Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Climacteric. 2020 Feb 28;23(3):311–315. doi: 10.1080/13697137.2020.1727876

Prior endogenous and exogenous estrogen and incident dementia in the 10th decade of life: The 90+ Study

Annlia Paganini-Hill 1, Maria M Corrada 1,2, Claudia H Kawas 1,3
PMCID: PMC7210048  NIHMSID: NIHMS1578968  PMID: 32107945

Abstract

Objective

To investigate the association of endogenous and exogenous estrogen exposure with risk of incident dementia in the oldest-old (age 90+ years).

Methods

Participants were part of The 90+ Study, a longitudinal study begun in 2003 of aging and dementia among people aged 90+ years. Menstrual, reproductive and menopausal data were collected in the 1980s as part of the population-based Leisure World Cohort Study. Cognitive status at baseline was determined from an in-person neurological evaluation with biannual follow-up through June 2019. Hazard ratios (HRs) of dementia associated with estrogen-related variables were estimated using Cox regression analysis. No adjustment was made for multiple comparisons.

Results

424 women without dementia at baseline had at least 1 follow-up evaluation. Mean age was 68.5 at enrollment in the Leisure World Cohort Study, 93.2 at enrollment in The 90+ Study, 96.5 at last follow-up. During follow-up (mean 3.4 years) dementia was diagnosed in 209 (49%). No individual menstrual, reproductive, menopausal or estrogen replacement variable was associated with risk of incident dementia after age 90. However, women with a high endogenous estrogen exposure index (summarizing exposure from menarche to menopause) had a non-significant 25% lower risk (HR=0.75, 95% CI 0.53–1.06).

Conclusions

Prior exposure to estrogen, endogenous or exogenous, had little effect on risk of dementia in the 10th decade of life.

Keywords: dementia, incidence, estrogen, cohort, oldest-old, women

Introduction

Exposure to endogenous or exogenous estrogen may protect against dementia, but evidence is equivocal. In women, endogenous estrogen exposure occurs primarily during the reproductive phase. Estrogen levels rise during pregnancy, but fall postnatally, particularly with breastfeeding, and are lower after a first pregnancy than in nulliparous women. Earlier menarche, null or lower parity, and older age at birth of first child, and later menopause are therefore proxy indicators of lifetime endogenous estrogen exposure [1, 2]. Few studies have examined the effects of endogenous estrogen exposure and incident dementia. In case-control studies, Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) was associated with increasing age at menarche [3], increasing number of pregnancies [4], childlessness (inversely) [5]; and decreasing age at natural menopause [6]. However, the large population-based Rotterdam cohort study of 3601 postmenopausal women aged 55 years or older and followed for a median of 6.3 years (21,046 person years) had findings counter to these results [7]. Women with natural menopause and more reproductive years had an increased risk of dementia (RR = 1.78 for highest vs lowest quarter). In the largest prospective cohort study of 8466 women (the 10/66 population-based cohort with sites in Cuba, Dominican Republic, Puerto Rico, Venezuela, Peru, Mexico, and China), no association was observed between incident dementia and ages at menarche, birth of first child, and menopause; nulliparity; or index of cumulative endogenous estrogen exposure [8].

Some evidence supports a role of estrogen deprivation following menopause in the pathogenesis of dementia and a protective effect of exogenous estrogen exposure [9, 10]. However, more recent studies [11] and a review and meta-analysis concluded that the existing heterogenous evidence does not support an association between prolonged exposure to exogenous estrogen and lower dementia risk [12].

We investigated the potential association of both endogenous and exogenous estrogen exposure on dementia risk in a population-based longitudinal cohort of the oldest-old (age 90+ years), the age group with the highest incidence of dementia [13].The study of reproductive and menstrual factors as they impact risk of hormone-sensitive cancers has increasingly relied on models that combine multiple markers into a single variable [14, 15] which provide greater predictive power than consideration of individual markers. Therefore, we calculated an estrogen exposure index across the span from menarche to menopause and explored its relationship with dementia.

