Skip to main content
JAMA Network logoLink to JAMA Network
. 2020 May 13;77(9):1–11. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.0821

Outcomes of Randomized Clinical Trials of Interventions to Enhance Social, Emotional, and Spiritual Components of Wisdom

A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis

Ellen E Lee 1,2,3,7, Katherine J Bangen 1,2,7, Julie A Avanzino 1,3,7, BaiChun Hou 1,3,7, Marina Ramsey 1,7, Graham Eglit 1,2,3,7, Jinyuan Liu 3,4, Xin M Tu 3,4, Martin Paulus 1,5,7, Dilip V Jeste 1,3,6,7,
PMCID: PMC7221873  PMID: 32401284

Key Points

Question

How effective are interventions to enhance individual components of wisdom?

Findings

Despite heterogeneity of studies and publication bias, this meta-analysis and meta-regression found that interventions to enhance prosocial behaviors, emotional regulation, and spirituality were generally effective, especially among older participants.

Meaning

Per this analysis, interventions to enhance prosocial behaviors, emotional regulation, and spirituality are effective in a proportion of individuals with mental or physical illnesses and people from the community.


This systematic review and meta-analysis examines studies that investigated social, emotional, and spiritual aspects of wisdom.

Abstract

Importance

Wisdom is a neurobiological personality trait made up of specific components, including prosocial behaviors, emotional regulation, and spirituality. It is associated with greater well-being and happiness.

Objective

To evaluate the effectiveness of interventions to enhance individual components of wisdom.

Data Sources

MEDLINE and PsycINFO databases were searched for articles published through December 31, 2018.

Study Eligibility Criteria

Randomized clinical trials that sought to enhance a component of wisdom, used published measures to assess that component, were published in English, had a minimum sample size of 40 participants, and presented data that enabled computation of effect sizes were included in this meta-analysis.

Data Extraction and Synthesis

Random-effect models were used to calculate pooled standardized mean differences (SMDs) for each wisdom component and random-effects meta-regression to assess heterogeneity of studies.

Main Outcomes and Measures

Improvement in wisdom component using published measures.

Results

Fifty-seven studies (N = 7096 participants) met review criteria: 29 for prosocial behaviors, 13 for emotional regulation, and 15 for spirituality. Study samples included people with psychiatric or physical illnesses and from the community. Of the studies, 27 (47%) reported significant improvement with medium to large effect sizes. Meta-analysis revealed significant pooled SMDs for prosocial behaviors (23 studies; pooled SMD, 0.43 [95% CI, 0.22-0.3]; P = .02), emotional regulation (12 studies; pooled SMD, 0.67 [95% CI, 0.21-1.12]; P = .004), and spirituality (12 studies; pooled SMD, 1.00 [95% CI, 0.41-1.60]; P = .001). Heterogeneity of studies was considerable for all wisdom components. Publication bias was present for prosocial behavior and emotional regulation studies; after adjusting for it, the pooled SMD for prosocial behavior remained significant (SMD, 0.4 [95% CI, 0.16-0.78]; P = .003). Meta-regression analysis found that effect sizes did not vary by wisdom component, although for trials on prosocial behaviors, large effect sizes were associated with older mean participant age (β, 0.08 [SE, 0.04]), and the reverse was true for spirituality trials (β, −0.13 [SE, 0.04]). For spirituality interventions, higher-quality trials had larger effect sizes (β, 4.17 [SE, 1.07]), although the reverse was true for prosocial behavior trials (β, −0.91 [SE 0.44]).

Conclusions and Relevance

Interventions to enhance spirituality, emotional regulation, and prosocial behaviors are effective in a proportion of people with mental or physical illnesses and from the community. The modern behavioral epidemics of loneliness, suicide, and opioid abuse point to a growing need for wisdom-enhancing interventions to promote individual and societal well-being.

Introduction

Wisdom has been discussed in religious and philosophical texts for centuries. The empirical study of wisdom began only 4 decades ago, but the number of research articles on wisdom has been growing. Vaillant’s Harvard Study of Adult Development was the first to examine psychosocial and lifestyle factors associated with wisdom and well-being.1 Baltes and Staudinger2 defined wisdom as extensive pragmatic knowledge, focusing on the cognitive or intellectual aspect. Clayton and Birren3 added reflective and affective components to the definition of wisdom. Sternberg4 posited that wisdom resulted from an application of knowledge mediated by a balance of personal and societal interests. Ardelt5 conceptualized wisdom as an integration of cognitive, reflective, and affective (or compassionate) personality qualities. Cloninger’s research6 highlighted the relevance of certain personality traits to well-being.

We previously reviewed the empirical literature on wisdom7,8 and conducted a Delphi method study of consensus among international experts in wisdom,7 as well as a mixed-methods qualitative-quantitative study of wisdom in an ancient religious document. These studies suggested that wisdom is a complex human trait with several specific components (ie, prosocial behaviors such as empathy and compassion, emotional regulation, spirituality, self-reflection, social decision-making or social advising, acceptance of uncertainty, and decisiveness). Wisdom is thought to be adaptive rather than fixed and can increase with age and personal experience. Blazer,8 Williams et al,9 Grossman,10 and others have emphasized practical wisdom, considering the context that influences wise decision-making. Several investigations have reported that wisdom is associated with positive outcomes, including better overall physical and mental health,11 well-being,12 happiness,13 life satisfaction,14 and resilience,15 as well as lower levels of loneliness.15

We also reviewed the literature on neurobiological basis of wisdom components and found that they seemed to share similar brain regions, specifically the prefrontal cortex (dorsolateral, ventromedial, and anterior cingulate) and limbic striatum.16 Furthermore, we found a number of published case reports of damage to these areas that produced a loss of these components (eg, increased emotional lability, impulsivity, indecisiveness, lack of compassion) without affecting other cognitive abilities, such as in the case of Phineas Gage17 and patients with frontotemporal dementia.18 While there have been no prospective longitudinal studies of changes in wisdom with aging using standardized rating scales, older adults have been reported in multiple investigations to have greater emotional regulation,19 positivity,20 prosocial behaviors,21 and self-reflection19 compared with younger adults. Considerable empirical evidence indicating neuroplasticity of aging, especially in adults who are physically, cognitively, and socially active, supports the potential to modify wisdom-type traits in later life. Aging-associated brain adaptations, including reduced lateralization of functioning,22 posterior-to-anterior shift in brain activity,23 and diminished amygdala response to negative or stressful stimuli,24 may facilitate wisdom-relevant brain function in later life. Distinct patterns of brain activation on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in response to moral dilemmas have been observed in individuals with higher vs lower scores on a validated scale for assessing overall wisdom.25

Personality traits, such as resilience and optimism, have been shown to be moderately heritable (with estimates of 33%26 to 52%27), suggesting they are also influenced by environmental factors. A recent investigation28 reported 50% to 58% heritability of self-directedness, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence in a large, population-based study that was replicated in multiple samples. These components overlap with those in our definition of wisdom. Several recent studies have reported increases in resilience and optimism with behavioral interventions.29,30 While acknowledging their limitations, these findings support the possibility that positive personality traits can be enhanced through psychosocial interventions and thereby potentially lead to improvement of health and well-being. Yet we found no published reviews of interventions for increasing wisdom or its components; thus, it was unclear whether such interventions were effective. Therefore, we conducted a systematic review with meta-analysis and meta-regression analysis of randomized clinical trials (RCTs) intended to enhance one of the specific components of wisdom.

Methods

Procedures

We conducted a literature search for interventions targeting wisdom components, as outlined in the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) diagram (Figure 1). To identify articles for review, we surveyed the MEDLINE and PsycINFO online databases through December 31, 2018, with the following criteria: any study that (1) included one of the wisdom components listed above, (2) was published in English, (3) was an RCT, (4) had a minimum sample size of 40 participants, (5) included a published assessment tool to measure that component before and after the intervention, and (6) presented data that enabled computation of the intervention’s effect size for enhancing the wisdom component.

Figure 1. PRISMA Flow Diagram for Literature Review.

Figure 1.

The following MEDLINE search terms were used: “((Intervention [Title/Abstract] OR Interventions [Title/Abstract])AND (wisdom[Title] OR pro-social[Title] OR empathy[Title] OR compassion[Title] OR emotion regulation[Title] OR emotional regulation[Title] OR spirituality[Title] OR self-reflection[Title] OR decisiveness[Title] OR social advising[Title] OR altruism[Title]) AND (“0001/01/01”[PDat]: “2018/12/31”[PDat]) AND English[lang]).” The following search terms were used in the PsycINFO database: “(ti(interventions OR intervention) OR ab(interventions OR intervention)) AND ti(wisdom OR pro-social OR empathy OR compassion OR self-reflection OR emotion regulation OR emotional regulation OR spirituality OR decisiveness OR social advising OR altruism)),” with the additional limits of English language and publication prior to December 31, 2018.

This search yielded 513 articles of potential interest after the removal of duplicates, of which 153 were deemed relevant based on a review of their abstracts. At least 2 authors (of a group of 4: E.E.L., J.A.A., B.H., and G.E.) independently examined each full-text journal article for defined eligibility criteria. The κ statistic for study selection was 0.97. Disagreements on inclusion criteria were settled by a third author from the same group of 4. The final search resulted in 57 studies reported in 54 articles.

Statistical Analysis

For the meta-analysis, outcome data (assessment of wisdom component) were extracted from each study, including means, SDs, and sample sizes for the intervention and control groups. Data extraction was conducted by several authors in duplicate (E.E.L., M.R., and B.H.). The standardized mean differences (SMDs) for each RCT and the pooled SMD for each wisdom component were calculated using a random-effects model, given the heterogeneity of the interventions and outcome measures. Pooled SMDs were interpreted as small (0.2≤SMD<0.5), medium (0.5≤SMD<0.8), and large (0.8≤SMD).31

The quality of these studies was evaluated using a modified Scale for Assessing Scientific Quality of Investigations (SASQI).32,33 Thirteen original items were retained in the modified version; the 3 excluded items referred to educational interventions that used technology.

The I2 statistic was used to assess statistical heterogeneity of the study outcomes, with the following interpretation about its importance: low (0%-40%), moderate (30%-60%), substantial (50%-90%), and high (75%-100%).34 Publication bias was assessed using the Egger test (a measure of the asymmetry of the funnel plot),35 as well as the Duval and Tweedie trim-and-fill procedure36 to adjust the funnel plot and pooled SMD for missing studies.

To perform meta-regression analyses, we first converted different types of effect sizes into η2 and logit-transformed η2 to remove range restriction. We then performed a meta-regression model using the generalized estimating equation, weighted by sample size of each study.37 This led to models with the following factors included: type of patient (community based vs physically ill vs psychiatrically ill), mean age of participants, percentage of female participants, intervention format (individual vs group), number of sessions, length of individual sessions (minutes), and SASQI score (with the median split into 2 levels). The variance inflation factor was calculated to detect potential multicollinearity.38 Additional factors included interaction terms: mean age by wisdom component, SASQI median split by wisdom component, and length of sessions by wisdom component based on variance inflation factor analyses. Significance was defined as an α less than .05 (2-tailed) for all analyses. All analyses were conducted using RevMan version 5.3 (Cochrane Reviews) and R version 3.6.1 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing).

Results

Study Characteristics

Of the 57 intervention studies that met our review criteria (Table 1; eTable 1 in the Supplement), 29 focused on prosocial behaviors39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65 (eg, empathy, compassion, altruism), 13 on emotional regulation,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77 and 15 on spirituality.78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92 We did not find any published RCTs for self-reflection, social decision-making or social advising, acceptance of uncertainty, and decisiveness.

Table 1. Summary of Intervention Characteristicsa.

Characteristic Wisdom component, No.
Prosocial behaviors Emotional regulation Spirituality
Randomized clinical trial
With inert control group 17 8 8
With active control group 9 5 7
Age groups
Child/adolescent 6 5 0
Adult 20 8 15a
Sample characteristics
Community based 18 5 3
Physically ill 1 0 8
Psychiatrically ill 7 8 4
Intervention format
Group 19 10 10
Individual 7 3 5
Sample size, mean (SD), No. 116.1 (115.0) 130.9 (115.0) 143.3 (116.6)
Age, mean (SD), y 28.7 (13.9) 28.2 (13.7) 42.2 (16.1)
Women, mean (SD), % 58.9 (23.1) 60.2 (38.1) 72.0 (20.7)
Sessions, mean (SD), No. 8.8 (8.6) 10.0 (4.6) 5.6 (3.7)
Length of sessions, mean (SD), min 123.8 (102.3) 87.3 (24.8) 102.5 (102.8)
SASQI score, mean (SD) 10.0 (2.0) 10.9 (1.3) 10.3 (1.5)

Abbreviations: SASQI, Scale for Assessing Scientific Quality of Investigations (modified version).

a

One study78 included both adolescents and young adults (aged 13-25 years).

Forty-five studies43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,55,56,57,58,59,60,62,63,64,65,67,70,71,73,74,76,77,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92 included only adult participants. Twenty-nine reports39,41,42,43,44,45,46,51,53,54,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,70,72,79,89,92 had community-based participants, 19 reports40,47,48,49,52,55,68,69,71,73,74,75,76,77,78,81,90,91 included persons with psychiatric illnesses or behavioral problems, and 9 had participants with physical illnesses.50,80,82,83,84,85,86,87,88 Forty interventions39,42,43,44,46,49,50,53,54,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,64,65,66,67,68,71,72,73,74,75,76,80,81,83,84,86,87,89,90,91,92 took place in a group setting. Across the wisdom components, the interventions varied in length, duration, and outcome measures used (eTable 1 in the Supplement). The mean (SD) intervention duration was 748 (753) minutes. Study quality was generally high because of the criteria for selecting reports for this review, with mean (SD) modified SASQI scores of 10.44 (1.7; possible range, 0-13; observed range, 5-13; eTable 2 in the Supplement). Compared with control groups, 27 RCTs (47%) reported significantly improved wisdom components with a medium to large effect size.39,45,47,49,50,51,55,56,57,59,60,62,63,64,65,67,69,70,72,74,77,82,83,84,88,90,92

The proportion of trials with medium to large effect sizes did not differ across the 3 components: prosocial behaviors (15 of 29 studies39,45,47,49,50,51,55,56,57,59,60,62,63,65), emotional regulation (6 of 13 studies67,69,70,72,74,77), and spirituality (6 of 15 studies82,83,84,88,90,92). Among the prosocial behavior RCTs, the trials with medium to large effect sizes had older mean participant ages (t20 = −3.59; P = .002; d, −1.57). Among the emotional regulation studies, the interventions with medium to large effect sizes were more likely to occur within individual-based formats (3 of 369,70,77) compared with group settings (3 studies67,72,74 of 10 studies66,67,68,71,72,73,74,75,76) (χ21 = 4.55; P = .03).

The outcome measures used varied across the reports (eTable 3 in the Supplement). Forty-eight of 57 studies41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,65,70,71,72,73,74,75,77,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92 (84%) used self-rated measures. Ten trials (17.5%) used objective outcomes: 5 on prosocial behaviors (3 task-based,53,63,64 1 researcher-rated,39 and 1 parent-rated40), and 5 on emotional regulation in 4 studies (teacher-rated or parent-rated).66,67,68,69

Prosocial Behavior Interventions

Twenty-nine RCTs focused on prosocial behaviors of empathy, compassion (including self-compassion), and altruism.39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65 Twenty-three studies39,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,55,56,57,58,59,60,62,63,64,65 included adults. Eight trials40,47,48,49,52,55 included persons with psychiatric or behavioral problems, 1 trial50 included people with physical illness (diabetes mellitus), and 21 were community based.41,42,43,44,45,46,51,53,54,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65 While empathy and compassion were hypothesized to reduce cyberbullying41,54 and aggression61 and improve grades and learning53 in young people, self-compassion was hypothesized to decrease distress and improve well-being.50,62

The meta-analysis calculations for these interventions were based on 23 RCTs from 21 articles,39,40,41,43,45,46,47,49,50,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,65 with a pooled SMD of 0.43 (95% CI, 0.22-0.3; P = .01; Figure 2). Heterogeneity of the studies was considerable (I2 = 84%; P < .001). Altogether, the interventions had a statistically significant but modest association with prosocial behaviors.

Figure 2. Forest Plot for Interventions for Prosocial Behaviors.

Figure 2.

SMD indicates standardized mean differences.

Emotional Regulation Interventions

There were 13 studies (in 12 articles66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77) of emotional regulation interventions in either psychiatrically ill or community-based populations, with 10 using group settings.66,67,68,71,72,73,74,75,76 These RCTs sought to improve binge-eating behaviors,71 behavioral problems,69 test anxiety,72 and family relationships.67

The meta-analysis calculations for these interventions were based on 12 studies from 11 published RCTs,66,67,69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77 with a pooled SMD of 0.67 (95% CI, 0.21-1.12; P = .004; Figure 3). Heterogeneity of study outcomes was considerable (I2 = 93%; P < .001). Altogether, the interventions had a statistically significant, medium-sized association with emotional regulation outcomes.

Figure 3. Forest Plots for Interventions for Emotional Regulation and Spirituality.

Figure 3.

SMD indicates standardized mean differences.

Spirituality Interventions

There were 15 spirituality-focused RCTs,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92 all in adults, and 8 of these80,82,83,84,85,86,87,88 were in people with serious and/or terminal medical illnesses. Four RCTs included psychiatric samples: patients with opiate use disorders stabilized with methadone maintenance,90 adults with depression/anxiety,78,81 and women with eating disorders.91 Ten interventions were conducted in group settings80,81,83,84,86,87,89,90,91,92 and 2 drew from specific religions (Buddhist teachings80 or Islamic traditions82). Spirituality was hypothesized to reduce psychological suffering and improve quality of life.78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92

The meta-analysis calculations for these interventions were based on 12 studies from 12 published RCTs,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92 with a pooled SMD of 1.00 (95% CI, 0.41-1.60; P = .001; Figure 2C). Heterogeneity of study outcomes was considerable (I2 = 96%; P < .001). Altogether, the interventions had a statistically significant large-sized association with spiritual outcomes.

Quality of Included Studies

The quality rating for each study is reported in eTable 2 in the Supplement, using the information from the modified SASQI scale. Overall, 29 studies40,41,42,43,45,46,48,50,51,54,55,57,59,60,64,65,69,71,74,75,76,78,81,82,85,87,88,89,90 (51%) described the method used to randomize participants to the intervention vs control groups, 23 (40%) included an active control group,40,44,47,49,51,54,58,59,61,63,66,68,74,75,76,79,82,85,87,88,91,92 and 21 (37%) examined whether participants who dropped out differed significantly from those who completed the study.40,45,46,48,49,50,51,55,56,69,73,74,75,77,78,82,83,85,90,92 The lower-quality reports (those in the lower 50th percentile; 25 studies vs 32 higher-quality reports) were less likely to describe randomization methods (6 of 25 lower-quality studies vs 13 of 32 higher-quality studies), inclusion and exclusion criteria (14 lower-quality studies vs 30 higher-quality studies), withdrawals or dropouts (11 lower-quality studies vs 31 higher-quality studies), comparison of demographic variables between the control and intervention groups (18 lower-quality studies vs 31 higher-quality studies), and analytical plan to address differences between control vs intervention groups (15 lower-quality studies vs 29 higher-quality studies) and between participants who dropped out vs completed the study (0 lower-quality studies vs 29 higher-quality studies).

Publication Bias

The Egger test indicated the presence of publication bias among the studies of prosocial behavior (z, 3.48; P < .001) and emotional regulation (z,  9.01; P < .001), but not the spirituality studies (z, 0.10; P = .92). Funnel plots using the trim-and-fill method36 resulted in the following adjusted SMDs (prosocial behavior: SMD, 0.4 [95% CI, 0.16-0.78]; P = .003; emotional regulation: SMD, 1.26 [95% CI, -0.53 to 3.04]; P = .17; spirituality: SMD, 1.00 [95% CI, 0.46-1.54]; P < .001; eFigures 1, 2, and 3 in the Supplement).

Meta-regression Analysis

The meta-regression analyses found that effect sizes did not differ among different wisdom components. Among trials on prosocial behavior, sample populations with older mean ages had higher effect sizes (β, 0.08 [SE, 0.04]); for spirituality trials, sample populations with younger mean ages had higher effect sizes (β, −0.13 [SE, 0.04]) (Table 2). Among spirituality interventions, studies with higher SASQI scores had larger effect sizes (β, 4.17 [SE, 1.07]), while the reverse was true for prosocial behavior trials (β, −0.91 [SE, 0.44]).

Table 2. Results of Meta-regression Analysis.

Characteristic Multivariate model of all studies
β (SE) Wald statistic P value
Intercept −1.12 (0.80) 1.93 .17
Wisdom component
Emotional regulation (vs prosocial behaviors) −0.12 (0.53) 0.05 .82
Spirituality (vs prosocial behaviors) −0.32 (0.42) 0.57 .45
Participant type
With physical illness (vs community based) 0.59 (0.56) 1.11 .29
With psychiatric illness (vs community based) 0.09 (0.34) 0.07 .79
Mean age, y 0.08 (0.04) 5.11 .02
Women, % 0.003 (0.008) 0.16 .69
Intervention format
Individual (vs group) 0.39 (0.42) 0.87 .35
Sessions, No. −0.02 (0.03) 0.55 .46
Length of sessions, min 0.002 (0.002) 1.03 .31
SASQI
Scores in top 50th percentile (vs bottom 50th percentile) −0.91 (0.44) 4.39 .04
Interaction terms
Wisdom component (prosocial behavior) × mean age 0.00 [Reference] NA NA
Wisdom component (emotional regulation) × mean age −0.06 (0.04) 2.18 .14
Wisdom component (spirituality) × mean age −0.13 (0.04) 9.64 .002
Wisdom component (prosocial behavior) × SASQI Score (top 50th percentile) 0.00 [Reference] NA NA
Wisdom component (emotional regulation) × SASQI Score (top 50th percentile) 0.78 (0.87) 0.80 .37
Wisdom component (spirituality) × SASQI Score (top 50th percentile) 4.17 (1.07) 15.1 <.001

Abbreviations: NA, not applicable; SASQI, Scale for Assessing Scientific Quality of Investigations (modified version).

Discussion

Overall, our meta-analysis of RCTs supports significant increase in levels of the wisdom components with small to large effect sizes, with no difference among the 3 wisdom components. However, the studies had several limitations.

The large range of SMD values could reflect differing outcome measures, as well as variability in types of study participants, control conditions, and interventions. The meta-regression analysis took into account several variables listed in Table 2. The intervention methodology varied widely from cognitive behavioral therapy to mindfulness-based group therapies to spiritual counseling. Other participant characteristics and nuances in intervention type, adherence, and outcome measures that could not be assessed in this study might also affect SMD value.

Importance of Participant Characteristics

Prosocial behavioral interventions conducted in samples with older mean ages had greater effect sizes. This finding is consistent with published cross-sectional studies reporting higher levels of certain wisdom components in participants of older age.93,94,95 These include emotional regulation,96,97,98 with decreased likelihood of using destructive strategies to manage conflict99 and lower recall of negative emotional experiences.100 Similarly, older adults exhibit greater emotional empathy (empathic concern)101 and altruistic behavior,21,102 despite deficits in cognitive empathy (accurate perceptions of others’ feelings).101

The percentage of female participants varied across studies, and male and female participants had similar responses to interventions. However, women have been reported to have greater baseline empathy and compassion,103 as well as different emotional neurocircuitry than men.104,105,106 One study67 of emotional regulation in family units found a medium to large effect size in mothers but not in fathers, which was attributed to greater caregiving roles taken by mothers and thus greater involvement with the intervention, which had relied heavily on home-based work. Further investigation of sex differences is needed to clarify the underlying neurobiological mechanisms of positive psychological traits.

Features of Interventions

Intervention features, such as session length, frequency, and format, were not associated with outcomes in the meta-regression. While several interventions included home-based practice, such sessions had variable guidelines and adherence levels and therefore could not be included in the meta-regression. Informal skills practice may be essential to honing such traits and should be considered in developing future interventions for wisdom components. Of note, the comparison of trials with medium to large effect sizes with trials with small effect sizes showed that, among the emotional regulation interventions, individual-based interventions were more likely to have medium to large effect sizes than group-based interventions. This finding could partially reflect the fact that fewer RCTs used individual-based formats than group formats because of cost difference and potential gains from group-based settings (eg, social and peer support).

Study Quality

The modified SASQI scores (lower vs higher than the 50th percentile) reflect the quality of the included studies. While study quality did not differ among different wisdom components, less rigorous RCTs of prosocial behavior had larger effect sizes overall (ie, confounding factors could inflate differences found between the intervention and control group). Interestingly, specifically among spirituality interventions, higher-quality studies reported larger effect sizes. Interventions to improve spirituality may face the additional challenges of its broad definition, ranging from awareness of other powers or forces that influence the universe to living a spiritual life (which is more similar to religious ideals),107,108 connection to personality traits,6 and involvement of multiple brain regions.69

Study Outcomes

Unsurprisingly, most of the studies used self-rated or subjective measures as outcomes. While there is great value in assessing the subjective experience of individuals for assessing personality traits such as wisdom, objective assessments should be encouraged. An example is a study by Bonvicini and colleagues39 objectively assessing physician empathy based on audio recordings of patient-physician interactions.

Future Directions

Future studies should be based on hypotheses generated from this review—for example, that (1) enhancement of wisdom components will contribute to improvement of overall wisdom, health, and well-being, and (2) older people are more likely to have improvements in wisdom components than younger persons. Exploratory analyses should include examination of hedonic vs eudemonic well-being, interventions, sex differences in response to interventions, and validity of objective vs subjective ratings of wisdom components. Use of objective measures of wisdom components, techniques such as ecological momentary assessments, technology such as artificial intelligence, relevant biomarkers, and longer-term follow-ups are recommended.

We believe that the relevance of wisdom-associated interventions expands beyond the individual level to the societal level. Over the past 3 decades, annual rates of deaths from opioid overdose and suicides have been rising rapidly,109,110 resulting in a decline in the mean American lifespan for the first time in half a century.111 Loneliness and social isolation have been reported to lead to higher stress levels, as well as physical and psychiatric morbidity, including substance abuse and suicidal behavior.112,113,114,115 These recent behavioral epidemics of loneliness, suicides, and opioid abuse112,113,114,115 will require behavioral or psychosocial vaccines or antidotes. The strong inverse correlation between loneliness and wisdom found in a recent study15 suggests that wisdom may be a remedy for behavioral toxins, such as loneliness.116

Limitations

This review has several limitations. Improvement in individual components of wisdom is not the same as increase in overall wisdom. Studies focusing on wisdom as an entity are clearly warranted. We found only 1 RCT117 with overall wisdom as an outcome measure, but it did not meet other selection criteria. However, a recently published study by Treichler and colleagues118 showed increased overall wisdom using the San Diego Wisdom Scale with a 1-month group-based intervention in senior housing communities, which also reduced perceived stress and increased resilience without affecting overall well-being.

Next, search terms were limited to the specific wisdom components and thus did not include negative terms associated with lack of wisdom components (eg, impulsivity, selfishness), and may have missed some relevant studies. Outcomes were limited to the specific wisdom component, because many studies did not assess well-being or other health-associated measures. Most studies relied on self-report assessments of wisdom components, which have potential for bias because of socially desirable responses and problems with recall accuracy. Objective and technology-based measures (eg, reports by participants’ close associates, ecological momentary assessments, and video game–based tasks) are warranted. Whether improvements in a wisdom component generalized to everyday life was not examined. Rationale for the study design, study participants’ sociodemographic and clinical characteristics, trial methodology, outcomes evaluated, and statistics used varied across the RCTs, contributing to significant heterogeneity among studies, publication bias, and challenges in analyzing all the study features. Follow-up periods were often short, making longer-term efficacy of the interventions unknown. Another limitation pertains to exclusion of articles that were not in the English language. Also, we could not include distal outcomes that were sometimes the goals of the original RCTs (eg, reducing binge-eating behaviors, preventing cyberbullying),41,55,81 because of marked heterogeneity. Finally, only a few trials were of pragmatic type (ie, combining efficacy with effectiveness, using manualized interventions, and ensuring implementation and dissemination potential in the real world).

Few studies used neurobiological assessments. One novel study, although it did not meet our selection criteria, represents the type of research required to improve our understanding of the neurobiological implications of wisdom-associated interventions. Klimecki and colleagues119 used an fMRI socioaffective video task to analyze brain activation in women after compassion training compared with memory training and reported enhanced brain activation in anterior insula and anterior midcingulate cortex (regions associated with empathy), as well as the ventral striatum, anterior cingulate, and medial orbitofrontal cortex (regions identified in putative wisdom neurocircuitry).16 Adding neurobiological assessments, such as regional brain activation on fMRI, with an emotion-based task (eg, seeing happy vs angry faces) would help broaden our knowledge of the brain-based mechanisms mediating improvements in wisdom or its components. Although a wisdom pill is unlikely in the near future, the next generation of advanced and targeted neurostimulation techniques could selectively activate or inhibit neurocircuits associated with components of wisdom.

Conclusions

Basic research is needed to better understand the neurobiology of wisdom and develop new biologically oriented, wisdom-associated interventions. Eventually, development of wisdom-enhancing interventions at societal level will become a priority, although a number of steps are required to enable development and testing of effective large-scale community-wide interventions. Balancing these 2 priorities will require political wisdom on the part of health care leaders. Increased wisdom in both individuals and communities is likely to confer broad advantages in well-being and health that would ultimately improve survival and flourishing of the society as a whole.

Supplement.

eTable 1. Descriptive Characteristics of Individual Wisdom Component Intervention Trials (N=57)

eTable 2. Risk of Bias

eTable 3. Outcome Measures used in the selected studies

eReferences.

