 20) |
Leebeek 2001 | alarm plus desmopressin | 2.77 (n = 43) | alarm | 2.21 (n = 39) |
Naitoh 2005 | alarm plus desmopressin | 2.5 (n = 35) | alarm | 3.1 (n = 37) |
Fournier 1987 | alarm plus imipramine | 1 (n = 8) | alarm | 2.5 (n = 8) |
Naitoh 2005 | alarm plus imipramine | 2.15 (n = 33) | alarm | 3.1 (n = 37) |
3. Mean number of wet nights a week after stopping treatment (no SDs).
Trial | Intervention | Mean wet nights per week | Comparator | Mean wet nights per week |
Alarm versus drugs | ||||
Evans 2011 | alarm | 1.5 (n = 59) | desmopressin | 2.1 (n = 192) |
Kolvin 1972 | alarm | 2.3 (n = 32) | imipramine | 3.4 (n = 35) |
Danquah 1975 | alarm | 1.49 (n = 10) | amitriptyline | 3.42 (n = 10) |
Alarm versus miscellaneous treatments | ||||
Danquah 1975 | alarm | 1.49 (n = 10) | shaming | 5.6 (n = 10) |
Alarm plus other intervention versus other intervention | ||||
Leebeek 2001 | alarm plus desmopressin | 2.72 (n = 41) | alarm | 1.90 (n = 37) |
Other potential sources of bias
Only three studies were at low risk of other potential sources of bias (Bradbury 1995; Kwak 2010#; Ozden 2008). Fourteen studies were at high risk due to having differential dropout rates between treatment arms, switching treatments if participants did not respond to initial treatment, replacing dropouts with new participants, requiring participants to pay for the enuresis alarm, or accepting study funding from a pharmaceutical company (Evans 2011; Faraj 1999; Fielding 1980; Geffken 1986a; Geffken 1986b; Leebeek 2001; Houts 1986; Ng 2005; Onol 2015; Ronen 1995; Seabrook 2005; Titawee 2000; Turner 1970; Tuygun 2007). The risk of other potential sources of bias was unclear for the remaining studies.
Effects of interventions
See: Table 1; Table 2; Table 3; Table 4; Table 5; Table 6; Table 7; Table 8
Summary of findings 1. Alarm compared to control or no treatment for nocturnal enuresis in children.
Alarm compared to control or no treatment for nocturnal enuresis in children | ||||||
Patient or population: children with nocturnal enuresis Setting: outpatient Intervention: alarm Comparison: no treatment or control | ||||||
Outcomes | Anticipated absolute effects* (95% CI) | Relative effect (95% CI) | № of participants (studies) | Quality of the evidence (GRADE) | Comments | |
Risk with no treatment or control | Risk with alarm | |||||
Mean number of wet nights a week | The mean number of wet nights a week in the control group ranged from 4.06 to 5.74 | Participants in the alarm group experienced 2.68 fewer wet nights a week (4.59 fewer to 0.78 fewer) compared with the control group. | ‐ | 127 (4 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOW a, b | ‐ |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 133 per 1000 | 958 per 1000 (186 to 1000) | RR 7.23 (1.40 to 37.33) | 827 (18 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOW a, c | 826 more childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm (from 53 more to 1000 more) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) | 18 per 1000 | 177 per 1000 (87 to 361) | RR 9.67 (4.74 to 19.76) | 366 (10 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOW b, d | 159 more childrenper 1000 maintaining 14 dry nights post‐treatment with alarm (from 68 more to 34310 more) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | Not reported | |||||
*The risk in the intervention group (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI). CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio; | ||||||
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence High quality: We are very confident that the true effect lies close to that of the estimate of the effect Moderate quality: We are moderately confident in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be close to the estimate of the effect, but there is a possibility that it is substantially different Low quality: Our confidence in the effect estimate is limited: The true effect may be substantially different from the estimate of the effect Very low quality: We have very little confidence in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be substantially different from the estimate of effect |
aDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of selection, attrition and reporting bias. bDowngraded by one level due to serious imprecision: few participants and wide 95% CIs. cDowngraded by one level due to asymmetrical funnel plot: smaller studies may have been conducted but not published. dDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of selection and reporting bias.
Summary of findings 2. Alarm compared to placebo drug for nocturnal enuresis in children.
Alarm compared to placebo drug for nocturnal enuresis in children | ||||||
Patient or population: children with nocturnal enuresis Setting: outpatient Intervention: alarm Comparison: placebo | ||||||
Outcomes | Anticipated absolute effects* (95% CI) | Relative effect (95% CI) | № of participants (studies) | Quality of the evidence (GRADE) | Comments | |
Risk with placebo | Risk with alarm | |||||
Mean number of wet nights a week | The mean number of wet nights a week in the placebo group was 4.22 | Participants in the alarm group experienced 0.96 fewer wet nights a week (2.32 fewer to 0.41 more) compared with the placebo group | ‐ | 47 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b | ‐ |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 397 per 1000 | 632 per 1000 (461 to 862) | RR 1.59 (1.16 to 2.17) | 181 (2 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWc, d | 234 more childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm (from 64 more to 465 more) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) | Not reported | |||||
Proportion of children with adverse events | 2 trials collected information related to adverse events but did not report any useable data. | |||||
*The risk in the intervention group (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI). CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio; | ||||||
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence High quality: We are very confident that the true effect lies close to that of the estimate of the effect Moderate quality: We are moderately confident in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be close to the estimate of the effect, but there is a possibility that it is substantially different Low quality: Our confidence in the effect estimate is limited: The true effect may be substantially different from the estimate of the effect Very low quality: We have very little confidence in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be substantially different from the estimate of effect |
aDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of selection, attrition and reporting bias. bDowngraded by two levels due to serious imprecision: single small study with wide 95% CIs crossing the line of no effect. cDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of selection and reporting bias. dDowngraded by one level due to imprecision: few participants and wide 95% CIs.
Summary of findings 3. Alarm compared to other types of alarm for nocturnal enuresis in children.
Alarm versus other types of alarm | ||||||
Patient or population: children with nocturnal enuresis Setting: outpatient Intervention: one alarm Comparison: other types of alarm | ||||||
Outcomes | Anticipated absolute effects* (95% CI) | Relative effect (95% CI) | № of participants (studies) | Quality of the evidence (GRADE) | Comments | |
Risk with other type of alarm | Risk with one alarm | |||||
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ bell and pad versus body‐worn alarm | 700 per 1000 | 700 per 1000 (469 to 1000) | RR 1.00 (0.67 to 1.50) | 40 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b |
0 fewer childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with bell and pad alarm (from 231 fewer to 350 more) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) ‐ bell and pad alarm versus body alarm | 550 per 1000 | 501 per 1000 (275 to 902) | RR 0.91 (0.50 to 1.64) | 40 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b |
49 fewer childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing after stopping treatment with bell and pad alarm (from 275 fewer to 352 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ standard alarm versus intermittent alarm | 700 per 1000 | 742 per 1000 (574 to 952) | RR 1.06 (0.82 to 1.36) | 86 (3 RCTs) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWb, c, d |
42 more childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with standard alarm (from 126 fewer to 252 more) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) ‐ standard alarm vs intermittent alarm | 429 per 1000 | 300 per 1000 (159 to 579) | RR 0.70 (0.37 to 1.35) | 59 (2 RCTs) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWb,c |
129 fewer childrenper 1000 maintaining 14 dry nights post‐treatment with standard alarm (from 150 more to 270 fewer) |
Mean number of wet nights a week ‐ immediate alarm versus delayed alarm | The mean number of wet nights per week in the delayed alarm group was 4.19 | Participants in the immediate alarm group experienced 2.5 fewer wet nights a week (1.01 fewer to 3.99 fewer) compared with the delayed alarm group | ‐ | 36 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWe, f | We could not include an additional study into analyses due to missing standard deviations The size of difference in means reported was largely similar to those observed in analysed data |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ immediate alarm versus delayed alarm | 258 per 1000 | 485 per 1000 (250 to 937) | RR 1.88 (0.97 to 3.63) | 62 (2 RCTs) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWe, c |
227 more childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with delayed alarm (from 8 fewer to 679 more) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) ‐ immediate alarm versus delayed alarm | 154 per 1000 | 462 per 1000 (114 to 1000) | RR 3.00 (0.74 to 12.21) | 26 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b |
308 more childrenper 1000 maintaining 14 dry nights post‐treatment with immediate alarm (from 40 fewer to 51,000 more) |
Mean number of wet nights per week ‐ alarm versus twin signal | The mean number of wet nights per week in the twin signal group was 3.58 | Participants in the twin‐signal alarm group experienced 0.68 fewer wet nights per week (2.23 fewer to 0.87 more) compared with the alarm group | ‐ | 30 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b | ‐ |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm versus twin‐signal alarm | 897 per 1000 | 844 per 1000 (709 to 1000) | RR 0.94 (0.79 to 1.13) | 78 (3 RCTs) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWb, c, d |
54 fewer childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm (from 117 more to 188 fewer) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) ‐ alarm versus twin‐signal alarm | 583 per 1000 |
543 per 1000 (327 to 893) |
RR 0.93 (0.56 to 1.53) | 48 (2 RCTs) |
⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, e |
41 fewer childrenper 1000 maintaining 14 dry nights post‐treatment with alarm (from 257 fewer to 309 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ bell and pad versus electric shock alarms | 83 per 1000 | 83 per 1000 (6 to 1000) | RR 1.00 (0.79 to 1.27) | 24 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, e |
0 fewer childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with bell and pad alarm (from 132 more to 324 fewer) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) ‐ bell and pad alarm vs electric shock alarm | 500 per 1000 | 585 per 1000 (280 to 1000) | RR 1.17 (0.56 to 2.45) | 24 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, e |
85 more childrenper 1000 maintaining 14 dry nights post‐treatment with bell and pad alarm (from 220 fewer to 725 more) |
Proportion of children with adverse events ‐ bell and pad alarm vs electric shock alarm | In the electric shock alarm 3/12 children received corrosive skin burns and 2/12 children discontinued treatment due to fear of using the electric shock alarm. No adverse events data were reported for the bell and pad alarm group. | 36 (1 RCT) |
‐ | ‐ | ||
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ loud versus quiet alarms | 600 per 1000 | 798 per 1000 (534 to 1000) | RR 1.33 (0.89 to 1.99) | 40 (2 RCTs) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, e |
198 more childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with loud alarm (from 66 fewer to 594 more) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 dry consecutive nights post‐treatment) ‐ loud alarm vs quiet alarm | 500 per 1000 | 450 per 1000 (235 to 855) | RR 0.90 (0.47 to 1.71) | 40 (2 RCTs) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d, e |
50 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing after stopping treatment with loud alarm (from 265 fewer to 355 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ audio vs vibrating alarms | 833 per 1000 | 567 per 1000 (375 to 842) | RR 0.68 (0.45 to 1.01) | 47 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, e |
267 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with vibrating alarm (from 8 more to 458 fewer) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ code‐word alarm versus control alarm | 667 per 1000 |
740 per 1000 (647 to 847) |
RR 1.11 (0.97 to 1.27) |
353 (1 RCT) |
⊕⊕⊕⊝ MODERATEf |
73 more childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with control alarm (from 20 fewer to 180 more) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) ‐ code word alarm versus control alarm | 712 per 1000 |
648 per 1000 (562 to 747) |
RR 0.91 (0.79 to 1.05) | 353 (1 RCT) |
⊕⊕⊕⊝ MODERATEf |
64 fewer childrenper 1000 maintaining 14 dry nights post‐treatment with code‐word alarm (from 36 more to 149 fewer) |
Proportion of children with adverse events ‐ code‐word alarm versus control alarm | 102 per 1000 |
136 per 1000 (76 to 242) |
RR 1.34 (0.75 to 2.38) |
353 (1 RCT) |
⊕⊕⊕⊝ MODERATEf |
Another 4 studies reported information relating to adverse events. One study did not mention the alarm that caused the adverse effects. Discomfort wearing the alarm, displeasure when woken by the alarm, family disruption, and technical problems with the alarm were reported for all alarms |
*The risk in the intervention group (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI). CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio; | ||||||
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence High quality: We are very confident that the true effect lies close to that of the estimate of the effect Moderate quality: We are moderately confident in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be close to the estimate of the effect, but there is a possibility that it is substantially different Low quality: Our confidence in the effect estimate is limited: The true effect may be substantially different from the estimate of the effect Very low quality: We have very little confidence in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be substantially different from the estimate of effect |
aDowngraded by two levels due to very serious imprecision: single trial with small sample size. bDowngraded by two levels due to very serious risk of bias: selection bias and attrition bias. cDowngraded by one level due to serious imprecision: small sample sizes. dDowngraded by one level due to inconsistency: different directions of effect. eDowngraded by two levels due to very serious risk of bias: inadequate blinding and selective reporting. fDowngraded by one level due to serious imprecision: single trial.
Summary of findings 4. Alarm compared to other behavioural interventions for nocturnal enuresis in children.
Alarm compared to other behavioural interventions for nocturnal enuresis in children | ||||||
Patient or population: children with nocturnal enuresis in children Setting: outpatient Intervention: alarm Comparison: other behavioural interventions (waking, bladder training, dry‐bed training, and star chart with rewards) | ||||||
Outcomes | Anticipated absolute effects* (95% CI) | Relative effect (95% CI) | № of participants (studies) | Quality of the evidence (GRADE) | Comments | |
Risk with other behavioural interventions | Risk with alarm | |||||
Mean number of wet nights a week | The mean number of wet nights a week in the comparator groups ranged from 1.11 to 3.75 | Participants in the alarm group experienced 0.81 fewer wet nights a week (2.01 fewer to 0.38 more) compared with the behavioural interventions group. | ‐ | 95 (3 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWa, b | Behavioural interventions were waking, bladder training, and star chart with rewards |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 310 per 1000 | 548 per 1000 (303 to 987) | RR 1.77 (0.98 to 3.19) | 179 (6 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWa, b | Behavioural interventions were waking, bladder training, star chart with rewards, and dry‐bed training. 239 more childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm (from 6 fewer to 678 more) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) | 318 per 1000 | 442 per 1000 (258 to 767) | RR 1.39 (0.81 to 2.41) | 89 (3 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWb, c | Behavioural interventions were waking,dry‐bed training, and star chart with rewards 124 more childrenper 1000 maintaining 14 dry nights post‐treatment with alarm (from 60 fewer to 449 more) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | Not reported | |||||
*The risk in the intervention group (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI). CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio; | ||||||
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence High quality: We are very confident that the true effect lies close to that of the estimate of the effect Moderate quality: We are moderately confident in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be close to the estimate of the effect, but there is a possibility that it is substantially different Low quality: Our confidence in the effect estimate is limited: The true effect may be substantially different from the estimate of the effect Very low quality: We have very little confidence in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be substantially different from the estimate of effect |
aDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of allocation, attrition and reporting bias. bDowngraded by one level due to serious imprecision: few participants and wide 95% CIs crossing the line of no effect. cDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of allocation and attrition bias.
Summary of findings 5. Alarm compared to desmopressin for nocturnal enuresis in children.
Alarm compared to drugs for nocturnal enuresis in children | ||||||
Patient or population: children with nocturnal enuresis Setting: outpatient Intervention: alarm Comparison: desmopressin | ||||||
Outcomes | Anticipated absolute effects* (95% CI) | Relative effect (95% CI) | № of participants (studies) | Quality of the evidence (GRADE) | Comments | |
Risk with desmopressin | Risk with alarm | |||||
Mean number of wet nights a week | The mean number of wet nights a week in the desmopressin group was 1.76 to 2.68 | Participants in the alarm group experienced 0.64 fewer wet nights a week (1.77 fewer to 0.49 more) compared with the desmopressin group. | ‐ | 285 (4 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWa, b | ‐ |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 398 per 1000 | 446 per 1000 (370 to 541) | RR 1.12 (0.93 to 1.36) | 1168 (12 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWc, d | 48 more childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm (from 28 fewer to 143 more) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) | 254 per 1000 | 315 per 1000 (234 to 468) | RR 1.30 (0.92 to 1.84) | 565 (5 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊕⊝ MODERATEa | 76 more childrenper 1000 maintaining 14 dry nights post‐treatment with alarm (from 20 fewer to 214 more) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | 212 per 1000 | 81 per 1000 (42 to 150) |
RR 0.38 (0.20 to 0.71) |
565 (5 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊕⊝ MODERATEa | 131 fewer childrenper 1000 experiencing adverse events with alarm (from 169 fewer to 61 fewer) |
*The risk in the intervention group (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI). CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio; | ||||||
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence High quality: We are very confident that the true effect lies close to that of the estimate of the effect Moderate quality: We are moderately confident in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be close to the estimate of the effect, but there is a possibility that it is substantially different Low quality: Our confidence in the effect estimate is limited: The true effect may be substantially different from the estimate of the effect Very low quality: We have very little confidence in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be substantially different from the estimate of effect |
aDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of bias (reporting bias, attrition bias and industry funding). bDowngraded by one level due to imprecision: few participants and wide 95% CIs, indicating that the true effect may be appreciable benefit or harm. cDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of attrition bias and selective reporting. dDowngraded by one level due to asymmetrical funnel plot: smaller studies may have been conducted but not published.
Summary of findings 6. Alarm compared to tricyclics for nocturnal enuresis in children.
Alarm compared to drugs for nocturnal enuresis in children | ||||||
Patient or population: children with nocturnal enuresis Setting: outpatient Intervention: alarm Comparison: tricyclics | ||||||
Outcomes | Anticipated absolute effects* (95% CI) | Relative effect (95% CI) | № of participants (studies) | Quality of the evidence (GRADE) | Comments | |
Risk with tricyclics | Risk with alarm | |||||
Mean number of wet nights a week | The mean number of wet nights a week in the tricyclics ranged from 2.05 to 3.3 | Participants in the alarm group experienced 1.13 fewer wet nights per week (2.74 fewer to 0.48 more) compared with the tricyclics group. | ‐ | 29 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b | ‐ |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 204 per 1000 | 418 per 1000 (271 to 646) | RR 2.05 (1.33 to 3.17) | 208 (3 RCTs) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWb, c | 214 more childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm (from 67 more to 442 more) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) | 5/12 in the alarm group and 0/12 in the tricyclics group maintained complete response. | RR 11.00 (0.67 to 179.29) | 24 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b | ‐ | |
Proportion of children with adverse events | 1 trial collected information relating to adverse events but the data were not reported by intervention groups | |||||
*The risk in the intervention group (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI). CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio; | ||||||
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence High quality: We are very confident that the true effect lies close to that of the estimate of the effect Moderate quality: We are moderately confident in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be close to the estimate of the effect, but there is a possibility that it is substantially different Low quality: Our confidence in the effect estimate is limited: The true effect may be substantially different from the estimate of the effect Very low quality: We have very little confidence in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be substantially different from the estimate of effect |
aDowngraded by one level for risk of bias: unclear across all domains due to lack of information in the trial report. bDowngraded for imprecision: few participants and wide 95% CIs indicating the possibility of appreciable harm or benefit. cDowngraded by two levels due to very serious risk of bias: high risk of attrition and reporting bias and insufficient information about randomisation, allocation and blinding.
Summary of findings 7. Alarm compared to miscellaneous treatments for nocturnal enuresis in children.
Alarm versus miscellaneous treatments for nocturnal enuresis in children | ||||||
Patient or population: children with nocturnal enuresis Setting: outpatient Intervention: alarm Comparison: miscellaneous treatments | ||||||
Outcomes | Anticipated absolute effects* (95% CI) | Relative effect (95% CI) | № of participants (studies) | Quality of the evidence (GRADE) | Comments | |
Risk with miscellaneous treatment | Risk with alarm | |||||
Mean number of wet nights a week ‐ alarm versus cognitive behavioural therapy | The mean number of wet nights a week in the cognitive behavioural therapy group was 1.03 | Participants in the alarm group experienced 0.20 more wet nights per week (2.65 fewer to 3.05 more) | ‐ | 33 (1 RCT) |
⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b | An additional study, comparing alarms to shaming, could not be incorporated into analyses due to missing standard deviations. The mean number of wet nights was similar to the observed data |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm versus cognitive behavioural therapy | 750 per 1000 | 630 per 1000 (413 to 968) | RR 0.84 (0.55 to 1.29) | 39 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b | 120 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm (from 218 more to 337 fewer) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) ‐ alarm versus cognitive behavioural therapy | 833 per 1000 | 400per 1000 (208 to 767) | RR 0.48 (0.25 to 0.92) | 33 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b | ‐ |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm versus psychotherapy | 129 per 1000 | 457 per 1000 (177 to 1000) | RR 3.62 (1.38 to 9.50) | 116 (2 RCTs) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, c | 328 morechildren per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm (from 48 more to 1000 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm versus hypnotherapy | 146 per 1000 | 438per 1000 (206 to 932) | RR 3.00 (1.41 to 6.39) | 96 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWb, d | 292 morechildrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm, (from 60 more to 786 more) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) ‐ alarm versus hypnotherapy | 104 per 1000 | 313per 1000 (123 to 792) | RR 3.00 (1.18 to 7.60) | 96 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWb, d | ‐ |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm versus restricted diet | 13 per 1000 | 307per 1000 (43 to 1000) | RR 23.00 (3.19 to 165.98) | 150 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWd, e | 293 morechildren per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm (from 29 more 1000 more) |
Proportion of children adverse events | Not reported | |||||
*The risk in the intervention group (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI). CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio; | ||||||
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence High quality: We are very confident that the true effect lies close to that of the estimate of the effect Moderate quality: We are moderately confident in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be close to the estimate of the effect, but there is a possibility that it is substantially different Low quality: Our confidence in the effect estimate is limited: The true effect may be substantially different from the estimate of the effect Very low quality: We have very little confidence in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be substantially different from the estimate of effect |
aDowngraded by two levels due to very serious risk of bias: inadequate descriptions of randomisation and allocation concealment, attrition bias, selective reporting. bDowngraded by two levels due to very serious imprecision: single trial with small sample size. cDowngraded by two levels due to very serious imprecision: small sample sizes and unbalanced numbers of participants across groups. dDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of bias: attrition bias and selective reporting. eDowngraded by one level due to very serious imprecision: single trial with wide 95% confidence interval.
Summary of findings 8. Alarm plus other intervention versus other intervention for nocturnal enuresis in children.
Alarm plus other intervention versus other intervention for nocturnal enuresis in children | ||||||
Patient or population: children with nocturnal enuresis Setting: outpatient Intervention: alarm + other intervention Comparison: other intervention | ||||||
Outcomes | Anticipated absolute effects* (95% CI) | Relative effect (95% CI) | № of participants (studies) | Quality of the evidence (GRADE) | Comments | |
Risk with other intervention | Risk with alarm + other intervention | |||||
Mean number of wet nights a week ‐ alarm + bladder training versus bladder training | The mean number of wet nights a week in the bladder training group was 1.4 | Participants in the alarm + bladder training group experienced 2.35 fewer wet nights a week (4.2 fewer to 0.5 fewer) compared with the bladder training group. | ‐ | 22 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b | We could not include an additional study into analyses due to missing standard deviations. The size of difference in means reported was largely similar to those observed in analysed data |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + bladder training versus bladder training | 167 per 1000 | 500 per 1000 (122 to 1000) | RR 3.00 (0.73 to 12.27) | 22 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b | ‐ |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + dry‐bed training versus dry‐bed training | 250 per 1000 |
948 per 1000 (463 to 1000) |
RR 3.79 (1.85 to 7.77) | 80 (1 RCT) |
⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWb, c | ‐ |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) ‐ alarm + dry bed training versus dry bed training | 150 per 1000 |
84 per 1000 (23 to 318) |
RR 0.56 (0.15 to 2.12) | 80 (1 RCT) |
⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWb, c | ‐ |
Mean number of wet nights a week ‐ alarm + desmopressin versus desmopressin | The mean number of wet nights per week in the desmopressin groups ranged from 1.76 to 2.6 | Participants in the alarm + desmopressin group experienced 0.88 fewer wet nights a week (0.38 fewer to 1.38 fewer) compared with the desmopressin group | ‐ | 156 (2 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWd, e | ‐ |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + desmopressin versus desmopressin | 421 per 1000 | 555 per 1000 (454 to 682) | RR 1.32 (1.08 to 1.62) | 359 (5 RCTs) |
⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWe, f | 135 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + desmopressin (34 more to 261 more) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) ‐ alarm + desmopressin versus desmopressin | 143 per 1000 | 333 per 1000 (180 to 613) | RR 2.33 (1.26 to 4.29) | 161 (2 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWd, e | 190 more childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing after stopping treatment with alarm + desmopressin (37 more to 470 more) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | In 1 trial of alarm + desmopressin versus desmopressin 1 child in each group experienced adverse events. In another trial of alarm + desmopressin versus desmopressin no child experienced adverse events. | |||||
*The risk in the intervention group (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI). CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio; | ||||||
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence High quality: We are very confident that the true effect lies close to that of the estimate of the effect Moderate quality: We are moderately confident in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be close to the estimate of the effect, but there is a possibility that it is substantially different Low quality: Our confidence in the effect estimate is limited: The true effect may be substantially different from the estimate of the effect Very low quality: We have very little confidence in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be substantially different from the estimate of effect |
aDowngraded by two levels due to very serious risk of bias: inadequate descriptions of randomisation and allocation concealment, attrition bias, selective reporting. bDowngraded by one level due to serious imprecision: single small trial with wide 95% CIs. cDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of bias: selective reporting. dDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of bias: attrition bias and drug company funding. eDowngraded by one level due to serious imprecision: few participants. fDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of bias: attrition bias and selective reporting.
We have described effects of interventions under seven comparisons: alarm versus no treatment or control; alarm versus placebo drug; alarm versus other types of alarm; alarm versus other behavioural interventions; alarm versus drugs; alarm versus miscellaneous interventions; and alarm combined with another intervention versus the other intervention.
Alarm versus control or no treatment
Eighteen trials compared an enuresis alarm with no treatment or wait list (Baker 1969; Bennett 1985; Bollard 1981a; Bollard 1981b; Hojsgaard 1979; Houts 1986; Jehu 1977; Lynch 1984; Moffatt 1987; Nawaz 2002; Ronen 1995; Sacks 1974; Shakiba 2001; Sloop 1973; Wagner 1982; Wagner 1985; Werry 1965; Wright 1974); one with a control (non‐functioning) alarm (Elinder 1985); and one with a control alarm that rang in the parents' room 20 minutes later (Finley 1973).
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
Alarm training may reduce the number of wet nights a week compared to no treatment or control (MD −2.68, 95% CI −4.59 to −0.78; 4 studies, 127 children; Analysis 1.1, Table 1; low‐quality evidence).
1.1. Analysis.
Comparison 1: Alarm versus no treatment or control, Outcome 1: Mean number of wet nights a week
There may be a difference between a standard alarm compared to no treatment or control, and a delayed alarm compared to no treatment or control. The mean number of wet nights with standard alarm may be lower than with no treatment or control (MD −3.42, 95% CI −4.98 to −1.87; 4 studies, 100 children; Analysis 1.1.1). We are uncertain whether there is a difference in the mean wet nights between an alarm with a three‐minute delayed trigger compared to no treatment (MD 0.13, 95% CI −1.37 to 1.63; 1 study, 27 children; Analysis 1.1.2).
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
Alarms may substantially increase the proportion of children who achieve a complete response compared to no treatment or control. In 18 studies, 65% using standard alarm training achieved a complete response compared to 13% with no treatment (RR 7.23, 95% CI 1.40 to 37.33; 18 trials, 827 children; Analysis 1.2, Table 1; low‐quality evidence). We downgraded the quality of evidence to low due to risks of bias arising from selective reporting of outcomes, poor reporting of allocation processes across the studies and possible publication bias (see Figure 4).
1.2. Analysis.
Comparison 1: Alarm versus no treatment or control, Outcome 2: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights
4.
Funnel plot of comparison: 1 Alarm versus no treatment or control, outcome: 1.2 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights.
There may be a difference between a standard alarm compared to no treatment or control, and other types of alarm compared to no treatment or control. Standard alarms may substantially increase the proportion of children who achieve a complete response compared to no treatment or control (RR 8.63, 95% CI 5.23 to 14.26; 16 trials, 650 children; Analysis 1.2.1).
We are uncertain whether more children will achieve a complete response using the following different types of alarms compared to no treatment or control:
Delayed alarm (RR 2.98, 95% CI 0.60 to 14.93; 2 studies, 47 children; Analysis 1.2.2).
Intermittent alarm (RR 9.27, 95% CI 0.64 to 134.31; 1 study, 15 children; Finley 1973; Analysis 1.2.3).
Electrical shock alarm (RR 1.00, 95% CI 0.92 to 1.10; 2 studies, 115 children; Analysis 1.2.4).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Alarms may substantially increase the proportion of children who maintain a complete response after stopping treatment compared to no treatment or control. Forty‐three per cent of children maintained 14 consecutive dry nights compared with 2% in the control group (RR 9.67, 95% CI 4.74 to 19.76; 10 studies, 366 children; Analysis 1.3, Table 1, low‐quality evidence). The funnel plot did not suggest evidence of publication bias (see Figure 5), so the effect estimates reported in these studies may be considered reliable. However, we downgraded the quality of the evidence to low due to imprecision and risk of bias related to selection reporting and allocation processes.
1.3. Analysis.
Comparison 1: Alarm versus no treatment or control, Outcome 3: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment
5.
Funnel plot of comparison: 1 Alarm versus no treatment or control, outcome: 1.3 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment.
In single small trials that compared non‐standard alarm training to control, we are uncertain if there is a difference between intermittent alarms and alarms that sounded in the parents' room 20 minutes later (RR 8.18, 95% CI 0.56 to 119.85; 1 trial, 15 children; Finley 1973; Analysis 1.3.2). We are also uncertain if there is a difference between the delayed alarm and no treatment (Wagner 1985; Analysis 1.3.3).
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Alarms versus placebo drug
Five trials compared alarms to placebo drugs (Fournier 1987; Kolvin 1972; Longstaffe 2000; Turner 1970; Wright 1974).
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment between standard bell and pad alarms and placebo drugs (MD −0.96, 95% CI −2.32 to 0.41; 1 study, 47 children; Turner 1970; Analysis 2.2, Table 2; very low‐quality evidence).
2.2. Analysis.
Comparison 2: Alarm versus placebo drug, Outcome 2: Mean number of wet nights a week
Three trials reported the mean number of wet nights at the end of treatment without including standard deviations (Fournier 1987; Kolvin 1972; Wright 1974; Table 10).
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
Alarms may lead to slightly more children achieving a complete response compared to placebo drug (RR 1.59, 95% CI 1.16 to 2.17; 2 studies, 181 children; Analysis 2.3; Table 2; Longstaffe 2000; Turner 1970; low‐quality evidence).
2.3. Analysis.
Comparison 2: Alarm versus placebo drug, Outcome 3: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights: subgroup analysis
The test for subgroup differences indicated little difference in treatment effect between children with monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis (MNE) compared with non‐monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis (NMNE) (Chi2 = 0.14, df = 1 (P = 0.71), I2 = 0%; Analysis 2.3).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Not reported.
Proportion of children with adverse events
One trial, comparing alarms to placebo drugs, collected information relating to adverse events but the data were not specifically reported by intervention groups (Fournier 1987). Another trial comparing alarms to placebo drugs reported adverse events including non‐compliance, failure or family disruption, which caused high attrition, but did not provide any useable data (Turner 1970).
Alarm versus other types of alarm
Thirteen studies compared different types of alarms (Bollard 1981a; Butler 1990a; Caldwell 2015; Finley 1973; Finley 1977; Lovibond 1964a; Lovibond 1964b; Lovibond 1964c; Lynch 1984; Taylor 1975; Tobias 2001; Turner 1970; Wagner 1985). The alarms studied included: bell and pad alarms (Butler 1990a; Lovibond 1964a; Lovibond 1964b; Lovibond 1964c; Tobias 2001); body‐worn alarms (Butler 1990a); electric shock alarms (Lovibond 1964a); alarms of varying loudness (Finley 1977); and vibration alarms (Tobias 2001). Comparisons were also made about the timing of the alarm trigger (Finley 1973; Lovibond 1964a; Lovibond 1964c; Lynch 1984; Taylor 1975; Turner 1970; Wagner 1985).
Bell and pad versus body‐worn alarms
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
In one trial comparing bell and pad alarms to body‐worn alarms, the mean number of wet nights at the end of treatment was reported without standard deviations (Butler 1990a; Table 10).
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at the end of treatment between bell and pad alarms and body‐worn alarms (RR 1.00, 95% CI 0.67 to 1.50; 1 study, 40 children; Butler 1990a; Analysis 3.2; very low‐quality evidence; Table 3).
3.2. Analysis.
Comparison 3: Alarm versus other types of alarm, Outcome 2: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights at the end of treatment
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment between those using the bell and pad alarms and body‐worn alarms (RR 0.91, 95% CI 0.50 to 1.64; 1 study, 40 children; Butler 1990a; Analysis 3.3; very low‐quality evidence; Table 3).
3.3. Analysis.
Comparison 3: Alarm versus other types of alarm, Outcome 3: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Continuous versus intermittent alarms
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
The mean number of wet nights at the end of treatment was reported in one study without standard deviations (Finley 1973; Table 10).
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at the end of treatment between those using an alarm that triggered continuously after wetting and an alarm that triggered intermittently (RR 1.06, 95% CI 0.82 to 1.36; 3 studies, 86 children; Finley 1973; Taylor 1975; Turner 1970; Analysis 3.2 very low‐quality evidence; Table 3).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children remaining dry at follow‐up after stopping treatment between those using an alarm that triggered continuously after wetting and an alarm that triggered intermittently (RR 0.70, 95% CI 0.37 to 1.35; 2 studies, 59 children; Finley 1973; Taylor 1975; Analysis 3.3 very low‐quality evidence; Table 3).
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Immediate versus delayed alarms
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether those using the immediate alarm had fewer wet nights at the end of treatment compared with the delayed alarm, which triggered after a three‐minute delay, as the quality of evidence was assessed as very low (MD −2.50, 95% CI −1.01 to −3.99; 1 study, 36 children; Lynch 1984; Analysis 3.1Table 3).
3.1. Analysis.
Comparison 3: Alarm versus other types of alarm, Outcome 1: Mean number of wet nights a week
Another study reported the mean number of wet nights at the end of treatment without providing standard deviations (Wagner 1985; Table 10).
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
Two small trials compared alarms with an immediate versus a delayed trigger after wetting (Lynch 1984; Wagner 1985). We are uncertain whether more children achieved a complete response at the end of treatment using an immediate alarm, as the quality of the evidence is very low (RR 1.88, 95% CI 0.97 to 3.63; 2 studies, 62 children; I2 = 74%; Analysis 3.2).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children remaining dry after stopping treatment between immediate and delayed alarms (RR 3.00, 95% CI 0.74 to 12.21; 1 study, 26 children; Wagner 1985; Analysis 3.3; very low‐quality evidence; Table 3).
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Single versus twin‐signal alarms
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the mean number of wet nights at the end of treatment between single and twin‐signal alarms (MD −0.68, 95% CI −2.23 to 0.87; 1 study, 30 children; Turner 1970; Analysis 3.1; very low‐quality evidence; Table 3).
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights between those using a single signal alarm and a twin‐signal alarm (RR 0.94, 95% CI 0.79 to 1.13; 3 studies, 78 children; I2 = 0%; Lovibond 1964a; Lovibond 1964c; Turner 1970; Analysis 3.2 very low‐quality evidence; Table 3).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children remaining dry at follow‐up after stopping treatment between a single‐signal alarm and a twin‐signal alarm (RR 0.93, 95% CI 0.56 to 1.53; 2 studies, 48 children; I2 = 0%; Lovibond 1964a; Lovibond 1964c; Analysis 3.3 very‐low quality evidence; Table 3).
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Bell and pad versus electric shock alarms
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at the end of treatment between those using a standard bell and pad alarm that triggered an audio stimulus and an alarm that delivered an electric shock (RR 1.00, 95% CI 0.79 to 1.27; 1 study, 24 children; Lovibond 1964a; Analysis 3.2 very low‐quality evidence; Table 3).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children remaining dry at follow‐up after stopping treatment between those using a standard bell and pad alarm that triggered an audio stimulus and an alarm that delivered an electric shock (RR 1.17, 95% CI 0.56 to 2.45; 1 study, 24 children; Lovibond 1964a; Analysis 3.3 very low‐quality evidence; Table 3).
Proportion of children with adverse events
In one trial comparing a standard bell and pad alarm that triggered an audio stimulus with an alarm that delivered an electric shock, three children received corrosive skin burns and two children discontinued treatment due to fear of using the electric shock alarm (Lovibond 1964a).
Loud versus quiet alarms
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at the end of treatment between those using loud and quiet alarms (RR 1.33, 95% CI 0.89 to 1.99; 2 studies, 40 children; I2 = 15%; Finley 1973; Finley 1977; Analysis 3.2; very low‐quality evidence; Table 3).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children remaining dry after stopping treatment between those using loud and quiet alarms (RR 0.90, 95% CI 0.47 to 1.71; 2 studies, 40 children; Finley 1973; Finley 1977; Analysis 3.3; very low‐quality evidence; Table 3).
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Audio versus vibration alarms
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at the end of treatment between body‐worn audio alarms and vibration alarms, as the quality of the evidence is very low (RR 0.68, 95% CI 0.45 to 1.01; 1 study, 47 children; Tobias 2001; Analysis 3.2Table 3).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Not reported.
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Code word versus standard alarm
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
Moderate‐quality evidence from one trial comparing a code‐word alarm to a commercially‐available wireless alarm suggested the code‐word alarm probably slightly increases the number of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights (RR 1.11, 95% CI 0.97 to 1.27; 1 study, 353 children; Caldwell 2015; Analysis 3.2, Table 3). This means that, probably, 7.3% more children achieve 14 consecutive dry nights with code‐word alarm than with a commercially‐available wireless alarm (2% fewer to 18% more).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Moderate‐quality evidence from one trial comparing a code‐word alarm to a commercially‐available wireless alarm suggested that there is probably no difference in the proportion of children remaining dry after stopping treatment (RR 0.91, 95% CI 0.79 to 1.05; 1 study, 353 children; Caldwell 2015; Analysis 3.3, Table 3).
Proportion of children with adverse events
Moderate‐quality evidence from one trial comparing a code‐word alarm to a commercially‐available wireless alarm suggested that there is probably no difference in the proportion of children experiencing an adverse event (24/176 and 18/177, respectively) (RR 1.34, 95% CI 0.75 to 2.38; 1 study, 353 children; Caldwell 2015; Table 3). The number of children experiencing sleep disturbance in the code‐word group was 14/176 compared to 17/177 in the control group. The number of children frightened by the alarm was 2/176 in the code‐word group compared to 4/177 in the control group, while children experiencing nightmares or sleepwalking triggered by the alarm was 1/176 in the code‐word group compared to 1/177 in the control group.
Alarm versus other behavioural interventions
Ten studies compared alarm training with other behavioural interventions; dry‐bed training (Azrin 1978; Bollard 1981b; Caceres 1982), waking (Baker 1969; Fournier 1987; Lovibond 1964b; Turner 1970), bladder training (Bennett 1985; Bryant 2003), and star chart with rewards (Ronen 1995).
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week by the end of treatment
Alarms may have similar numbers or reduce the number of wet night nights a week compared with behavioural interventions (MD ‐0.81, 95% CI ‐2.01 to 0.38; 3 trials, 95 children; Analysis 4.1; Table 4; low‐quality evidence). The behavioural interventions investigated in the trials were waking to void (Turner 1970), bladder training (Bennett 1985) and star chart plus rewards (Ronen 1995).
4.1. Analysis.
Comparison 4: Alarm versus other behavioural interventions, Outcome 1: Mean number of wet nights a week
We are uncertain if there are differences between standard alarm compared to waking to void (MD −0.33, 95% CI −2.26 to 1.60; 1 study, 22 children; Analysis 4.1.1) and twin‐signal alarm compared to waking to void (MD 0.35, 95% CI −1.44 to 2.14; 1 study, 23 children; Analysis 4.1.2).
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
Alarms may have similar or increased numbers of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment compared with behavioural interventions (RR 1.77, 95% CI 0.98 to 3.19; 6 trials; n = 179; Analysis 4.2; Table 4; low‐quality evidence). Alarms may lead to 23.8% more children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights compared with behavioural interventions (0.6% fewer to 67.8% more).
4.2. Analysis.
Comparison 4: Alarm versus other behavioural interventions, Outcome 2: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
One small trial found that there were fewer wet nights a week post‐treatment with alarms than with bladder training (MD −2.60, 95% CI −4.53 to −0.67; 1 study, 21 children; Bennett 1985; Analysis 4.3). The other trials investigating alarms compared to behavioural interventions did not report this outcome.
4.3. Analysis.
Comparison 4: Alarm versus other behavioural interventions, Outcome 3: Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Alarms may slightly increase the number of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment compared with behavioural interventions (RR 1.39, 0.81 to 2.41; 3 studies, 89 children; Analysis 4.4; low‐quality evidence; Table 4). However, the confidence intervals are wide enough to include potentially important harms as well as benefits. For example, alarms may lead to 12.4% more children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment compared with behavioural interventions (6% fewer to 44.9% more).
4.4. Analysis.
Comparison 4: Alarm versus other behavioural interventions, Outcome 4: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Alarm versus drugs
Twenty‐two trials compared alarm training to an active drug treatment. Active drug treatments included:
desmopressin (13 trials: Ahmed 2013; Bak 2007; Evans 2011; Fagundes 2017; Faraj 1999; Kwak 2010#; Longstaffe 2000; Ng 2005; Onol 2015; Tuncel 2014; Tuygun 2007; Vogt 2010; Wille 1986);
tricyclics (nine trials, comprising imipramine in seven trials (Fournier 1987; Hoseinzadeh 1997; Kolvin 1972; McKendry 1975; Motavalli 1994; Netley 1984; Wagner 1982), clomipramine in one trial (Motavalli 1994) and amitriptyline in one trial (Danquah 1975));
a combined amphetamine, ephedrine and atropine sulphate preparation in one trial (Wright 1974).
Alarms versus desmopressin
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
We are uncertain if alarms have any effect on the mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment compared with desmopressin because the quality of evidence is low and the 95% CI spans possible benefits and harms (MD −0.64, 95% CI −1.77 to 0.49; 4 studies, 285 children; Ahmed 2013; Ng 2005; Tuygun 2007; Wille 1986; Analysis 5.1; Table 5). Another trial reported the mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment but standard deviations were not given (Evans 2011; Table 10).
5.1. Analysis.
Comparison 5: Alarm versus desmopressin, Outcome 1: Mean number of wet nights per week
The test for subgroup differences indicates that the effectiveness of alarms compared to desmopressin may be different in children with MNE compared with NMNE (Chi2 = 5.35, df = 1 (P = 0.02), I2 = 81.3%; Analysis 5.1). However, the overlapping 95% CIs of the two subgroup effect estimates and the very low number of children with MNE means we cannot be certain about the extent of this difference between subgroups.
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
We are uncertain if alarms have any effect on the number of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at the end of treatment compared with desmopressin because the quality of evidence is low and the 95% CI spans possible benefits and harms (RR 1.12, 95% CI 0.93 to 1.36; 12 studies, 1168 children; Ahmed 2013; Bak 2007; Evans 2011; Fagundes 2017; Faraj 1999; Kwak 2010#; Longstaffe 2000; Ng 2005; Onol 2015; Tuncel 2014; Vogt 2010; Wille 1986; Analysis 5.3; Table 5; low‐quality evidence). Alarms may lead to a 12% less children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at the end of treatment compared to desmopressin (7% more to 36% less).
5.3. Analysis.
Comparison 5: Alarm versus desmopressin, Outcome 3: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights
The asymmetrical funnel plot suggests evidence of publication bias (Figure 6). It may therefore be that smaller trials investigating alarm therapy and desmopressin have not been published, so that we cannot be certain that the effect estimate presented here is reliable.
6.
Funnel plot of comparison: 5 Alarm versus desmopressin, outcome: 5.3 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights.
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Alarm therapy may reduce the mean number of wet nights a week after stopping treatment compared with desmopressin (MD −1.47, 95% CI −1.90 to −1.04; 3 studies, 195 children; Ahmed 2013; Ng 2005; Wille 1986; Analysis 5.2). Three trials reported the mean number of wet nights a week after stopping treatment without standard deviations (Table 11).
5.2. Analysis.
Comparison 5: Alarm versus desmopressin, Outcome 2: Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Alarm therapy probably slightly increases the number of children remaining dry after stopping treatment compared with desmopressin (RR 1.30, 95% CI 0.92 to 1.84; Ahmed 2013; Evans 2011; Kwak 2010#; Ng 2005; Wille 1986; 565 children; Analysis 5.4; moderate‐quality evidence; Table 5). Alarms probably lead to 7.6% more children remaining dry after stopping treatment compared with desmopressin (2% fewer to 21.4% more).
5.4. Analysis.
Comparison 5: Alarm versus desmopressin, Outcome 4: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment
Proportion of children with adverse events
Compared to desmopressin, alarm therapy probably reduces the number of children experiencing adverse events (RR 0.38, 95% CI 0.20 to 0.71; Ahmed 2013; Evans 2011; Kwak 2010#; Ng 2005; Wille 1986; 565 children; Analysis 5.5; Table 5; moderate‐quality evidence). Alarms probably lead to 13.1% fewer children experiencing adverse events compared with desmopressin (6.1% fewer to 16.9% fewer).
5.5. Analysis.
Comparison 5: Alarm versus desmopressin, Outcome 5: Numbers with adverse events
Alarms versus tricyclics (imipramine, clomipramine, amitriptyline)
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
We are uncertain if there is any difference in the mean number of wet nights at the end of treatment between those using alarm training and tricyclics (MD −1.13, 95% CI −2.74 to 0.48; 1 study, 29 children; Motavalli 1994; Analysis 6.1; Table 6; very low‐quality evidence). The mean number of wet nights a week was reported without standard deviations at the end of treatment in five trials (Danquah 1975; Fournier 1987; Kolvin 1972; Wagner 1982; Wright 1974; see Table 11).
6.1. Analysis.
Comparison 6: Alarm versus tricyclics, Outcome 1: Mean number of wet nights a week
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
Due to very low‐quality evidence, we cannot be certain that the proportion of children achieving a complete response was higher when using alarms than with imipramine (RR 2.05, 95% CI 1.33 to 3.17; 3 studies, 208 children; McKendry 1975; Netley 1984; Wagner 1982; Analysis 6.2; Table 6).
6.2. Analysis.
Comparison 6: Alarm versus tricyclics, Outcome 2: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
The mean number of wet nights a week after stopping treatment was reported in two trials without standard deviations (Danquah 1975; Kolvin 1972; Table 11).
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
We are uncertain whether more children maintained 14 dry nights after stopping treatment with alarm therapy compared with imipramine, as the quality of the evidence was rated as being very low (RR 11.00, 95% CI 0.67 to 179.29; 1 study, 24 children; Wagner 1982; Analysis 6.3; Table 6).
6.3. Analysis.
Comparison 6: Alarm versus tricyclics, Outcome 3: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment
Proportion of children with adverse events
One trial, comparing alarms to imipramine, collected information relating to adverse events but the data were not reported by intervention group (Fournier 1987).
Alarms versus drugs other than desmopressin and tricyclics
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
One small trial compared alarm training with a drug preparation (amphetamine, ephedrine and atropine sulphate), reporting the mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment without providing standard deviations (Wright 1974; Table 10).
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
Not reported.
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Not reported.
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Alarm versus miscellaneous interventions
Six studies compared alarms with other treatments not classified elsewhere, including: cognitive behavioural therapy (Ronen 1995); psychotherapy (Sacks 1974; Werry 1965); hypnotherapy (Seabrook 2005); shaming (Danquah 1975); and restricted diet (McKendry 1975).
Alarms versus cognitive behavioural therapy
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether there is a difference in the mean number of wet nights a week between alarm training and cognitive behavioural therapy, as the quality of the evidence is rated as being very low (MD 0.20, 95% CI −2.65 to 3.05; 1 study, 33 children; Ronen 1995; Table 7).
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether there is any difference in the proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights between alarm training and cognitive behavioural therapy, as the quality of the evidence is rated as being very low (RR 0.84, 95% CI 0.55 to 1.29; 1 study, 39 children; Ronen 1995; Analysis 7.2; Table 7).
7.2. Analysis.
Comparison 7: Alarm versus miscellaneous treatments, Outcome 2: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
We are uncertain whether the proportion of children maintaining 14 dry nights after stopping treatment was higher for those receiving cognitive behavioural therapy compared with alarm training, as the quality of the evidence is rated as being very low (RR 0.48, 95% CI 0.25 to 0.92; 1 study, 33 children; Ronen 1995; Analysis 7.3; Table 7).
7.3. Analysis.
Comparison 7: Alarm versus miscellaneous treatments, Outcome 3: Number achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Alarms versus psychotherapy
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
Because of the very low‐quality evidence, we cannot be certain whether more children achieve a complete response at the end of treatment with alarm training compared with psychotherapy (RR 3.62, 95% CI 1.38 to 9.50; 2 studies, 116 children; Sacks 1974; Werry 1965; Analysis 7.2, Table 7).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Not reported.
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Alarms versus hypnotherapy
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether more children achieve a complete response at the end of treatment with alarm training compared with hypnotherapy, as the quality of the evidence is rated as being very low (RR 3.00, 95% CI 1.41 to 6.39; 1 study, 96 children; Seabrook 2005; Analysis 7.2, Table 7).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
We are uncertain whether more children maintained 14 dry nights after stopping treatment with alarm training than after stopping hypnotherapy, as the quality of the evidence is rated as being very low (RR 3.00, 95% CI 1.18 to 7.60; 1 study, 96 children; Seabrook 2005; Analysis 7.3; Table 7).
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Alarms versus restricted diet
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
We are uncertain if more children achieved 14 consecutive dry night at the end of treatment with alarm training compared with a restricted diet, as the quality of the evidence is rated as being very low (RR 23.00, 95% CI 3.19 to 165.98; 1 study, 150 children; McKendry 1975; Analysis 7.2, Table 7).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Not reported.
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Alarms versus shaming
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
The mean number of wet nights at the end of treatment and at follow‐up after stopping treatment was reported for alarm training compared with shaming without providing standard deviations (Danquah 1975; Table 10).
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
Not reported.
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
The mean number of wet nights a week after stopping treatment was reported without standard deviations (Table 11).
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Not reported.
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Alarms combined with another intervention versus other interventions
Alarms plus bladder training versus bladder training alone
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether the addition of alarm training to bladder training reduces the mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment, as the quality of the evidence is rated as being very low (MD −2.35, 95% CI −4.20 to −0.50; 1 study, 22 children; Bennett 1985; Analysis 8.1, Table 8). One study comparing alarm plus bladder training to bladder training alone reported the mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment without standard deviations (Bryant 2003; Table 10).
8.1. Analysis.
Comparison 8: Alarm + other intervention versus other intervention, Outcome 1: Mean number of wet nights a week
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
We are uncertain whether the addition of alarm training to bladder training makes any difference in the proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at the end of treatment as the quality of the evidence is rated as very low (RR 3.00, 95% CI 0.73 to 12.27; 1 study, 22 children; Bennett 1985; Analysis 8.3, Table 8).
8.3. Analysis.
Comparison 8: Alarm + other intervention versus other intervention, Outcome 3: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
We are uncertain whether the addition of alarm training to bladder training makes any difference to the mean number of wet nights a week after stopping treatment (MD −1.10, 95% CI −3.42 to 1.22; 1 study, 22 children; Bennett 1985; Analysis 8.5).
8.5. Analysis.
Comparison 8: Alarm + other intervention versus other intervention, Outcome 5: Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Not reported.
Proportion of children with adverse effects
Not reported.
Alarms plus dry‐bed training versus dry‐bed training alone
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
Adding alarm training to dry‐bed training may increase the proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at the end of treatment (RR 3.79, 95% CI 1.85 to 7.77; 1 study, 80 children; low‐quality evidence; Bollard 1981b; Analysis 8.3, Table 8).
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
We are uncertain whether adding alarm training to dry‐bed training increases the proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment because they quality of evidence is low and the confidence interval is consistent with both appreciable harm and appreciable benefit (RR 0.56, 95% CI 0.15 to 2.12; 1 study, 80 children; Bollard 1981b; Analysis 8.6; low‐quality evidence; Table 8).
8.6. Analysis.
Comparison 8: Alarm + other intervention versus other intervention, Outcome 6: Number achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Alarms plus desmopressin versus desmopressin alone
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
Low‐quality evidence suggests adding alarm training to desmopressin may reduce the mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment (MD −0.88, 95% CI −1.38 to −0.38; 2 studies, 156 children; Ahmed 2013; Ng 2005; Analysis 8.1, Table 8).
The test for subgroup differences indicates little difference in treatment effect between children with MNE and children with NMNE (Chi2= 0.79, df = 1 (P = 0.37), I2 = 0%; Analysis 8.2).
8.2. Analysis.
Comparison 8: Alarm + other intervention versus other intervention, Outcome 2: Mean number of wet nights a week: alarm + desmopressin v desmopressin subgroup analysis
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
The addition of alarm training to desmopressin may increase the proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights a week at the end of treatment (RR 1.32, 95% CI 1.08 to 1.62; 5 studies, 359 children; Ahmed 2013; Bak 2007; Fagundes 2017; Ma 2007; Ng 2005; Analysis 8.3; low‐quality evidence; Table 8).
The test for subgroup differences indicates little difference in treatment between children with MNE and children with NMNE (Chi2 = 0.07, df = 1 (P = 0.79), I2 = 0%; Analysis 8.4).
8.4. Analysis.
Comparison 8: Alarm + other intervention versus other intervention, Outcome 4: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights: alarm + desmopressin v desmopressin subgroup analysis
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
The addition of alarm training to desmopressin seems to reduce the mean number of wet nights a week after stopping treatment (MD −1.40, 95% CI −1.86 to −0.94; 2 studies, 149 children; Ahmed 2013; Ng 2005; Analysis 8.5).
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Low‐quality evidence suggests adding alarm training to desmopressin may increase the proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment compared with desmopressin alone (RR 2.33, 95% CI 1.26 to 4.29; 2 studies, 161 children; Ahmed 2013; Ng 2005; Analysis 8.6, Table 8).
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Alarms plus imipramine versus imipramine alone
Primary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment
One study comparing alarm plus imipramine to imipramine alone reported the mean number of wet nights a week at the of treatment without standard deviations (Fournier 1987; Table 10).
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment
Not reported.
Secondary outcomes
Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up after stopping treatment
Not reported.
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment)
Not reported.
Proportion of children with adverse events
Not reported.
Alarms combined with another intervention versus alarm monotherapy or other treatment combinations
Twenty‐one studies compared alarms combined with another intervention versus alarm monotherapy (Bak 2007; Bennett 1985; Bollard 1981b; Bradbury 1995; Bryant 2003; Fielding 1980; Fournier 1987; Geffken 1986a; Geffken 1986b; Gibb 2004; Houts 1986; Kennedy 1968; Leebeek 2001; Nawaz 2002; Naitoh 2005; Ng 2005; Ozden 2008; Rodriguez 2001; Taylor 1975; Van Londen 1993; Young 1972). Since this type of comparison does not directly address the question of the effectiveness of alarm therapy itself, but rather investigates the effects of combining interventions together, we have not included the data from these trials in the main analysis (see Analysis 9.1; Analysis 9.2; Analysis 9.3; Analysis 9.4 for more details).
9.1. Analysis.
Comparison 9: Alarm + other intervention versus alarm monotherapy, Outcome 1: Mean number of wet nights a week
9.2. Analysis.
Comparison 9: Alarm + other intervention versus alarm monotherapy, Outcome 2: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights
9.3. Analysis.
Comparison 9: Alarm + other intervention versus alarm monotherapy, Outcome 3: Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up
9.4. Analysis.
Comparison 9: Alarm + other intervention versus alarm monotherapy, Outcome 4: Number achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment
An assessment of the quality of evidence from the trials investigating alarm therapy combined with another intervention versus alarm monotherapy shows that there is very little certainty around the effectiveness of these combinations of interventions. We have included our GRADE assessment in Appendix 1.
Ten studies compared alarms combined with another intervention versus a variety of other treatment combinations (Bennett 1985; Bollard 1981b; Fournier 1987; Houts 1986; Ma 2007; Naitoh 2005; Nawaz 2002; Scholander 1968; Titawee 2000; Van Londen 1993). As it is difficult to infer anything about the effectiveness of alarms from these comparisons, we have not attempted to meta‐analyse the data. However, we have presented the outcomes relevant to this review in Appendix 2.
Previous versions of this review collated individual trial results in the Characteristics of included studies. For brevity, we now report these in Appendix 4.
Discussion
This review includes 74 studies, 19 of which are new for this version. In order to assess the additive effect of alarm therapy to another intervention, this version also contains an analysis of trials that compare alarm training combined with another intervention (such as other behavioural therapy or drugs) to the other intervention alone. In addition, it provides details about risks of bias for each study and evidence tables for trials that compare alarm training with important treatments of current clinical relevance.
Summary of main results
Mean number of wet nights at the end of treatment
Compared to control or no treatment, and to behavioural interventions, alarm training may lead to a decrease in the number of wet nights at the end of treatment (Table 1; Table 4).
Compared to desmopressin, we are not certain if alarm training has any effect on the number of wet nights a week because the quality of evidence is low and the wide confidence interval is consistent with appreciable benefit as well as appreciable harm (Table 5).
The very low‐quality evidence we identified means that we cannot be certain about many of the conclusions presented here. Based on the quality of evidence, we cannot be certain whether at the end of treatment standard alarm training is better than placebo drugs (Table 2), delayed or twin signal alarms (Table 3), tricyclics (Table 6), or cognitive behavioural therapy (Table 7).
Similarly, we are uncertain whether the mean number of wet nights a week is lower for alarm plus bladder training compared with bladder training alone (Table 8). However, adding alarm training to desmopressin may reduce the number of wet nights a week compared with desmopressin monotherapy (Table 8).
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at the end of treatment
The proportion of children achieving a complete response at the end of treatment may be increased with standard alarm training compared to control or no treatment, or to placebo drugs (Table 1; Table 2).
Moderate‐quality evidence suggests alarm training with the code‐word alarm probably slightly increases the number of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights compared with the control alarm (Table 3).
Alarms may increase the number of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights compared with behavioural interventions (Table 4).
We are not certain if alarms have any effect on the number of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights compared with desmopressin because the quality of evidence is low and the wide confidence interval is consistent with appreciable benefit as well as appreciable harm (Table 5).
Furthermore, we are uncertain whether there is any difference in complete response rates when various other types of alarms were compared with each other (Table 3), with tricyclics (Table 6) or when alarms were compared with cognitive behavioural therapy, psychotherapy, hypnotherapy or restricted diet (Table 7).
It is not certain whether there is any difference between alarm training plus bladder training compared with bladder training alone (Table 8). Alarm training plus dry‐bed training may increase the number of children achieving a complete response compared to dry‐bed training alone (Table 8). Similarly, more children may achieve a complete response with alarm training plus desmopressin compared to desmopressin alone (Table 8).
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up
After stopping treatment, alarm therapy may increase the number of children remaining dry compared with control or no treatment (Table 1). Compared with desmopressin, alarm therapy probably increases the number of children remaining dry after stopping treatment (Table 5).
We cannot be certain whether alarm therapy has a more sustained effect compared to most other interventions. We are uncertain whether more children remain dry with standard alarm training compared to behavioural interventions (Table 4), tricyclics (Table 6), cognitive behavioural therapy or hypnotherapy (Table 7), or whether there is any difference between the various types of alarms when treatment is stopped, due to the very low‐quality evidence (Table 3). Moderate‐quality evidence suggests there is probably little difference between training with the code‐word alarm compared with the control alarm for children remaining dry after stopping treatment, but the wide confidence interval around the effect estimate is consistent with appreciable benefit as well as appreciable harm (Table 3).
It may be that more children remain dry after stopping alarm training plus desmopressin compared with desmopressin monotherapy (Table 8), but it is unclear if there is any difference between alarm training plus dry‐bed training compared with dry‐bed training alone due to the very wide confidence interval, which is consistent with appreciable benefit as well as appreciable harm (Table 8).
Adverse events
Notwithstanding the moderate‐quality evidence, we do not have a precise estimate about whether a code‐word alarm or a control alarm makes any difference to the proportion of children experiencing adverse events because the confidence intervals are wide and span appreciable benefit and harm (Table 3). Alarms probably reduce the risk of adverse events compared with desmopressin (Table 5) but we have insufficient evidence about the risk of adverse events with alarms added to desmopressin compared with desmopressin alone (Table 8). Due to the low quality and paucity of evidence available, we cannot be certain about the risk of adverse events with any other treatments compared with alarm training.
Overall completeness and applicability of evidence
The included studies do not address all of the objectives of this review because many of them did not report data in a useable way, nor did we identify sufficient evidence relating to adverse events. However, overall, the evidence we identified was relevant to the participants, interventions and outcomes of interest in our review.
Many studies are old and include some treatments that are no longer used in clinical practice. We have studied the effects of alarm training combined with other interventions in a separate section of this review, because combination therapy is increasingly used for treating enuresis that is resistant to standard single therapies.
Quality of the evidence
Despite the large number of included trials, the quality of evidence is inadequate to draw robust conclusions.
We downgraded the evidence due to risks of bias and imprecision for many outcomes, but we downgraded for inconsistency, indirectness and publication bias less frequently (see 'Summary of findings' tables 1 to 8). Most of the 74 trials in this review were small, with 30 studies having 50 or fewer participants. This led to downgrading the quality of evidence because underpowered trials are likely to be less precise. Most comparisons were assessed in one or two trials, which prevented meta‐analysis of study data. Although participants were followed up after treatment stopped in 52 trials, useful data about treatment response and relapse were available in only 35 trials, limiting our ability to judge whether treatment effects were sustained.
For the main comparison of alarms versus no treatment, we downgraded the quality of evidence for the proportion of children achieving dry nights due to risk of bias and possible publication bias. Whilst the 61% difference in the proportion of children achieving 14 dry nights with alarms may overestimate the benefit, an effect of this magnitude is unlikely to be entirely attributable to biases of study design and the non‐availability of negative studies. Overall, the poor quality of the evidence limits the strength of our conclusions about the relative superiority of any single treatment or combination of treatments. We regard these comparisons as forming the most important research questions for future studies.
Assessing the quality of evidence and the risk of bias in the included trials was limited by poor reporting of study methods. Future trials should adhere to CONSORT guidelines to ensure transparency of trial methods (Schulz 2010). Selection, performance, detection and reporting biases were evident or unclear in most reports. Incomplete outcome data suggested a high or unclear risk of attrition bias in many trials. We judged no trials to be at low risk of bias overall.
Potential biases in the review process
Despite our best efforts, we may have missed some of the published literature. We based our quality assessment on published study reports. We may have unintentionally downgraded the perceived quality of blinding and allocation concealment if no information was provided in the report. Given the absence of trial registration for most trials included in this review, it was difficult to ascertain bias due to selective reporting of outcomes. Although the search was undertaken more than 12 months ago, we are not aware of any studies in progress that would conceivably change the conclusions of the review.
Agreements and disagreements with other studies or reviews
Increased methodological rigour applied in this update means that our findings are presented with full consideration given to the quality of the evidence, an approach which was lacking in previous reviews. The findings of this updated review are consistent with those from the previous versions, which suggested that alarm therapy is an effective treatment for enuresis. However, we cannot be as certain about our conclusions due to the recognition of the low quality of the included studies.
Authors' conclusions
Implications for practice.
Alarm therapy appears to be effective for treating enuresis. Alarm therapy combined with desmopressin may be more effective than desmopressin alone. We are uncertain if alarm therapy is more effective than many of the other active interventions for enuresis due to risk of bias and low numbers of children involved in the included trials.
There are minimal adverse events with alarm therapy (with the exception of the electric shock alarm, which is now no longer used in clinical practice). There is probably a lower risk of adverse events with alarm therapy than with desmopressin, but we cannot be certain about the risk of adverse events compared to other treatments. Given that parents are usually reluctant to use medication, which has a greater potential risk of side effects, alarm therapy continues to have an important place in the treatment of enuresis in children.
Implications for research.
A number of trials have suggested that alarm therapy is an effective treatment for enuresis. However, most trials to date have been subject to various biases due to suboptimal methodological quality, which has influenced the validity and applicability of results. Although the quality of evidence for the comparison between alarms and no‐treatment controls is low, the size of the effect is large and unlikely to justify new research in this area. The priority for future trials will be to focus on better study design to minimise bias, to strengthen the validity of comparisons between alarms and active treatments identified in this review, and to generate findings that will provide the basis for further research. A key design feature for new trials will be to use the standardised outcome measures defined by the ICCS to facilitate comparison and pooling of results. Other considerations include differentiating between children with monosymptomatic and non‐monosymptomatic enuresis, using larger sample sizes to improve statistical power, conducting longer follow‐up after stopping treatment to assess long‐term success, and reporting adverse events to indicate treatment harms. To broaden the applicability of results, future trials should also include children with complex enuresis.
Current evidence suggests that the addition of alarm training to other interventions may be more effective than the interventions alone but little is known about other factors that enhance the effectiveness of alarm training. More research is needed about the pathophysiology and clinical spectrum of enuresis to identify new ways of augmenting the response to alarm training. In addition, more research is needed to identify the specific biological mechanisms and patient attributes that predict treatment success, as well as the extent of treatment response. Better understanding of the clinical predictors of successful alarm training will help practitioners to tailor alarm therapy to likely responders, and other treatment regimens to likely non‐responders. Given the need to find new ways to enhance the effectiveness of alarms, future trials of different types of combination alarm therapy will be necessary.
What's new
Date | Event | Description |
---|---|---|
7 December 2021 | Amended | Republished to fix minor errors with links in the text. |
History
Protocol first published: Issue 1, 2001 Review first published: Issue 1, 2001
Date | Event | Description |
---|---|---|
12 March 2021 | Amended | Republished to fix a technical issue with the PRISMA diagram |
22 April 2020 | New search has been performed | For this fifth update, published in 2020, we made the following changes: 1. We updated the search to June 2018 and included 19 new trials (Ahmed 2013; Bak 2007; Bryant 2003; Caldwell 2015; Evans 2011; Fagundes 2017; Hoseinzadeh 1997; Kwak 2010#; Ma 2007; Naitoh 2005; Onol 2015; Ozden 2008; Seabrook 2005; Shakiba 2001; Titawee 2000; Tuncel 2014; Tuygun 2007; Van Hoeck 2008; Vogt 2010). 2. We have substantially updated the methodology in the review to include risks of bias and implemented the GRADE approach to assess the quality of the body of evidence. We have also developed 'Summary of findings' tables. |
22 April 2020 | New citation required but conclusions have not changed | This version has not changed previous findings and remains in agreement with other systematic reviews. However, due to the low‐quality evidence of most of the included studies and the adoption of GRADE, we are uncertain of the conclusions that we drew in the previous review. Our conclusion for this update is therefore that response rates with alarm therapy compared to rewards, waking and drugs are similar at the end of treatment, but fewer children relapse with alarm therapy after stopping treatment. Alarms combined with desmopressin or overlearning have an additive effect. |
20 August 2008 | Amended | Converted to new review format. |
28 February 2007 | New search has been performed | Third (minor) update (Issue 3 2007). One new trial comparing desmopressin and alarms (Ng 2005) was added. The review conclusion that children are less likely to relapse after alarm treatment than after desmopressin was not altered. |
22 February 2005 | New citation required and conclusions have changed | Substantive amendment. Second update Issue 2 2005. Three new trials (Gibb 2004, Nawaz 2002 and Tobias 2001) were added. There was some evidence that dry bed training may reduce the relapse rate. |
26 February 2003 | New citation required and conclusions have changed | Substantive amendment. First update Issue 2 2003. Twelve trials which were previously included only in a sensitivity analysis and 20 new trials were added. Two previously included trials were excluded. Compared to the previous version, there was more evidence that alarms were better than no treatment, desmopressin or tricyclics. Overlearning may reduce the relapse rate. |
Notes
This review is an update of previous reviews based on the original review by Lister‐Sharp and colleagues at the Centre for Reviews and Dissemination at the University of York, UK (Glazener 2001; Glazener 2003; Glazener 2005; Glazener 2007; Lister‐Sharp 1997).
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Kushal Agrawal, Aniruddh Deshpande, Cathryn Glazener, Peter Herbison, Rebecca Morgan, Muhammad Imran Omar, Penny Robson, Sheila Wallace and Luke Vale for valuable comments and advice in conducting this update. In addition, we are grateful for the contribution of the previous authors: Jonathan HC Evans, Cathryn MA Glazener and Rachel E Peto.
This review was based on an updated review by Cathryn Glazener (University of Aberdeen), Jonathan Evans (Nottingham University Hospitals NHS Trust) and Rachel E Peto (University of York). Their first review was based on one originally written by the NHS Centre for Reviews and Dissemination, University of York, UK (Lister‐Sharp 1997). The York review authors obtained information from a variety of sources, including organisations, manufacturers and individuals, listed in the NHS Centre for Reviews and Dissemination Report. We acknowledge their contribution to this review.
We are grateful to the authors of, and contributors to, previous versions of this review. This includes the first version, which was written by the NHS Centre for Reviews and Dissemination, University of York, UK (Lister‐Sharp 1997) and for which information was obtained from a variety of sources, including organisations, manufacturers and individuals. We also acknowledge the support for the editorial base of Cochrane Incontinence by the Chief Scientist Office, Scottish Executive Health Department and National Health Service Research and Development Programme during the development of this version of the review.
Appendices
Appendix 1. Summary of findings: alarm plus other intervention versus alarm monotherapy
Outcomes | Anticipated absolute effects* (95% CI) | Relative effect (95% CI) | № of participants (studies) | Quality of the evidence (GRADE) | Comments | |
Risk with ALARM MONOTHERAPY | Risk with ALARM + OTHER INTERVENTION | |||||
Mean number of wet nights a week ‐ alarm + bladder training versus alarm | The mean number of wet nights per week in the alarm monotherapy group ranged from 1.0 to 2.3 | Participants in the alarm + bladder training group experienced0.13 more wet nights a week (0.92 fewer to 1.17 more) compared with the alarm monotherapy group | ‐ | 59 (3 RCTs) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b | ‐ |
Mean number of wet nights a week ‐ alarm + dry‐bed training (with alarm) versus alarm | The mean number of wet nights per week in the alarm monotherapy group was 0.83 | Participants in the alarm + dry bed training (with alarm) group experienced 2.42 fewer wet nights a week (4.13 fewer to 0.71 fewer) compared with the alarm monotherapy group | ‐ | 24 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d | ‐ |
Mean number of wet nights a week ‐ alarm + desmopressin versus alarm | The mean number of wet nights per week in the alarm monotherapy group ranged from 1.8 to 2.9 | Participants in the alarm + desmopressin experienced 0.8 fewer wet nights a week (1.33 fewer to 0.26 fewer) compared with the alarm monotherapy group | ‐ | 528 (6 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊕⊝ MODERATEc | ‐ |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + overlearning versus alarm | 619 per 1000 | 638 per 1000 (402 to 1,000) | RR 1.03 (0.65 to 1.63) | 43 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWd, e |
19 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + overlearning (217 fewer to 390 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + overlearning versus intermittent alarm | 500 per 1000 | 635 per 1000 (365 to 1,000) | RR 1.27 (0.73 to 2.23) | 40 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOW 4 5 | 135 more childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm (135 fewer to 615 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + bladder training + over‐learning versus alarm | 750 per 1000 | 690 per 1000 (428 to 1,000) | RR 0.92 (0.57 to 1.50) | 25 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWd, f | 60 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + bladder training + over‐learning (323 fewer to 375 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + bladder training vs alarm | 483 per 1000 | 521 per 1000 (396 to 690) | RR 1.08 (0.82 to 1.43) | 117 (5 RCTs) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWa, b | 39 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + bladder training (87 fewer to 208 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + dry‐bed training versus alarm | 594 per 1000 | 843 per 1000 (659 to 1,000) | RR 1.42 (1.11 to 1.81) | 104 (2 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWb, c |
249 more childrenper 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + dry bed training (65 more to 481 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + desmopressin versus alarm | 387 per 1000 | 465 per 1000 (395 to 546) | RR 1.20 (1.02 to 1.41) | 770 (9 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWb, e |
77 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + desmopressin (8 more to 159 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + imipramine versus alarm | 297 per 1000 | 211 per 1000 (92 to 485) | RR 0.71 (0.31 to 1.63) | 70 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
86 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + imipramine (187 more to 205 fewer) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + methedrine versus alarm | 600 per 1000 | 960 per 1000 (570 to 1,000) | RR 1.60 (0.95 to 2.68) | 18 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
360 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + methedrine (30 fewer to 1,000 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + immediate reward for correct behaviour versus alarm | 722 per 1000 | 975 per 1000 (787 to 1,00) | RR 1.35 (1.09 to 1.66) | 74 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
253 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + immediate reward (65 more to 477 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ alarm + delayed reward if dry in morning versus alarm | 722 per 1000 | 153 per 1000 (61 to 381) | RR 1.17 (0.92 to 1.49) | 75 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d | 123 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + delayed reward (58 fewer to 354 more) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ pretreatment (placebo) + alarm versus alarm | 767 per 1000 | 537 per 1000 (360 to 790) | RR 0.70 (0.47 to 1.03) | 60 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d | 230 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + pretreatment (placebo) (23 more to 406 fewer) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ pretreatment (bladder training + placebo) + alarm vs alarm | 767 per 1000 | 583 per 1000 (406 to 843) | RR 0.76 (0.53 to 1.10) | 59 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
184 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + pretreatment (bladder training + placebo) (77 more to 360 fewer) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ pretreatment (oxybutynin) + alarm vs alarm | 767 per 1000 | 567 per 1000 (391 to 820) | RR 0.74 (0.51 to 1.07) | 60 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
199 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + pretreatment (oxybutynin) (54 more to 376 fewer) |
Proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment ‐ pretreatment (bladder training + oxybutynin) + alarm versus alarm | 767 per 1000 | 537 per 1000 (360 to 790) | RR 0.70 (0.47 to 1.03) | 60 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
230 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights with alarm + pretreatment (bladder training + oxybutynin) (23 more to 406 fewer) |
Proportion of children dry at follow‐up (maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights post‐treatment) ‐ alarm + overlearning vs alarm | 507 per 1000 | 751 per 1000 (589 to 959) | RR 1.48 (1.16 to 1.89) | 144 (2 RCTs) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWb, e |
244 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing with alarm + overlearning (81 more to 452 more) |
Proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment ‐ alarm + overlearning vs intermittent alarm | 278 per 1000 | 500 per 1000 (214 to 1,000) | RR 1.80 (0.77 to 4.23) | 40 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWd, e |
222 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing with alarm + overlearning (64 fewer to 897 more) |
Proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment ‐ alarm + bladder training + over‐learning versus alarm | 500 per 1000 | 615 per 1000 (300 to 1,00) | RR 1.23 (0.60 to 2.50) | 25 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWd, f |
115 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing with alarm + bladder training + overlearning (200 fewer to 750 more) |
Proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment ‐ alarm + bladder training versus alarm | 414 per 1000 | 480 per 1000 (269 to 844) | RR 1.16 (0.65 to 2.04) | 58 (2 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWa, b |
66 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing with alarm + bladder training (145 fewer to 430 more) |
Proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment ‐ alarm + dry‐bed training versus alarm | 375 per 1000 | 653 per 1000 (420 to 1,000) | RR 1.74 (1.12 to 2.70) | 104 (2 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWb, c |
277 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing with alarm + dry bed training (45 more to 638 more) |
Proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment ‐ alarm + desmopressin versus alarm | 355 per 1000 | 483 per 1000 (312 to 472) | RR 1.08 (0.88 to 1.33) | 589 (6 RCTs) | ⊕⊕⊝⊝ LOWa, b |
28 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing with alarm + desmopressin (43 fewer to 117 more) |
Proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment ‐ alarm + imipramine versus alarm | 297 per 1000 | 122 per 1000 (42 to 345) | RR 0.41 (0.14 to 1.16) | 70 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
175 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing with alarm + imipramine (48 more to 256 fewer) |
Proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment ‐ alarm + immediate reward for correct behaviour vs alarm | 722 per 1000 | 708 per 1000 (534 to 946) | RR 0.98 (0.74 to 1.31) | 74 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
14 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing with alarm + immediate reward (188 fewer to 224 more) |
Proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment ‐ alarm + delayed reward if dry in morning versus alarm | 722 per 1000 | 462 per 1000 (311 to 686) | RR 0.64 (0.43 to 0.95) | 75 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
260 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing with alarm + delayed reward (36 fewer to 412 fewer) |
Proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment ‐ pretreatment (placebo) + alarm versus alarm | 633 per 1000 | 703 per 1000 (488 to 1,000) | RR 1.11 (0.77 to 1.58) | 60 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
70 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing with alarm + pretreatment (placebo) (146 fewer to 367 more) |
Proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment ‐ pretreatment (bladder training + placebo) + alarm vs alarm | 633 per 1000 | 652 per 1000 (450 to 956) | RR 1.03 (0.71 to 1.51) | 59 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
19 more children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing with alarm + pretreatment (bladder training + placebo) (184 fewer to 323 more) |
Proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment ‐ pretreatment (oxybutynin) + alarm versus alarm | 633 per 1000 | 564 per 1000 (374 to 855) | RR 0.89 (0.59 to 1.35) | 60 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
70 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing with alarm + pretreatment (oxybutynin) (222 more to 260 fewer) |
Proportion of children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment ‐ pretreatment (bladder training + oxybutynin) + alarm versus alarm | 633 per 1000 | 532 per 1000 (348 to 823) | RR 0.84 (0.55 to 1.30) | 60 (1 RCT) | ⊕⊝⊝⊝ VERY LOWc, d |
101 fewer children per 1000 achieving 14 dry nights or not relapsing with alarm + pretreatment (bladder training + oxybutynin) (190 more to 285 fewer) |
Adverse events | Not reported |
*The risk in the intervention group (and its 95% confidence interval) is based on the assumed risk in the comparison group and the relative effect of the intervention (and its 95% CI).
CI: Confidence interval; RR: Risk ratio;
GRADE Working Group grades of evidence High quality: We are very confident that the true effect lies close to that of the estimate of the effect Moderate quality: We are moderately confident in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be close to the estimate of the effect, but there is a possibility that it is substantially different Low quality: Our confidence in the effect estimate is limited: The true effect may be substantially different from the estimate of the effect Very low quality: We have very little confidence in the effect estimate: The true effect is likely to be substantially different from the estimate of effect
Footnotes
aDowngraded by two levels due to very serious risk of bias: inadequate randomisation, attrition bias, selective reporting and potentially biased source of funding. bDowngraded by one level due to serious imprecision: small sample sizes. cDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of bias: inadequate randomisation and selective reporting. dDowngraded by two levels due to very serious imprecision: single trial with small sample size. eDowngraded by two levels due to very serious risk of bias: inadequate randomisation, attrition bias and selective reporting. fDowngraded by one level due to serious risk of bias: selective reporting and potentially biased source of funding.
Appendix 2. Combination therapies
Alarm plus overlearning plus bladder training versus no treatment
Outcome | Alarm + overlearning + bladder training | No treatment | Result |
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment | not reported | ||
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 4/13 | 11/11 |
Favoursalarm + overlearning + bladder training RR 0.34 (95% CI 0.16 to 0.72) (Houts 1986) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | 5/13 | 11/11 | RR 0.41 (95% CI 0.21 to 0.79) (Houts 1986) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | not reported |
Alarm plus bladder training versus no treatment
Outcome | Alarm + bladder training | No treatment | Result |
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment | n = 22 | n = 21 |
Favours alarm + bladder training MD −3.96 (95% CI −5.01 to −2.90) Bennett 1985; Nawaz 2002 |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 9/35 | 31/32 |
Favours alarm + bladder training RR 0.33 (95% CI 0.12 to 0.94) (Bennett 1985; Houts 1986; Nawaz 2002) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | 11/25 | 22/23 |
Favours alarm + bladder training RR 0.47 (95% CI 0.30 to 0.73) (Houts 1986; Nawaz 2002) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | not reported |
Alarm plus desmopressin versus alarm plus imipramine
Outcome | Alarm + desmopressin | Alarm + imipramine | Result |
Mean number of wet nights per week at the end of treatment | 2.5 (n = 35) |
2.15 (n = 33) |
No SDs reported (Naitoh 2005) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 26/33 | 26/37 | RR 1.12 (95% CI 0.85 to 1.48) (Naitoh 2005) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | not reported | ||
Proportion of children with adverse events |
Alarm plus imipramine versus placebo
Outcome | Alarm + imipramine | Placebo | Result |
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment | 1 (n = 8) | 5 (n = 8) | No SDs reported (Fournier 1987) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | not reported | ||
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | |||
Proportion of children with adverse events |
Alarm plusimipramine versus random waking
Outcome | Alarm + imipramine | Random waking | Result |
Mean number of wet nights per week at the end of treatment | 1 (n = 8) | 3.3 (n = 8) | No SDs reported (Fournier 1987) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | not reported | ||
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | |||
Proportion of children with adverse events |
Alarm plusnortriptyline versus alarm + placebo
Outcome | Alarm + nortriptyline | Alarm + placebo | Result |
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment | not reported | ||
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 6/15 | 9/15 | RR 0.67 (95% CI 0.32 to 1.40) (Scholander 1968) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | 6/15 | 9/15 | RR 0.67 (95% CI 0.32 to 1.40) (Scholander 1968) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | not reported |
Alarm plus behavioural therapy versus desmopressin
Outcome | Alarm + behavioural therapy | Desmopressin | Result |
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment | Not reported | ||
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 13/52 | 24/46 |
Favours alarm + behavioural therapy RR 0.48 (95% CI 0.28 to 0.83) (Ma 2007) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | 15/52 | 33/36 |
Favours alarm + behavioural therapy RR 0.40 (95% CI 0.25 to 0.64) (Ma 2007) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | not reported |
Alarm plus behavioural therapy plus desmopressin versus desmopressin
Outcome | Alarm + behavioural therapy + desmopressin | Desmopressin | Result |
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment | not reported | ||
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 6/40 | 24/46 |
Favours alarm+ behavioural therapy + desmopressin RR 0.29 (95% CI 0.13 to 0.63) (Ma 2007) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | 8/40 | 33/46 |
Favours alarm+ behavioural therapy + desmopressin RR 0.28 (95% CI 0.15 to 0.53) (Ma 2007) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | not reported |
Alarm plus behavioural therapy plus desmopressin versus alarm plus behavioural therapy
Outcome | Alarm + behavioural therapy + desmopressin | Alarm + behavioural therapy | Result |
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment | not reported | ||
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 6/40 | 13/52 | RR 0.60 (95% CI 0.25 to 1.44) (Ma 2007) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | 8/40 | 15/52 | RR 0.69 (95% CI 0.33 to 1.47) (Ma 2007) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | not reported |
Alarm plus behavioural therapy versus fluid restriction & lifting + behavioural therapy
Outcome | Alarm + behavioural therapy | Fluid restriction & lifting + behavioural therapy | Result |
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment | not reported | ||
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 11/40 | 34/40 |
Favours alarm + behavioural therapy RR 0.32 (95% CI 0.19 to 0.54) (Titawee 2000) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | 19/40 | 39/40 |
Favours alarm + behavioural therapy RR 0.49 (95% CI 0.35 to 0.68) (Titawee 2000) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | not reported |
Alarm plus dry bed training versus no treatment
Outcome | Alarm + dry bed training | No treatment | Result |
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment | 0 (n = 60) | 4.4 (n = 20) | No SDs (Bollard 1981b) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 0/60 | 18/20 |
Favours alarm + dry bed training RR 0.01 ( 95% CI 0.00 to 0.15) (Bollard 1981b) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | 15/60 | 20/20 |
Favours alarm + dry bed training RR 0.26 (95 CI 0.17 to 0.40) (Bollard 1981b) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | not reported |
Alarm plus dry bed training versus dry bed training (no alarm)
Outcome | Alarm + dry bed training | Dry bed training (no alarm) | Result |
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment | 0 (n = 60) | 3.8 (n = 20) | No SDs (Bollard 1981b) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 0/60 | 15/20 |
Favours alarm + dry bed training RR 0.01 (95% CI 0.00 to 0.18) (Bollard 1981b |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | 15/60 | 17/20 |
Favours alarm + dry bed training RR 0.29 (95% CI 0.18 to 0.47) (Bollard 1981b |
Proportion of children with adverse events | not reported |
Alarm plus immediate reward versus alarm plus delayed alarm
Outcome | Alarm + immediate reward | Alarm + delayed alarm | Result |
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment | not reported | ||
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 1/38 | 6/39 | RR 0.17 (95% CI 0.02 to 1.35) (Van Londen 1993) |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | 1/38 | 21/39 |
Favours alarm + immediate reward RR 0.05 (95% CI 0.01 to 0.35) (Van Londen 1993) |
Proportion of children with adverse events | not reported |
Alarm plus pretreatment A versus alarm plus pretreatment B
Outcome | Alarm + pretreatment A | Alarm + pretreatment B | Result |
Mean number of wet nights a week at the end of treatment | not reported | ||
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment | 12/29 (bladder training and placebo) |
14/30 (placebo) |
RR 0.89 (95% CI 0.50, 1.58) |
14/30 (bladder training and oxybutynin) | |||
24/30 (oxybutynin and bladder training) |
14/30 (placebo) |
RR 1.00 (95% CI 0.58 to 1.72) | |
Proportion of children failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after stopping treatment | 10/29 (bladder training and placebo) |
9/30 (placebo) |
RR 1.15 (95%% CI 0.55 to 2.41) |
14/30 (bladder training and oxybutynin) |
RR 0.74 (95% CI 0.39 to 1.39) | ||
14/30 (oxybutynin and bladder training) |
9/30 (placebo) |
RR 1.56 (95% CI 0.80 to 3.03) | |
Proportion of children with adverse events | not reported |
Appendix 3. Cochrane Incontinence Specialised Register search terms
The terms used to search the Cochrane Incontinence Specialised Register are given below: (TOPIC.URINE.ENURESIS*) AND ({DESIGN.CCT*} OR {DESIGN.RCT*})
(All searches were of the keyword field of Reference Manager 2012).
Date of the most recent search of the Register for this review: 25 June 2018.
Appendix 4. Results data from included trials
Ahmed 2013 | Wet nights on treatment = mean (SD) wet nights at end of treatment: A (n = 45): 2.06 (1.31). B (n = 46): 1.76 (1.59). C (n = 45): 1.00 (1.13) Mean (SD) wet nights at end of follow‐up: A (n = 45): 3.04 (1.24). B (n = 46): 4.45 (1.20). C (n = 45): 2.97 (1.19) Number not achieving 14 consecutive dry nights (failure of therapy) A 39/45. B 34/46. C 27/45 Complete response (no wet nights) at end of treatment: A 6/45. B 12/46. C 18/45 Nonresponse (defined as < 50% reduction in the number of wet nights) at end of treatment: A 13/45. B 10/46. C 4/45 Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up: A = 39/45. B = 42/46. C = 33/45 Adverse events: ‘No adverse events were seen during the study period’ |
Azrin 1974 | Fewer wet nights in first 2 weeks with A+C vs B+D (median 1 vs 5 in second week, P < 0.005). More children achieving 6 dry nights in A+C |
Azrin 1978 | Per cent wet nights during first 2 weeks: A 15%; B 76% (P < 0.00001) No. of children swapping to other group after 2 weeks of treatment: A 0/27; B 23/27 |
Bak 2007 | Wet nights on treatment = mean wet nights at end of treatment: A 4.8 ± 6.5 B 2.9 ± 4.1. C 1.9 ± 2.5 Number not achieving 14 consecutive dry nights (failure of therapy) A: 23; B: 25; C: 22 Complete response: A 30%. B 27%. C 35% Number failed or relapsed after end of treatment = number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up: Failed or relapsed: A 30; B: 26; C:25. Relapse: A = 67% x 10 = 6.7. B = 11% x 9 = 1. C = 22% x 12 = 2.5 |
Baker 1969 | Mean number of wet nights a week in last 3 weeks of treatment: A: 1.8; B: 3.1; C: 5.9 (no SDs) Minor behavioural adverse events, self‐limiting |
Bennett 1985 | Mean dry nights in last 2 weeks of 12 weeks follow‐up: A, 12 (SD 3.9); B, 7.5 (5.2); C, 11.2 (3.6); D, 3.7 (3.0) Number achieving 14 dry nights: A, 4/9; B, 2/12; C, 5/10; D, 0/9 Mean dry nights a fortnight at follow‐up: A, 12.3 (3.1); B, 7.1 (5.8); C, 9.3 (5.3). (D treated after 12 weeks) |
Bollard 1981a | Experiment 1
Mean number of wet nights at end of 20 weeks: A: 0.8, B: 2.2, C: 4.6 2 treatment groups did not differ significantly in number of wet beds at end of 20 weeks or number of days taken to reach dryness criterion Number achieving 14 consecutive dry nights: A: 12, B: 9, C: 0 Number relapsing at 12 month follow‐up: A: 4, B: 5 |
Bollard 1981b | Experiment 2
Comparing DBT vs alarm ‐ DBT significantly more effective in terms of number of wet nights and days to dryness Mean number of wet nights a week at end of week 20: ‐ (including dropouts) A: 0, B: 0, C: 0, D: 3.8 (n = 20), E: 0.6, F: 4.4 ‐ (excluding dropouts) A: 0, B: 0, C: 0, D: 1.3 (n = 8), E: 0.6, F: 4.4 Number achieving 14 consecutive dry nights: A:20, B:20, C: 20, D: 5, E: 16, F: 2 (P < 0.05) Number relapsing: A: 5, B: 6, C: 4, D: 2, E: 6, F: 2 NS (i.e. number failing or relapsing: A: 5/20, B: 6/20, C: 4/20, D: 17/20, E: 10/20, F: 20/20) |
Bollard 1982a | Mean number of wet nights during 20‐week treatment period:
A: 27, B: 13, C: 24, D: 23, E: 14, F: 10, G: 21, H: 11 Number of cases becoming dry: A: 31, B: 12, C: 11, D: 10, E: 12, F: 12, G: 11, H: 20 Significant difference in response rate of group with waking schedule versus those without (Chi2 = 13.04, df = 3, P < 0.01) |
Bradbury 1995 | Mean dry nights a week: A: n = 33, mean = 6.1, 95% CI 5.6 to 6.7; B: 27, 4.8, 4.0 to 5.6 Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 6/33, B: 11/27 Number failing or relapsing: A: 10/33, B: 14/27 Side effects: none reported Subgroup analysis in more severe group: A still better than B |
Bryant 2003 | Wet nights per 28 nights: At 16 weeks: A: 12, B: 16, C: 17 At 26 weeks: A: 9, B: 16, C: 10 At 52 weeks: A: 9, B: 4, C: 8 |
Butler 1988 | Mean number of dry nights in last 4 weeks
A: 20.76, B: 23.79, F (1.46) = 1.77
Number of children achieving 14 dry nights:
A: 20/28 (71%), B: 25/35 (71%); no significant difference Mothers in dropout group significantly more angry with bedwetting than other groups |
Butler 1990a | Experiment 1
Mean number of wet nights in 16 weeks: A: 18.9, B: 15.3 Number (%) children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights: A: 14 (70), B: 14 (70) Mean number of wet nights until achievement of 14 consecutive dry nights A: 54.8, B: 35.3 (t = 2.8, df = 26, P < 0.01) Number (%) children relapsing A: 4/14 (29) B: 3/14 (21) Most children preferred body‐worn alarm to pad and bell |
Butler 1990b | Experiment 2
Mean number of wet nights in 16 weeks A: 28.7, B: 25.0 Number (%) attaining 14 consecutive dry nights A: 14 (58), B: 20 (83) Mean number of wet nights to achievement of 14 consecutive dry nights A: 53.7, B: 40.7 Number (%) children relapsing: A: 7 (50), B: 9 (45) |
Caceres 1982 | Not cured on original treatment: A: 0/7, B: 5/7 |
Caldwell 2015 | Number not achieving response (50% or more reduction in wetting): A: 46/176, B: 59/177 Relapse (within 6 months): A: 62/176, B: 51/177 Participants with adverse events: A: 24/176, B: 18/177 Sleep disturbance: A: 14/176, B: 17/177 Frightened by the alarm: A: 2/176, B: 4/177 Alarm triggering sleepwalking/nightmares: A: 1/176, B: 1/177 Children satisfied with treatment: A: 90% (158/176?), B: 86% (152/177?) |
Danquah 1975 | Mean number wet nights a week at end of treatment: A: 5.6, B: 4.00, C: 3.2 Mean number wet nights a week at follow‐up: A: 5.61, B: 3.42, C: 1.49 Recipients of traditional shaming seemed depressed and showed evidence of loss of self‐esteem and isolating themselves from friends Drug treatment was said to cause drowsiness at first. Parents not disturbed by alarm because they slept outside |
Elinder 1985 | Number not cured: A: 0/36, B: 0/17 Adverse events: mentioned technical problems (same numbers in each group) |
Evans 2011 | Mean wet nights at end of treatment: A: 4.28, B: 5.0 Number not achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at end of treatment: A: 131/192, B: 37/59 Mean wet nights at end of follow‐up: A: 2.1. B: 1.5 Number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up: A: 156/192 (81.2%), B: 46/59 (78%) Adverse events: A: 30% (4 severe dysuria, urgency, appendicitis, rash, 3 headaches), B: 14% (1 severe anxiety) Other: compliance, mean sleep duration |
Fagundes 2017 | "Response to treatment was classified using the ICCS 2014 criteria for initial success." Number not achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment: A: 20/30, B: 14/20, C: 14/24 Adverse events: not reported |
Faraj 1999 | Dry nights at 3 months: A: 85%, B: 90%
Dry nights at six months: A: 78%, B: 94% Number not achieving 14 dry nights at 3 months: A: 12/39, B: 6/37 Adverse events: not mentioned |
Fielding 1980 | Mean number of wet nights in third month of alarm: A: 6.2, B: 2.3
Number not achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment: A: 5/16, B: 3/17
Number (%) relapsing after 3 months: A: 3 (28), B: 4 (29)
After 6 months: A: 3 (28), B: 5 (36)
After 12 months: A: 4 (36), B: 8 (57)
Number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing after 12 months: A: 9/16, B: 11/17 Adverse events: not mentioned |
Finley 1973 | Mean wet episodes during 6th week: A: 0.2, B: 0.6, C: 8
Number not achieving 7 dry nights: A: 1/10, B: 2/10, C: 10/10 Number dry at 6 weeks: A: 9/10, B: 8/10, C: 0/10 Number failing or relapsing after 'cure': A: 5/10, B: 3/10, C 10/10 |
Finley 1977 | Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 3/10, B: 6/10 Relapse rate: A: 3/7, B: 1/4 Failed or relapsed: A: 6/10, B: 7/10 |
Fournier 1987 | Mean number of wet nights a week: A: 1.9, B: 2.5, C: 5, D: 3.3, E: 1
No results for F, G or H 4 boys dropped out because of side effects or non‐compliance, 1 girl with UTI Adverse events: 4 boys dropped out because of side effects or non‐compliance, but adverse events by individual interventions not specifically mentioned |
Geffken 1986a | Mean (SD) number of wet nights a week: A: 1.7 (1.2), C: 2.5 (0.9) Significant interaction between maximal functional bladder capacity and treatment: F (1, 33) = 4.90, P = 0.03 Number of children achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment: A: 9, C: 9 Number of children relapsing during follow‐up: A: 4, C: 4 |
Geffken 1986b | Mean (SD) number of wet nights a week: B: 2.3 (1.0), D: 1.6 (1.1) Significant interaction between maximal functional bladder capacity and treatment: F (1, 33) = 4.90, P = 0.03 Number of children achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment: B: 10, D: 9 Number of children relapsing during follow‐up: B: 6, D: 3 |
Gibb 2004 | Cure = 28 dry nights, relapse = 2 wet nights in 2 weeks Wet nights during treatment (number, mean (SD)): A: 101, 1.8 (1.13), B: 106, 2.4 (1.53) Cure during treatment: A: 52/101, B: 51/106 P = 0.63 (failed: A: 49/101, B: 55/106) Relapse after treatment stopped: A: 7, B: 3 Failed or relapsed: A: 56/101, B: 58/106 Adverse events: A: 1 (headache), B: 1 (nose bleed) Other: compliance same in both groups Cure in daytime wetting: A: 6/11, B: 3/8 |
Hojsgaard 1979 | Cured: A: 20/32, B: 17/30 Improved: A: 5/32, B: 6/30 |
Hoseinzadeh 1997 | "1st 45 days the drug was much more effective but after 90 days there was no obvious difference between them, in case of recurrence 45 days after stopping treatments there was no difference between 2 groups.” |
Houts 1986 | A: cured 9, failed 4, dropout 2; B: cured 13, dropout 2; C: cured 9, failed 3, dropout 3; D: none cured (11/11 failed) Relapse at end of study after retreatment if necessary: A: 1, B: 6, C: 3 i.e. failed or relapsed: A 5/13, B: 6/13, C: 6/12 |
Jehu 1977 | Mean number of wet nights a week 12: A: 0.3, B: 5.3 Achieved 14 dry nights: A: 18/19, B: 0/20 1 had absconded (counted as failure) 3 children had relapsed at 6 months and another at 8 months (needed repeat treatment) |
Kennedy 1968 | Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 0/5, B: 0/5, C: 0/3, D: 4/5 |
Kolvin 1972 | Mean number of wet night in final month (% improvement): A: 9.3 (64), B: 9.1 (62), C: 11.0 (53) At follow‐up mean number of wet nights a month (% improvement): A: 13.4 (43), B: 9.3 (64), C: 11.3 (54) |
Kwak 2010# | Mean % decrease in wet nights at end of treatment (3 months): A: 69.5% decrease from 23.1 = 7.0, B: 78.4% decrease from 21.7 = 4.7 Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights at end of treatment: A 34/54, B 25/50 Mean wet nights at follow‐up: Unable to calculate as cross‐over Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after cure: A: 34 + 10/54, B: 25 + 3/50 Adverse events: Abdominal pain, voiding difficulty (n = 1, desmopressin group) |
Leebeek 2001 | Wet nights (number of participants, mean):
After 3 weeks: A: 47, 2.93, B: 46, 3.86 (P = 0.014) After 6 weeks: A: 44, 2.18, B: 42, 3.57 (not significant) After 9 weeks: A: 43, 2.77, B: 39, 2.21 (not significant) Number cured (90% or greater reduction in wet nights a week compared to baseline): 2 weeks after end of treatment: A: 15/47, B: 18/46 6 months after end of treatment: A: 17/47, B: 17/46 i.e. failed at six months: A: 30/47, B: 29/46 Wet nights at 6 months (number of participants, mean): A:41, 2.72, B:37, 1.90 Adverse events: none in either group |
Longstaffe 2000 | Number not achieving 14 dry nights after 6 months: A: 26/61, B: 31/60, C: 38/61 All children improved psychologically, e.g. behaviour and self‐concept, regardless of outcome or treatment assignment Adverse events: not mentioned |
Lovibond 1964a | Experiment 1 Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 0/12, B: 1/12, C: 1/12 Number failing or relapsing after trial: A: 5/12, B: 6/12, C: 5/12 Adverse events: B: corrosive skin burns (3); discontinued due to fear of shocks (2) |
Lovibond 1964b | Experiment 2 Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A1: 0/5, A2: 0/5, B1: 0/5, B2: 0/5 Number failing or relapsing after 24 months: A1: 3/5, A2: 3/5, B1: 3/5, B2: 2/5 |
Lovibond 1964c | Experiment 3 Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 0/12, B: 2/12 Number failing or relapsing: A: 5/12, B: 6/12 |
Lynch 1984 | Wet nights in last 2 weeks, n, mean (SD): A: 18, 3.38 (4.55), B: 18, 8.38 (4.55), C: 18, 8.11 (3.25) Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 11/18, B: 17/18, C: 18/18 |
Ma 2007 | Number not achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment: A = 25% x 52 = 13, B = 52.2% x 46 = 24, C = 15% x 40 = 6, D = 100% x 45 = 45 Number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up: A = 28.8% x 52 = 15, B = 71.7% x 46 = 33, C = 20% x 40 = 8, D = 100% x 45 = 45 Adverse events: none reported |
McKendry 1975 | Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 63/64, B: 49/62, C: 20/43 Adverse events: A: 2/12 children became aggressive; B: 3/16 had headaches, abdominal pain or fatigue, C: 10/16 showed fear or anxiety about the machine. For C, electric shocks resulted in skin erythema, discolouration, painless cold burns and ulceration |
Moffatt 1987 | Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 19/61, B: 54/55 Adverse events: 4 of A could not cope with alarm method |
Motavalli 1994 | Mean (SD) frequency of wetting during final 2 weeks of treatment: A: 4.1 (2.6), B: 6.6 (5.5), C: 2.8 (4.3) |
Naitoh 2005 | Mean wet nights at end of treatment: A: 6.2/2 wk, B: 5/2 wk, C: 4.3/2 wk (cf baseline, P < 0.0001 in each group) Number not achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at end of treatment: A: 26/37 (70%), B: 28/35 (80%) – P 0.059 cf A, C: 26/33 (79%) – P 0.082 cf A Number failing to achieve 14 dry nights or relapsing after cure at the end of treatment (this included those who relapsed after achieving 14 consecutive dry nights): A: 26/37, B: 31/35, C: 29/33 Adverse events: not mentioned |
Nawaz 2002 | Wet nights a week during trial (final week): A: mean 0.83 (SD 1.40), B: 3.25 (2.67), C: 5 (2.26) Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 4/12, B: 9/12, C: 11/12 Number relapsing after end of trial: A: 1, B: 1 Fail or relapse rate: A: 5/12, B: 10/12 |
Netley 1984 | Final outcome, after 2 dry months: A: 13/17 failed, B: 7/18 failed |
Ng 2005 | Wet nights during trial (N, mean (SD)): A: 28, 2.8 (2.2), B: 36, 2.6 (2.4), C: 29, 1.3 (1.9) Not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 27/35, B: 22/38, C: 12/32 (ITT, dropouts = failure) Wet nights after trial (N, mean (SD)): A: 24, 2.5 (2.4), B: 34, 3.4 (2.5), C: 24, 2.6 (2.7) Not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing after: A: 25/35, B: 30/38, C: 19/32 (ITT, dropouts = failure) Adverse events: none All children who responded completely to the alarm stayed dry afterwards |
Onol 2015 | Success defined according to ICCS 2006 criteria. Full response = ‘cure, or 1 or fewer bed‐wetting episodes monthly.' Number not achieving full response at 3 months: A: 51/77, B: 22/65 Adverse events: not reported |
Ozden 2008 | Number of wet nights a week during treatment: A: 2.0 ± 0.8 per week, B: 1.8 ± 0.54 a week Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights during treatment: A: 1.9 ± 0.9 a week, B: 1.9 ± 0.7 a week Number of wet nights a week at follow‐up: A: 27/30, B: 24/28 Adverse events: Not reported. |
Rodriguez 2001 | Response: A: 73.3%, B: 58.6% Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 8/30, B: 12/29 All children treated with desmopressin if not cured at 6 months, therefore follow‐up not possible Adverse events: Not reported |
Ronen 1995 | Cured (3 consecutive dry weeks): A: 15/20, B: 12/19, C: 6/20, D: 0/18 Failed (intention‐to‐treat, including dropouts as failures): A: 5/20, B: 7/19, C: 14/20, D: 18/18 Number of wet nights in 3 weeks at end of treatment: A: N = 18; mean 1.03, (SD 2.15), B: N = 15; 1.23 (SD 5.82), C: N = 14; 3.33 (SD 5.8), D: N = 16; 17.22 (SD 9) Actual failure or relapse after 6 months (excluding dropouts): A: 3/18, B: 9/15, C: 8/14 Adverse events: not mentioned |
Sacks 1974 | Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 13/64, B: 8/10, C: 7/9 |
Scholander 1968 | Number with no wet nights in final week of drug/placebo treatment: A: 9/15, B: 6/15 Side effects: 1 child frightened of the mattress, 'a few' children on nortriptyline had dry mouth or troubled sleep |
Seabrook 2005 | Dry at end of treatment (3 months): A: 55.3% x 38 = 21, B: 19.4% x 36 = 7 Number not achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at end of treatment: A: (48 ‐ 21) = 27, B: (48 – 7) = 41 Number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up (this includes those who relapsed after achieving 14 consecutive dry nights): A: 27 + 6 = 33, B: 41 + 2 = 43 Adverse events: Not mentioned |
Shakiba 2001 | Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights during treatment: A: 17/30, B: 29/30 Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up: A: 20/30, B: 29/30 Adverse events: Alarm breakdown |
Sloop 1973 | Number of wet nights in 7 weeks (boys, n = 11) A: 46, B: 108
No significant difference for girls (no data) Number (%) dry: A: 11/21 (52), B: 1/21 (5) Number relapsed: A: 4/11, B: 0/1 |
Sukhai 1989# | Mean (SE) dry nights during treatment: A: 5.1 (0.4), B: 4.1 (0.4) 6‐week follow‐up: 14 dry, 5 relapsed 4.5 month follow‐up: 9 remained dry Adverse events: None reported Mean urine osmolality significantly increased from baseline Significantly higher urine osmolality with DDAVP than placebo Steady significant increase in body weight |
Taylor 1975 | Number (%) achieving no more than 1 wetting incidence in 28 days: A: 13/21 (62), B: 9/18 (50), C: 13/22 (59)
Chi2 = 0.6 df = 2 not significant Number (%) of successes who relapsed A: 9/13 (69), B: 4/9 (44), C: 3/13 (23) Chi2 = 5.6 df = 2 P = 0.001 |
Titawee 2000 | Mean wet nights at end of treatment: Data for participants who achieved remission (A = 20, B = 6), not whole group Complete response at end of treatment: A: 29/40 (72.5%), B: 6/40 (15%) Number not achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment: A: 11, B: 34 Mean wet nights at follow‐up: Data for participants who achieved remission (A = 20, B = 6), not whole group Number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up: A: 11 + 8 = 19/40, B: 34 + 5 = 39/40 Adverse events: None reported |
Tobias 2001 | Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A: 10/23, B: 14/24 Adverse events: some children did not wake, some were upset when woken, false alarm Vibrating alarm was reported to be more uncomfortable than the audio alarm |
Tuncel 2014 | Number not achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment: A: 16/49, B: 24/55 Adverse events: not reported |
Turner 1970 | Failed at 4 weeks: A: 12/15, B: 13/15, D: 14/15, E: 13/17 Mean wet nights a week at 4 weeks: A: 2.9 (SD 2.27), B: 3.58 (2.07), D; 3.23 (2.09), E: 4.22 (2.37) Failure at end of treatment: A: 3/15, B: 4/15, C: 1/12 Long‐term failure at 3 years: A + B combined: 13/20, C: 3/11 Adverse events: non‐compliance, failure or family disruption caused high dropout rate but adverse events by individual interventions not specifically mentioned |
Tuygun 2007 | Number of wet nights a week during treatment: A: 3.41/month ± 7.68, B: 10.7/ month ± 10.94 Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights during treatment: A: 15/35, B: 24/49 (this might be an overestimation as full response is > 90% improvement from baseline and theoretically you can get full response without achieving 14 dry nights) Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after cure: Unable to calculate (numbers do not reflect 14 consecutive dry nights as also include partial responders A: 16/35, B: 36/49) Adverse events: none reported |
Van Hoeck 2008 | Full response: 1 wet night or less in last 28 nights Number not achieving 14 dry nights during treatment: A: 12/25, B: 14/29, C: 14/28, D: 13/27, E: 7/28 Number not achieving 14 dry nights during treatment or relapsing after cure: A: 10/22, B: 14/28, C: 9/26, D: 13/26, E: 11/27 Adverse events: None reported |
Van Londen 1993 | Number not achieving 14 dry nights during trial: A: 1/38, B: 6/39, C: 10/36 Failure and relapse rate during follow‐up period: A: 11/38, B: 21/39, C: 10/36 Final failure and relapse rate at follow‐up*: A: 1/38, B: 2/39, C: 4/36 *children who failed or relapsed received further treatment (repeat of trial arm, other alarm method, desmopressin, imipramine, homeopathy, hospital referral, reduced drinking, regular 'wake‐up' alarm clock, chiropractic, iriscopist, acupuncture, supervised child care) more often in B + C than in A |
Vogt 2010 | Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights during treatment (first 12 weeks): A: 20/24, B: 14/19 Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after cure: A: 5/24, B: 3/19 (at 24 weeks = 6 months) A: 6/24, B: 3/19 (at 12 months) Adverse events: Not mentioned |
Wagner 1982 | Per cent wet nights in 14th week: A: 8.25%, B: 39.25%, C: 60.83% (A significantly better than B or C) Number not achieving 14 dry nights: A:2/12, B:8/12, C:11/12 Number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing: A: 7/12, B: 12/12, C:12/12 Time from cure to relapse: A: 37.8 days (range 25 to 44), B: 17 (3 to 27) |
Wagner 1985 | Per cent of wet nights a week in week 12: A: 5.38, B: 20.67, C: 72.90 Number achieving 14 consecutive dry nights: A: 8, B: 7, C: 1 Number relapsing: A: 2/8, B: 5/7, C: 1/1 Malfunction significantly greater problem for delayed alarm as compared with contiguous model |
Werry 1965 | Failure to achieve 14 dry nights (defined as no wet beds in preceding month): A: 26/27, B:19/21, C:15/21 C significantly better than A or B C (alarm) most economic as required least professional input |
Wille 1986 | Mean (SEM) number of dry nights ar week in first week: A: 4.2 (0.5), B: 2.5 (0.4); in last week of treatment: A: 4.9 (0.5), B: 6.3 (0.4) Number failing during treatment (> 5 wet nights in 28 or no change in enuresis score): A: 7/24, B: 3/22 A: 10 relapses given 3 months more desmopressin treatment. Successful for 7/10 but 4/7 relapsed immediately and 1/7 after 2 months. B: 1 relapsed and further alarm treatment unsuccessful Number not cured after treatment (failed or relapsed): A: 15/24, B: 4/22 Mean (SEM) dry nights after trial: A: 3.5 (0.4), B: 5.7 (0.4) Adverse events: A: nasal discomfort (5); nose bleeds (1); bad taste in throat (2); B: false alarms (21); alarm did not go off (5); alarm did not wake child (15); other family members woken (15); child frightened by alarm (1) Lab tests: urine osmolality and density higher during treatment with desmopressin and urine osmolality in alarm group lower during treatment than before |
Wright 1974 | Mean number of wet nights in final week of treatment: A + B: 4.1, C: 3.5, D: 1.7 |
Young 1972 | Number relapsing (failing to remain dry or regain dryness): A: 7/55, B: 16/46 |
Data and analyses
Comparison 1. Alarm versus no treatment or control.
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
1.1 Mean number of wet nights a week | 4 | 127 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐2.68 [‐4.59, ‐0.78] |
1.1.1 alarm vs control | 4 | 100 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐3.42 [‐4.98, ‐1.87] |
1.1.2 delayed alarm vs control | 1 | 27 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.13 [‐1.37, 1.63] |
1.2 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights | 18 | 827 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 7.23 [1.40, 37.33] |
1.2.1 alarm vs no treatment | 16 | 650 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 8.63 [5.23, 14.26] |
1.2.2 delayed alarm vs no treatment | 2 | 47 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 2.98 [0.60, 14.93] |
1.2.3 intermittent alarm vs control | 1 | 15 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 9.27 [0.64, 134.31] |
1.2.4 electric shock alarm vs control | 2 | 115 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 1.00 [0.92, 1.10] |
1.3 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment | 10 | 366 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 9.67 [4.74, 19.76] |
1.3.1 alarm vs control | 10 | 331 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 10.43 [4.82, 22.56] |
1.3.2 intermittent alarm vs control | 1 | 15 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 8.18 [0.56, 119.85] |
1.3.3 delayed alarm vs control | 1 | 20 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 2.86 [0.16, 52.42] |
Comparison 2. Alarm versus placebo drug.
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
2.1 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights | 2 | 181 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.59 [1.16, 2.17] |
2.1.1 alarm vs placebo | 2 | 143 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.53 [1.08, 2.18] |
2.1.2 twin‐signal alarm vs placebo | 1 | 21 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.47 [0.62, 3.45] |
2.1.3 intermittent alarm vs placebo | 1 | 17 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 2.29 [0.77, 6.80] |
2.2 Mean number of wet nights a week | 1 | 47 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐0.96 [‐2.32, 0.41] |
2.2.1 alarm vs placebo | 1 | 23 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐1.32 [‐3.32, 0.68] |
2.2.2 twin‐signal alarm vs placebo | 1 | 24 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐0.64 [‐2.51, 1.23] |
2.3 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights: subgroup analysis | 2 | 181 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.59 [1.16, 2.17] |
2.3.1 Monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis | 1 | 122 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.52 [1.03, 2.24] |
2.3.2 Non‐monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis | 1 | 59 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.72 [1.02, 2.91] |
2.1. Analysis.
Comparison 2: Alarm versus placebo drug, Outcome 1: Numbers achieving 14 dry nights
Comparison 3. Alarm versus other types of alarm.
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
3.1 Mean number of wet nights a week | 2 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | Totals not selected | |
3.1.1 immediate alarm vs delayed alarm | 1 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | Totals not selected | |
3.1.2 alarm vs twin‐signal alarm | 1 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | Totals not selected | |
3.2 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights at the end of treatment | 11 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
3.2.1 bell and pad alarm vs body alarm | 1 | 40 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.00 [0.67, 1.50] |
3.2.2 alarm vs intermittent alarm | 3 | 86 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.06 [0.82, 1.36] |
3.2.3 immediate alarm vs delayed alarm | 2 | 62 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.88 [0.97, 3.63] |
3.2.4 alarm vs twin‐signal alarm | 3 | 78 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.94 [0.79, 1.13] |
3.2.5 bell and pad alarm vs electric shock alarm | 1 | 24 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.00 [0.79, 1.27] |
3.2.6 loud alarm vs quiet alarm | 2 | 40 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.33 [0.89, 1.99] |
3.2.7 body‐worn audio alarm vs body‐worn vibrating alarm | 1 | 47 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.68 [0.45, 1.01] |
3.2.8 Code‐word alarm vs standard alarm | 1 | 353 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.11 [0.97, 1.27] |
3.3 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment | 8 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
3.3.1 bell and pad alarm vs body alarm | 1 | 40 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.91 [0.50, 1.64] |
3.3.2 alarm vs intermittent alarm | 2 | 59 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.70 [0.37, 1.35] |
3.3.3 immediate alarm vs delayed alarm | 1 | 26 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 3.00 [0.74, 12.21] |
3.3.4 alarm vs twin‐signal alarm | 2 | 48 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.93 [0.56, 1.53] |
3.3.5 bell and pad alarm vs electric shock alarm | 1 | 24 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.17 [0.56, 2.45] |
3.3.6 loud alarm vs quiet alarm | 2 | 40 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.90 [0.47, 1.71] |
3.3.7 code‐word alarm vs standard alarm | 1 | 353 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.91 [0.79, 1.05] |
3.4 Numbers with adverse events | 1 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | Totals not selected |
3.4. Analysis.
Comparison 3: Alarm versus other types of alarm, Outcome 4: Numbers with adverse events
Comparison 4. Alarm versus other behavioural interventions.
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
4.1 Mean number of wet nights a week | 3 | 95 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.81 [‐2.01, 0.38] |
4.1.1 alarm vs waking | 1 | 22 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.33 [‐2.26, 1.60] |
4.1.2 twin‐signal alarm vs waking | 1 | 23 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.35 [‐1.44, 2.14] |
4.1.3 alarm vs bladder training | 1 | 21 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐2.75 [‐4.70, ‐0.80] |
4.1.4 alarm vs star chart + rewards | 1 | 29 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.70 [‐2.05, 0.65] |
4.2 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights | 6 | 179 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 1.77 [0.98, 3.19] |
4.2.1 alarm vs waking | 2 | 32 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 1.45 [0.19, 10.84] |
4.2.2 twin‐signal alarm vs waking | 2 | 33 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 1.00 [0.71, 1.40] |
4.2.3 alarm vs bladder training | 1 | 21 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 2.67 [0.62, 11.49] |
4.2.4 alarm vs dry‐bed training (no alarm) | 2 | 54 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 3.13 [1.66, 5.87] |
4.2.5 alarm vs star chart + rewards | 1 | 39 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 2.11 [0.99, 4.47] |
4.3 Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up | 1 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | Totals not selected | |
4.4 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment | 3 | 89 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.39 [0.81, 2.41] |
4.4.1 alarm vs waking | 1 | 10 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.67 [0.18, 2.42] |
4.4.2 twin‐signal alarm vs waking | 1 | 10 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.00 [0.22, 4.56] |
4.4.3 alarm vs dry‐bed training (no alarm) | 1 | 40 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 3.33 [1.08, 10.34] |
4.4.4 alarm vs star chart + rewards | 1 | 29 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.93 [0.39, 2.22] |
Comparison 5. Alarm versus desmopressin.
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
5.1 Mean number of wet nights per week | 4 | 285 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.64 [‐1.77, 0.49] |
5.1.1 Monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis | 1 | 84 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐1.83 [‐2.82, ‐0.84] |
5.1.2 Non‐monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis | 3 | 201 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.20 [‐1.16, 0.77] |
5.2 Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up | 3 | 195 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐1.47 [‐1.90, ‐1.04] |
5.3 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights | 12 | 1168 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 1.12 [0.93, 1.36] |
5.3.1 Monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis | 8 | 854 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 1.19 [0.97, 1.47] |
5.3.2 Non‐monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis | 4 | 314 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 0.89 [0.54, 1.47] |
5.4 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment | 5 | 565 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Random, 95% CI) | 1.30 [0.92, 1.84] |
5.5 Numbers with adverse events | 5 | 565 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.38 [0.20, 0.71] |
Comparison 6. Alarm versus tricyclics.
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
6.1 Mean number of wet nights a week | 1 | 29 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐1.13 [‐2.74, 0.48] |
6.1.1 alarm v imipramine | 1 | 15 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐0.65 [‐2.70, 1.40] |
6.1.2 alarm vs clomipramine | 1 | 14 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | ‐1.90 [‐4.50, 0.70] |
6.2 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights | 3 | 208 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 2.05 [1.33, 3.17] |
6.3 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment | 1 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | Totals not selected |
Comparison 7. Alarm versus miscellaneous treatments.
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
7.1 Mean number of wet nights a week | 1 | 33 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.20 [‐2.65, 3.05] |
7.1.1 alarm vs cognitive behavioural therapy | 1 | 33 | Mean Difference (IV, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.20 [‐2.65, 3.05] |
7.2 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights | 5 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
7.2.1 alarm vs cognitive behavioural therapy | 1 | 39 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.84 [0.55, 1.29] |
7.2.2 alarm vs psychotherapy | 2 | 116 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 3.62 [1.38, 9.50] |
7.2.3 alarm vs hypnotherapy | 1 | 96 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 3.00 [1.41, 6.39] |
7.2.4 alarm vs restricted diet | 1 | 150 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 23.00 [3.19, 165.98] |
7.3 Number achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment | 2 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | Totals not selected | |
7.3.1 alarm vs cognitive behavioural therapy | 1 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | Totals not selected | |
7.3.2 alarm vs hypnotherapy | 1 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | Totals not selected |
7.1. Analysis.
Comparison 7: Alarm versus miscellaneous treatments, Outcome 1: Mean number of wet nights a week
Comparison 8. Alarm + other intervention versus other intervention.
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
8.1 Mean number of wet nights a week | 3 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
8.1.1 alarm + bladder training vs bladder training | 1 | 22 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐2.35 [‐4.20, ‐0.50] |
8.1.2 alarm + desmopressin vs desmopressin | 2 | 156 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.88 [‐1.38, ‐0.38] |
8.2 Mean number of wet nights a week: alarm + desmopressin v desmopressin subgroup analysis | 2 | 156 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.88 [‐1.38, ‐0.38] |
8.2.1 Monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis | 1 | 91 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.76 [‐1.33, ‐0.19] |
8.2.2 Non‐monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis | 1 | 65 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐1.30 [‐2.35, ‐0.25] |
8.3 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights | 7 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
8.3.1 alarm + bladder training vs bladder training | 1 | 22 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 3.00 [0.73, 12.27] |
8.3.2 alarm + dry‐bed training vs dry‐bed training | 1 | 80 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 3.79 [1.85, 7.77] |
8.3.3 alarm + desmopressin vs desmopressin | 5 | 359 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.32 [1.08, 1.62] |
8.4 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights: alarm + desmopressin v desmopressin subgroup analysis | 5 | 359 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.32 [1.08, 1.62] |
8.4.1 Monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis | 3 | 221 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.30 [1.02, 1.64] |
8.4.2 Non‐monosymptomatic enuresis | 2 | 138 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.38 [0.94, 2.02] |
8.5 Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up | 3 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
8.5.1 alarm + bladder training vs bladder training | 1 | 22 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐1.10 [‐3.42, 1.22] |
8.5.2 alarm + desmopressin vs desmopressin | 2 | 149 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐1.40 [‐1.86, ‐0.94] |
8.6 Number achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment | 3 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
8.6.1 alarm + dry‐bed training vs dry bed training | 1 | 80 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.56 [0.15, 2.12] |
8.6.2 alarm + desmopressin vs desmopressin | 2 | 161 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 2.33 [1.26, 4.29] |
Comparison 9. Alarm + other intervention versus alarm monotherapy.
Outcome or subgroup title | No. of studies | No. of participants | Statistical method | Effect size |
---|---|---|---|---|
9.1 Mean number of wet nights a week | 10 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
9.1.1 alarm + bladder training vs alarm | 3 | 59 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 0.13 [‐0.92, 1.17] |
9.1.2 alarm + dry bed training vs alarm | 1 | 24 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐2.42 [‐4.13, ‐0.71] |
9.1.3 alarm + desmopressin vs alarm | 6 | 528 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.80 [‐1.33, ‐0.26] |
9.2 Numbers achieving 14 dry nights | 21 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
9.2.1 alarm + overlearning vs alarm | 1 | 43 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.03 [0.65, 1.63] |
9.2.2 alarm + overlearning vs intermittent alarm | 1 | 40 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.27 [0.73, 2.23] |
9.2.3 alarm + bladder training + over‐learning vs alarm | 1 | 25 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.92 [0.57, 1.50] |
9.2.4 alarm + bladder training vs alarm | 5 | 117 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.08 [0.82, 1.43] |
9.2.5 alarm + dry‐bed training vs alarm | 2 | 104 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.42 [1.11, 1.81] |
9.2.6 alarm + desmopressin vs alarm | 9 | 770 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.20 [1.02, 1.41] |
9.2.7 alarm + imipramine vs alarm | 1 | 70 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.71 [0.31, 1.63] |
9.2.8 alarm + methedrine vs alarm | 1 | 18 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.60 [0.95, 2.68] |
9.2.9 alarm + nortriptyline vs alarm + placebo | 1 | 30 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.50 [0.71, 3.16] |
9.2.10 alarm + immediate reward for correct behaviour vs alarm | 1 | 74 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.35 [1.09, 1.66] |
9.2.11 alarm + immediate reward for correct behaviour vs alarm + delayed reward if dry in morning | 1 | 77 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.15 [1.00, 1.33] |
9.2.12 alarm + delayed reward if dry in morning vs alarm | 1 | 75 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.17 [0.92, 1.49] |
9.2.13 alarm + pretreatment (placebo) vs alarm | 1 | 60 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.70 [0.47, 1.03] |
9.2.14 alarm + pretreatment (bladder training + placebo) vs alarm | 1 | 59 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.76 [0.53, 1.10] |
9.2.15 alarm + pretreatment (oxybutynin) vs alarm | 1 | 60 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.74 [0.51, 1.07] |
9.2.16 alarm + pretreatment (bladder training + oxybutynin) vs alarm | 1 | 60 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.70 [0.47, 1.03] |
9.3 Mean number of wet nights a week at follow‐up | 4 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
9.3.1 alarm + bladder training vs alarm | 1 | 19 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | 1.50 [‐0.43, 3.43] |
9.3.2 alarm + desmopressin vs alarm | 3 | 196 | Mean Difference (IV, Random, 95% CI) | ‐0.02 [‐0.33, 0.29] |
9.4 Number achieving 14 dry nights post‐treatment | 14 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | Subtotals only | |
9.4.1 alarm + overlearning vs alarm | 2 | 144 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.48 [1.16, 1.89] |
9.4.2 alarm + overlearning vs intermittent alarm | 1 | 40 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.80 [0.77, 4.23] |
9.4.3 alarm + bladder training + over‐learning vs alarm | 1 | 25 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.23 [0.60, 2.50] |
9.4.4 alarm + bladder training vs alarm | 2 | 58 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.16 [0.65, 2.04] |
9.4.5 alarm + dry‐bed training vs alarm | 2 | 104 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.74 [1.12, 2.70] |
9.4.6 alarm + desmopressin vs alarm | 6 | 589 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.08 [0.88, 1.33] |
9.4.7 alarm + imipramine vs alarm | 1 | 70 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.41 [0.14, 1.16] |
9.4.8 alarm + immediate reward for correct behaviour vs alarm | 1 | 74 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.98 [0.74, 1.31] |
9.4.9 alarm + immediate reward for correct behaviour vs alarm + delayed reward if dry in morning | 1 | 77 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 2.11 [1.50, 2.97] |
9.4.10 alarm + delayed reward if dry in morning vs alarm | 1 | 75 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.64 [0.43, 0.95] |
9.4.11 alarm + pretreatment (placebo) vs alarm | 1 | 60 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.11 [0.77, 1.58] |
9.4.12 alarm + pretreatment (bladder training + placebo) vs alarm | 1 | 59 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 1.03 [0.71, 1.51] |
9.4.13 alarm + pretreatment (oxybutynin) vs alarm | 1 | 60 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.89 [0.59, 1.35] |
9.4.14 alarm + pretreatment (bladder training + oxybutynin) vs alarm | 1 | 60 | Risk Ratio (M‐H, Fixed, 95% CI) | 0.84 [0.55, 1.30] |
Characteristics of studies
Characteristics of included studies [ordered by study ID]
Ahmed 2013.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting specifically excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting: participants were consecutively enrolled from those referred to urology and paediatric clinics at hospitals in Saudi Arabia Number of participants (boys/girls): 136 (A 32/13; B 30/16; C 33/12) Number of dropouts: 21 (A 9, B 4, C 8) Inclusion criteria: 6 to 14 years of age, having wetting episodes at least twice a week over 3 consecutive months, and whose parents agreed to allocation of treatment. Exclusion criteria: identifiable LUTS other than NE and those with other disorders, such as diabetes; abnormal urine analysis results; renal, urological, cardiovascular, or neurological diseases; or previously treated with an enuresis alarm or desmopressin Mean (SD) age: A 8.21 (2.05); B 7.97 (2.10); C 8.01 (1.96) Baseline wetting: mean (SD) number wet nights a week: A 4.56 (1.71); B 5.04 (1.58); C 4.73 (1.66) |
|
Interventions | A (n = 45): alarm only. Children were instructed to test the alarm before sleep, with the sound (or vibration) in mind, and the families were instructed to awake the child immediately after onset of the alarm and to send him to the bathroom to finish voiding B (n = 46): desmopressin only. An oral lyophilizate form of desmopressin (melt formulation) was used. The participants were advised to place the tablet under the tongue and to allow it to dissolve completely without water. They started on 120 µg 1 hour before bedtime. If no considerable response was achieved after 2 weeks (> 1 wet night/week), the doses were increased to 240 µg C (n = 45): alarm + desmopressin All participants (groups A, B, C) were advised to urinate just before going to bed and to restrict their fluid intake (not to drink more than sufficient to satisfy thirst) from 1 hour before bedtime. They were also instructed to avoid drinking liquids with a diuretic effect at night, to keep regular sleep hours, and to keep a record of nightwetting episodes. In addition, the children used a star chart for recording the number of dry and wet nights and compliance with treatment Duration of treatment: 12 weeks Duration of follow‐up: 12 weeks after end of treatment Dosage of desmopressin: 120 µg, increasing to 240 µg if clinically necessary |
|
Outcomes | Wet nights on treatment = mean (SD) wet nights at end of treatment Mean (SD) wet nights at end of follow‐up Number not achieving 14 consecutive dry nights (failure of therapy) Complete response (no wet nights) at end of treatment Non‐response (defined as < 50% reduction in the number of wet nights) at end of treatment Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up Adverse events |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Randomly divided into 3 groups through a computer randomisation programme |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible to blind participants |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No differential attrition, ITT analysis carried out Quote: "Counting defaulters due to noncompliance issues as a form of failure" |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Low risk | Outcomes reported in full |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Azrin 1974.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting: not mentioned | |
Participants | Setting: respondents to a newspaper advertisement for enuretics Number of children (boys): 26 (19) Exclusion criteria: medical causes Ages: 3+, mean 8 years Baseline wetting: 7 days/week. |
|
Interventions | Experiment 1
A (n = 7): DBT with alarm that woke parent and child
B (n = 7): standard (child‐only) alarm for first 2 weeks, then DBT with alarm that woke parent and child Experiment 2 C (n = 6): DBT with alarm that woke only parent D (n = 6): standard (child‐only) alarm for first 2 weeks, then DBT with alarm that woke only parent |
|
Outcomes | Median wet nights in first 2 weeks Numbers achieving 6 dry nights | |
Notes | Pairs matched for age, sex and frequency of wetting No useable data | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Each pair was randomly assigned by a coin flip |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Investigators did not know the upcoming assignments |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | 2 children remained dry in the first 2 weeks of treatment for more than 6 nights in the standard urine alarm group. Possibly dropped out and not included in the analysis. Dropout < 10% |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Pairs matched for age, sex and frequency of wetting at randomisation |
Azrin 1978.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (details not given) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Setting: respondents to a newspaper advertisement for enuretics Number of children (boys): 55 (41) Number of dropouts: 4 Inclusion criteria: age at least 3 years; no daytime wetting; wetting at least 4 times a week; able to understand instructions Age: mean 7.1 years (range 3 to 14) (20 < 6 years) Baseline wetting: 91% of nights |
|
Interventions | A (n = 28): intensive DBT plus rehearsing during the day with increased fluid intake, stream interruption exercises, retention control training, repeated awakening, rewards for dry nights or compliance but no alarm B (n = 27): alarm (pad and buzzer) Duration of trial: 2 weeks, after which parents could swap to other arm | |
Outcomes | % wet nights during first 2 weeks Number of children swapping to other group after 2 weeks of treatment | |
Notes | Comparability of groups at baseline not reported
No follow‐up possible after 2 weeks
No SDs
Very young children (20 < 6 years) included Reasons for children not swapping groups after 2 weeks treatment: A: 27/28 were dry most nights; 1/28 dropped out after first night B: 3/27 were dry most nights; 1/27 thought DBT inconvenient |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Randomly assigned but method not given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | 2 weeks treatment results presented for 54 children. 1 dropped out after 1 night of treatment. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Comparability of groups at baseline not reported Systematic baseline measure of wetting: mentioned Organic causes excluded: mentioned Daytime wetting excluded: mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Bak 2007.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: not mentioned Organic causes excluded: unknown Daytime wetting specifically excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: teaching hospital, single centre Country: Turkey Number of participants (boys): 101 (67) Number of dropouts: not mentioned Inclusion criteria: nocturnal enuresis Exclusion criteria: not mentioned Mean age: 10.7 ± 2.4 years (range 5 to 16 years) Baseline wetting: 14.9 ± 6.1 days a month overall A = 14.5 ± 5.7 B = 14.1 ± 5.9 C = 16.2 ± 6.9 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 33): desmopressin B (n = 34): alarm C (n = 34): combined Duration of treatment: 6 months Duration of follow‐up: 8 months (2 months after end of treatment) Dosage of desmopressin not recorded |
|
Outcomes | Wet nights on treatment Number not achieving 14 consecutive dry nights (failure of therapy) Complete response Failed or relapsed Relapse |
|
Notes | Abstract only Have contacted author for clarification about excluded patients |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Said to be "randomised" but details not mentioned |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Missing outcome data, only percentages given |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Abstract only, insufficient information given about prespecified outcomes |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Abstract only, insufficient information to judge other risks |
Baker 1969.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned | |
Participants | Number of children (boys): 30 (20) Inclusion criteria: recruited from newspaper advertisement, primary (26) and secondary (4) enuresis Age: median 8 years, range 6 to 12 Baseline wetting: half wetting every night | |
Interventions | A (n = 10): alarm B (n = 10): wake up using alarm clock C (n = 10): waiting list control Duration of treatment: 10 weeks, or 50 wet episodes | |
Outcomes | Mean number of wet nights a week in last 3 weeks of treatment Adverse events | |
Notes | Waiting list controls subsequently given an intervention, results not presented separately No SDs given | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Enuretics were randomly assigned to each of the 3 experimental conditions |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Outcome assessors were parents |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | 3 participants excluded because they did not complete treatment |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Adverse events mentioned Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Bennett 1985.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: not mentioned Daytime wetting excluded: yes if only negligible | |
Participants | Number of children (boys): 40 (25) Dropouts: 32 (A: 9; B: 11; C: 10; D: 3) Inclusion criteria: primary nocturnal enuresis, referred to enuresis service by general practitioner, negligible daytime wetting Exclusion criteria: encopresis, previous behavioural intervention, gross psychopathology Age: mean 8.5 years (SD 3.2), range 5 to 12 Baseline wetting: dry nights, boys 3/14; girls 2.4/14; A: 3.7/14 (SD 3.1); B: 2.8/14 (2.9); C: 2.7/14 (2.3); D: 1.9/14 (2.1) | |
Interventions | A (n = 9): alarm (bell and pad)
B (n = 12): stop‐start training (sphincter muscle exercises, bladder training)
C (n = 10): DBT including alarm
D (n = 9): waiting list control (used star chart after first dry night)
Duration of treatment: 10 weeks Follow‐up: 12 weeks after end of treatment |
|
Outcomes | Mean dry nights in last 2 weeks of 12 weeks follow‐up Number achieving 14 dry nights Mean dry nights per fortnight at follow‐up | |
Notes | All children got star charts after their first dry night High dropout rate: A 0%, B 8%, C 0%, D 33% | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Eligible children were randomly assigned to one of four treatments but then dropouts were replaced by others (not randomly). |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | High dropouts replaced by others |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Unbalanced attrition across groups. Withdrawal: A 0%, B 8%, C 0%, D 33% |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Not all prespecified outcomes mentioned Side effects not mentioned Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: not mentioned Daytime wetting excluded: yes if only negligible |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Bollard 1981a.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned | |
Participants | Setting: outpatient service Number of children (boys): 45 (A: 11, B: 11, C: 10) Number of dropouts: A: 0, B: 3, C: 0 Inclusion criteria: no underlying organic pathology Previous treatment: no details Mean age (years.months): A: 9.10 B: 9.9 C: 9.5 (range 5 to 14 years) Baseline wetting: mean number of wet nights a week; A: 5.3, B: 5.4, C: 4.2 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 15): enuresis alarm ‐ supervised (weekly follow‐up) B (n = 15): enuresis alarm ‐ unsupervised C (n = 15): waiting list control Duration of treatment: until achievement of 14 consecutive dry nights or 20 weeks Follow‐up: 3, 6 and 12 months |
|
Outcomes | Mean number of wet nights at end of 20 weeks Number achieving 14 consecutive dry nights Number relapsing at 12 month follow‐up |
|
Notes | No blinding Comparability of groups not reported Graphical data No SDs No baseline data for control group 2 analyses provided: a) intention‐to‐treat basis; b) excluding dropouts | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Children were assigned at random to 1 of 3 groups |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Children were selected from a waiting list in accordance with hospital policy that those waiting longest should be treated first and that older children be given priority. Not enough information |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Data are presented including and excluding the 3 dropouts |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned Comparability of groups: not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Bollard 1981b.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned | |
Participants | Setting: outpatient service Number of children: 100 Number of boys: A: 14, B: 13, C: 16, D: 14, E: 14, F: 11 Number of dropouts: 12 from D Inclusion criteria: thorough medical examination normal; regularly wetting at least 1 night a week; no other treatment during trial Previous treatment: no details Mean age (years.months): A: 9.3, B: 8.11, C: 9.7, D: 8.6, E: 8.8, F: 8.10 (range 4 to 15) Baseline wetting: mean number of wet nights: A: 5.8, B: 5.2, C: 6.0, D: 5.7, E: 6.0, F: 4.7 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 20): DBT (and alarm) with therapist at home
B (n = 20): DBT (and alarm) with therapist at hospital
C (n = 20): DBT (and alarm) with parents as therapists at home
D (n = 20): DBT with parents as therapists at home without enuresis alarm
E (n = 20): alarm
F (n = 20): waiting list control Duration of treatment: until 14 consecutive dry nights or 20 weeks Follow‐up at 3, 6 and 12 months |
|
Outcomes | Mean number of wet nights a week at end of week 20 Number achieving 14 consecutive dry nights Number relapsing |
|
Notes | No details of blinding DBT no alarm group (D): younger than others and more girls in waiting list control group (F) No SDs Analysed on intention‐to‐treat basis and with dropouts included | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Children were assigned at random to 1 of 3 groups |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Data presented with 12 dropouts |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | More girls in waiting list control group |
Bollard 1982a.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: mainly RCT but also comparison with previous study [A] and [H] from another study Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned | |
Participants | Setting: outpatient service Number of children (2 groups combined) (boys): 127 (88) Inclusion criteria: no underlying organic pathology Previous treatment: many had previously sought help but none undergoing any form of enuresis‐related drug or psychotherapy at the time of the study Mean age: 9 years 10 months (range 5 to 17 years) Baseline wetting: overall mean number of wet nights a week = 5.5 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 35): alarm only (A)
B (n = 12): alarm (A) + waking schedule (W)
C (n = 12): A + retention control training
D (n = 12): A + positive practice (PP) + cleanliness training (CT)
E (n = 12): A + W + retention control training F (n = 12): A + W + PP + CT G (n = 12): A + retention control training + PP + CT H (n = 20): full DBT Duration of treatment: 20 weeks Follow‐up: none |
|
Outcomes | Mean number of wet nights during 20‐week treatment period Number of cases becoming dry |
|
Notes | Groups A and H from another trial, data not used No analysis of comparability of groups No blinding No SDs No follow‐up | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | No analysis of comparability of groups Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Bradbury 1995.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (quota allocation system based on age, baseline wetting, family or housing problems, gender, previous alarm use, daytime wetting and previous dry periods) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: no | |
Participants | Setting: single centre Country: England Number of children: 71 (boys 48) Dropouts: A: 3, B: 8 (lost to follow‐up) Inclusion criteria: nocturnal enuresis at least 1 night a week (40/71 = severe > 4 times a week) Exclusion criteria: neuropathic bladder, urinary tract abnormalities, cystic fibrosis, allergic rhinitis, deafness/learning difficulties, UTI Mean age: 6.6 ± 2.9 years (range 6 to 15 years) Previous treatment: 29 had used alarms Baseline wetting: mean (SD) wet beds per fortnight: 10.6 (4.9) |
|
Interventions | A (n = 36): desmopressin 40 µg intranasally and alarm (bell and pad or Mini Drinite)
B (n = 35): alarm only
Duration 6 weeks or until dry 6‐month follow‐up |
|
Outcomes | Mean dry nights a week Number not achieving 14 dry nights Number failing or relapsing Side effects | |
Notes | Mini Drinite = body‐worn alarm Relapsing = 2 wet nights in 2 weeks after 4 weeks dry Authors recommended using combined desmopressin and alarm only for children with severe wetting problems | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned
Quote: "Quota allocation system" Comment: unsure if this is stratification |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Even though reasons for dropouts were given, more children dropped out of the alarm monotherapy group, which may be related to poorer treatment response |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Low risk | All prespecified outcomes have been reported |
Other bias | Low risk | The study appears to be free of other sources of bias |
Bryant 2003.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: no | |
Participants | Setting: community continence clinic in Sydney Country: Australia Number of children (boys): 116 (86) Dropouts: 55 Inclusion criteria: children presenting to community continence clinic with nocturnal enuresis Exclusion criteria: developmental disability, voiding dysfunction, UTI, urinary tract abnormalities Age: 5 to 14 years Baseline wetting (number of wet nights per 28 nights): A: 26, B: 25, C: 24 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 34): body‐worn alarm B (n = 37): bladder training C (n = 45): body‐worn alarm and bladder training Duration of treatment: unknown Follow‐up: at 16 weeks, 24 weeks and 52 weeks. | |
Outcomes | Mean number of wet nights per 28 nights, measured at at 16, 26 and 52 weeks | |
Notes | Author contacted, additional information provided. Duration of treatment variable. Number achieving 14 consecutive dry nights was not given Other outcomes assessed were: mean net wet pull‐up weight, diurnal average/maximum voided volume Adverse events not mentioned |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer random number generated (contact with author) |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Central allocation (contact with author) |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Impossibe to bind participants. Personnel not blinded (contact with author) |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | 55/116 children withdrew, outcomes not reported |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Not all prespecified outcomes mentioned. Daytime wetting not excluded. No SD given. Adverse events not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Butler 1988.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned | |
Participants | Setting: outpatients Number of children (boys): 74 (A: 18, B: 29) Dropouts: 11 excluded after baseline assessments Inclusion criteria: age at least 6 years; wetting at least 5 nights a week for a month; normal clinical exam; normal urine on microscopy; normal intelligence (assessed by reference to educational background and parental‐child interview); not having any form of enuresis‐related drug or psychotherapeutic treatment Previous treatment: 36 (48.6%) enuresis alarm Mean age: A: 8.99, B: 9.86 Baseline wetting: mean number of dry nights during 4 weeks; A: 1.07, B: 1.02 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 28): standard enuresis alarm treatment (A) B (n = 35): modified DBT + alarm (A + W + PP + retention control training) without reprimands during CT Duration of treatment: 16 weeks Follow‐up: none | |
Outcomes | Mean number of dry nights in last 4 weeks Number of children achieving 14 dry nights | |
Notes | No significant difference between groups for demographic factors but modified‐DBT group more likely to have previously used alarm. Analysis of covariance adjusted for the effects of previous experience with enuresis alarm No blinding Not intention‐to‐treat No SDs | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Children were randomly assigned to treatment according to a predetermined schedule |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Dropout > 20%. Not included in analysis |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Daytime wetting exclusion not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Butler 1990a.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: CCT (alternate allocation) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned | |
Participants | Setting: outpatients Number of children: 40 (boys A: 14, B: 11) Dropouts: A: 3, B: 2 Inclusion criteria: wetting at least 4 nights a week for a month; normal physical examination; normal urine microscopy; normal intelligence (assessed by reference to educational background and parent/child interview) Previous treatment: none Mean age: A: 8.2, B: 9.1 (range 6.1 to 15.6 years) Baseline wetting: mean number of dry nights per week: A: 1.2, B: 0.7 No significant difference between groups on any variable |
|
Interventions | A (n = 20): pad and bell alarm
B (n = 20): body‐worn alarm Duration of treatment: 16 weeks Follow‐up after 6 months |
|
Outcomes | Mean number of wet nights in 16 weeks Number (%) children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights Mean number of wet nights until achievement of 14 consecutive dry nights Number (%) children relapsing |
|
Notes | Small groups Experiment 1 No blinding Unclear if intention‐to‐treat analysis No SDs |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Not random allocation |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Participants were assigned alternately to treatment |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Unclear intention‐to‐treat analysis Dropout rate not mentioned |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Daytime wetting at baseline not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Unclear |
Butler 1990b.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: CCT (alternate allocation) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Setting: outpatients Number of children: 48 (boys A: 20, B:20) Number of dropouts: A: 2, B: 1 Inclusion: wetting at least 4 nights a week for a month; normal physical examination; normal urine microscopy; normal intelligence (assessed by reference to educational background and parent/child interview); no associated diurnal enuresis Previous treatment: unsuccessful treatment with pad and bell alarm Mean age (years): A: 10.2, B: 11.2 (range 7.4 to 14.7 years) Severity at baseline: mean number of dry nights a week: A: 1.2, B: 1.3 Groups did not differ significantly on any variable |
|
Interventions | A (n = 24): modified DBT + pad and bell alarm (A + W + retention control training)
B (n = 24): body‐worn alarm (A) Duration of treatment: 16 weeks Follow‐up after 6 months |
|
Outcomes | Mean number of wet nights in 16 weeks Number (%) attaining 14 consecutive dry nights Mean number of wet nights to achievement of 14 consecutive dry nights Number (%) children relapsing |
|
Notes | Experiment 2 Unclear if intention‐to‐treat analysis No SDs Dry bed training included a pad and bell alarm, a waking schedule and retention control training |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Not random allocation |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Children were allocated alternately to treatment |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Adverse events not mentioned |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Unclear intention‐to‐treat analysis |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Caceres 1982.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT, "double blind" Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: no Daytime wetting excluded: no | |
Participants | Setting: outpatients Number of children (boys): 14 (9) Inclusion criteria: enuresis, or behaviour problem + enuresis. Some were not daytime toilet‐trained Previous treatment: all had failed with psychotherapy, drugs or fluid restriction Age: mean 9 years (range 6 to 14) Baseline wetting: every night |
|
Interventions | A (n = 7): enuresis alarm (Mowrer's pad and bell)
B (n = 7): DBT (but without alarm) + rewards Duration: 1 month, then crossed over to other arm if not 50% improved |
|
Outcomes | Not cured on original treatment | |
Notes | Children crossed over to alternative treatment if not successful (5 of B group changed to A) Cure rates given while on first treatment. We did not use cross‐over data |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Randomly assigned to treatment but no details |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Parents collected follow‐up data |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | All participants accounted for in first 4 weeks. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Organic causes excluded: no Daytime wetting excluded: no |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Caldwell 2015.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting specifically excluded: no |
|
Participants | Setting/recruitment: tertiary paediatric centre in Sydney; children were referred by their family doctor or paediatrician Country: Australia Number of participants (boys): 353 (A 109/176, B 107/177) Number of dropouts: A: 50, B: 68 Inclusion criteria: aged 6 to 18 years with enuresis at least 3 nights a week in the preceding 6 months, who were referred by their family doctor or paediatrician to the study for alarm therapy Exclusion criteria: children with enuresis secondary to neurological or urological problems Mean median age: A: 8.4, B: 8.1 Baseline wetting (in preceding 6 months) n (%): Less than once a month: A: 1 (1), B: 1 (1) 1 to 3 times a month: A: 1 (1), B: 0 1 to 2 times a week: A: 6 (3), B: 7 (4) 3 to 7 times a week: A: 111 (63), B: 119 (67) More than once a night: A: 55 (31), B: 50 (28) Not sure: A: 2 (1), B: 0 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 176): code‐word alarm. The code‐word alarm delivered a prerecorded personalised code word when the child wet at night. Children were encouraged to remember the code word the next morning. The code‐word alarm comprised a wireless wetness sensor (secured in a pad worn inside the child’s underpants) and an alarm box (containing a digital voice recorder for parents to record personalised code words for their child) located beside the child’s bed. When wetting occurred, moisture was detected by the sensor and the alarm sounded. When the child woke and deactivated the alarm, the prerecorded code word was announced and the child was encouraged by their parents to wake and void in the toilet. This code word changed daily B (n = 177): control alarm. Commercially‐available alarm. The attachment of the sensor and location of the alarm box were similar to the experimental group. When wetting occurred, the alarm sounded with an identical noise to the code‐word alarm and the child was encouraged by their parents to wake and void in the toilet. However, there was no prerecorded code word with the control alarm Duration of treatment: 16 weeks Duration of follow‐up: 6 months |
|
Outcomes | Number not achieving response (50% or more reduction in wetting) Relapse (within 6 months) Participants with adverse events Sleep disturbance Frightened by the alarm Alarm triggering sleepwalking/nightmares Children satisfied with treatment |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer‐generated and stratified according to gender and age by the method of minimisation and performed by an independent randomisation service |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Allocation occurred through a telephone call to the service, with investigators, clinicians and data analysts unaware of group assignment |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Families were aware of their assigned alarm but were unaware which was the ‘novel’ arm because the control alarm was not commercially available in Australia at the time of the study and therefore both alarms looked unfamiliar |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | ITT ‐ included participants who discontinued alarm training before achieving a full response in the denominator |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Low risk | Outcomes reported in full |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Danquah 1975.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT but mention of matching Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned | |
Participants | Setting: Ghanaian fishing community Number of children: 30 (all boys) Exclusion criteria: more than a week of traditional treatment Mean age: 10.4 years Mean number of wet nights a week at baseline: A: 6.7, B: 6.07, C: 6.50 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 10): traditional shaming (consisted of being carried from home by a singing mob and being thrown into the lagoon)
B (n = 10): amitriptyline hydrochloride
C (n = 10): alarm Duration of treatment: 7 weeks Follow‐up after 3 months |
|
Outcomes | Mean number wet nights a week at end of treatment Mean number wet nights a week at follow‐up |
|
Notes | No details of dropouts No SDs No details of previous treatment Groups comparable in age and intelligence | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | All t30 children were matched as closely as possible in age and measured intelligence. The 3 groups of 10 each were then allocated randomly to the 3 treatments |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Attrition was not mentioned |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Response to treatment defined as 21 consecutive dry nights. Relapse defined as "a return to a wetting frequency of once or more per week after initial response to treatment…" Results for these were not presented. Only frequency of wetting was presented. Daytime wetting exclusion not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Elinder 1985.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Setting: outpatient Number of children (boys): 53 (45) Number of dropouts: A: 9, B: 6 due to technical problems or discomfort Inclusion: aged at least 7 years; primary nocturnal enuresis; at least 3 wet nights a week; no daytime wetting Exclusion: physical or psychological/psychiatric disease Previous treatment: tricyclics (A: 20, B: 7); alarm (1, 2) Ages: 39 7 to 11 years; 14 over 11 years |
|
Interventions | A (n = 36): functioning Uristop device B (n = 17): non‐functioning Uristop device Duration of treatment: 6 weeks Follow‐up 12 months | |
Outcomes | Number not cured Adverse events |
|
Notes | Device delivers electric impulse to pudendal nerve in groin when urine is passed Power calculation given Groups comparable at baseline, except more upsetting life events and psychiatric contact in B. Failure ascribed to incorrect theory or incorrect construction (wrong placement of electrodes or impulse too low) | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Randomised but no other details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Both groups received Uristop alarm but uncertain whether functioning device could be differentiated clinically from nonfunctioning device by children and personnel |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Both groups received Uristop alarm but uncertain whether functioning device could be differentiated clinically from nonfunctioning device by parents assessing outcome |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | 25% dropout rate |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Baseline comparison given; 1 group with higher rate of upsetting life events and previous psychiatric contact Higher dropout rate in non‐functioning Uristop group No intention‐to‐treat analysis |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Evans 2011.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systemic baseline measure of wetting: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting/recruitment: multicentre, 29 clinics, April to July 2004 Country: UK Number of participants (boys): 251 (182) Number of dropouts: end of treatment: A: 78, B; 34. End of follow‐up: A: 85, B: 34 Inclusion criteria: untreated primary nocturnal enuresis or treated > 1 year ago and/or for < 4 weeks. 6 or more wet nights a fortnight during screening Exclusion criteria: diurnal symptoms, renal or central DI, UTI in past month, SIADH ± cardiac failure, encopresis, clinically significant diseases or medications Ages: 5 to 15 years Mean age: A: 5.6; B: 5.5 Baseline wetting: A: 5.6 nights a week; B: 5.5 nights a week |
|
Interventions | A (n = 192): desmopressin B (n = 59): alarm Duration of treatment: 6 months Duration of follow‐up: 12 months |
|
Outcomes | Mean wet nights at end of treatment Number not achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at end of treatment Mean wet nights at end of follow‐up Number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up Adverse events Other: compliance, mean sleep duration |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer‐generated block randomisation by centre |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Central phone allocation |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | No blinding possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | No blinding possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | High loss to follow‐up: A 85/192(44%); B 34/59 (58%) |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Low risk | All prespecified outcomes reported. Analysis by ITT and per protocol (i.e. treatment received, excluding early dropouts) |
Other bias | High risk | Differential dropout rate with higher dropout in alarm group (32.2% vs 6.8%) |
Fagundes 2017.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting specifically excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting/recruitment: Pediatric Nephrology and Sleep Physiology Units of Instituto da Criança of Hospital das Clinicas of the University of São Paulo Medical School, the University of São Paulo Physical Therapy Division, and the Behavioral Therapy Unit of the Department of Psychology of the University of São Paulo, Brazil Number of participants (boys): 75 (not reported) Number of dropouts: 14/75 Inclusion criteria: age 6 to 16, monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis diagnosed by multidisciplinary assessment, complete Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) questionnaire Exclusion criteria: apnoea–hypopnoea index of > 10 events an hour, consistent with severe apnoea, were referred to receive specialised treatment and excluded from the intervention protocol. Neurological, genetic and chronic renal conditions, NMNE and severe known comorbidities Mean (SD) age: 9.5 (2.7) Baseline wetting: 65/82 participants had ≥ 4 episodes/week |
|
Interventions | A (n = 30): alarm only, ‘Bell and carpet’ produced by the University of São Paulo Behavior Therapy Laboratory of the Psychology Institute B (n = 20): desmopressin only, participants were instructed to avoid water intake for a period of 1 ‐ 2 hours before desmopressin administration C (n = 25): alarm and desmopressin Duration of treatment: 4 ‐ 7 months and/or until the child could achieve 14 consecutive dry nights Duration of follow‐up: 12 months after end of treatment Dosage of desmopressin: 0.2 mg. Participants who did not achieve a > 50% reduction in the number of NE episodes after 30 days of use were instructed to double the dose of the medication. Therapy was maintained for a period of 4 months, followed by a gradual dose reduction of 0.1 mg every 2 weeks for those on a daily dose of 0.2 mg or alternate days for 30 to 60 days for those on a daily dose of 0.4 mg, respectively, in an attempt to minimise recurrence |
|
Outcomes | Quote: "Response to treatment was classified using the ICCS 2014 criteria for initial success." Number not achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Quote: "randomized by draw into the three treatment groups" |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible to blind participants, other blinding not reported |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Withdrawals during treatment: A 9/30; B 1/20; C 4/25 |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Not all outcomes mentioned; no report of adverse effects |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Baseline data not reported by treatment group. |
Faraj 1999.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Number of children: 135
Dropouts: 23 (A) and 36 (B) non‐compliant or lost to follow‐up Failed and changed treatment at 3 months: 12 (A) and 6 (B) Inclusion criteria: monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis, age > 5 years Exclusion criteria: previous treatment with DDAVP or alarm, urological pathology, diurnal enuresis, UTI Mean age: 11.2 (range 6 to 16 years) Baseline wetting: A: 21% dry nights, B: 14% dry nights |
|
Interventions | A (n = 62): desmopressin 20 µg intranasally increasing to 40 µg if response partial
B (n = 73): alarm (pad and bell)
Duration of treatment: 6 months Changed to alternative arm if failed initial treatment at 3 months Follow‐up: 4 ‐ 6 months |
|
Outcomes | Dry nights at 3 months
Dry nights at 6 months Number not achieving 14 dry nights at 3 months |
|
Notes | Abstract only French study Differential dropout rate with higher dropout rate in alarm group (50%) |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Random‐number tables |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Unable to blind |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Unable to blind |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Many children excluded due to non‐compliance, loss to follow‐up or treatment switch during study |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Not all prespecified outcomes mentioned Adverse events not mentioned |
Other bias | High risk | Many children switched treatments during the study if no response at 3 months |
Fielding 1980.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes (study included day‐ and nightwetting in a parallel design but daytime wetting excluded for this review) | |
Participants | Setting: outpatient clinics Number of children (boys): 45 (6 lost at baseline) (30) Dropouts: 11 Inclusion criteria: age 5 to 15; no urinary tract infection; no evidence of organic pathology; not treated within previous 12 months; no daytime wetting Age range: 5 years 2 months to 13 years 10 months Baseline wetting: mean number of wet nights in 4 weeks: A: 23.5, B: 24.7 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 16): retention control training (4 weeks) + alarm (14 weeks)
B (n = 17): alarm (14 weeks) Note: total randomised (45) less dropouts (11) = 34 participants (data given only for 33) Duration of treatment: retention control training 4 weeks and alarm for 14 weeks Follow‐up after 3, 6 and 12 months |
|
Outcomes | Mean number of wet nights in third month of alarm Number not achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment Number (%) relapsing after 3, 6 and 12 months Number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing after 12 months | |
Notes | High number of dropouts were excluded from analysis: 11/45 children who were randomised withdrew from the study and were not included in the analysis. Intention‐to‐treat analysis for children completing study Parallel study specifically includes diurnal wetters (results not given here) Not reported if comparable groups Data given for first month of treatment before alarms were introduced for group A. Thus outcomes for retention control (without alarm) can be compared with alarm monotherapy: mean wetting frequency was 5.8/week 6 for retention control training; 1.3/week for alarm training |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Participants were randomly allocated to treatment but details not given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Analysis excluded dropouts post‐randomisation |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Adverse events: not mentioned |
Other bias | High risk | Group A (retention control) also had alarm training after 4 weeks, so treatment outcomes for group A likely to be confounded |
Finley 1973.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Setting: Children's Medical Centre, Tulsa, Oklahoma Country: USA Number of children (boys): 30 (30) Inclusion criteria: primary nocturnal enuresis, at least 3 wet nights a week, own room and bed Exclusion criteria: daytime wetting, organic cause for enuresis, emotional disturbance Ages: 6 ‐ 8 years Baseline wetting: 7 ‐ 8 wet episodes a week |
|
Interventions | A (n = 10): enuresis alarm (105 dB bell) + light (continuous reinforcement) B (n = 10): enuresis alarm (80 dB bell) with intermittent reinforcement (70% active) C (n = 10): alarm (78 dB) ring in parents' room 20 minutes after wetting Duration of treatment: 6 weeks Follow‐up: 3 months | |
Outcomes | Mean wet episodes during sixth week
Number not achieving 7 dry nights Number dry at 6 weeks Number failing or relapsing after 'cure' |
|
Notes | Groups were blindly constituted, parents were not aware of the other available treatments No SDs Data estimated from graph |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Children were assigned on a random basis but details not given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Unclear as data estimated from graph |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | No systematic baseline measure of wetting. |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Unclear |
Finley 1977.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Setting: Children's Medical Centre, Tulsa, Oklahoma Country: USA Number of children (boys): 20 (20) Inclusion criteria: primary nocturnal enuresis Exclusion criteria: emotional disturbance, organic causes, daytime wetting Age: 6 ‐ 9 years Baseline wetting: 6 ‐ 7 wet nights a week |
|
Interventions | A (n = 10): alarm with 105 dB bell
B (n = 10): alarm with 80 dB bell Duration of treatment: 7 weeks Follow‐up: 16 to 24 months |
|
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights Relapse rate Failed or relapsed | |
Notes | Group comparability at baseline not stated | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Boys were assigned treatment on a random basis but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Unclear, technician installed the alarm |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Parents were outcome assessors |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Group comparability at baseline not stated No systematic baseline measure of wetting |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Fournier 1987.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (double‐blind) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting: not mentioned | |
Participants | Setting: newspaper advertisements and local paediatricians Number of children (boys): 64 (47) completed the study; 5 extra children dropped out Inclusion criteria: no treatment in past 3 months Mean age: 8 years 5 months (range 5 to 14 years) Baseline wetting: mean number of wet nights in week 2: A: 5.3, B: 6, C: 4.5, D: 4.2, E: 4.5 Differences in baseline severity of wetting: MANOVA used |
|
Interventions | A (n = 8): imipramine B (n = 8): enuresis alarm C (n = 8): placebo D (n = 8): random awakening E (n = 8): alarm + imipramine F (n = 8): alarm + placebo G (n = 8): random awakening + placebo H (n = 8): imipramine + random awakening Duration of treatment: 6 weeks Follow‐up: 3 months, but some children continued on treatments | |
Outcomes | Mean number of wet nights a week | |
Notes | Parallel groups Differences in baseline severity of wetting ‐ MANOVA used 5 dropouts (4 side effects or non‐compliance, 1 UTI) No SDs given For random waking, parents were instructed to wake the child to void any time before midnight |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Random assignment to one of the treatment groups |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Children, parents and the psychiatrist interviewing the children were blind to the treatment the child received. Following a double‐blind procedure, a child received active medication of 25 mg tablets of Tofranil or the placebo, which was an identical nonlactose tablet. Only the tablets could be blinded. Alarm and random awakening could not be blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible for all groups |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | No details of dropouts No outcomes measured for 3 groups |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Results were related to dry/wet nights over period of study Adverse events not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Geffken 1986a.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT
Children were stratified by maximal functional bladder capacity Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: outpatient service Number of children: n = 50 in total study; part a = large bladder capacity (n = 25) (boys: A: 8, C: 6) 10 dropouts in total study Inclusion criteria: nocturnal enuresis of at least 3 months duration; at least 2 wetting episodes a week Mean age: A: 9.0, C: 9.4 (range 5 to 13 years) Baseline wetting: mean (SD) number of wet nights a week A: 4.9 (1.7), C: 5.4 (1.1) |
|
Interventions | Large maximal functional bladder capacity:
A (n = 10): alarm
C (n = 10): alarm + retention control training Duration of treatment: 14 weeks Follow‐up: after 8 or more weeks |
|
Outcomes | Mean (SD) number of wet nights a week Significant interaction between maximal functional bladder capacity and treatment Number of children achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment Number of children relapsing during follow‐up |
|
Notes | 25 randomised to treatment Children who failed to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights at end of treatment were given the option to continue alarm training beyond 14 weeks Therefore, mean number of wet nights at follow‐up after cessation of treatment not used in analysis Not intention‐to‐treat analysis Short follow‐up No details of previous treatment Payment required For those who completed treatment there were no significant differences between the groups in terms of sex, age, child adjustment measures or the Tolerance and Nuisance Scales Retention control training consisted of increasing fluid intake and delaying urination for increasing periods of time to expand bladder capacity |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Participants were randomly assigned to treatment and stratified by maximal functional bladder capacity but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | No data on dropouts (20%) |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | No intention‐to‐treat analysis done |
Other bias | High risk | Charges for treatment were based on the parent's ability to pay and ranged from USD 15 to USD 150. Short follow‐up |
Geffken 1986b.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned | |
Participants | Setting: outpatient service Number of children (boys): n = 50 in total study; part b = small bladder capacity (n = 25) (Boys: B: 5, D: 6) 10 dropouts in total study Inclusion criteria: nocturnal enuresis of at least 3 months duration; at least 2 wetting episodes a week Mean age: B: 7.7, D: 8.0 Baseline wetting: Mean (SD) number of wet nights a week B: 5.7 (1.3), D: 4.9 (1.2) |
|
Interventions | Small maximal functional bladder capacity:
B (n = 10): alarm
D (n = 10): alarm + retention control training Duration of treatment: 14 weeks Follow‐up: after 8 or more weeks |
|
Outcomes | Mean (SD) number of wet nights a week Significant interaction between maximal functional bladder capacity and treatment Number of children achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment Number of children relapsing during follow‐up |
|
Notes | Children who failed to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights at end of treatment were given the option to continue alarm training beyond 14 weeks. Therefore, mean number of wet nights at follow‐up after cessation of treatment not used in analysis Not intention‐to‐treat analysis Short follow‐up No details of previous treatment Payment required For those who completed treatment there were no significant differences between the groups in terms of sex, age, child adjustment measures or the Tolerance and Nuisance Scales Retention control training consisted of increasing fluid intake and delaying urination for increasing periods of time to expand bladder capacity |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Participants were randomly assigned to treatment but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | No data on dropouts (20%) |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | No intention‐to‐treat analysis done. |
Other bias | High risk | Charges for treatment were based on the parent's ability to pay and ranged from USD 15 to USD 150. Short follow‐up |
Gibb 2004.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (drug dispensed randomly by pharmacist) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: no |
|
Participants | Setting: paediatric outpatients, Children's Hospital, Melbourne Country: Australia Number of children (boys): 207/210 (A: 64, B: 78) Dropouts: 10 eligible children declined, incomplete data on A: 9/101, B: 17/106, but dropouts counted as failures for analysis Inclusion criteria: non‐responders to desmopressin treatment (< 50% reduction in wet nights), age 6 to 16 years, wetting at least twice a week, some daytime wetting (A: 11, B: 8) Exclusion criteria: neuropathic bladder, urinary tract abnormality, cystic fibrosis, allergic rhinitis, UTI in previous 2 weeks, imipramine or diuretics Previous treatment: some had alarm (A: 37, B: 32) or desmopressin (A: 31, B: 28) Age: mean 9.4 years (SD 2.08) Baseline wet nights in 28 days: A: 23.9 (SD 5.05), B: 23.7 (5.83) |
|
Interventions | A (n = 84/101): desmopressin (40 µg nasal spray) + alarm (pad and bell)
B (n = 85/106): placebo (nasal spray) + alarm (pad and bell) Duration of treatment: 8 weeks Follow‐up: 2 months |
|
Outcomes | Cure = 28 dry nights, relapse = 2 wet nights in 2 weeks Wet nights during treatment (number, mean (SD)) Cure during treatment Relapse after treatment stopped Failed or relapsed Adverse events Cure in daytime wetting | |
Notes | Intention‐to‐treat analysis Groups comparable at baseline on age, wetting, gender, family history, secondary enuresis, daytime wetting and previous treatment | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Low risk | All blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Low risk | All blinded |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | High dropout rate, but author reported that reasons for dropout were similar between groups. |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Low risk | Low risk but exclusion of daytime urinary incontinence not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Hojsgaard 1979.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned | |
Participants | Number of children (boys): 62 | |
Interventions | A (n = 32): Uristop device (alarm) B (n = 30): no treatment | |
Outcomes | Cured Improved | |
Notes | Danish language Uristop device gives "electrical stimulation" to the children. | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | "Randomized" but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | No systematic baseline measure of wetting Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not mentioned. Danish language no translation |
Hoseinzadeh 1997.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: unknown Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting specifically excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: children living at home Number of participants (boys): 50 Number of dropouts: unknown Inclusion criteria: healthy children with nightwetting at least twice a week for 3 months; living with their parents Exclusion criteria: other disease or health problem connected with nightwetting, previous treatment for enuresis; abnormal IQ; parents with only primary school level education Age: 6 to 12 years Baseline wetting: unknown |
|
Interventions | A: alarm B: imipramine Duration of treatment: 90 days Duration of follow‐up: 90 days + 45 days = total 135 days (45 days after stopping treatment) |
|
Outcomes | Recurrence 45 days after stopping | |
Notes | Partial translation available; no useable data. We contacted authors of the trial but they did not reply |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | The translators' text suggests they paired up children and then randomised one to receive intervention and other gets the control |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Open random allocation schedule |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible to blind participants; other blinding not reported |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Insufficient information |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Insufficient information |
Houts 1986.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: no Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned | |
Participants | Setting: media recruitment and paediatric referrals Number of children (boys): 45 (35) Dropouts: A: 2, B: 2, C: 3 Inclusion criteria: Primary enuresis Age: 5 to 13 years Baseline wetting: mean 5.41 (SD 1.63) wet nights/week |
|
Interventions | A (n = 15): enuresis alarm + overlearning + retention control training (Full Spectrum Home Training Package) B (n = 15): enuresis alarm + retention control training C (n = 15): enuresis alarm alone D (n = 11): waiting list control Duration of treatment: 16 weeks Follow‐up: 1 year | |
Outcomes | Numbers cured, failed and dropped out Relapse at end of study after retreatment if necessary | |
Notes | Groups comparable at baseline A, B and C received 1‐hour group training and CT Relapses were retreated with initial treatment allocated Children who failed were older and dropouts were younger | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Randomly assigned to treatment but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | All dropouts explained adequately |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Organic causes not excluded and daytime wetting exclusion not mentioned |
Other bias | High risk | Parents had to pay for treatment |
Jehu 1977.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT
Analysis curtailed after 12 weeks to accommodate the loss of some control children Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: children resident in children's homes but attending normal rather than special school so that treatment not impractical (e.g. children only spent weekends or school holidays at the home) Number of children: 39 (boys A: 8 B: 17); 1 dropout Inclusion criteria: age 4 years or over; wetting frequency of at least 4 nights a week during baseline; not previously treated by alarm within last year; no gross physical handicap Previous treatment: drug therapy (7), alarm treatment (2) Mean age: 9 years 4 months (range 4 years 9 months to 14 years 7 months) Baseline wetting: for treatment group only mean number of wet nights a week = 4 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 19): enuresis alarm
B (n = 20): no treatment control Duration of treatment: 3 or 4 months ‐ until success achieved (achieved 14 dry nights) Follow‐up: after 6 months, then 20 months |
|
Outcomes | Mean number of wet nights in week 12 Achieved 14 dry nights Number of children relapsed at 6 and 8 months |
|
Notes | Comparability of groups not reported
No baseline for control; probably should compare from week 4 for control to compensate for this
Not intention‐to‐treat
More girls in alarm group
No SDs Mention of disruption of alarm training to child and staff |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Treatment allocated at random but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | 1 dropout only |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Not intention‐to‐treat analysis Comparability of groups not reported No baseline for control; probably should compare from week 4 for control to compensate for this. |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Kennedy 1968.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (alternate allocation) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned | |
Participants | Setting: A + B clinic attenders and paediatric referrals; C + D residents in Sunland Training Centre, Florida Number of children (boys): A + B: 10 (8), C + D: 8 Inclusion criteria: C and D had learning difficulties Exclusion criteria: organic cause for enuresis Age: A + B: 6 to 12 years, C + D: 9 to 12 years |
|
Interventions | A (n = 5): alarm + methedrine 5 mg B (n = 5): alarm only C (n = 3): alarm + methedrine 5 mg D (n = 5): alarm only Duration of treatment: 8 weeks Follow‐up: 13 months | |
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights | |
Notes | Groups A + C and B + D combined for analysis Baseline comparability not mentioned Drug did not affect outcome but numbers too small to be reliable | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Alternate patients were allocated to 2 arms. |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No loss to follow‐up |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Only wet nights a week measured as outcome, no systematic baseline measure of wetting Adverse events not reported |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Kolvin 1972.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: schools Number of children (boys): 94 (56); 2 dropouts Inclusion criteria: wetting at least 3 nights a week; not receiving treatment elsewhere Previous treatment: no details Mean age: 9 years 4 months (range 8 to 10) Baseline wetting: mean number of wet nights per month A: 22.7, B: 22.0, C: 20.9 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 35): imipramine
B (n = 32): pad and buzzer alarm
C (n = 27): placebo Duration of treatment: 2 months Follow‐up: after 4 months |
|
Outcomes | Mean number of wet night in final month (% improvement) At follow‐up mean number of wet nights a month (% improvement) |
|
Notes | No details of blinding Not reported if comparable groups Not intention‐to treat‐analysis No SDs | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | The children were randomly divided in 3 groups for treatment but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible for alarm group |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Loss to follow‐up at 4 months = 10 (placebo 1; buzzer 4; imipramine 5). No reason given for higher loss to follow‐up in the imipramine group |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Protocol not available Not reported if comparable groups Not intention‐to‐treat analysis |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Kwak 2010#.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: cross‐over RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting: single centre. January 2007 to December 2009 Number of subjects (boys): 104 (45) Number of dropouts: 7 (A: 3, B: 4) Inclusion criteria: children 6 to 15 years, no daytime LUTS Exclusion criteria: LUTS, neuropathic bladder, spinal dysraphism, anatomical abnormality, desmopressin treatment in previous 3 months Ages: 6 to 15 years. Desmopressin: 8.1 ± 2; alarm: 8.6 ± 2.2 Baseline wetting: mean wet night every 4 weeks: desmopressin: 23.1 ± 4.1; alarm: 21.7 ± 4.9 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 54): DDAVP B (n = 50): alarm Duration of treatment: 12 weeks Duration of follow‐up: no follow‐up beyond 12 weeks because of cross‐over, no washout period |
|
Outcomes | Mean % decrease in wet nights at end of treatment (3 months) Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights at end of treatment Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after cure Adverse events |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Random permutated block design |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | No blinding of parents Blinding of study personnel not mentioned Blinding of data analysts not mentioned |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | No blinding possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | All participants were accounted for and reasons for dropout given. Primary outcome data at 3 months for dropouts were included |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Low risk | Reported on all prespecified outcomes |
Other bias | Low risk | None |
Leebeek 2001.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (double‐blind parallel‐group study) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Setting: outpatients Number of children (boys): 93 (62) Number of dropouts: end of treatment 11 (A: 4, B: 7); end of follow‐up 15 (A: 6, B: 9) Inclusion criteria: at least 6 wet nights a week Exclusion criteria: treatment in previous 2 weeks; daytime wetting/urgency; urological or psychological disease; poor motivation to use alarm Previous treatment: none in previous 2 weeks Age: 6 to 14 years Baseline wetting: mean number of wet nights: A: 6.14, B: 6.12 (NS) |
|
Interventions | A (n = 47): alarm and desmopressin 40 µg intranasal for 3 weeks, then alarm and desmopressin 20 µg for 3 weeks, then alarm alone for 3 weeks
B (n = 46): alarm and placebo for 6 weeks, then alarm alone for 3 weeks Follow‐up at 2 weeks and 6 months after end of trial |
|
Outcomes | Wet nights (number of participants, mean) after 3, 6 and 9 weeks Number cured (90% or greater reduction in wet nights a week compared to baseline) 2 weeks and 6 months after end of treatment Wet nights at 6 months Adverse events |
|
Notes | Power calculation provided
Groups comparable for sex and age
SDs not given (authors contacted for more information)
Study supported by drug company (Ferring Pharmaceutical) Analysed on intention‐to‐treat basis and with dropouts included |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Double‐blind, placebo controlled |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Double‐blind, placebo‐controlled |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | All participants accounted for but reasons for dropout not given |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Not all the study's prespecified outcomes were met. No SD to enter data |
Other bias | High risk | Study supported by Ferring (pharmaceutical company for desmopressin) |
Longstaffe 2000.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Setting: recruited from hospital clinic and advertising Number of children: 182 At 6 months, 17 withdrew due to failure (A 8; B 5; C 4) Inclusion criteria: primary monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis, age > 7 years, wet > 3 times a week, normal bladder capacity Exclusion criteria: daytime wetting, CNS disorder, developmental delay, current alarm or DDAVP treatment, encopresis, other medical problems |
|
Interventions | A (n = 61): alarm B (n = 60): desmopressin intranasally C (n = 61): placebo Duration 6 months, then failures crossed over to alternative arm for 6 months (not randomised) | |
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights after 6 months | |
Notes | Dose of DDAVP not given No follow‐up as failures assigned to alternative treatment Blinding to method not possible for alarm group | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer‐generated |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Low risk | Computer‐generated |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding of medication but blinding with alarm not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding of medication but blinding with alarm not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Less than 10% lost to follow‐up |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Not all studies prespecified outcomes mentioned ‐ no mention of adverse events |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Lovibond 1964a.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (stratified by age and sex) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: school children and GP referrals Number of children (boys): 36 (20) Dropouts: B: 2 Inclusion criteria: wet at least 3 times a week; co‐operative families Exclusion criteria: organic causes Ages: drawn from 3 age groups: 6 ‐ 7 years 5 months; 7 years 6 months to 10 years 5 months; 10 years 6 months to 14 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 12): twin‐signal alarm (hooter then buzzer) + 'escape training'
B (n = 12): Crosby Dri‐nite (electric shock alarm but without genital electrode)
C (n = 12): Mowrer pad and bell Duration of treatment: until 14 dry nights achieved, fluid intake increased if dry for 7 nights (= overlearning) or 50 days Follow‐up: 31 months |
|
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights Number failing or relapsing after trial Adverse events | |
Notes | Experiment 1 | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Allocated at random to treatment groups, stratified by age and sex but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | All participants accounted for |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | No systematic baseline measure of wetting |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Lovibond 1964b.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (stratified by age, sex and wetting frequency) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: school children and GP referrals Number of children (boys): 20 (12) Inclusion criteria: wet at least 3 times a week; co‐operative families Exclusion criteria: organic causes Ages: 8 to 12 years |
|
Interventions | A1 (n = 5): twin‐signal alarm triggered by wetting A2 (n = 5): twin‐signal alarm triggered by parents (not wetting) after 2 dry nights (false alarm) B1 (n = 5): Mowrer pad and bell alarm triggered by wetting B2 (n = 5): Mowrer pad and bell alarm triggered by parents (not wetting) after 2 dry nights (false alarm) Duration: until 14 dry nights achieved including increased fluid intake after 7 dry nights (= overlearning) or 50 days Follow‐up: 24 months | |
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights Number failing or relapsing after 24 month | |
Notes | Experiment 2 'False' alarm equivalent to 'waking' by parents Data from 'standard' versus 'false' only used |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Allocated at random to treatment groups and stratified by age, sex and wetting frequency but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | All participants accounted for |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | No systematic baseline measure of wetting |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Lovibond 1964c.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (stratified by age and sex) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: incidental referrals Number of children (boys): 24 (12) Inclusion criteria: wet at least 3 times a week; co‐operative families Exclusion criteria: organic causes Age: 7 to 14 years |
|
Interventions | A (n = 12): modified twin‐signal (bell instead of buzzer, then second weaker alarm) B (n = 12): Mowrer pad and bell alarm Duration: until 14 dry nights achieved including increased fluid intake after 7 dry nights (= overlearning) or 50 days Follow‐up: 24 months | |
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights Number failing or relapsing | |
Notes | Experiment 3 | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Allocated at random to treatment groups and stratified by age and sex but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | All participants accounted for |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | No systematic baseline measure of wetting |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Lynch 1984.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: no Daytime wetting excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting: school or paediatric referrals Number of children (boys): 60 Number of dropouts: A: 2, B: 2, C: 2 Inclusion criteria: at least 2 wet nights a week Exclusion criteria: daytime wetting Ages: 5 to 12 Baseline wetting in 14 nights, mean (SD): A: 11.11 (2.9), B: 11.33 (2.99), C: 11.55 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 20): star chart for 2 weeks then enuresis alarm ‐ immediate alarm B (n = 20): star chart for 2 weeks then enuresis alarm ‐ 3‐minute delay + CT C (n = 20): control, no treatment Duration of treatment: 10 weeks Follow‐up: none | |
Outcomes | Wet nights in last 2 weeks Number not achieving 14 dry nights | |
Notes | Groups comparable at baseline 1 dropout from alarm group due to stress from alarm | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Randomly assigned to treatment but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Dropouts 10% |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | No intention‐to‐treat analysis. Organic causes not excluded. |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Ma 2007.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: not mentioned Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: Shanghai Children's Medical Centre Number of participants (boys): 183 (92) Number of dropouts: none Inclusion criteria: monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis Exclusion criteria: unknown Ages: 5 to 16 (8.26 ± 2.84 years) A = 8.54 ± 2.15: B = 8.33 ± 2.07: C = 8.03 ± 2.21 years Baseline wetting: needs interpretation |
|
Interventions | A (n = 52): alarm + behavioural/psychological treatment B (n = 46): DDAVP C (n = 40): combined (alarm, behavioural, DDAVP) D (n = 45): "control" (refused allocated treatment) Duration of treatment: 4 months Duration of follow‐up: 3 more months |
|
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment Number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up |
|
Notes | Only abstract in English Trying to obtain data from authors or get formal translation |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Children who did not accept allocated treatment were analysed as “control” i.e. not intention‐to‐treat |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No dropouts |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Not all prespecified outcomes mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
McKendry 1975.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: no |
|
Participants | Setting: paediatric outpatients, Toronto Country: Canada Number of children (boys): 222 (151) Number of dropouts: 53 (A: 9, B: 12, C: 32) Inclusion criteria: primary nocturnal enuresis, "a few" had diurnal wetting Exclusion criteria: organic causes Ages: mean 9 years (range 5 to 17) Baseline wetting (self‐reported): A: 83.4%, B: 82.3%, C: 87.4% |
|
Interventions | A (n = 73): restricted diet B (n = 74): imipramine 10 mg at bedtime, increased to max 40 mg for age 5 ‐ 9 years, up to 60 mg for age 10 or more years C (n = 75): Mozes Detector (body‐worn detector, sounds alarm + delivers electric shock when a few drops of urine passed) Duration of treatment: 2 months Follow‐up: A: 3 months, B: 19 months, C: 14 months | |
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights Adverse events | |
Notes | Diet: no dairy, eggs, citrus, tomato, chocolate A: most parents requested transfer to another treatment within 1 ‐ 2 months due to finding diet unsuccessful and restricting C: high dropout rate was due to parents refusing to allow their child to use the Mozes detector, finding it too expensive, or children fearing it especially if under age 8 years Data entered counting dropouts as failures (for above reasons) | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Quote: “Each was assigned to one of three treatments by random allocation.” |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Of the 222 children who entered the study, 169 completed treatment, 73 were assessed more fully at follow‐up Quote: “...the duration of treatment varied.” Comment: Not adequately explained, high dropout rate (A 12%, B 16%, C 43% withdrew from the study) |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Protocol not available but data given for outcomes stated in Methods. No measure of baseline wetting and daytime wetting not excluded |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Moffatt 1987.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: no Daytime wetting excluded: no |
|
Participants | Setting: enuresis clinic, Montreal Children's Hospital Number of children: 121 Number of dropouts: A: 5 Inclusion: primary nocturnal enuresis, spoke English or French, 7 had daytime urgency (treated with retention control training or anticholinergic drugs) Ages: 8 to 14 years Baseline wetting: 64% wet nights in each group |
|
Interventions | A (n = 66): enuresis alarm + overlearning if successful B (n = 55): waiting list control Duration of treatment mean (SD): A: 18.4 weeks (5.8), B: 13.2 weeks (1.9) | |
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights Adverse events | |
Notes | Groups comparable at baseline but A assessed later if likely to be successful Children's self‐concept improved when they were successful | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Randomisation was performed by having the participant select an opaque envelope that assigned the child to the treatment group or to a waiting period of 3 months |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Insufficient details but used opaque envelopes |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Low dropout rate |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Organic causes excluded: no Daytime wetting excluded: no |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Motavalli 1994.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: outpatients Number of children (boys): 29 (A: 6 B: 4 C: 4) Inclusion criteria: age 5 to 14; no organic causes; normal intelligence; wetting 2+ times a week; no treatment in previous 2 months Mean age: A: 9.1 years, B: 9.2, C: 8.3 (range 5 to 14) Baseline wetting: mean (SD) number of wet nights in 15 days: A: 9.1 (4.1), B: 11.2 (3.8), C: 10.9 (3.3) |
|
Interventions | A (n = 10): imipramine ‐ dose depended on age
B (n = 9): clomipramine
C (n = 10): alarm Duration of treatment: 8 weeks Follow‐up: none |
|
Outcomes | Mean (SD) frequency of wetting during final 2 weeks of treatment | |
Notes | Turkish language No significant difference between groups in terms of age or IQ Not blinded Unclear if intention‐to‐treat | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated: Quote: "randomly divided into three groups" |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not blinded |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Unclear if intention‐to‐treat analysis, adverse events or if daytime wetting excluded |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Naitoh 2005.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting specifically excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting/recruitment: hospital urology clinic, Kyoto Country: Japan Number of participants (boys): 105 (76) Number of dropouts: unknown Inclusion criteria: monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis Exclusion criteria: daytime symptoms including daytime incontinence, frequency ≥ 8 times a day, urological anomalities Ages: mean age 9.4 years (6 to 13 years). A: 9.32, B: 9.31 Baseline wetting: A: 13.1 days every 2 weeks, B: 12.6 every 2 weeks, C: 12.3 every 2 weeks |
|
Interventions | A (n = 37): alarm B (n = 35): alarm + desmopressin C (n = 33): alarm + imipiramine Duration of treatment: 6 months Duration of follow‐up: nil after 6 months |
|
Outcomes | Mean wet nights at end of treatment: Number not achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at end of treatment Number failing to achieve 14 dry nights or relapsing after cure at the end of treatment |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Insufficient information about sequence generation. Selective randomisation: Quote: "Patients without previous treatment were enrolled randomly into the three groups, and the patients who had had previous treatment were not given the same treatment again." |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Quote: "patients who had had previous treatment were not given the same treatment again" |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Numbers lost to follow‐up not stated No dropouts according to Table 2 denominators Outcomes reported as rates rather than numbers |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Not all prespecified outcomes mentioned Adverse events not addressed |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Insuffient information to assess whether an important risk of bias exists |
Nawaz 2002.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (random allocation to groups following matching on age and sex) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Country: Scotland Number of children (boys): 36 (18) Number of dropouts: none Inclusion criteria: age 7 to 12 years, baseline wetting at least twice a months, attending mainstream school and willing to be randomised Exclusion criteria: medical, physiological or psychiatric pathology, diurnal enuresis, encopresis Previous treatment: some, but stopped during trial Age: mean 9.9 years (SD 1.83) Baseline wetting: mean 5.67 per week (SD 1.26) for 4 weeks |
|
Interventions | A (n = 12): DBT + alarm
B (n = 12): alarm only
C (n = 12): untreated controls continued recording wet nights for 16 weeks, then offered treatment they preferred
A + B also received standardised instructions (manual and videotape) and had 2 weekly telephone calls Duration of treatment: 16 weeks or until 14 dry nights if earlier Follow‐up: 6 months |
|
Outcomes | Wet nights a week during trial (final week) Number not achieving 14 dry nights Number relapsing after end of trial Fail or relapse rate | |
Notes | Groups comparable on age, sex, baseline wetting and DepCat (deprivation) scores DBT described as intensive first night, arousing child and taking them to toilet, accident contingencies and normal routine | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Participants were randomly allocated to treatment following matching on age and sex but no other details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No loss to follow‐up |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Low risk | All outcomes reported |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Netley 1984.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: Hospital for Sick Children Enuresis Clinic, Toronto Number of children (boys): 62 Number of dropouts: 27 Inclusion criteria: primary nocturnal enuresis, age 6 to 12 years Ages: mean A: 9 years, B: 10.7 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 31): imipramine
B (n = 31): Mozes detector (buzzer + electric shock to abdominal wall on wetting)
Duration of treatment: unclear. Participants followed up at 2 months after entry into the study and then again an average of 16 months later but the duration of treatment is not stated Cure defined as absence of nocturnal enuresis for 2 months |
|
Outcomes | Number failed after 2 dry months | |
Notes | High dropout rate (44%) Groups not comparable on age at baseline Younger children (< 8 years) apprehensive about detector Authors conclude Mozes detector is suitable for children aged > 8 years | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | The children were randomly assigned to 1 of 2 groups. No other details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | High dropout rate. 17/31 in group 1 (imipramine) and 18/31 in group 2 (alarm) were assessed on all t3 occasions. Most of the participants who dropped out of the study had failed to keep their appointments for the final assessment. No further information given about those who were lost to follow‐up |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Protocol not available Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Ng 2005.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Setting: Department of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, Pamela Youde Nethersole Eastern Hospital, Hong Kong SAR Country: China Number of children: 105 Number of dropouts: 12 (defaulted from treatment: A: 7, B: 2, C: 3; defaulted from follow‐up A: 2, B: 2, C: 5) Inclusion criteria: primary nocturnal enuresis Exclusion criteria: UTI in previous 3 months, daytime wetting, polyuric disorders, abnormal urinalysis, renal disease, previous diuretics, unwilling to be randomised Previous treatment: none (excluded if had had desmopressin, alarms or tricyclics) Age: range from 7 to 12 years Baseline wetting: at least 3 wet nights in baseline 2 weeks |
|
Interventions | A (n = 35): alarm ('Wet‐Stop' alarm) B (n = 38): oral desmopressin 200 µg, increased to 400 µg if > 1 wet night C (n = 32): alarm + desmopressin Duration of treatment: 12 weeks Follow‐up: 12 weeks | |
Outcomes | Wet nights during trial Not achieving 14 dry nights Wet nights after trial Not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing after cure at the end of treatment Adverse effects: none | |
Notes | All children had star charts and kept wetting diaries Comparable at baseline on wetting frequency, age, gender, urine osmolality More children failed to comply in Group A (alarm only); these were included as failures in the dry night analyses | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Quote: "Randomly allocated by consecutive sealed envelopes", but no further details given. |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Used sealed opaque envelopes but unclear if sequential allocation |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | 21/105 (20%) participants lost to follow‐up by end of follow‐up period |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Low risk | All studies prespecified outcomes mentioned |
Other bias | High risk | Research grant from Ferring (pharmaceutical company for desmopressin). |
Onol 2015.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting specifically excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting/recruitment: clinic Country: Turkey Number of children (boys): 142 (A: 48/73, B: 32/45*) Number of dropouts: A: 4, B: 20 Inclusion criteria: children 6 to 15 years old who were diagnosed with PMNE (6 wet nights or more every 2 weeks) Exclusion criteria: voiding and bowel dysfunction, daytime lower urinary tract symptoms (urgency, frequency, daytime wetting, squatting and holding manoeuvres) and defaecation problems (frequency, shape of stool, presence of faecal soiling and constipation), bladder dysfunction secondary to neurogenic or anatomical abnormalities, a history of documented urinary tract infection, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder or a history of any treatment for PMNE within the preceding 3 months. Mean (SD) age*: A: 8.75 (3), B: 9.51 (2.98) Baseline wetting*: mean (SD) wet nights a week: A: 5.92 (1.34), B: 5.9 (1.15) |
|
Interventions | A (n = 77): desmospressin, 1 hour before bed B (n = 65): body‐worn alarm device (Aymed®). The child was instructed to wake up as soon as possible when the alarm sounded, void in the toilet and reset the device. Parents were particularly warned about the importance of assisting the child in awakening and reattaching the device, especially during the first 6 ‐ 8 weeks of treatment Duration of treatment: 6 months. Assessed at 3 months, if partial or response, continued for another 3 months. If full response, treatment ceased over 2 weeks at 3 months Duration of follow‐up: 3 months and 6 months after end of treatment Dosage of desmopressin: 120 µg daily, with an increase to 240 µg daily if a substantial response was not achieved after 2 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Success defined according to ICCS 2006 criteria. Full response = "cure, or 1 or fewer bed‐wetting episodes monthly." Number not achieving full response at 3 months |
|
Notes | *Baseline characteristics reported for completers only | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Quote: "simple, unrestricted randomisation" |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible to blind participants, other blinding not reported |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Much higher attrition in alarm group Methods state ITT analysis was done where dropouts are considered treatment failures but in results ITT analysis is reported using last observation carried forward |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Not all outcomes reported in full at all follow‐up points |
Other bias | High risk | Baseline characteristics reported for completers only. Treatment altered at 3 months depending on response |
Ozden 2008.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes (during treatment,families kept a record of number of alarms per night) Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting: not mentioned Number of participants (boys): 52 (31) Number of dropouts: 3 in group A, 5 in group B Inclusion criteria: MNE with 3+ wet nights a week Exclusion criteria: diurnal enuresis, UTI, polyuric disorders, renal disease, hypertension, genitourological abnormalities, mental retardation, neurological disease, diuretic use Ages: 6 to 15 years (mean 10.1 ± 2.01 years). A: 9.9 ±1.8, B: 10.3 ± 2.2 Baseline wetting: A: 5.9 ± 1.5 a week, B: 5.7± 1.3 a week |
|
Interventions | A (n = 30): desmopressin for 6 weeks + alarm for 6 weeks B (n = 28): alarm for 6 weeks Duration of treatment: 12 weeks Duration of follow‐up: 24 weeks from start |
|
Outcomes | Number of wet nights a week during treatment Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights during treatment Number of wet nights a week at follow‐up |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Quote: "patients randomised into two groups" Comment: insufficient information about sequence generation process to permit judgement |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned. |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | 8 participants abandoned therapy, with more in the alarm group who abandoned therapy. Paper did not state why therapy was abandoned |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Participants who abandoned therapy were not included in the analysis |
Other bias | Low risk | Study appears to be free of other risk of bias |
Rodriguez 2001.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Setting: hospital clinic Country: Spain Number of children: 84 (80% boys) Number of dropouts: 3 dropouts after 3 months Inclusion criteria: wetting at least once a week, age > 7 years Exclusion criteria: diurnal enuresis, encopresis, neurological abnormalities Previous treatment: 38% of children Age: range from 7 to 14 years |
|
Interventions | A (n = 30): bed alarm B (n = 29): alarm and desmopressin 20 µg or 40 µg for more frequent wetters (> 2 times a week) Duration 4 ‐ 6 months | |
Outcomes | Response Number not achieving 14 dry nights | |
Notes | Spanish language All children treated with desmopressin if not cured at 6 months, therefore follow‐up not possible | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Loss of over 10% of participants in each group at follow‐up |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Not all prespecified outcomes mentioned ‐ no baseline wetting data, no adverse effects mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Unclear |
Ronen 1995.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned | |
Participants | Setting: community mental health clinic Number of children (boys): 77 (39) Dropouts: 23 (A: 2, B: 4, C: 6, D: 11) Inclusion criteria: children attending a community mental health clinic with primary enuresis Exclusion criteria: medical or developmental problems; age < 5 years Age: mean 10.05 (SD 2.28) Baseline wetting: mean wet nights in 3 weeks (SD): A: 19.8 (1.73), B: 19.8 (2.14), C: 18.9 (2.21), D: 18 (8.72) |
|
Interventions | A (n = 20): cognitive and behavioural self‐control education therapy counselling B (n = 19): alarm (bell and pad) C (n = 20): token economy (star chart and rewards) D (n = 18): control (waiting list for 3 months) Duration: 18 weeks Follow‐up at 6 months | |
Outcomes | Cured (3 consecutive dry weeks) Failed Number of wet nights in 3 weeks at end of treatment Actual failure or relapse after 6 months | |
Notes | A (cognitive treatment) had lowest dropout, highest success and lowest relapse compared with B, C or D Ronen 1992 and Ronen 1995 are both the same trial |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Assigned by chronological order |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | No concealment |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Number of participants at the end of treatment less than at baseline |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | The study only reported results they thought were "reliable and trustworthy". |
Other bias | High risk | Imbalance in dropouts between interventions |
Sacks 1974.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: referral to child guidance centre Number of children: 83 (from previous study) Exclusion criteria: severe psychosis or organic causes Ages: 5.5 to 14 years |
|
Interventions | A (n = 64): conditioning (alarm) B (n = 10): psychotherapy, counselling (12 weekly 40‐min sessions + 20 mins with mother) C (n = 9): control Duration of treatment: B 12 weeks | |
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights | |
Notes | Control group was older and had fewer boys | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Randomly assigned, disproportionate allocation but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No dropouts |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Systematic baseline wetting not measured |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Control group was older and had fewer boys |
Scholander 1968.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (double‐blind) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Number of children (boys): 30 (23)
Number of dropouts: none
Previous treatment: all had received imipramine, amitriptyline or nortriptyline Age range: 7 to 17 years Severity of wetting at baseline: wet bed between 2 and 12 times a week |
|
Interventions | First week:
A (n = 15): enuresis alarm but switched off
B (n = 15): enuresis alarm but switched off Second and third weeks: A (n = 15): enuresis alarm + nortriptyline 25 to 50 mg B (n = 15): enuresis alarm + placebo Duration of treatment: 2 weeks on tablets Follow‐up after 6 to 12 months |
|
Outcomes | Number with no wet nights in final week of drug/placebo treatment Side effects |
|
Notes | Swedish language No details of inclusion or exclusion criteria Groups comparable in age and frequency of wet nights | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Double‐blind |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Double‐blind |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No dropouts |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Exclusion of children with daytime wetting not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Seabrook 2005.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes (chart of day and night wetting for 2 weeks) Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting specifically excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting/recruitment: enuresis clinic, Children’s Hospital, Western Ontario Number of participants (boys): 96 (alarm 68.4% = 33, hypnotherapy 74.8% = 36) Number of dropouts: 22 (failure to complete treatment 15 ‐ alarm 6, hypnotherapy 9; refusal to participate 4; concomitant medical condition 3) Inclusion criteria: aged 7 to 12 years, nocturnal enuresis at least 1 wet night a month Exclusion criteria: ADHD, large urine volume conditions, neurological problems, bladder abnormalities, daytime wetting, previous medication for enuresis Mean age: 8.8 years, unclear if for children starting or completing study Baseline wetting: "at least 1 wet night per month", no other data |
|
Interventions | A (n = 48): alarm (38 + 6 + 4) B (n = 48): hypnotherapy (36 + 9 + 3) Duration of treatment: 3 months Duration of follow‐up: not mentioned, not sure whether relapse happened within or after treatment period |
|
Outcomes | Dry at end of treatment (3 months) Number not achieving 14 consecutive dry nights at end of treatment Number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up |
|
Notes | Stated number of children completing study (alarm 38, hypnotherapy 36) but not number of children randomised to each group (assumed to be equal, n = 48) Other bias: percentages (no numbers) provided for outcomes indicate per protocol analysis, not intention‐to‐treat After hypnotherapy failure, children were offered alarm and then desmopressin |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Quote: “Randomly assigned” by block randomisation |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | All participants accounted for but large loss to follow‐up (more than 20%) |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Adverse events not mentioned |
Other bias | High risk | Percentages provided (no numbers) for outcomes indicate analysis was per protocol, not intention‐to‐treat |
Shakiba 2001.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: not stated Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not stated | |
Participants | Setting: not stated Number of children: 60 (80% boys) Number of dropouts: 4 in treatment group after 1 ‐ 2 weeks Inclusion criteria: primary nocturnal enuresis Exclusion criteria: not stated Age: dropouts more than 10 years; others unknown |
|
Interventions | A (n = 30): alarm
B (n = 30): control (no treatment)
Duration: 2 ‐ 3 months Follow‐up: 2 months |
|
Outcomes | Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights during treatment Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up Adverse effects |
|
Notes | Iranian study Abstract only |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Randomisation method not given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not available |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Low dropout rate (< 10%) |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Systematic baseline measure of wetting and exclusion of daytime wetting not stated |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Sloop 1973.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: residential training centres for learning‐disabled children Number of children: 42 (boys: A: 11 B: 11) Exclusion criteria: epileptics; severe behaviour problems; encopretics; residents in beds with side rails which prevent them arising; residents on nightly tranquillising medications; measured IQ below 20; not wetting bed at least once during baseline Previous treatment: none Mean age: A: 13 years, B: 12 (range 7 to 18) Baseline wetting: mean number of wet nights: Boys: A: 4.18 B: 4 Girls: A: 3.64 B: 3.54 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 21): enuresis alarm
B (n = 21): control ‐ usual "potting" procedure ‐ taken to the toilet twice a night Duration of treatment: 11 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Number of wet nights in 7 weeks Number (%) dry Number relapsed |
|
Notes | Treatment was for up to 11 weeks. Those who achieved 14 consecutive dry nights stopped treatment but if they relapsed they were offered more treatment within the 11‐week time period Boys and girls analysed separately Not clear if intention‐to‐treat One pair of boys switched after 3 nights of treatment |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Randomly allocated to treatment but no other details given. |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Allocation done in pairs. Participants paired on IQ, sex, age and number of wet nights during baseline then one from each pair randomly allocated to intervention. One pair of boys switched after 3 nights of treatment |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not clear if intention‐to‐treat was followed |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Daytime wetting exclusion and adverse events not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Sukhai 1989#.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (double‐blind randomised cross‐over with 2 weeks washout) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Number of children (boys): 28 (21)
Inclusion criteria: normal urine concentration capacity of 800 milliosmol/kg or higher; 3+ wet nights a week during observation period; informed parental consent; no urological or renal disorder; no history of daytime wetting; no chronic urinary tract infection; no neurological or cardiovascular disease
Previous treatment: 19 had previous treatment attempts, including alarm (n = 9) and tricyclic antidepressants (n = 10)
Age: mean 11 years (range 7 to 16) Severity at baseline: mean (SEM) number of dry nights a week = 1.4 (0.3) No dropouts Setting: 20 children attended normal primary school, 8 attended special classes for learning difficulties |
|
Interventions | A (n = 28): enuresis alarm and bedtime dose of 20 µg DDAVP intranasally
B (n = 28): enuresis alarm and bedtime dose of placebo 2‐week washout period between treatments Duration of treatment: 2 weeks in each condition Follow‐up: 4 weeks to 6 months |
|
Outcomes | Mean (SE) dry nights during treatment
Number dry at follow‐up at 6 weeks and 4.5 months Number relapsed at follow‐up at 6 weeks and 4.5 months Adverse events |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Double‐blind randomised cross‐over study but no other details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Blinded |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | All participants completed treatment phase and followed up |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Low risk | All stated study outcomes mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Some equipment donated by Ferring (pharmaceutical company for desmopressin). |
Taylor 1975.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: CCT (sequential allocation) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: no |
|
Participants | Setting: outpatient clinic Number of children (boys): 82 (68) Number of dropouts: unclear because some replaced by next admission to enuresis clinic Inclusion criteria: aged between 4 and 16; parents saw enuresis as a problem; no relevant organic pathology Previous treatment: no details Age range: 4 to 15 years; mean boys = 8.8; mean girls = 9.3 Daytime wetting: 16 children Baseline wetting: no details Severity of wetting at baseline: no details |
|
Interventions | A (n = 21): continuous alarm ‐ bed alarm triggered as soon as bed wet
B (n = 18): alarm + intermittent reinforcement schedule ‐ continuous alarm for 14 days then parents told to switch alarm off whenever indicated by reinforcement schedule (50% of the time). Child unaware of reinforcement schedule
C (n = 22): alarm + overlearning ‐ when participant achieved 7 consecutive dry nights, fluid intake increased by 1 ‐ 2 pints prior to going to bed This regime continued until success criterion achieved. Duration of treatment: until success criterion met i.e. no more than 1 wetting incidence in 28 days Follow‐up: after 3 months |
|
Outcomes | Number (%) achieving no more than 1 wetting incidence in 28 days Number (%) of successes who relapsed |
|
Notes | Results from 61 participants analysed
Number of participants at any given stage unclear
Probably atypical population due to referral process
Comparability of groups at baseline not reported
Not intention‐to‐treat
No details of previous treatment
Includes children with diurnal wetting (n = 6), encopresis (n = 10) and both (n = 6) Overlearning is initiated after successful alarm treatment (e.g. achievement of 14 consecutive dry nights). Extra drinks are given at bedtime to cause additional stress to the detrusor muscles in the bladder. Alarm treatment is then continued until 14 consecutive dry nights are again achieved |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Sequential allocation to treatment |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | No intention‐to‐treat. Number of participants at any given stage unclear. Number of dropouts unclear because some replaced by next admission to enuresis clinic |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Comparability of groups at baseline not reported. Daytime wetting not excluded |
Other bias | Unclear risk | No details of previous treatment |
Titawee 2000.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting specifically excluded: no |
|
Participants | Setting: psychiatry department, tertiary hospital Country: Thailand Number of participants (boys): 80 (30); Number of dropouts: 2 Inclusion criteria: bedwetting 2 nights a week Exclusion criteria: organic disease, mental disorder, unable to follow up Age: 6+ years (no further details) Baseline wetting: not provided |
|
Interventions | A (n = 40): pad and bell alarm + social approval, star chart, tangible reward B (n = 40): fluid restriction, night lifting + social approval, star chart, tangible reward Duration of treatment: 3 months Duration of follow‐up: 6 months |
|
Outcomes | Mean wet nights at end of treatment Complete response at end of treatment Number not achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment Mean wet nights at follow‐up Number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing at follow‐up Adverse events |
|
Notes | Other bias: high risk Not generalisable ‐ tertiary patients – worse disease/harder to treat, prior treatments (traditional methods) already tried before enrolment and failed |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Randomised but no other details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Reported blinding of assessor (the clinician) from report provided by the parent and child |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Weekly follow‐up Accounted for all participants, 2 dropouts |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | No baseline wetting data Mean number of wet nights for subgroup of children who achieved dryness only |
Other bias | High risk | Not generalisable ‐ tertiary patients – worse disease/harder to treat, prior treatments (traditional methods) already tried before enrolment and failed |
Tobias 2001.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting: urban paediatric medical centre Number of children (boys): 54 (33) Number of dropouts: 7 lost to follow‐up or did not comply with treatment Inclusion criteria: primary or secondary enuresis, 3 wet nights of 7, age 6 to 12 years Exclusion criteria: daytime wetting, chronic illness, UTI, urinary tract pathology Previous treatment: yes, but children asked to stop these while in trial Age: mean 8.6 years (SD 1.9) |
|
Interventions | A (n = 23): body‐worn audio alarm
B (n = 24): body‐worn vibrating alarm Duration of treatment: 90 nights or until 14 dry nights achieved Follow‐up: none |
|
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights Adverse events |
|
Notes | All children used star charts to record wet and dry nights but rewards for dry nights were not mentioned | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Low risk | Random‐number table was used |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | 7 lost to follow‐up (more than 10%) |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Systematic baseline wetting not measured |
Other bias | Unclear risk | No follow‐up |
Tuncel 2014.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: not reported Daytime wetting specifically excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting: hospital Country: Turkey Number of children (boys): 104 (59) Number of dropouts: none Inclusion criteria: significant primary MNE Exclusion criteria: not reported Mean age: A: 11, B: 10 Baseline wetting: not reported |
|
Interventions | A (n = 49): desmospressin Minirin Melt® B (n = 55): alarm Enurin® Duration of treatment: 3 months Duration of follow‐up: no further follow‐up after end of treatment Dosage of desmopressin: 120 µg daily |
|
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights at end of treatment | |
Notes | Abstract only | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Quote: "randomized into 2 groups" |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not reported |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible to blind participants |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No withdrawals |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Not all prespecified outcomes mentioned, adverse events not discussed |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Insufficient information |
Turner 1970.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (stratified by age (4 ‐ 7 and 7 ‐ 15) and sex (12:8 M:F)) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: school health clinics Number of children (boys): 115 (80) Dropouts: 39/81 allocated to an alarm (A, B or C), due to failure to use equipment properly, disruption or domestic problems. 1/32 from D or E Inclusion criteria: primary (103) and secondary (12) enuresis Exclusion criteria: organic pathology; adverse home conditions; bedwetting < 3 times a week; previous alarm treatment Age: mean 7.5 years (SD 2.6) (range 4 to 15 years) Baseline data not provided for individual interventions |
|
Interventions | A (n = 15): alarm, continuous signal (Mowrer‐type bell alarm) B (n = 15): alarm, twin signal C (n = 12): alarm, intermittent twin signal (after first 2 weeks, alarm sometimes disconnected) D (n = 15): random wakening E (n = 17): placebo tablet Duration: 4 weeks for D and E only. If no success, withdrawn from study for alternative treatment Mean duration: A: 6.8 weeks, B: 6.2 weeks, C: 10.2 weeks | |
Outcomes | Failed at 4 weeks Mean wet nights a week at 4 weeks Failure at end of treatment Long‐term failure at 3 years Adverse events | |
Notes | Dropouts replaced by next child referred to the clinic. Some failures treated with methedrine Unable to reach required sample size of 20 children per group during 5‐year recruitment period Adverse events caused high dropout rate in alarm groups due to inability to use the equipment or family disruption, so long‐term data not useable Random waking ‐ parents given a chart of when the child should be woken each night, with the times varying randomly |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Quote: "Allocated at random... within 2 age bands", Comment: stratified by age and sex. When child dropped out, vacancy filled by next child referred to the clinic |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Dropouts replaced by next child referred to the clinic |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not mentioned |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | 115 children enrolled but data only available for 74 |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Baseline data not provided for individual interventions Daytime wetting not mentioned |
Other bias | High risk | When treatment had to be stopped due to poor parental co‐operation, the vacancy was filled by the next child referred to the clinic. Differential compliance rates between groups: higher non‐compliance rates in the alarm conditioning groups (39/81) compared with the control (placebo + random waking) groups (1/32) |
Tuygun 2007.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes (method not given for baseline, diary used for outcome) Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting: single centre Country: Turkey Number of children (boys): 84/56 Number of dropouts: none Inclusion criteria: MNE, at least 3 wet nights a week Exclusion criteria: diurnal incontinence, polyuric disorders, genitourinary system abnormalities, recurrent UTIs, neurological disorders Ages: 6 to 13 years (median 8) Baseline wetting: A: 23.2 ± 6.23/month, B: 23.44 ± 6.3/month |
|
Interventions | A (n = 35): alarm B (n = 49): desmopressin Duration of treatment: 3 months Duration of follow‐up: 6 months (3 months after treatment) |
|
Outcomes | Number of wet nights a week during treatment Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights during treatment Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after cure Adverse events |
|
Notes | Participants from desmopressin group who relapsed at 3 months were given alarm therapy (group 3) The study was to compare those on alarm therapy as first line with those who had alarm therapy after failure with desmopressin. However, we did not look at this group. Rebound patients were defined as those who had the same number of wet nights at the end of treatment as at baseline |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Insufficient information about the sequence generation to permit judgement |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Insufficient information to permit judgement |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | All participants accounted for |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Combination of partial and complete response to represent success meant the success was overestimated |
Other bias | High risk | Study design meant not all participants in group 2 had alarm therapy.Therefore when we compare participants from Group 1 and Group 3, all participants in Group 3 had medication + alarm, and outcomes measured at different time points. However, as they were not randomised, we do not feel that it is appropriate to therefore compare with Group 1 (A) and Group 2 (B). Group 3 excluded. Sample size for both groups not equal |
Van Hoeck 2008.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned Setting: University Hospital, Antwerp, Belgium |
|
Participants | Setting: University Hospital, Antwerp Country: Belgium Number of participants (boys): 149 (108) Number of dropouts: 20 Inclusion criteria: at least 14 wet nights in the 4 weeks before randomisation Exclusion criteria: previous treatment with alarm, desmopressin or anticholinergics completed < 3 months before run‐in period, age < 5 years at randomisation, puberty beyond Tanner stage I Age: range 5.9 to 12.7 years, median 7.5 years Baseline wetting: not given |
|
Interventions | A (n = 29): holding exercises plus placebo (12 weeks) followed by alarm (12 weeks) B (n = 30): holding exercises plus oxybutynin (12 weeks) followed by alarm (12 weeks) C (n = 30): placebo only (12 weeks) followed by alarm (12 weeks) D (n = 30): oxybutynin only (12 weeks) followed by alarm (12 weeks) E (n = 30): alarm only (12 weeks) followed by no treatment (12 weeks) Children unresponsive to alarm at 24 weeks were offered continuation of alarm, desmopressin 10 to 40 mcg intranasally once daily, 0.2 mg/kg oxybutynin orally once daily, or combination therapy Duration of treatment: 24 weeks Duration of follow‐up: 52 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Full response Number not achieving 14 dry nights during treatment Number not achieving 14 dry nights during treatment or relapsing after cure Adverse events |
|
Notes | ||
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Stratified randomisation used, method not stated |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | Incomplete data small, accounted for all participants |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Intention‐to‐treat analysis used for prespecified outcomes Systematic baseline measure of wetting and daytime wetting not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Van Londen 1993.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: no Organic causes excluded: no Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: parents who contacted an alarm rental agency, Utrecht Country: Holland Number of children (boys): 127 (89) Number of dropouts: 14 (A: 3, B: 2, C: 9) Inclusion criteria: primary (110) or secondary (17) enuresis Ages: mean 8.6 years (range 6 to 12) |
|
Interventions | A (n = 38): alarm + reward stickers for correct behaviour at the time of wetting ('arousal therapy' = turning off alarm in 3 minutes, going to bathroom to empty bladder, resetting alarm) B (n = 39): alarm + reward stickers in morning for dry bed and penalty (1 sticker) for wet bed C (n = 36): alarm only Duration of treatment: 20 weeks Follow‐up: at 2½ years | |
Outcomes | Number not achieving 14 dry nights during trial Failure and relapse rate during follow‐up period Final failure and relapse rate at follow‐up | |
Notes | Groups comparable at baseline on age, gender, diagnosis and frequency of wetting 'Arousal therapy' described as 'bibliotherapy' because parents received their instructions in written form | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Children were assigned sequentially to 1 of 3 conditions in the order in which they contacted the firm |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Allocation not concealed |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No dropouts during treatment. The children they could not reach at 2 years (14) did not differ from the original group on the outcome variables |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | No systematic baseline measure, organic causes not excluded |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Vogt 2010.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes, by diary Organic causes excluded: yes (renal disease) Daytime wetting excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting: outpatient clinic at Leipzig University Number of children: 43 (gender not stated) Number of dropouts: 13 Inclusion criteria: previously untreated children aged > 5 and ≤ 15 years with MNE Exclusion criteria: treatment of enuresis within the last 12 months, daytime symptoms and renal disease Ages: A: 6.7 years, B: 6.4 years Baseline wetting: A: 9.81 (2.93) a fortnight; B: 10.5 (3.59) a fortnight |
|
Interventions | A (n = 24): desmopressin followed by alarm at 3 months if symptomatic (14) B (n = 19): alarm followed by desmopressin if symptomatic at 3 months (16) Duration of treatment: 12 weeks monotherapy + 12 weeks combination therapy (24 weeks) Duration of follow‐up: 12 months |
|
Outcomes | Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights during treatment (first 12 weeks) Number failing to achieve 14 consecutive dry nights or relapsing after cure |
|
Notes | Did not use follow‐up data at 6 and 12 months as children who failed monotherapy (first 3 months) received combination therapy (second 3 months) in both groups | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | "Randomised" but insufficient information about sequence generation to permit judgement. |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | 13/43 excluded after 3 months Data not added to analysis |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Adverse events not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Insufficient information to assess whether an important risk of bias exists |
Wagner 1982.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes |
|
Participants | Setting: referrals from paediatric clinics, doctors, schools and newspaper advertisements Number of children (boys): 49 (40) Number of dropouts: 13 Inclusion criteria: age 6 to 16 years, IQ > 70, primary nocturnal enuresis Exclusion criteria: daytime wetting, physical or neurological disorders, treatment with drugs or alarms in previous year Ages: 6 to 16 Baseline wetting: min 3 times a week, A: 75%, B: 77%, C: 64% |
|
Interventions | A (n = 12): alarm (pad and bell)
B (n = 12): imipramine (if < 32 kg, 25 mg a day, if > 32 kg, 50 mg a day)
C (n = 12): waiting list Duration of treatment: 14 week or until dry for 14 nights Follow‐up: maximum 44 days |
|
Outcomes | % wet nights in 14th week Number not achieving 14 dry nights Number not achieving 14 dry nights or relapsing Time from cure to relapse | |
Notes | Groups comparable on baseline wetting No SDs or means | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Participants were "randomly assigned" to 1 of 3 groups but no further details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not blinded |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not blinded |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | 13/49 (more than 10%) dropped out |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Study protocol not available No information given about dropouts |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Wagner 1985.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: CCT (alternate allocation to groups) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: institutional centre for mental retardation Number of children (boys): 39 (20) Number of dropouts: none Inclusion criteria: age between 5 and 16 years old; IQs not less than 70; no physical or neurologic disorders as assessed by the child's physician; wet the bed at least 3 nights a week before treatment; not had conditioning treatment for at least a year; agreed to random assignment Previous treatment: no details Mean age: 7.9 years (range: 5 to 14) Severity at baseline: % wet nights a week: A: 80, B: 83, C: 90 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 13): continuous enuresis alarm
B (n = 13): delayed response enuresis alarm ‐ 3‐second delay
C (n = 13): waiting list control Duration of treatment: 12 weeks Follow‐up: after 6 months |
|
Outcomes | % of wet nights a week in week 12 Number achieving 14 consecutive dry nights Number relapsing |
|
Notes | Clinicians blind to specific purpose of the study
No significant differences in groups in terms of age, sex, recruitment source, psychological measures, baseline wetting frequencies
No SDs or means Contiguous alarm sounds as soon as wetting occurs Delayed alarm sounds after a 3‐second delay |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Alternate allocation to groups |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Allocation not concealed |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | No dropouts |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Daytime wetting exclusion not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Werry 1965.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (initial allocation by random numbers, in second half of trial stratified by age and sex) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: enuresis clinic, Paediatric Hospital, Montreal Country: Canada Number of children (boys): 70 (46) Number of dropouts: 10 (A: 1, B: 4, C: 5) Inclusion criteria: primary enuresis Exclusion criteria: dry > 3 months, organic causes Ages: mean 9.99 years (SD 2.5) Baseline wetting: minimum once a week (mean 5 ‐ 6 times a week) |
|
Interventions | A (n = 27): control (no treatment for 4 months)
B (n = 21): brief psychotherapy (6 ‐ 8 sessions over 3 months)
C (n = 22): alarm (bed buzzer) once a night Duration of treatment: 3 ‐ 4 months Follow‐up: none |
|
Outcomes | Failure to achieve 14 dry nights (defined as no wet beds in preceding month) | |
Notes | Groups comparable on age, class, wetting severity and psychopathology Most enuretic children were not emotionally disturbed (similar to non‐enuretic siblings) | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | High risk | Children were allocated to 1 of 3 treatment groups, originally by a system of random numbers, but toward the end of the study by age and sex in an attempt to balance the 3 groups on these variables |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | High risk | Allocation not concealed for all children |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Blinding not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Differential dropout rate between groups |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Daytime wetting exclusion not mentioned. No harmful psychological effects were observed |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Wille 1986.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: yes | |
Participants | Setting: referral to paediatrician Number of children: 50 (boys and girls) Number of dropouts: not included in analysis Inclusion criteria: age over 6 years; not dry for > 6 months; at least 3 wet nights a week at baseline; written informed parental consent Exclusion criteria: treatment for enuresis during previous year; daytime wetting; cardiovascular disease; renal disorder; neurological disease; urinary tract infection Distribution of social class of parents in 2 groups was similar Age: > 6 years Baseline wetting: mean number of dry nights a week: A: 2.1, B: 1.9 Number completing treatment: A: 24, B: 22 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 25): intranasal desmopressin (20 µg)
B (n = 25): enuresis alarm
Duration of treatment: 3 months Failures crossed over to alternative, relapses continued on same treatment |
|
Outcomes | Mean (SEM) number of dry nights a week in first week Number failing during treatment (> 5 wet nights in 28 or no change in enuresis score) Number not cured after treatment (failed or relapsed) Mean (SEM) dry nights after trial Adverse events Lab tests |
|
Notes | Direct comparison of desmopressin and alarm
Not intention‐to‐treat analysis
Results taken from graph Data from end of treatment (not end of first week) used for review Cure/relapse rates based on less strict definition of cure than usual |
|
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Randomised but no other details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | 4 participants excluded after randomisation (< 10%). |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Did not report all prespecified outcomes. Data graphically entered |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Insufficient information to assess whether other risk of bias exists |
Wright 1974.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (medications on double‐blind basis) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Number of children: 23
Number of dropouts: A: 0, B: 0, C: 2, D: 0 Age: range 4 to 10 years Baseline wetting: mean number of wettings per week: A + B: 4.9, C: 3.0, D: 6.6 |
|
Interventions | A (n = 3): amphetamine sulphate (2.5 mg)
B (n = 5): ephedrine sulphate (75 mg) + atropine sulphate (Enuretrol, 1.15 mg)
C (n = 5): placebo twice daily
D (n = 10): enuresis alarm Duration of treatment: 5 weeks Follow‐up: after 4 weeks |
|
Outcomes | Mean number of wet nights in final week of treatment | |
Notes | Groups seem very different at baseline More likely to detect more wettings a night in pad and bell group All active drugs groups combined No details of inclusion or exclusion criteria Data estimated from graph No SDs | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Mentioned but no details given |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Only for drugs, not possible for the alarm group |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Only for drugs, not possible for the alarm group |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | Low risk | All data accounted for |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | High risk | Side effects not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not stated |
Young 1972.
Study characteristics | ||
Methods | Design: RCT (children chosen at random from children cured on alarm treatment) Systematic baseline measure of wetting: yes Organic causes excluded: yes Daytime wetting excluded: not mentioned |
|
Participants | Setting: local authority enuresis clinic Country: UK Number of children (boys): 144 (99) Number of dropouts: A: 6, B: 37 Inclusion criteria: primary and secondary nocturnal enuresis, age > 4 years, already cured using alarm treatment Ages: 4 to 15 years Baseline wetting: minimum 2 wet nights a week before cure |
|
Interventions | A (n = 61): alarm + overlearning
B (n = 83): alarm alone (already cured) Duration of treatment:3 months or until relapse or cured again Follow‐up: at 3 and 6 months, then 6‐monthly until 2 years |
|
Outcomes | Number relapsing (failing to remain dry or regain dryness) | |
Notes | All children were initially given alarm treatment and of the children who were cured, some were randomly assigned to overlearning. A further 6 were withdrawn from group A due to severe recurrence of enuresis with overlearning but they then regained dryness | |
Risk of bias | ||
Bias | Authors' judgement | Support for judgement |
Random sequence generation (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Unclear method of randomisation to overlearning group. |
Allocation concealment (selection bias) | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
Blinding of participants and personnel (performance bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias) All outcomes | Unclear risk | Not possible |
Incomplete outcome data (attrition bias) All outcomes | High risk | Differential loss to follow‐up (higher in alarm only group) |
Selective reporting (reporting bias) | Unclear risk | Daytime wetting exclusion not mentioned |
Other bias | Unclear risk | Not mentioned |
A = Alarm; Alarm = enuresis alarm triggered by wetting; cc = cubic centilitres; CCT = controlled clinical trial with quasi‐randomised method of allocation; Clock Alarm = clock set to ring at specified time after going to bed irrespective of wetting; CT = Cleanliness training (changing the bed); dB = decibels (measure of volume); DBT = Dry‐bed training (training night, CT, PP and W); DDAVP: desmopressin acetate; FSHT = Full spectrum home training (A + CT + Overlearning + Retention Control Training); LUTS: lower urinary tract symptoms; Overlearning = giving extra fluids at bedtime to child already cured using alarm treatment; PMNE: primary monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis; PP = Positive Practice (practising getting up and voiding repeatedly); Random wakening = wakening the child to urinate at random times; Retention control training = increasing fluid intake and delaying urination for increasing periods of time to expand bladder capacity; SD = Standard deviation; SEM = standard error of the mean; SIADH: syndrome of inappropriate diuretic hormone; Stream interruption exercises = practising interruption of urination; UTI = urinary tract infection; Volume alarm = alarm triggered by ultrasound measurement of bladder volume, triggered at prespecified volume before wetting occurs; W = waking (waking child to void, earlier on subsequent nights if dry).
Characteristics of excluded studies [ordered by study ID]
Study | Reason for exclusion |
---|---|
Akoglu 2006 | Ineligible study design and intervention |
Aman 2005 | Ineligible intervention |
Arai 1974 | Ineligible intervention |
Azrin 1973 | Participants were adults |
Bollard 1977 | Ineligible study design |
Bollard 1982b | Ineligible study design |
Burr 1980 | Ineligible intervention |
Butler 1990 | Ineligible study design |
Butler 2001 | Not an RCT |
Cederblad 2015 | Ineligible comparison: comparing basic bladder advice with no bladder advice |
Collins 1973 | Ineligible study design |
Collins 1975 | Ineligible study design and study population (geriatric patients) |
Cracco 1982 | Ineligible study design and intervention |
Crisp 1984 | Participants were adults |
de Leon 1966 | Ineligible study design |
Eggert 2012 | Ineligible study design |
El‐Anany 1999 | Ineligible intervention |
Elmissiry 2013 | Diagnostic test study |
Fera 2004 | Ineligible intervention |
Fera 2011 | Ineligible intervention |
Finley 1982 | Organic causes not excluded |
Fordham 1989 | Ineligible study design |
Forrester 1964 | Ineligible comparison of interventions because alarm group did not receive consistent interventions and any treatment response cannot be clearly attributed to the alarm intervention |
Forsythe 1970 | Ineligible study design |
Freyman 1963 | Ineligible study design |
Ghanizadeh 2012 | Ineligible intervention |
Gillison 1958 | Ineligible study design |
Goel 1984 | Ineligible study design |
Halliday 1987 | Study for daytime incontinence |
Hansen 1995 | Ineligible study design |
Hanson 1988 | Population is young adults (13 to 29) and organic causes not excluded |
Hu 1995 | Ineligible intervention |
Hyams 1992 | Ineligible study design |
Iester 1991 | Ineligible intervention |
Jain 2010 | Ineligible intervention |
Juul 2011 | No intervention (gender difference in antidiuretic response to desmopressin in adults) |
Kahane 1955 | Ineligible study design |
Kajiwara 2010 | Ineligible intervention |
Kaplan 1988 | Ineligible study design |
Kazemi 2013 | Ineligible intervention |
Komissarov 1990 | Ineligible study design |
Kooijman 1986 | Ineligible study design |
Kosilov 2015 | Ineligible study design |
Kyneb 1975 | Ineligible study design |
Liu 2000 | Ineligible intervention |
Lovibond 1963 | Ineligible study design |
Lovibond 1964d | Ineligible study design |
Mahler 2012 | No intervention (effect of sleep deprivation on urine production and composition) |
Mahony 1973 | Ineligible intervention |
McConaghy 1969 | Unclear treatment allocation (alarm, imipramine, amphetamine, behavioural). Randomised to 4 groups then non‐randomised to other treatment making final group numbers unclear. Children moved between trial arms, data therefore unreliable. Organic causes not excluded |
Merks 2012 | Ineligible intervention |
Monda 1995 | Ineligible study design |
Montaldo 2012 | Ineligible intervention |
Muller 1999 | Ineligible intervention |
NCT00209261 | Ineligible study design |
NCT00230594 | Ineligible intervention |
NCT00240812 | Ineligible intervention |
NCT00270621 | Ineligible intervention |
NCT00404638 | Ineligible intervention |
NCT01078753 | Ineligible intervention |
NCT01575678 | Ineligible intervention |
Osterberg 2006 | Ineligible study design |
Park 2010 | Ineligible intervention |
Perrin 2015 | Ineligible study design |
Peterson 1969 | Ineligible study design |
Philpott 1970 | Ineligible study design |
Pretlow 1999 | Ineligible study design |
Radvanska 2011 | Ineligible intervention |
Raheem 2012 | Ineligible intervention |
Raheem 2013 | Ineligible intervention |
Redsell 2003 | Ineligible intervention |
Said 1991 | Ineligible study design |
Sanchetee 1986 | Ineligible intervention |
Scott 1993 | Ineligible intervention |
Shulz 1978 | Ineligible study design |
Sireling 1983 | Ineligible study design |
Skoog 1998 | Ineligible intervention |
Smith 1979 | Ineligible intervention |
Su 2004 | Ineligible intervention |
Taghavi 2010 | Ineligible intervention |
Tang 2010 | Ineligible intervention |
Taylor 1963 | Ineligible study design |
Van Kampen 2009 | Ineligible intervention |
Wickes 1958 | Ineligible study design |
Wu 2007 | Ineligible intervention |
Young 1965 | Ineligible study design |
Yuan 1995 | Ineligible intervention |
Zhang 1990 | Ineligible intervention |
Zivkovic 2012 | Ineligible intervention |
DBT: dry‐bed training (complex behavioural intervention); DDAVP: desmopressin acetate; CT: conditioning training; RCT: randomised controlled trial.
Characteristics of studies awaiting classification [ordered by study ID]
Unuvar 2005.
Methods | RCT |
Participants | Fifty‐five children with primary monosymptomatic enuresis nocturna between 5 and 15 years of age. |
Interventions | Group 1: Twenty enuretics having intranasal desmopressin 1 x 20 ug treatment for 2 months Group 2: Twenty enuretics having conditioning therapy for 2 months. Group 3: Fifteen enuretics having intranasal izotonic solutions as placebo. The control group consisted of 15 healthy children. |
Outcomes | Urine osmolality, sodium, potassium, chloride, magnesium and creatinine levels were investigated in both daytime and nighttime urines. |
Notes | Clarification sought about whether the conditioning therapy includes alarm therapy. |
Characteristics of ongoing studies [ordered by study ID]
NCT03389412.
Study name | NCT03389412 |
Methods | Design: RCT Parallel groups Masking: children and parents Setting: multi‐centre ‐ Denmark, Belgium, China. University hospitals. |
Participants | Planning to enrol 400 participants Ages: 6 to 14 years Gender: any Diagnosis: 3 or more wet nights a week. Planning to exclude organic causes |
Interventions | Randomised to treatment based on "home recordings" of "measurement of nocturnal urine production and maximal voided volumes" Group A: "randomized to either an enuresis alarm or desmopressin without evaluating the home recordings" Group B: "The home recordings will be evaluated and treatment will be based on the recordings. Desmopressin will be administered to the children with nocturnal polyuria and the conditional alarm to the children with reduced bladder capacity. Children with nocturnal polyuria and reduced bladder capacity will be treated with both desmopressin and conditional alarm. Children with neither nocturnal polyuria nor reduced bladder capacity will again be randomized to either desmopressin or alarm treatment." |
Outcomes | All outcomes to be measured at 8 weeks and to be evaluated by "home recordings." Primary outcome: reduction in number of wet nights Secondary outcomes: 1. Reduction in nocturnal urine production 2. Increase in bladder capacity (maximal voided volume) |
Starting date | 1 October 2017 (start date). Planned completion date: 1 September 2021 |
Contact information | Principal investigator: Professor Søren Rittig, University of Aarhus, Denmark ‐ rittig@clin.au.dk |
Notes | Information taken from the ClinicalTrials.gov trials registration form at: clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT03389412 |
RCT: randomised controlled trial
Differences between protocol and review
For the 2020 update, we made the following changes:
We substantially modified the methodology of the review in accordance with current Cochrane guidance, including reconsidering the outcomes and comparisons, adding 'Summary of findings' tables and assessing the quality of the body of evidence using the GRADE approach.
We assessed all studies for selective reporting bias according to five criteria. We rated studies at low risk of reporting bias if trialists performed systematic baseline measurements of enuresis before the start of treatment, excluded children with organic causes of enuresis or daytime urinary incontinence, mentioned adverse events in the study report, provided full data sets, and reported all prespecified outcomes in keeping with the study protocol.
We removed the comparison 'supervised versus unsupervised alarm training', because we deemed it to be focused on the effectiveness of supervision of alarm training and therefore uninformative about the effectiveness of alarm training itself.
This is an update of previous systematic reviews in which treatment outcomes were mean wet nights at the end of treatment and at follow‐up after stopping treatment, the proportion of children achieving 14 consecutive dry nights by the end of treatment and the proportion of children dry at follow‐up (children maintaining 14 consecutive dry nights after stopping treatment). We have used the same outcomes as the previous reviews (but altered the wording slightly), even though they are not consistent with ICCS‐defined outcomes.
Contributions of authors
PC: assessed studies identified by the updated searches and extracted data from the included studies, reviewed the included studies from the previous review and updated the 'Risk of bias' tables, analysed the data, assisted in writing the updated review, provided a clinical perspective and interpretation. MC: assessed studies identified by the updated searches and extracted data from the included studies, reviewed the included studies from the previous review and updated the 'Risk of bias' tables, analysed the data, assisted in writing the updated review. FS: assessed studies identified by the updated searches and extracted data from the included studies, reviewed the included studies from the previous review and updated the 'Risk of bias' tables, analysed the data, assisted in writing the updated review. DH: assessed studies identified by the updated searches and extracted data from the included studies, analysed the data, assisted in writing the updated review. PS: assessed studies identified by the updated searches and extracted data from the included studies, reviewed the included studies from the previous review and updated the 'Risk of bias' tables.
Sources of support
Internal sources
No sources of support provided
External sources
-
National Institute for Health Research, UK
This project was supported by the National Institute for Health Research, via Cochrane Infrastructure funding to Cochrane Incontinence. The views and opinions expressed therein are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the Systematic Reviews Programme, NIHR, NHS or the Department of Health. The NIHR is the largest single funder of Cochrane Incontinence.
Declarations of interest
PC: is one of the inventors of an enuresis alarm. The Children's Hospital at Westmead and the University of Sydney jointly owned the patent to that alarm but the patent has now lapsed. Commercialisation is possible but not assured. The Children's Hospital at Westmead has also received a research grant and payment for development of educational material on enuresis by a pharmaceutical company. MC: none known FS: none known DH: none known PS: none known
PC and PS are authors of one of the studies included in the review (Caldwell 2015), but did not contribute to the data extraction, data analysis and discussions about that study. This was completed by FS and DH.
Edited (no change to conclusions)
References
References to studies included in this review
Ahmed 2013 {published data only}
- Ahmed AF, Amin MM, Ali MM, Shalaby EA. Efficacy of an enuresis alarm, desmopressin, and combination therapy in the treatment of saudi children with primary monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis. Korean Journal of Urology 2013;54(11):783-90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Azrin 1974 {published data only}
- Azrin NH, Sneed TJ, Foxx RM. Dry-bed training: rapid elimination of childhood enuresis. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1974;12(3):147-56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Azrin 1978 {published data only}
- Azrin NH, Thienes PM. Rapid elimination of enuresis by intensive learning without a conditioning apparatus. Behaviour Therapy 1978;9:342-54. [Google Scholar]
Bak 2007 {published data only}
- Bak M, Serdaroglu E, Bicilioglu Y. Comparing desmopressin, alarm and combined treatments in nocturnal enuretic children (Abstract number 531 (P)). Pediatric Nephrology 2007;22(9):1542. [Google Scholar]
Baker 1969 {published data only}
- Baker BL. Symptom treatment and symptom substitution in enuresis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1969;74(1):42-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bennett 1985 {published data only}
- Bennett GA, Walkden VJ, Curtis RH, Burns LE, Rees J, Gosling JA, et al. Pad-and-buzzer training, dry-bed training, and stop-start training in the treatment of primary nocturnal enuresis. Behavioural Psychotherapist 1985;13:309-19. [Google Scholar]
Bollard 1981a {published data only}
- Bollard J, Nettelbeck T. A comparison of dry-bed training and standard urine-alarm conditioning treatment of childhood bedwetting. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1981;19(3):215-26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bollard J. A 2-year follow-up of bedwetters treated by dry-bed training and standard conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1982;20(6):571-80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bollard 1981b {published data only}
- Bollard J, Nettelbeck T. A comparison of dry-bed training and standard urine-alarm conditioning treatment of childhood bedwetting. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1981;19(3):215-26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bollard J. A 2-year follow-up of bedwetters treated by dry-bed training and standard conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1982;20(6):571-80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bollard 1982a {published data only}
- Bollard J, Nettelbeck T. A component analysis of dry-bed training for treatment for bedwetting. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1982;20(4):383-90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bradbury 1995 {published data only}
- Bradbury M. Combination therapy for nocturnal enuresis with desmopressin and an alarm device. Scandinavian Journal of Urology and Nephrology. Supplementum 1997;183:61-3. [sr-incont5212] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradbury MG, Meadow SR. Combined treatment with enuresis alarm and desmopressin for nocturnal enuresis. Acta Paediatrica 1995;84(9):1014-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bryant 2003 {published data only}
- Bryant C, Fairbrother G. Baseline results: randomised trial of bladder training vs enuresis alarm vs bladder training plus enuresis alarm among children with nocturnal enuresis (Abstract 12.09 PP). In: Joint meeting of the International Children's Continence Society (ICCS) and the Asia-Pacific Association of Pediatric Urology (APAPU), 2002 Dec 10-13, Hong Kong. 2002:80.
- Bryant C, Fairbrother G. Randomised trial of bladder training vs enuresis alarm vs combined interventions in the management of nocturnal enuresis in children (Abstract). Conference unknown 2003. [SR-INCONT34172]
Butler 1988 {published data only}
- Butler RJ, Brewin CR, Forsythe WI. A comparison of two approaches to the treatment of nocturnal enuresis and the prediction of effectiveness using pre-treatment variables. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 1988;29(4):501-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Butler 1990a {published data only}
- Butler RJ, Forsythe WI, Robertson J. The body-worn alarm in the treatment of childhood enuresis. British Journal of Clinical Practice 1990;44(6):237-41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Butler 1990b {published data only}
- Butler RJ, Forsythe WI, Robertson J. The body-worn alarm in the treatment of childhood enuresis. British Journal of Clinical Practice 1990;44(6):237-41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Caceres 1982 {published data only}
- Caceres J. Comparative efficacy between two methods for the treatment of enuresis. Revista de Psicologia General y Aplicada 1980;35(4):597-616. [Google Scholar]
- Caceres J. Enuresis: Cortical control or social reinforcement? Behaviour Therapist 1982;5(2):65-7. [Google Scholar]
- Caceres J. Enuresis: Cortical control or social reinforcement? Revista de Psicologia General y Aplicada 1979;34(161):1067. [Google Scholar]
Caldwell 2015 {published data only}
- Caldwell P. The Nocturnal Enuresis Alarm Therapy (NEAT) study. anzctr.org.au/ACTRN12609000070235.aspx (first received 11 December 2008). [ACTRN12609000070235]
- Caldwell PH, Sureshkumar P, Kerr M, Hamilton S, Craig JC. The NEAT (Nocturnal Enuresis Alarm Therapy) study: an RCT of a novel alarm that enhances waking (Abstract). In: Joint Congress of the International Children's Continence Society (ICCS), Education and Resources for Improving Childhood Continence (ERIC) and the British Association of Paediatric Urology (BAPU), 2012 Oct 12-14, London. 2012. [sr-incont63865]
- Caldwell PH, Sureshkumar P, Kerr M, Hamilton S, Craig JC. The NEAT (Nocturnal Enuresis Alarm Therapy) study: a randomized controlled trial of a novel alarm that enhances waking (Abstract number O4). Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health 2012;48(Suppl 2):1-2. [sr-incont63864] [Google Scholar]
- Caldwell PH, Sureshkumar P, Kerr MI, Hamilton S, Teixeira-Pinto A, Macaskill P, et al. A randomised controlled trial of a code-word enuresis alarm. Archives of Disease in Childhood 2015;101(4):326-31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Danquah 1975 {published data only}
- Danquah SA. Comparative treatment of nocturnal enuresis among Ghanaian school children. Psychopathologie Africaine 1975;11(3):363-73. [Google Scholar]
Elinder 1985 {published data only}
- Elinder G, Soback S. Effect of Uristop on primary nocturnal enuresis. A prospective randomized double-blind study. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica 1985;74(4):574-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Evans 2011 {published data only}
- Evans J, Lottmann H. "Desmopressin response in the treatment of primary nocturnal enuresis in the United Kingdom" DRIP UK: an open-label, randomised comparative study of oral desmopressin versus enuresis alarm (Abstract number 225). In: 37th Annual Meeting of the International Continence Society (ICS), 2007 August 20-24, Rotterdam, the Netherlands. 2007.
- Evans J, Malmsten B, Maddocks A, Popli HS, Lottmann H, UK study group. Randomized comparison of long-term desmopressin and alarm treatment for bedwetting. Journal of Pediatric Urology 2011;7(1):21-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans J [personal communication]. RCT of alarm vs desmopressin. Email to: C Glazener 4 June 2004.
Fagundes 2017 {published data only}
- Fagundes SN, Lebl AS, Azevedo SL, Sousa E Silva GJ, Silvares EF, Koch VH. Monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis in pediatric patients: multidisciplinary assessment and effects of therapeutic intervention. Pediatric Nephrology (Berlin, Germany) 2017;32(5):843-51. [sr-incont74194] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Faraj 1999 {published data only}
- Faraj G, Cochat P, Cavailles ML, Chevallier C. Treatment of isolated nocturnal enuresis: alarm or desmopressin? [Traitement de l'enuresie nocturne isolee: alarme sonore ou desmopressine?]. Archives de Pediatrie 1999;6(3):271-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Fielding 1980 {published data only}
- Fielding D. The response of day and night wetting children and children who wet only at night to retention control training and the enuresis alarm. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1980;18(4):305-17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Finley 1973 {published data only}
- Finley WW, Besserman RL, Bennett LF, Clapp RK, Finley PM. The effect of continuous, intermittent, and "placebo" reinforcement on the effectiveness of the conditioning treatment for enuresis nocturna. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1973;11(3):289-97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Finley 1977 {published data only}
- Finley WW, Wansley RA. Auditory intensity as a variable in the conditioning treatment of enuresis nocturna. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1977;15(2):181-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Fournier 1987 {published data only}
- Fournier JP, Garfinkel BD, Bond A, Beauchesne H, Shapiro SK. Pharmacological and behavioral management of enuresis. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 1987;26(6):849-53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Geffken 1986a {published data only}
- Geffken G, Johnson SB, Walker D. Behavioral interventions for childhood nocturnal enuresis: the differential effect of bladder capacity on treatment progress and outcome. Health Psychology 1986;5(3):261-72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Geffken 1986b {published data only}
- Geffken G, Johnson SB, Walker D. Behavioral interventions for childhood nocturnal enuresis: the differential effect of bladder capacity on treatment progress and outcome. Health Psychology 1986;5(3):261-72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Gibb 2004 {published data only}
- Gibb S, Nolan T, South M, Noad L, Bates G, Vidmar S. Evidence against a synergistic effect of desmopressin with conditioning in the treatment of nocturnal enuresis. Journal of Pediatrics 2004;144(3):351-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hojsgaard 1979 {published data only}
- Højsgaard A, Genster H. Effect of Uristop in children with enuresis. A prospective randomized clinical trial [Effekten af uristop hos born med enuresis. En prospektiv randomiseret klinisk undersogelse]. Ugeskrift for Laeger 1979;141(10):647-8. [sr-incont1576] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hoseinzadeh 1997 {published data only}
- Hoseinzadeh E, Jalili B, Alhanie F. Comparison of drug therapy and conditioning the treatment of enuresis. Andeesheh Va Raftar 1997;3(2):79-84. [sr-incont40215] [Google Scholar]
Houts 1986 {published data only}
- Houts AC, Peterson JK, Whelan JP. Prevention of relapse in full-spectrum home training for primary enuresis: A components analysis. Behaviour Therapy 1986;17:462-9. [Google Scholar]
Jehu 1977 {published data only}
- Jehu D, Morgan RT, Turner RK, Jones A. A controlled trial of the treatment of nocturnal enuresis in residential homes for children. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1977;15(1):1-16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Kennedy 1968 {published data only}
- Kennedy WA, Sloop EW. Methedrine as an adjunct to conditioning treatment of nocturnal enuresis in normal and institutionalized retarded subjects. Psychological Reports 1968;22:997-1000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Kolvin 1972 {published data only}
- Kolvin I, Taunch J, Currah J, Garside RF, Nolan J, Shaw WB. Enuresis: a descriptive analysis and a controlled trial. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 1972;14(6):715-26. [sr-incont1607] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Kwak 2010# {published data only}
- Kwak KW, Lee YS, Park KH, Baek M. Efficacy of desmopressin and enuresis alarm as first and second line treatment for primary monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis: prospective randomized crossover study. Journal of Urology 2010;184(6):2521-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Leebeek 2001 {published data only}
- Leebeek-Groenewegen AN, Blom J, Sukhai R, Van der Heijden B. Efficacy of desmopressin combined with alarm therapy for monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis. Journal of Urology 2001;166(6):2456-8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Longstaffe 2000 {published data only}
- Longstaffe S, Moffatt ME, Whalen JC. Behavioral and self-concept changes after six months of enuresis treatment: a randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics 2000;105(4 Pt 2):935-40. [sr-incont11928] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Lovibond 1964a {published data only}
- Lovibond SH. Field experiment I: Experimental comparison of twin signal, Crosby and Mowrer instruments. In: Conditioning and Enuresis. New York: MacMillan (Pergamon Press), 1964:94-101. [Google Scholar]
Lovibond 1964b {published data only}
- Lovibond SH. Field experiments on the reduction of the relapse rate. In: Conditioning and Enuresis. New York: MacMillan (Pergamon Press), 1964:121-31. [Google Scholar]
Lovibond 1964c {published data only}
- Lovibond SH. Field experiments on the reduction of the relapse rate. In: Conditioning and Enuresis. New York: MacMillan (Pergamon Press), 1964:121-31. [Google Scholar]
Lynch 1984 {published data only}
- Lynch NT, Grunert BK, Vasudevan SV, Severson RA. Enuresis: comparison of two treatments. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 1984;65(2):98-100. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Ma 2007 {published data only}
- Ma J, Zhang Y-W, Wu H, Jiang F, Jin X-M. A randomized controlled clinical trial for treatment of children with primary nocturnal enuresis. Zhonghua Er Ke za Zhi. Chinese Journal of Pediatrics 2007;45(3):167-71. [sr-incont23089] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
McKendry 1975 {published data only}
- McKendry JB, Stewart DA, Khanna F, Netley C. Primary enuresis: relative success of three methods of treatment. Canadian Medical Association Journal 1975;113(10):953-5. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Moffatt 1987 {published data only}
- Moffatt ME, Kato C, Pless IB. Improvements in self-concept after treatment of nocturnal enuresis: randomized controlled trial. Journal of Pediatrics 1987;110(4):647-52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Motavalli 1994 {published data only}
- Motavalli N, Tuzun U, Tuna S, Yargic I, Aydogmus K. Comparison of the effectiveness of three different treatment modalities in enuresis nocturna. Noropsikiyatri Arsivi (Archives of Neuropsychiatry - Turkish) 1994;31(3):146-50. [EMBASE: 1995015454] [sr-incont7010] [Google Scholar]
Naitoh 2005 {published data only}
- Naitoh Y, Kawauchi A, Yamao Y, Seki H, Soh J, Yoneda K, et al. Combination therapy with alarm and drugs for monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis not superior to alarm monotherapy. Urology 2005;66(3):632-5. [DOI: 10.1016/j.urology.2005.03.022] [sr-incont21437] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Nawaz 2002 {published data only}
- Nawaz S, Griffiths P, Tappin D. Parent-administered modified dry-bed training for childhood nocturnal enuresis: evidence for superiority over urine-alarm conditioning when delivery factors are controlled. Behavioral Interventions 2002;17(4):247-60. [EMBASE: 2002415343] [sr-incont16300] [Google Scholar]
Netley 1984 {published data only}
- Netley C, Khanna F, McKendry JB, Lovering JS. Effects of different methods of treatment of primary enuresis on psychologic functioning in children. Canadian Medical Association Journal 1984;131(6):577-9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Ng 2005 {published data only}
- Ng CF-N, Wong SN, Hong Kong Childhood Enuresis Study Group. Comparing alarms, desmopressin, and combined treatment in Chinese enuretic children. Pediatric Nephrology 2005;20(2):163-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Onol 2015 {published data only}
- Onol FF, Guzel R, Tahra A, Kaya C, Boylu U. Comparison of long-term efficacy of desmopressin lyophilisate and enuretic alarm for monosymptomatic enuresis and assessment of predictive factors for success: a randomized prospective trial. Journal of Urology 2015;193(2):655-61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tramma D, Dokousli V. Comparison of long-term efficacy of desmopressin lyophilisate and enuretic alarm for monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis and assessment of predictive factors for success (Abstract). Pediatric Nephrology (Berlin, Germany) 2015;30(9):1594. [sr-incont70800] [Google Scholar]
Ozden 2008 {published data only}
- Ozden C, Ozdal OL, Aktas BK, Ozelci A, Altinova S, Memis A. The efficacy of the addition of short-term desmopressin to alarm therapy in the treatment of primary nocturnal enuresis. International Urology and Nephrology 2008;40(3):583-6. [sr-incont27895] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Rodriguez 2001 {published data only}
- Rodriguez do Forno A, Ariceta Iraola G. Results of a therapeutic strategy against monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis background. Anales Espanoles de Pediatria 2001;54(1):38-43. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Ronen 1995 {published data only}
- Ronen T, Rahav G, Wozner Y. Self-control and enuresis. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy 1995;9:249-58. [EMBASE: 1996053557] [sr-incont6966] [Google Scholar]
- Ronen T, Wozner Y, Rahav G. Cognitive intervention in enuresis. Child & Family Behavior Therapy 1992;14(2):1-14. [sr-incont7998] [Google Scholar]
Sacks 1974 {published data only}
- Sacks S, De Leon G, Blackman S. Psychological changes associated with conditioning functional enuresis. Journal of Clinical Psychology 1974;30(3):271-6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Scholander 1968 {published data only}
- Scholander T. Treating enuresis nocturna by a combination of medicines and conditioning [Behandling av enuresis nocturna med betingning och medikamenter]. Lakartidningen 1968;65(46):4552-6. [sr-incont4565] [PMID: ] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Seabrook 2005 {published data only}
- Seabrook JA, Gorodzinsky F, Freedman S. Treatment of primary nocturnal enuresis: a randomized clinical trial comparing hypnotherapy and alarm therapy. Paediatrics & Child Health 2005;10(10):609-10. [sr-incont32051] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Shakiba 2001 {published data only}
- Shakiba M, Golshan M. Management of nocturnal enuresis using alarm device. Journal of Shahid Sadoughi University of Medical Sciences and Health Services 2001;9(3):17-20. [sr-incont45792] [Google Scholar]
Sloop 1973 {published data only}
- Sloop EW, Kennedy WA. Institutionalized retarded nocturnal enuretics treated by a conditioning technique. American Journal of Mental Deficiency 1973;77(6):717-21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Sukhai 1989# {published data only}
- Sukhai RN, Mol J, Harris AS. Combined therapy of enuresis alarm and desmopressin in the treatment of nocturnal enuresis. European Journal of Pediatrics 1989;148(5):465-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Taylor 1975 {published data only}
- Taylor PD, Turner RK. A clinical trial of continuous, intermittent and overlearning 'bell and pad' treatments for nocturnal enuresis. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1975;13(4):281-93. [sr-incont1587] [PMID: ] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Titawee 2000 {published data only}
- Titawee K, Samorn A, Wajjanin R. Alarm conditioning and traditional method for enuresis treatment in Thai children: randomized controlled trial. Siriraj Hospital Gazette 2000;52(1):10-22. [Google Scholar]
- Titawee K. Pad and Bell Compared to Traditional Method for Enuresis Treatment in Thai Children: Randomized Controlled Trial [thesis]. Bangkok, Thailand: Chulalongkorn University, 1998. [Google Scholar]
Tobias 2001 {published data only}
- Tobias NE, McCain GC. A comparison of two enuresis alarms. Urologic Nursing 2001;21(5):349-53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Tuncel 2014 {published data only}
- Tuncel A, Balci M, Oguz U, Aslan Y, Bilgin O, Atan A. Comparison of efficacy of desmopressin fast-melting formulation and enuretic alarm-only in the treatment of nocturnal enuresis (Abstract number UP.343). Urology 2014;84(4 Suppl 1):S273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Turner 1970 {published data only}
- Turner RK, Young GC, Rachman S. Treatment of nocturnal enuresis by conditioning techniques. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1970;8(4):367-91. [sr-incont5762] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Tuygun 2007 {published data only}
- Tuygun C, Eroglu M, Bakirtas H, Gucuk A, Zengin K, Imamoglu A. Is second-line enuretic alarm therapy after unsuccessful pharmacotherapy superior to first-line therapy in the treatment of monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis? Urologia Internationalis 2007;78(3):260-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Van Hoeck 2008 {published data only}
- Van Hoeck K, Bael A, Hirche H, Lax H, Van Gool J, Trouet D. Increasing functional bladder capacity in monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis? A randomized controlled trial in 149 pre-pubertal children (Abstract number 545 (P)). Pediatric Nephrology 2007;22(9):1546. [Google Scholar]
- Van Hoeck KJ, Bael A, Lax H, Hirche H, Bernaerts K, Vandermaelen V, et al. Improving the cure rate of alarm treatment for monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis by increasing bladder capacity--a randomized controlled trial in children [see comment]. Journal of Urology 2008;179(3):1122-6; discussion 1126-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Hoeck KJ, Bael A, Van Dessel E, Van Renthergem D, Bernaerts K, Vandermaelen V, et al. Do holding exercises or antimuscarinics increase maximum voided volume in monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis? A randomized controlled trial in children. Journal of Urology 2007;178(5):2132-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Van Londen 1993 {published data only}
- Van Londen A, Van Londen-Barentsen MW, Van Son MJ, Mulder GA. Arousal training for children suffering from nocturnal enuresis: a 2 1/2 year follow-up. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1993;31(6):613-5. [sr-incont8009] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Londen A, Van Londen-Barentsen MW, Van Son MJ, Mulder GA. Relapse rate and subsequent parental reaction after successful treatment of children suffering from nocturnal enuresis: a 2 1/2 year follow-up of bibliotherapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1995;33(3):309-11. [sr-incont4208] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Vogt 2010 {published data only}
- Vogt M, Lehnert T, Till H, Rolle U. Evaluation of different modes of combined therapy in children with monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis. BJU International 2010;105(10):1456-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Wagner 1982 {published data only}
- Wagner W, Johnson SB, Walker D, Carter R, Wittner J. A controlled comparison of two treatments for nocturnal enuresis. Journal of Pediatrics 1982;101(2):302-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Wagner 1985 {published data only}
- Wagner WG, Matthews R. The treatment of nocturnal enuresis: a controlled comparison of two models of urine alarm. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 1985;6(1):22-6. [sr-incont6599] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Werry 1965 {published data only}
- Werry JS, Cohrssen J. Enuresis - an etiologic and therapeutic study. Journal of Pediatrics 1965;67(3):423-31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Wille 1986 {published data only}
- Wille S. Comparison of desmopressin and enuresis alarm for nocturnal enuresis. Archives of Disease in Childhood 1986;61(1):30-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wille S. Primary nocturnal enuresis in children. Background and treatment. Scandinavian Journal of Urology and Nephrology. Supplementum 1994;156:1-48. [sr-incont4221] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Wright 1974 {published data only}
- Wright L, Craig SC. A comparative study of amphetamine, ephedrine-atropine mixture, placebo and behavioral conditioning in the treatment of nocturnal enuresis. Journal - Oklahoma State Medical Association 1974;67(10):430-3. [sr-incont1602] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Young 1972 {published data only}
- Young GC, Morgan RT. Overlearning in the conditioning treatment of enuresis: a long-term follow-up study. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1972;10(4):419-20. [sr-incont6635] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young GC, Morgan RT. Overlearning in the conditioning treatment of enuresis. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1972;10(2):147-51. [sr-incont5761] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
References to studies excluded from this review
Akoglu 2006 {published data only}
- Akoglu E, Gorur S, Atik E, Okuyucu S, Sangun O. Effect of 1-deamino 8-D-arginine vasopressin spray on nasal cytology and mucociliary clearance in patients with nocturnal enuresis. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 2006;70(11):1919-22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Aman 2005 {published data only}
- Aman MG, Arnold LE, McDougle CJ, Vitiello B, Scahill L, Davies M, et al. Acute and long-term safety and tolerability of risperidone in children with autism. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology 2005;15(6):869-84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Arai 1974 {published data only}
- Arai M, Oogoshi R, Fukuyama Y, Mitsuya H, Uchimura N, Kodama M, et al. Treatment of analysis of comparative double-blind trials of clomipramine, amitriptyline and placebo. Rinsho Hyoka (Clinical Evaluation) 1974;2(1):47-67. [Google Scholar]
Azrin 1973 {published data only}
- Azrin NH, Sneed TJ, Foxx RM. Dry bed: a rapid method of eliminating bedwetting (enuresis) of the retarded. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1973;11(4):427-34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bollard 1977 {published data only}
- Bollard RJ, Woodroffe P. The effect of parent-administered Dry-Bed training on nocturnal enuresis in children. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1977;15(2):159-65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bollard 1982b {published data only}
- Bollard J, Nettelbeck T, Roxbee L. Dry-bed training for childhood bedwetting: a comparison of group with individually administered parent instruction. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1982;20(3):209-17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Burr 1980 {published data only}
- Burr ML, Dean BV, Merrett TG, Neale E, St Leger AS, Verrier-Jones ER. Effects of anti-mite measures on children with mite-sensitive asthma: a controlled trial. Thorax 1980;35(7):506-12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Butler 1990 {published data only}
- Butler RJ, Brewin CR, Forsythe WI. Relapse in children treated for nocturnal enuresis: prediction of response using pre-treatment variables. Behavioural Psychotherapy 1990;18:65-72. [Google Scholar]
Butler 2001 {published data only}
- Butler RJ, Holland P, Robinson J. Examination of the structured withdrawal program to prevent relapse of nocturnal enuresis. Journal of Urology 2001;166(6):2463-6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Cederblad 2015 {published data only}
- Cederblad M, Sarkadi A, Engvall G, Neveus T. No effect of basic bladder advice in enuresis: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Pediatric Urology 2015;11(3):153-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Collins 1973 {published data only}
- Collins RW. Importance of the bladder-cue buzzer contingency in the conditioning treatment for enuresis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1973;82(2):299-308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Collins 1975 {published data only}
- Collins RW, Plaska T. Mowrer's conditioning treatment for enuresis applied to geriatric residents of a nursing home. Behavior Therapy 1975;6(5):632-8. [Google Scholar]
Cracco 1982 {published data only}
- Cracco L, Facco L, Fava G. Positive reinforcement and enuresis: a controlled study. Formazione Psichiatrica 1982;2:65-8. [Google Scholar]
Crisp 1984 {published data only}
- Crisp AH, Sireling LI, Faizey J. Nocturnal activity and the enuresis alarm device. Postgraduate Medical Journal 1984;60(702):280-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macaulay AJ, Gupta M, Crisp AH, Bhat AV. The relationship between nocturnal motility and the enuresis alarm device. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 1986;30(1):63-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
de Leon 1966 {published data only}
- De Leon G, Mandell W. A comparison of conditioning and psychotherapy in the treatment of functional enuresis. Journal of Clinical Psychology 1966;22(3):326-30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Eggert 2012 {published data only}
- Eggert P, Freischmidt S, Bismarck PV, Schulz-Jurgensen S. Differentiation of subgroups of monosymptomatic enuresis according to prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex. Acta Paediatrica 2012;101(7):e304-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
El‐Anany 1999 {published data only}
- El-Anany FG, Maghraby HA, Shaker SE, Abdel-Moneim AM. Primary nocturnal enuresis: a new approach to conditioning treatment. Urology 1999;53(2):405-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Elmissiry 2013 {published data only}
- Elmissiry M, Abdelkarim A, Badawy H, Elsalmy S, Ali GA. Refractory enuresis in children and adolescents: how can urodynamics affect management and what is the optimum test? Journal of Pediatric Urology 2013;9(3):348-52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Fera 2004 {published data only}
- Fera P, Glashan R, Lelis MA, Gonzales SR, Nogueira MD, Almeida F, et al. Immediate outcome of DDAVP versus behavioural modification for treatment of monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis: a prospective randomized study of 30 patients (Abstract). Neurourology and Urodynamics 2004;23(5/6):555-6. [Google Scholar]
Fera 2011 {published data only}
- Fera P, Lelis MA, Glashan Rde Q, Pereira SG, Bruschini H. Desmopressin versus behavioral modifications as initial treatment of primary nocturnal enuresis. Urologic Nursing 2011;31(5):286-9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Finley 1982 {published data only}
- Finley WW, Rainwater AJ, Johnson G 3rd. Effect of varying alarm schedules on acquisition and relapse parameters in the conditioning treatment of enuresis. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1982;20(1):69-80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Fordham 1989 {published data only}
- Fordham KE, Meadow SR. Controlled trial of standard pad and bell alarm against mini alarm for nocturnal enuresis. Archives of Disease in Childhood 1989;64(5):651-6. [MEDLINE: ] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Forrester 1964 {published data only}
- Forrester RM, Stein Z, Susser MW. A trial of conditioning therapy in nocturnal enuresis. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 1964;6:158-66. [14141737] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Forsythe 1970 {published data only}
- Forsythe WI, Redmond A. Enuresis and the electric alarm: study of 200 cases. British Medical Journal 1970;1(690):211-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Freyman 1963 {published data only}
- Freyman R. Follow-up study of enuresis treated with a bell apparatus. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 1963;4:199-206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Ghanizadeh 2012 {published data only}
- Ghanizadeh A, Haghighat R. Nortriptyline for treating enuresis in ADHD-a randomized double-blind controlled clinical trial. Pediatric Nephrology 2012;27(11):2091-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Gillison 1958 {published data only}
- Gillison TH, Skinner JL. Treatment of nocturnal enuresis by the electric alarm. British Medical Journal 1958;ii:1268-72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Goel 1984 {published data only}
- Goel KM, Thomson RB, Gibb EM, McAinsh TF. Evaluation of nine different types of enuresis alarms. Archives of Disease in Childhood 1984;59(8):748-55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Halliday 1987 {published data only}
- Halliday S, Meadow SR, Berg I. Successful management of daytime enuresis using alarm procedures: a randomly controlled trial. Archives of Disease in Childhood 1987;62(2):132-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hansen 1995 {published data only}
- Hansen AF, Jorgensen TM. Treatment of nocturnal enuresis with the bell-and-pad system. Scandinavian Journal of Urology and Nephrology. Supplementum 1995;173:101-2. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hanson 1988 {published data only}
- Hanson RH, Thompson T, Wieseler NA. Methodological considerations in enuresis-treatment research. A three-treatment comparison. Behavior Modification 1988;12(3):335-52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hu 1995 {published data only}
- Hu WR, Xu LJ. SuoQuanLing Tie FuDai in treating enuresis. Shanghai Journal of Traditional Chinese Medicine (Shang Hai Zhong Yi Yao Za Zhi) 1995;41(9):26-7. [Google Scholar]
Hyams 1992 {published data only}
- Hyams G, McCoull K, Smith PS, Tyrer SP. Behavioural continence training in mental handicap: a 10-year follow-up study. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 1992;36(Pt 6):551-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Iester 1991 {published data only}
- Iester A, Marchesi A, Cohen A, Iester M, Bagnasco F, Bonelli R. Functional enuresis: pharmacological versus behavioral treatment. Childs Nervous System 1991;7(2):106-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Jain 2010 {published data only}
- Jain CM, Gupta A. Clinical study of an ayurvedic compound (divyadi yoga) in the management of shayyamutrata (enuresis). AYU 2010;31(1):67-75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Juul 2011 {published data only}
- Juul KV, Klein BM, Sandstrom R, Erichsen L, Norgaard JP. Gender difference in antidiuretic response to desmopressin. American Journal of Physiology - Renal Physiology 2011;300(5):F1116-22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Kahane 1955 {published data only}
- Kahane M. An experimental investigation of a conditioning treatment and a preliminary study of the psychoanalytic theory of the etiology of nocturnal enuresis. American Psychologist 1955;10:369-70. [Google Scholar]
Kajiwara 2010 {published data only}
- Kajiwara M, Miyamoto K, Masumoto H, Oki M, Teishima J, Matsubara A. Combination treatment with solifenacin plus desmopressin for children with persistent nocturnal incontinence which lasts after obtaining complete cure of urge urinary incontinence (Abstract number 194). Neurourology and Urodynamics 2010;29(6):1083-4. [Google Scholar]
Kaplan 1988 {published data only}
- Kaplan SL, Breit M, Gauthier B, Busner J. A comparison of three nocturnal enuresis treatment methods. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 1988;28(2):282-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Kazemi 2013 {published data only}
- Kazemi Rashed F, Nourizade D, Hajebrahimi S, Hasanzade K, Otoofat A. Does combination therapy with desmopressin and tolterodine improve the treatment outcomes of patients with monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis? A randomized clinical controlled trial. ISRN Urology 2013;2013:413146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Komissarov 1990 {published data only}
- Komissarov VI, Tret'iakova EE. The time-course of changes in function of the kinesthetic and acoustic analyzers in children with enuresis as influenced by acupuncture. Zhurnal Nevropatologii i Psikhiatrii Imeni S - S - Korsakova 1990;90(8):44-7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Kooijman 1986 {published data only}
- Kooijman MJ, Bosch JD. Supportive research on treatment of enuresis with a urine alarm. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor Psychology en haar Grensgebieden 1986;41(7):325-8. [Google Scholar]
Kosilov 2015 {published data only}
- Kosilov KV, Loparev SA, Ivanovskaya MA, Kosilova LV. Sensibility of children of different age groups to alarm intervention for enuresis. Journal of Nepal Paediatric Society 2015;35(1):49-56. [sr-incont70803] [Google Scholar]
Kyneb 1975 {published data only}
- Kyneb P, Biorn-Henriksen T. Treatment of enuresis of schoolchildren with 'waking-apparatus' [Behandling af enuresis hos skoleborn ved vaekkeapparat]. Maanedsskrift for Praktisk Laegegerning 1975;53(5):250-9. [Google Scholar]
Liu 2000 {published data only}
- Liu X, Zhai WS, Huang S, Huang YJ, Chen SM. Clinical observation of the treatment of infantile enuresis with suoquan capsule. Chinese Journal of Information on Traditional Chinese Medicine [Zhong Guo Zhong Yi Yao Xin Xi Za Zhi] 2000;7(6):55-6. [Google Scholar]
Lovibond 1963 {published data only}
- Lovibond SH. Intermittent reinforcement in behaviour therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1963;1(2-4):127-32. [sr-incont11232] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Lovibond 1964d {published data only}
- Lovibond SH. Field experiments on the reduction of the relapse rate. In: Conditioning and Enuresis. New York: MacMillan (Pergamon Press), 1964:121-31. [Google Scholar]
Mahler 2012 {published data only}
- Mahler B, Kamperis K, Schroeder M, Frokiaer J, Djurhuus JC, Rittig S. Sleep deprivation induces excess diuresis and natriuresis in healthy children. American Journal of Physiology. Renal Physiology 2012;302(2):F236-43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Mahony 1973 {published data only}
- Mahony DT, Laferte RO, Mahoney JE. Studies of enuresis. VI. Observations on sphincter-augmenting effect of imipramine in children with urinary incontinence. Urology 1973;1(4):317-23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
McConaghy 1969 {published data only}
- McConaghy N. A controlled trial of imipramine, amphetamine, pad-and-bell conditioning and random awakening in the treatment of nocturnal enuresis. Medical Journal of Australia 1969;2(5):237-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Merks 2012 {published data only}
- Merks BT, Burger H, Willemsen J, Van Gool JD, De Jong TP. Melatonin treatment in children with therapy-resistant monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis. Journal of Pediatric Urology 2012;8(4):416-20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Monda 1995 {published data only}
- Monda JM, Husmann DA. Primary nocturnal enuresis: a comparison among observation, imipramine, desmopressin acetate and bed-wetting alarm systems. Journal of Urology 1995;154(2 Pt 2):745-8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Montaldo 2012 {published data only}
- Montaldo P, Tafuro L, Rea M, Narciso V, Iossa AC, Gado RD. Desmopressin and oxybutynin in monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial and an assessment of predictive factors. BJU International 2012;110(8 Pt B):E381-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Muller 1999 {published data only}
- Muller D. Central nervous effect of DDAVP in children with primary nocturnal enuresis: side-effect or effect? European Journal of Pediatrics 1999;158(2):27. [Google Scholar]
NCT00209261 {published data only}
- NCT00209261, Drug company. A 6-week open label cross-over study with 2 different daily doses of Minirin© oral lyophilisate in children and adolescents with primary nocturnal enuresis (PNE) (PALAT) [A 6-week open label cross-over study with 2 different daily doses of Minirin® oral lyophilisate (120 micro g and 240 micro g) and 2 different daily doses of Minirin® tablet (0.2 mg and 2 x 0.2 mg) in children and adolescents with primary nocturnal enuresis (PNE)]. clinicaltrials.gov/show/NCT00209261 (first received 21 September 2005). [EUCTR2004-000593-32-DE] [NCT00209261] [sr-incont46750]
NCT00230594 {published data only}
- NCT00230594, Drug company. Desmopressin response in the young (DRY) [Desmopressin response in the young: a double-blind, randomised, placebo-controlled, dose-titration study with three different doses (120 mcg, 240 mcg and 360 mcg) of desmopressin administered as a new melt tablet in children and adolescents with primary nocturnal enuresis (PNE)]. clinicaltrials.gov/show/NCT00230594 (first received 3 October 2005). [NCT00230594] [sr-incont46748]
NCT00240812 {published data only}
- NCT00240812, Drug company. A study to determine if ibuprofen in combination with pseudoephedrine HCl is more effective than each drug alone in the treatment of nighttime bedwetting [A comparative study of coadministered doses of ibuprofen and pseudoephedrine HCl and each drug alone in the treatment of primary nocturnal enuresis in children]. clinicaltrials.gov/show/NCT00240812 (first received 18 October 2005). [NCT00240812] [sr-incont46749]
NCT00270621 {published data only}
- NCT00270621, McGrath PJ. Family Help Program: nighttime enuresis treatment program [Family Help Program: primary care delivery by telephone for psychological and behavioural problems (pediatric enuresis)]. clinicaltrials.gov/show/NCT00270621 (first received 28 December 2005). [NCT00270621] [sr-incont46753]
NCT00404638 {published data only}
- NCT00404638, Cohen HA. Treatment of enuresis nocturna by circular muscle exercise (Paula method). clinicaltrials.gov/show/NCT00404638 (first received 29 November 2006). [NCT00404638] [sr-incont46752]
NCT01078753 {published data only}
- NCT01078753, Drug company. Study with two different doses of desmopressin orally lyophilisate tablet with nocturnal enuresis (PNE) [A double-blind, placebo-controlled, phase III comparative study with FE992026 in children and adolescents with reduced night-time urinary osmolarity-type nocturnal enuresis]. clinicaltrials.gov/show/NCT01078753 (first received 2 March 2010). [NCT01078753] [sr-incont46747]
NCT01575678 {published data only}
- NCT01575678, Rittig S, Borg B. The effect of melatonin on nocturnal enuresis. clinicaltrials.gov/show/NCT01575678 (first received 11 April 2012). [EUCTR2011-004138-33] [NCT01575678] [sr-incont46751]
Osterberg 2006 {published data only}
- Osterberg O, Savic RM, Karlsson MO, Simonsson US, Norgaard JP, Walle JV, et al. Pharmacokinetics of desmopressin administrated as an oral lyophilisate dosage form in children with primary nocturnal enuresis and healthy adults. Journal of Clinical Pharmacology 2006;46(10):1204-11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Park 2010 {published data only}
- Park SJ, Kim JH, Shin JI. Insight on the mechanism of the effects of hydrochlorothiazide on nocturnal enuresis. Urologia Internationalis 2010;85(2):248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Perrin 2015 {published data only}
- Perrin N, Sayer L, While A. The efficacy of alarm therapy versus desmopressin therapy in the treatment of primary mono-symptomatic nocturnal enuresis: a systematic review. Primary Health Care Research and Development 2015;16(1):21-31. [sr-incont68081] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Peterson 1969 {published data only}
- Peterson RA, Wright RL, Hanlon CC. The effects on extending the CS-UCS interval of the effectiveness of the conditioning treatment for nocturnal enuresis. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1969;7(4):351-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Philpott 1970 {published data only}
- Philpott MG. The treatment of enuresis; further clinical experience with imipramine. British Journal of Clinical Practice 1970;24(8):327-9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Pretlow 1999 {published data only}
- Pretlow RA. Treatment of nocturnal enuresis with an ultrasound bladder volume controlled alarm device. Journal of Urology 1999;162(3 Pt 2):1224-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Radvanska 2011 {published data only}
- Radvanska E, Kamperis K, Kleif A, Kovacs L, Rittig S. Effect of laser acupuncture for monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis on bladder reservoir function and nocturnal urine output. Journal of Urology 2011;185(5):1857-62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Raheem 2012 {published data only}
- Raheem AA, Farahat Y, EL-Gamal O, Ragab M, Radwan M, El-Bahnasy AH, et al. The role of posterior tibial nerve stimulation in the treatment of refractory monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis. A pilot study (Abstract number 260). Neurourology and Urodynamics 2012;31(6):1065-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Raheem 2013 {published data only}
- Raheem AA, Farahat Y, EL-Gamal O, Ragab M, Radwan M, El-Bahnasy AH, et al. Role of posterior tibial nerve stimulation in the treatment of refractory monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis: a pilot study. Journal of Urology 2013;189(4):1514-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Redsell 2003 {published data only}
- Redsell SA, Collier J, Garrud P, Evans JH, Cawood C. Multimedia versus written information for nocturnal enuresis education: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Child: Care, Health and Development 2003;29(2):121-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Said 1991 {published data only}
- Said JA, Wilson PH, Hensley VR. Primary versus secondary enuresis: differential response to urine-alarm treatment. Child & Family Behavior Therapy 1991;13(2):1-13. [DOI: 10.1300/J019v13n02_01] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Sanchetee 1986 {published data only}
- Sanchetee S, Sanchetee A, Mehta LM. A placebo-controlled study of pyrithinol (encephabol) in nocturnal enuresis. Indian Medical Gazette 1986;120(4):127-9. [Google Scholar]
Scott 1993 {published data only}
- Scott MA. Facilitating Pelvic Floor Conditioning in Primary Nocturnal Enuresis [doctoral thesis]. Memphis, Tennessee, USA: University of Memphis, 1993. [Google Scholar]
Shulz 1978 {published data only}
- Shulz D, Sureth H, Lubisch G. A comparison of two behaviour therapeutic methods in the treatment of enuresis. Zeitschrift fur Klinische Psychologie 1978;7(2):141-8. [Google Scholar]
Sireling 1983 {published data only}
- Sireling LI, Crisp AH. Sleep and the enuresis alarm device. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 1983;76(2):131-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Skoog 1998 {published data only}
- Skoog S, Stowks A, Turner K. Oral desmopressin: a randomized double-blind placebo controlled study of effectiveness in children with primary nocturnal enuresis. Mezhdunarodnyi Meditsinskii Zhurnal 1998;4:173-8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Smith 1979 {published data only}
- Smith PS. A comparison of different methods of toilet training the mentally handicapped. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1979;17(1):33-43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Su 2004 {published data only}
- Su H. Treating 106 cases of enuresis with Yi Niao Gao navel administration. Chinese Journal of Information on Traditional Chinese Medicine [Zhong Guo Zhong Yi Yao Xin Xi Za Zhi] 2004;11(6):531. [Google Scholar]
Taghavi 2010 {published data only}
- Taghavi Ardakani A, Honarpisheh A, Fakharian E, Talebian A, Jamali M, Moosavi GA, et al. Oral versus nasal vasopressin in the treatment of nocturnal enuresis in 5- to 12-year-old children. Iranian Journal of Child Neurology 2010;4(1):13-6. [Google Scholar]
Tang 2010 {published data only}
- Tang FZ, Liu YL, Wen FQ, Zhang ZX. Comparison of therapeutic effects in severe nocturia: gradual versus immediate drug withdrawal. Zhongguo Dang Dai Er Ke za Zhi [Chinese Journal of Contemporary Pediatrics] 2010;12(3):198-200. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Taylor 1963 {published data only}
- Taylor IO. A scheme for the treatment of enuresis by electric buzzer apparatus. Medical Officer 1963;110:139-340. [Google Scholar]
Van Kampen 2009 {published data only}
- Van Kampen M, Bogaert G, Lemkens H, Akinwuntan E. The influence of age, motivation and pelvic floor muscle exercises on the full-spectrum therapy for nocturnal enuresis: a randomized controlled trial (Abstract number 47). Neurourology and Urodynamics 2005;24(5/6):478-80. [Google Scholar]
- Van Kampen M, Lemkens H, Deschamps A, Bogaert G, Geraerts I. Influence of pelvic floor muscle exercises on full spectrum therapy for nocturnal enuresis. Journal of Urology 2009;182(4 Suppl):2067-71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Wickes 1958 {published data only}
- Wickes IG. Treatment of persistent enuresis with the electric buzzer. Archives of Disease in Childhood 1958;33(168):160-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Wu 2007 {published data only}
- Wu F, Yiao MH. Clinical observation of Reyiting decoction in treatment of infant enuresis with sthenic fever due to retention of heat in the spleen and stomach. Zhongguo Zhong Yao za Zhi [China Journal of Chinese Materia Medica] 2007;32(15):1572-4. [Google Scholar]
Young 1965 {published data only}
- Turner RK. CNS stimulant drugs and conditioning treatment of nocturnal enuresis: a long term follow-up study. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1966;4(3):225-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young GC, Turner RK. CNS stimulant drugs and conditioning treatment of nocturnal enuresis. Behaviour Research and Therapy 1965;3(2):93-101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Yuan 1995 {published data only}
- Yuan B. The treatment of enuresis in children by He-Ne laser irradiation in 32 cases. Journal of Nanjing College of Traditional Chinese Medicine [Nanjing Zhong Yi Xue Yuan Xue Bao] 1995;11(3):54-5. [Google Scholar]
Zhang 1990 {published data only}
- Zhang H, Huang J. Preliminary study of traditional Chinese medicine treatment of minimal brain dysfunction: analysis of 100 cases. Chung Hsi i Chieh Ho Tsa Chih - Chinese Journal of Integrated Traditional & Western Medicine 1990;10(5):278-9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Zivkovic 2012 {published data only}
- Zivkovic V, Lazovic M, Vlajkovic M, Slavkovic A, Dimitrijevic L, Stankovic I, et al. Diaphragmatic breathing exercises and pelvic floor retraining in children with dysfunctional voiding. European Journal of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine 2012;48(3):413-21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
References to studies awaiting assessment
Unuvar 2005 {published data only}
- Unuvar T, Sonmez F. The role of urine osmolality and ions in the pathogenesis of primary enuresis nocturna and in the prediction of responses to desmopressin and conditioning therapies. International Urology and Nephrology 2005;37(4):751-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
References to ongoing studies
NCT03389412 {published data only}
- EUCTR2017-002169-23-DK. The effect of clinical characterization of children with bedwetting on the efficacy of therapy [The effect of clinical characterization of children with monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis on the efficacy of desmopressin and alarm therapy]. clinicaltrialsregister.eu/ctr-search/search?query=eudract_number:2017-002169-23 (first received 30 June 2017). [EUCTR2017-002169-23-DK] [NCT03389412] [sr-incont77744]
- NCT03389412, Rittig S, Jorgensen CS, Dossche L, Wen J. The effect of selecting treatment with desmopressin or alarm to children with enuresis based on home recordings [The effect of clinical characterization of children with monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis on the efficacy of desmopressin and alarm therapy]. clinicaltrials.gov/show/NCT03389412 (first received 3 January 2018). [EUCTR2017-002169-23-DK] [NCT03389412] [TrialID.DRYCHILD.] [sr-incont77912]
Additional references
APA 1980
- American Psychiatric Association. Functional enuresis. In: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3rd (revised) edition. Washington: American Psychiatric Association, 1980. [Google Scholar]
APA 2013
- American Psychiatric Association. Enuresis. In: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th edition. Arlington: American Psychiatric Association, 2013. [Google Scholar]
Austin 2014
- Austin PF, Bauer SB, Bower W, Chase J, Franco I, Hoebeke P, et al. The standardization of terminology of lower urinary tract function in children and adolescents: update report from the Standardization Committee of the International Children's Continence Society. Journal of Urology 2014;191(6):1863-5. [DOI: 10.1016/j.juro.2014.01.110] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bakwin 1971
- Bakwin H. Enuresis in twins. American Journal of Diseases of Childhood 1971;121(3):222-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bakwin 1973
- Bakwin H. The genetics of enuresis. In: Kolvin I, MacKeith RC, Meadow SR, editors(s). Bladder Control and Enuresis. London: William Heinemann Medical Books, 1973. [Google Scholar]
Blackwell 1989
- Blackwell C. A Guide to Enuresis: A Guide to Treatment of Enuresis for Professionals. Bristol: ERIC, 1989. [Google Scholar]
Butler 2000
- Butler RJ, Holland P. The three systems: a conceptual way of understanding nocturnal enuresis. Scandinavian Journal of Urology and Nephrology 2000;34(4):270-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Caldwell 2013
- Caldwell PH, Nankivell G, Sureshkumar P. Simple behavioural interventions for nocturnal enuresis in children. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2013, Issue 7. Art. No: CD003637. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD003637.pub3] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Caldwell 2016
- Caldwell PH, Sureshkumar P, Wong WCF. Tricyclic and related drugs for nocturnal enuresis in children. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2016, Issue 1. Art. No: CD002117. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD002117.pub2] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
De Jonge 1973
- De Jonge GA. Epidemiology of enuresis: A survey of the literature. In: Kolvin I, MacKeith RC, Meadow SR, editors(s). Bladder Control and Enuresis. London: William Heinemann Medical Books, 1973. [Google Scholar]
Deshpande 2011
- Deshpande AV, Craig JC, Smith GH, Caldwell PH. Factors influencing quality of life in children with urinary incontinence. Journal of Urology 2011;186(3):1048-52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Deshpande 2012
- Deshpande AV, Caldwell PH, Sureshkumar P. Drugs for nocturnal enuresis in children (other than desmopressin and tricyclics). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2012, Issue 12. Art. No: CD002238. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD002238.pub2] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Devlin 1991
- Devlin JB. Prevalence and risk factors for childhood nocturnal enuresis. Irish Medical Journal 1991;84(4):118-20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Djurhuus 1992
- Djurhuus JC, Norgaard JP, Rittig S. Monosymptomatic bedwetting. Scandinavian Journal of Urology and Nephrology. Supplementum 1992;141:7-17; discussion 18-9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Eiberg 1995
- Eiberg H, Berendt I, Mohr J. Assignment of dominant inherited nocturnal enuresis (ENUR1) to chromosome 13q. Nature Genetics 1995;10(3):354-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Fitzwater 1992
- Fitzwater D, Macknin ML. Risk/benefit ratio in enuresis therapy. Clinical Pediatrics (Philadelphia) 1992;31(5):308-10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Forsythe 1974
- Forsythe WI, Redmond A. Enuresis and spontaneous cure rate. A study of 1129 enuretics. Archives of Disease in Childhood 1974;49(4):259-63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Forsythe 1989
- Forsythe WI, Butler RJ. Fifty years of enuretic alarms. Archives of Disease in Childhood 1989;64(6):879-85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Glazener 2002
- Glazener CM, Evans JH. Desmopressin for nocturnal enuresis in children. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2002, Issue 3. Art. No: CD002112. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD002112] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Glazener 2004
- Glazener CM, Evans JH, Peto RE. Complex behavioural and educational interventions for nocturnal enuresis in children. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2004, Issue 1. Art. No: CD004668. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD004668] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Guyatt 2008
- Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Vist GE, Kunz R, Falck-Yitter Y, Alonso-Coello P, et al. GRADE: an emerging consensus on rating quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.) 2008;336(7650):924-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hagglof 1998
- Hagglof B, Andren O, Bergstrom E, Marklund L, Wendelius M. Self-esteem in children with nocturnal enuresis and urinary incontinence: improvement of self-esteem after treatment. European Urology 1998;33(Suppl 3):16-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Higgins 2003
- Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.) 2003;327(7414):557-60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Higgins 2011
- Higgins JP, Green S, editor(s). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions, Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from www.cochrane-handbook.org.
Higgins 2017
- Higgins JP, Altman DG, Sterne JA, editor(s). Chapter 8: Assessing risk of bias in included studies. In: Higgins JP, Churchill R, Chandler J, Cumpston MS, editor(s), Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 5.2.0 (updated June 2017), The Cochrane Collaboration, 2017. Available from www.training.cochrane.org/handbook.
Huang 2011
- Huang T, Hsu X, Huang YS, Cheuk DK. Complementary and miscellaneous interventions for nocturnal enuresis in children. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2011, Issue 12. Art. No: CD005230. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD005230.pub2] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Jarvelin 1989
- Jarvelin MR. Developmental history and neurological findings in enuretic children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 1989;31(6):728-36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Koff 1995
- Koff SA. Why is desmopressin sometimes ineffective at curing bedwetting? Scandinavian Journal of Urology and Nephrology. Supplementum 1995;173:103-8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Krantz 1994
- Krantz I, Jylkas E, Ahlberg BM, Wedel H. On the epidemiology of nocturnal enuresis--a critical review of methods used in descriptive epidemiological studies on nocturnal enuresis. Scandinavian Journal of Urology and Nephrology. Supplementum 1994;163:75-82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Loening‐Baucke 1997
- Loening-Baucke V. Urinary incontinence and urinary tract infection and their resolution with treatment of chronic constipation of childhood. Pediatrics 1997;100(2 Pt 1):228-32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Maizels 1993
- Maizels M, Gandhi K, Keating B, Rosenbaum D. Diagnosis and treatment for children who cannot control urination. Current Problems in Pediatrics 1993;23(10):402-50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
McGrath 2008
- McGrath KH, Caldwell PH, Jones MP. The frequency of constipation in children with nocturnal enuresis: a comparison with parental reporting. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health 2008;44(1-2):19-27. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2007.01207.x] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Moffatt 1989
- Moffatt ME. Nocturnal enuresis: psychologic implications of treatment and nontreatment. Journal of Pediatrics 1989;114(4 Pt 2):697-704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Morgan 1970
- Morgan R. The treatment of enuresis amongst child clients of a social services department. British Journal of Social Work 1970;9(2):217-31. [Google Scholar]
Mowrer 1938
- Mowrer OH, Mowrer WM. Enuresis - a method for its study and treatment. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 1938;8(3):436-59. [Google Scholar]
Norgaard 1993
- Norgaard JP, Djurhuus JC. The pathophysiology of enuresis in children and young adults. Clinical Pediatrics 1993;Spec No:5-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
O'Regan 1986
- O'Regan S Yazbeck S, Hamberger B, Schick E. Constipation a commonly unrecognized cause of enuresis. American Journal of Diseases of Children 1986;140(3):260-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Reference Manager 2012 [Computer program]
- Reference Manager Professional Edition Version 12. New York: Thomson Reuters, 2012.
Rutter 1973
- Rutter M, Yule W, Graham P. Enuresis and behavioural deviance. Some epidemiological considerations. In: Kolvin I, MacKeith RC, Meadow SR, editors(s). Bladder Control and Enuresis. London: William Heinemann Medical Books, 1973. [Google Scholar]
Schulz 2010
- Schulz KE, Altman DG, Moher D, CONSORT Group. CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.) 2010;340:c332. [DOI: 10.1136/bmj.c332 ] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Shaffer 1977
- Shaffer D. Enuresis. In: Rutter M, editors(s). Child Psychiatry: Modern Approaches. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications, 1977. [Google Scholar]
Sureshkumar 2009
- Sureshkumar P, Jones M, Cumming R, Craig J. A population based study of 2,856 school-age children with urinary incontinence. Journal of Urology 2009;181(2):808-15; discussion 815-6. [PMID: ] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Warzak 1992
- Warzak WJ. Psychosocial implications of nocturnal enuresis. Clinical Pediatrics 1992;Spec No:38-40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
WHO 1992
- WHO. Nonorganic enuresis. In: WHO, editors(s). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: WHO, 1992. [Google Scholar]
Yeung 2004
- Yeung CK, Sihoe JD, Sit FK, Bower W, Sreedhar B, Lau J. Characteristics of primary nocturnal enuresis in adults: an epidemiological study. BJU International 2004;93(3):341-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Yeung 2006
- Yeung CK, Sreedhar B, Sihoe JD, Sit FK, Lau J. Differences in characteristics of nocturnal enuresis between children and adolescents: a critical appraisal from a large epidemiological study.. BJU International 2006;97(5):1069-73. [DOI: 10.1111/j.1464-410X.2006.06074.x] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
References to other published versions of this review
Glazener 2001
- Glazener CM, Evans JH. Alarm interventions for nocturnal enuresis in children. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2001, Issue 1. Art. No: CD002911. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD002911] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Glazener 2003
- Glazener CM, Evans JH, Peto RE. Alarm interventions for nocturnal enuresis in children. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2003, Issue 2. Art. No: CD002911. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD002911] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Glazener 2005
- Glazener CM, Evans JH, Peto RE. Alarm interventions for nocturnal enuresis in children. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2005, Issue 2. Art. No: CD002911. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD002911.pub2] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Glazener 2007
- Glazener CM, Evans JH, Peto RE. Alarm interventions for nocturnal enuresis in children. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2007, Issue 3. Art. No: CD002911. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD002911.pub2] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Lister‐Sharp 1997
- Lister-Sharp D, O'Meara S, Bradley M, Sheldon TA, NHS Centre for Reviews and Dissemination. A systematic review of the effectiveness of interventions for managing childhood nocturnal enuresis. NHS Centre for Reviews and Dissemination, University of York; Report No.: CRD-11 1997.