Methods

Study Population

Participants were part of The 90+ Study, a longitudinal study of aging and dementia among people aged 90 years and older [16]. These subjects were originally members of the Leisure World Cohort Study, a population-based epidemiological health study established in the early 1980s of a California retirement community (Leisure World Laguna Hills) [17]. This cohort is composed of moderately affluent, highly educated, and health conscious individuals; two-thirds are women. Persons alive and aged 90 years and older on January 1, 2003 (n=1143); on January 1, 2008 (n=440); and on or after January 1, 2009 (n=521) were invited to participate in The 90+ Study. Of the 2104 eligible cohort members, 1622 joined The 90+ Study. Women numbered 1267.

Estrogen Exposure

In the early 1980s a health survey was mailed to residents of Leisure World. This survey asked of the women: age at first menstrual period, ever pregnant, age at first child, number of children, age at last menstrual period, hysterectomy, age at hysterectomy, number of ovaries removed, age at first ovary removal, age a second ovary removal. Questions about estrogen replacement therapy asked for use of estrogens (separately for oral, injectable, cream or suppository) age started, current use, age stopped; for Premarin® (the most commonly used oral estrogen) all doses taken and that taken for the longest amount of time, and total number of years of Premarin®, other oral estrogens, injections, creams or suppositories.

In addition to calculating reproductive years (age at last menstrual period minus age at menarche), we developed an Endogenous Estrogen Exposure Index (EEEI) to include markers from menarche to the time of menopause with clear effects on estrogen levels. Those with increased estrogen exposure included early age at menarche, age at first child, later age at menopause and with decreased estrogen exposure included later age at menarche, parity, early age at menopause, oophorectomy. Simply stated, estrogen exposure starts at menarche, moves regularly to the first full term pregnancy (with a one-time increase) then drops below that seen in nulliparous women, starts declining at the time of perimenopause, and levels off at the time of natural menopause (last menstrual period). Oophorectomy causes an early drop in estrogen level. For our EEEI, we made several assumptions: (1) for women who had a natural menopause, age at menopause = reported age at last menstrual period, (2) for women who had a surgical menopause without bilateral oophorectomy before the age of 51, age at menopause = 51 (the average age at menopause in the US [18] and in this cohort), (3) for women who had a bilateral oophorectomy (unless it was after age 51), age at menopause = age at oophorectomy. Additionally, we assumed that the perimenopausal period starts about four years before menopause (generally mid- to late- 40s and ends at age at menopause[19]). If a women had no child, we made age at first child = age at start of perimenopausal period or age at bilateral oophorectomy. We then calculated

EEEI=f0*(ageatfirstchildageatmenarche)+f1*(ageatperimenopauseageafirstchild)+f2*(ageatmenopauseageatperimenopause)+f3*numberofchildren,

where the coefficients relate to the relative estrogen exposure during different periods with f0=1.0 (after menarche), f1=0.8 (drop after first birth), f2=0.4 (drop during perimenopause, average from 0.8 to 0), f3=1.4 (increase with each child). The value 0.8 was chosen for f1 based on the finding of 22% lower estrogen levels in parous women compared with nulliparous women [20]. Estrogen levels increase during pregnancy especially during the last trimester; a five-fold increase for 3–4 months suggested a 1.4 increase for each pregnancy [21].

If a woman had bilateral oophorectomy before age 47, there is no perimenopausal period and

EEEI=f0*(ageatfirstchildageatmenarche)+f1*(ageatoophorectomyageafirstchild)+f3*numberofchildren.

Assessments

Participants in The 90+ Study were asked to undergo longitudinal in-person evaluations, either at the research office or at their home. The evaluation included a neurological examination (with mental status testing[22] and assessment of functional abilities[23] by a trained physician or nurse practitioner and a neuropsychological test battery that included the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [24]. For participants whose poor health, frailty, disability, or unwillingness did not allow an in-person evaluation, information was obtained by telephone or with informant. Participants evaluated by telephone completed the short version of the Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI-short) [25]. For participants evaluated through informants, the Dementia Questionnaire (DQ) [26] was completed over the telephone. All participants (or their informants) completed a questionnaire that included demographics (including education), past medical history, and medication use. In addition, informants of all participants were asked about the participant’s cognitive status and functional abilities using a mailed questionnaire. Evaluations were repeated every 6 months for in-person participants and annually for participants evaluated by telephone and through informants. The DQ was completed for all participants shortly after death.

Determination of Cognitive Status

For all participants included in this analysis, cognitive status at baseline was determined from an in-person evaluation, either a neurological exam (91%) or MMSE score (9%). Cognitive status at follow-up was also determined from an in-person evaluation for most participants (81%) [13]. However, when an in-person evaluation at follow-up was not possible, we used any available information in the following hierarchical order: (1) neurological exam, (2) MMSE, (3) informant questionnaires, and (4) CASI-short. The neurological examiner determined cognitive status applying Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV) criteria for dementia [27]. For the MMSE, we used age- and education-specific cutoff scores for dementia derived from this cohort [28]. For the CASI-short, we used a score ≤25 as the cutoff score for dementia. Computer algorithms were used to apply DSM-IV criteria for dementia to the questionnaires obtained from informants. Details about the application of the algorithms and the validity of these methods are published elsewhere [29].

Statistical Analyses

We restricted our analyses to the 424 women who had no dementia at baseline as ascertained by an in-person evaluation, and who had at least 1 additional follow-up evaluation. Hazard ratios (HRs) of incident dementia associated with estrogen-related variables as measured by the Leisure World Cohort Study were estimated using Cox regression analysis [30]. Age at study entry was the age at enrollment in The 90+ Study (delayed entry) and the event of interest was age at dementia diagnosis. HRs for each estrogen-related variable were adjusted for age using age (continuous) as the fundamental time scale and with and without the covariate of education (high school, vocational school or some college, college graduate). We also controlled for years of exogenous estrogen use in the EEEI analysis. Participants were followed until age of dementia diagnosis, death or last visit. Statistical analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC). Separate analyses were performed for each estrogen-related factor and the EEEI and no adjustment in the p-values was made for multiple comparisons.

Results

For the 424 participants the mean (standard deviation) was 68.5 (5.0) for age at enrollment in the Leisure World Cohort Study, 93.2 (2.6), for age at enrollment in The 90+ Study, 96.5 (3.2) for age at last follow-up, 3.4 (2.5) for years of follow-up. Dementia was diagnosed in 209 (49%) at a mean age of 96.5 (3.1).

No menstrual / reproductive variable (Table 1) was associated with risk of dementia after age 90. Adjustment for education had essentially no effect on the observed HRs. Women with a late age (30+) at first child or who had no child tended to have reduced dementia risk (HR= 0.69, 95% CI 0.42–1.13). However, the result was not statistically significant.

Table 1.

Risk of incident dementia (2003–2019) by menstrual and reproductive data collected in the 1980s.

Hazard Ratio (95% CI)

No dementia (n=215) Dementia (n=209) Univariate Adjusted for education
Age at menarche
 ≤ 12 78 (36%) 67 (32%) 1.00 1.00
 13 62 (29%) 68 (33%) 1.22 (0.87–1.72) 1.22 (0.87–1.72)
 14+ 75 (35%) 74 (35%) 0.86 (0.61–1.20) 0.85 (0.61–1.19)
Number of children (5 missing)
 0 66 (31%) 52 (25%) 1.00 1.00
 1 28 (13%) 34 (16%) 0.99 (0.64–1.53) 0.98 (0.64–1.52)
 2 76 (36%) 76 (37%) 1.23 (0.86–1.75) 1.23 (0.86–1.75)
 3+ 42 (20%) 45 (22%) 1.14 (0.76–1.70) 1.18 (0.79–1.76)
Age at first child (9 missing)
 ≤ 20 11 (5%) 20 (10%) 1.00 1.00
 21–24 46 (22%) 40 (20%) 0.85 (0.49–1.45) 0.88 (0.51–1.51)
 25–29 55 (26%) 65 (32%) 0.99 (0.60–1.64) 1.06 (0.63–1.77)
 30+ 34 (16%) 26 (13%) 0.58 (0.32–1.05) 0.63 (0.35–1.15)
 No child 66 (31%) 52 (26%) 0.75 (0.45–1.27) 0.79 (0.47–1.34)
Menopause (2 missing)
 Natural 139 (65%) 132 (63%) 1.00 1.00
 Surgical without bilateral oophorectomy 33 (15%) 35 (17%) 1.28 (0.88–1.86) 1.24 (0.85–1.81)
 Bilateral oophorectomy 29 (14%) 28 (13%) 1.17 (0.78–1.76) 1.14 (0.75–1.72)
 Surgical NOS 12 (6%) 14 (7%) 0.97 (0.56–1.69) 0.94 (0.54–1.63)
Menopause (2 missing)
 Natural 139 (65%) 132 (63%) 1.00 1.00
 Surgical 74 (35%) 77 (37%) 1.17 (0.88–1.55) 1.14 (0.85–1.51)
Age at last menstrual period (4 missing)
 ≤ 44 56 (26%) 43 (21%) 1.00 1.00
 45–54 125 (59%) 137 (66%) 1.19 (0.84–1.68) 1.19 (0.84–1.68)
 55+ 31 (15%) 28 (13%) 1.09 (0.67–1.76) 1.13 (0.70–1.82)
Reproductive years (4 missing)
 ≤ 32 67 (32%) 63 (30%) 1.00 1.00
 33–38 69 (33%) 81 (39%) 1.06 (0.76–1.48) 1.06 (0.76–1.47)
 39+ 76 (36%) 64 (31%) 0.84 (0.59–1.19) 0.84 (0.59–1.20)
Endogenous estrogen exposure index (13 missing)
 ≤ 32.6 63 (30%) 71 (35%) 1.00 1.00
 32.7–35.8 72 (34%) 70 (35%) 0.96 (0.69–1.33) 0.95 (0.68–1.33)
 35.9+ 74 (35%) 61 (30%) 0.75 (0.53–1.06) 0.77 (0.54–1.08)

EEEI ranged from 10.6 to 48.4, mean=33.9, standard deviation=4.3 and was marginally associated with risk of incident dementia (HR=0.99, 95% CI 0.95–1.02). Women with a high EEEI (≥ 35.9) had a non-significant reduced risk (HR=0.75, 95% CI 0.53–1.06) relative to women with EEEI < 32.7) (Table 1). Adjustment for education had little effect on the HR. Nor did years of exogenous estrogen use; for women with a high EEEI the adjusted HR was 0.74, (95% CI 0.52–1.04).

Neither dose, duration, type nor time since last use of estrogen replacement was associated with risk of dementia after age 90 with or without adjustment for education (Table 2).

Table 2.

Risk of incident dementia (2003–2019) by use of estrogen replacement therapy data collected in the 1980s.

Hazard Ratio (95% CI)*
No dementia (n=215) Dementia (n=209) Univariate Adjusted for education
Estrogen replacement therapy (ERT)
 Never 68 (32%) 59 (28%) 1.00 1.00
 Ever 147 (68%) 150 (72%) 0.96 (0.71–1.30) 0.94 (0.69–1.28)
ERT duration in years (2 missing)
 ≤ 3 39 (18%) 49 (23%) 1.02 (0.70–1.49) 1.04 (0.71–1.53)
 4–14 50 (23%) 62 (30%) 1.27 (0.89–1.83) 1.32 (0.92–1.91)
 15+ 56 (26%) 39 (19%) 0.84 (0.56–1.27) 0.85 (0.57–1.28)
ERT years since last use (2 missing)
 15+ 19 (9%) 33 (16%) 1.04 (0.68–1.59) 1.06 (0.69–1.64)
 2–14 66 (31%) 67 (32%) 1.10 (0.77–1.56) 1.13 (0.79–1.61)
 0–1 60 (28%) 50 (24%) 0.99 (0.68–1.45) 1.00 (0.68–1.46)
ERT dose of Premarin® in mg (76 other estrogen)
 ≤ 0.62 52 (29%) 54 (32%) 1.05 (0.72–1.53) 1.07 (0.73–1.55)
 ≥ 1.25 60 (33%) 55 (33%) 0.95 (0.66–1.38) 0.95 (0.66–1.38)
Estrogen type
 Oral estrogen only 90 (42%) 94 (45%) 1.00 (0.72–1.38) 1.02 (0.73–1.42)
 Oral and other estrogen 45 (21%) 46 (22%) 1.17 (0.79–1.73) 1.18 (0.80–1.75)
 Injection and cream 12 (6%) 10 (5%) 0.99 (0.51–1.95) 0.99 (0.51–1.95)
*

All hazard ratios are relative to category “Never estrogen”

Discussion

We found little evidence to support the theory that exposure to endogenous or exogenous estrogen influences dementia risk in late life. We analyzed separately the factors commonly recognized as relative to estrogen and found none to be related to risk. This extends the lack of association of endogenous exposure and dementia risk to the oldest-old group (90+ years).

We also calculated an EEEI to include markers up to the time of menopause with clear effects on estrogen levels. We had no information on breast feeding and therefore could not include its effect in the index. It is difficult to know the relative weights to give to different periods of estrogen exposure, but the proposed EEEI provides a shorthand description of several key estrogen exposure periods and warrants further study. The only finding approaching significance in the current study was a reduced risk with high EEEI.

Exogenous estrogen exposure in this cohort was limited to menopausal estrogen use. We had no information on use of oral contraceptives (OC). OC were first approved by the FDA in 1960. At that time, our youngest participant was aged 57 and OC use, if any, was likely to be minimal.

Strengths of this study are its longitudinal design, dementia diagnosis procedures, complete follow-up, and estrogen variables accessed 25 years earlier (nearer to the time of the events and long before potential cognitive dysfunction might affect recall); limitations include its small size. With a larger size, the observed trend of increased risk of dementia with high endogenous estrogen exposure would be more precise. Additionally, as reproductive history and post-menopausal estrogen use likely effect mortality, selective removal of those at risk of developing dementia may occur. Our results are also limited to dementia risk in the oldest-old (those who survive to age 90).

Although estrogens exert potentially neuroprotective effects on brain structure and function [31] and have been found to be related to better cognitive performance [12], its influence on dementia risk in this oldest-old cohort was marginal.

Acknowledgments

Funding

This work was supported by NIH grants R01CA32197 and R01AG021055 and the Earl Carroll Trust Fund.

Footnotes

Potential Conflict of Interest

The authors report no conflict of interest and are alone responsible for the content and writing of the paper.

References

  • 1.Smith CA, et al. , Lifelong estrogen exposure and cognitive performance in elderly women. Brain Cogn, 1999. 39(3): p. 203–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Heys M, et al. , Life long endogenous estrogen exposure and later adulthood cognitive function in a population of naturally postmenopausal women from Southern China: the Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2011. 36(6): p. 864–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Paganini-Hill A and Henderson VW, Estrogen deficiency and risk of Alzheimer’s disease in women. Am J Epidemiol, 1994. 140(3): p. 256–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Colucci M, et al. , The number of pregnancies is a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease. Eur J Neurol, 2006. 13(12): p. 1374–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ptok U, Barkow K, and Heun R, Fertility and number of children in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Womens Ment Health, 2002. 5(2): p. 83–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hong X, Zhang X, and Li H, [A case-control study of endogenous estrogen and risk of Alzheimer’s disease]. Zhonghua Liu Xing Bing Xue Za Zhi, 2001. 22(5): p. 379–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Geerlings MI, et al. , Reproductive period and risk of dementia in postmenopausal women. JAMA, 2001. 285(11): p. 1475–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Prince MJ, et al. , Reproductive period, endogenous estrogen exposure and dementia incidence among women in Latin America and China; A 10/66 population-based cohort study. PLoS One, 2018. 13(2): p. e0192889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.LeBlanc ES, et al. , Hormone replacement therapy and cognition: systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA, 2001. 285(11): p. 1489–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Zucchella C, et al. , Reproductive life events and Alzheimer’s disease in Italian women: a retrospective study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat, 2012. 8: p. 555–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ryan J, et al. , Impact of a premature menopause on cognitive function in later life. BJOG, 2014. 121(13): p. 1729–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Georgakis MK, et al. , Age at menopause and duration of reproductive period in association with dementia and cognitive function: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2016. 73: p. 224–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Corrada MM, et al. , Dementia incidence continues to increase with age in the oldest old: the 90+ study. Ann Neurol, 2010. 67(1): p. 114–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Colditz GA and Frazier AL, Models of breast cancer show that risk is set by events of early life: prevention efforts must shift focus. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev, 1995. 4(5): p. 567–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Pike MC, et al. , ‘Hormonal’ risk factors, ‘breast tissue age’ and the age-incidence of breast cancer. Nature, 1983. 303(5920): p. 767–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Paganini-Hill A, Kawas CH, and Corrada MM, Lifestyle Factors and Dementia in the Oldest-old: The 90+ Study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord, 2016. 30(1): p. 21–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Paganini-Hill A, Ross RK, and Henderson BE, Prevalence of chronic disease and health practices in a retirement community. J Chronic Dis, 1986. 39(9): p. 699–707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Society, N.A.M. Menopause 101: A primer for the perimenopausal. 2020. 1/25/2020]; http://www.menopause.org/for-women/menopauseflashes/menopause-symptoms-and-treatments/menopause-101-a-primer-for-the-perimenopausal].
  • 19.Hoyt LT and Falconi AM, Puberty and perimenopause: reproductive transitions and their implications for women’s health. Soc Sci Med, 2015. 132: p. 103–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bernstein L, et al. , Estrogen and sex hormone-binding globulin levels in nulliparous and parous women. J Natl Cancer Inst, 1985. 74(4): p. 741–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.O’Leary P, et al. , Longitudinal assessment of changes in reproductive hormones during normal pregnancy. Clin Chem, 1991. 37(5): p. 667–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Morris JC, The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology, 1993. 43(11): p. 2412–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Pfeffer RI, et al. , Measurement of functional activities in older adults in the community. J Gerontol, 1982. 37(3): p. 323–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, and McHugh PR, “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res, 1975. 12(3): p. 189–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Teng EL, et al. , The Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI): a practical test for cross-cultural epidemiological studies of dementia. Int Psychogeriatr, 1994. 6(1): p. 45–58; discussion 62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Silverman JM, et al. , Reliability of the family history method in genetic studies of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias. Am J Psychiatry, 1986. 143(10): p. 1279–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Association, A.P., Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). 1994, American Psychiatric Association: Wahington, DC. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Whittle C, et al. , Neuropsychological data in nondemented oldest old: the 90+ Study. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol, 2007. 29(3): p. 290–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Corrada MM, et al. , Prevalence of dementia after age 90: results from the 90+ study. Neurology, 2008. 71(5): p. 337–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cox DR, Regression Models and Life-Tables. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B-Statistical Methodology, 1972. 34(2): p. 187–+. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Engler-Chiurazzi EB, Singh M, and Simpkins JW, From the 90’s to now: A brief historical perspective on more than two decades of estrogen neuroprotection. Brain Res, 2016. 1633: p. 96–100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES