Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Jul 6;9:e53850. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53850

Consistent patterns of distractor effects during decision making

Bolton KH Chau 1,2,, Chun-Kit Law 1, Alizée Lopez-Persem 3,4, Miriam C Klein-Flügge 3, Matthew FS Rushworth 3
Editors: Thorsten Kahnt5, Joshua I Gold6
PMCID: PMC7371422  PMID: 32628109

Abstract

The value of a third potential option or distractor can alter the way in which decisions are made between two other options. Two hypotheses have received empirical support: that a high value distractor improves the accuracy with which decisions between two other options are made and that it impairs accuracy. Recently, however, it has been argued that neither observation is replicable. Inspired by neuroimaging data showing that high value distractors have different impacts on prefrontal and parietal regions, we designed a dual route decision-making model that mimics the neural signals of these regions. Here we show in the dual route model and empirical data that both enhancement and impairment effects are robust phenomena but predominate in different parts of the decision space defined by the options’ and the distractor’s values. However, beyond these constraints, both effects co-exist under similar conditions. Moreover, both effects are robust and observable in six experiments.

Research organism: Human

Introduction

Independence of irrelevant alternatives is one of the assumptions of decision theory and behavioural economics: optimally, decisions between two options should be made in the same way regardless of whether or not a third option – a distractor – is also present. In practice, however, several lines of evidence suggest distractors impact on the neural mechanisms underlying choice representation and decision making and, as a result, subtly but significantly, alter the choices people and animals take (Chau et al., 2014; Louie et al., 2015; Louie et al., 2011; Louie et al., 2013; Louie et al., 2014; Noonan et al., 2017; Noonan et al., 2010).

Two forms of distractor effects have recently received attention. First, it has been reported that the relative accuracy of decisions made between two choosable options – the frequency with which the better option is chosen -- decreases as the value of a third distractor increases. This has been interpreted as a consequence of divisive normalisation – the representation of any option’s value is normalised by the sum of the values of all options present including distractors (Chau et al., 2014; Louie et al., 2015; Louie et al., 2011; Louie et al., 2013; Louie et al., 2014; Noonan et al., 2017; Noonan et al., 2010). The argument is bolstered by the observation that in many sensory systems, neural codes are adaptive and the rate of neural activity recorded in response to a given stimulus is related to the range of other stimuli encountered in the same context. For example, in the context of a bright light stimulus that leads to a high rate of neural responding in the visual system, the neural responses to two dimmer stimuli will be more similar to one another than they would otherwise be (Carandini and Heeger, 2012). As a result, discerning the brighter of the two dim stimuli becomes more difficult. The contention is that a similar normalisation process occurs during value-guided decision making when a high value distractor is present.

However, it has also been found that in some circumstances the presence of a high value distractor can have a positive effect and lead to an increase in accuracy when deciding between two choosable options (Chau et al., 2014). This finding was explained by reference to a cortical attractor model in which decisions are made when choice representations in a neural network occupy a high firing attractor state (Wang, 2002; Wang, 2008; Wong and Wang, 2006). Competition between representations of choices in the network is mediated by a pool of inhibitory interneurons. In turn the activity levels of the inhibitory interneurons are driven by pools of recurrently interconnected excitatory neurons that each represents a possible choice. Increasing activity in the inhibitory interneuron pool mediating the comparison process improves decision accuracy although it may also slow decisions (Jocham et al., 2012). Because higher value distractors lead to more activity in the inhibitory interneuron pool, decisions between the choosable options are more accurate in the presence of high value distractors.

Although the explanation was framed in terms of the cortical attractor model, any model in which comparison mechanisms interact makes similar predictions. For example, if decisions are modelled by a drift diffusion process, then a similar prediction is made if the diffusion process proceeds for longer on trials when there is strong evidence for choosing the distractor instead. This is because initiating a response towards the distractor and then inhibiting it takes a finite amount of time. This allows additional time for the comparison between the choosable options to proceed concurrently and, hence, the better of the two choosable options is more likely to be taken. Other task manipulations that allow more time for the comparison process, for example by simply providing an opportunity for a second decision, after there has been time for more evidence accumulation, also make the ‘correct’ choice more likely to emerge from the comparison process and be chosen (Resulaj et al., 2009; van den Berg et al., 2016).

Gluth et al., 2018, however, have recently reported a series of experiments in which they claim there is no evidence of either divisive normalisation or positive distractor effects. Here, we therefore review the evidence for divisive normalisation and positive distractor effects. First, we explain how divisive normalisation and positive distractor effects are not ‘opposing results’ as is sometimes claimed (Gluth et al., 2018). We explain how it is possible for both effects to co-exist within the same data set but to predominate in different parts of the decision space. Second, we re-analyse Gluth et al., 2018 data and show that both divisive normalisation and positive distractor effects are robustly and consistently present. Similarly re-analysis of other previously published data (Chau et al., 2014) again confirms both effects are present. In addition we report a new data set, collected at a third site, which again exhibits both effects.

Third, we investigate further the nature of the positive distractor and divisive normalisation effects by examining their manifestation in decisions in which participants make choices between options that lead to losses rather than gains. We find that the impact of larger distractor values flips from being facilitative (positive distractor effect) for gains to being disruptive (negative distractor effect) for losses while the divisive normalisation effect continues to manifest in the same direction (negative distractor effect) as originally shown for gains. This pattern of results suggests that divisive normalisation effects are truly related to the value of the distractor while the positive distractor effect is related to both the distractor’s value and salience (its unsigned value – the size of its value regardless of whether it is positive or negative).

Finally, we consider a third consequence of the presence of a distractor; sometimes people choose the distractor itself even if this runs contrary to task instructions. Such choices have been termed attentional capture effects (Gluth et al., 2018). Again we show that such attentional capture effects exist in other data sets. However, we also show that the existence of attentional capture effects is not mutually exclusive with either positive distractor or divisive normalisation effects. In fact, whether or not attentional capture by the distractor occurs is itself subject to similar positive distractor and divisive normalisation effects. In a final experiment we use eye tracking data to demonstrate that in fact a relationship exists between the attentional capture effect and the positive distractor effect; positive distractor effects are particularly prominent after attentional capture by the distractor. This makes it possible to link the positive distractor effect to other situations in which the provision of a greater opportunity for evidence accumulation and comparison leads to more accurate decision making, for example when allowing participants extra time to revise their initial decisions (Resulaj et al., 2009; van den Berg et al., 2016).

Results

Divisive normalisation of value and positive distractor effects should predominate in different parts of the decision space

In order to evaluate the evidence for divisive normalisation and positive distractor effects, it is necessary to realise that reports of each are not ‘opposing results’ (Gluth et al., 2018) that are mutually incompatible. Instead, quite the converse is the case: both effects can theoretically co-exist and do so in practice. This is because the impacts of divisive normalisation and of the positive distractor effect are more likely to be seen in different parts of the ‘decision space’ defined by the values of the higher value (HV) and lower value (LV) choosable options.

It is equally important to realise that small changes in the organisation of a neural network for making decisions could result in different distractor effects. To demonstrate this, first, we established a mutual inhibition model by simplifying a biophysically plausible model reported elsewhere that exhibits a positive distractor effect (Chau et al., 2014). In brief, it involves three pools of excitatory neurons that receive noisy input from the HV, LV and distractor (D) options that compete via a common pool of inhibitory neurons (Figure 1a). To visualise the impact of the distractor, we plotted in Figure 1b the model’s choice accuracy as a function of relative distractor value (i.e. D-HV) and choice difficulty (HV-LV; as HV-LV becomes smaller it means that HV and LV have increasingly similar values and it is harder and harder to select the better option during the decision). When the distractor value is relatively large (left-to-right), the model makes more accurate choices (brighter colors). A similar trend is observed on both hard (Figure 1c, bottom) and easy trials (Figure 1c, top). In addition, we applied a simple general linear model (GLM) to analyse the simulated choice accuracy data (GLM1a). This GLM involves regressors that describe the difficulty (the difference between HV and LV: HV-LV), the relative value of the distractor (D-HV), and as well as an interaction term (HV-LV)(D-HV) that tests whether the distractor effect is modulated as a function of difficulty. Consistent with the pattern in Figure 1b, the results show that the model exhibits a positive D-HV effect (β = 0.296, t104 = 231.858, p<10−142; Figure 1d). There was also a positive HV-LV effect (β = 0.697, t104 = 663.224, p<10−189) and a positive (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect (β = 0.094, t104 = 83.377, p<10−96).

Figure 1. Distractor effects predicted by mutual inhibition model, divisive normalisation model, and a dual route model that combines both other models.

(a) A mutual inhibition model involves three pools of excitatory neurons that receive excitatory input from the HV, LV or D options (PHV, PLV and PD). Concurrently, all excitatory neurons further excite a common pool of inhibitory neurons PInh, which in turn inhibit all excitatory neurons to the same extent. The HV or LV option is chosen once its accumulated evidence (yHV or yLV respectively) reaches a decision threshold. (b) The decision accuracy of the model is plotted across a decision space defined by the difficulty level (i.e. value difference between HV and LV) and the relative distractor value (D–HV). The model predicts a positive distractor effect – the decision accuracy increases (brighter colors) as a function of relative distractor value (left-to-right side). (c) A positive distractor effect is found on both hard (bottom) and easy (top) trials. (d) A GLM analysis shows that the model exhibits a positive HV-LV effect, a positive D-HV effect and a positive (HV-LV)(D–HV) effect. (e) Alternatively, a divisive normalisation model involves only two pools of excitatory neurons that receive input from either the HV or LV option. The input of each option is normalised by the value sum of all options (i.e. HV+LV+D), such that the distractor influences the model’s evidence accumulation at the input level. (f) Unlike the mutual inhibition model, the divisive normalisation model predicts that larger distractor values (left-to-right side) will have a negative effect (darker colours) on decision accuracy. (g) A negative distractor effect is found on both hard (bottom) and easy (top) trials. (h) A GLM analysis shows that the model exhibits a positive HV-LV effect, a negative D-HV effect, and a negative (HV-LV)(D–HV) effect. (i) A dual route model involves evidence accumulation via mutual inhibition and divisive normalisation components independently. A choice is made by the model when one of the components accumulates evidence that reaches the decision threshold. (j) The current model predicts that on hard trials (bottom) larger distractor values (left-to-right side) will have a positive effect (brighter colors) on decision accuracy. In contrast, on easy trials (top) larger distractor values will have a negative effect (the colors change from white to yellow from left to right). (k) The opposite distractor effects are particularly obvious when focusing on the hardest (bottom) and easiest (top) trials. (l) A GLM analysis shows that the model exhibits a positive HV-LV effect, a positive D-HV effect and a negative (HV-LV)(D–HV) effect.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. In the dual route model the positive D-HV effect on hard trials is mainly contributed by the mutual inhibition component and the negative D-HV effect on easy trials is mainly contributed by the divisive normalisation component.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(a,b) Replica of Figure 1j and k showing choice accuracy predicted by the dual route model as a function of relative distractor value D-HV and difficulty level. (c) The choices made by the mutual inhibition (Mutual) component and divisive normalisation (DivNorm) components are analysed separately. On hard trials (bottom), accuracy of choices made by the Mutual component (blue lines) increases as a function of D-HV. A similar trend is observed on easy trials (top), but with a smaller slope. In contrast, accuracy of choices made the DivNorm component (red lines) decreases as a function of D-HV in both hard and easy trials and the negative slope is steeper on easy trials. (d) There are overall four possible types of choices made by the dual route model – accurate and error choices made by the Mutual component and accurate and error choices made by the DivNorm component. (Bottom) On hard trials, in choices made by the Mutual component the proportion of errors (light blue bars) decreases much more rapidly than the accurate choices (dark blue bars) as a function of D-HV. In choices made by the DivNorm component, the proportions of accurate (red) and error (pink) choices made by the divisive normalisation model increase to a similar extent. Thus, there is an overall net increase in accuracy on hard trials as a function of D-HV. (Top) In contrast, on easy trials errors made by the Mutual component are rare and there is little variability as a function of D-HV. However, errors made by the divisive normalisation component increase as a function of D-HV. Thus, there is an overall net decrease in accuracy on easy trials as a function D-HV. The green lines show the proportion of overall accurate choices, which are identical to the lines on panel b.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Reaction time (RT) of choices made by the dual route model.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(a) RT of the mutual inhibition component when the divisive normalisation component is switched off. (b) RT of the divisive normalisation component when the mutual inhibition component is switched off. Although the RT of the mutual inhibition component is generally faster than that of the divisive normalisation component, due to variability in the RTs of both components, choices are sometimes made by the divisive normalisation component (see Figure 1—figure supplement 1d).

It is possible to slightly adapt the mutual inhibition model to produce a divisive normalisation effect. In the divisive normalisation model, there are only two pools of excitatory neurons that are related to the HV and LV options (Figure 1e). The HV and LV inputs are normalised by the value sum of all options (HV+LV+D). Hence, instead of ‘competing’ directly with the HV and LV options, the distractor influences the model’s evidence accumulation at the input level. In Figure 1f and g, the model shows poorer accuracy when D was relatively larger. When applying GLM1a, the results show that the model exhibits a negative D-HV effect (β = −0.277, t104 = −229.713, p<10−141; Figure 1h). There were also a positive HV-LV effect (β = 0.694, t104 = 598.596, p<10−185) and a negative (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect (β = −0.094, t104 = −71.658, p<10−89).

Although the mutual inhibition and divisive normalisation models produce opposite distractor effects, the effects can co-exist with each predominating in different parts of decision space. To demonstrate this, we designed a dual route model, which is inspired by the fact that multiple brain structures have been identified with decision making. It is likely that they compete to select choices with one system predominating in some situations and another system in other situations. For example, in many situations, the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) carries decision-related signals (Glimcher, 2002; Gold and Shadlen, 2007; Hanks et al., 2006; O'Shea et al., 2007; Platt and Glimcher, 1999; Shadlen and Kiani, 2013; Shadlen and Newsome, 1996) and unlike decision-related signals elsewhere in the brain they remain present even when there is limited time in which to act (Jocham et al., 2014) or when decisions have become easy because of over-training (Grol et al., 2006). Moreover, divisive normalisation is present in activity recorded in IPS (Chau et al., 2014; Louie et al., 2011; Louie et al., 2014).

Another region with activity that is similarly decision-related is ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) (Boorman et al., 2009; Chau et al., 2014; De Martino et al., 2013; FitzGerald et al., 2009; Hunt et al., 2012; Lopez-Persem et al., 2016; Noonan et al., 2010; Papageorgiou et al., 2017; Wunderlich et al., 2012). However, in contrast to IPS, the impact of divisive normalisation in vmPFC is less prominent or absent (Chau et al., 2014), vmPFC activity diminishes with task practice (Hunt et al., 2012), and it is only engaged when more time is available to make the decision (Jocham et al., 2014). VmPFC lesions particularly disrupt the most difficult decisions but have less impact on easier ones (Noonan et al., 2010). The fact that lesions of vmPFC increase the impact of divisive normalisation in decision making (Noonan et al., 2017; Noonan et al., 2010) suggests divisive normalisation effects are mediated by a different region of the brain, such as IPS, and are perhaps even mitigated by the operation of vmPFC.

Based on these observations from vmPFC and IPS, the dual route model comprises both mutual inhibition and divisive normalisation models as components (Figure 1i). The evidence is accumulated independently in the two component models. A choice is made once one of the component models has accumulated sufficient evidence. Interestingly, when the model’s decision accuracy is plotted in Figure 1j, the pattern looks very similar to the empirical data produced by human participants reported in Figure 2c of Chau et al., 2014. On hard trials (Figure 1k, bottom) the decision accuracy increases as a function of the relative distractor value, whereas on easy trials (Figure 1k, top) the decision accuracy decreases as a function of the relative distractor value. This pattern should be best captured in GLM1a by the (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction term, as it reflects how the distractor effect changes as a function of the trial difficulty level (i.e. HV-LV). The results of GLM1a indeed revealed a significant negative (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction effect on decision accuracy (β = −0.047, t104 = −32.195, p<10−55). In addition, the distractor effect is slightly biphasic on hard trials and even more so on trials with intermediate difficulty levels – the accuracy increases from low to medium D-HV values and then decreases from medium to large D-HV values. This is due to the partial effects from the divisive normalisation route and mutual inhibition route. The biphasic pattern of the distractor effect is also reported in an alternative model (Li et al., 2018). Finally, there was a positive HV-LV effect (β = 0.728, t104 = 526.591, p<10−179) and an absence of D-HV main effect (β <0.001, t104 = 0.218, p=0.828; Figure 1i).

In the dual route model positive and negative distractor effects predominate in different parts of the decision space. It is possible to understand the underlying reasons by analysing the choices made by the mutual inhibition and divisive normalisation components separately (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). On hard trials, when the distractor value becomes larger, the errors made by the mutual inhibition component decrease more rapidly in frequency than the increase in errors made by the divisive normalisation component, resulting in a net positive distractor effect. In contrast, on easy trials when the distractor value becomes larger the decrease in errors made by the mutual inhibition model is much less than the increase in errors made by the divisive normalisation model. Figure 1—figure supplement 2 shows the reaction time of choices made by each component when the other component is switched off.

Both divisive normalisation of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites

These predictions are borne out by the available data from several laboratories using a multi-attribute decision making task (Figure 2; Materials and methods: multi-attribute decision-making task). First, as in Figure 2c of Chau et al., 2014, we visualised how the relative accuracy of decisions between HV and LV varied as a function of the HV-LV difference and the relative value of a distractor. Chau and colleagues manipulated distractor value with respect to HV because part of their investigation was concerned with comparing the HV-LV and HV-D signals present in neuroimaging data. However, the HV-D term has a negative relationship with the value of the distractor itself: D. This makes it less intuitive for understanding how the distractor value, D, influences choices. Here we present relative distractor value using the more intuitive D-HV term. Larger values of this term are correlated with larger values of the distractor, D.

Figure 2. Behavioural task in Experiments 1–7.

Figure 2.

(a) The behavioural task was first described by Chau et al., 2014 as follows. In the initial phase of two-option trials participants saw two stimuli indicating two choices. These were immediately surrounded by orange squares, indicating that either might be chosen. A subsequent color change in one box indicated which choice the participant took. In the outcome phase of the trial the outline color of the chosen stimulus indicated whether the reward had been won. The final reward allocated to the participant on leaving the experiment was calculated by averaging the outcome of all trials. Distractor trials unfolded in a similar way but, in the decision phase, one stimulus, the distractor, was surrounded by a purple square to indicate that it could not be chosen while the presentation of orange squares around the other options indicated that they were available to choose. (b) Prior to task performance participants learned that stimulus orientation and color indicated the probability and magnitude of rewards if the stimulus was chosen.

© 2014 Springer Nature

Figure 2 is reproduced from Chau et al., 2014, Nature Neuroscience, by permission of Springer Nature (copyright, 2014). This figure is not covered by the CC-BY 4.0 licence and further reproduction of this panel would need permission from the copyright holder.

Figure 3a and c show the data from the fMRI experiment (Experiment 1 fMRI2014; n = 21) reported by Chau et al., 2014 and Gluth et al., 2018 experiment 4 (Experiment 2 Gluth4; n = 44) respectively. It is important to consider these two experiments first because they employ an identical schedule. Specifically, Chau and colleagues reported both divisive normalisation effects and positive distractor effects, while Gluth and colleagues claimed they were unable to replicate these effects in their own data and when they analysed this data set from Chau and colleagues. Here we found that both data sets show a positive D-HV distractor effect. In both data sets, when decisions are difficult (HV-LV is small) then high value D-HV is associated with higher relative accuracy in choices between HV and LV; for example, the bottom rows of Figure 3a and c turn from black/dark red to yellow moving from left to right, indicating decisions are more accurate. However, when decisions were easy (HV-LV is large) then the effect is much less prominent or even reverses as would be predicted if divisive normalisation becomes more important in this part of the decision space. As in the predictions of the dual route model (Figure 1j,k), on easy trials although there was an overall decreasing trend in accuracy as a function of D-HV, there was an increasing trend at very low HV-LV levels. Overall, a combination of positive and negative D-HV effects on hard and easy trials respectively suggests that there should be a negative (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction effect on choice accuracy.

Figure 3. Decision accuracy across the decision space.

Accuracy (light-yellow indicates high accuracy, dark-red indicates low accuracy) is plotted across the decision space defined by decision difficulty (HV-LV) and relative distractor value (D–HV) from (a) Experiment 1 fMRI2014, (c) Experiment 2 Gluth4, (e) Experiment 3 Hong Kong. In the case of each experiment, GLM analysis indicates that similar factors influence accuracy. The difference in value between the better and worse choosable option (HV-LV) is a major influence on accurately choosing the better option HV. However, accurate choice of HV is also more likely when the distractor is high in value (D-HV is high) and this effect is more apparent when the decision is difficult (negative interaction of (HV-LV)(D–HV)) in the data from (b) Experiment 1 fMRI2014, (d) Experiment 2 Gluth4, (f) Experiment 3 Hong Kong. (g) A model comparison shows that participants’ behaviour in Experiments 1 to 3 is best described by the dual route model, as opposed to the null, mutual inhibition, or divisive normalisation models. (h) Posterior probability of each model in accounting for the behaviour of individual participants. Null: null model; Mutual: mutual inhibition model; DivNorm: divisive normalisation model; Dual: dual route model. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. (a–f) Error bars indicate standard error. (g–h) Error bars indicate standard deviation.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. A similar (HV-LV)(D–HV) effect was observed when the HV+LV term was added to GLM1a which was done in GLM1b.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

A negative (HV-LV)(D–HV) interaction effect was found in (a) Experiment 1, (b) Experiment two and (c) Experiment 3. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. The dual route model is better than the mutual inhibition, divisive normalisation and null models in predicting participants’ accuracy and reaction time.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(a) A plot showing the choice accuracy of participants of Experiments 1–3 as a function of HV-LV and D-HV. In other words, it shows the averaged data of Figure 3a,c,e. (b) The fitted parameters were applied back to the corresponding models to predict choices in the exact same trials that the participants performed. The predictions were repeated for 1000 iterations and the absolute deviation of the predictions from the empirical data was calculated (i.e. absolute difference between models’ and participants’ accuracy). The results show that overall the dual route model (top-left) is better than (darker colors) the mutual inhibition (top-right), divisive normalisation (bottom-left) and null (bottom-right) models in predicting participants’ accuracy. The same procedure was then repeated to test the models’ prediction of reaction time (RT). (c) Participants’ reaction times were slower (yellow colors) on trials that were harder (bottom rows) and with larger distractor values (right columns). (d) As in (b), the absolute deviation in RT between models’ predictions and participants’ behaviour is plotted. The dual route model (top-left) is better than (darker colors) the mutual inhibition (top-right), divisive normalisation (bottom-left) and null (bottom-right) models in predicting participants’ RT.
Figure 3—figure supplement 3. The dual route model provides the best account of participants’ behaviour regardless of the parameterisation of non-decision time Tnd and inhibition level f.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

(a) Replica of Figure 3g and h. In this set of models, Tnd is fixed at 0.3 s and f is fixed at 0.5. (b) In this set of models, Tnd is fixed at 0.3 s and f is a free parameter with a prior at 0.5. In the dual route model, the f of each route is fitted independently. (c) In this set of models, Tnd is a free parameter with a prior at 0.3 s and f is also a free parameter with a prior at 0.3. In all three versions of model set, the dual route model provide the best fit of participants’ behaviour. (d) All twelve models in a–c) are compared in a single analysis. The dual route model with fixed Tnd and f provides the best account of participant behaviour (Ef = 0.413, Xp = 1.000). Null: null model; Mutual: mutual inhibition model; DivNorm: divisive normalisation model; Dual: dual route model. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

When the behavioural data from human participants are analysed with the same GLM (GLM1a) that was used to analyse model data, the results are consistent with the illustrations in Figure 1j and k. In Experiment 1 fMRI2014, the results showed that the critical (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect was negative (β = −0.243, t20 = −3.608, p=0.002; Figure 3b). Just as in the dual route model, the negative (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction term suggested that that the D-HV effect was particularly positive on hard trials where HV-LV was small. There was also a positive main effect of HV-LV (β = 0.738, t20 = 8.339, p<10−7) and no main effect of D-HV (β = 0.046, t20 = 0.701, p=0.491). Similarly in Experiment 2 Gluth4, there was a negative (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect (β = −0.068, t43 = −2.043, p=0.047; Figure 3d). Interestingly, there was also a strong positive D-HV effect (β = 0.122, t43 = 5.067, p<10−5), suggesting that even though the distractor effect varied as a function of difficulty level, the effect was generally positive in these participants. There was a positive HV-LV effect (β = 0.571, t43 = 15.159, p<10−18).

