Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychiatry logoLink to Frontiers in Psychiatry
. 2020 May 12;11:401. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00401

Exploring Relationships Between Autism Spectrum Disorder Symptoms and Eating Disorder Symptoms in Adults With Anorexia Nervosa: A Network Approach

Jess Kerr-Gaffney 1, Daniel Halls 1, Amy Harrison 2,3, Kate Tchanturia 1,3,4,*
PMCID: PMC7235355  PMID: 32477185

Abstract

Over the past few decades, research has accumulated to suggest a relationship between anorexia nervosa (AN) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Elevated ASD traits are present in around one third of those with AN, and there is some evidence to suggest that ASD traits are associated with more severe eating disorder (ED) psychopathology. The current study aimed to examine relationships between ED and ASD symptoms in individuals with a lifetime history of AN using network analysis. One hundred and one participants completed the ED Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q) and the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS-2). A regularized partial correlation network was estimated using a graphical least absolute shrinkage and selection operator. Expected influence (EI) and bridge EI values were calculated to identify central and bridge symptoms respectively. Isolation, difficulties with relating to others, and feelings of tension during social situations were most central to the network, while poor self-confidence, concerns over eating around others, and concerns over others seeing one's body were the strongest bridge symptoms. Our findings confirm that interpersonal problems are central to ED psychopathology. They also suggest poor self-confidence and social anxiety-type worries may mediate the relationship between ED and ASD symptoms in those with a lifetime diagnosis of AN. Longitudinal studies examining fluctuations in symptoms over time may be helpful in understanding direction of causality.

Keywords: anorexia nervosa, comorbidity, autism spectrum disorder, self-report, social behavior

Introduction

Over the past few decades, evidence suggesting a relationship between autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and anorexia nervosa (AN) has accumulated (1, 2). ASD is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by difficulties in social communication and interaction, as well as restrictive, repetitive patterns of behavior or interests (3). ASD is a lifelong condition, and is more commonly diagnosed in males than females (4). On the other hand, AN is a severe eating disorder (ED) associated with persistent restriction of energy intake, fear of weight gain, and disturbances in the way in which one's body shape or weight is experienced (3). AN is more prevalent in females, and peak age of onset is in late adolescence (5, 6).

Despite the apparent differences between the two disorders, empirical research has shown a number of similarities in the phenotypic expressions of AN and ASD. For example, in the socio-emotional domain, considerable research has documented difficulties in emotion recognition (7), empathy (8), and theory of mind (ToM) (9) in individuals with ASD. These difficulties are also seen in those with AN, although are often less pronounced than is seen in ASD (1012). Furthermore, high levels of alexithymia (13, 14), social anxiety (15, 16), and differences in social attention (1719) are associated with both disorders. In the neurocognitive domain, both AN and ASD are associated with weak central coherence (20, 21), increased attention to detail (22, 23), and difficulties in set-shifting (24, 25), an executive function that allows for flexible thinking and behavior.

As well as these similarities in socio-emotional and neurocognitive profiles, those with AN show high levels of ASD traits. For example, it is reported that between 4% and 52.5% of individuals with AN score above clinical cut-offs on diagnostic assessment tools for ASD1. It has been suggested that high levels of ASD traits found in a proportion of those with AN may be due to the effects of starvation, and do not represent true ASD (26). However, several studies have found that body mass index (BMI), which is often used as a measure of illness severity, is not associated with ASD traits in individuals with AN (2733). A few of these studies also examined associations with illness duration, finding that those with high ASD traits had not been ill for a significantly longer period of time than those with low ASD traits (29, 33). Finally, a significant proportion of individuals recovered from AN also show elevated ASD traits compared to HCs (27, 30, 34). Therefore, it seems the association between ASD and AN is not a product of starvation, yet the exact nature of the relationship remains unclear.

However, there is some evidence to suggest that ASD symptoms are positively associated with severity of ED psychopathology in those with AN. For example, in a large sample of inpatients with AN, Tchanturia et al. (31) reported that scores on the Autism Quotient (AQ) (35) were positively associated with scores on the ED Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q) (36). A similar association between AQ scores and ED symptoms has been reported in nonclinical populations (37, 38). Further, the presence of ASD traits in AN is associated with more frequent and longer inpatient stays (29), less improvement during treatment (39, 40), and poorer outcomes (4143). Why might a more severe ED presentation be associated with high ASD traits? One possibility is that some of the neurocognitive traits associated with ASD, such as cognitive rigidity, increased attention to detail, and sensitivity to order may perpetuate a narrow focus on food and weight in individuals with AN and make change difficult (44). Indeed, Westwood and colleagues (45) reported that individuals with AN and high ASD traits showed higher levels of cognitive rigidity and set-shifting difficulties than individuals with low ASD traits. As well as a significant proportion of individuals with AN showing high levels of ASD traits on dimensional measures, a number of studies have found that 8%–29% meet full diagnostic criteria for ASD (34, 41, 4649). Given that social difficulties are an important predictor of poor outcomes in AN (43, 5052), another possibility is that those with comorbid ASD and AN have particularly poor outcomes due to the social communication difficulties associated with ASD. Yet another possibility is that avoidance of certain foods due to sensory sensitivities in ASD may reinforce food restriction. Such hypotheses remain to be tested empirically.

A potentially useful method for examining the nature of the relationship between AN and ASD symptoms is provided by network theory. Network theories of psychopathology represent psychiatric disorders as constellations of symptoms, activating one another (53). The relationships between symptoms are key to the development and maintenance of psychopathology; symptoms can form feedback loops, eventually producing a set of symptoms that are recognized as a psychiatric disorder. This theory has important implications for understanding comorbidity. Symptoms are often shared among different psychiatric disorders, for example feelings of guilt are common in obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) and are also a central feature of major depression (3). Because symptoms in a network have causal relationships with one another, clusters of symptoms belonging to one disorder can activate those of another disorder, resulting in diagnostic comorbidity (54).

Psychological networks can be estimated using network analysis. Networks are made up of nodes (symptoms) and edges (relationships between symptoms). It is possible to calculate which nodes or symptoms have most connections in the network (node centrality) and therefore are most important in maintaining psychopathology. Further, it is possible to calculate which symptoms of a given disorder are most connected to symptoms in another disorder cluster (bridge nodes), and therefore may maintain comorbidity. Currently, only a few studies have examined comorbidity using network analysis in individuals with EDs. These studies have most often focussed on comorbidity between anxiety and ED symptoms, finding that avoidance of social eating is an important bridge symptom (5557). Others have examined depression (55, 58) and OCD symptoms (59), however no study to date has focussed on ASD and ED symptom comorbidity.

The aim of the current study was to examine relationships between ED and ASD symptoms in individuals with AN using network analysis. Because ASD symptoms have been shown to persist in individuals recovered from AN, suggesting independence from clinical state, our sample included those with a current or past diagnosis of AN. We aimed to identify central nodes in order to understand which symptoms may be most important in maintaining the symptom network as a whole. Bridge nodes were also identified in order to detect symptoms most important in explaining potential comorbidity of AN and ASD.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The study was cross-sectional. Ethical approval was obtained from the National Health Service (NHS) Research Ethics Committee (Camberwell St Giles, 17/LO/1960). Participants provided their written informed consent to participate in the study. Participants with a lifetime history of AN were recruited from two specialist NHS ED services in London, online advertisements, and through the King's College London university research recruitment email. Participants were required to be between 18 and 55 years old and fluent in English. Exclusion criteria were a history of brain trauma or learning disability. A past or current diagnosis of AN was confirmed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Disorders, research version (SCID-5-RV) Module I “Feeding and Eating Disorders” (60).

Procedure and Materials

Participants attended a testing session as part of a wider study on socio-emotional processing at the Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, however where participants were inpatients, testing took place at their place of treatment. Written consent was obtained, and the following measures administered:

The EDE-Q (36) was used to measure severity of ED psychopathology. Twenty-two of the 28 items are rated for frequency during the past 28 days on a seven-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating more eating, shape, or weight concerns and behaviors. The remaining six items assessing frequency of various behaviors are not included in total or subscale score calculations, as these items can take on any value. The EDE-Q demonstrates good psychometric properties, correlating with measures of similar constructs (61). Cronbach's alpha was 0.91.

The Social Responsiveness Scale-2nd edition, adult self-report form (SRS-2) (62) is a 65-item questionnaire assessing symptoms associated with ASD, with higher scores indicating more autistic symptoms. There are five subscales: social awareness (ability to recognize social cues), social cognition (interpreting social behavior), social communication (reciprocal communication in social situations), social motivation (motivation to participate in social interactions), and restrictive interests and repetitive behavior (circumscribed interests and stereotypy). Respondents indicate their agreement with each item on a four-point Likert scale, rating their behavior over the past six months. The SRS-2 has been used extensively in ASD research, and is also recommended for use in diagnostic assessments in adults with ASD (63). Validation studies have found measurement invariance across the sexes, and few sex, age, or rater effects (6466). Scores on the SRS-2 have been shown to predict whether individuals with AN score above the clinical cut-off on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd edition (ADOS-2) (67), a “gold-standard” clinical interview measure for ASD (68). Cronbach's alpha was 0.96.

Demographic information was also collected, along with weight and height measurements to calculate BMI (height/weight2).

Network Analysis

Analyses were performed in R version 3.6.1 (69). R codes are provided in the Supplementary Information.

Item Selection

Network analysis assumes that each node in the network represents a distinct construct. Given that some of the questionnaire items are very similar in content, the goldbricker function in R package networktools (70) was used to select items to include in the network. Goldbricker compares dependent overlapping correlations (i.e., items with high multicollinearity) for all items in the network. If a certain proportion of correlations between node A and all other nodes do not significantly differ from those between node B and all other nodes (e.g., items share ≥75% of correlations), nodes A and B are assumed to be overlapping items measuring the same construct (“bad pairs”). One of the nodes is subsequently removed. The 22 Likert items from the EDE-Q and all 65 items from the SRS-2 were entered. After dropping the bad pairs, 18 EDE-Q and 55 SRS-2 items were left for inclusion in the network. The full list of EDE-Q and SRS-2 items and those included in the network are provided in the Supplementary Information.

Network Estimation and Accuracy

A regularized partial correlation network with weighted edges was estimated using a graphical least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO) using the qgraph R package (71). This method limits the total sum of absolute parameter values and drops edges that are close to zero out of the model, keeping only those that are most robust and likely to represent genuine associations. The tuning parameter (λ) was set to 0.25. This value is typically set between 0 and 0.5, with higher values resulting in simpler models with fewer edges, and lower values favoring discovery but more likely to estimate spurious edges (72).

Accuracy of edge-weights were assessed using nonparametric bootstrapping using the bootnet package (73). Bootstrapping involves repeatedly estimating a model under sampled or simulated data and estimating the statistic of interest, in this case, edges. The bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals (CIs) indicate the sampling variation, and the strength of a given edge is difficult to interpret if bootstrapped CIs are wide. Correlation stability (CS) coefficients were calculated to assess the stability of expected influence (EI) and bridge EI. In this case, a case-dropping bootstrap is used to indicate whether the centrality indices remain the same after reestimating the network using only a subset of cases from the sample. The CS coefficient indicates the proportion of the sample that can be dropped to retain a correlation >0.7 with the original sample. It should not be below 0.25, and preferably above 0.5 (73). Finally, bootstrapped difference tests (a = 0.05) were run to test for significant differences in centrality indices between nodes.

Network Interpretation

Central nodes were identified by calculating EI using the networktools package. EI is similar to the more commonly used metric, strength, as it is calculated by summing all of the edges a given node has with all other nodes in a network. However unlike strength, which does not distinguish between positive and negative edges, EI accounts for the direction of associations. This is an important distinction in networks which include psychopathological symptoms of more than one disorder, where some negative relationships are likely (74).

Bridge nodes were identified by calculating bridge EI using the networktools package. Both bridge EI one-step (bridge EI1) and bridge EI two-step (bridge EI2) were calculated. Bridge EI1 identifies the strength and directionality of the relationships a node in one cluster has with all nodes of another cluster. Bridge EI2 additionally takes into account the secondary influence of a node via the influences of its immediate neighbors. For centrality and bridge indices, higher values represent greater influence. Z-scores are reported throughout for ease of interpretation.

Results

Sample Characteristics

In total, 101 participants took part in the study. Fifty-one were acutely ill with AN, while fifty were recovered. Demographic and clinical information is displayed in Table 1. On the SRS-2, 43% of participants scored within the “normal” range, scores within this range are not associated with clinically significant symptoms. Seventeen percent of participants scored within the “mild” range, indicating deficiencies in reciprocal social behavior that are clinically significant and may lead to mild to moderate interference with daily living. A further 21% scored within the “moderate” range, indicating clinically significant difficulties which lead to substantial interference with social behavior. Finally, 19% of participants scored in the “severe” range, indicating severe and enduring difficulties with social behavior. Scores within the moderate and severe range are typical for individuals with a diagnosis of ASD.

Table 1.

Demographic and clinical information of participants with lifetime anorexia nervosa (AN) (N = 101).

Mean (SD) Range
Age (years) 26.95 (8.27) 18.16 – 54.59
% female 95.0
BMI 18.39 (3.25) 12.90 – 27.00
Years of education 16.33 (2.89) 10.00 – 27.00
Illness length (years) 6.47 (6.89) 0.50 – 35.00
% on psychiatric medication 43.6
EDE-Q total 2.84 (1.75) 0.00 – 5.69
SRS-2 total 77.66 (33.11) 17.00 – 160.00

AN, anorexia nervosa; BMI, body mass index; EDE-Q, eating disorder examination questionnaire; SRS-2, social responsiveness scale; SD, standard deviation.

Network Estimation and Accuracy

Questionnaire data from three participants contained missing values (representing 0.08% of the total questionnaire data). Given that nodes in our network did not rely on subscale or total score calculations from questionnaires, the rest of the data from these participants was included in analyses. The network structure composed of EDE-Q and SRS-2 symptom scores is displayed in Figure 1. Green edges represent positive relationships, while red indicates negative ones. The thicker the edge, the stronger the regularized partial correlation.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Graphical least absolute shrinkage and selection operator network. Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q) items: ED1, limit food; ED2, fasting; ED3, excluding foods; ED4, eating rules; ED8, concentration affected by shape/weight; ED9, fear of loss of control; ED10, fear of weight gain; ED12, desire to lose weight; ED19, eating in secret; ED20, guilt over eating; ED21, concern over other people seeing you eat; ED22, weight overvaluation; ED23, shape overvaluation; ED24, reaction to weighing; ED25, weight dissatisfaction; ED26, shape dissatisfaction; ED27, uncomfortable seeing own body; ED28, uncomfortable over others seeing own body. Social responsiveness scale (SRS-2) items: SRS1, uncomfortable in social situations; SRS2, facial expressions; SRS4, rigid behavior; SRS6, prefer to be alone; SRS7, aware of others feelings; SRS8, strange behavior; SRS9, dependent on others; SRS10, take things literally; SRS11, good self-confidence; SRS12, communicate feelings; SRS13, awkward in turn taking interactions; SRS14, not well coordinated; SRS15, understand change in tone/facial expression; SRS16, avoid/unusual eye contact; SRS17, recognize unfairness; SRS18, difficulty making friends; SRS19, frustrated in conversations; SRS20, sensory interests; SRS21, imitate others'; SRS22, interact appropriately; SR23, avoid social events; SRS25, don't mind being out of step with others; SR26, offer comfort to others; SRS27, avoid starting social interactions; SRS28, think about the same thing over and over; SRS29, regarded as odd; SRS30, upset in situations with lots going on; SRS31, can't get mind off something; SRS32, good personal hygiene; SRS33, socially awkward; SRS34, avoid people who want to be emotionally close to me; SRS35, have trouble keeping up with conversations; SRS36, difficulty relating to family; SRS37, difficulty relating to adults outside family; SRS38, respond to others' moods; SRS39, interested in too few topics; SRS40, imaginative; SRS41, wander aimlessly between activities; SRS42, sensory sensitivity; SRS43, enjoy small talk; SRS45, interested in what others' are attending to; SRS49, do well at intellectual tasks; SRS50, repetitive behaviors; SRS51, difficulty answering questions directly; SRS52, overly loud; SRS53, monotone voice; SRS54, thing about people and objects in the same way; SRS55, invade others' personal space; SRS56, walk between two people; SRS57, isolate myself; SRS59, suspicious; SRS61, inflexible; SRS63, unusual greeting; SRS64, tense in social settings; SRS65, stare into space.

Plots displaying the bootstrapped CIs of estimated edge-weights, bootstrapped centrality indices, and bootstrapped differences tests are reported in the Supplementary Material. The EI CS coefficient was 0.67, and the bridge EI CS coefficient was 0.59, indicating both EI and bridge EI can be interpreted meaningfully (73).

Centrality

EI is plotted in Figure 2. The items with the highest EI were SRS-2 37 “I have difficulty relating to adults outside of my family,” SRS-2 57 “I tend to isolate myself,” and SRS-64 “I am much more tense in social situations than when I am by myself.”

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Centrality plot depicting expected influence (EI) of each node. Values are given as Z scores, higher values represent greater centrality in the network.

Bridge Nodes

Bridge EI values are plotted in Figure 3. For bridge EI1, the strongest ED bridge symptom was EDE-Q 21 “How concerned have you been about other people seeing you eat?”, and the strongest ASD bridge symptom was SRS-11 “I have good self-confidence” (reverse coded). For bridge EI2, the strongest ED bridge symptom was EDE-Q 28 “How uncomfortable have you felt about others seeing your shape or figure”, and the strongest ASD bridge symptom was again SRS-11 “I have good self-confidence.”

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Bridge expected influence (EI) plots. Values are given as Z scores, and higher values indicate more influential nodes in the network.

Discussion

The current study is the first to examine relationships between ED and ASD symptoms in individuals with past or current AN using network analysis. Constructing a network of partial correlations allowed us to examine connections between symptoms, independent of the effects of other symptoms in the network. Firstly, we aimed to identify core symptoms in the network. The three nodes with the highest centrality in the network were all SRS-2 items: SRS-2 37 “I have difficulty relating to adults outside of my family,” SRS-2 57 “I tend to isolate myself”, and SRS-64 “I am much more tense in social situations than when I am by myself.” The former two items are from the social communication subscale of the SRS-2, while the latter is from the social motivation subscale. These results suggest that difficulties in social communication and isolation may be core symptoms in AN psychopathology, over and above conventional ED symptoms, such as weight and shape concern. However, it must be noted that the inclusion of recovered individuals, some of whom had rather low EDE-Q symptom scores may have influenced these results. Had our sample only included individuals in the acute stage of AN, EDE-Q items might have been more central to the network. Nonetheless, our study is not the first to demonstrate the importance of social difficulties in AN psychopathology. For example, Monteleone and colleagues (58) found that depression and personal alienation were the nodes with highest centrality in their network of symptoms in children and adolescents with AN. Personal alienation, a subscale of the EDs Inventory (EDI), reflects a sense of emotional emptiness, aloneness, and feeling separated from others. Somewhat similar findings were reported by Somli and colleagues (75), who found that in adolescents and adults with AN, depression, anxiety, interpersonal sensitivity, and ineffectiveness were most central to the network. Interpersonal sensitivity, a subscale of the Symptom Checklist 90 (SCL-90), assesses feelings of inadequacy and inferiority in comparison to others, as well as self-consciousness and discomfort during social interactions.

Our second aim was to identify bridge nodes; those that connect ED and ASD symptom clusters. The strongest ASD bridge symptom was SRS-2 11 “I have good self-confidence” (reverse coded), while the strongest ED bridge symptoms were EDE-Q 21 “How concerned have you been about other people seeing you eat?” (bridge EI1) and EDE-Q 28 “How uncomfortable have you felt about others seeing your shape or figure?” (bridge EI2). The self-confidence item belongs to the social motivation subscale of the SRS-2. Our results suggest that a lack of self-confidence may be important in understanding the link between ED psychopathology and ASD symptoms in those with lifetime AN. However, it must be noted that low self-confidence is a rather nonspecific psychiatric symptom, commonly reported in depression, anxiety, substance abuse disorders, and EDs (76). Interestingly, our finding is very similar to that of Forrest and colleagues (56), who found that the low self-confidence item of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) was the strongest trait anxiety bridge node linked to ED symptoms in a mixed ED sample. It could be that elevated scores on ASD assessments found in individuals with AN are partly due to high anxiety, a symptom shared by both disorders. In an analysis of 18 previously published comorbidity networks, Jones and colleagues (54) observed that several symptoms emerged as bridge symptoms across multiple networks. The networks included several different disorders, including anxiety, bulimia nervosa (BN), OCD, depression, and ASD, and also used a wide variety of symptom scales. This demonstrates that certain symptoms may not only explain comorbidity between two disorders, but may be important transdiagnostic factors across psychiatric disorders.

Regarding ED bridge nodes, it is notable that both involve concern over being observed by others. The concern over others seeing you eat item has repeatedly been shown to be the strongest ED bridge node connecting ED and anxiety symptoms in those with AN (55) and mixed ED groups (56, 57). High social anxiety is a feature of both ASD (16, 77) and AN (15), and our findings might suggest that social anxiety worries are important in explaining comorbidity between AN and ASD. It has been hypothesized that some of the core symptoms of ASD may increase the risk of developing social anxiety (78, 79). For example, poor social skills or difficulties in recognizing emotions and mental states in others may lead to rejection from peers and isolation during formative years, factors which are implicated in the development of social anxiety disorder (SAD). There is also evidence to suggest that SAD may be a risk factor for the development of AN. In those with both disorders, SAD precedes AN onset in around two thirds of cases (80, 81). Fears around eating in front of others may lead to avoidance of social eating in those with SAD, a potential pathway by which other eating disordered behaviors may form. Qualitative work has also provided some insight into how these factors may interact and contribute to the development of AN. For example, Kinnaird et al. (82) found that participants with AN and ASD felt that their ED had developed as a way of dealing with the social confusion and difficulties relating to other people associated with their ASD. Participants also described how social difficulties, such as dealing with noise and social chat during meal times, made inpatient treatment difficult. Similarly, based on their qualitative study of AN and ASD comorbidity, Brede and colleagues (83) hypothesized that AN may develop through both direct and indirect pathways. In the indirect pathway, ASD-related difficulties are proposed to give rise to negative emotional consequences, and restrictive eating behaviors are employed as an attempt to cope with this. For example, individuals with ASD may have a longstanding history of being bullied and socially ostracized, resulting in low self-esteem and emotional distress. Restricting food intake can provide a sense of control and numbing of strong emotions. Given our study was cross-sectional, hypotheses about the direction of causality between symptoms are preliminary. Future longitudinal research using network analysis could help disentangle interactions between ASD, ED, and social anxiety symptoms.

Our findings have important theoretical and clinical implications. That the symptoms most central to the network all concerned social difficulties provides support for models emphasizing the role of interpersonal problems in the development and maintenance of AN. For example, the cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model of AN proposes that anxious, avoidant, and socio-emotional traits, including sensitivity to stress and negative emotions, anxious and avoidant attachment, and negative self-evaluations are predisposing factors (44). While some interpersonal difficulties are worsened by the ill state, this group of traits are proposed to be present before and after the illness, and often also in family members. Further, our results support the need for therapies to target interpersonal functioning as a key maintaining factor in individuals with AN, such as interpersonal psychotherapy (84), cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) (85), and the Maudsley model of AN treatment for adults (MANTRA) (86). Secondly, it is of note that none of the items from the restrictive interests and repetitive behavior subscale of the SRS-2 had particularly high EI or bridge EI values. This may suggest that unlike social and communication difficulties, this group of symptoms play a relatively small part in AN and ASD comorbidity. Indeed, previous studies have shown that while social and communication difficulties are often elevated in those with AN, restricted and repetitive behaviors are often less pronounced (33, 49). This might be in part due to AN samples being mostly female, a factor which is associated with lower levels of restricted interests in those with ASD (87). However, there is some evidence to suggest that males and females with ASD show differences in the types of topics they are interested in (e.g., people/animals in females rather than objects/things in males), therefore ASD symptom rating scales may not be sensitive to more female-typical presentations (88) Finally, our study examined relationships between symptoms on a group basis, however an interesting direction for future research would be to construct networks based on an individual basis. It is now possible to examine temporal associations between symptoms within individuals, potentially providing insight into which symptoms may be maintaining psychopathology and therefore could be targeted during treatment (89). Thus, network analysis could be a useful tool in the move toward more personalized treatments in psychiatry.

Several limitations of the current study should be noted. Our sample size was relatively small given the number of items included in the network, therefore the findings require replications in larger samples. Nonetheless, our stability analyses indicated the centrality indices were stable enough to be interpreted meaningfully. Secondly, only items from self-report questionnaires were considered as nodes in the network. It is likely that vulnerability factors not measured in this study are also important in explaining comorbidity between AN and ASD. For example, given the similarities in neuropsychological profiles, performance on set-shifting or other tests of executive functioning could be included as nodes in comorbidity networks. Although other aspects of psychopathology and social cognition were collected as part of our wider study, these were not included as we wanted to focus on ASD and AN comorbidity specifically, and adding more nodes to the network may have resulted in reductions in the stability and accuracy of the network. Finally, although we confirmed a past or current diagnosis of AN in our sample, we did not confirm whether participants held a diagnosis of ASD. Although scores on the SRS-2 suggested high levels of ASD traits in our sample, there may be qualitative differences in relations between symptoms between individuals with lifetime AN who do and do not have a formal diagnosis of ASD. Previous research suggests around 10% of those with AN meet full diagnostic criteria, and a further 40% display high ASD traits (49). Future studies using a network analytic approach may be useful in establishing which symptoms reflect “true” ASD, and which may be a consequence of starvation.

In conclusion, our results suggest that isolation, difficulties in relating to others, and feelings of tension during social situations may be central symptoms maintaining AN and ASD psychopathology. These symptoms are most strongly connected to other symptoms in the network, and it is suggested that targeting these symptoms in treatment may lead to improvements in the mental health of individuals with past or current AN who also show ASD traits. It must be noted that while central symptoms may be causally influential, longitudinal studies are required to confirm the directionality of relationships between symptoms. We also identified bridge nodes from each disorder cluster; those with the strongest connections to symptoms to the other symptom cluster. Poor self-confidence (ASD cluster), concern around social eating (ED cluster), and concern over other's seeing one's body were (ED cluster) were the strongest bridge symptoms. These symptoms may be important in understanding AN and ASD comorbidity.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by NHS Camberwell St Giles Research Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JK-G contributed to the conception and design of the study. JK-G and DH performed the statistical analysis. JK-G wrote the manuscript. DH, AH, and KT contributed to manuscript revision, read and approved the submitted version. KT lead the research group under which the study took place.

Funding

JK-G is supported by a doctoral studentship from the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC), and received research funding from the Psychiatry Research Trust. AH is funded by the Medical Research Council (MRC) (MR/S020381/1). KT would like to acknowledge: MRC-MRF fund (MR/R004595/1); the Health Foundation, an independent charity committed to bring better health care for people in the UK (1115447); and the Maudsley Charity for their support.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the participants who took part in the research for their time.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00401/full#supplementary-material

References

  • 1. Westwood H, Tchanturia K. Autism Spectrum Disorder in Anorexia Nervosa: An Updated Literature Review. Curr Psychiatry Rep (2017) 19(7):41.   10.1007/s11920-017-0791-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Zucker NL, Losh M, Bulik CM, LaBar KS, Piven J, Pelphrey K. Anorexia Nervosa and Autism Spectrum Disorders: Guided Investigation of Social Cognitive Endophenotypes. Psychol Bull (2007) 133(6):976–1006.   10.1037/0033-2909.133.6.976 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th Ed. Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing; (2013). [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Fombonne E. Epidemiology of Pervasive Developmental Disorders. Pediatr Res (2009) 65(6):591–8.   10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819e7203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Hoek HW. Incidence, Prevalence and Mortality of Anorexia Nervosa and Other Eating Disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2006) 19(4):389–94.   10.1097/01.yco.0000228759.95237.78 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Herpertz-Dahlmann B, van Elburg A, Castro-Fornieles J, Schmidt U. ESCAP Expert Paper: New Developments in the Diagnosis and Treatment of Adolescent Anorexia Nervosa - a European Perspective. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2015) 24(10):1153–67.   10.1007/s00787-015-0748-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Harms MB, Martin A, Wallace GL. Facial Emotion Recognition in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Review of Behavioral and Neuroimaging Studies. Neuropsychol Rev (2010) 20(3):290–322.   10.1007/s11065-010-9138-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Harmsen IE. Empathy in Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord (2019) 49(10):3939–55.   10.1007/s10803-019-04087-w [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Peñuelas-Calvo I, Sareen A, Sevilla-Llewellyn-Jones J, Fernández-Berrocal P. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test in Autism-Spectrum Disorders Comparison with Healthy Controls: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J Autism Dev Disord (2019) 49(3):1048–61.   10.1007/s10803-018-3814-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Bora E, Kose S. Meta-Analysis of Theory of Mind in Anorexia Nervosa and Bulimia Nervosa: A Specific Impairment of Cognitive Perspective Taking in Anorexia Nervosa? Int J Eat Disord (2016) 49(8):739–40.   10.1002/eat.22572 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Kerr-Gaffney J, Harrison A, Tchanturia K. Cognitive and Affective Empathy in Eating Disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front Psychiatry (2019) 10:102.   10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Leppanen J, Sedgewick F, Treasure J, Tchanturia K. Differences in the Theory of Mind Profiles of Patients with Anorexia Nervosa and Individuals on the Autism Spectrum: A Meta-Analytic Review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2018) 90:146–63.   10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Kinnaird E, Stewart C, Tchanturia K. Investigating Alexithymia in Autism: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Eur Psychiatry (2019) 55:80–9.   10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.09.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Westwood H, Kerr-Gaffney J, Stahl D, Tchanturia K. Alexithymia in Eating Disorders: Systematic Review and Meta-Analyses of Studies Using the Toronto Alexithymia Scale. J Psychosom Res (2017) 99:66–81.   10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.06.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Kerr-Gaffney J, Harrison A, Tchanturia K. Social Anxiety in the Eating Disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Psychol Med (2018) 48(15):2477–91.   10.1017/S0033291718000752 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Spain D, Sin J, Linder KB, McMahon J, Happé F. Social Anxiety in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic Review. Res Autism Spectr Disord (2018) 52:51–68.   10.1016/j.rasd.2018.04.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Chita-Tegmark M. Social Attention in ASD: A Review and Meta-Analysis of Eye-Tracking Studies. Res Dev Disabil (2016) 48:79–93.   10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Harrison A, Watterson SV, Bennett SD. An Experimental Investigation into the Use of Eye-Contact in Social Interactions in Women in the Acute and Recovered Stages of Anorexia Nervosa. Int J Eat Disord (2019) 52(1):61–70.   10.1002/eat.22993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Watson KK, Werling DM, Zucker NL, Platt ML. Altered Social Reward and Attention in Anorexia Nervosa. Front Psychol (2010) 1:36.   10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Frith U, Happé F. Autism: Beyond “Theory of Mind”. Cognition (1994) 50(1–3):115–32.   10.1016/0010-0277(94)90024-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Lang K, Lopez C, Stahl D, Tchanturia K, Treasure J. Central Coherence in Eating Disorders: An Updated Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. World J Biol Psychiatry (2014) 15(8):586–98.   10.3109/15622975.2014.909606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Jolliffe T, Baron-Cohen S. Are People with Autism and Asperger Syndrome Faster than Normal on the Embedded Figures Test? J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1997) 38(5):527–34.   10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01539.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Roberts ME, Tchanturia K, Treasure JL. Is Attention to Detail a Similarly Strong Candidate Endophenotype for Anorexia Nervosa and Bulimia Nervosa? World J Biol Psychiatry (2013) 14(6):452–63.   10.3109/15622975.2011.639804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. D'Cruz A, Ragozzino ME, Mosconi MW, Shrestha S, Cook EH, Sweeney JA. Reduced Behavioral Flexibility in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Neuropsychology (2013) 27(2):152–60.   10.1037/a0031721 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Westwood H, Stahl D, Mandy W, Tchanturia K. The Set-Shifting Profiles of Anorexia Nervosa and Autism Spectrum Disorder Using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Psychol Med (2016) 46:1809–27.   10.1017/S0033291716000581 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Hiller R, Pellicano L. Anorexia and Autism – a Cautionary Note. Psychologist (2013) 26(11):780. [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Bentz M, Jepsen JRM, Pedersen T, Bulik CM, Pedersen L, Pagsberg AK, et al. Impairment of Social Function in Young Females with Recent-Onset Anorexia Nervosa and Recovered Individuals. J Adolesc Heal (2017) 60(1):23–32.   10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.08.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Calderoni S, Fantozzi P, Balboni G, Pagni V, Franzoni E, Apicella F, et al. The Impact of Internalizing Symptoms on Autistic Traits in Adolescents with Restrictive Anorexia Nervosa. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat (2015) 11:75–85.   10.2147/NDT.S73235 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Nazar BP, Peynenburg V, Rhind C, Hibbs R, Schmidt U, Gowers S, et al. An Examination of the Clinical Outcomes of Adolescents and Young Adults with Broad Autism Spectrum Traits and Autism Spectrum Disorder and Anorexia Nervosa: A Multi Centre Study. Int J Eat Disord (2018) 51(2):174–9.   10.1002/eat.22823 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Sedgewick F, Kerr-Gaffney J, Leppanen J, Tchanturia K. Anorexia Nervosa, Autism, and the ADOS: How Appropriate Is the New Algorithm in Identifying Cases? Front. Psychiatry (2019) 10:507.   10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00507 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Tchanturia K, Adamson J, Leppanen J, Westwood H. Characteristics of Autism Spectrum Disorder in Anorexia Nervosa: A Naturalistic Study in an Inpatient Treatment Programme. Autism (2019) 23(1):123–30.   10.1177/1362361317722431 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Vagni D, Moscone D, Travaglione S, Cotugno A. Using the Ritvo Autism Asperger Diagnostic Scale-Revised (RAADS-R) Disentangle the Heterogeneity of Autistic Traits in an Italian Eating Disorder Population. Res Autism Spectr Disord (2016) 32:143–55.   10.1016/j.rasd.2016.10.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Westwood H, Mandy W, Tchanturia K. Clinical Evaluation of Autistic Symptoms in Women with Anorexia Nervosa. Mol Autism (2017) 8:12.   10.1186/s13229-017-0128-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Dinkler L, Rydberg Dobrescu S, Råstam M, Gillberg IC, Gillberg C, Wentz E, et al. Visual Scanning during Emotion Recognition in Long-Term Recovered Anorexia Nervosa: An Eye-Tracking Study. Int J Eat Disord (2019) 52(6):691–700.   10.1002/eat.23066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/High-Functioning Autism, Males and Females, Scientists and Mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord (2001) 31(1):5–17.   10.1023/A:1005653411471 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Fairburn CG, Beglin SJ. Assessment of Eating Disorders: Interview or Self-Report Questionnaire? Int J Eat Disord (1994) 16(4):363–70.   [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Coombs E, Brosnan M, Bryant-Waugh R, Skevington SM. An Investigation into the Relationship between Eating Disorder Psychopathology and Autistic Symptomatology in a Non-Clinical Sample. Br J Clin Psychol (2011) 50(3):326–38.   10.1348/014466510X524408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Carton AM, Smith AD. Assessing the Relationship between Eating Disorder Psychopathology and Autistic Traits in a Non-Clinical Adult Population. Eat Weight Disord (2014) 19(3):285–93.   10.1007/s40519-013-0086-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Stewart CS, McEwen FS, Konstantellou A, Eisler I, Simic M. Impact of ASD Traits on Treatment Outcomes of Eating Disorders in Girls. Eur Eat Disord Rev (2017) 25(2):123–8.   10.1002/erv.2497 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Tchanturia K, Larsson E, Adamson J. How Anorexia Nervosa Patients with High and Low Autistic Traits Respond to Group Cognitive Remediation Therapy. BMC Psychiatry (2016) 16(1):334.   10.1186/s12888-016-1044-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Anckarsäter H, Hofvander B, Billstedt E, Gillberg IC, Gillberg C, Wentz E, et al. The Sociocommunicative Deficit Subgroup in Anorexia Nervosa: Autism Spectrum Disorders and Neurocognition in a Community-Based, Longitudinal Study. Psychol Med (2012) 42(9):1957–67.   10.1017/S0033291711002881 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Nielsen S, Anckarsäter H, Gillberg C, Gillberg C, Råstam M, Wentz E. Effects of Autism Spectrum Disorders on Outcome in Teenage-Onset Anorexia Nervosa Evaluated by the Morgan-Russell Outcome Assessment Schedule: A Controlled Community-Based Study. Mol Autism (2015) 6(1):14.   10.1186/s13229-015-0013-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Wentz E, Gillberg IC, Anckarsäter H, Gillberg C, Råstam M. Adolescent-Onset Anorexia Nervosa: 18-Year Outcome. Br J Psychiatry (2009) 194(2):168–74.   10.1192/bjp.bp.107.048686 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Treasure J, Schmidt U. The Cognitive-Interpersonal Maintenance Model of Anorexia Nervosa Revisited: A Summary of the Evidence for Cognitive, Socio-Emotional and Interpersonal Predisposing and Perpetuating Factors. J Eat Disord (2013) 1:13.   10.1186/2050-2974-1-13 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Westwood H, Mandy W, Tchanturia K. The Association between Symptoms of Autism and Neuropsychological Performance in Females with Anorexia Nervosa. Psychiatry Res (2017) 258:531–7.   10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Gillberg C, Råstam M. Do Some Cases of Anorexia Nervosa Reflect Underlying Autistic-like Conditions? Behav Neurol (1992) 5(1):27–32.   10.3233/BEN-1992-5105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Gillberg IC, Råstam M, Gillberg C. Anorexia Nervosa Outcome: Six-Year Controlled Longitudinal Study of 51 Cases Including a Population Cohort. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (1994) 33(5):729–39.   10.1097/00004583-199406000-00014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Råstam M, Gillberg C, Wentz E. Outcome of Teenage-Onset Anorexia Nervosa in a Swedish Community-Based Sample. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2003) 12:78–90.   10.1007/s00787-003-1111-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Westwood H, Mandy W, Simic M, Tchanturia K. Assessing ASD in Adolescent Females with Anorexia Nervosa Using Clinical and Developmental Measures: A Preliminary Investigation. J Abnorm Child Psychol (2018) 46(1):183–92.   10.1007/s10802-017-0301-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Deter HC, Schellberg D, Köpp W, Friederich HC, Herzog W. Predictability of a Favorable Outcome in Anorexia Nervosa. Eur Psychiatry (2005) 20(2):165–72.   10.1016/j.eurpsy.2004.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Zipfel S, Löwe B, Reas DL, Deter H-C, Herzog W. Long-Term Prognosis in Anorexia Nervosa: Lessons from a 21-Year Follow-up Study. Lancet (2000) 355((9205)):721–2.   10.1016/S0140-6736(99)05363-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Franko DL, Keshaviah A, Eddy KT, Krishna M, Davis MC, Keel PK, et al. A Longitudinal Investigation of Mortality in Anorexia Nervosa and Bulimia Nervosa. Am J Psychiatry (2013) 170(8):917–25.   10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12070868 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Borsboom D. A Network Theory of Mental Disorders. World Psychiatry (2017) 16((1)):5–13.   10.1002/wps.20375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Jones PJ, Ma R, McNally RJ. Bridge Centrality: A Network Approach to Understanding Comorbidity. Multivariate Behav Res (2019).   10.1080/00273171.2019.1614898. Advance online publication. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 55. Elliott H, Jones PJ, Schmidt U. Central Symptoms Predict Posttreatment Outcomes and Clinical Impairment in Anorexia Nervosa: A Network Analysis. Clin Psychol Sci (2019) 8(1):139–154.   10.1177/2167702619865958. Advance online publication. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Forrest LN, Sarfan LD, Ortiz SN, Brown TA, Smith AR. Bridging Eating Disorder Symptoms and Trait Anxiety in Patients with Eating Disorders: A Network Approach. Int J Eat Disord (2019) 52(6):701–11.   10.1002/eat.23070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Levinson CA, Brosof LC, Vanzhula I, Christian C, Jones P, Rodebaugh TL, et al. Social Anxiety and Eating Disorder Comorbidity and Underlying Vulnerabilities: Using Network Analysis to Conceptualize Comorbidity. Int J Eat Disord (2018) 51(7):693–709.   10.1002/eat.22890 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Monteleone AM, Mereu A, Cascino G, Criscuolo M, Castiglioni MC, Pellegrino F, et al. Re-conceptualization of Anorexia Nervosa Psychopathology: A Network Analysis Study in Adolescents with Short Duration of the Illness. Int J Eat Disord (2019) 52(11):1263–73.   10.1002/eat.23137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Meier M, Kossakowski JJ, Jones PJ, Kay B, Riemann BC, McNally RJ. Obsessive–Compulsive Symptoms in Eating Disorders: A Network Investigation. Int J Eat Disord (2019) 53(3):362–71.   10.1002/eat.23196 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. First MB, Williams JB, Karg RS, Spitzer RL. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Disorders, Research Version. Arlington: American Psychiatric Association; (2015). [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Berg KC, Peterson CB, Frazier P, Crow SJ. Psychometric Evaluation of the Eating Disorder Examination and Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Int J Eat Disord (2012) 45(3):428–38.   10.1002/eat.20931 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Constantino JN, Gruber CP. Social Responsiveness Scale-Second Edition (SRS-2). Torrance: Western Psychological Services; (2012). [Google Scholar]
  • 63. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) Autism: Recognition, Referral, Diagnosis and Management of Adults on the Autism Spectrum (CG142). London: NICE; (2012). [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Frazier TW, Ratliff KR, Gruber C, Zhang Y, Law PA, Constantino JN. Confirmatory Factor Analytic Structure and Measurement Invariance of Quantitative Autistic Traits Measured by the Social Responsiveness Scale-2. Autism (2014) 18(1):31–44.   10.1177/1362361313500382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Chan W, Smith LE, Hong J, Greenberg JS, Mailick MR. Validating the Social Responsiveness Scale for Adults with Autism. Autism Res (2017) 10(10):1663–71.   10.1002/aur.1813 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Takei R, Matsuo J, Takahashi H, Uchiyama T, Kunugi H, Kamio Y. Verification of the Utility of the Social Responsiveness Scale for Adults in Non-Clinical and Clinical Adult Populations in Japan. BMC Psychiatry (2014) 14(1):302.   10.1186/s12888-014-0302-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Lord C, Rutter M, Dilavore P, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2) Modules 1-4. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; (2012). [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Kerr-Gaffney J, Harrison A, Tchanturia K. The Social Responsiveness Scale Is an Efficient Screening Tool for Autism Spectrum Disorder Traits in Adults with Anorexia Nervosa. Eur Eat Disord Rev (2020).   10.1002/erv.2736. Advance online publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 69. R Core Team R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: (2019) [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Jones P. (2019). Networktools: Tools for Identifying Important Nodes in Networks. [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Epskamp S, Cramer AOJ, Waldorp LJ, Schmittmann VD, Borsboom D. Qgraph: Network Visualizations of Relationships in Psychometric Data. J Stat Software (2012) 48(4).   10.18637/jss.v048.i04 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Foygel R, Drton M. Extended Bayesian Information Criteria for Gaussian Graphical Models. Adv Neural Inf Process Syst (2010). 23:2020–8 [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Epskamp S, Borsboom D, Fried EI. Estimating Psychological Networks and Their Accuracy: A Tutorial Paper. Behav Res Methods (2018) 50(1):195–212.   10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Robinaugh DJ, Millner AJ, McNally RJ. Identifying Highly Influential Nodes in the Complicated Grief Network. J Abnorm Psychol (2016) 125(6):747–57.   10.1037/abn0000181 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Solmi M, Collantoni E, Meneguzzo P, Tenconi E, Favaro A. Network Analysis of Specific Psychopathology and Psychiatric Symptoms in Patients with Anorexia Nervosa. Eur Eat Disord Rev (2019) 27(1):24–33.   10.1002/erv.2633 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Silverstone PH, Salsali M. Low Self-Esteem and Psychiatric Patients: Part I – The Relationship between Low Self-Esteem and Psychiatric Diagnosis. Ann Gen Hosp Psychiatry (2003) 2(1):2.   10.1186/1475-2832-2-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Maddox BB, White SW. Comorbid Social Anxiety Disorder in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord (2015) 45(12):3949–60.   10.1007/s10803-015-2531-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Halim AT, Richdale AL, Uljarević M. Exploring the Nature of Anxiety in Young Adults on the Autism Spectrum: A Qualitative Study. Res Autism Spectr Disord (2018) 55:25–37.   10.1016/j.rasd.2018.07.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Spain D, Happé F, Johnston P, Campbell M, Sin J, Daly E, et al. Social Anxiety in Adult Males with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord (2016) 32:13–23.   10.1016/j.rasd.2016.08.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Godart NT, Flament MF, Lecrubier Y, Jeammet P. Anxiety Disorders in Anorexia Nervosa and Bulimia Nervosa: Co-Morbidity and Chronology of Appearance. Eur Psychiatry (2000) 15(1):38–45.   10.1016/S0924-9338(00)00212-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Carrot B, Radon L, Hubert T, Vibert S, Duclos J, Curt F, et al. Are Lifetime Affective Disorders Predictive of Long-Term Outcome in Severe Adolescent Anorexia Nervosa? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2017) 26:969–78.   10.1007/s00787-017-0963-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Kinnaird E, Norton C, Stewart C, Tchanturia K. Same Behaviours, Different Reasons: What Do Patients with Co-Occurring Anorexia and Autism Want from Treatment? Int Rev Psychiatry (2019) 31(4):308–17.   10.1080/09540261.2018.1531831 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Brede J, Babb C, Jones C, Elliott M, Zanker C, Tchanturia K, et al. “For Me, the Anorexia Is Just a Symptom, and the Cause Is the Autism”: Investigating Restrictive Eating Disorders in Autistic Women. J Autism Dev Disord (2020).   10.1007/s10803-020-04479-3. Advance online publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 84. Murphy R, Straebler S, Basden S, Cooper Z, Fairburn CG. Interpersonal Psychotherapy for Eating Disorders. Clin Psychol Psychother (2012) 19(2):150–8.   10.1002/cpp.1780 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Shafran R. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy for Eating Disorders: A “Transdiagnostic” Theory and Treatment. Behav Res Ther (2003) 41:509–28.   10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00088-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Schmidt U, Wade TD, Treasure J. The Maudsley Model of Anorexia Nervosa Treatment for Adults (MANTRA): Development, Key Features, and Preliminary Evidence. J Cogn Psychother (2014) 28(1):48–71.   10.1891/0889-8391.28.1.48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Frazier TW, Georgiades S, Bishop SL, Hardan AY. Behavioral and Cognitive Characteristics of Females and Males with Autism in the Simons Simplex Collection. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2014) 53(3):329–340.e3.   10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Auyeung B, Chakrabarti B, Baron-Cohen S. Sex/Gender Differences and Autism: Setting the Scene for Future Research. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2015) 54(1):11–24.   10.1016/j.jaac.2014.10.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Epskamp S, van Borkulo CD, van der Veen DC, Servaas MN, Isvoranu AM, Riese H, et al. Personalized Network Modeling in Psychopathology: The Importance of Contemporaneous and Temporal Connections. Clin Psychol Sci (2018) 6(3):416–27.   10.1177/2167702617744325 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.


Articles from Frontiers in Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES