Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Mar 23.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Biol. 2020 Mar 5;30(6):1063–1076.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.12.065

Bidirectional regulation of sleep and synapse pruning after neural injury

Prabhjit Singh 1, Jeffrey M Donlea 1,2
PMCID: PMC7199647  NIHMSID: NIHMS1572050  PMID: 32142703

Summary

Following acute neural injury, severed axons undergo programmed Wallerian degeneration over several following days. While sleep has been linked with synaptic reorganization under other conditions, the role of sleep in responses to neural injuries remain poorly understood. To study the relationship between sleep and neural injury responses, we examined Drosophila melanogaster following removal of antennae or other sensory tissues. Daytime sleep is elevated after antennal or wing injury, but sleep returns to baseline levels within 24 hours after injury. Similar increases in sleep are not observed when olfactory receptor neurons are silenced or when other sensory organs are severed, suggesting that increased sleep after injury is not attributed to sensory deprivation, nociception, or generalized inflammatory responses. Neuroprotective disruptions of the E3 ubiquitin ligase highwire and c-Jun N-terminal kinase basket in olfactory receptor neurons weaken the sleep-promoting effects of antennal injury, suggesting that post-injury sleep may be influenced by the clearance of damaged neurons. Finally, we show that pre-synaptic active zones are preferentially removed from severed axons within hours after injury, and that sleep depriving recently injured flies slows the removal of both active zones and damaged axons. These data support a bidirectional interaction between sleep and synapse pruning after antennal injury: locally increasing the need to clear neural debris is associated with increased sleep, which is required for efficient active zone removal after injury.

ETOC Blurb

Singh and Donlea find that severing the antennal nerves of the fruit fly acutely increases sleep. Sleep responses of injured flies can be weakened by manipulations that protect severed axons from removal. Sleep deprivation after injury slows the removal of neural debris from severed axons, indicating a role for sleep in recovery from neural damage.

Introduction

Sleep is a state that has been broadly conserved across animal species [1,2], but its basic biological functions remain poorly understood. Because deficits in memory and neural plasticity are among the earliest consequences of sleep loss, it is likely that basic functions of sleep promote plasticity [3]. While recent studies support a role for sleep in the homeostatic downscaling and pruning of synapses to prevent circuit saturation and retain the capacity to encode new information [49], the contexts during which sleep contributes to synapse remodeling require further examination. Here, we explore the relationship between sleep and the removal of presynapses and axons after axotomy. Drosophila antennal transection provides a reliable assay to acutely increase local axonal pruning [10,11], but the role for sleep in clearing injured axons has not been examined in the fly. A growing literature, however, links traumatic brain injury and sleep/wake changes in humans and rodents with several reports describing hypersomnia occurring acutely after neural trauma [1214]. This increased sleep may facilitate functional recovery after injury; maximizing sleep after injury can reduce neuron loss and limit behavioral impairments in rodents [15,16]. Similarly, disrupted sleep after traumatic brain injury is associated with decreased functional recovery in humans, and treatment for sleep disorders after brain injury can significantly aid cognitive recovery [17]. These studies implicate a role for sleep in acute neural pruning after injury, and Drosophila provides a model system with possibilities for precise targeting of both neural damage and genetic manipulations.

To characterize the conditions that promote synaptic pruning during sleep, we have examined Drosophila following antennal injury. Primary olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) send axonal projections from the antennae, where they detect odorants in the external world, to neuropil glomeruli in the antennal lobe [1820]. In these olfactory glomeruli, ORNs synapse upon dendrites of secondary projection neurons, which convey olfactory signals to circuits in the Mushroom bodies and Lateral horns [18,21,22]. When the antennae are transected, the distal segment of severed ORN axons fragment and are removed over several following days [10]. Similar degeneration has also been observed in chemo- and mechano-sensory neurons after injuries to other appendages, including the maxillary palps, wings, and legs [10,23], indicating that the underlying mechanisms may be generalizable to circuits throughout the nervous system. Interestingly, the fragmentation and clearance of damaged ORNs is controlled by a molecular program that is shared with vertebrate models of degeneration after axonal injury [10,2427]. Over the past decade, the molecular and genetic programs in Drosophila that promote the clearance of damaged axons after neural injury have been carefully dissected [10,2830]. Removal of injured ORN axons relies upon the neural expression of several genes, including the E3 ubiquitin ligase highwire (hiw); mutations in hiw delay the removal of injured axons from the antennal lobe by weeks [29]. These studies provide an opportunity to test the functional role for sleep in pre-synaptic removal following neural injury. Here, we show that sleep is acutely increased after neural injury. Importantly, injuries that do not sever neurons and injuries in flies expressing neuroprotective manipulations of highwire and basket do not elicit an increase in sleep, suggesting that post-injury sleep may be linked with the degeneration of damaged neurons. Finally, we measure synaptic pruning and membrane clearance from damaged axons and find that sleep may promote the removal of both synaptic terminals and axonal membrane after injury.

Results

Sleep is temporarily increased following antennal transection

To measure the effect of neural injury on sleep, 4–7 day old Canton-S (Cs) females were monitored for two days of baseline sleep, then briefly anesthetized for ~20 seconds with CO2 while antennae were bilaterally transected at the third segment using forceps (See schematic in Figure 1A). After antennal injury, each fly was placed back into a glass tube containing standard Drosophila media and sleep was monitored during recovery. The sleep of injured Cs flies was compared to those of siblings that received the same handling and anesthesia exposure, but whose antennae remained intact. Following antennal transection, sleep is strongly increased for ~9–10 hours in injured Cs flies compared to uninjured siblings (Figure 1B; the time window of antennal injury is depicted with yellow shading). This temporary sleep increase from injured Cs flies dissipates by the following day, when sleep time of injured flies returns to the same levels as in uninjured controls (Figure 1C). Increased sleep after antennal injury can be attributed to a lengthening of sleep bout duration (Figure 1D). Because prolonged sleep bout length is hypothesized to be a marker of sleep intensity in Drosophila [31], we also exposed flies to varying durations of mechanical vibration to test arousability in injured flies and uninjured siblings during the day of antennal transection. A smaller portion of injured flies was awakened by 1, 5, or 10 second-long mechanical stimulations using a 0.5g mechanical vibration (Figure S1A, left). A more intense 1.5g stimulus, however, was sufficient to awaken the majority of flies in injured and control groups (Figure S1A, right), suggesting that antennal injury promotes a state of deep sleep with elevated arousal thresholds. To further verify that sleep is increased after antennal injury, we monitored locomotion using multi-beam activity monitors that contain 17 infrared sensors across a 51mm tube length (Trikinetics Inc; Waltham MA, USA). Experiments using these multi-beam monitors confirmed that sleep is increased after antennal transection (Figure S1C). Single beam monitors found a modest but significant decrease in waking activity as measured by counts/waking minute after antennal injury (Figure 1G); measurements using multibeam monitors found no significant main effect of injury on hourly averages of movements/waking minute during the day of injury (Figure S1D). We also tested whether sleep drive is increased after antennal injury by testing the responses of short-sleeping insomniac (inc) [32,33] and sleepless (sss) mutants [34]. Neither inc nor sss mutants had any significant sleep increase following antennal transection (Figure S1EJ), suggesting a link between increased sleep after antennal injury and previously characterized sleep regulatory genes.

Figure 1 – Sleep is temporarily increased following antennal transection.

Figure 1 –

(A) Schematic of primary olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) in Drosophila melanogaster. Antennae are bilaterally transected at the 3rd segment to sever ORN axons.

(B) Sleep in minutes/hour in Canton-S (Cs) flies from either control (grey) or antennal transection (blue) groups. Sleep is acutely increased during the hours after antennal injury, but returns to control levels by 24-hours later. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA of hourly sleep timecourse finds a significant time by treatment interaction (F(10,10010)=22.37, p<0.0001; n=65 injured, 80 control).

(C-D) Sleep time (C) and mean sleep bout length (D) during the day is increased immediately following injury, but decreases back to baseline on the following day of recovery. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds a significant day by treatment interaction for day sleep time (F(2,286)=63.98, p<0.0001) and day bout length (F(2,286)=26.24, p<0.0001).

(E) Sleep time is decreased in Cs flies during the night immediately following antennal transection. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds a significant day by treatment interaction (F(2,290)=5.497, p=0.0045). * signifies pairwise p<0.05.

(F) Antennal transection does not alter the length of sleep bouts during the night. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds no day by treatment interaction (F(2,290)=1.167, p=0.3128).

(G) Activity counts/waking minute during baseline, injury day, and recovery day in controls in injured flies. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds a significant day by treatment interaction (F(2,290)=18.06, p<0.0001).

(H) Antennal injury at ZT11–12 is followed by an increase in sleep during the subsequent day. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA finds a significant time-bytreatment interaction (F(70,7280) = 7.617, p < 0.0001; mean ± SEM).

(I) Daytime sleep is increased for one day following antennal transection at ZT11–12 and returns to control levels by the following day of recovery. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA finds a significant day-by-treatment interaction (F(2,208) = 12.40, p < 0.0001).

See also Figures S1 and S2.

Interestingly, sleep during the night following antennal transection is modestly decreased in injured Cs flies (Figure 1E), but the length of night-time sleep bouts was not different between injured flies and controls (Figure 1F). We tested whether sleep during the night is increased immediately following injury by observing the responses to antennal transection shortly before the beginning of the night (ZT11–12). As shown in Figure 1H, flies that underwent antennal transection prior to lights-off did not show an increase in sleep at night, but did increase their sleep for several hours of the subsequent morning. These results indicate that increases in sleep are restricted to the day, possibly due to circadian influence. This response follows the pattern of homeostatic sleep drive that is observed when flies are released into recovery from sleep deprivation at the end of the light phase [35]. Wild-type Cs flies housed in constant darkness respond to antennal transection with an increase in sleep after antennal injury (Figure S1B), and mutants for the core circadian genes clock (ClkJRK [36]) and cycle (cyc01 [37]) show a significant increase in sleep after antennal injury (Figure S2AD). We also tested responses of null mutants for timeless (tim01 [38]) and period (per01 [39]) in two genetic backgrounds. tim01 flies slept more after antennal injury in one background (yw) (Figure S2EG), while per01 mutants did not increase their sleep in either genetic background (Figure S2HJ). Together, these results suggest that an intact circadian clock is not required for increased sleep after antennal injury, but that period may influence injury responses via processes independent of circadian timekeeping. Interestingly, mutants for period, but not other circadian genes, also exhibit memory deficits [4,40], consistent with non-circadian functions for per.

Increased sleep following injury is dependent upon site of neural damage

After finding that sleep was increased after antennal injury, we next tested whether post-injury sleep was related specifically to neural damage or to generalized inflammatory responses to damage of any tissue. First, we measured whether the amount of post-injury sleep is proportional to the number of damaged axons by measuring the effect of injuring one or two antennae on subsequent sleep. As shown in Figure 2A, severing both antennae produces a larger increase in sleep than removing only one antenna, suggesting that post-injury sleep is correlated with the number of damaged ORNs. To further test the relationship between the magnitude of neural injury and post-injury changes in sleep, we tested whether damage to other sensory tissues also affects sleep. We focused these experiments on the maxillary palps, a second pair of olfactory organs that each contain ~125 ORNs [41], the wings, which contain ~450 sensory neurons per wing in chemosensory sensilla along their anterior edge [42], and the halteres, which each contain ~335 proprioceptive sensory neurons [43]. Our experiments avoided injuring other peripheral areas that contribute to locomotion, such as the legs, or to the entrainment of the circadian clock, including the visual system. A significant increase in post-injury sleep was detected after wing injuries (Figure 2B), but transections of the maxillary palp (Figure 2C) and haltere transections (Figure 2D) had little effect on subsequent sleep. While a modest decrease in sleep was observed during the night after antennal injuries (Figure 1E), wing injury resulted in increased sleep during several night-time hours (Figure 2B). These differing effects on night-time sleep could indicate that the more intense sleep response following antennal injury may decrease homeostatic sleep drive during the following night. Further studies will be required to examine the differential effects of antennal and wing injuries on sleep during the following night.

Figure 2 – Post-injury increases in sleep vary with site of injury.

Figure 2 –

(A) Hourly sleep in wild-type Cs flies after transection of 2 antennae (dark blue) or 1 antenna (light blue) compared to uninjured controls (gray). Post-injury daytime sleep totals for each group is shown on right. One-way ANOVA finds a significant effect of condition on post-injury daytime sleep (F(2,104)=54.05, p<0.0001, n=29 in each injured group, 49 controls). * signifies pairwise p<0.05.

(B) Hourly sleep totals (left) and post-injury daytime sleep totals (right) from Cs flies that underwent wing injury (green) and uninjured controls (gray). Two-way unpaired t-test for post-injury sleep totals finds a significant effect of wing injury (t= 4.078; p< 0.0001; n=58 injured, 55 controls).

(C) Sleep timecourse (left) and post-injury daytime sleep totals (right) for Cs flies after maxillary palp removal (orange) compared to uninjured siblings (gray). Two-way unpaired t-test for post-injury sleep totals finds no significant effect of palp injury (t= 1.078; p= 0.2834; n=54 injured, 49 controls).

(D) Hourly sleep totals (left) and post-injury daytime sleep totals (right) from Cs flies that underwent haltere removal (red) and uninjured controls (gray). Two-way unpaired t-test for post-injury sleep totals finds no significant effect of haltere injury (t= 0.9145; p= 0.3648; n=28 injured, 24 controls).

(E) Sleep time course (left) and post-injury sleep totals (right) for flies that underwent injuries to both the maxillary palps and halteres (red with orange outline) and uninjured controls (gray). Two-way unpaired t test finds no significant difference in post-injury sleep time (t = 0.8543; p = 0.3970; n = 25 injured, 27 controls).

(F) Hourly sleep timecourse (left) and post-injury sleep totals (right) for flies that underwent injuries to both the wings and halteres (red with green outline) and uninjured controls (gray). Two-way unpaired t-test finds a significant effect of injury on post-injury sleep time (t=3.060, p=0.005, n=15 injured, 14 controls).

Next, we tested whether combining injuries at multiple sites might alter sleep during the following hours by subjecting flies to both maxillary palp and haltere injuries (Figure 2E) or both wing and haltere injuries (Figure 2F). These combined injury conditions did not result in additive effects compared to responses after injuries to individual sites, consistent with the possibilities that sleep is significantly increased only after injuries to the wings or antennae, or that the increase in sleep after neural injury is determined by the largest site of injury. Interestingly, these results include injuries to sensory neurons that project directly into the brain from the antennae and maxillary palps, as well as neurons from the wings and halteres that limit their axons to the ventral nerve cord (VNC). While wing and haltere injuries likely drive degeneration of sensory neuron projections in the VNC, it is not clear whether they also lead to the reorganization of secondary projections into the central brain.

Loss of olfaction does not increase sleep

While the increased sleep after antennal transection is consistent with a role for sleep in neural degradation after injury, it is also possible that the change in sleep might be influenced by nociceptive signaling or loss of sensory perception. To test whether post-injury increases in sleep could be attributed to loss of olfactory perception, we acutely silenced ORNs by expressing the inward rectifying potassium channel Kir2.1 under the control of orco-GAL4 and the GAL4 repressor tubP-GAL80TS [4446]. Flies were raised and baseline sleep was measured at 18°C (Figure 3A), then transferred to 29°C for two days to permit Kir2.1 transgene expression in ORNs before sleep was measured again (Figure 3B). No significant difference in daytime sleep was observed between the experimental flies expressing Kir2.1 in ORNs and genetic controls at either 18°C or 29°C (Figure 3C), and a significant reduction in nighttime sleep was detected in experimental flies at 29°C (Figure 3D). We verified that expression of Kir2.1 in ORNs impaired olfactory perception by using an olfactory trap assay. When housed at 18°C, experimental orco-GAL4/tubP-GAL80TS; UAS-kir2.1/+ flies and genetic controls showed similar preferences to traps containing apple juice versus water (Figure S3A, left). After two days at 29°C, however, experimental flies expressing Kir2.1 in ORNs exhibited a significant loss of attraction to apple juice (Figure S3A, right). These results indicate that loss of olfactory perception is not sufficient to increase sleep.

Figure 3 – Loss of olfactory perception does not contribute to post-injury increases in sleep.

Figure 3 –

(A-B) Silencing ORNs by expressing UAS-kir2.1 under the control of orco-GAL4 does not alter sleep. When housed at 18°C to repress Kir2.1 expression in orco-GAL4/UAS-kir2.1; tubP-GAL80TS/+ flies (A) or at 29°C to increase ORN expression of Kir2.1 (B).

(C) Day sleep totals at 18°C and 29°C for flies shown in (A-B). Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds a significant main effect for temperature (F(1,122)=105.7, p<0.001) and for genotype (F(2,122)=6.159, p=0.0028).

(D) Total night sleep amounts for flies shown in (A-B) at 18°C and 29°C. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds a genotype x temperature interaction (F(1,122)=4.468, p=0.0134).

(E-F) Hourly sleep for parasbl−1/+ controls (E) and parasbl−1 homozygous mutants (F) during the day of antennal injury. Injured flies increased their sleep for several hours after injury compared to uninjured siblings. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds a significant time x injury interaction for parasbl−1/+ controls (F(22,550)=15.36, p<0.0001, n=12–15 flies/group) and for parasbl−1 mutants (F(22,638)=8.113, p<0.0001, n=15–16 flies/group).

(G) Daytime sleep for parasbl−1/+ heterozygotes (left; injured flies in dark gray, controls in light gray) and parasbl−1 homozygous mutants (right; injured flies in dark blue, controls in light blue) during baseline, injury, and recovery days. Both parasbl−1/+ heterozygotes and parasbl−1 mutants exhibit similar increases in sleep after antennal transection. Two-way ANOVA finds a significant day x group interaction (F(6,108)=15.71, p<0.0001, n=12–16 flies/group).

See also Figure S3.

Similarly, if increased sleep after injury can be attributed to clearance of damaged axons and not loss of sensory perception, then mutants with generalized olfactory deficits should exhibit the same post-injury sleep responses as wild-type flies. To test this hypothesis, we performed antennal injuries on smellblind (parasbl−1) mutant flies, which exhibit deficits in discriminating pure odorants [47,48] and socially-relevant pheromonal cues [49,50]. Homozygous parasbl−1 mutants and heterozygous parasbl−1/+ controls both showed significant increases in sleep after antennal injury (Figure 3EG), suggesting that post-injury sleep responses are not primarily caused by sensory loss. Furthermore, flies in which ORNs are silenced by expression of Kir2.1 exhibit a similar increase in sleep after antennal injury as genetic controls (Figure S3B). However, certain para mutants that exhibit seizures at elevated temperatures have also been shown to prevent the removal of neurites and synapses from severed axons [51], potentially confounding our hypothesis. We therefore performed antennal transections in parasbl−1 mutant males and observed robust losses of both ORN axonal membrane, labelled with CD8::GFP, and a genetic reporter for the pre-synaptic protein BRP (Synaptic Tagging with Recombination, STaR [52]) within 24 hours after injury (Figure S3CD). Taken together, these data are consistent with our hypothesis that increased sleep after injury can be attributed to neural injury, not altered olfactory perception.

Intact ORN axons are required for increased post-injury sleep

If increased sleep after antennal transection is correlated with the degeneration of damaged ORNs, then sleep should not be changed after injury in flies with reduced ORN innervation of the antennal lobes. To test this hypothesis, we measured sleep after injury in flies mutant for the olfactory co-receptor orco, which is required for olfactory receptor proteins to form odorantgated ion channels [53,54]. Without any electrical activity, ORNs in orco mutants are unable to maintain their axonal projections and withdraw from the antennal lobe [55]. To verify that ORN axons retract in orco mutants, we expressed CD8::GFP in ORNs that project their axons into the DM2 glomerulus using OR22a-GAL4. DM2 glomeruli in orco mutant backgrounds show a 84–93% reduction in GFP-labelled ORN axons compared to heterozygous controls (Figure 4AB). To test whether intact ORN axons are required for increased sleep after injury, 4–6 day old flies carrying orco1 and/or orco2 mutations [54] were loaded into activity monitors, then underwent antennal transection after 2–3 days of baseline sleep. While heterozygous controls (orco1/+ and orco2/+) showed a wild-type increase in sleep after injury, no post-injury changes in sleep could be detected in homozygous orco1 or orco2 mutants or in orco1/orco2 transheterozygotes (Figure 4CE). Similarly, post-antennal injury sleep bouts were lengthened in heterozygous controls (orco1/+ and orco2/+), but not in orco mutant flies (orco1, orco2, and orco1/orco2) (Figure S4). Because orco mutants show significant increases in sleep after wing injury (Figure 4FH), their lack of sleep responses after antennal transection is likely attributable to ORN loss.

Figure 4 – orco mutations drive ORN axon degeneration during early adulthood and prevent later changes in sleep after injury.

Figure 4 –

(A) Representative images of membrane-bound CD8::GFP in ORNs under the control of OR22a-GAL4 in a heterozygous background (orco1/+ or orco2/+; top) or in orco mutants (orco1, orco2, and orco1/orco2). Scale bars represent 20μm.

(B) Quantification of OR22a-GAL4 driven UAS-CD8::GFP in orco mutants (orco1, orco2, and orco1/orco2) and heterozygous controls (orco1/+ and orco2/+). One-way ANOVA finds a significant effect of genotype (F(4,111)=191.3, p<0.0001, n=20–26 hemispheres/group).

(C) Hourly sleep timecourse during the day of antennal injury for orco1/+ flies. Sleep amounts for injured flies shown in dark gray and for uninjured controls in light gray. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds a significant injury x time interaction (F(23,1288)=11.92, p<0.0001; n=27 injured, 31 control).

(D) Hourly sleep timecourse for injured orco1 homozygous mutant flies (dark blue) and uninjured sibling controls (light blue). Injured orco1 mutants show a dampened increase in sleep relative to heterozygous orco1/+ flies. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds a significant injury x time interaction (F(23,1334)=1.629, p=0.031; n=27 injured, 29 control).

(E) Post-injury daytime sleep totals for orco heterozygous controls (orco1/+ and orco2/+; grays) and orco mutant genotypes (orco1, orco2, and orco1/orco2; blues). Injured are depicted in dark shades, uninjured siblings in light shades. Each dot represents an individual fly, group means depicted by black bars. Two-way ANOVA finds a significant genotype x injury interaction (F(4,286)=27.71, p<0.0001, n=27–31 flies/group).

(F) Hourly sleep timecourse for orco1/+ flies during the day of wing injury. Sleep amounts for injured flies shown in dark grey and for uninjured controls in light grey. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds a significant time x injury interaction (F(23,598)=3.562, p<0.0001, n=13–15 flies/group).

(G) Hourly sleep timecourse on the day of wing injury for orco1 homozygous mutants. Sleep amounts for injured mutants in dark blue and for uninjured mutants in light blue. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds a significant effect of injury (F(1,27)=9.087, p=0.0055, n=14–15 flies/group).

(H) Daytime sleep totals on the day of wing injury for orco mutants (orco1, orco2, and orco1/orco2) and heterozygous controls (orco1/+ and orco2/+). For each genotype, injured flies showed an increase in daytime sleep after injury compared to uninjured siblings. Two-way ANOVA finds a significant effect of injury (F(1,135)=84.21, p<0.0001) and genotype (F(4,135)=12.55, p<0.0001).

See also Figure S4.

Highwire is required for post-injury sleep induction

Several studies over the past decade have identified a network of genes expressed in Drosophila neurons that influence the kinetics of fragmentation and clearance of injured axons [10,27,28,30,5658]. This network includes the E3 ubiquitin ligase highwire (hiw), which acts as a core regulator for degeneration of injured axons [29,57]. Mutations in hiw prevent the fragmentation and removal of injured ORN axons; severed ORN axons after antennal transection remain largely intact in hiw mutants for at least 20 days after injury [29]. To test whether hiw is also required to increase sleep after antennal injury, we tested flies with two mutant alleles for hiw: hiwΔN and hiwND8 [59,60]. hiw mutant flies show no change in sleep time after antennal transection, while heterozygous controls increase their sleep comparably to wild-type flies (Figure 5AC). Because hiw mutations alter synaptic connectivity during development [60] and reduce nociceptive responses [61], we observed ORN morphology in hiwΔN mutants. We used OR22a-GAL4 to drive expression of CD8::GFP and STaR labelling for BRP; while baseline levels of axonal CD8::GFP and BRP::V5 were significantly decreased in hiwΔN mutants compared to wild-type controls (Figure S5AD), we found that hiwΔN mutants show no loss in BRP::V5 for at least 33 hours after antennal transection (Figure S5EF). Because wild-type flies exhibit a robust increase in sleep after transection of 50% of ORNs with the removal of one antenna (Figure 2A), it is unlikely that the post-injury sleep phenotype in hiw mutants can be attributed solely to the baseline reduction in ORN pre-synapses. Next, we tested whether hiw mutants are capable of increasing their sleep in response to a second physiological stressor, a 1-hr exposure to a 37°C heat stress [62]. Like heterozygous controls, hiw mutants exhibit robust increases in sleep after heat stress for 1 hour from ZT0–1 (Figure 5DF; time of heat exposure depicted with orange shading). This result indicates that the suppressed responses of hiw mutants after antennal injury is likely related to the neuroprotective effects of hiw loss, not to any developmental changes in sleep regulatory circuits. To test whether the lack of post-injury sleep induction in hiw mutants can also be attributed to ORN dysfunction, we expressed an RNAi hairpin targeted against hiw transcripts (UAS-hiwRNAi) in ORNs. As shown in Figures 5GH, flies expressing hiwRNAi show a weakened increase in sleep after antennal injury when compared to genetic controls. A similar effect can be also observed when orco-GAL4 drove expression of a partially dominant negative transgene for hiw, UAS-hiwΔRING [59] (Figure S5GH). To test whether other genes that influence axonal degeneration after injury are also required for flies to increase their sleep after injury, we also tested the effects of expressing a dominant-negative transgene for the MAPK basket (bskDN) in ORNs [63]. bsk is homologous to the mammalian c-Jun N-terminal kinase (JNK) [64], and JNK is activated in response to injury and is required for the degeneration of severed axons in flies and mammals [27,6568]. Injured orco-GAL4/UAS-bskDN flies showed a dampened increase in sleep after injury compared to genetic controls (Figure 5IJ). While hiw has been shown to be an upstream regulator of bsk [64], it is unclear whether the two proteins act in a common pathway or independently to promote sleep after antennal injury. Together, these data indicate that both hiw and MAPK activity are required in injured neurites to promote sleep, and that the sleep-inducing effects of these molecules are consistent with a cell autonomous mechanism within injured neurons.

Figure 5 – Expression of hiw is required for increased post-injury sleep.

Figure 5 –

(A) Hourly sleep timecourse for hiwΔN/+ heterozygous flies during the day of antennal injury. Sleep is significantly elevated in injured heterozygotes (dark gray) compared to uninjured controls (light gray) in the hours after injury (time of injury marked with yellow shading). Two-way repeated measures ANOVA shows a significant injury x time interaction (F(23,575)=9.519, p<0.0001; n = 12 injured, 15 controls).

(B) Injury day sleep timecourse for homozygous hiwΔN mutants. No significant change in sleep was detected between injured mutants (dark red) and control siblings (light red). Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds no significant effect of injury (F(1,53)=1.019, p=0.3173; n = 26 injured, 29 controls).

(C) Total daytime sleep during the hours between injury and lights off for hiw heterozygotes (hiwΔN/+ and hiwND8/+; injured shown in dark gray, controls in light gray) and mutants (hiwΔN/hiwΔN, hiwND8/hiwND8, and hiwΔN/hiwND8; injured shown in dark red, controls in light red). Two-way ANOVA shows a significant injury x genotype interaction (F(4,180)=11.43, p<0.001)

(D) Hourly sleep timecourse for hiwΔN/+ heterozygotes following 1-hour of housing at 37°C (heated flies shown in dark gray, controls in light gray). Sleep was increased in heated flies for several hours compared to siblings that were maintained at 25°C. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds a significant heat x time interaction (F(23,690)=8.545, p<0.0001; n=16 each group).

(E) Hourly sleep timecourse after heat stress for hiwΔN homozygous mutants (heated flies in dark red, controls in light red). Heated flies show a robust sleep increase throughout the daytime relative to unheated siblings. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA finds a significant heat x time interaction (F(23,552)=8.468, p<0.0001; n=12 heated, 14 control).

(F) Daytime sleep after heat stress in hiw heterozygotes (hiwΔN/+ and hiwND8/+; heated flies shown in dark gray, controls in light gray) and mutants (hiwΔN/hiwΔN, hiwND8/hiwND8, and hiwΔN/hiwND8; heated shown in dark red, controls in light red). All genotypes exhibited a significant increase in sleep after heat stress. Two way ANOVA finds a significant heat x genotype interaction (F(4,137)=5.867, p=0.0002).

(G) Post-injury daytime sleep responses are weakened in flies expressing hiwRNAi in ORNs (injured flies in dark red, controls in light red) compared to parental controls (UAS-dcr2/+; orco-gal4/+ and UAS-hiwRNAi/+; injured in dark gray, controls in light gray). Two-way ANOVA finds a significant genotype x injury interaction (F(2,175)=14.00, p<0.0001).

(H) The magnitude of increased sleep after injury is reduced by expression of hiwRNAi in ORNs (red) compared to genetic controls (UAS-dcr2/+; orco-gal4/+ and UAS-hiwRNAi/+; gray). Data for this panel were calculated using the same flies that were included in Figure 5G. One-way ANOVA finds a significant effect for genotype (F(2,87)=18.15, p<0.0001).

(I) Post-injury daytime sleep in flies expressing a dominant-negative transgene for basket within ORNS (orco-GAL4/UAS-bskDN; injured in dark red, controls in light red) compared to genetic controls (orco-GAL4/+ and UAS-bskDN/+; injured in dark gray, controls in light gray). The amount of increased sleep after injury is reduced in orco-GAL4/UAS-bskDN relative to both genetic controls. Two-way ANOVA shows a significant genotype x injury interaction (F(2,170)=27.5, p<0.0001); * signifies p<0.05 using pairwise Tukey comparisons.

(J) Increased sleep after injury is dampened in orco-GAL4/UAS-bskDN flies (red) compared to orco-GAL4/+ and UAS-bskDN/+ controls. Data for this panel were calculated using the same flies that were included in Figure 5I. One-way ANOVA finds a significant main effect of genotype (F(2,87)=45.12, p<0.0001, n=29–31 flies/group).

See also Figure S5.

Sleep promotes the removal of active zones and plasma membrane from injured ORN axons

Wallerian degeneration of injured ORN axons is an actively regulated process that unfolds over roughly five days after injury [10]. Because the post-injury increase in sleep shown in Figure 1 lasts for only several hours, we became interested in observing the degeneration of injured ORNs within the first day after antennal transection. To detail the morphological changes in damaged ORNs that coincide with post-injury sleep changes, we used OR22a-GAL4 to label ORN axons in the DM2 glomerulus with both a membrane-targeted GFP (CD8::GFP) and a flp-based reporter for the active zone protein Bruchpilot (BRP::V5) using STaR [52]. This combination of reporters enabled us to simultaneously image the abundance of remaining axonal plasma membrane (CD8::GFP) and pre-synaptic active zones (BRP::V5) at several timepoints after antennal transection. Over the initial hours post-injury, the amount of GFP-tagged membrane remains unchanged, but a significant loss of STaR-tagged ORN active zones occurs within 6-hours of injury and labelled active zones are nearly entirely removed by 24 hours after injury (Figure 6AB).

Figure 6 – Sleep deprivation reduces axon and synapse clearance following antennal transection.

Figure 6 –

(A) Representative images from OR22a-GAL4>STaR flies (OR22a-GAL4/LexAOP-CD8::GFP; UAS-flp, brp(FRT.Stop)V5–2A-LexA-VP16/+) either without antennal injury or 3, 6, 9, or 24 hours post-antennal transection. OR22a cells labelled with membrane-targeted CD8::GFP in green, and pre-synapses from the same cells labelled by STaR (BRP::V5) in magenta. Scale bars represent 20μm.

(B) Quantification of BRP::V5 (Magenta) and CD8::GFP (Green) in OR22a-expressing ORNs after injury. BRP::V5 labelling is significantly more reduced over the 24 hours following injury than membrane-bound CD8::GFP. Two-way ANOVA finds a significant time x reporter interaction (F(4,542)=11.79, p<0.0001, n=26–70 hemispheres per group).

(C) Representative images from injured flies that were either allowed sleep (left) or sleep deprived (right) for 9 hours after antennal transection. StaR-labelled ORN synapses were largely removed when flies were allowed to rest after injury, but StaR signal remained high when animals were sleep deprived after injury. Scale bars represent 20μm.

(D) Relative BRP::V5 intensity (left; uninjured controls shown in gray, injured in magenta), magenta shows injured hemispheres) or CD8::GFP (right; uninjured controls shown in gray, injured in green) in OR22a-positive antennal lobe glomeruli in flies that were allowed 9 h of ad libitum recovery sleep after antennal transection or control handling. Two-way ANOVA finds a significant sleep condition-by-injury interaction for BRP::V5 (F(1,202) = 31.54, p < 0.0001; n = 48–56 hemispheres/group) and a significant effect of injury for CD8::GFP (F(1,202) = 4.863, p = 0.0316; n = 48–56 hemispheres/group).

(E) Representative images from flies that were dissected 24 hours after antennal injury or control handling. Images from flies that were allowed ad libitum sleep shown on left and from sleep deprived flies on right. STaR signal, magenta, is reduced in both rested and sleep deprived flies at 24 hours after injury, but CD8::GFP shows a stronger reduction after injury in flies that were allowed to sleep.

(F) Relative fluorescence signal for BRP::V5 (left; uninjured controls in gray, injured flies in magenta) and CD8::GFP (right; uninjured controls in gray, injured flies in green) under the control of OR22a-GAL4 at 24 h after injury or control handling. Two-way ANOVA finds a significant effect of injury for BRP::V5 (F(1,160) = 136.0, p < 0.0001; n = 36–44 hemispheres/group) and an injury-by-sleep interaction for CD8::GFP (F(1,308) = 14.24, p = 0.0002; n = 72–82 hemispheres/group).

See also Figure S6.

Because post-injury sleep is most strongly elevated during the same time that most active zones were pruned from damaged ORNs, we next tested whether depriving flies of sleep after injury slows the removal of pre-synapses from injured ORNs. Flies were injured in the hour following lights-on (ZT0–1), and either sleep deprived or allowed to sleep ad libitum for 9 hours (Figure 6CD, S6A) or 24 hours (Figure 6EF, S6B) prior to brain dissection, fixation, and staining. Sleep deprivation was not sufficient to alter either CD8::GFP or STaR labelling in uninjured control flies at either post-injury timepoint (Figure 6CF). Flies that were allowed ad libitum sleep for 9 h after injury removed more STaR-labeled ORN active zones than siblings who were sleep deprived following injury (Figures 6C, D). No significant effect of sleep on axonal membrane labeled with CD8::GFP was detected 9 h after injury (Figures 6C, D). When flies were dissected 24 hours after injury, STaR labelling was significantly reduced in both rested and sleep deprived groups (Figures 6E, F), but the flies that were sleep deprived after injury showed a significantly weaker reduction in membrane-tethered CD8::GFP (Figures 6E, F). These results suggest that sleep deprivation slows the rate at which both active zone machinery and plasma membrane are removed from severed axons following antennal transection.

Discussion

Sleep contributes to neural remodeling in a variety of neural circuits across several model species. To better understand how sleep contributes to neural remodeling, we have examined the responses of Drosophila to injuries targeted to populations of sensory neurons. Injuries delivered to either the antennae or the wings drive an increase in sleep that lasts for several hours. Because no increase in sleep occurred when ORNs were acutely silenced or when antennal injuries were delivered to orco mutants with few intact ORNs, we hypothesized that post-injury increases in sleep were associated with the clearance of debris from damaged neurites. Our results are consistent with this hypothesis: sleep deprivation slowed the clearance of STaR-labelled pre-synaptic active zones and GFP-tagged plasma membrane from severed axons.

The fragmentation and clearance of severed axons is controlled by a network of proteins that interact with the E3 ubiquitin ligase hiw. Manipulations of these molecules can permit severed axons to remain intact for days or weeks after injury [10,2730,66]. Immunostaining for active zone proteins have indicated that larval NMJ pre-synapses are removed prior to axonal fragmentation, and can be delayed by manipulations that also slow axonal fragmentation [51,68,69]. The diversity and density of synapses in the antennal lobes, however, have prevented similar characterization of synapse removal in adult ORNs. The recent development of StaR [52], along with other reporters that genetically tag synaptic proteins in targeted neuronal populations [70], now broaden the opportunities to observe active zone clearance in injured ORN neurites in the central brains of adult Drosophila. Our data suggest that pre-synapses may be selectively removed from injured axons before the axons are cleared themselves, and that the period of increased sleep after antennal injury closely parallels the timecourse of ORN synapse pruning. Further, sleep after antennal injury plays a functional role in presynaptic elimination: flies allowed to sleep ad libitum after injury cleared a significant portion of the pre-synaptic terminals from damaged ORNs, while sleep deprivation slowed post-antennal injury active zone clearance. Although we find that sleep disruption slows both the removal of pre-synapses and of neurites, it is not clear whether sleep promotes these processes via independent or shared functions. It is possible, for instance, that sleep facilitates active zone removal in the first hours after injury and that axonal fragmentation occurs only after synaptic contacts are dismantled. This model is consistent with previous reports of a latent period between axonal injury and membrane fragmentation [7173], and raises the possibility that active zone removal may be an initial checkpoint of axonal degeneration that must precede axon fragmentation and clearance.

Our results provide evidence for sleep-dependent pruning of synapses from damaged ORNs, but the processes that mediate synapse removal during sleep will require future investigation. One likely mechanism of active zone clearance would be phagocytosis by ensheathing glia. This glial subset shares functional similarities with mammalian microglia, is responsible for engulfing and degrading axonal debris after antennal injury [11, 74], and its activation in response to injury is dampened by sleep loss [75]. Similar effects can also be observed when clathrin-mediated endocytosis is disrupted in glial cells that contribute to the blood-brain barrier [76], suggesting that other types of glia may also influence sleep-dependent clearance of debris from the brain. Because results from the fly parallel recent observations that sleep deprivation can increase activation of and synaptic engulfment by mouse astrocytes and microglia [77], neuro-glial interactions may provide an evolutionarily conserved mechanism for pruning synapses during sleep.

While we find a role for sleep in pruning active zones from damaged axons, the functional consequences of this pruning require extensive investigation. Sleep deprivation after antennal injury, for instance, increases the acute retention of ORN pre-synapses within the antennal lobe. Protecting these synapses may provide the benefit of sustaining maximal circuit stability. It is also possible, however, that pre-synapses and axons from damaged neurons need to be pruned and cleared to permit circuit recovery and the regeneration of functional neural connections. Indeed, Kazama et al., (2011) found that removing one antenna immediately eliminates odor-evoked responses from olfactory projection neurons, the post-synaptic partners of ORNs, on the injured side of the brain. After a recovery period, however, neural projections from the contralateral hemisphere strengthen to the injured side and odor-evoked responses can be partially restored. Importantly, the recovery of olfactory responses in the injured hemisphere requires the clearance of debris from damaged ORNs [78]. Complementary results have also been found after axonal injury in C. elegans, where engulfment receptors expressed by phagocytes may also act as adhesion molecules for regenerating motor neuron axons [79]. Sleep has also been linked with increased rates of olfactory granule cell apoptosis in mice following sensory deprivation [80]. It is possible, therefore, that sleep-dependent synapse removal prepares the circuit for subsequent rebalancing and recovery.

Our findings link pre-synapse clearance during sleep with responses to neural injury, but many of the molecules that mediate synapse removal after axonal transection are also shared with the regulation of synapse pruning in other contexts [81,82]. While sleep has also been linked with synapse removal during development or after experience-dependent plasticity [4,8,9,83], it remains unclear whether similar mechanisms may influence the removal of synapses under physiological conditions and following neural trauma. Hiw and bsk both influence the growth of developing synapses at the larval neuromuscular junction [60,64], indicating that the injury response pathway is likely conserved to other plastic conditions. Hiw protein also localizes to pre-synaptic active zones and regulates evoked neurotransmitter release, suggesting that the molecules that impart injury responses may be required more broadly to maintain synapse structure and physiology [84]. A recent study also found that the sleep regulatory gene insomniac is also required for homeostatic plasticity in the larval neuromuscular junction, indicating a link between the molecular pathways that regulate synaptic plasticity and sleep [85]. Interestingly, expression of hiwΔRING in different cell types has opposing effects on the need for sleep: expressing hiwΔRING in circadian ventral lateral neurons suppresses sleep and causes learning impairments, while expression in mushroom body Kenyon cells enables flies to maintain wild-type learning after sleep loss [86]. These data support the possibility that hiw functions during sleep to regulate structural plasticity in a circuit- and/or context-dependent manner. Our results, in aligning with these data and with historical predictions of sleep function, indicate that our assay will enable the identification and dissection of further mechanisms that promote synaptic pruning during sleep.

STAR Methods

Lead contact and materials availability

Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Jeffrey M. Donlea (jdonlea@ucla.edu). This study did not generate new unique reagents.

Experimental model and subject details

Fly Strains

Fly stocks were reared on standard cornmeal media (per 1L H20: 12g agar, 29g Red Star yeast, 71g cornmeal, 92g molasses, 16mL methyl paraben 10% in EtOH, 10mL propionic acid 50% in H20) at 25°C with 60% relative humidity and entrained to a daily 12hr light, 12hr dark schedule. Canton-S flies were from Gero Miesenböck (University of Oxford), w1118 flies were from David Krantz (UCLA), LexAOP-CD8::GFP; UAS-flp, PBac{y[+mDint2]w[+mc]=brp(FRT.Stop)V5–2A-LexA-VP16} [52] was from Lawrence Zipursky (UCLA), yw,per01 [39] and yw; tim01 [38] were supplied by Amita Sehgal (University of Pennsylvania), and UAS-hiwΔRING [59] was from Aaron Diantonio (Washington University in St. Louis). UAS-hiwRNAi [87] was ordered from the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center. w+, per01 [39], w+;tim01 [38], cyc01 [37], ClkJRK [36], OR22a-GAL4 [20], UAS-kir2.1::GFP [44], tubP-GAL80TS [46], UAS-CD8::GFP [88,89], parasbl−1 and parasbl−2 [47], orco-GAL4 [45], hiwΔN [60], hiwND8 [59], and UAS-bskDN were obtained from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center.

Method Details

Behavioral Analysis

Sleep

Sleep was measured as previously described [2]. Briefly, 3–7 day old female flies were individually loaded into 65mm-long glass tubes and inserted into Drosophila activity monitors (Trikinetics Inc; Waltham MA, USA). Periods of inactivity lasting at least 5 minutes were classified as sleep. Sleep deprivation occurred mechanically via the Sleep Nullifying Apparatus (SNAP) [2]. Trikinetics activity records were analyzed for sleep using custom Visual Basic scripts [2] in Microsoft Excel or the Sleep and Circadian Analysis MATLAB Program (SCAMP) scripts [92] in Matlab R2016B (Mathworks; Natick MA, USA). Antennal, maxillary palp, haltere, and wing transections were completed under CO2 anesthesia using fine forceps. Control flies received identical handling and CO2 anesthesia exposure as the injured groups, without any injury. Multibeam monitoring experiments used MB5 monitors (Trikinetics Inc; Waltham MA, USA) to observe fly movements.

Arousability

Arousability was tested by attaching Trikinetics activity monitors to microplate adapters on vortexers (VWR). Vibration force intensities were measured using Vibration 3.83 (Diffraction Limited Design; Southington CT, USA). Arousal tests used a 0.5g or 1.5g stimulation of 1, 5, or 10 second duration, and were delivered during the first 8 hours after injury (ZT1–9).

Olfactory preference

Olfactory preference tests were based on previous descriptions [93,94]. Groups of 10 flies were placed in circular petri dishes (100 mm diameter x 15 mm depth) containing two ~7mm diameter holes on the bottom surface leading to recessed traps. One trap contained 1mL of deionized water while the other contained 1mL unfiltered apple juice. Traps were placed in dark incubators, then the number of flies contained in each trap were counted manually. Odor preference indices were calculated as follows: PI = (# of flies in apple juice trap − # of flies in water trap)/(# of flies in apple juice trap + # of flies in water trap).

Immunohistochemistry and confocal microscopy

Drosophila brains were dissected in PBS (1.86 mM NaH2PO4, 8.41 mM Na2HPO4, 175 mM NaCl; Sigma-Aldrich) and fixed in 4% (w/v) paraformaldehyde (Electron Microscopy; Hatfield PA, USA) in PBS for 30–45 minutes on ice. For GFP immunostaining, brains were incubated in primary antibody (1:1000 chicken anti-GFP, Molecular Probes) overnight followed by secondary antibody (1:1000 anti-chicken antibody conjugated to Alexa488, Molecular Probes) for roughly 24 hours. Immunostaining for V5 used a 48 hour incubation period in mouse anti-V5 conjugated with DyLight550. All brains were mounted in Vectashield (Vector Labs; Burlingame CA, USA) and imaged on a Zeiss LSM880 using a confocal slice thickness of 1 uM. All image processing was completed using Fiji [94]. Quantification of antennal lobe glomerular fluorescent signal intensity used a sum slices projection including all z-slices through DM2 glomeruli, followed by outlining of the labelled glomeruli to measure area and mean GFP or anti-V5 intensity signal.

Quantification and statistical analysis

Statistical Analysis

Data were analyzed in Prism 8 (GraphPad; San Diego CA, USA). Group means were compared using two-tailed T-tests or one- or two-way ANOVAs, with repeated measures where appropriate, followed by planned pairwise comparisons with Holm-Sidak multiple comparisons tests. Mann-Whitney or Kruskal-Wallis tests, followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons tests, were used in cases where assumptions of normality did not hold. Sample sizes for each experiment are depicted in each figure panel or in the appropriate figure legend. All group averages shown in data panels depict mean ± SEM.

Data and code availability

All software used during this study is described in the Key Resources Table. Further information about analysis or data are available upon reasonable request from the Lead Contact, Jeffrey M. Donlea (jdonlea@ucla.edu).

Supplementary Material

1
2

Highlights.

  • Sleep in Drosophila is temporarily increased following antennal removal

  • Increased sleep after injury can be attributed to neural damage

  • Sleep after injury promotes the removal of pre-synaptic proteins and axonal debris

Acknowledgments

We thank members of the Donlea lab for many helpful discussions and Drs. Lawrence Zipursky, Gero Miesenboeck, Aaron DiAntonio, David Krantz, Nicholas Stavropoulos, William Joiner, and Amita Sehgal; the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center; and the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center for fly lines. This project was funded by a Klingenstein-Simons Fellowship in Neuroscience and NIH grant NS105067.

Footnotes

Declaration of Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Everson CA, Bergmann BM, Rechtschaffen A. (1989). Sleep deprivation in the rat: III. Total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 12, 13–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Shaw PJ, Tononi G, Greenspan RJ, Robinson DF. (2002). Stress response genes protect against lethal effects of sleep deprivation in Drosophila. Nature. 417, 287–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Rogers NL, Dorrian J, Dinges DF. (2003). Sleep, waking and neurobehavioural performance. Front Biosci. 8, s1056–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Donlea JM, Ramanan N, Shaw PJ (2009). Use-dependent plasticity in clock neurons regulates sleep need in Drosophila. Science. 324, 105–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Donlea JM, Thimgan MS, Suzuki Y, Gottschalk L, Shaw PJ. (2011). Inducing sleep by remote control facilitates memory consolidation in Drosophila. Science. 332,1571–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.de Vivo L, Bellesi M, Marshall W, Bushong EA, Ellisman MH, Tononi G, Cirelli C. (2017). Ultrastructural evidence for synaptic scaling across the wake/sleep cycle. Science. 355, 507–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Diering GH, Nirujogi RS, Roth RH, Worley PF, Pandey A, Huganir RL. (2017). Homer1a drives homeostatic scaling-down of excitatory synapses during sleep. Science. 355, 511–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Bushey D, Tononi G, Cirelli C. (2011). Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: structural evidence in Drosophila. Science. 332, 1576–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gilestro GF, Tononi G, Cirelli C. (2009). Widespread changes in synaptic markers as a function of sleep and wakefulness in Drosophila. Science. 324, 109–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.MacDonald JM, Beach MG, Porpiglia E, Sheehan AE, Watts RJ, Freeman MR. (2006). The Drosophila cell corpse engulfment receptor Draper mediates glial clearance of severed axons. Neuron. 50, 869–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Doherty J, Logan MA, Taşdemir OE, Freeman MR. (2009). Ensheathing glia function as phagocytes in the adult Drosophila brain. J Neurosci. 29, 4768–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Watson NF, Dikmen S, Machamer J, Doherty M, Temkin N. (2007). Hypersomnia following traumatic brain injury. J Clin Sleep Med. 3, 363–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kaiser PR, Valko PO, Werth E, Thomann J, Meier J, Stocker R, Bassetti CL, Baumann CR. (2010). Modafinil ameliorates excessive daytime sleepiness after traumatic brain injury. Neurology. 75, 1780–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Masel BE, Scheibel RS, Kimbark T, Kuna ST. (2001). Excessive daytime sleepiness in adults with brain injuries. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 82, 1526–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Weil ZM, Norman GJ, Karelina K, Morris JS, Barker JM, Su AJ, Walton JC, Bohinc S, Nelson RJ, DeVries AC. (2009). Sleep deprivation attenuates inflammatory responses and ischemic cell death. Exp Neurol. 218, 129–36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Moldovan M, Constantinescu AO, Balseanu A, Oprescu N, Zagrean L, Popa-Wagner A. (2010). Sleep deprivation attenuates experimental stroke severity in rats. Exp Neurol. 222, 135–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Wiseman-Hakes C, Murray B, Moineddin R, Rochon E, Cullen N, Gargaro J, Colantonio A. (2013). Evaluating the impact of treatment for sleep/wake disorders on recovery of cognition and communication in adults with chronic TBI. Brain Inj. 27, 1364–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Stocker RF, Lienhard MC, Borst A, Fischbach KF. (1990) Neuronal architecture of the antennal lobe in Drosophila melanogaster. Cell Tissue Res. 262, 9–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Stocker RF. (1994) The organization of the chemosensory system in Drosophila melanogaster: a review. Cell Tissue Res. 275, 3–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Vosshall LB, Wong AM, Axel R. (2000). An olfactory sensory map in the fly brain. Cell. 102, 147–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Marin EC, Jefferis GSXE, Komiyama T, Zhu H, Luo L. (2002). Representation of the glomerular olfactory map in the Drosophila brain. Cell. 109, 243–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wong AM, Wang JW, Axel R. (2002) Spatial representation of the glomerular map in the Drosophila protocerebrum. Cell. 109, 229–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Purice MD, Ray A, Münzel EJ, Pope BJ, Park DJ, Speese SD, Logan MA. (2017). A novel Drosophila injury model reveals severed axons are cleared through a Draper/MMP-1 signaling cascade. Elife. 6,e23611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Coleman MP, Freeman MR. Wallerian degeneration, wld(s), and nmnat. (2010). Annu Rev Neurosci. 33, 245–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Martin SM, O’Brien GS, Portera-Cailliau C, Sagasti A. (2010). Wallerian degeneration of zebrafish trigeminal axons in the skin is required for regeneration and developmental pruning. Development. 137, 3985–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Lyon MF, Ogunkolade BW, Brown MC, Atherton DJ, Perry VH. (1993). A gene affecting Wallerian nerve degeneration maps distally on mouse chromosome 4. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 90, 9717–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Miller BR, Press C, Daniels RW, Sasaki Y, Milbrandt J, DiAntonio A. (2009). A dual leucine kinase-dependent axon self-destruction program promotes Wallerian degeneration. Nat Neurosci. 12, 387–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Osterloh JM, Yang J, Rooney TM, Fox AN, Adalbert R, Powell EH, Sheehan AE, Avery MA, Hackett R, Logan MA, et al. (2012). dSarm/Sarm1 is required for activation of an injury-induced axon death pathway. Science. 337,481–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Xiong X, Hao Y, Sun K, Li J, Li X, Mishra B, Soppina P, Wu C, Hume RI, Collins CA. (2012). The Highwire ubiquitin ligase promotes axonal degeneration by tuning levels of Nmnat protein. PLoS Biol. 10, e1001440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Neukomm LJ, Burdett TC, Seeds AM, Hampel S, Coutinho-Budd JC, Farley JE, Wong J, Karadeniz YB, Osterloh JM, Sheehan AE, Freeman MR. (2017). Axon Death Pathways Converge on Axundead to Promote Functional and Structural Axon Disassembly. Neuron. 95,78–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.van Alphen B, Yap MHW, Kirszenblat L, Kottler B, van Swinderen B. (2013). A dynamic deep sleep stage in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 33,6917–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Stavropoulos N, Young MW. (2011) insomniac and Cullin-3 regulate sleep and wakefulness in Drosophila. Neuron. 72, 964–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Pfeiffenberger C, Allada R. (2012) Cul3 and the BTB adaptor insomniac are key regulators of sleep homeostasis and a dopamine arousal pathway in Drosophila. PLoS Genet. 8, e1003003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Koh K, Joiner WJ, Wu MN, Yue Z, Smith CJ, Sehgal A. (2008). Identification of SLEEPLESS, a sleep-promoting factor. Science. 321, 372–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Seugnet L, Suzuki Y, Vine L, Gottschalk L, Shaw PJ. (2008). D1 receptor activation in the mushroom bodies rescues sleep-loss-induced learning impairments in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 18, 1110–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Allada R, White NE, So WV, Hall JC, Rosbash M. (1998). A mutant Drosophila homolog of mammalian Clock disrupts circadian rhythms and transcription of period and timeless. Cell. 93,791–804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Rutila JE, Suri V, Le M, So WV, Rosbash M, Hall JC. (1998). CYCLE is a second bHLH-PAS clock protein essential for circadian rhythmicity and transcription of Drosophila period and timeless. Cell. 93, 805–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sehgal A, Price JL, Man B, Young MW. (1994). Loss of circadian behavioral rhythms and per RNA oscillations in the Drosophila mutant timeless. Science. 263, 1603–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Konopka RJ, Benzer S. (1971). Clock mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 68, 2112–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Sakai T, Tamura T, Kitamoto T, Kidokoro Y. (2004). A clock gene, period, plays a key role in long-term memory formation in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 101, 16058–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Naresh Singh R, Nayak SV. (1985). Fine structure and primary sensory projections of sensilla on the maxillary palp of Drosophila melanogaster Meigen (Diptera : Drosophilidae). International Journal of Insect Morphology and Embryology. 14, 291–06. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hartenstein V, Posakony JW. (1989). Development of adult sensilla on the wing and notum of Drosophila melanogaster. Development. 107, 389–405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Lehmann F-O, Bartussek J. (2017). Neural control and precision of flight muscle activation in Drosophila. J Comp Physiol a Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol. 203, 1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hardie RC, Raghu P, Moore S, Juusola M, Baines RA, Sweeney ST. (2001). Calcium Influx via TRP Channels Is Required to Maintain PIP2 Levels in Drosophila Photoreceptors. Neuron. 30, 149–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wang JW, Wong AM, Flores J, Vosshall LB, Axel R. (2003). Two-photon calcium imaging reveals an odor-evoked map of activity in the fly brain. Cell. 112, 271–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.McGuire SE, Le PT, Osborn AJ, Matsumoto K, Davis RL. (2003). Spatiotemporal rescue of memory dysfunction in Drosophila. Science. 302, 1765–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lilly M, Carlson J. (1990). smellblind: a gene required for Drosophila olfaction. Genetics. 124, 293–302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Rodrigues V, Siddiqi O. (1978). Genetic analysis of chemosensory pathway. Proc Indian Acad Sci. 87, 147–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Gailey DA, Lacaillade RC, Hall JC. (1986). Chemosensory elements of courtship in normal and mutant, olfaction-deficient Drosophila melanogaster. Behav Genet. 16, 375–405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Levine JD, Funes P, Dowse HB, Hall JC. (2002). Resetting the circadian clock by social experience in Drosophila melanogaster. Science. 298:2010–2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Mishra B, Carson R, Hume RI, Collins CA. (2013). Sodium and potassium currents influence Wallerian degeneration of injured Drosophila axons. J Neurosci. 33, 18728–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Chen Y, Akin O, Nern A, Tsui CYK, Pecot MY, Zipursky SL. (2014). Cell-type-specific labeling of synapses in vivo through synaptic tagging with recombination. Neuron. 81, 280–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Butterwick JA, Del Mármol J, Kim KH, Kahlson MA, Rogow JA, Walz T, Ruta V. (2018). Cryo-EM structure of the insect olfactory receptor Orco. Nature. 560, 447–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Larsson MC, Domingos AI, Jones WD, Chiappe ME, Amrein H, Vosshall LB. (2004). Or83b encodes a broadly expressed odorant receptor essential for Drosophila olfaction. Neuron. 43:703–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Chiang A, Priya R, Ramaswami M, VijayRaghavan K, Rodrigues V. (2009). Neuronal activity and Wnt signaling act through Gsk3-beta to regulate axonal integrity in mature Drosophila olfactory sensory neurons. Development. 136, 1273–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Fang Y, Soares L, Teng X, Geary M, Bonini NM. (2012). A novel Drosophila model of nerve injury reveals an essential role of Nmnat in maintaining axonal integrity. Curr Biol. 22, 590–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Brace EJ, Wu C, Valakh V, DiAntonio A. (2014). SkpA restrains synaptic terminal growth during development and promotes axonal degeneration following injury. J Neurosci. 34, 8398–410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Farley JE, Burdett TC, Barria R, Neukomm LJ, Kenna KP, Landers JE, Freeman MR. (2018). Transcription factor Pebbled/RREB1 regulates injury-induced axon degeneration. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 23, 1358–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Wu C, Wairkar YP, Collins CA, DiAntonio A. (2005). Highwire Function at the Drosophila Neuromuscular Junction: Spatial, Structural, and Temporal Requirements. J Neurosci. 25, 9557–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Wan HI, DiAntonio A, Fetter RD, Bergstrom K, Strauss R, Goodman CS. (2000). Highwire regulates synaptic growth in Drosophila. Neuron. 26, 313–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Honjo K, Tracey WD Jr. (2018). BMP signaling downstream of the Highwire E3 ligase sensitizes nociceptors. PLoS Genet. 14, e1007464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Lenz O, Xiong J, Nelson MD, Raizen DM, Williams JA. (2015). FMRFamide signaling promotes stress-induced sleep in Drosophila. Brain Behav Immun. 47, 141–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Bossuyt W, De Geest N, Aerts S, Leenaerts I, Marynen P, Hassan BA. (2009). The atonal proneural transcription factor links differentiation and tumor formation in Drosophila. PLoS Biol. 7, e40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Collins CA, Wairkar YP, Johnson SL, DiAntonio A. (2006). Highwire restrains synaptic growth by attenuating a MAP kinase signal. Neuron. 51, 57–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Valakh V, Frey E, Babetto E, Walker LJ, DiAntonio A. (2015). Cytoskeletal disruption activates the DLK/JNK pathway, which promotes axonal regeneration and mimics a preconditioning injury. Neurobiol Dis. 77, 13–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Walker LJ, Summers DW, Sasaki Y, Brace EJ, Milbrandt J, DiAntonio A. (2017). MAPK signaling promotes axonal degeneration by speeding the turnover of the axonal maintenance factor NMNAT2. Elife. 6, 545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Yang J, Wu Z, Renier N, Simon DJ, Uryu K, Park DS, Greer PA, Tournier C, Davis RJ, Tessier-Lavigne M. (2015). Pathological axonal death through a MAPK cascade that triggers a local energy deficit. Cell. 160, 161–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Xiong X, Collins CA. (2012). A conditioning lesion protects axons from degeneration via the Wallenda/DLK MAP kinase signaling cascade. J Neurosci. 32, 610–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Hao Y, Waller TJ, Nye DM, Li J, Zhang Y, Hume RI, Rolls MM, Collins CA. (2019). Degeneration of injured axons and dendrites requires restraint of a protective JNK signaling pathway by the transmembrane protein Raw. J Neurosci. 39, 8457–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Williams JL, Shearin HK, Stowers RS. (2019). Conditional Synaptic Vesicle Markers for Drosophila. G3 (Bethesda) 9, 737–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Moldovan M, Alvarez S, Krarup C. (2009). Motor axon excitability during Wallerian degeneration. Brain. 132:511–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Vargas ME, Yamagishi Y, Tessier-Lavigne M, Sagasti A. (2015). Live Imaging of Calcium Dynamics during Axon Degeneration Reveals Two Functionally Distinct Phases of Calcium Influx. J Neurosci. 35, 15026–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Tsao JW, George EB, Griffin JW. (1999). Temperature Modulation Reveals Three Distinct Stages of Wallerian Degeneration. J Neurosci. 19, 4718–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Logan MA, Hackett R, Doherty J, Sheehan A, Speese SD, Freeman MR. (2012). Negative regulation of glial engulfment activity by Draper terminates glial responses to axon injury. Nat Neurosci. 15, 722–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Stanhope BA, Jaggard JB, Gratton M, Brown EB, and Keene AC (2020). Sleep regulates glial plasticity and expression of the engulfment receptor draper following neural injury. Current Biology 30 Published online March 5, 2020. 10.1016/j.cub.2020.02.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Stahl BA, Peco E, Davla S, Murakami K, Caicedo Moreno NA, van Meyel DJ, Keene AC. (2018). The Taurine Transporter Eaat2 Functions in Ensheathing Glia to Modulate Sleep and Metabolic Rate. Curr Biol. 28, 3700–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Artiushin G, Zhang SL, Tricoire H, Sehgal A. (2018). Endocytosis at the Drosophila blood-brain barrier as a function for sleep. Elife 7, 523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Bellesi M, de Vivo L, Chini M, Gilli F, Tononi G, Cirelli C. (2017). Sleep Loss Promotes Astrocytic Phagocytosis and Microglial Activation in Mouse Cerebral Cortex. J Neurosci. 37, 5263–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Kazama H, Yaksi E, Wilson RI. (2011). Cell death triggers olfactory circuit plasticity via glial signaling in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 31, 7619–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Chiu H, Zou Y, Suzuki N, Hsieh Y-W, Chuang C-F, Wu Y-C, Chang C. (2018). Engulfing cells promote neuronal regeneration and remove neuronal debris through distinct biochemical functions of CED-1. Nat Commun. 9:4842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Yokoyama TK, Mochimaru D, Murata K, Manabe H, Kobayakawa K, Kobayakawa R, Sakano H, Mori K, Yamaguchi M. (2011). Elimination of adult-born neurons in the olfactory bulb is promoted during the postprandial period. Neuron. 71,883–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Geden MJ, Deshmukh M. (2016). Axon degeneration: context defines distinct pathways. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 39, 108–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Tedeschi A, Bradke F. (2013). The DLK signalling pathway--a double-edged sword in neural development and regeneration. EMBO Reports. 14, 605–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Li W, Ma L, Yang G, Gan W-B. (2017). REM sleep selectively prunes and maintains new synapses in development and learning. Nat Neurosci. 20, 427–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Russo A, Goel P, Brace EJ, Buser C, Dickman DK, DiAntonio A. (2019). The E3 ligase Highwire promotes synaptic transmission by targeting the NAD-synthesizing enzyme dNmnat. EMBO Reports. 3, e1001440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Kikuma K, Li X, Perry S, Li Q, Goel P, Chen C, Kim D, Stavropoulos N, Dickman DK. (2019). Cul3 and insomniac are required for rapid ubiquitination of postsynaptic targets and retrograde homeostatic signaling. Nat Commun. 10, 2998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Seugnet L, Dissel S, Thimgan MS, Cao L, Shaw PJ. (2017). Identification of Genes that Maintain Behavioral and Structural Plasticity during Sleep Loss. Front Neural Circuits. 11, 79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Dietzl G, Chen D, Schnorrer F, Su K-C, Barinova Y, Fellner M, Gasser B, Kinsey K, Oppel S, Scheiblauer S, et al. (2007). A genome-wide transgenic RNAi library for conditional gene inactivation in Drosophila. Nature. 448, 151–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Lee T, Luo L. (1999). Mosaic analysis with a repressible cell marker for studies of gene function in neuronal morphogenesis. Neuron. 22, 451–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Pfeiffer BD, Ngo T-TB, Hibbard KL, Murphy C, Jenett A, Truman JW, Rubin GM. (2010). Refinement of tools for targeted gene expression in Drosophila. Genetics. 186, 735–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Donelson NC, Donelson N, Kim EZ, Slawson JB, Vecsey CG, Huber R, Griffith LC. (2012). High-resolution positional tracking for long-term analysis of Drosophila sleep and locomotion using the “tracker” program. PLoS ONE. 7, e37250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Chow DM, Theobald JC, Frye MA. (2011). An olfactory circuit increases the fidelity of visual behavior. J Neurosci. 31, 15035–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Min S, Ai M, Shin SA, Suh GSB. (2013). Dedicated olfactory neurons mediating attraction behavior to ammonia and amines in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 110, E1321–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, Preibisch S, Rueden C, Saalfeld S, Schmid B, et al. (2012). Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat Methods. 9, 676–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1
2

Data Availability Statement

All software used during this study is described in the Key Resources Table. Further information about analysis or data are available upon reasonable request from the Lead Contact, Jeffrey M. Donlea (jdonlea@ucla.edu).

RESOURCES