In addition to being present in the data reported by Chau et al., 2014 and Gluth et al., 2018 the same effect emerged in a third previously unreported data set (Experiment 3 Hong Kong; n = 40) employing the same schedule but collected at a third site (Hong Kong). The results were highly comparable not only when the choice accuracy data were visualised using the same approach (Figure 3e), but also when the same GLM was applied to analyse the choice accuracy data (Figure 3f). There was a significant (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect (β = −0.089, t39 = −2.242, p=0.031). Again there was a positive D-HV effect (β = 0.207, t39 = 5.980, p<10−6) and a positive HV-LV effect (β = 0.485, t39 = 12.448, p<10−14). The pattern of results was consistent regardless of whether an additional HV+LV term was included in the GLM, as in Chau et al., 2014; a significant (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect was found in Experiments 1–3 when an additional HV+LV term was included in the GLM (GLM1b; Figure 3—figure supplement 1).

It is clear that data collected under the same conditions in 105 participants at all three sites are very similar and that a positive distractor effect consistently recurs when decisions are difficult. Next, we aggregated the data collected from the three sites and repeated the same GLM to confirm that the (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction (β = −0.101, t104 = −4.366, p<10−4), D-HV (β = 0.223, t104 = 6.400, p<10−8) and HV-LV (β = 0.529, t104 = 20.775, p<10−38) effects were all collectively significant. Additional control analyses suggest that these effects were unlikely due to any statistical artefact (see ‘Distractor effects are not driven by statistical artefact’ for details).

As in the empirical data from human participants, in the dual route model it is particularly obvious that there is a negative (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction effect on the simulated choices, which is contributed by a combination of positive D-HV effect on hard trials and negative D-HV effect on easy trials. In contrast, in the mutual inhibition and divisive normalisation models only one of the two D-HV effects is present (Figure 1). To ascertain which model best describes the behaviour of the participants, each model was fitted to the empirical data, that is to each participant’s choices and reaction time (RT) data. An additional null model that assumes that no distractor is present was also included in the model comparison. After fitting the models, a Bayesian model selection was performed to compare the goodness-of-fit of the models. Interestingly, the dual route model provided the best account of the participants’ behaviour when Experiments 1–3 were considered as a whole (estimated frequency Ef = 0.898; exceedance probability Xp = 1.000, Figure 3g–h) and when individual experiments were considered separately (Experiment 1: Ef = 0.843, Xp = 1.000; Experiment 2: Ef = 0.924, Xp = 1.000; Experiment 3: Ef = 0.864, Xp = 1.000). Furthermore, the fitted parameters were applied back to each model to predict participants’ behaviour (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). The results show that the dual route model is better than the mutual inhibition, divisive normalisation, and null models in predicting both choice accuracy and reaction time.

Additional models were run to confirm that the dual route model is a better model. The above models involve assigning fixed values for the non-decision time Tnd (at 0.3 s) and inhibition level f. In one set of analysis the f is fitted as a free parameter (Figure 3—figure supplement 3b) and in another set of analysis both Tnd and f are fitted as free parameters (Figure 3—figure supplement 3c). In both cases, as in the models with fixed Tnd and f, the dual route model is a better fit compared to the other three alternative models (Ef = 0.641, Xp = 1.000 and Ef = 0.587, Xp = 1.000 respectively). Finally, a comparison of all twelve models (four models × three versions of free parameter set) shows that the dual route model with fixed Tnd and f is the best fit (Ef = 0.413, Xp = 1.000; Figure 3—figure supplement 3d).

The next step is to examine whether the (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction effect from GLM1a and 1b arises because of the presence of a divisive normalisation effect (i.e. negative D-HV effect) on easy trials, a positive distractor effect on hard trials, or both effects. In other words, we need to establish which component of the interaction (or in other words, which main effect) is driving the interaction. To establish which is the case, the data were median split as a function of difficulty, defined as HV-LV, so that it is possible to easily visualise the separate predictions of the divisive normalisation and positive distractor accounts (Figure 4a; a similar approach was also used by Chau and colleagues in their Supplementary Figure SI.4). Then, to analyse each half of the choice accuracy data we applied GLM2a in a stepwise manner. Step one included the regressor HV-LV to partial out any choice variance shared between this term and the relative distractor term D-HV. Another regressor HV+LV was also included in the same step to completely partial out any remaining choice variance shared between the HV/LV options and D-HV. Step two then only included the regressor D-HV and was fitted on the residual choice variance of step one to determine the unique impact of the distractor. In the simulated choices of the dual route model, a positive distractor effect is found on hard trials; in other words, high D-HV value is associated with significantly greater accuracy on hard trials (Figure 4a, blue bar; β = 0.008, t104 = 32.173, p<10−55). The same model also exhibited a negative distractor effect, which is predicted by the divisive normalisation account, on easy trials (Figure 4a, red bar; β = −0.003, t104 = −17.105, p<10−31). A very similar pattern was also found in the empirical data across all three experiments. There was a positive distractor effect on hard trials (Figure 4b, blue bars – Experiment 1 fMRI2014: β = 0.020, t20 = 2.173, p=0.042; Experiment 2 Gluth4: β = 0.021, t43 = 3.366, p=0.002; Experiment 3 Hong Kong: β = 0.017, t39 = 3.081, p=0.004). Moreover, in Experiment 1 fMRI2014 high value D-HV was marginally associated with less accuracy on easy trials (β = −0.015, t20 = −2.080, p=0.051; Figure 4b red bars) and in Experiment 3 Hong Kong it was significantly associated with less accuracy on easy trials (β = −0.016, t39 = −3.339, p=0.002). Although the effect was not significant in Experiment 2 Gluth4, a similar negative trend was found (β = −0.005, t43 = −0.914, p=0.366), which at least supports the notion that the D-HV effect became less positive from hard to easy trials in this particular experiment. The D-HV effects were significantly different between hard and easy trials across all three experiments (Experiment 1 fMRI2014: t20=2.706, p=0.014; Experiment 2 Gluth4: t43=3.001, p=0.005; Experiment 3 Hong Kong: t39=4.847, p<10−4). Similar results were found if we tested the effect of the absolute value of D instead of the relative D-HV effect (GLM2b; Figure 4—figure supplement 1a). In summary, a positive distractor effect is present when decisions are difficult in all three data sets. Divisive normalisation is apparent on easier trials, at least in two of the three data sets.

Figure 4. Distractors had opposite effects on decision accuracy as a function of difficulty in all experiments.

The main effect of the distractor was different depending on decision difficulty. (a) In accordance with the predictions of the dual route model, high value distractors (D-HV is high) facilitated decision making when the decision was hard (blue bars), whereas there was a tendency for high value distractors to impair decision making when the decision was easy (red bars). Data are shown for (b) Experiment 1 fMRI2014, Experiment 2 Gluth4, Experiment 3 Hong Kong. (c) The same is true when data from the other experiments, Experiments 4–6 (i.e. Gluth1-3), are examined in a similar way. However, participants made decisions in these experiments in a different manner: they were less likely to integrate probability and magnitude features of the options in the optimal manner when making decisions and instead were more likely to choose on the basis of a weighted sum of the probability and magnitude components of the options. Thus, in Experiments 4–6 (i.e. Gluth1-3), the difficulty of a trial can be better described by the weighted sum of the magnitude and probability components associated with each option rather than the true objective value difference HV-LV. This may be because these experiments included additional ‘decoy’ trials that were particularly difficult and on which it was especially important to consider the individual attributes of the options rather than just their integrated expected value. Whatever the reason for the difference in behaviour, once an appropriate difficulty metric is constructed for these participants, the pattern of results is the same as in panel a. # p<0.1, *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Error bars indicate standard error.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Similar difficulty-dependent distractor effects were observed when accuracy is explained using the value of D, rather than the relative value of D in comparison to HV (i.e. D–HV).

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(a) Positive and negative D effects were found on hard and easy trials respectively in Experiments 1 fMRI2014, Experiment 2 Gluth4, and Experiment 3 Hong Kong, using GLM2b. As in Figure 4a, difficulty was described by HV-LV in these experiments. (b) The same pattern of D effects was also found in Experiments 4–6 (Gluth1-3) when difficulty was described by a weighted sum of the probability and magnitude components of the options, as in Figure 4b (GLM2d). (c) It is also possible to observe similar difficulty-dependent distractor effects in all six experiments by analysing all data with one single approach. First ‘novel’ trials in Experiments 2, 4–6, added by Gluth and colleagues to test decoy effects were excluded. Then choice difficulty was estimated based on a combination of HV-LV and HV+LV, instead of either the HV-LV difference or the weighted sum of magnitude and probability differences. Here HV+LV was also included because it is possible that the total value sum also contributes to the subjective difficulty level. Finally, GLM2b was applied to estimate the effect of D on hard and easy trials. # p<0.1, *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. The dual-route model best describes participant behaviour in Experiments 1–6.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(a) A model comparison shows that participant behaviour in Experiments 4 to 6, as well as Experiments 1–3 (Figure 3g–h), is best described by the dual route model, as opposed to the null, mutual inhibition, or divisive normalisation models. (b) Posterior probability of each model in accounting for the behaviour of individual participants. Null: null model; Mutual: mutual inhibition model; DivNorm: divisive normalisation model; Dual: dual route model. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

It might be asked why the presence of distractor effects in their data was not noted by Gluth et al., 2018. The answer is likely to be complex. A fundamental consideration is that it is important to examine the possibility that both distractor effects exist rather than just the possibility that one or other effect exists. This means that it is necessary to consider not just the main effect of D-HV but also D-HV interaction effects. Gluth and colleagues, however, focus on the main effect of D-HV in most sections of their paper, apart from their table S2. Careful scrutiny of their table S2 reveals that the (D-HV)(HV-LV) interaction is reliably significant in their data. A further consideration concerns the precise way in which the impact of the distractor D is indexed in the GLM particularly on control trials where no distractor is actually presented. Gluth et al., 2018 recommend that a notional value of D is assigned to control trials which corresponds to the distractor’s average value when it appears on distractor trials. In addition, they emphasise that HV-LV and D-HV should be normalised (i.e. demeaned and divided by the standard deviation) before calculating the (HV-LV)(D-HV) term. If we run an analysis of their data in this way then we obtain similar results to those described by Gluth and colleagues in their Table S2 (Supplementary file 1A here). Although a D-HV main effect was absent, the (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction term was significant when data from all their experiments are considered together. While Gluth and colleagues omitted any analysis of the data from Experiment 1 fMRI, we have performed this analysis and once again a significant (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect is clear (Supplementary file 1A).

Another possible reason for the discrepancy in the interpretation concerns the other three experiments reported by Gluth and colleagues. We turn to these next.

Examining distractor effects in further experiments

We can also examine the impact of distractors in three further experiments reported by Gluth and colleagues. Below, we show that essentially the same pattern of results emerges in these experiments (Figure 4c): Experiment 4 Gluth3; Experiment 5 Gluth2; Experiment 6 Gluth1. Before we examine the data in detail, however, it is worth noting some differences between the experiments. First, the way in which participants made decisions in these next experiments was different. In the first three experiments participants tended to combine the information that the choice stimuli provided about both reward probability and reward magnitude; their choices indicated that they tended to choose the option where the product of reward probability and reward magnitude was larger than that of the alternative. By contrast in the next three experiments participants were still attracted by large reward probability options and large reward magnitude options but they did not always integrate reward magnitude and reward probability information to choose the option with the larger overall value. This is apparent when two simple GLMs (GLM3a and GLM3b) were used to describe the accuracy of each participant’s decision making. GLM3a involves two terms that relate to the expected values of the HV and LV option. The first term is, as previously, the HV-LV value difference term. The second term is an HV+LV term which captures the remaining variance associated with the HV and LV expected values. GLM3b, however, included four separate attribute-based terms. It involves two terms that describe the difference in reward magnitude and the difference in reward probability of the HV and LV options. It also involves two terms that describe the sum in reward magnitude and sum in probability of the two options, which capture the remaining variance of the attributes of the two options. The Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) value was significantly smaller in the attribute-based GLM3b than the value-based GLM3a in all Gluth’s experiments: that is Experiments 2 (t43 = 3.540, p<0.001), 4 (t22 = 1.942, p=0.065; where the difference was marginal), 5 (t48 = 5.616, p<10−5) and 6 (t30 = 3.635, p=0.004). This was not the case for the experiments conducted elsewhere: Experiments 1 (t20 = 1.067, p=0.299) and 3 (t39 = 1.311, p=0.198).

In summary, in participants from Experiments 2 and 4–6 (i.e. Gluth1-4), the difficulty of a trial could be better described by the weighted sum of the magnitude and probability components associated with each option rather than the true objective value difference HV-LV. It is not clear why Gluth and colleagues’ participants performed the task in this way but we know that people and animals often make decisions in a similar manner in other experiments (Farashahi et al., 2019; Scholl et al., 2014; Scholl et al., 2015). Indeed, while the behaviour of the participants in experiments 1 and 3 is not explained better by an attribute-based model than by the more normative model employing values based on the integration of both magnitude and probability, the integrative model is not a significantly better one. This suggests that they may have a tendency to use attribute-based heuristics. One way of interpreting such behaviour is that participants are not acting as if they estimate the HV and LV values from their magnitude and probability components in the optimal multiplicative manner but instead they are acting as if using an additive heuristic. Further consideration of the details of Experiments 4–6 suggest possible reasons why participants might have been particularly prone to behave in this way in these experiments. The stimuli used in all six experiments were two dimensional in nature; stimulus color and orientation respectively indicated the expected magnitude and probability of the reward that participants would receive for taking a choice. This approach was taken by Chau and colleagues in their initial experiments because it was conjectured that positive distractor effects might be linked to vmPFC/mOFC and it is known that vmPFC/mOFC plays an important role when decisions are made between multi-attribute options (Fellows, 2006; Hunt et al., 2012; Papageorgiou et al., 2017). However, because Gluth et al., 2018 were interested in the possibility of ‘decoy’ effects they included a number of ‘novel’ trials in Experiments 4–6 in addition to those that were used in the initial experiment by Chau and colleagues. Some decoy effects occur when decisions are difficult (HV and LV are close together in value) but the value associated with one of the components of the distractor is close to the value of one of the components associated with either HV or LV. They therefore included additional trials that were particularly difficult and on which it was especially important to consider the individual attributes of the options rather than just their integrated expected value. These additional trials accounted for 27% of all trials in Experiments 4–6 and it is possible that this caused the participants to use an attribute-based approach when making their choices.

When we median split the data from Experiments 4–6 as a function of difficulty described by HV-LV, we were not able to find any distractor effects on both hard (|t| < 1.393, p>0.178) and easy trials (|t| < 1.072, p>0.295). However, it is not so surprising that such an analysis was unable to reveal any difficulty-dependent distractor effect because for these participants in these experiments, difficulty is a function of the individual attributes rather than the correctly integrated expected value of each option. Thus, we extracted the weighting of each individual attribute on choice accuracy estimated in GLM3b above and calculated the difference in weighting between HV and LV for each component attribute. We then estimated the sum of the weighted probability difference and weighted magnitude difference and used this to estimate the subjective difference in the values of HV and LV and used this as an index of difficulty.

Interestingly, when we median split the trials according to this attribute-based difficulty index and applied GLM2c, which only differs from GLM2a by how difficulty is defined, the results of Experiments 4–6 were then highly comparable to those found in Experiments 1–3. On hard trials, a positive D-HV effect was found in Experiments 4 (β = 0.038, t22 = 3.958, p<0.001), 5 (β = 0.046, t48 = 7.403, p<10−8) and 6 (β = 0.0343, t30 = 4.278, p<0.001; Figure 4c). On easy trials, a negative D-HV effect was found in Experiment 5 (β = −0.014, t45 = −3.997, p<0.001) and Experiment 6 (β = −0.013, t30 = −2.590, p=0.015), while a similar trend was found in Experiment 4 (β = −0.011, t22 = −1.684, p=0.106). We note that it was difficult to fit models to the data in three of the 149 participants (all were participants in Experiment 5) because their choices were all accurate on hard trials and so their data were omitted from the analysis. The D-HV effect was significantly different between hard and easy trials across all three experiments (t > 4.428, p<0.001). Again, a similar pattern of results emerged when the analyses employed the absolute value of D rather than the relative D-HV term (Figure 4—figure supplement 1b).

We realised that it is possible to observe similar difficulty-dependent distractor effects in all six experiments by analysing all data with one single approach. This is consistent with the fact that, even in Experiments 1 and 3, participants do not integrate probability and magnitude in the normatively optimal way but have some bias towards attribute-based heuristics even if it is not as strong as in Experiments 2, and 4–6. We excluded all ‘novel’ trials in Experiments 2, 4–6, added by Gluth and colleagues for testing decoy effects and applied a simple regression GLM2c (see Figure 4—figure supplement 1c for details). We found that there was a significant difference in the D effect between hard and easy trials in each of the six experiments (t > 2.220, p<0.034). The same is also true when we combined the data from all six experiments (t207 = 7.679, p<10−12). Finally, there was a positive distractor effect on hard trials (β = 0.026, t207 = 6.080, p<10−7) and a negative distractor effect on easy trials (β = −0.017, t207 = −4.732, p<10−5) when all six experiments were considered together.

Finally, the four models (dual-route, mutual inhibition, divisive normalisation and null) were applied to fit the data of Experiments 4–6. Again, the dual-route model provided the best account of participants’ behaviour when individual experiments were considered separately (Experiment 4: Ef = 0.806, Xp = 0.999; Experiment 5: Ef = 0.649, Xp = 1.000; Experiment 6: Ef = 0.946, Xp = 1.000; Figure 4—figure supplement 2) or when Experiments 1–6 were considered as a whole (Ef = 0.846, Xp = 1.000).

Distractor effects are not driven by statistical artefact

Several considerations suggest that the presence of the difficulty-dependent distractor effect is not due to some unusual statistical artefact. First, all of the interaction terms are calculated after their component parts, the difficulty and distractor terms, are z-scored (i.e. centered by the mean and then divided by the standard deviation). Second, the interaction effects were further confirmed by median splitting the data by difficulty level and testing the distractor effect on each half of the trials. The finding of opposite distractor effects on hard and easy trials when analysed separately is a key characteristic of true interaction effects. Additional simulations and control analyses also confirmed that the difficulty-dependent distractor effect was not due to any statistical artefact (Appendices 1 and 2).

A more complete analysis of distractor effects

The dual route model predicts that the distractor effect varies to a degree as a function of difficulty (HV-LV; Figure 1j,k). Other factors also mean that different types of distractor effects should be seen in different parts of the decision space. Even, in isolation, the divisive normalisation model predicts that the distractor effect varies strongly as a function of another term, the value sum: HV+LV. Since the overall normalisation effect depends on the total integrated value of the options (HV+LV+D), variance in this term mainly reflects variance in D when HV+LV is small. Thus, the dual route model predicts that negative distractor effect driven by the divisive normalisation component should become weaker when the value sum HV+LV is large and positive distractor effect driven by the mutual inhibition component should become stronger. This is exactly what has been shown in further analyses of the simulated data of the dual route model and the empirical data of the actual participants (Appendix 3).

Attentional capture distractor effects and further evidence for positive distractor effects and divisive normalisation

So far we have established that both positive and negative distractor effects on choice accuracy (i.e. choosing HV over LV) were each most robust in different parts of the decision space. Despite the fact that in all experiments participants were instructed not to choose the distractor, it was still chosen by mistake on some trials. These trials were excluded in all the analyses presented above. However, Gluth and colleagues emphasised that the distractor has an attentional-capture effect that impaired choice accuracy. As such, they found that when the distractor value was large, participants made more mistakes by choosing the distractor itself more often.

We consider that the attentional-capture effect and the difficulty-dependent distractor effects that we have described above (i.e. both the positive and negative distractor effects) should not be thought of as mutually exclusive. To illustrate this, we ran an analysis that also included trials where the distractor was chosen and then applied an analysis similar to GLM1a again. Unlike Gluth and colleagues who ran a binomial logistic regression to test whether HV or collectively LV and D were chosen, here in GLM1c we ran a multinomial logistic regression. In this analysis, we were able to test the effects of distractor value on the choice ratio between HV and LV and on the choice ratio between HV and D separately. This means that a single analysis has the power to reveal both attentional capture effects as well as other distractor effects. We also only included participants with at least three trials on which the distractor was chosen (n = 180). We removed another six participants that showed exceptionally large beta weights (|β|>10) for the constant term on the HV/D choice ratio (remaining participants: n = 174), although the removal of these participants did not change the pattern of the results. First, we again replicated the finding that there was a negative (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect (β = −0.076, t173 = −4.044, p<10−4) and also a positive HV-LV effect (β = −0.530, t173 = −23.953, p<10−56) on the choice ratio between HV and LV (i.e. choice accuracy; Figure 5a). In addition, as the attentional capture model suggested, there was also a positive HV-D effect on the choice ratio between HV and D (β = 1.176, t173 = 18.996, p<10−43; Figure 5b) – the HV was chosen more often when its own value was large and the same was true for the distractor when D value was large.

Figure 5. A further replication of distractor effects in 174 participants.

Figure 5.

On some occasions ‘attentional capture’ occurs and participants, contrary to their instructions choose the distractor itself. It is possible to analyse how participants distribute their choices between the choosable options HV and LV and between the high value choosable option HV and the distractor D using multinomial logistic regression. (a) Multinomial regression confirms that HV is chosen over LV if the HV-LV difference is large but also confirms that there is an interaction between this term and the difference in value between D and HV. (b) There are also, however, parallel effects when decisions between HV and D are now considered. In parallel to the main effect seen in decisions between HV and LV, decisions between HV and D are mainly driven by the difference in value between these two chooseable options – the HV-D difference. In parallel to the distractor effect seen when decisions are made between HV and LV, there is also an effect of the third option, now LV, when decisions between HV and D are considered; there is an (HV-D)(LV-HV) interaction. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Error bars indicate standard error.