eFigure 1. Funnel plot of Interventions for Pro-social Behaviors

eFigure 2. Funnel Plot of Interventions for Emotional Regulation

eFigure 3. Funnel Plot of Interventions for Spirituality

References

  • 1.Vaillant GE. Adaptation to Life. Harvard University Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Baltes PB, Staudinger UM. Wisdom: a metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. Am Psychol. 2000;55(1):122-136. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Clayton VP, Birren JE. The development of wisdom across the life-span: A reexamination of an ancient topic In: Baltes PB, Brim OG, eds. Life-span development and behavior. Vol 3 Academic Press; 1980:103-135. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Sternberg RJ. A balance theory of wisdom. Rev Gen Psychol. 1998;2(4):347-365. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.4.347 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ardelt M. Empirical assessment of a three-dimensional wisdom scale. Res Aging. 2003;25(3):275-324. doi: 10.1177/0164027503025003004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Josefsson K, Cloninger CR, Hintsanen M, Jokela M, Pulkki-Råback L, Keltikangas-Järvinen L. Associations of personality profiles with various aspects of well-being. J Affect Disord. 2011;133(1-2):265-273. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jeste DV, Ardelt M, Blazer D, Kraemer HC, Vaillant G, Meeks TW. Expert consensus on characteristics of wisdom. Gerontologist. 2010;50(5):668-680. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnq022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Blazer DG. Religion, spirituality, and mental health. Can J Psychiatry. 2009;54(5):281-282. doi: 10.1177/070674370905400501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Williams PB, Mangelsdorf HH, Kontra C, Nusbaum HC, Hoeckner B. The relationship between mental and somatic practices and wisdom. PLoS One. 2016;11(2):e0149369. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149369 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Grossmann I. Wisdom in context. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2017;12(2):233-257. doi: 10.1177/1745691616672066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Webster JD, Westerhof GJ, Bohlmeijer ET. Wisdom and mental health across the lifespan. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014;69(2):209-218. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ardelt M, Jeste DV. Wisdom and hard times. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2018;73(8):1374-1383. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bergsma A, Ardelt M. Self-reported wisdom and happiness: an empirical investigation. J Happiness Stud. 2012;13(3):481-499. doi: 10.1007/s10902-011-9275-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ardelt M. Antecedents and effects of wisdom in old age—a longitudinal perspective on aging well. Res Aging. 2000;22(4):360-394. doi: 10.1177/0164027500224003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lee EE, Depp C, Palmer BW, et al. . High prevalence and adverse health effects of loneliness in community-dwelling adults across the lifespan. Int Psychogeriatr. 2019;31(10):1447-1462. doi: 10.1017/S1041610218002120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Meeks TW, Jeste DV. Neurobiology of wisdom. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(4):355-365. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Haas LF. Phineas Gage and the science of brain localisation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001;71(6):761-761. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.71.6.761 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bang J, Spina S, Miller BL. Frontotemporal dementia. Lancet. 2015;386(10004):1672-1682. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00461-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Brassen S, Gamer M, Peters J, Gluth S, Büchel C. Don’t look back in anger! responsiveness to missed chances in successful and nonsuccessful aging. Science. 2012;336(6081):612-614. doi: 10.1126/science.1217516 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Mather M, Canli T, English T, et al. . Amygdala responses to emotionally valenced stimuli in older and younger adults. Psychol Sci. 2004;15(4):259-263. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00662.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Beadle JN, Sheehan AH, Dahlben B, Gutchess AH. Aging, empathy, and prosociality. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015;70(2):215-224. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Cabeza R. Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol Aging. 2002;17(1):85-100. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.85 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Davis SW, Dennis NA, Daselaar SM, Fleck MS, Cabeza R. Que PASA? the posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(5):1201-1209. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm155 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.St Jacques PL, Bessette-Symons B, Cabeza R. Functional neuroimaging studies of aging and emotion: fronto-amygdalar differences during emotional perception and episodic memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(6):819-825. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709990439 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Thomas ML, Martin AS, Eyler L, et al. . Individual differences in level of wisdom are associated with brain activation during a moral decision-making task. Brain Behav. 2019;9(6):e01302. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1302 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Amstadter AB, Moscati A, Oxon MA, Maes HH, Myers JM, Kendler KS. Personality, cognitive/psychological traits and psychiatric resilience. Pers Individ Dif. 2016;91:74-79. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Boardman JD, Blalock CL, Button TM. Sex differences in the heritability of resilience. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2008;11(1):12-27. doi: 10.1375/twin.11.1.12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Zwir I, Del-Val C, Arnedo J, et al. . Three genetic-environmental networks for human personality. Mol Psychiatry. 2019. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0579-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Adler AB, Williams J, McGurk D, Moss A, Bliese PD. Resilience training with soldiers during basic combat training. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2015;7(1):85-107. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Loprinzi CE, Prasad K, Schroeder DR, Sood A. Stress Management and Resilience Training (SMART) program to decrease stress and enhance resilience among breast cancer survivors. Clin Breast Cancer. 2011;11(6):364-368. doi: 10.1016/j.clbc.2011.06.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992;112(1):155-159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Jeste DV, Dunn LB, Folsom DP, Zisook D. Multimedia educational aids for improving consumer knowledge about illness management and treatment decisions. J Psychiatr Res. 2008;42(1):1-21. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Jadad AR, Moore RA, Carroll D, et al. . Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials. Control Clin Trials. 1996;17(1):1-12. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(95)00134-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Higgins JPT, Green S, eds. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0. The Cochrane Collaboration; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629-634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill. Biometrics. 2000;56(2):455-463. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Tang W, He H, Tu XM. Applied Categorical and Count Data Analysis. Chapman & Hall/CRC; 2012. doi: 10.1201/b12123 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Seber G, Lee A. Linear Regression Analysis. 2nd ed John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 2003. doi: 10.1002/9780471722199 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bonvicini KA, Perlin MJ, Bylund CL, Carroll G, Rouse RA, Goldstein MG. Impact of communication training on physician expression of empathy in patient encounters. Patient Educ Couns. 2009;75(1):3-10. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2008.09.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Solantaus T, Paavonen EJ, Toikka S, Punamäki RL. Preventive interventions in families with parental depression. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;19(12):883-892. doi: 10.1007/s00787-010-0135-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.DeSmet A, Bastiaensens S, Van Cleemput K, et al. . The efficacy of the Friendly Attac serious digital game to promote prosocial bystander behavior in cyberbullying among young adolescents. Comput Human Behav. 2018;78:336-347. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Influs M, Pratt M, Masalha S, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. A social neuroscience approach to conflict resolution. Soc Neurosci. 2019;14(4):378-389. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2018.1479983 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Larti N, Ashouri E, Aarabi A. The effect of an empathy role-play program for operating room nursing students. J Educ Eval Health Prof. 2018;15:29. doi: 10.3352/jeehp.2018.15.29 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Romero-Martínez Á, Lila M, Gracia E, Moya-Albiol L. Improving empathy with motivational strategies in batterer intervention programmes. Br J Clin Psychol. 2019;58(2):125-139. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sommers-Spijkerman MPJ, Trompetter HR, Schreurs KMG, Bohlmeijer ET. Compassion-focused therapy as guided self-help for enhancing public mental health. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2018;86(2):101-115. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000268 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Buffel du Vaure C, Lemogne C, Bunge L, et al. . Promoting empathy among medical students. J Psychosom Res. 2017;103:102-107. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Westerhof-Evers HJ, Visser-Keizer AC, Fasotti L, et al. . Effectiveness of a Treatment for Impairments in Social Cognition and Emotion Regulation (T-ScEmo) after traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2017;32(5):296-307. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000332 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Ascone L, Sundag J, Schlier B, Lincoln TM. Feasibility and effects of a brief compassion-focused imagery intervention in psychotic patients with paranoid ideation. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2017;24(2):348-358. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.de Bruin EI, van der Zwan JE, Bögels SM. A RCT comparing daily mindfulness meditations, biofeedback exercises, and daily physical exercise on attention control, executive functioning, mindful awareness, self-compassion, and worrying in stressed young adults. Mindfulness (N Y). 2016;7(5):1182-1192. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0561-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Friis AM, Johnson MH, Cutfield RG, Consedine NS. Kindness matters: a randomized controlled trial of a mindful self-compassion intervention improves depression, distress, and HbA1c among patients with diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2016;39(11):1963-1971. doi: 10.2337/dc16-0416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Galante J, Bekkers MJ, Mitchell C, Gallacher J. Loving-kindness meditation effects on well-being and altruism. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2016;8(3):322-350. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Toole AM, Craighead LW. Brief self-compassion meditation training for body image distress in young adult women. Body Image. 2016;19:104-112. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Flook L, Goldberg SB, Pinger L, Davidson RJ. Promoting prosocial behavior and self-regulatory skills in preschool children through a mindfulness-based Kindness Curriculum. Dev Psychol. 2015;51(1):44-51. doi: 10.1037/a0038256 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Garaigordobil M, Martínez-Valderrey V. Effects of Cyberprogram 2.0 on “face-to-face” bullying, cyberbullying, and empathy. Psicothema. 2015;27(1):45-51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Kelly AC, Carter JC. Self-compassion training for binge eating disorder. Psychol Psychother. 2015;88(3):285-303. doi: 10.1111/papt.12044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Lor KB, Truong JT, Ip EJ, Barnett MJ. A randomized prospective study on outcomes of an empathy intervention among second-year student pharmacists. Am J Pharm Educ. 2015;79(2):18. doi: 10.5688/ajpe79218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Erogul M, Singer G, McIntyre T, Stefanov DG. Abridged mindfulness intervention to support wellness in first-year medical students. Teach Learn Med. 2014;26(4):350-356. doi: 10.1080/10401334.2014.945025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Mantzios M, Wilson JC. Making concrete construals mindful: a novel approach for developing mindfulness and self-compassion to assist weight loss. Psychol Health. 2014;29(4):422-441. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2013.863883 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Smeets E, Neff K, Alberts H, Peters M. Meeting suffering with kindness: effects of a brief self-compassion intervention for female college students. J Clin Psychol. 2014;70(9):794-807. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Yadavaia JE, Hayes SC, Vilardaga R. Using acceptance and commitment therapy to increase self-compassion. J Contextual Behav Sci. 2014;3(4):248-257. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.09.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Castillo R, Salguero JM, Fernández-Berrocal P, Balluerka N. Effects of an emotional intelligence intervention on aggression and empathy among adolescents. J Adolesc. 2013;36(5):883-892. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Neff KD, Germer CK. A pilot study and randomized controlled trial of the mindful self-compassion program. J Clin Psychol. 2013;69(1):28-44. doi: 10.1002/jclp.21923 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Weng HY, Fox AS, Shackman AJ, et al. . Compassion training alters altruism and neural responses to suffering. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(7):1171-1180. doi: 10.1177/0956797612469537 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Riess H, Kelley JM, Bailey RW, Dunn EJ, Phillips M. Empathy training for resident physicians. J Gen Intern Med. 2012;27(10):1280-1286. doi: 10.1007/s11606-012-2063-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Wallmark E, Safarzadeh K, Daukantaitė D, Maddux RE. Promoting altruism through meditation. Mindfulness. 2013;4(3):223-234. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0115-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Houck CD, Barker DH, Hadley W, et al. . The 1-year impact of an emotion regulation intervention on early adolescent health risk behaviors. Health Psychol. 2016;35(9):1036-1045. doi: 10.1037/hea0000360 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Ravindran N, Engle JM, McElwain NL, Kramer L. Fostering parents’ emotion regulation through a sibling-focused experimental intervention. J Fam Psychol. 2015;29(3):458-468. doi: 10.1037/fam0000084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Derella OJ, Johnston OG, Loeber R, Burke JD. CBT-enhanced emotion regulation as a mechanism of improvement for childhood irritability. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2019;48(sup1):S146-S154. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2016.1270832 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Weiss JA, Thomson K, Burnham Riosa P, et al. . A randomized waitlist-controlled trial of cognitive behavior therapy to improve emotion regulation in children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2018;59(11):1180-1191. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12915 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Ma Y, She Z, Siu AF-Y, Zeng X, Liu X. Effectiveness of online mindfulness-based interventions on psychological distress and the mediating role of emotion regulation. Front Psychol. 2018;9:2090. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02090 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Rahmani M, Omidi A, Asemi Z, Akbari H. The effect of dialectical behaviour therapy on binge eating, difficulties in emotion regulation and BMI in overweight patients with binge-eating disorder. Ment Health Prev. 2018;9:13-18. doi: 10.1016/j.mhp.2017.11.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Shahidi S, Akbari H, Zargar F. Effectiveness of mindfulness-based stress reduction on emotion regulation and test anxiety in female high school students. J Educ Health Promot. 2017;6:87. doi: 10.4103/jehp.jehp_98_16 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Spidel A, Lecomte T, Kealy D, Daigneault I. Acceptance and commitment therapy for psychosis and trauma: Improvement in psychiatric symptoms, emotion regulation, and treatment compliance following a brief group intervention. Psychol Psychother. 2018;91(2):248-261. doi: 10.1111/papt.12159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Tornås S, Løvstad M, Solbakk AK, Schanke AK, Stubberud J. Goal management training combined with external cuing as a means to improve emotional regulation, psychological functioning, and quality of life in patients with acquired brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2016;97(11):1841-1852.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2016.06.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Schuppert HM, Timmerman ME, Bloo J, et al. . Emotion regulation training for adolescents with borderline personality disorder traits. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(12):1314-1323.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Berking M, Wupperman P, Reichardt A, Pejic T, Dippel A, Znoj H. Emotion-regulation skills as a treatment target in psychotherapy. Behav Res Ther. 2008;46(11):1230-1237. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Slee N, Spinhoven P, Garnefski N, Arensman E. Emotion regulation as mediator of treatment outcome in therapy for deliberate self-harm. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2008;15(4):205-216. doi: 10.1002/cpp.577 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Rickhi B, Kania-Richmond A, Moritz S, et al. . Evaluation of a spirituality informed e-mental health tool as an intervention for major depressive disorder in adolescents and young adults—a randomized controlled pilot trial. BMC Complement Altern Med. 2015;15:450. doi: 10.1186/s12906-015-0968-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Sollgruber A, Bornemann-Cimenti H, Szilagyi IS, Sandner-Kiesling A. Spirituality in pain medicine: a randomized experiment of pain perception, heart rate and religious spiritual well-being by using a single session meditation methodology. PLoS One. 2018;13(9):e0203336. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0203336 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Chimluang J, Thanasilp S, Akkayagorn L, Upasen R, Pudtong N, Tantitrakul W. Effect of an intervention based on basic Buddhist principles on the spiritual well-being of patients with terminal cancer. Eur J Oncol Nurs. 2017;31:46-51. doi: 10.1016/j.ejon.2017.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Sankhe A, Dalal K, Save D, Sarve P. Evaluation of the effect of spiritual care on patients with generalized anxiety and depression. Psychol Health Med. 2017;22(10):1186-1191. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2017.1290260 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Sajadi M, Niazi N, Khosravi S, Yaghobi A, Rezaei M, Koenig HG. Effect of spiritual counseling on spiritual well-being in Iranian women with cancer. Complement Ther Clin Pract. 2018;30:79-84. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2017.12.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Labelle LE, Lawlor-Savage L, Campbell TS, Faris P, Carlson LE. Does self-report Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) on spirituality and posttraumatic growth in cancer patients? J Posit Psychol. 2015;10(2):153-166. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2014.927902 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Brown J, Hanson JE, Schmotzer B, Webel AR. Spirituality and optimism: a holistic approach to component-based, self-management treatment for HIV. J Relig Health. 2014;53(5):1317-1328. doi: 10.1007/s10943-013-9722-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Breitbart W, Poppito S, Rosenfeld B, et al. . Pilot randomized controlled trial of individual meaning-centered psychotherapy for patients with advanced cancer. J Clin Oncol. 2012;30(12):1304-1309. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2011.36.2517 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Chan CHY, Chan CLW, Ng EHY, et al. . Incorporating spirituality in psychosocial group intervention for women undergoing in vitro fertilization. Psychol Psychother. 2012;85(4):356-373. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.2011.02040.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Chochinov HM, Kristjanson LJ, Breitbart W, et al. . Effect of dignity therapy on distress and end-of-life experience in terminally ill patients. Lancet Oncol. 2011;12(8):753-762. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(11)70153-X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Ando M, Morita T, Akechi T, Okamoto T; Japanese Task Force for Spiritual Care . Efficacy of short-term life-review interviews on the spiritual well-being of terminally ill cancer patients. J Pain Symptom Manage. 2010;39(6):993-1002. doi: 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2009.11.320 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Bay PS, Ivy SS, Terry CL. The effect of spiritual retreat on nurses’ spirituality. Holist Nurs Pract. 2010;24(3):125-133. doi: 10.1097/HNP.0b013e3181dd47dd [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Margolin A, Beitel M, Schuman-Olivier Z, Avants SK. A controlled study of a spirituality-focused intervention for increasing motivation for HIV prevention among drug users. AIDS Educ Prev. 2006;18(4):311-322. doi: 10.1521/aeap.2006.18.4.311 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Richards PS, Berrett ME, Hardman RK, Eggett DL. Comparative efficacy of spirituality, cognitive, and emotional support groups for treating eating disorder inpatients. Eat Disord. 2006;14(5):401-415. doi: 10.1080/10640260600952548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Wachholtz AB, Pargament KI. Is spirituality a critical ingredient of meditation? comparing the effects of spiritual meditation, secular meditation, and relaxation on spiritual, psychological, cardiac, and pain outcomes. J Behav Med. 2005;28(4):369-384. doi: 10.1007/s10865-005-9008-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Grossmann I, Na J, Varnum ME, Park DC, Kitayama S, Nisbett RE. Reasoning about social conflicts improves into old age. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(16):7246-7250. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001715107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Happé FG, Winner E, Brownell H. The getting of wisdom. Dev Psychol. 1998;34(2):358-362. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.2.358 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Mickler C, Staudinger UM. Personal wisdom: validation and age-related differences of a performance measure. Psychol Aging. 2008;23(4):787-799. doi: 10.1037/a0013928 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Van Patten R, Lee EE, Daly R, Twamley E, Tu XM, Jeste DV. Assessment of 3-dimensional wisdom in schizophrenia: associations with neuropsychological functions and physical and mental health. Schizophr Res. 2019;208:360-369. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.01.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Lee EE, Martin AS, Tu X, Palmer BW, Jeste DV. Childhood adversity and schizophrenia: the protective role of resilience in mental and physical health and metabolic markers. J Clin Psychiatry. 2018;79(3):17m11776. doi: 10.4088/JCP.17m11776 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Khan A, Ferrari M. Wisdom of young adults with high functioning autism in Canada and Pakistan: a cross-cultural study. Journal of Education & Social Sciences. 2018;6(1):3-22. doi: 10.20547/jess0611806101 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Birditt KS, Fingerman KL. Do we get better at picking our battles? age group differences in descriptions of behavioral reactions to interpersonal tensions. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2005;60(3):121-128. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.3.P121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Charles ST, Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and emotional memory: the forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2003;132(2):310-324. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Sun B, Luo Z, Zhang W, Li W, Li X. Age-related differences in affective and cognitive empathy. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2018;25(5):655-672. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2017.1360835 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Sze JA, Gyurak A, Goodkind MS, Levenson RW. Greater emotional empathy and prosocial behavior in late life. Emotion. 2012;12(5):1129-1140. doi: 10.1037/a0025011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Tracy LM, Giummarra MJ. Sex differences in empathy for pain. Psychophysiology. 2017;54(10):1549-1558. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12895 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Wu Y, Li H, Zhou Y, et al. . Sex-specific neural circuits of emotion regulation in the centromedial amygdala. Sci Rep. 2016;6:23112. doi: 10.1038/srep23112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Derntl B, Finkelmeyer A, Eickhoff S, et al. . Multidimensional assessment of empathic abilities. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(1):67-82. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Whittle S, Yücel M, Yap MB, Allen NB. Sex differences in the neural correlates of emotion. Biol Psychol. 2011;87(3):319-333. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Wink P, Helson R. Practical and transcendent wisdom: their nature and some longitudinal findings. J Adult Dev. 1997;4(1):1-15. doi: 10.1007/BF02511845 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Perry CL, Komro KA, Jones RM, Munson K, Williams CL, Jason L. The measurement of wisdom and its relationship to adolescent substance use and problem behaviors. J Child Adolesc Subst Abuse. 2002;12(1):45-63. doi: 10.1300/J029v12n01_03 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Scholl L, Seth P, Kariisa M, Wilson N, Baldwin G. Drug and opioid-involved overdose deaths—United States, 2013-2017. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2018;67(5152):1419-1427. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm675152e1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Hedegaard H, Curtin SC, Warner M Suicide rates in the United States continue to increase. Published 2018. Accessed March 31, 2020. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/databriefs/db309.pdf [PubMed]
  • 111.Arias E, Xu JQ United States life tables, 2017. Published 2019. Accessed March 31, 2020. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nvsr/nvsr68/nvsr68_07-508.pdf
  • 112.Rudd RA, Aleshire N, Zibbell J, Gladden RM Increases in drug and opioid overdose deaths—United States, 2000–2014. Published 2016. Accessed March 31, 2020. https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm6450a3.htm?s_cid=mm6450a3_w [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 113.Devitt M. CDC data show U.S. life expectancy continues to decline. Published 2018. Accessed March 31, 2020. https://www.aafp.org/news/health-of-the-public/20181210lifeexpectdrop.html
  • 114.CDC Suicide rising across the US:. Published 2019. Accessed March 31, 2020. https://www.cdc.gov/vitalsigns/suicide/index.html
  • 115.Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC. Social isolation and health, with an emphasis on underlying mechanisms. Perspect Biol Med. 2003;46(3)(suppl):S39-S52. doi: 10.1353/pbm.2003.0049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Jeste DV, Lee EE, Cacioppo S. Battling the modern behavioral epidemic of loneliness. JAMA Psychiatry. Published online March 4, 2020. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.0027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Daniels LR, Boehnlein J, McCallion P. Aging, depression, and wisdom: a pilot study of life-review intervention and PTSD treatment with two groups of Vietnam veterans. J Gerontol Soc Work. 2015;58(4):420-436. doi: 10.1080/01634372.2015.1013657 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Treichler EBH, Glorioso D, Lee EE, et al. . A pragmatic trial of a group intervention in senior housing communities to increase resilience. Int Psychogeriatr. 2020;32(2):173-182. doi: 10.1017/S1041610219002096 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Klimecki OM, Leiberg S, Lamm C, Singer T. Functional neural plasticity and associated changes in positive affect after compassion training. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23(7):1552-1561. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplement.

eTable 1. Descriptive Characteristics of Individual Wisdom Component Intervention Trials (N=57)

eTable 2. Risk of Bias

eTable 3. Outcome Measures used in the selected studies

eReferences.

eFigure 1. Funnel plot of Interventions for Pro-social Behaviors

eFigure 2. Funnel Plot of Interventions for Emotional Regulation

eFigure 3. Funnel Plot of Interventions for Spirituality


Articles from JAMA Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of American Medical Association

RESOURCES