Interestingly, in the same analysis we found additional evidence of both difficulty-dependent distractor effects and the attentional capture distractor effect. This is because in each of the six experiments we have considered so far there is effectively a second data set that co-exists somewhat like a palimpsest alongside the main one. Normally, the D option is regarded as a distractor when the HV/LV choice ratio (i.e. choice accuracy) is considered. However, analogously in the multinomial analysis the LV option can be regarded as a ‘distractor’ when we consider the HV/D choice ratio. Hence, we can also test the difficulty-dependent ‘distractor’ effect of LV on the HV/D choice ratio using the (HV-LV)(D-HV) term. It may be helpful to point out that the analogous term for testing the LV distractor effect on the HV/D choice ratio is perhaps most obviously (HV-D)(LV-HV) but this is mathematically identical to (HV-LV)(D-HV). Critically, we found a negative (HV-D)(LV-HV) effect on the HV/D choice ratio (β = −0.142, t173 = −2.674, p=0.008; Figure 5b), suggesting that on hard trials (HV-D is the relevant metric of difficulty when considering the HV/D choice ratio; a small HV-D difference means that it is difficult to select the HV option over the D option) more HV choices were chosen over D when the value of the ‘distracting’ LV option was large. Ideally, a follow-up analysis of the negative (HV-D)(LV-HV) effect should also be run to examine how the distractor LV-HV effect varied from hard (small HV-D) to easy (large HV-D) trials. Since choices of the D option were rare, splitting the trials further according to the median HV-D index is obviously likely to result in even smaller numbers of D choices in each half of the data, especially in the half with large HV-D. This, in turn, is likely to lead to unreliable estimates of effect sizes. However, we still attempted to perform this analysis (which is analogous to those performed in Figure 4). In particular, we applied a GLM that involved regressors HV-D, HV+D and LV-HV and focused on the LV-HV distractor effect in each half of the data. The results showed that on hard (small HV-D) trials there was a positive distractor LV-HV effect on the HV/D choice ratio (β = 0.170, t171 = −2.517, p=0.013). Another two participants were excluded in this analysis due to exceptionally large beta weights (|β|>10). Next, we repeated the same analysis on easy (large HV-D) trials. As expected, a large number of participants had to be excluded in this analysis – 107 participants did not choose the D option on these trials and 22 participants showed exceptionally large beta weights (|β|>10). Nevertheless, on easy (large HV-D) trials in the remaining 45 participants there was a lack of distractor LV-HV effect (β = 0.017, t44 = 1.407, p=0.936). Although it is expected that the distractor LV-HV effect should be negative on easy trials, it is possible that this is due to the scarcity of D choices. Finally, there was a significant difference in LV-HV effect between the hard and easy trials in these participants (t44 = 6.126, p<10−6).

Taken together, the multinomial logistic regression provided evidence supporting both difficulty-dependent distractor effects and attentional capture distractor effects both when we consider the HV/LV choice ratio that has been the focus of previous studies but also when we consider the HV/D choice ratio that is often simultaneously present in these studies due to erroneous choices of the distractor.

Experiment 7: Loss experiment

The attentional capture model raises the question of whether any distractor effect on choice accuracy is due to the value or the salience of the distractor. This is difficult to test in most reward-based decision making experiments because the value and salience of an option are often collinear – more rewarding options are both larger in value and more salient – and it is not possible to determine which factor drives behaviour. One way of breaking the collinearity between value and salience is to introduce options that lead to loss (Kahnt et al., 2014). As such, the smallest value options that lead to great loss are very salient (Figure 6a, bottom), the medium value options that lead to small gains or losses are not salient and the largest value options that lead to great gain are again very salient. Having a combination of gain and loss scenarios in an experiment enables the investigation of whether the positive and negative distractor effects, related to mutual inhibition and divisive normalisation respectively, are driven by the distractor’s value, salience or both. Figure 6a and b show four hypothetical cases of how the distractor may influence accuracy. Hypothesis one suggests that larger distractor values (Figure 6a, first row, left-to-right), which correspond to fewer losses or more gains, are related to greater accuracies (brighter colors). This is also predicted by the mutual inhibition component of the dual route model (Figure 1) and can be described as a positive D effect (Figure 6b). Hypothesis two suggests larger distractor saliences (Figure 6a, second row, center-to-sides) are related to greater accuracies (brighter colors). This can be described as a positive |D| effect (Figure 6b). Under this hypothesis the mutual inhibition decision making component receives salience, rather than value, as an input. Hypotheses 3 and 4 are the opposites of Hypotheses 1 and 2, and predict negative distractor effects as a result of the divisive normalisation component depending on whether the input involves value or salience. Hypothesis three predicts a value-based effect in which larger distractor values (Figure 6a, third row, left-to-right) are related to poorer accuracies (darker colors). Hypothesis four predicts a salience-based effect in which larger distractor saliences (Figure 6a, fourth row, center-to-sides) are related to poorer accuracies (darker colors). It is important to note that these four hypotheses are not necessarily mutually exclusive. The earlier sections have demonstrated that positive and negative distractor effects can co-exist and predominate in different parts of decision space. Value-based and salience-based distractor effects can also be teased apart with a combination of gain and loss scenarios.

Figure 6. Loss Experiment.

Figure 6.

(a, bottom) Value and salience are collinear when values are only related to rewards (right half) but it is possible to determine whether each factor has an independent effect by also looking at choices made to avoid losses (both left and right halves). (a, top and b) Four hypothetical effects of distractor on accuracy. The first and second hypotheses suggest that the distractor effect is positive, which is predicted by the mutual inhibition model and is related to the distractor’s value and salience respectively. The third and fourth hypotheses suggest that the distractor effect is negative, which is predicted by the divisive normalisation model, and is related to its value and salience respectively. All four hypothesis are not mutually exclusive – value and salience are orthogonal factors and positive/negative distractor effects can predominate different parts of decision space. (c, right) A plot identical to that in Figure 3e that shows the data from the gain trials of Experiment 3 Hong Kong. Accuracy (light-yellow indicates high accuracy, dark-red indicates low accuracy) is plotted across the decision space defined by decision difficulty (HV-LV) and relative distractor value (D–HV). (c, left) A similar plot using the data from the loss trials of Experiment 7 Loss Experiment is shown. (d) GLM analysis indicates the distractor value D had a negative effect, suggesting that accuracy was more impaired on trials with distractors that were associated with fewer losses or more gains. In contrast, the distractor salience |D| had a positive effect, suggesting that accuracy was more facilitated on trials with more salient distractors (i.e. those related to larger gains or losses). (e, left) The negative value D effect was significant on easy trials (orange) and reversed and became positive on hard trials (blue). (e, right) In contrast, the positive salience |D| effect was significant on both hard and easy trials. In the dual route model, there are two components that guide decision making in parallel. This pattern suggests that the positive distractor effect of the mutual inhibition component is related to the salience and value of the distractor whereas the negative distractor effect of the divisive normalisation component is most closely related to the value of the distractor. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Error bars indicate standard error.

To test these hypotheses, we adopted this approach in an additional experiment performed at the same time as Experiment 3 Hong Kong, in which half of the trials included options that were all positive in value (gain trials) and the other half of the trials included options that were all negative in value (loss trials; the loss trials were not analysed in the previous sections). We therefore refer to these additional trials as belonging to Experiment 7 Loss Experiment (n = 40 as in Experiment 3 Hong Kong). The effect of signed D reflects the value of the distractor while the effect of the unsigned, absolute size of D (i.e. |D|) reflects the salience of the distractor. The correlation between these two parameters was low (r = 0.005), such that it was possible to isolate the impact that they each had on behaviour.

As in other experiments, we first plotted the accuracy as a function of difficulty (HV-LV) and relative distractor value (D-HV). For ease of comparison, Figure 3e that illustrates the accuracy data for the gain trials in Experiments three is shown again in the right panel of Figure 6c. As described before, when the decisions were hard (bottom rows) larger distractor values were associated with greater accuracies (left-to-right: the colors change from dark to bright; also see Figure 4b) and when the decisions were easy, larger distractor values were associated with poorer accuracies (left-to-right: the colors change from bright to dark; also see Figure 4b). In a similar manner, the left panel of Figure 6c shows the accuracy data of the loss trials in Experiment 7. Strikingly, on both hard and easy trials (top and bottom rows), larger distractor values were associated with poorer accuracies (left-to-right: the colours changes from bright to dark).

To isolate the value-based and salience-based effects of D, we performed GLM6a (Materials and methods) to analyse both the gain and loss trials in Experiments 3 and 7 at the same time. GLM6 includes the signed and unsigned value of D (i.e. D and |D| respectively). We also included a binary term, GainTrial, to describe whether the trial presented gain options or loss options and, as in GLM1a, we included the HV-LV term and its interaction with D but now also with |D| [i.e. (HV-LV)D and (HV-LV)|D| respectively]. The results showed a negative effect of value D (β = −0.236, t39 = −2.382, p=0.022; Figure 6d) and a negative effect of (HV-LV)D interaction (β = −0.205, t39 = −2.512, p=0.016). In addition, there was a positive effect of salience |D| (β = 0.152, t39 = 3.253, p=0.002) and a positive effect of (HV-LV)|D| (β = 0.219, t39 = 3.448, p=0.001). Next, we examined closely the value-based and salience-based effect in different parts of decision space.

As in the analysis for Experiments 1–6 in Figure 6e, we split the data (which included both gain and loss trials) according to the median HV-LV, such that the distractor effects can be examined on hard and easy trials separately. We applied GLM6b that first partialled out the effects of HV-LV, HV+LV and GainTrial from the accuracy data and then tested the overall effect of value D across the gain and loss trials. Similar to Experiments 1–6, a positive value D effect was identified on hard trials (β = 0.008, t39 = 2.463, p=0.017; Figure 6e, left) and a negative value D effect was identified on easy trials (β = −0.011, t38 = −3.807, p<10−3; note that one participant was excluded due to the lack of variance in the accuracy data). Then we applied GLM6c which was similar to GLM6b but the value D term was replaced by the salience |D| term. The results showed that there were positive salience |D| effects on both hard (β = 0.011, t39 = 2.119, p=0.041; Figure 6e, right) and easy trials (β = 0.009, t38 = 2.338, p=0.025).

Taken together, in Experiments 1–6 a positive distractor effect predicted by the mutual inhibition model and a negative distractor effect predicted by the divisive normalisation model were found on hard and easy trials respectively. The results of Experiments 3 and 7 suggest that these effects are value-based and that the effects are continuous across the gain and loss value space. In addition, however, there was also a positive distractor effect that was salience-based that appeared on both hard and easy trials, suggesting that the effects driven by the mutual inhibition decision making component can be both value-based and salience-based.

In the future it might be possible to extend the models outlined in Figure 1 and S1 to provide a more quantitative descriptions of behaviour. While this topic is of great interest it will require modelers to agree on how loss trials might be modelled. For example, one possibility is to have a negative drift rate in the models that we have used. This implies that the decisions will then be about which option to avoid rather than which option to choose, because options that are more negative in value will reach the decision threshold more quickly. It is unclear, however, whether participants used such an avoidance approach in Experiment 7. Hence, we have refrained from modelling the results of Experiment 7.

A relationship between attentional capture and positive distractor-salience effects

Although Gluth et al., 2018 reported an absence of divisive normalisation and positive distractor effects, as we have noted they emphasised the attentional capture effect. In essence the attentional capture effect is the observation that sometimes participants attempted to choose the distractor itself rather that one or other of the choosable options (even though this ran contrary to the instructions they had been given). In a similar vein, when analysing the data from both gain and loss trials in Experiments 3 and 7, we also found that the positive effect of the distractor reflected its salience, in addition to value; as suggested by Gluth and colleagues, salient distractors capture attention. We therefore considered, in a final experiment, whether there is any relationship between these two findings – attentional capture and the positive distractor-salience effect.

We performed Experiment 8 Eye Movement (n = 35), using a new procedure (Figure 7), in which we probed the relationship between attentional capture and positive salience-distractor effects. First, as in other experiments, when GLM5 was applied we found a similar negative (HV-LV)D effect (β = −0.258, t33 = −2.593, p=0.014) and a positive (HV+LV)D effect (β = 0.743, t33 = 5.417, p<10−5) on choice accuracy (Figure 7—figure supplement 1) suggesting the presence of both positive distractor effects and divisive normalisation effects respectively. Note that in this analysis, data from one outlier participant was removed since the (HV-LV)D effect was larger than the mean by 4.1 times the standard deviation.

Figure 7. Experiment 8: eye tracking experiment.

The behavioural paradigm was adapted from that used in Experiments 1–7. (a) Each choosable option was represented by a pair of circles. Each distractor option was represented by a pair of heptagons. The screen was divided into four quadrants and each quadrant had two positions for presenting a pair of stimuli associated with an option, making up a total of eight positions. The eight positions were all 291 pixels from the center of the screen and equally separated. Options and distractors appeared in different quadrants on each trial. (b) The reward magnitude of an option/distractor was represented by the color of one component stimulus whereas the angle of the tick on the other component stimulus indicated reward probability.

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. As in Experiments 1–6, in Experiment eight the distractor effect was modulated as a function of difficulty and the total value of the choosable options.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

The choice accuracy data in Experiment eight was analysed by GLM5 that includes all main effects of HV-LV, HV+LV, and D and also the two- and three-way interactions involving these terms. Similar to Experiments 1–6, the (HV-LV)D interaction is negative and the (HV+LV)D interaction is positive. These suggest that the distractor effect on decision accuracy was particularly positive when decisions were difficult and also when the total HV+LV value was large. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Error bars indicate standard error.

Next, we analysed the eye movement data collected from a subset of participants (n = 21) and tested the relationships between option value and fixation frequency. We found evidence for attentional capture when D had a high value; participants fixated D more frequently when D’s value was high (Figure 8a; GLM7; β = 0.058, t20 = 4.719, p<0.001). The attractor network model (Chau et al., 2014; Wang, 2002; Wang, 2008) or a mutual inhibition model such as the one outlined in Figure 1a also predicts that high value distractors should become the focus for behaviour. For example, in the cortical attractor network model, this is due to the pulse of activity in the inhibitory interneuron pool that follows from the high-valued D input. This slows the selection of all options, which subsequently allows more time for evidence in favor of HV rather than LV to be accumulated. However, such models do more than simply predict the unavailable distractor option, D should be the focus of behaviour. In addition they argue that participants should go on to shift focus to the HV option as they realise the distractor option D is unavailable, and so no longer attend to it, so that the distractor boosts rather than lessens accuracy.

Figure 8. Larger D values were associated with more gaze shifts from D to HV and more accurate decisions.

(a) A multivariate regression analysis showed that larger D values were associated with attentional capture effects as indexed by more fixations at D (right, purple bar). In addition, larger HV-LV difference was associated with fewer fixations at LV (left, blue bar) and larger total HV+LV values were associated with fewer fixations in general (middle). (b) As D value increased so did gaze shifts between D and HV (right, green bar), while gaze shifts between D and LV decreased (right, blue bar). These effects could not merely be due to more fixations at HV or LV per se because the effects of fixations at HV, LV and D on the gaze shifts were partialled out before testing the relationships between D value and gaze shifts. (c) The effect was directionally specific; larger D values were associated with more D-to-HV shifts and fewer D-to-LV shifts but not the opposite (right, green and blue bars; HV-to-D or LV-to-D shifts). (d) In turn, more D-to-HV shifts and fewer D-to-LV shifts predicted greater decision accuracy. FixHV fixation at HV; FixLV fixation at LV; FixD fixation at D; ShiftHV-LV gaze shift between HV and LV; ShiftHV-D gaze shift between HV and D; ShiftLV-D gaze shift between LV and D. # p<0.1, *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Error bars indicate standard error.

Figure 8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1. The same analysis performed in Figure 8 was performed by including one additional participant and the results remained similar.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

A posterior power analysis indicated that 22 participants were required to meet a power of 80% and alpha of 0.05. Initially an inclusion criterion of eye movement data validity >85% resulted in 21 participants, which is one participant less than the required number. Then, the inclusion criterion was relaxed to 70% to include one additional participant (i.e. n = 22). The results remained similar to those reported in Figure 8. FixHV fixation at HV; FixLV fixation at LV; FixD fixation at D; ShiftHV-LV gaze shift between HV and LV; ShiftHV-D gaze shift between HV and D; ShiftLV-D gaze shift between LV and D. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Error bars indicate standard error.

Consistent with this prediction in GLM8 we found that as the value of D increased so did gaze shifts between D and HV (Figure 8b; β = 0.022, t20 = 2.937, p=0.008), while gaze shifts between D and LV decreased (β = −0.031, t20 = −3.365, p=0.003). These effects could not merely be due to more fixations at HV or LV per se because we partialled out the effects of fixations at HV, LV and D on the gaze shifts before we tested the relationships between D value and gaze shifts. In addition, the effect was directionally specific. We applied the same GLM8 to predict the proportion of gaze shift from D to HV, D to LV, HV to D, and LV to D. The results showed that larger D values were associated with more D-to-HV shifts (Figure 8c; β = 0.028, t20 = 4.589, p<0.001) and with fewer D-to-LV shifts (β = −0.027, t20 = −4.001, p<0.001). In contrast, the value of D was unrelated to the frequencies of the opposite HV-to-D shift (β = −0.005, t20 = −0.890, p=0.384) and LV-to-D shift (β = −0.004, t20 = −0.633, p=0.533). A two-way ANOVA confirmed that there was a Direction (from/to D)×Option (HV/LV) interaction effect (F1,20=11.360, p=003). Finally, more D-to-HV shifts (Figure 8d; GLM9; β = 0.028, t20 = 2.782, p=0.012) and fewer D-to-LV shifts (β = −0.043, t20 = −5.360, p<10−4) were related to higher choice accuracies.

In summary, both Gluth and colleagues (Gluth et al., 2018) and we think attention is captured by D (Figure 8a). However, our data also suggest D fixations guided by large D values were followed by D-to-HV gaze shifts (Figure 8b,c). This was associated with more accurate HV choices because there was a higher chance that the HV option was last fixated (Figure 8d).

Discussion

There has been considerable interest in the mechanisms mediating decision making. The majority of studies have focused on binary choice scenarios in which participants decide between two options (Glimcher, 2002; Shadlen and Kiani, 2013) but it is increasingly clear that the presence of an additional distractor may have an impact on which decisions are made between two choosable options. The nature of this impact varies as a function of the decision’s position within a decision space defined by the values of the choosable options (HV, LV) and the distractor (Figures 1, 3 and 4). When the values of choosable options and distractor are relatively low and high respectively then divisive normalisation means that decision accuracy is robustly and consistently impaired in data sets from a total of 243 participants from three sites (Experiments 1–6 and 8) including data sets from which such effects have previously been claimed to be absent (Figures 46, Appendix 3—figure 1). When decisions are difficult (HV-LV is small) then high value as opposed to low value distractors robustly and consistently lead to an increase in accuracy in the same data sets.

A further experiment demonstrated a key difference in the nature of the positive distractor effect and the divisive normalisation effect. While the divisive normalisation effect is related to the distractor value on easy trials and the positive distractor effect is related to the distractor value on hard trials, an additional positive distractor effect is also related to the salience of distractor on both hard and easy trials. When participants make decisions between options associated with losses, the presence of more appealing distractor options (i.e. those associated with less loss) are also less salient. On easy trials (Figure 6c, left panel, top rows) the distractors exert a combination of a value-based divisive normalization effect and a salience-based distractor effect (i.e. the reverse of the ‘positive distractor effect’) that results in a particularly strong negative effect on accuracy. In contrast, on hard trials (Figure 6c, left panel, bottom rows) the distractors exert a combination of a value-based positive distractor effect and a salience-based effect (i.e. reversed ‘positive distractor effect’) that result in a particularly weak effect on accuracy.

Positive distractor effects may be a consequence of interactions not just between the representations of choosable options in neural networks but interactions between these representations and the representation of the distractor. Chau et al., 2014 argued that one way to think about these interactions is in terms of the cortical attractor model of decision making (Wang, 2002; Wang, 2008; Wong and Wang, 2006). In such models separate pools of recurrently connected excitatory interneurons represent each possible choosable option and the distractor. Each pool receives an excitatory input proportional to its value. A common pool of inhibitory interneurons mediates competition or comparison between the pools of excitatory neurons representing the choosable options and distractor. As each option pool becomes more active it increases the activity in the inhibitory pool and this in turn leads to inhibition of the other option pools. Ultimately this means that the network ends up in one of a limited number of attractor states in which the pool representing the chosen option is in a high firing state but the pools representing the other options are in low firing states. If the option that is left in the high firing state is the highest value option (HV) then the decision taken is accurate. This should be the outcome of the competition because this pool received the highest input. However, the presence of some degree of stochasticity in the neural activity levels means that this is not always the case and when decisions are difficult (HV-LV is low) then sometimes the wrong choice is taken. Such effects are less likely to occur when the inhibitory interneurons mediating the decision process are more active, so that the evidence accumulation process is extended and decisions are less influenced by the noise. Sometimes the inhibitory interneuron pool is more active simply because of an individual difference; individuals with higher levels of inhibitory neurotransmitters such as gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) in vmPFC/mOFC are more accurate (Jocham et al., 2012). The inhibitory interneuron pool will also be more active when the distractor has a high value (Chau et al., 2014). In other situations, allowing participants more time to consider a decision, by giving them a later opportunity to revise their initial decision, also leads to greater accuracy (Resulaj et al., 2009; van den Berg et al., 2016).

Such predictions are not, however, limited to the cortical attractor model. A similar prediction might be made by other models, such as the mutual inhibition model used here, which is essentially a diffusion model but which posits interactions between the diffusion process comparing the values of the choosable options and the processes involved in selecting (and subsequently inhibiting selection) of the distractor (Figure 1a,c).

Decision making has been linked to neural processes in a number of brain circuits and it is increasingly clear that more than a single neural mechanism for decision making exists and that they operate in parallel (Hunt and Hayden, 2017; Rushworth et al., 2012). The mechanisms are not completely redundant with one another. Different mechanisms operate on different time scales (Meder et al., 2017; Wittmann et al., 2016), are in receipt of different types of information (Hunt et al., 2018; Kennerley et al., 2009), and are anatomically placed to exert different types of influence on behaviour (Hunt and Hayden, 2017; Rushworth et al., 2012). There has been particular interest in two cortical regions in humans and other primates such as macaques: vmPFC/mOFC and IPS (Boorman et al., 2009; Chau et al., 2014; Hunt and Hayden, 2017; Hunt et al., 2012; Papageorgiou et al., 2017; Philiastides et al., 2010; Shadlen and Kiani, 2013). The positive distractor effect may be particularly associated with vmPFC/mOFC. Activity in vmPFC/mOFC reflects the key decision variable – the difference in value between choosable options – during decision making (Basten et al., 2010; Boorman et al., 2009; Chau et al., 2014; De Martino et al., 2013; Fouragnan et al., 2019; Hunt et al., 2012; Lim et al., 2011; Lopez-Persem et al., 2016; Papageorgiou et al., 2017; Philiastides et al., 2010; Strait et al., 2014; Wunderlich et al., 2012). However, the size of the vmPFC/mOFC signal reflecting the difference in value between choosable options increases as D increases (Chau et al., 2014; Figure 4). A similar phenomenon has also been noted in vmPFC/mOFC in macaques (Fouragnan et al., 2019). Moreover, individual variation in the size of the neural effect is related to individual variation in the size of the positive distractor effect in behaviour (Figure 5 in Chau et al., 2014). In addition, it is noteworthy that lesions in vmPFC/mOFC disrupt the balance between positive and negative distractor effects. VmPFC/mOFC lesions leave both macaques and human patients more vulnerable to the disruptive effects of distractors that are predicted by divisive normalisation (Noonan et al., 2017; Noonan et al., 2010).

Both lesion and neuroimaging experiments suggest that vmPFC/mOFC is especially important in novel as opposed to over-trained decisions and in decisions involving multi-attribute options (Fellows, 2006; Hunt et al., 2012; Papageorgiou et al., 2017). It is therefore possible that the positive distractor effects seen in the current experiment and elsewhere (Fouragnan et al., 2019) may be most prominent when decisions are being made between multi-attribute options or option values that have been newly learned or which are changing. A valuable distractor slows down the decision-making process and reduces choice stochasticity.

By contrast, divisive normalisation may be particularly associated with IPS. Divisive normalisation is a useful feature for a neural network model to have if it is to adapt to contexts with different overall levels of input (Carandini and Heeger, 2012). Chau and colleagues (Figure 7 in Chau et al., 2014) reported a divisive normalisation-like decrement in performance that was associated with a decrease in the decision variable signal in IPS. Moreover, individual variation in the IPS-signal change was correlated with individual variation in the behavioural effect. Divisive normalisation has also been reported in the value signals recorded from individual neurons in IPS (Louie et al., 2011; Louie et al., 2014). IPS neurons frequently have sensorimotor fields and given the prominence of divisive normalisation effects throughout sensory brain circuits it is perhaps not surprising that normalisation is also found in value-driven signals in IPS (Louie et al., 2015). In contrast to vmPFC/mOFC, decision-related signals in IPS become more prominent rather than less prominent with practice and training (Grol et al., 2006). If divisive normalisation is indeed linked to IPS then it may become more prominent with familiarity. Other types of distractor effects may be mediated by activity changes in yet other circuits (Li et al., 2018).

Materials and methods

Summary of approach

We first conducted a series of computational modelling analyses to make predictions about the effect of distractors on choice accuracy. Then, we re-analysed empirical data from a series of experiments reported by Chau et al., 2014 and Gluth et al., 2018 and report data from three new experiments. In the first experiment, we re-analysed a data set that comes from a group of participants who performed an fMRI study. We refer to this first experiment as Experiment 1 fMRI2014. The second data set comes from Gluth and colleagues’ experiment 4 (Experiment 2 Gluth4). We focus on this data set next because it employs an identical schedule to the one used by Chau and colleagues. The third experiment again uses the same schedule but is previously unpublished. It was collected at a third site – Hong Kong – and so we refer to it as Experiment 3 Hong Kong. The fourth, fifth, and sixth experiments are re-analyses of Gluth and colleagues experiments 1, 2, and 3 and are therefore referred to as Experiment 4 Gluth1, Experiment 5 Gluth2, Experiment 6 Gluth3 respectively. An additional analysis was based on additional trials also collected at the same time as those collected for Experiment three and which again have not previously been published. The procedure used in these trials was similar to that used in all previous experiments and the schedule is very similar to that used in Experiments 1, 2, and 3. The difference between these trials and those in all earlier experiments is that participants’ choices lead them to lose rather than win money. This experiment was conducted to establish whether positive distractor effects and divisive normalisation effects were related to the value or the salience of the distractor. It is therefore referred to as Experiment 7 loss experiment. The final experiment is also previously unpublished and used a new experimental procedure that made it possible to examine eye movements. It is therefore referred to as Experiment 8 Eye Movement. Finally, we note that although we have previously shared our data with Gluth and colleagues, the arrangement was not reciprocal, and so this is the first time we have been able to analyse the data from their experiments.

Computational modelling

The mutual inhibition model is a simplified form of a biophysically plausible model that is reported elsewhere (Chau et al., 2014). It involves three pools of excitatory neurons Pi, each receives noisy input Ei from an option i (i.e. HV, LV or D option) at time t:

Ei,t~N(dVi,σ2)

where d is the drift rate, Vi is the value of option i (HV, LV or D) and σ is the standard deviation of the Gaussian noise. The noisy input of all options (HV, LV and D) are all provided simultaneously.

All excitatory neurons excite a common pool of neurons PInh that exerts inhibition It to the excitatory neurons to the same extent:

It=fikEi,tk

where k is the number of excitatory neuron pools (i.e. k=3 in this model) and f is the level of inhibition (set at 0.5).

The evidence yi,t+1 of choosing option i at time t+one follows:

yi,t+1=yi,t+(Ei,t-It)

The HV or LV option, but not the D option, is chosen when its cumulative evidence exceeds a decision threshold of 1. The reaction time is defined as the duration of the evidence accumulation before the decision threshold is reached added by a fixed non-decision time (Tnd) of 300 ms to account for lower-order cognitive processes before choice information reaches the excitatory neurons (Grasman et al., 2009; Ratcliff et al., 1999; Tuerlinckx, 2004). If no option is selected within 6 s, the trial is considered as indecisive and is omitted from the analysis.

The divisive normalisation model follows the same architecture, except that there are only two pools of excitatory neurons, and each receives normalised input from the HV or LV option. The D only participates in this model by normalising the input from the HV and LV options. The normalised input of the HV or LV option follows the following equation:

Ei,t~N(dViVHV+VLV+VD,σ2)

where d is the drift rate, Vi is the value of option i (HV or LV) and σ is the standard deviation of the Gaussian noise. The inhibition It and evidence yi,t+1 follow the same equations as the mutual inhibition model.

The dual route model involves a mutual inhibition component route and a divisive normalisation component route that run in parallel and independently. A decision is made as soon as one of the component routes contains evidence that exceeds the decision threshold.

Finally, the null model is similar to the mutual inhibition model, except that it lacks a pool of excitatory neurons that receive input from the D.

In Figure 1, the value sum HV+LV is set at [3.0, 3.2, 3.4 … 5]. The value difference HV-LV is set at [0.6, 0.8, 1.0 … 2]. The relative distractor value (D-HV) is set at [-1.2, -0.8, -0.4 … 4.8]. The simulation of each model was run for all combinations of value sum, value difference and relative distractor value and for 5000 iterations for each combination. Simulated data from each iteration were then randomly assigned to 105 “simulated participants”; this ensured that the number of simulated participants matched the total number of participants actually tested in Experiments 1-3. The levels of d and σ of each model (i.e. the mutual inhibition, divisive normalisation or dual route model) were selected in order to produce an overall choice accuracy of 0.85. For the mutual inhibition model, d, and f were set at 1.3 s−1, 1 s−1 and 0.5 respectively. For the divisive normalisation model, d, and f were set at 5.5 s−1, 0.6 s−1 and 0.5 respectively. For the dual route model, d, and f of the mutual inhibition component were set at 0.8 s−1, 0.6 s−1 and 0.5 respectively; d, and f of the divisive normalisation component were set at 1.0 s−1, 0.6 s−1 and 0.5 respectively. The proportions of indecision trials (no decision made in 6 s) are less than 0.2% in all models in Figure 1. In the models that apply fitted parameters (Figure 3—figure supplement 2) the proportions of indecision trials are 3.9% in the mutual inhibition model and less than 0.1% in the dual route, divisive normalisation, and null models.

Model fitting and comparison

The d and σ parameters of each model were fitted separately at the individual level to the choices and RTs of each participant in Experiments 1–6, using the VBA-toolbox (http://mbb-team.github.io/VBA-toolbox/). In the dual route model, the mutual inhibition and divisive normalisation components involved separate d and σ parameters during the fitting. The other model parameters (i.e. f, Tnd, Vi and k) were fixed at the values mentioned above, except for some models reported in Figure 3—figure supplement 3. The parameter estimation procedure involved a two-stage procedure that employed a Variational Bayesian analysis under the Laplace approximation (Daunizeau et al., 2014). First, an initial search was performed over a grid of fixed priors (i.e. with zero variance) with d at [0.01, 0.1, 1, 10] and σ at [0.1, 1]. Second, the set of priors that shows the best fit (highest log-evidence) was selected, assigned with a variance of 1, and fitted to participants’ behaviour.

To compare the goodness-of-fit between the models, the log-model evidence of all models and all participants were then tested using a group-level random-effect Bayesian model selection (BMS) procedure (Penny et al., 2010). BMS estimates the exceedance probability (xp) that indicates how likely a model is the best fit model compared to other competing models in the population from which participants were drawn. The same model comparison procedures were repeated by including participants from the same experiment only.

Human participants

A total of 243 participants participated in the experiments including 21 (9 female; age range 19–34 years) in Experiment 1, 44 (36 female; age range 18–46) in Experiment 2, 40 (20 female; age range 18–27) in Experiment 3, 23 (14 female; age range 18–54) in Experiment 4, 49 (24 female; age range 19–46) in Experiment 5, 31 (21 female; age range 20–47) in Experiment 6, 40 in Experiment 7, who were the same participants as in Experiment 3, and 35 (20 female; age range 19–42) in Experiment 8. Eye movement data were collected from 25 of these 35 participants in Experiment 8. Experiments 3, 7 and 8 were approved by ethics committee of The Hong Kong Polytechnic University and Experiment one was approved by that of University of Oxford.

We conducted a posteriori power analysis to confirm that the sample sizes of the experiments were adequate. All analyses were conducted using criteria of power = 80% and alpha = 0.05 two tailed. Experiments 2–6 were replicates of Experiment one in which they all tested whether there was a difficulty-dependent distractor effect via the (HV-LV)(D-HV) term in GLM1a. In Experiment 1, the effect size in Cohen’s d for the (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect was d = 0.790. The required sample size was calculated at 13 participants and Experiments 1–6 all involved larger samples. Experiment seven was conducted simultaneously with Experiment three in which it tested the salience-based effect of the distractor via the |D| term in GLM6a. The effect size in Cohen’s d for the |D| effect was d = 0.514. The required sample size was calculated at 30 participants and the sample of Experiment seven exceeded this size. Experiment eight examined the impact of attentional capture on decision making. One key analysis was to examine whether more gaze shifts from D to HV were related to greater accuracies (GLM9), in which the effect size in Cohen’s d was 0.607. The required sample size was calculated at 22 participants. The sample size of Experiment 8, after excluding four participants using the inclusion criterion of data validity >85% (see Eye tracking experiment procedures), was one participant less than this number estimated in a posteriori power analysis. However, when we relaxed the inclusion criterion to >70% data validity, a total of 22 participants were included and the results remained similar (Figure 8 and Figure 8—figure supplement 1).

Multi-attribute decision-making task (Experiments 1–7)

The experimental task used in Experiment one has previously been described by Chau et al., 2014 as follows (Figure 1). Participants chose repeatedly between stimuli associated with different reward magnitudes (£2, £4, £6, £8, £10, £12) and probabilities (12.5%, 25%. 37.5%, 50%, 62.5%, 75%, 87.5%), represented by colors (red to blue) and orientations (0° to 90°) of rectangular bars. However, associations between visual features and decision variables were counterbalanced across subjects. Participants were presented with 150 trials of two-option trials (in which no distractor was presented) and 150 distractor trials (300 trials in total) randomly interleaved. All the option value configurations in the two-option trials were matched with the available options in the distractor trials.

Each trial began with a central fixation cross indicating an inter-trial interval (3–6 s) followed by an initial phase in which two (two-option trials) or three (distractor trials) stimuli were presented in randomly selected screen quadrants. The initial phase was brief – only 0.1 s. Then, in the decision phase, orange boxes were presented around two stimuli indicating those options were available for choice. In distractor trials, a purple box was also presented around the third stimulus to indicate a distractor. Subjects were instructed to select one of the available options within 1.5 s. Subjects were warned they were ‘too slow’ if no response was made within 1.5 s and a new trial began. After an option was chosen, the box surrounding it turned red in the interval phase (1–3 s). Then the edge of each stimulus turned either yellow or grey in the outcome phase to indicate, respectively, whether the choice had been rewarded or not (1–3 s). The final reward allocated to the subject on leaving the experiment was calculated by averaging the outcome of all trials. Subjects learned the task and visual feature associations and experienced Distractor trials in a practice session before scanning. At the end of practice, all subjects chose HV on >70% of two-option trials when it was associated with both higher reward magnitude and probability.

We provided incentives for subjects to attend to the visual features of every stimulus by interleaving ‘catch’ trials between decision phase and interval phase in 15% of all the trials. In this way we ensured that it was unlikely that subjects would ignore the distractor values. In a ‘catch’ trial, the word ‘MATCH’ was presented once subjects selected an option in the decision phase (1 s). Then, an exemplar stimulus was presented at the center of the screen and subjects had to indicate, within 2 s, the position of the same stimulus presented before and during the decision phase. Feedback was then given to indicate whether the response was correct or not (1.5 s). The trial then continued with the resumption of the interval phase, followed by the outcome phase. Each correct response in the ‘catch’ trial added an extra 10 pence to the final reward.

A very similar procedure was used in Experiment three with the exception that the reward magnitudes were presented in Hong Kong dollar ($25, $50, $75, $100, $125, $150). A very similar approach was used in Experiment 7. Participants performed trials that were identical to those in Experiment three with the exception that now participants ran the risk of losing rather than gaining money of the same amount. The same participants were involved in both Experiments 3 and 7 in randomised order. The final reward was calculated by the average gain in Experiment three deducted by the average loss in Experiment 7.

Similar approaches were used in Experiments 2, 4, 5, and six reported by Gluth et al., 2018 with the exception that the reward magnitudes were presented in Swiss Francs (CHF2, CHF4, CHF6, CHF8, CHF10, CHF12). In Experiments 4–6, 56 additional ‘novel’ distractor trials were introduced to test the presence of any decoy effects in their study. The same number of two-option trials with identical HV and LV options but lacking the distractor were also included.

Eye tracking experiment procedures

The behavioural paradigm used in the eye tracking experiment (Experiment 8 Eye Movement) was adapted from that of our 2014 study (Chau et al., 2014). In this experiment, the choosable and distractor options were represented by circles and heptagons respectively (Figure 7a). Each choosable option was composed of a combination of two circles and the distractor consisted of two heptagons. The reward magnitude of an option was indicated by the color of one of the component shapes, whereas the angle of the tick on the other component shape indicated the option’s reward probability (Figure 7b). The screen was divided into four quadrants and each quadrant had two positions for presenting a pair of stimuli associated with an option, making up a total of eight positions. The eight positions were all 291 pixels from the center of the screen and equally separated.

The experiment consisted of 150 Distractor Trials (two options available for choice and one distractor, unavailable for choice, were presented) and 150 Two-Option Trials (two choosable options in the absence of a distractor were presented) in randomised order. On each Distractor Trial, a fixation cross was presented for 1 to 2 s. Then two available options and one distractor option were presented on three of the four quadrants of the screen and a choice had to be made within 5 s by pressing one of the four buttons corresponding to the four quadrants. The frame of the circles related to the chosen option then turned red in color for 1 to 1.5 s. Finally, a circle was presented at the center of the screen for 1 to 1.5 s, indicating whether the chosen option had led to a reward (the color of the circle matched with that of the chosen option) or not (a grey circle).

Eye movement data were recorded using a Tobii TX300 eye tracker with a sampling rate of 300 Hz. The screen attached to the eye tracker was 23 inch in size, 16:9 aspect ratio and 1920 × 1080 pixels resolution. Each circle/heptagon stimulus associated with an option was 256 × 256 pixel in size. Eye movement data were recorded from both eyes. The dominant eye of each subject was determined using the Miles test and only data from the dominant eye was analysed. Validity of each sample was determined by the Tobii Pro SDK software package. Eye movement data of a subject were excluded when the average validity across all samples was below 85% and so eye movement data from four of the initial 25 subjects tested were not analysed further (although the behavioural data from all 25 subjects were still analysed together with the behavioural data from 10 participants used to pilot the behavioural paradigm). Only data recorded during the decision phase were analysed. Trials where average validity within a trial was below 85% was excluded from analysis. On each trial, an area-of-interest (AOIs) of 300 × 300 pixel were drawn surrounding each option stimulus. The size of an AOI was slightly larger than the size of a stimulus (256 × 256 pixel) to account for any minor calibration error. A fixation was defined when the gaze stayed within an AOI for more than 50 ms.

Analysis procedures

Behaviour was analysed using a series of GLMs containing the following regressors:

  • GLM1a: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(D-HV) + β3 z(HV-LV) z(D-HV) + ε

  • GLM1b: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + β3 z(D-HV) + β4 z(HV-LV) z(D-HV) + ε

  • GLM1c: ln(PHV/Pj) = βj,0 + βj,1 z(HV-LV) + βj,2 z(D-HV) + βj,3 z(HV-LV) z(D-HV) + εj

where HV, LV, and D refer to the values of the higher value choosable option, the lower value choosable option, and the distractor respectively. PHV and Pj refer to the probability of choosing the HV option or option j. j = [LV, D]. ε refers to the unexplained error. GLM1a and GLM1c only differed by that the regressions were binomial and multinomial respectively. z(x) refers to z-scoring of term x, which is applied to all terms in all GLMs in this study. In addition, all interaction terms in all GLMs are calculated after the component terms are z-scored.

GLMs2a-c involved a stepwise procedure to partial out the effects of HV and LV from the choice accuracy data before the distractor effects were tested on the residual ε0 in the second step:

  • GLM2a: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + ε1

  • Step 2, ε1 = β3 + β4 z(D-HV) + ε2

  • GLM2b:

  • Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + ε1

  • Step 2, ε1 = β3 + β4 z(D) + ε2

  • GLM2c: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(Difficulty) + ε1

  • Step 2, ε1 = β2 + β3 z(D-HV) + ε2

  • GLM2d: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(Difficulty) + ε1

  • Step 2, ε1 = β2 + β3 z(D) + ε2

Where in GLM2c,d Difficulty = w1 z[Mag(HV)-Mag(LV)) + w2 z[Mag(HV)+Mag(LV)) +w3 z[Prob(HV)-Prob(LV)] + w4 z[Prob(HV)+Prob(LV)] (HV)/Prob(HV) and Mag(LV)/Prob(LV) are the reward magnitude/probability of the HV and LV options respectively. Difficulty is the weight sum of the differences and sums between the attributes of HV and LV option. The weights w1, w2, w3, w4 are extracted from GLM 3b (i.e. β1, β2, β3, and β4 in GLM3b respectively).

  • GLM3a: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + ε

  • GLM3b: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z[Mag(HV)-Mag(LV)) + β 2 z[Mag(HV)+Mag(LV)] + β 3 z[Prob(HV)-Prob(LV)]4 z[Prob(HV)+Prob(LV)] + ε

  • GLM4: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(SubjDiff) + β2 z(Congruence) + β3 z(D-HV)4 z(SubjDiff) z(D-HV) + ε

SubjDiff refers to the subjective difficulty estimate best explaining behaviour in each experiment. SubjDiff is defined simply as HV-LV in Experiment 1 fMRI2014 and Experiment 3 Hong Kong and as the weighted sum of probability and magnitude differences in Experiment two and Experiments 4, 5, 6 (Gluth1-4). Congruence is a binary variable indexing whether or not both probability and magnitude components of one option are better than the probability and magnitude components of the other options. In contrast to congruent trials, the probability of one option is higher and the magnitude of the other option is higher on incongruent trials.

  • GLM5: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + β3 z(D) + β4 z(HV-LV) z(D) + β5 z(HV+LV) z(D) + β6 z(HV-LV) z(HV+LV) + β7 z(HV-LV) z(HV+LV) z(D) + ε

  • GLM6a: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(GainTrial) + β2 z(HV-LV) + β3 z(D) + β4 z(HV-LV) z(D) + β5 z(|D|) +

  • β5 z(HV-LV) z(|D|) + ε

  • GLM6b: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(GainTrial) + β2 z(HV-LV) + β3 z(HV+LV) + ε1

  • Step 2, ε1 = β4 + β5 z(D) + ε2

  • GLM6c: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(GainTrial) + β2 z(HV-LV) + β3 z(HV+LV) + ε1

  • Step 2, ε1 = β4 + β5 z(|D|) + ε2

GainTrial is a binary variable indexing whether a given trial in the Experiment 3/Experiment seven data set was a trial on which participants could potentially win or lose money.

  • GLM7: Fixj = βj,0 + βj,1 z(HV-LV) + βj,2 z(HV+LV) + βj,3 z(D) + εj where Fixj refers to the frequency of fixation on an option. j = [HV, LV, D].

  • GLM8: Step 1, Shiftj = βj,0 + βj,1 z[Fix(HV)] + βj,2 z[Fix(LV)] + βj,3 z[Fix(D)] + εj,1

  • Step 2, εj,1 = βj,4 + βj,5 z(HV) + βj,6 z(LV) + βj,7 z(D) + εj,2

  • where Shiftj refers to the frequency of bidirectional gaze shifts between two options. j=[HV-LV, HV-D, LV-D].

  • GLM9: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + β3 z(D) + ε1

  • Step 2, ε1 = β4 + β5 z[Shift(D-to-HV)] + β6 z[Shift(D-to-LV)] + β7 z[Shift(HV-to-D)] + β8 z[Shift(LV-to-D)] +

  • β9 z[Shift(LV-to-HV)] + β10 z[Shift(HV-to-LV)] + ε2

The autocorrelations between regressors and variance inflation factors of each regressor in each GLM is shown in Figure 9 and Figure 9—figure supplement 1 respectively. These analyses were run using the Statistics and Machine Learning toolbox of Matlab 2018a (The MathWorks, Inc, USA).

Figure 9. Correlations between regressors.

Each plot shows the levels of correlation between regressors in the general linear models used. Correlation is presented as Pearson’s r averaged across participants.

Figure 9.

Figure 9—figure supplement 1. Variance inflation factor of each regressor.

Figure 9—figure supplement 1.

Each plot shows the levels of variance inflation factor (VIF) of each regressor in the general linear models used.

Choice simulation procedures

We ran Simulations 1–3 to test whether any difficulty-dependent distractor effect emerged as a result of statistical artefact. In Stimulations 1 and 2, simulated choices were generated by incorporating the effect of HV-LV only. Then GLM1a was applied to analyse the accuracy data of the simulated choices to test whether the D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV) effects could be found when these effects were supposed to be absent in the simulated choice. The simulations involved the same number of hypothetical participants as in Experiments 1–6 (i.e. n = 208) and the simulation of each hypothetical participant was iterated for 1000 times. In Simulation 1, the effect of HV-LV that was used to generate the simulated choice in each hypothetical participant was estimated from the choices of one actual participant using GLM1a, where D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV) were also present in the model. In Simulation 2, the effect of HV-LV was estimated from another GLM where only the HV-LV term was present.

Simulation three was run as a positive control analysis to confirm that the distractor effects should be present in the data when they are introduced during the generation of the simulated choices. Similar procedures to those used in Simulations 1 and 2 were employed to generate the simulated choices, however, now all the effects of the terms present in GLM1a, i.e. HV-LV, D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV), were incorporated.

Data availability

The codes for running the mutual inhibition model, divisive normalisation model, dual route model and null model can be found at: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.k6djh9w3c. Behavioural data of Experiments 1, 3 and 7 can be found at: https://datadryad.org/stash/dataset/doi:10.5061/dryad.040h9t7. Behavioural data of Experiments 2, 4–6 can be found from a link provided by Gluth et al., 2018 at: https://osf.io/8r4fh/. Behavioural and eye tracking data of Experiment eight can be found at: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.k6djh9w3c.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Hong Kong Research Grants Council (25610316), Wellcome Trust (WT100973AIA; 203139/Z/16/Z) and Medical Research Council (MR/P024955/1).

Appendix 1

Simulation suggests that the difficulty-dependent distractor effect was not due to any statistical artefact

To test whether the (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect, for example in GLM1a, could emerge as a statistical artefact if it were absent in a hypothetical situation, we simulated three sets of choice data based on these GLMs. We ran Simulations 1 and 2 where simulated choices were generated using a GLM including only the empirically measured HV-LV effect size as a coefficient estimate. The simulations employed the same set of trials as those performed by the participants. Simulations 1 and 2 differ in whether the effect size of HV-LV is estimated by GLM1a or by a similar GLM in which HV-LV is the only regressor. In both cases, the simulated choices should only contain the HV-LV effect and any distractor effect observed in these simulated choices should be considered a statistical artefact. When we applied GLM1a to analyse the simulated choices, we found a significant HV-LV effect (Simulation 1: β = 0.534, t207 = 20.970, p<10−52; Simulation 2: β = 0.577, t207 = 22.337, p<10−56). Importantly, we found no D-HV effect (Simulation 1: β = −0.009, t207 = −0.583, p=0.561; Simulation 2: β = −0.004, t207 = −0.216, p=0.830) and (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect (Simulation 1: β = 0.010, t207 = 0.537, p=0.592; Simulation 2: β = 0.007, t207 = 0.410, p=0.682).

In addition, a complementary test was performed to confirm that distractor effects should be detectable with the GLMs that were used if the distractor effect is introduced into the simulated choice data. In order to do this, we ran Simulation 3 that incorporated effects of all terms in GLM1a with effect sizes corresponding to those that had been empirically observed, that is HV-LV, D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV), while the simulated choices were generated. When re-fitting the choices produced by this simulation, as expected, we found a positive HV-LV effect (β = 0.545, t207 = 18.432, p<10−44), positive D-HV effect (β = 0.149, t207 = 5.564, p<10−7) and negative (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect (β = −0.108, t207 = −4.129, p<10−4). Thus, these simulations confirm that the difficulty-dependent distractor effect was not due to any statistical artefact (Palminteri et al., 2017).

Moreover, we conducted additional simulations to test whether Type I error is inflated in GLM1a. We repeated Simulation 1 (which assumes only an HV-LV, but no D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV), effect in the accuracy data) for 1000 iterations and estimated the p value of each regressor in each iteration. Appendix 1—figure 1a shows the cumulative distribution of p values of each regressor for all 1000 iterations. Since the statistical threshold is set at p=0.05, to determine the likelihood of Type I errors of D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV) it is critical to examine the proportion of simulations that show p<0.05 (at the dotted line in Appendix 1—figure 1a). The result show that the Type I error of D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV) are 5.6% and 5.1% respectively. Similarly, the Type II error of the HV-LV term can be determined by examining the proportion of iterations with p>0.05 (i.e. 1-cumulative probability at Simulated p=0.05; Appendix 1—figure 1a). The results show that Type II error of the HV-LV term is 0%. These results are summarized in Supplementary file 1B, GLM1a.

Appendix 1—figure 1. No clear indication of inflated statistical artefact in GLM1a.

Appendix 1—figure 1.

GLM1a includes regressors HV-LV, D-HV and (HV-LV)(D–HV) to predict choice accuracy. (a) In Simulation 1, choice accuracy data was simulated using the effect size of HV-LV estimated by GLM1a from actual participants and assuming that the D-HV and (HV-LV)(D–HV) effects are absent. Then GLM1a was applied again to analyse the simulated choice accuracy data. In 1000 iterations, the chance of obtaining a significant effect (p<0.05; at the dashed line) from the HV-LV term is 100% (i.e. 0% Type II error; orange). For the D-HV and (HV-LV)(D–HV) terms, for which no effect is assumed, the chances of obtaining Type I errors are 5.6% and 5.1% respectively (yellow and purple respectively). (b) Simulation 2 is similar to Simulation 1, except that the effect size of HV-LV was estimated from the empirical data using a GLM that only includes the HV-LV term. (c) Simulation 3 assumes that HV-LV, D-HV and (HV-LV)(D–HV) effects are all present in the simulated choice accuracy data. The chances of obtaining a significant effect are 0%, 0.1% and 4.7% respectively. Note that in (a) and (b) the blue and orange lines overlap with each other; in (c) the blue, orange and yellow lines overlap with each other.

In a similar manner, we performed these additional simulations for Simulation 2. As before, these two simulations only differ in how the effect of HV-LV is introduced into the simulated accuracy data. In Simulation 1 it was estimated by applying GLM1a to participants’ empirical data. In Simulation 2 it was estimated by applying a GLM with only the HV-LV term to participants’ empirical data. The results show that Type I errors of D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV) are 4.1% and 5.4% respectively and the Type II error of HV-LV is 0% (Appendix 1—figure 1b; also see Supplementary file 1C, GLM1a).

Finally, the additional simulations for Simulation 3 assume that effects of HV-LV, D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV) all exist in the accuracy data. The probabilities of successfully detecting a significant effect of HV-LV, D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV) are 100%, 99.8% and 97.1% respectively (Appendix 1—figure 1c; also see Supplementary file 1D, GLM1a). In other words, Type II errors of these terms are 0%, 0.2% and 2.9% respectively.

Similar simulation procedures are performed with GLMs 1b and 5 and 6 and there is no clear indication that the distractor effects (i.e. the effects of the D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV) terms) are driven by statistical artefact. The results are summarized in Supplementary file 1B–D.

Appendix 2

Control analyses suggested that the difficulty-dependent distractor effect was not due to any statistical artefact

In another set of control analyses we focused on control trials where only two options, but no distractors, were presented. In two sets of experiments, fMRI2014 and Gluth1-4, an equal number of precisely matched trials were collected in which HV and LV options with exactly the same probabilities and magnitudes (and thus values) were employed but no distractor was presented. Thus the same participants chose between the same HV and LV options and a ‘notional distractor’ of the same value – D – was assigned to the trial but not actually presented. Unlike in the previous analysis, if there is no collinearity between the interaction term and the main effect of difficulty (HV-LV), then there should now be a stronger (Difficulty)(D-HV) interaction effect on distractor trials than the matched ‘two-choice’ trials. This is what Chau et al., 2014 examined in their analysis SI.3 and thus we applied a very similar analysis (GLM4) to test the data from fMRI2014 and Gluth1-4. When difficulty was defined as HV-LV in fMRI2014, the distractor-difficulty interaction effect (Difficulty)(D-HV) was stronger on distractor trials than two-choice trials (t109 = 2.381, p=0.0279). Similarly, when difficulty was defined as the weighted sum of probability and magnitude differences in Gluth1-4 the (Difficulty)(D-HV) effect was also stronger on distractor trials than two-choice trials (t109 = 2.314, p=0.023). Note that there were no ‘two-choice’ trials in Experiment 3 Hong Kong and so it is not possible to run this analysis with those data.

Appendix 3

A more complete analysis of distractor effects

In order to examine distractor effects as a function of both difficulty (HV-LV) and the integrated value of the options (HV+LV+D), we plotted the effect of the distractor on accuracy (rather than accuracy itself as in Figure 3) as a function of both HV-LV and HV+LV in the dual route model and Experiments 1 to3 (Appendix 3—figure 1). Again, in both model’s simulated choices and participants’ actual choice a positive distractor effect (yellow area) is most prominent on the hard trials at the bottom of Appendix 3—figure 1a-d while the negative distractor effect (blue area) is most prominent on the easy trials at the top of these figures. As predicted by divisive normalization models the negative distractor effect is most prominent at the left of these figures when the total sum of HV and LV is small.

Hence, rather than just looking at the (HV-LV)D interaction, a more complete three-way GLM (GLM5, Materials and methods) can be performed which also includes the (HV+LV)D term. For completeness we include the other possible interactions terms such as (HV-LV)(HV+LV) and (HV-LV)(HV+LV)D as well. Now the opposite effects exerted by the distractor as a function of difficulty (HV-LV) manifest as a negative (HV-LV)D interaction term; the distractor has a positive effect on decision accuracy when decisions are difficult but a negative effect when decisions are easy just as was seen in previous analyses (Figures 3 and 4 and Figure 4—figure supplement 1). The divisive normalization model also predicts that the negative impact of D is reduced when HV+LV becomes larger and hence the positive distractor effect predicted by a cortical attractor or diffusion model should become predominant. This manifests as a positive (HV+LV)D interaction term. These interaction effects are clearly present in the data of all six experiments that we have surveyed above; there is a negative effect of the (HV-LV)D interaction term (β = −0.116, t207 = −4.262, p<10−4; Appendix 3—figure 2a) and a positive effect of the (HV+LV)D interaction term (β = 0.414, t207 = 13.066, p<10−28). All experiments showed the same trend in these key interaction terms. A one-way ANOVA showed that they were comparable in size across experiments [Appendix 3—figure 2b; (HV-LV)D: F5,202=1.524, p=0.184; (HV+LV)D: F5,202=0.625, p=0.681]. Finally, a follow-up analysis was run to confirm how the D effect varied as a function of HV-LV or HV+LV. These questions related to the (HV-LV)D and (HV+LV)D interaction effects respectively. We applied a mean split by HV-LV and then estimated the effect of D using GLM2b. On hard trials (small HV-LV), larger D values were related to greater choice accuracies (β = 0.056, t207 = 4.113, p<10−4); whereas on easy trials (large HV-LV), larger D values were related to poorer choice accuracies (β = −0.007, t206 = −2.049, p=0.042; note that one participant was excluded from this analysis because of the lack of behavioral variance – there was only one inaccurate choice). On trials with a small HV+LV sum, larger D values were associated with poorer choice accuracies (β = −0.009, t207 = −2.530, p=0.012); whereas on trial with large HV+LV sums, larger D values were only marginally associated with greater choice accuracies (β = 0.006, t207 = 1.810, p=0.072).

Appendix 3—figure 1. Distractor effects measured across the decision space.

Appendix 3—figure 1.

As in Figure 1, the distractor effect (yellow = positive, blue = negative) is plotted across the decision space defined by difficulty (HV-LV) and the total value of the choosable options (HV+LV). Results are shown for (a) the dual route model, (b) Experiment 1 fMRI2014, (c) Experiment 2 Gluth4, (d) Experiment 3 Hong Kong.

Appendix 3—figure 2. The distractor effect was modulated as a function of difficulty and the total value of the chooseable options.

Appendix 3—figure 2.

(a) Here the data have been analysed with a more complete GLM (GLM5) including all main effects and two- and three-way interactions. Across all experiments the (HV-LV)D interaction is negative – the distractor has a positive effect on decision accuracy when decisions are difficult but a negative effect when decisions are easy. This resembles the effects illustrated in Figures 35. Moreover, across all experiments the (HV+LV)D interaction is positive – when the total value of the choosable options changed from large to small the distractor effect also changed from positive to negative. The divisive normalization model predicts that the negative impact of D is greatest when HV+LV is small (Figure 1) but its impact is reduced when HV+LV is large and so the positive distractor effect predicted by the interacting diffusion process model predominates when HV+LV is large. (b) The same two key interaction terms, (HV-LV)D and (HV+LV)D, indexing the positive distractor and divisive normalization effects are illustrated for each component experiment that was included in panel a. ***p<0.001. Error bars indicate standard error.

Finally, we found that there was a significant (HV-LV)(HV+LV)D effect in GLM5 (β = 0.362, t207 = 10.417, p<10−19; Appendix 3—figure 2a). Next we examined how the (HV+LV)D effect or (HV-LV)D effect varied as a function of the third variable (HV-LV). One way to examine this is to look at simple effects in sub-sections of the data but because we are now considering a three way interaction, the necessary subsection may be only a quarter in size of the original data, which could produce unreliable results. Hence, we took an alternative approach by using the beta weights estimated in GLM5 and investigating the three-way interaction effect. In particular, small and large HV-LV (or HV+LV) was defined as the 25th percentile (z score = −0.675) and 75th percentile (z score = 0.675) respectively. Then we tested the two-way (HV+LV)D effect at small and large HV-LV:

  • GLM5: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + β3 z(D) + β4 z(HV-LV) z(D) + β5 z(HV+LV) z(D) + β6 z(HV-LV) z(HV+LV) + β7 z(HV-LV) z(HV+LV) z(D) + ε

  • GLM5 at small HV-LV: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 (−0.675) + β3 z(D) + β4 (−0.675) z(D) + β5 z(HV+LV) z(D) + β6 (−0.675) z(HV+LV) + β7 (−0.675) z(HV+LV) z(D) + ε

  • GLM5 at large HV-LV: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 (0.675) + β3 z(D) + β4 (0.675) z(D) + β5 z(HV+LV) z(D) + β6 (0.675) z(HV+LV) + β7 (0.675) z(HV+LV) z(D) + ε

As such, the effect of (HV+LV)D at small HV-LV is (β5 - 0.675β7) and that at large HV-LV is (β5 + 0.675β7). The analysis showed that at small HV-LV there was a positive (HV+LV)D effect (β = 0.169, t207 = 5.656, p<10−7) and at large HV-LV the (HV+LV)D effect was even more positive (β = 0.659, t207 = 13.999, p<10−31). A similar procedure was applied to test the (HV-LV)D effect at small or large HV+LV. The results showed that at small HV+LV there was a negative (HV-LV)D effect (β = −0.361, t207 = −9.208, p<10−16), whereas at large HV+LV there was a positive (HV-LV)D effect (β = 0.128, t207 = 3.953, p<10−3).

We tested the effect of D at different levels of HV-LV and HV+LV. When both HV-LV and HV+LV was small, there was a lack of D effect (β = −0.023, t207 = −0.778, p=0.437). When HV-LV was small and HV+LV was large, there was a positive D effect (β = 0.206, t207 = 7.119, p<10−10). When HV-LV was large and HV+LV was small, there was a negative D effect (β = −0.510, t207 = −11.414, p<10−22). When HV-LV was large and HV+LV was large, there was a positive D effect (β = 0.379, t207 = 9.606, p<10−17).

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Bolton KH Chau, Email: boltonchau@gmail.com.

Thorsten Kahnt, Northwestern University, United States.

Joshua I Gold, University of Pennsylvania, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Research Grants Council, University Grants Committee 25610316 to Bolton K H Chau.

  • Wellcome WT100973AIA to Matthew F S Rushworth.

  • Wellcome 203139/Z/16/Z to Matthew F S Rushworth.

  • Medical Research Council MR/P024955/1 to Matthew F S Rushworth.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Human subjects: Experiments 3, 7 and 8 were approved by ethics committee of The Hong Kong Polytechnic University and Experiment 1 was approved by that of University of Oxford.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Supplementary tables.

(A) Regression coefficients estimated using the analysis procedures suggested by Gluth et al., 2018. (B) Simulation 1: testing the probabilities of Type I and II errors when distractor effects were assumed to be absent in the simulated choice data. (C) Simulation 2: an alternative approach for testing the probabilities of Type I and II errors when distractor effects were assumed to be absent in the simulated choice data. (D) Simulation 3: testing the probabilities of Type I and II errors when distractor effects were assumed to be present in the simulated choice data.

elife-53850-supp1.docx (33.7KB, docx)
Transparent reporting form

Data availability

The codes for running the mutual inhibition model, divisive normalization model, dual route model and null model can be found at: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.k6djh9w3c Behavioural data of Experiments 1, 3 and 7 can be found at: https://datadryad.org/stash/dataset/doi:10.5061/dryad.040h9t7 Behavioural data of Experiments 2, 4-6 can be found from a link provided by Gluth and colleagues (2018) at: https://osf.io/8r4fh/ Behavioural and eye tracking data of Experiment 8 can be found at: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.k6djh9w3c.

The following dataset was generated:

Chau BKH, Law C, Lopez-Persem A, Klein-Flügge MC, Rushworth MFS. 2019. Consistent patterns of distractor effects during decision making. Dryad Digital Repository.

The following previously published datasets were used:

Chau BKH, Kolling N. 2018. Data from: A neural mechanism underlying failure of optimal choice with multiple alternatives. Dryad Digital Repository.

Spektor MS, Gluth S, Rieskamp J. 2018. Value-based attentional capture affects multi-alternative decision making. Open Science Framework. 8r4fh

References

  1. Basten U, Biele G, Heekeren HR, Fiebach CJ. How the brain integrates costs and benefits during decision making. PNAS. 2010;107:21767–21772. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908104107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Boorman ED, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Rushworth MF. How green is the grass on the other side? frontopolar cortex and the evidence in favor of alternative courses of action. Neuron. 2009;62:733–743. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Carandini M, Heeger DJ. Normalization as a canonical neural computation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13:51–62. doi: 10.1038/nrn3136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Chau BK, Kolling N, Hunt LT, Walton ME, Rushworth MF. A neural mechanism underlying failure of optimal choice with multiple alternatives. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:463–470. doi: 10.1038/nn.3649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Daunizeau J, Adam V, Rigoux L. VBA: a probabilistic treatment of nonlinear models for neurobiological and behavioural data. PLOS Computational Biology. 2014;10:e1003441. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. De Martino B, Fleming SM, Garrett N, Dolan RJ. Confidence in value-based choice. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:105–110. doi: 10.1038/nn.3279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Farashahi S, Donahue CH, Hayden BY, Lee D, Soltani A. Flexible combination of reward information across primates. Nature Human Behaviour. 2019;3:1215–1224. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0714-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Fellows LK. Deciding how to decide: ventromedial frontal lobe damage affects information acquisition in multi-attribute decision making. Brain. 2006;129:944–952. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. FitzGerald TH, Seymour B, Dolan RJ. The role of human orbitofrontal cortex in value comparison for incommensurable objects. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:8388–8395. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0717-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Fouragnan EF, Chau BKH, Folloni D, Kolling N, Verhagen L, Klein-Flügge M, Tankelevitch L, Papageorgiou GK, Aubry JF, Sallet J, Rushworth MFS. The macaque anterior cingulate cortex translates counterfactual choice value into actual behavioral change. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:797–808. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0375-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Glimcher P. Decisions, decisions, decisions: choosing a biological science of choice. Neuron. 2002;36:323–332. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00962-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gluth S, Spektor MS, Rieskamp J. Value-based attentional capture affects multi-alternative decision making. eLife. 2018;7:e39659. doi: 10.7554/eLife.39659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gold JI, Shadlen MN. The neural basis of decision making. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2007;30:535–574. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Grasman RPPP, Wagenmakers E-J, van der Maas HLJ. On the mean and variance of response times under the diffusion model with an application to parameter estimation. Journal of Mathematical Psychology. 2009;53:55–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2009.01.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Grol MJ, de Lange FP, Verstraten FA, Passingham RE, Toni I. Cerebral changes during performance of overlearned arbitrary visuomotor associations. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:117–125. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2786-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hanks TD, Ditterich J, Shadlen MN. Microstimulation of macaque area LIP affects decision-making in a motion discrimination task. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:682–689. doi: 10.1038/nn1683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hunt LT, Kolling N, Soltani A, Woolrich MW, Rushworth MF, Behrens TE. Mechanisms underlying cortical activity during value-guided choice. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:470–476. doi: 10.1038/nn.3017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hunt LT, Malalasekera WMN, de Berker AO, Miranda B, Farmer SF, Behrens TEJ, Kennerley SW. Triple dissociation of attention and decision computations across prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:1471–1481. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0239-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hunt LT, Hayden BY. A distributed, hierarchical and recurrent framework for reward-based choice. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2017;18:172–182. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Jocham G, Hunt LT, Near J, Behrens TE. A mechanism for value-guided choice based on the excitation-inhibition balance in prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:960–961. doi: 10.1038/nn.3140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jocham G, Furlong PM, Kröger IL, Kahn MC, Hunt LT, Behrens TE. Dissociable contributions of ventromedial prefrontal and posterior parietal cortex to value-guided choice. NeuroImage. 2014;100:498–506. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kahnt T, Park SQ, Haynes JD, Tobler PN. Disentangling neural representations of value and salience in the human brain. PNAS. 2014;111:5000–5005. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1320189111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kennerley SW, Dahmubed AF, Lara AH, Wallis JD. Neurons in the frontal lobe encode the value of multiple decision variables. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:1162–1178. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Li V, Michael E, Balaguer J, Herce Castañón S, Summerfield C. Gain control explains the effect of distraction in human perceptual, cognitive, and economic decision making. PNAS. 2018;115:E8825–E8834. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1805224115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lim SL, O'Doherty JP, Rangel A. The decision value computations in the vmPFC and striatum use a relative value code that is guided by visual attention. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:13214–13223. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1246-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lopez-Persem A, Domenech P, Pessiglione M. How prior preferences determine decision-making frames and biases in the human brain. eLife. 2016;5:e20317. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Louie K, Grattan LE, Glimcher PW. Reward value-based gain control: divisive normalization in parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:10627–10639. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1237-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Louie K, Khaw MW, Glimcher PW. Normalization is a general neural mechanism for context-dependent decision making. PNAS. 2013;110:6139–6144. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1217854110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Louie K, LoFaro T, Webb R, Glimcher PW. Dynamic divisive normalization predicts time-varying value coding in decision-related circuits. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:16046–16057. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2851-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Louie K, Glimcher PW, Webb R. Adaptive neural coding: from biological to behavioral decision-making. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2015;5:91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Meder D, Kolling N, Verhagen L, Wittmann MK, Scholl J, Madsen KH, Hulme OJ, Behrens TEJ, Rushworth MFS. Simultaneous representation of a spectrum of dynamically changing value estimates during decision making. Nature Communications. 2017;8:1942. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02169-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Noonan MP, Walton ME, Behrens TE, Sallet J, Buckley MJ, Rushworth MF. Separate value comparison and learning mechanisms in macaque medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex. PNAS. 2010;107:20547–20552. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1012246107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Noonan MP, Chau BKH, Rushworth MFS, Fellows LK. Contrasting Effects of Medial and Lateral Orbitofrontal Cortex Lesions on Credit Assignment and Decision-Making in Humans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:7023–7035. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0692-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. O'Shea J, Johansen-Berg H, Trief D, Göbel S, Rushworth MF. Functionally specific reorganization in human premotor cortex. Neuron. 2007;54:479–490. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.04.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Palminteri S, Wyart V, Koechlin E. The importance of falsification in computational cognitive modeling. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2017;21:425–433. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Papageorgiou GK, Sallet J, Wittmann MK, Chau BKH, Schüffelgen U, Buckley MJ, Rushworth MFS. Inverted activity patterns in ventromedial prefrontal cortex during value-guided decision-making in a less-is-more task. Nature Communications. 2017;8:1886. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01833-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Penny WD, Stephan KE, Daunizeau J, Rosa MJ, Friston KJ, Schofield TM, Leff AP. Comparing families of dynamic causal models. PLOS Computational Biology. 2010;6:e1000709. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Philiastides MG, Biele G, Heekeren HR. A mechanistic account of value computation in the human brain. PNAS. 2010;107:9430–9435. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001732107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Platt ML, Glimcher PW. Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature. 1999;400:233–238. doi: 10.1038/22268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ratcliff R, Van Zandt T, McKoon G. Connectionist and diffusion models of reaction time. Psychological Review. 1999;106:261–300. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Resulaj A, Kiani R, Wolpert DM, Shadlen MN. Changes of mind in decision-making. Nature. 2009;461:263–266. doi: 10.1038/nature08275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Rushworth MF, Kolling N, Sallet J, Mars RB. Valuation and decision-making in frontal cortex: one or many serial or parallel systems? Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2012;22:946–955. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Scholl J, Günthner J, Kolling N, Favaron E, Rushworth MFS, Harmer CJ, Reinecke A. A role beyond learning for NMDA receptors in Reward-Based Decision-Making—a Pharmacological Study Using d-Cycloserine. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:2900–2909. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Scholl J, Kolling N, Nelissen N, Wittmann MK, Harmer CJ, Rushworth MF. The good, the bad, and the irrelevant: neural mechanisms of learning real and hypothetical rewards and effort. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:11233–11251. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0396-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Shadlen MN, Kiani R. Decision making as a window on cognition. Neuron. 2013;80:791–806. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. Motion perception: seeing and deciding. PNAS. 1996;93:628–633. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.2.628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Strait CE, Blanchard TC, Hayden BY. Reward value comparison via mutual inhibition in ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2014;82:1357–1366. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Tuerlinckx F. The efficient computation of the cumulative distribution and probability density functions in the diffusion model. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 2004;36:702–716. doi: 10.3758/BF03206552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. van den Berg R, Anandalingam K, Zylberberg A, Kiani R, Shadlen MN, Wolpert DM. A common mechanism underlies changes of mind about decisions and confidence. eLife. 2016;5:e12192. doi: 10.7554/eLife.12192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wang XJ. Probabilistic decision making by slow reverberation in cortical circuits. Neuron. 2002;36:955–968. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01092-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wang XJ. Decision making in recurrent neuronal circuits. Neuron. 2008;60:215–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Wittmann MK, Kolling N, Akaishi R, Chau BK, Brown JW, Nelissen N, Rushworth MF. Predictive decision making driven by multiple time-linked reward representations in the anterior cingulate cortex. Nature Communications. 2016;7:12327. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Wong KF, Wang XJ. A recurrent network mechanism of time integration in perceptual decisions. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:1314–1328. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3733-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wunderlich K, Dayan P, Dolan RJ. Mapping value based planning and extensively trained choice in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:786–791. doi: 10.1038/nn.3068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Thorsten Kahnt1
Reviewed by: Rani Moran2

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

This manuscript presents a re-analysis of previous data as well as new data on the effects of distractors on value-based choices. It is an ongoing debate in the field whether such effects exist and what form they take. The current results convincingly show that across multiple experiments, distractors both improve and impair choice accuracy, depending on the difficulty of the decision. Moreover, these effects are reproduced by a dual-route model that combines divisive normalization and mutual inhibition.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Consistent patterns of distractor effects during decision making" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by Joshua Gold as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Rani Moran (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

In the current study, the authors show robust distractor effects both improving and impairing accuracy in different "parts" of the decision space, despite previous reports (Gluth et al.) that these effects do not exist in some of the datasets (analyzed here). The authors present a "dual-route" model that explains the co-existence of these effects and they relate the facilitation and impairment effects to distractor salience and value respectively.

All reviewers agreed that this research is important, informative and of interest for a broad readership. Reviewers also identified a number of issues which need to be addressed before the paper can be accepted for publication. Most importantly, it would be critical to determine why exactly the authors come to such vastly different conclusions than Gluth et al., when analyzing the same data.

Essential revisions:

1) The paper is extremely long and detailed. The reviewers agreed that it would be good to edit it by delegating non-essential material to the supplement. For example, the sections "distractor effect are not driven by artefacts" and "A more complete analysis of distractor effects" could simply be referenced from the main text but detailed in Supplementary Information. Additionally, the manuscript contains substantial redundancy which could be streamlined.

2) The current paper directly contrasts with the results of Gluth, 2018, whose datasets comprise a portion of the results presented. Critically, both Gluth and the current authors examine identical datasets (Chau, 2014 and Gluth, 2018 Experiment 4) but come to different conclusions about the existence of distracter effects. While the authors in places do offer suggestions about why the conclusions are different, they provide no definite answers. It would be important to do more to address this directly. A simple explanation might be that two opposing effects exist in the data, that these on average can cancel out (given specific HV-LV conditions) – therefore the focus on the interaction term between (HV-LV)(D-HV). However, in the same dataset (Gluth, 2018 Experiment 4), Gluth sees no positive distractor effect (in fact he finds the opposite) and no interaction effect; the authors see both. Barring error, this means the authors are running different analyses and the critical question is what exactly is different?

2.1) Gluth et al., make a very specific technical point about the importance of centering or standardizing HV-LV and D-HV before computing the interaction term; without doing so, the interaction term can be highly correlated with one or both of the main predictor variables. Are predictors centered here prior to calculating interaction terms? Subsection “Distractor effects are not driven by statistical artefact” suggests so, but it is unclear what "normalization" means. Despite the statement in subsection “Distractor effects are not driven by statistical artefact”, there is no mention in the Materials and methods section of (1) whether there is centering before interaction terms are calculated, or (2) if so, in which GLMs. The authors should be explicit here.

2.2) The original analyses in Chau, 2014 and Gluth, 2018 included LV+HV as a covariate in the main analyses, which is not the case here for GLM1 which documents the main finding. Was this excluded for a specific reason, and what are the results if it is included? HV+LV *is* included in the stepwise regression in GLM2, but that is not a straightforward comparison.

2.3) The authors suggest (subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites”) that including two-choice trials (with a nominal D value of zero) may have biased previous results. This sounds plausible but is speculative. It would help if the authors re-ran their analyses with these trials included. A different result would not only back up their assertion but would provide a more definite explanation for the reported differences in findings.

3) Given the reliance on regression measures throughout the paper, reviewers were concerned about whether there are potential multicollinearity issues, particularly because the predictor variables HV-LV and D-HV may be related (due to task design), and due to interaction terms. Illustrations in Figure 9 suggest that some of the GLMs feature strong correlations.

3.1) Please state whether or not the task design orthogonalized HV, LV, and D.

3.2) Please report multicollinearity measures (e.g. variance inflation factors) for the different regression models. This is a concern for all the models, but in particular GLM5 which has many regressors with related terms.

4) In analyzing Experiment 7, it would be important to investigate interactions with D or |D| (e.g., D*(HV-LV), |D|*(HV-LV)) as such interactions play a critical role in studying distractor effects in the rest of the paper. Additionally, it would be highly informative to present panels as in Figure 1 for this experiment and separately for the reward/loss conditions. Do the patterns look different for gains and losses? And can the dual route model account for separate effects of value and salience? Relatedly, how do the authors think negative values are handled in the normalization model?

5) There is now some history between the authors and Gluth et al. This shows in multiple places in the paper. For the sake of de-escalation, the authors are encouraged to tone down their language. Specific examples include (but are not limited to) the subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites”.

6) In many places, statistical interactions are not interpreted using "simple effects". When an interaction (e.g., X*Y) is significant it is unclear whether the main effects (e.g. of X) is meaningful or whether the simple effects change sign depending on the other variable (e.g., Y). It would be important to conduct follow-up simple effect analyses. Some of the analyses even contain triple interactions. If these are not interpreted it is difficult to understand what the patterns of results mean.

7) The dual route model is attractive as a simple conceptual mechanism for a combination of effects, but there were some questions about the precise implementation, model comparison, and whether the models can account for RT data:

7.1) As reported in Chau, 2014, distracter input to the mutual inhibition only occurs for a brief period of time (before it is indicated as unchoosable); is the same format used for the divisive normalization model?

7.2) How were relevant model parameters (d and σ) determined in the dual model? It appears that for individual mutual inhibition and normalization models, they were chosen to give 85% correct choices. Is the same thing true for all 4 parameters in the dual model?

7.3) It would be more informative to show model predictions based on parameters that were derived from model fits vis a vis empirical data (and show qualitative aspects that the dual route model fits better than the other models) as these parameters are more relevant.

7.4) The authors suggest that the ability of the model to generate both effects is due to the relative speed of each model component in different value conditions. While intuitive, it would be helpful if the authors actually showed this to be the case in the simulation data. Since the two processes act entirely in parallel, it would be simple to perform the simulations for the individual component models (using the dual model parameters) and report average RTs (in the [HV-LV, D-HV] space). In other words, rather than showing solely predictions for accuracy, it would be important to show also predictions for RT. Additionally, in models for RT it is essential to include a "residual, non-decision, time". This doesn't seem to be the case here but should be.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for submitting your article "Consistent patterns of distractor effects during decision making" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by Joshua Gold as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Kenway Louie (Reviewer #1); Rani Moran (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

We would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). Specifically, we are asking editors to accept without delay manuscripts, like yours, that they judge can stand as eLife papers without additional data, even if they feel that they would make the manuscript stronger. Thus the revisions requested below only address clarity and presentation.

Summary:

All reviewers agreed that the authors have done a very thorough job of addressing the essential revisions and minor comments, and that the revised manuscript is much improved. In particular, the discussion of the significance of interaction effects in Gluth, 2018 is illuminating, as it supports the general conclusion in this manuscript. There are, however, a few remaining issues that the authors should address before publication.

Essential revisions:

1) In comment 6, the reviewers previously raised the issue of interpreting triple interactions (e.g., analyses pertaining to GLM5). The authors focus on interpretations of 2-way interactions (HV+LV)D and (HV-LV)D whose signs are of theoretical importance in their framework. The concern is that these interactions are qualified by a significant triple interaction (HV-LV)(HV+LV)D. This means that the sign of each of the 2-way interactions can change as a function of the third variable. Therefore, a simple effect analysis here should examine simple 2 way interactions as a function of the third variable. This analysis is critical for the interpreting the findings.

2) Some questions remain about the modelling.

2.1) In comment 7.4. Reviewers previously raised the importance of modeling residual time. In response the authors included RT but arbitrarily fixed it to 300ms rather that allowing it to vary freely. They argue in their response letter that "because non-decision time has no reason to be different across our models, it would not bring more evidence in favor of one or another model during model comparisons.". It may be impossible to determine this a-priori because in each model residual-RT might trade-off differently with the other parameters. It would be important to re-fit the model with free residual time parameters to see which model is best. It is important to rule out that the results of model-comparison are due to arbitrary assumptions about residual rt.

2.2) A very similar issues pertains to the parameter f (inhibition) which was also fixed to a constant value rather than being a free parameter. This could potentially affect model comparison results.

2.3) It is still unclear whether in fitting the dual-channel model, each channel had its own free parameters or whether they were identical for both channels.

2.4) Why are models comparisons reported only for Experiments 1-3 but not for the other experiments?

3) In comment 4 the reviewers previously raised questions about the loss trials. The revised version does not fully address these questions.

3.1) There are still questions pertaining to how to model loss trials. According to the current equations, it seems that drift rates can be negative for the mutual inhibition model, and in the normalization model, the drift will be strongest for the highest loss option. Clearly, if this is correct, the model will require adjustments to account for loss trials and these have to be explained.

3.2) There are also questions pertaining to whether and how the model can account for differences between gain and loss trials. These are important issues because the results seem quite different for gain and loss trials. It would be important to perform a model comparison for the loss trial to determine the best model for these trials. It is not clear if the dual route model, or one of the simpler models, is best for loss trials. Additionally- looking at Figure 6 and the results of the regression (panel d) it seems that when D is positive (i.e., D = abs(D)) corresponding regression effects for these two terms offset each other but when D is negative (D = -abs(D)) they compound. So this could simply mean that the distractor effects are stronger for losses than for gains. Is this true? This can be seen, by including in the regression, interactions terms with trial identity (what the authors call GainTrial) instead of terms with abs(D). Furthermore, if distractors effects are indeed stronger for losses then these stronger effects could presumably be caused by adjusting model parameters (e.g., inhibition strength or other parameters). It is important to examine this. In sum, the authors should consider fitting models to loss trials to see (1) which model provides the best account for loss trials, (2) what account do the mechanistic models provide loss trials and for differences between gain and loss trials. This will provide a much more informative understanding of the gain-loss issue as compared to the current reliance on the regression model. Currently the authors argue that there are 2 separate effects in play, one for distractor value and one for distractor saliency. But a more informative way to understand the data might be that the context (gain/loss) modulated the distractor value effect, and to query the mechanistic models to identify the locus of this modulations.

4) Reviewers were still unclear about the meaning of terms in the GLMs. This needs to be clarified so that the models are better understood and evaluated. Just for example consider GLM1:

logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1(HV-LV) + β2(D-HV) + β3(HV-LV)(D-HV) + ε

The authors state that "All interaction terms in all GLMs are calculated after the component terms are z-scored". Does this mean that the terms are z-scored only for the purpose of calculating the interaction, or are they z-scored for the main effects as well? Reviewers think they should be z-scored in all terms not just in interaction terms. Additionally, just to be sure- did the authors z-score HV and LV separately or z-score the difference (HV-LV)? A clearer way to write the model to avoid confusions could be:

logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z_(HV-LV) + β2z_(D-HV) + β3z_(HV-LV)*z_(D-HV) + ε (underscore indicates subscript).

eLife. 2020 Jul 6;9:e53850. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53850.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) The paper is extremely long and detailed. The reviewers agreed that it would be good to edit it by delegating non-essential material to the supplement. For example, the sections "distractor effect are not driven by artefacts" and "A more complete analysis of distractor effects" could simply be referenced from the main text but detailed in Supplementary Information. Additionally, the manuscript contains substantial redundancy which could be streamlined.

We have moved the subsection "Distractor effects are not driven by artefacts" and subsection "A more complete analysis of distractor effects" to the appendices as requested. While we have moved these sections as requested and while we can see the advantages of moving subsection "Distractor effect are not driven by artefacts" to the appendices we would argue that subsection "A more complete analysis of distractor effects” should remain in the main manuscript. This subsection provides a rationale for when divisive normalization effects should and should not appear and then tests whether or not the predictions are fulfilled (and indeed they are). We think that this an important conceptual point that is not made elsewhere in the main manuscript. In addition, we have tried to streamline the rest of the manuscript while still including the additional analyses suggested by the reviewers in the points below.

2) The current paper directly contrasts with the results of Gluth, 2018, whose datasets comprise a portion of the results presented. Critically, both Gluth and the current authors examine identical datasets (Chau, 2014 and Gluth, 2018 Experiment 4) but come to different conclusions about the existence of distracter effects. While the authors in places do offer suggestions about why the conclusions are different, they provide no definite answers. It would be important to do more to address this directly. A simple explanation might be that two opposing effects exist in the data, that these on average can cancel out (given specific HV-LV conditions) – therefore the focus on the interaction term between (HV-LV)(D-HV). However, in the same dataset (Gluth, 2018 Experiment 4), Gluth sees no positive distractor effect (in fact he finds the opposite) and no interaction effect; the authors see both. Barring error, this means the authors are running different analyses and the critical question is what exactly is different?

In the revised manuscript we have tried to address these points. It is indeed the case that, in brief, our argument is, as the reviewer says “A simple explanation might be that two opposing effects exist in the data, that these on average can cancel out (given specific HV-LV conditions) – therefore the focus on the interaction term between (HV-LV)(D-HV)”.

The reviewer, however, points out that “in the same dataset (Gluth Experiment 4), Gluth sees no positive distractor effect (in fact he finds the opposite) and no interaction effect; the authors see both”. We understand why the reviewer makes this argument. Indeed we initially believed exactly the same; that Gluth et al., made both these argument. It is only after reading Gluth’s et al., paper many times that we realized that only the first part of this statement is true “Gluth sees no positive distractor effect”. The second part of the statement “Gluth sees … no interaction effect” is not true. Very close reading of the paper reveals that although Gluth et al., state that our results were “neither replicable nor reproducible” in both their abstract and main text, they do not provide any statistics regarding the interaction effect in their main paper.

We realize that statement maybe surprising to the reviewer. But Gluth et al., only briefly touch on the interaction term at the end of the Results section. At that point in the main text Gluth et al., note an “incorrect implementation of the interaction term (HV-LV)x(HV-D) in the performed regression analysis. After correcting this error, the effect of HV-D disappears”. On first reading this section we were left with exactly the same impression as the reviewer “Gluth sees … no interaction effect”. It is likely that most other readers will have received the same impression. However, if you read this sentence again you will see that what they are arguing is that the main effect of the distractor is not observed after Gluth et al., perform the analysis with the correct interaction term. Gluth et al., actually avoid saying anywhere in the main text of their paper whether the interaction term itself remains significant. It does; the interaction term is robust even when Gluth et al., analyze the data in the way they suggest. The key statistics are not referred to or presented in their main manuscript but instead appear only in Supplementary file 2 (and some are omitted even from Supplementary file 2).

Careful reading of this table shows that Gluth et al.,, in fact, found a negative (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction effect on choice accuracy (Gluth et al., 2018) just as we do. In Supplementary file 2 of their paper, they presented the distractor effects on choice accuracy across 5 experiments. We reproduce the table below. Since Gluth’s analysis involved the terms HV-D and (HV-LV)(HV-D), we edited the Table slightly by flipping these terms so that they become D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV) respectively and are therefore consistent with the other analyses in our manuscript and the terminology used in the reviewer’s comments and elsewhere in our reply:

Critically, in four out of five experiments, Gluth et al., observed negative (HV-LV)(D-HV) effects. These effects were significant in two experiments (Experiment 3 and Experiment 2 LP) and were marginally significant in another two experiments (Experiment 1 and Experiment 4). They also present an additional analysis that combined data from four experiments (All (exc. 2LP)), albeit excluding one dataset in which the interaction effect was clear, and again there was a significant (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction effect. Hence, we are very sympathetic to the fact that it is easy to read Gluth’s paper and come away with the impression “that they see no interaction term”. This is the impression that we had too and that we believe most readers will have. However, careful reading of the supplementary material suggests that, on the contrary, there is indeed a significant interaction effect – a distractor effect on accuracy (i.e. relative choice accuracy as in Gluth et al., 2018) regardless of which approach is taken to the data.

Careful reading reveals that Gluth et al., explain their suggestion for the specific procedures that should be followed while computing the (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction term (see Gluth et al., 2018). A critical point concerns how the absence of distractors in the control trials are treated. Gluth et al., argue that they should be assigned a notional value that is the mean of the distractor values taken from the main experimental trials in which distractors were presented. We followed these procedures strictly and re-analyzed their data. We are able to generate virtually identical results to those in their Supplementary file 2. Importantly, there is an additional set of statistics that was omitted from Gluth’s et al.,’ Supplementary file 2; they omitted to show that when they take the approach they recommend with our original 2014 data (referred to as Experiment 1 fMRI in the current manuscript) using Gluth’s et al.,’ suggested analysis approach then yet again the interaction term is significant.

To the best of our knowledge, the only statistic from Gluth’s et al.,’ analysis, that is actually reported in their paper is the effect size of the HV-D term in Cohen’s d (approximately 0.35; see their Figure 5—figure supplement 3). In addition, when we calculated the Cohen’s d of the D-HV term we obtained a very similar effect size d=-0.352, suggesting that we had run the analysis in a very similar way to Gluth et al.,.

In summary, we concur with the reviewer that it is very easy to receive the impression that “Gluth sees no positive distractor effect (in fact he finds the opposite) and no interaction effect”. Moreover, we believe that most other readers will have had this impression too. However, careful scrutiny of the main text of Gluth’s paper reveals no mention of whether or not the interaction term is significant and careful scrutiny of Supplementary file 2 reveals that the interaction term is significant even when Gluth’s et al.,’ suggested analysis approach is used. We have, therefore tried to make this clear in the revised manuscript in a manner consistent with the reviewers’ and editor’s comment 5 as follows:

It might be asked why the presence of distractor effects in their data was not noted by Gluth et al., 2018. The answer is likely to be complex. A fundamental consideration is that it is important to examine the possibility that both distractor effects exist rather than just the possibility that one or other effect exists. This means that it is necessary to consider not just the main effect of D-HV but also D-HV interaction effects. Gluth et al., however, focus on main effects apart from in their table S2 where careful scrutiny reveals that the (D-HV)(HV-LV) interaction is reliably significant in their data. A further consideration concerns the precise way in which the impact of the distractor D is indexed in the GLM particularly on control trials where no distractor is actually presented. Gluth et al., 2018 recommend that a notional value of D is assigned to control trials which corresponds to the distractor’s average value when it appears on distractor trials. In addition, they emphasize that HV-LV and D-HV should be normalized (i.e. demeaned and divided by the standard deviation) before calculating the (HV-LV)(D-HV) term. If we run an analysis of their data in this way then we obtain similar results to those described by Gluth et al., in their Table S2 (Supplementary file 1 here). Although a D-HV main effect was absent, the (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction term was significant when data from all their experiments are considered together. While Gluth et al., omitted any analysis of the data from Experiment 1 fMRI, we have performed this analysis and once again a significant (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect is clear (Supplementary file 1).

Another possible reason for the discrepancy in the interpretation concerns the other three experiments reported by Gluth et al. We turn to these next. (subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites”)

2.1) Gluth et al., make a very specific technical point about the importance of centering or standardizing HV-LV and D-HV before computing the interaction term; without doing so, the interaction term can be highly correlated with one or both of the main predictor variables. Are predictors centered here prior to calculating interaction terms? Subsection “Distractor effects are not driven by statistical artefact” suggests so, but it is unclear what "normalization" means. Despite the statement in subsection “Distractor effects are not driven by statistical artefact”, there is no mention in the Materials and methods section of (1) whether there is centering before interaction terms are calculated, or (2) if so, in which GLMs. The authors should be explicit here.

We apologize for the confusion related to the term “normalization”. It actually means that predictors are z-scored before interaction terms are calculated. This involves all the component terms centered by their own mean and then divided by their own standard deviation. We applied the same procedures when calculating all interaction terms in every GLM. We have rephrased the sentence in subsection “Examining distractor effects in further experiments”:

“First, all of the interaction terms are calculated after their component parts, the difficulty and distractor terms, are z-scored (i.e. centered by the mean and then divided by the standard deviation).” (subsection “Examining distractor effects in further experiments”).

We have also added in the subsection “Analysis procedures the following line:

“All interaction terms in all GLMs are calculated after the component terms are z-scored.”

2.2) The original analyses in Chau, 2014 and Gluth, 2018 included LV+HV as a covariate in the main analyses, which is not the case here for GLM1 which documents the main finding. Was this excluded for a specific reason, and what are the results if it is included? HV+LV *is* included in the stepwise regression in GLM2, but that is not a straightforward comparison.

The reviewer makes a good point about the degree to which analyses might be compared. It is true that GLM1 is different from a key analysis in Chau et al., 2014 and Gluth et al., 2018 by lacking the HV+LV term. However, GLM1 has the strength of being very simple and straightforward by itself – it involves only the HV-LV and D-HV main effect terms and the interaction between the two. GLM2 is a follow-up analysis of GLM1 to investigate the nature of the (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction effect by testing trials with large and small HV-LVs separately. It involves an HV+LV term, in addition to the HV-LV, and a stepwise procedure in order to partial out completely the effects of HV and LV from the choice accuracy data (note that a combination of HV-LV and HV+LV terms is equivalent to a combination of HV and LV terms). This procedure has a strength of ruling out the possibility that any D-HV effect is driven by the variance of the HV.

It seems that a GLM (GLM1b) that involves both the (HV-LV)(D-HV) and HV+LV terms can fill the gap between GLMs 1 (now renamed as GLM1a) and 2. In other words, GLM1b includes the regressors HV-LV, HV+LV, D-HV and (HV-LV)(D-HV). We applied this to analyze the data in a similar way as that in Figure 3. Critically the results were highly comparable to those of GLM1a – Experiments 1-3 all showed a significant (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect. We have now added these results to our manuscript:

“In addition to being present in the data reported by Chau et al., 2014 and Gluth et al., 2018 the same effect emerged in a third previously unreported data set (Experiment 3 Hong Kong; n=40) employing the same schedule but collected at a third site (Hong Kong). The results were highly comparable not only when the choice accuracy data were visualized using the same approach (Figure 3E), but also when the same GLM was applied to analyze the choice accuracy data (Figure 3F). There was a significant (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect (β=-0.089, t39=-2.242, p=0.031). Again there was a positive D-HV effect (β=0.207, t39=5.980, p<10-6) and a positive HV-LV effect (β=0.485, t39=12.448, p<10-14). The pattern of results was consistent regardless of whether an additional HV+LV term was included in the GLM, as in Chau et al., 2014; a significant (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect was found in Experiments 1-3 when an additional HV+LV term was included in the GLM (GLM1b; Figure 3—figure supplement 1).” (Results section)

Behavior was analyzed using a series of GLMs containing the following regressors:

GLM1a: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1(HV-LV) + β2(D-HV) + β3(HV-LV)(D-HV) + ε

GLM1b: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1(HV-LV) + β2(HV+LV) + β3(D-HV) + β4(HV-LV)(D-HV) + ε” (Materials and methods section)

2.3) The authors suggest (subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites”) that including two-choice trials (with a nominal D value of zero) may have biased previous results. This sounds plausible but is speculative. It would help if the authors re-ran their analyses with these trials included. A different result would not only back up their assertion but would provide a more definite explanation for the reported differences in findings.

As we have noted above, in the first part of the response to point 2, it is easy to be confused as to whether Gluth’s claims of non-significance refer to the main effects of HV-D or the HV-D interaction terms. We have explained that actually the interaction terms are significant but they are only reported in Gluth’s Supplementary Table S2. We have tried to avoid compounding confusion by not reporting any analysis of two-choice trials with a nominal D value of zero because neither Gluth et al., nor we advocate this approach. Instead, when comparing the results of our analyses, with those adopted by Gluth we have used the one analysis approach that is explained and advocated by Gluth et al., (see Gluth , 2018). A critical point does indeed concern how the absence of distractors in the control trials is treated. Gluth et al., argued that, in addition to a binary term that describes the presence/absence of a distractor, non-distractor trials should be assigned a notional distractor value that is the mean of the distractor values taken from the main experimental trials in which distractors were presented. We followed these procedures strictly and re-analyzed their data. We make clear in the revised manuscript that when we use this approach, we obtain virtually identical results and the interaction term is significant. Moreover we obtain the same results when we applied it to the original data from our 2014 paper (here referred to as Experimental 1 fMRI). These findings are now reported in subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites” and Supplementary file 1 of our manuscript. As noted the revised text now reads as follows:

It might be asked why the presence of distractor effects in their data was not noted by Gluth et al., 2018. The answer is likely to be complex. A fundamental consideration is that it is important to examine the possibility that both distractor effects exist rather than just the possibility that one or other effect exists. This means that it is necessary to consider not just the main effect of D-HV but also D-HV interaction effects. Gluth et al., however, focus on main effects apart from in their Table S2 where careful scrutiny reveals that the (D-HV)(HV-LV) interaction is reliably significant in their data. A further consideration concerns the precise way in which the impact of the distractor D is indexed in the GLM particularly on control trials where no distractor is actually presented. Gluth et al., 2018 recommend that a notional value of D is assigned to control trials which corresponds to the distractor’s average value when it appears on distractor trials. In addition, they emphasize that HV-LV and D-HV should be normalized (i.e. demeaned and divided by the standard deviation) before calculating the (HV-LV)(D-HV) term. If we run an analysis of their data in this way then we obtain similar results to those described by Gluth et al., in their Table S2 (Supplementary file 1 here). Although a D-HV main effect was absent, the (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction term was significant when data from all their experiments are considered together. While Gluth et al., omitted any analysis of the data from Experiment 1 fMRI we have performed this analysis and once again a significant (HV-LV)(D-HV) effect is clear (Supplementary file 1).

Another possible reason for the discrepancy in the interpretation concerns the other three experiments reported by Gluth et al., We turn to these next.

3) Given the reliance on regression measures throughout the paper, reviewers were concerned about whether there are potential multicollinearity issues, particularly because the predictor variables HV-LV and D-HV may be related (due to task design), and due to interaction terms. Illustrations in Figure 9 suggest that some of the GLMs feature strong correlations.

In Comment 3.2 it is suggested that the variance inflation factor (VIF) can be calculated to indicate multicollinearity of individual regressors. This is a helpful suggestion and so we have calculated the VIFs in all general linear models (GLMs). Since there is a concern about the multicollinearity issue of the HV-LV and D-HV terms, we checked carefully the VIFs of these terms, as well as the related terms D, (HV-LV)(D-HV), (HV-LV)D, HV+LV and (HV+LV)D. These results are now reported in Figure 9—figure supplement 1. All regressors show VIFs that are less than 10, which is sometimes used as a threshold for indicating high multicollinearity. In addition, the VIFs of all critical terms that are related to the distractor are less than 5.

3.1) Please state whether or not the task design orthogonalized HV, LV, and D.

There are two subtly, but importantly, different ways in which the question whether “the task design orthogonalized HV, LV, and D” might be interpreted. First, it might be interpreted as asking whether the variance in HV, in LV, and in D was unrelated. This interpretation of the question is consistent with comment 3.2 which asks for multicollinearity measures such as variance inflation factors. As described below, in answer to comment 3.2, the variance in HV, LV, and D was not collinear and this is made clear in Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 2 in the revised manuscript. An alternative interpretation, however, is that the question is asking whether one regressor was made to be orthogonal to another by partialling out the shared variance from one of the two regressors. We have made clear whenever we partial out the variance related one regressor by explicitly using the phrase “partial out” to avoid any confusion (for example, subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites” and Subsection “Analysis procedures) but this was not done by default.

3.2) Please report multicollinearity measures (e.g. variance inflation factors) for the different regression models. This is a concern for all the models, but in particular GLM5 which has many regressors with related terms.

We would like to thank the reviewer for suggesting the calculation of variance inflation factor (VIF), which is a convenient indicator of multicollinearity of individual regressors. All general linear models (GLMs) include regressors with VIF less than 10. In GLM5, about which the reviewer is particularly concerned, the VIFs are all less than 3.106. We have now included the VIFs of all GLMs in Figure 9—figure supplement 1:

4) In analyzing Experiment 7, it would be important to investigate interactions with D or |D| (e.g., D*(HV-LV), |D|*(HV-LV)) as such interactions play a critical role in studying distractor effects in the rest of the paper. Additionally, it would be highly informative to present panels as in Figure 1 for this experiment and separately for the reward/loss conditions. Do the patterns look different for gains and losses? And can the dual route model account for separate effects of value and salience? Relatedly, how do the authors think negative values are handled in the normalization model?

The reviewer has made a very useful suggestion to better relate the effects in Experiment 7 to the earlier sections of the manuscript and to examine closely the interaction effects. To achieve that, we now present the results in Figure 6 in a similar fashion to those in Figure 1J and L, Figure 3E-F and Figure 4. In panels A and B, we first illustrate hypothetically how value-based and salience-based effects should manifest in the accuracy data. Then on the right side of panel C, it shows Figure 3E (which is in the same format as Figure 1B,F,Jj) once again to illustrate the changes in accuracy as a function of D-HV and HV-LV on gain trials. A similar plot that shows the data from loss trials is presented adjacent to it. In panel d, as in Figure 1D,H,L and Figure 3F, it shows the results of GLM6a. As suggested by the reviewer, GLM6a now includes the interaction terms (HV-LV)D and (HV-LV)|D|, in addition to the GainTrial, HV-LV, D and |D| terms. In panel e, as in Figure 4 and Figure 4—figure supplement 1, it shows the results of an analysis that tested the effects of value D and salience |D| on hard and easy trials separately. Critically, these further analyses revealed that the positive distractor effect was both value-based and salience-based, as opposed to our previous notion that it was only salience-based. We have now updated the subsection “Experiment 7: Loss experiment” substantially to describe the results of the additional analysis and how the results are related to the dual route model. The revised figure (Figure 6) and its accompanying legend are shown first below then we show the revised text that describes this part of the results.

The attentional capture model raises the question of whether any distractor effect on choice accuracy is due to the value or the salience of the distractor. This is difficult to test in most reward-based decision making experiments because the value and salience of an option are often collinear – more rewarding options are both larger in value and more salient – and it is not possible to determine which factor drives behavior. One way of breaking the collinearity between value and salience is to introduce options that lead to loss (Kahnt et al., 2014). As such, the smallest value options that lead to great loss are very salient (Figure 6A, bottom), the medium value options that lead to small gains or losses are not salient and the largest value options that lead to great gain are again very salient. Having a combination of gain and loss scenarios in an experiment enables the investigation of whether the positive and negative distractor effects, related to mutual inhibition and divisive normalization respectively, are driven by the distractor’s value, salience or both. Figures 6A and B show 4 hypothetical cases of how the distractor may influence accuracy. Hypothesis 1 suggests that larger distractor values (Figure 6A, first row, left-to-right), which correspond to fewer losses or more gains, are related to greater accuracies (brighter colors). This is also predicted by the mutual inhibition component of the dual route model (Figure 1) and can be described as a positive D effect (Figure 6B). Hypothesis 2 larger distractor saliences (Figure 6A, second row, center-to-sides) are related to greater accuracies (brighter colors). This can be described as a positive |D| effect (Figure 6B). Under this hypothesis the mutual inhibition decision making component receives salience, rather than value, as an input. Hypotheses 3 and 4 are the opposites of Hypotheses 1 and 2, and predict negative distractor effects as a result of the divisive normalization component depending on whether the input involves value or salience. Hypothesis 3 predicts a value-based effect in which larger distractor values (Figure 6A third row, left-to-right) are related to poorer accuracies (darker colors). Hypothesis 4 predicts a salience-based effect in which larger distractor saliences (Figure 6A, fourth row, center-to-sides) are related to poorer accuracies (darker colors). It is important to note that these four hypotheses are not necessarily mutually exclusive. The earlier sections have demonstrated that positive and negative distractor effects can co-exist and predominate in different parts of decision space. Value-based and salience-based distractor effects can also be teased apart with a combination of gain and loss scenarios.

To test these hypotheses, we adopted this approach in an additional experiment performed at the same time as Experiment 3 Hong Kong, in which half of the trials included options that were all positive in value (gain trials) and the other half of the trials included options that were all negative in value (loss trials; the loss trials were not analyzed in the previous sections). We therefore refer to these additional trials as belonging to Experiment 7 Loss Experiment (n=40 as in Experiment 3 Hong Kong). The effect of signed D reflects the value of the distractor while the effect of the unsigned, absolute size of D (i.e. |D|) reflects the salience of the distractor. The correlation between these two parameters was low (r=0.005), such that it was possible to isolate the impact that they each had on behavior.

As in other experiments, we first plotted the accuracy as a function of difficulty (HV-LV) and relative distractor value (D-HV). For ease of comparison, Figure 3E that illustrates the accuracy data for the gain trials in Experiments 3 is shown again in the right panel of Figure 6C. As described before, when the decisions were hard (bottom rows) larger distractor values were associated with greater accuracies (left-to-right: the colors change from dark to bright; also see Figure 4B) and when the decisions were easy larger distractor values were associated with poorer accuracies (left-to-right: the colors change from bright to dark; also see Figure 4B). In a similar manner, the left panel of Figure 6C shows the accuracy data of the loss trials in Experiment 7. Strikingly, on both hard and easy trials (top and bottom rows), larger distractor values were associated with poorer accuracies (left-to-right: the colours changes from bright to dark).

To isolate the value-based and salience-based effects of D, we performed GLM6a (Methods) to analyze both the gain and loss trials in Experiments 3 and 7 at the same time. GLM6 includes the signed and unsigned value of D (i.e. D and |D| respectively). We also included a binary term, GainTrial, to describe whether the trial presented gain options or loss options and, as in GLM1a, we included the HV-LV term and its interaction with D but now also with |D| [i.e. (HV-LV)D and (HV-LV)|D| respectively]. The results showed a negative effect of value D (β=-0.236, t39=-2.382, p=0.022; Figure 6d) and a negative effect of (HV-LV)D interaction (β=-0.205, t39=-2.512, p=0.016). In addition, there was a positive effect of salience |D| (β=0.152, t39=3.253, p=0.002) and a positive effect of (HV-LV)|D| (β=0.219, t39=3.448, p=0.001). Next, we examined closely the value-based and salience-based effect in different parts of decision space.

As in the analysis for Experiments 1-6 in Figure 6E, we split the data (which included both gain and loss trials) according to the median HV-LV, such that the distractor effects can be examined on hard and easy trials separately. We applied GLM6b that first partialled out the effects of HV-LV, HV+LV and GainTrial from the accuracy data and then tested the overall effect of value D across the gain and loss trials. Similar to Experiments 1-6, a positive value D effect was identified on hard trials (β=0.008, t39=2.463, p=0.017; Figure 6e, left) and a negative value D effect was identified on easy trials (β=-0.011, t38=-3.807, p<10-3; note that one participant was excluded due to the lack of variance in the accuracy data). Then we applied GLM6c which was similar to GLM6b but the value D term was replaced by the salience |D| term. The results showed that there were positive salience |D| effects on both hard (β=0.011, t39=2.119, p=0.041; Figure 6E, right) and easy trials (β=0.009, t38=2.338, p=0.025).

Taken together, in Experiments 1-6 a positive distractor effect predicted by the mutual inhibition model and a negative distractor effect predicted by the divisive normalization model were found on hard and easy trials respectively. The results of Experiments 3 and 7 suggest that these effects are value-based and that the effects are continuous across the gain and loss value space. In addition, however, there was also a positive distractor effect that was salience-based that appeared on both hard and easy trials, suggesting that the effects driven by the mutual inhibition decision making component can be both value-based and salience-based.

5) There is now some history between the authors and Gluth et al. This shows in multiple places in the paper. For the sake of de-escalation, the authors are encouraged to tone down their language. Specific examples include (but are not limited to) subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites”.

We have attempted to maintain a neutral tone throughout the manuscript. We have attempted to do this while acting in accord with the request made in comment 2 for clarity about the difference between the two different analysis approaches. We have paid particular attention to the paragraphs highlighted by the reviewer but are happy to change other paragraphs too. For example, the paragraphs highlighted by the reviewer have been changed as follows:

Figure 3A and C show the data from the fMRI experiment (Experiment 1 fMRI2014; n=21) reported by Chau et al., 2014 and Gluth et al., 2018 experiment 4 (Experiment 2 Gluth4; n=44) respectively. It is important to consider these two experiments first because they employ an identical schedule. Specifically, Chau et al., reported both divisive normalization effects and positive distractor effects, while Gluth et al., claimed they were unable to replicate these effects in their own data and when they analyzed this data set from Chau et al.,. Here we found that both data sets show a positive D-HV distractor effect. In both data sets, when decisions are difficult (HV-LV is small) then high value D-HV is associated with higher relative accuracy in choices between HV and LV; for example, the bottom rows of Figure 3A and c turn from black/dark red to yellow moving from left to right, indicating decisions are more accurate. However, when decisions were easy (HV-LV is large) then the effect is much less prominent or even reverses as would be predicted if divisive normalization becomes more important in this part of the decision space. As in the predictions of the dual route model (Figure 1J,K), on easy trials although there was an overall decreasing trend in accuracy as a function of D-HV, there was an increasing trend at very low D-HV levels. Overall, a combination of positive and negative D-HV effects on hard and easy trials respectively suggests that there should be a negative (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction effect on choice accuracy. (Subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites”)

Another possible reason for the discrepancy in the interpretation concerns the other three experiments reported by Gluth et al. We turn to these next. (Subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites”)

We have tried to make related changes elsewhere. For example the revised manuscript is changed as follows:

“It is clear thatdata collected under the same conditions in 105 participants at all three sites are very similar and that a positive distractor effect consistently recurs when decisions are difficult. Next, we aggregated the data collected from the three sites and repeated the same GLM to confirm that the (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction (β=-0.101, t104=-4.366, p<10-4), D-HV (β=0.223, t104=6.400, p<10-8) and HV-LV (β=0.529, t104=20.775, p<10-38) effects were all collectively significant. Additional control analyses suggest that these effects were unlikely due to any statistical artefact (see subsection “Distractor effects are not driven by statistical artefact” for details).” (subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites”).

6) In many places, statistical interactions are not interpreted using "simple effects". When an interaction (e.g., X*Y) is significant it is unclear whether the main effects (e.g. of X) is meaningful or whether the simple effects change sign depending on the other variable (e.g., Y). It would be important to conduct follow-up simple effect analyses. Some of the analyses even contain triple interactions. If these are not interpreted it is difficult to understand what the patterns of results mean.

The reviewer has made a very important point about how to clearly illustrate the pattern of interaction effects. We agree that testing the simple effects is critical and indeed we followed this procedure in the initial submission of our manuscript. For example, GLM1a (formerly GLM1; see below for the GLM) mainly tested any presence of (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction and that was followed by GLM2a that split the trials into small HV-LV (i.e. hard trials) and large HV-LV (i.e. easy trials), thereby testing for simple main effects, and tested the effects of D-HV (or D) separately (Figure 4 and Figure 4—figure supplement 1). In retrospect, however, we realize that we did not categorically state that in conducting this analysis we were examining the simple main effects. In the revised manuscript we make this point clear. In Figure 3, we also plotted how the accuracy varied as a function of D-HV at different levels of HV-LV as an illustration of the interaction effect. Again, in the revised manuscript, we make clear that this figure illustrates the simple main effects. It is also possible that the link between GLM1a and GLM2a was not very clear, since GLM2a has an additional HV+LV term (this is also suggested in comment 2.2). We have now added GLM1b that included also the HV+LV term to fill this gap and this is described in Subsection “Analysis procedures”:

GLM1a: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1(HV-LV) + β2(D-HV) + β3(HV-LV)(D-HV) + ε

GLM1b: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1(HV-LV) + β2(HV+LV) + β3(D-HV) + β4(HV-LV)(D-HV) + ε

GLM2a: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1(HV-LV) + β2(HV+LV) + ε1

Step 2, ε1 = β3 + β4(D-HV) + ε2

Further, we have made the following revision to improve the link between GLM1a,b and GLM2a (subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites”):

“The next step is to examine whether the (HV-LV)(D-HV) interaction effect from GLM1a and 1b arises because of the presence of a divisive normalization effect (i.e. negative D-HV effect) on easy trials, a positive distractor effect on hard trials, or both effects. In other words, we need to establish which component of the interaction (or in other words, which main effect) is driving the interaction. To establish which is the case, the data were median split as a function of difficulty, defined as HV-LV, so that it is possible to easily visualize the separate predictions of the divisive normalization and positive distractor accounts (Figure 4A; a similar approach was also used by Chau et al., in their supplementary Figure SI.4). Then, to analyze each half of the choice accuracy data we applied GLM2a in a stepwise manner.”

The legend for Figure 4 now reads as follows:

Figure 4. Distractors had opposite effects on decision accuracy as a function of difficulty in all experiments. The main effect of the distractor was different depending on decision difficulty. (a) In accordance with the predictions of the dual route model, high value distractors (D-HV is high) facilitated decision making when the decision was hard (blue bars), whereas there was a tendency for high value distractors to impair decision making when the decision was easy (red bars). Data are shown for (b) Experiment 1 fMRI2014, Experiment 2 Gluth4, Experiment 3 Hong Kong. (c) The same is true when data from the other experiments, Experiments 4-6 (i.e. Gluth1-3), are examined in a similar way. However, participants made decisions in these experiments in a different manner: they were less likely to integrate probability and magnitude features of the options in the optimal manner when making decisions and instead were more likely to choose on the basis of a weighted sum of the probability and magnitude components of the options. Thus, in Experiments 4-6 (i.e. Gluth1-3), the difficulty of a trial can be better described by the weighted sum of the magnitude and probability components associated with each option rather than the true objective value difference HV-LV. This may be because these experiments included additional “decoy” trials that were particularly difficult and on which it was especially important to consider the individual attributes of the options rather than just their integrated expected value. Whatever the reason for the difference in behavior, once an appropriate difficulty metric is constructed for these participants, the pattern of results is the same as in panel a. # p<0.1, * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001. Error bars indicate standard error.

Perhaps the simple effect analysis for GLM5 is less clear in our original manuscript. GLM5 involves:

GLM5: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1(HV-LV) + β2(HV+LV) + β3D + β4(HV-LV)D + β5(HV+LV)D + β6(HV-LV)(HV+LV) + β7(HV-LV)(HV+LV)D + ε

(Subsection “Analysis procedures”)

The critical interaction terms were (HV-LV)D and (HV+LV)D. The terms (HV-LV)(HV+LV) and (HV-LV)(HV+LV)D were regressors of no interest, but were added for the sake of completing a full three-way interaction model. To better describe the pattern of the interaction effects, we have now added the following simple effect analysis in Appendix 3:

“Finally, a follow-up analysis was run to confirm how the D effect varied as a function of HV-LV or HV+LV. These questions related to the (HV-LV)D and (HV+LV)D interaction effects respectively. We applied a mean split by HV-LV and then estimated the effect of D using GLM2b. On hard trials (small HV-LV), larger D values were related to greater choice accuracies (β=0.056, t207=4.113, p<10-4); whereas on easy trials (large HV-LV), larger D values were related to poorer choice accuracies (β=-0.007, t206=-2.049, p=0.042; note that one participant was excluded from this analysis because of the lack of behavioral variance – there was only one inaccurate choice). On trials with a small HV+LV sum, larger D values were associated with poorer choice accuracies (β=-0.009, t207=-2.530, p=0.012); whereas on trial with large HV+LV sums, larger D values were only marginally associated with greater choice accuracies (β=0.006, t207=1.810, p=0.072). These results are consistent with our predictions that the mutual inhibition model, associated with a positive D effect, was better at predicting behavior on hard trials and when the HV+LV sum was large. The divisive normalization model, associated with a negative D effect, was better at predicting behavior on easy trials and when the HV+LV sum was small.”

7) The dual route model is attractive as a simple conceptual mechanism for a combination of effects, but there were some questions about the precise implementation, model comparison, and whether the models can account for RT data:

Please refer to our point-by-point replies below.

7.1) As reported in Chau, 2014, distracter input to the mutual inhibition only occurs for a brief period of time (before it is indicated as unchoosable); is the same format used for the divisive normalization model?

We aimed to keep the mutual inhibition model as simple as possible, while only keeping some critical features (e.g. a pooled inhibition) of the biophysical model reported in Chau, 2014. As such, the mutual inhibition model involves distractor input for an equal amount of time to that of the HV and LV options. We have revised subsection “Summary of approach” to clarify this:

“The mutual inhibition model is a simplified form of a biophysically plausible model that is reported elsewhere (Chau et al., 2014). It involves three pools of excitatory neurons Pi, each receives noisy input Ei from an option i (i.e. HV, LV or D option) at time t:

Ei,tN(dVi,σ2) where d is the drift rate, Vi is the value of option i (HV, LV or D) and σ is the standard deviation of the Gaussian noise. The noisy input of all options (HV, LV and D) are all provided simultaneously.”

The divisive normalization does not involve direct distractor input, but instead the distractor influences the evidence accumulation process by normalizing the input of the HV and LV options. We have revised subsection “Computational modelling” to clarify this:

“The divisive normalization model follows the same architecture, except that there are only two pools of excitatory neurons, and each receives normalized input from the HV or LV option. The D only participates in this model by normalizing the input from the HV and LV options. The normalized input of the HV or LV option follows the following equation: Ei,tN(dViVHV+VLV+VD,σ2) where d is the drift rate, Vi is the value of option i (HV or LV) and σ is the standard deviation of the Gaussian noise. The inhibition It and evidence yi,t+1 follow the same equations as the mutual inhibition model.”

7.2) How were relevant model parameters (d and σ) determined in the dual model? It appears that for individual mutual inhibition and normalization models, they were chosen to give 85% correct choices. Is the same thing true for all 4 parameters in the dual model?

It is indeed the case that in the dual route model the four parameters (two ds and two sigmas) were selected in order to give 85% choice accuracy. We have now revised subsection “Computational modelling” such that this is more explicit:

“The levels of d and σ of each model (i.e. the mutual inhibition, divisive normalization or dual route model) were selected in order to produce an overall choice accuracy of 0.85. For the mutual inhibition model, d and σ were set at 1.3 s-1 and 1 s-1 respectively.”

7.3) It would be more informative to show model predictions based on parameters that were derived from model fits vis a vis empirical data (and show qualitative aspects that the dual route model fits better than the other models) as these parameters are more relevant.

Thank you for the suggestion. One clear way of showing how well each model fits with the empirical data is to plot how much the models’ predictions deviate from the empirical data across the decision space of HV-LV and D-HV. We have now shown in Figure 3—figure supplement 2A,C the empirical data in a format identical to Figure 3A,C,E. We then show in Figure 3—figure supplement 2B,D the degree in which the predictions of each model deviate from the empirical data. The results show that the dual route model is better than the mutual inhibition, divisive normalization, and null models in predicting participants’ accuracy and reaction time. These results are now reported in the Results section and Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

“Interestingly, the dual route model provided the best account of the participants’ behaviour when Experiments 1-3 were considered as a whole (estimated frequency Ef=0.898; exceedance probability Xp=1.000, Figure 3G-H) and when individual experiments were considered separately (Experiment 1: Ef=0.843, Xp=1.000; Experiment 2: Ef=0.924, Xp=1.000; Experiment 3: Ef=0.864, Xp=1.000). Furthermore, the fitted parameters were applied back to each model to predict participants’ behavior (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). The results show that the dual route model is better than the mutual inhibition, divisive normalization, and null models in predicting both choice accuracy and reaction time.” (Subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites”)

7.4) The authors suggest that the ability of the model to generate both effects is due to the relative speed of each model component in different value conditions. While intuitive, it would be helpful if the authors actually showed this to be the case in the simulation data. Since the two processes act entirely in parallel, it would be simple to perform the simulations for the individual component models (using the dual model parameters) and report average RTs (in the [HV-LV, D-HV] space). In other words, rather than showing solely predictions for accuracy, it would be important to show also predictions for RT. Additionally, in models for RT it is essential to include a "residual, non-decision, time". This doesn't seem to be the case here but should be.

The reviewer has provided very helpful suggestions for better understanding the predictions made by the dual route model. We have now plotted the reaction time of the model when the drift rate and noise level are set at zero for one of the two components at a time. These plots are now shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 2. However, it might not be straightforward to see how the distractor effects of the dual route model are linked to the reaction time of each component because it is necessary to consider not just which model component is likely to produce a response but also how likely it is to be correct. Hence, we suggest that the next analyses, described below, do achieve what we think the reviewer ultimately wants: an intuitive sense of which types of response, both correct and incorrect, that each model causes.

We analyzed the choices predicted by the dual route model when both components are assigned the non-zero parameters. We examined on each trial whether a choice is made by the mutual inhibition or divisive normalization component. Choices made by the mutual inhibition component show greater accuracy as a function of relative distractor value and choices made by the divisive normalization component show smaller accuracy as a function of relative distractor value (Figure 1—figure supplement 1C). These results are broadly similar to those presented in Figure 1A-H in which either the mutual inhibition model or divisive normalization model is run alone. Since the accuracy of the two components are plotted together on the same scale, it is clear, when comparing the slopes, that on hard trials the positive effect of the mutual inhibition component outweighs the negative effect of the divisive normalization component. On easy trials the negative effect of the divisive normalization component outweighs the positive effect of the mutual inhibition component. One key reason for these phenomena is because on easy trials, errors made by the mutual inhibition component are rare and the positive distractor effect on reducing these errors is therefore weaker (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D). We have now presented these analyses in our manuscript:

“In the dual route model positive and negative distractor effects predominate in different parts of the decision space. It is possible to understand the underlying reasons by analyzing the choices made by the mutual inhibition and divisive normalization components separately (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). On hard trials, when the distractor value becomes larger, the errors made by the mutual inhibition component decrease more rapidly in frequency than the increase in errors made by the divisive normalization component, resulting in a net positive distractor effect. In contrast, on easy trials when the distractor value becomes larger the decrease in errors made by the mutual inhibition model is much less than the increase in errors made by the divisive normalization model. Figure 1—figure supplement 2 shows the reaction time of choices made by each component when the other component is switched off.” (Subsection “Divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects should predominate in different parts of the decision space”)

In addition, we agree with the reviewer that a non-decision time is indeed a very important parameter in the framework of diffusion models. However, because non-decision time has no reason to be different across our models, it would not bring more evidence in favor of one or another model during model comparisons. Nevertheless, we still included a non-decision time [fixed at 300ms Grasman et al., 2009Ratcliff et al., 1999Tuerlinckx, 2004(; ; )] in all the models. This additional procedure is now described in subsection “Computational modelling”.

“The reaction time is defined as the duration of the evidence accumulation before the decision threshold is reached added by a fixed non-decision time of 300 ms to account for lower-order cognitive processes before choice information reaches the excitatory neurons Grasman et al., 2009Ratcliff et al., 1999Tuerlinckx, 2004(; ; ).”

The results of the model comparison remain similar – the dual route model best describes participants behaviour.

“Interestingly, the dual route model provided the best account of the participants’ behaviour when Experiments 1-3 were considered as a whole (estimated frequency Ef=0.898; exceedance probability Xp=1.000, Figure 3G-H) and when individual experiments were considered separately (Experiment 1: Ef=0.843, Xp=1.000; Experiment 2: Ef=0.924, Xp=1.000; Experiment 3: Ef=0.864, Xp=1.000).” (subsection “Both divisive normalization of value and positive distractor effects co-exist in data sets from three sites”).

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Essential revisions:

1) In comment 6, the reviewers previously raised the issue of interpreting triple interactions (e.g., analyses pertaining to GLM5). The authors focus on interpretations of 2-way interactions (HV+LV)D and (HV-LV)D whose signs are of theoretical importance in their framework. The concern is that these interactions are qualified by a significant triple interaction (HV-LV)(HV+LV)D. This means that the sign of each of the 2-way interactions can change as a function of the third variable. Therefore, a simple effect analysis here should examine simple 2 way interactions as a function of the third variable. This analysis is critical for the interpreting the findings.

Thank you for the suggestion. We have now followed these suggestions of testing the three-way (HV-LV)(HV+LV)D by examining how the (HV+LV)D effect or (HV-LV)D effect varied as a function of the third variable. Note, however, that while it is not uncommon to check the interpretation of two-way interactions by examining the simple effects it is less common to see this approach used to examine three way interaction effects. The reason is simple. Examining the simple effects associated with a two way interaction means looking at approximately half of the original data set. Examining the simple effects associated with a three way interaction means looking at approximately a quarter of the original data set which could produce unreliable results. Hence, we took an alternative approach that is recommended in such situations by using the β weights estimated in GLM5 and investigating the three-way interaction effect. In particular, small and large HV-LV (or HV+LV) was defined as the twenty-fifth percentile (z score=-0.675) and seventy-fifth percentile (z score=0.675) respectively. Then we tested the two-way (HV+LV)D effect at small and large HV-LV:

GLM5:

logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + β3 z(D) + β4 z(HV-LV) z(D) + β5 z(HV+LV) z(D) + β6 z(HV-LV) z(HV+LV) + β7 z(HV-LV) z(HV+LV) z(D) + ε

GLM5 at small HV-LV:

logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 (-0.675) + β3 z(D) + β4 (-0.675) z(D) + β5 z(HV+LV) z(D) + β6 (-0.675) z(HV+LV) + β7 (-0.675) z(HV+LV) z(D) + ε

GLM5 at large HV-LV:

logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 (0.675) + β3 z(D) + β4 (0.675) z(D) + β5 z(HV+LV) z(D) + β6 (0.675) z(HV+LV) + β7 (0.675) z(HV+LV) z(D) + ε

As such, the effect of (HV+LV)D at small HV-LV is (β5 – 0.675β7) and that at large HV-LV is (β5 + 0.675β7). The analysis showed that at small HV-LV there was a positive (HV+LV)D effect (β=0.169, t207=5.656, p<10-7) and at large HV-LV the (HV+LV)D effect was even more positive (β=0.659, t207=13.999, p<10-31). A similar procedure was applied to test the (HV-LV)D effect at small or large HV+LV. The results showed that at small HV+LV there was a negative (HV-LV)D effect (β=-0.361, t207=-9.208, p<10-16), whereas at large HV+LV there was a positive (HV-LV)D effect (β=0.128, t207 = 3.953, p<10-3).

Finally, we tested the effect of D at different levels of HV-LV and HV+LV. When both HV-LV and HV+LV was small, there was a lack of D effect (β=-0.023, t207=-0.778, p=0.437). When HV-LV was small and HV+LV was large, there was a positive D effect (β=0.206, t207=7.119, p<10-10). When HV-LV was large and HV+LV was small, there was a negative D effect (β=-0.510, t207=-11.414, p<10-22). When HV-LV was large and HV+LV was large, there was a positive D effect (β=0.379, t207=9.606, p<10-17).

We have now reported these results in Appendix 3.

Finally, we found that there was a significant (HV-LV)(HV+LV)D effect in GLM5 (β=0.362, t207=10.417, p<10-19; Appendix 3-figure 2A). Next we examined how the (HV+LV)D effect or (HV-LV)D effect varied as a function of the third variable (HV-LV). One way to examine this is to look at simple effects in sub-sections of the data but because we are now considering a three way interaction, the necessary subsection may be only a quarter in size of the original data, which could produce unreliable results. Hence, we took an alternative approach by using the β weights estimated in GLM5 and investigating the three-way interaction effect. In particular, small and large HV-LV (or HV+LV) was defined as the twenty-fifth percentile (z score=-0.675) and 75th percentile (z score=0.675) respectively. Then we tested the two-way (HV+LV)D effect at small and large HV-LV:

GLM5:

logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + β3 z(D) + β4 z(HV-LV) z(D) + β5 z(HV+LV) z(D) + β6 z(HV-LV) z(HV+LV) + β7 z(HV-LV) z(HV+LV) z(D) + ε

GLM5 at small HV-LV:

logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 (-0.675) + β3 z(D) + β4 (-0.675) z(D) + β5 z(HV+LV) z(D) + β6 (-0.675) z(HV+LV) + β7 (-0.675) z(HV+LV) z(D) + ε

GLM5 at large HV-LV:

logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 (0.675) + β3 z(D) + β4 (0.675) z(D) + β5 z(HV+LV) z(D) + β6 (0.675) z(HV+LV) + β7 (0.675) z(HV+LV) z(D) + ε

As such, the effect of (HV+LV)D at small HV-LV is (β5 – 0.675β7) and that at large HV-LV is (β5 + 0.675β7). The analysis showed that at small HV-LV there was a positive (HV+LV)D effect (β=0.169, t207=5.656, p<10-7) and at large HV-LV the (HV+LV)D effect was even more positive (β=0.659, t207=13.999, p<10-31). A similar procedure was applied to test the (HV-LV)D effect at small or large HV+LV. The results showed that at small HV+LV there was a negative (HV-LV)D effect (β=-0.361, t207=-9.208, p<10-16), whereas at large HV+LV there was a positive (HV-LV)D effect (β=0.128, t207 = 3.953, p<10-3).

We tested the effect of D at different levels of HV-LV and HV+LV. When both HV-LV and HV+LV was small, there was a lack of D effect (β=-0.023, t207=-0.778, p=0.437). When HV-LV was small and HV+LV was large, there was a positive D effect (β=0.206, t207=7.119, p<10-10). When HV-LV was large and HV+LV was small, there was a negative D effect (β=-0.510, t207=-11.414, p<10-22). When HV-LV was large and HV+LV was large, there was a positive D effect (β=0.379, t207=9.606, p<10-17).

2) Some questions remain about the modelling.

2.1) In comment 7.4. Reviewers previously raised the importance of modeling residual time. In response the authors included RT but arbitrarily fixed it to 300ms rather that allowing it to vary freely. They argue in their response letter that "because non-decision time has no reason to be different across our models, it would not bring more evidence in favor of one or another model during model comparisons.". It may be impossible to determine this a-priori because in each model residual-RT might trade-off differently with the other parameters. It would be important to re-fit the model with free residual time parameters to see which model is best. It is important to rule out that the results of model-comparison are due to arbitrary assumptions about residual rt.

Thank you for the suggestion. We have now allowed the non-decision time (Tnd) and inhibition level f (in response to comment 2.2) to vary freely. We have also applied these models to fit the data of Experiments 4-6, in addition to those of Experiments 1-3 that were fitted before (in response to comment 2.4). Critically, the results of the model comparison remain largely similar – in all six experiments the dual route model was a better fit compared to the other three alternative models. In addition, we have run another comparison between all models that we performed – the four new models (with free Tnd and free inhibition f level), the four old mdels (with Tnd fixed at 0.3 sec and f fixed at 0.5) and another four models (with Tnd fixed at 0.3 sec and free f level). The results show that the dual route model with fixed Tnd and f provide the best fit. To summarize, among the four models (dual route, mutual inhibition, divisive normalization, and null) the dual route model provides the best account of participants’ behaviour and it is especially the case when the Tnd and f parameters are fixed.

We have now included these additional models in the Results section:

Additional models were run to confirm that the dual route model is a better model. The above models involve assigning fixed values for the non-decision time Tnd (at 0.3 s) and inhibition level f. In one set of analysis the f is fitted as a free parameter (Figure 3—figure supplement 3B) and in another set of analysis both Tnd and f are fitted as free parameters (Figure 3—figure supplement 3C). In both cases, as in the models with fixed Tnd and f, the dual route model is a better fit compared to the other three alternative models (Ef=0.641, Xp=1.000 and Ef=0.587, Xp=1.000 respectively). Finally, a comparison of all twelve models (four models × three versions of free parameter set) shows that the dual route model with fixed Tnd and f is the best fit (Ef=0.413, Xp=1.000; Figure 3—figure supplement 3D).

2.2) A very similar issues pertains to the parameter f (inhibition) which was also fixed to a constant value rather than being a free parameter. This could potentially affect model comparison results.

We have now followed these useful suggestions. Please refer to the responses to comment 2.1 for details.

2.3) It is still unclear whether in fitting the dual-channel model, each channel had its own free parameters or whether they were identical for both channels.

Thank you for the comment. We agree that it was unclear whether the two components of the dual route model involved separate sets of free parameters or not and in fact they are separated. We have now clarified this in subsection “Model fitting and comparison”:

The d and σ parameters of each model were fitted separately at the individual level to the choices and RTs of each participant in Experiments 1-6, using the VBA-toolbox (http://mbb-team.github.io/VBA-toolbox/). In the dual route model, the mutual inhibition and divisive normalization components involved separate d and σ parameters during the fitting. The other model parameters (i.e. f, Tnd, Vi and k) were fixed at the values mentioned above, except for some models reported in Figure 3—figure supplement 3.”

2.4) Why are models comparisons reported only for Experiments 1-3 but not for the other experiments?

We have presented model comparisons for Experiments 1-3 because the data from these experiments are relatively straightforward and therefore there is little debate about how they might be modelled. It is difficult to apply these models to the data of Experiments 7-8 because of differences in tasks were designed (see responses to Comments 3.1 and 3.2). It is, however, also possible to apply these models to fit the data of Experiments 4-6 because they involve tasks that are very similar to those in Experiments 1-3. The results showed that among the four models, as in Experiments 1-3, the dual route model provided the best account of participants’ behaviour in Experiments 4-6. These results are now included in the Results section and Figure 4—figure supplement 2:

“Finally, the four models (dual-route, mutual inhibition, divisive normalization and null) were applied to fit the data of Experiments 4-6. Again, the dual-route model provided the best account of participants’ behaviour when individual experiments were considered separately (Experiment 4: Ef=0.806, Xp=0.999; Experiment 5: Ef=0.649, Xp=1.000; Experiment 6: Ef=0.946, Xp=1.000; Figure 4—figure supplement 2) or when Experiments 1-6 were considered as a whole (Ef=0.846, Xp=1.000). ” (subsection “Examining distractor effects in further experiments”).

3) In comment 4 the reviewers previously raised questions about the loss trials. The revised version does not fully address these questions.

3.1) There are still questions pertaining to how to model loss trials. According to the current equations, it seems that drift rates can be negative for the mutual inhibition model, and in the normalization model, the drift will be strongest for the highest loss option. Clearly, if this is correct, the model will require adjustments to account for loss trials and these have to be explained.

It is indeed an important to ask about how best to model loss trials in Experiment 7. We agree with the reviewer that one possibility is to have a negative drift rate in the models that are applied to Experiments 1-3. This also implies that the decisions should then be about which option to avoid rather than which option to choose, because options that are more negative in value will reach the decision threshold more quickly. It is unclear whether participants used an avoidance approach in Experiment 7. Hence, we consider that in Experiment 7 (loss experiment) the focus should be on participants’ empirical behavior and the fact that the experiments constitutes a test of whether the distractor affected choices by its value or salience. We think that more detailed modelling is an interesting question but because it requires a new series of arguments about whether it is appropriate to think of avoiding or “ruling out” options we think that it would detract from the discussion of all the other points, which is comprehensive. If, however, we were to add a section relating to whether model loss trials with negative drift rates, which is a whole topic unto itself then we would inevitably deal with this issue in a way that some readers might consider partial. We have, however, added some text to the revised manuscript that notes that it might be possible in the future to extend the model to look at this loss trials but that we have refrained from a detailed modelling of loss trials because of the number of other issues that need to be debated whenever a model of this type is constructed. We have therefore added the following paragraph to the end of subsection “Experiment 7: Loss experiment” reporting the results of Experiment 7:

In the future it might be possible to extend the models outlined in Figure 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 1 to provide a more quantitative descriptions of behavior. While this topic is of great interest it will require modelers to agree on how loss trials might be modelled. For example, one possibility is to have a negative drift rate in the models that we have used. This implies that the decisions will then be about which option to avoid rather than which option to choose, because options that are more negative in value will reach the decision threshold more quickly. It is unclear, however, whether participants used such an avoidance approach in Experiment 7. Hence, we have refrained from modelling the results of Experiment 7.

3.2) There are also questions pertaining to whether and how the model can account for differences between gain and loss trials. These are important issues because the results seem quite different for gain and loss trials. It would be important to perform a model comparison for the loss trial to determine the best model for these trials. It is not clear if the dual route model, or one of the simpler models, is best for loss trials. Additionally- looking at Figure 6 and the results of the regression (panel d) it seems that when D is positive (i.e., D = abs(D)) corresponding regression effects for these two terms offset each other but when D is negative (D = -abs(D)) they compound. So this could simply mean that the distractor effects are stronger for losses than for gains. Is this true? This can be seen, by including in the regression, interactions terms with trial identity (what the authors call GainTrial) instead of terms with abs(D). Furthermore, if distractors effects are indeed stronger for losses then these stronger effects could presumably be caused by adjusting model parameters (e.g., inhibition strength or other parameters). It is important to examine this. In sum, the authors should consider fitting models to loss trials to see (1) which model provides the best account for loss trials, (2) what account do the mechanistic models provide loss trials and for differences between gain and loss trials. This will provide a much more informative understanding of the gain-loss issue as compared to the current reliance on the regression model. Currently the authors argue that there are 2 separate effects in play, one for distractor value and one for distractor saliency. But a more informative way to understand the data might be that the context (gain/loss) modulated the distractor value effect, and to query the mechanistic models to identify the locus of this modulations.

Again we thank the reviewer for this interesting point but once again we feel that detailed modelling of the loss trials is beyond the scope of the current manuscript because of the number of other issues that need to be dealt with in order to consider how loss trials might be modelled with precision. We have dealt with the major points that were raised by the reviewers in the first round of review and we have moved large parts of the manuscript to the supplementary materials to make way for the many new sections that the reviewers have requested but detailed modelling of the loss trials really is beyond the scope of the current manuscript because it would require us to address several other, distinct areas of research. We feel that this is likely to detract from the main message our manuscript is conveying.

4) Reviewers were still unclear about the meaning of terms in the GLMs. This needs to be clarified so that the models are better understood and evaluated. Just for example consider GLM1:

logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1(HV-LV) + β2(D-HV) + β3(HV-LV)(D-HV) + ε

The authors state that "All interaction terms in all GLMs are calculated after the component terms are z-scored". Does this mean that the terms are z-scored only for the purpose of calculating the interaction, or are they z-scored for the main effects as well? Reviewers think they should be z-scored in all terms not just in interaction terms. Additionally, just to be sure- did the authors z-score HV and LV separately or z-score the difference (HV-LV)? A clearer way to write the model to avoid confusions could be:

logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z_(HV-LV) + β2z_(D-HV) + β3z_(HV-LV)*z_(D-HV) + ε (underscore indicates subscript).

We agree that there is still some potential ambiguity of how the GLMs are described. In this study, all terms are z-scored before entering into a GLM, which may not be clear in the previous version of the manuscript. We have now followed the format suggested by the reviewer and updated our Materials and methods section (only the lines showing the GLMs are shown here).

GLM1a: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(D-HV) + β3 z(HV-LV) z(D-HV) + ε

GLM1b: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + β3 z(D-HV) + β4 z(HV-LV) z(D-HV) + ε

GLM1c: ln(PHV/Pj) = βj,0 + βj,1 z(HV-LV) + βj,2 z(D-HV) + βj,3 z(HV-LV) z(D-HV) + εj

GLM2a: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + ε1

Step 2, ε1 = β3 + β4 z(D-HV) + ε2

GLM2b: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + ε1

Step 2, ε1 = β3 + β4 z(D) + ε2

GLM2c: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(Difficulty) + ε1

Step 2, ε1 = β2 + β3 z(D-HV) + ε2

GLM2d: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(Difficulty) + ε1

Step 2, ε1 = β2 + β3 z(D) + ε2

Where in GLM2c,d Difficulty = w1 z[Mag(HV)-Mag(LV)) + w2 z[Mag(HV)+Mag(LV)) +

w3 z[Prob(HV)-Prob(LV)] + w4 z[Prob(HV)+Prob(LV)]

GLM3a: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + ε

GLM3b: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z[Mag(HV)-Mag(LV)) + β 2 z[Mag(HV)+Mag(LV)] + β 3 z[Prob(HV)-Prob(LV)] +

β 4 z[Prob(HV)+Prob(LV)] + ε

GLM4: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(SubjDiff) + β2 z(Congruence) + β3 z(D-HV) +

β4 z(SubjDiff) z(D-HV) + ε

GLM5: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + β3 z(D) + β4 z(HV-LV) z(D) + β5 z(HV+LV) z(D) + β6 z(HV-LV) z(HV+LV) + β7 z(HV-LV) z(HV+LV) z(D) + ε

GLM6a: logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(GainTrial) + β2 z(HV-LV) + β3 z(D) + β4 z(HV-LV) z(D) + β5 z(|D|) +

β5 z(HV-LV) z(|D|) + ε

GLM6b: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(GainTrial) + β2 z(HV-LV) + β3 z(HV+LV) + ε1

Step 2, ε1 = β4 + β5 z(D) + ε2

GLM6c: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(GainTrial) + β2 z(HV-LV) + β3 z(HV+LV) + ε1

Step 2, ε1 = β4 + β5 z(|D|) + ε2

GLM7: Fixj = βj,0 + βj,1 z(HV-LV) + βj,2 z(HV+LV) + βj,3 z(D) + εj

GLM8: Step 1, Shiftj = βj,0 + βj,1 z[Fix(HV)] + βj,2 z[Fix(LV)] + βj,3 z[Fix(D)] + εj,1

Step 2, εj,1 = βj,4 + βj,5 z(HV) + βj,6 z(LV) + βj,7 z(D) + εj,2

GLM9: Step 1, logit(accuracy) = β0 + β1 z(HV-LV) + β2 z(HV+LV) + β3 z(D) + ε1

Step 2, ε1 = β4 + β5 z[Shift(D-to-HV)] + β6 z[Shift(D-to-LV)] + β7 z[Shift(HV-to-D)] + β8 z[Shift(LV-to-D)] +

β9 z[Shift(LV-to-HV)] + β10 z[Shift(HV-to-LV)] + ε2

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Chau BKH, Law C, Lopez-Persem A, Klein-Flügge MC, Rushworth MFS. 2019. Consistent patterns of distractor effects during decision making. Dryad Digital Repository. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
    2. Chau BKH, Kolling N. 2018. Data from: A neural mechanism underlying failure of optimal choice with multiple alternatives. Dryad Digital Repository. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
    3. Spektor MS, Gluth S, Rieskamp J. 2018. Value-based attentional capture affects multi-alternative decision making. Open Science Framework. 8r4fh [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Supplementary tables.

    (A) Regression coefficients estimated using the analysis procedures suggested by Gluth et al., 2018. (B) Simulation 1: testing the probabilities of Type I and II errors when distractor effects were assumed to be absent in the simulated choice data. (C) Simulation 2: an alternative approach for testing the probabilities of Type I and II errors when distractor effects were assumed to be absent in the simulated choice data. (D) Simulation 3: testing the probabilities of Type I and II errors when distractor effects were assumed to be present in the simulated choice data.

    elife-53850-supp1.docx (33.7KB, docx)
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    The codes for running the mutual inhibition model, divisive normalisation model, dual route model and null model can be found at: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.k6djh9w3c. Behavioural data of Experiments 1, 3 and 7 can be found at: https://datadryad.org/stash/dataset/doi:10.5061/dryad.040h9t7. Behavioural data of Experiments 2, 4–6 can be found from a link provided by Gluth et al., 2018 at: https://osf.io/8r4fh/. Behavioural and eye tracking data of Experiment eight can be found at: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.k6djh9w3c.

    The codes for running the mutual inhibition model, divisive normalization model, dual route model and null model can be found at: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.k6djh9w3c Behavioural data of Experiments 1, 3 and 7 can be found at: https://datadryad.org/stash/dataset/doi:10.5061/dryad.040h9t7 Behavioural data of Experiments 2, 4-6 can be found from a link provided by Gluth and colleagues (2018) at: https://osf.io/8r4fh/ Behavioural and eye tracking data of Experiment 8 can be found at: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.k6djh9w3c.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Chau BKH, Law C, Lopez-Persem A, Klein-Flügge MC, Rushworth MFS. 2019. Consistent patterns of distractor effects during decision making. Dryad Digital Repository.

    The following previously published datasets were used:

    Chau BKH, Kolling N. 2018. Data from: A neural mechanism underlying failure of optimal choice with multiple alternatives. Dryad Digital Repository.

    Spektor MS, Gluth S, Rieskamp J. 2018. Value-based attentional capture affects multi-alternative decision making. Open Science Framework. 8r4fh


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES