Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2020 Jul 22;10:12179. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-67893-8

Altered spatial summation optimizes visual function in axial myopia

Victoria Stapley 1, Roger S Anderson 1,2, Kathryn J Saunders 1, Pádraig J Mulholland 1,2,
PMCID: PMC7376210  PMID: 32699286

Abstract

This study demonstrates significant differences between the area of complete spatial summation (Ricco’s area, RA) in eyes with and without non-pathological, axial myopia. Contrast thresholds were measured for six stimuli (0.01–2.07 deg2) presented at 10º eccentricity in 24 myopic subjects and 20 age-similar non-myopic controls, with RA estimated using iterative two-phase regression analysis. To explore the effects of axial length-induced variations in retinal image size (RIS) on the measurement of RA, refractive error was separately corrected with (i) trial lenses at the anterior focal point (near constant inter-participant RIS in mm), and (ii) contact lenses (RIS changed with axial length). For spectacle corrected measurements, RA was significantly larger in the myopic group, with a significant positive correlation also being observed between RA and measures of co-localised peripheral ocular length. With contact lens correction, there was no significant difference in RA between the groups and no relationship with peripheral ocular length. The results suggest RA changes with axial elongation in myopia to compensate for reduced retinal ganglion cell density. Furthermore, as these changes are only observed when axial length induced variations in RIS are accounted for, they may reflect a functional adaptation of the axially-myopic visual system to an enlarged RIS.

Subject terms: Retina, Medical research

Introduction

Myopia is a common refractive condition, whereby the axial length of the globe is too great for its optical power. Whilst the optical refractive error of myopia can be corrected using spectacles or contact lenses, the axial elongation of the myopic eye can markedly increase the risk of sight-threatening conditions such as retinal detachment1, glaucoma2, and myopic macular degeneration3. In the absence of such pathological processes it has also been demonstrated that the globe elongation that occurs in myopia can lead to secondary peripheral retinal thinning46, in addition to a reduction in the density of both photoreceptors79 and retinal ganglion cells (RGCs)10,11.

Deficits in visual function have also been reported in the myopic, but otherwise healthy, visual system. Numerous studies have objectively investigated retinal function in myopia through measurement of standard electroretinograms (ERG)12,13 pattern ERG14 and multifocal ERG4,13,15. These studies have revealed altered responses in myopes, including reductions in amplitude1214 and longer implicit times4,13,15. Other studies have reported reductions in function when examined using clinical tests of visual acuity16,17, peripheral resolution acuity4,18,19, and contrast sensitivity20.

It may be hypothesized that changes in visual function observed in non-pathological myopia may be accounted for by reductions in the local density of retinal neurons (e.g., RGCs) and corresponding alterations in the basic visual process of spatial summation. This refers to the ability of visual system to integrate light energy over area and serves to maximize the detection of a signal in the presence of visual noise. Spatial summation is governed by Ricco’s law, this stating that for stimuli of sufficiently small area summation is complete, with the product of stimulus area and contrast at threshold being constant21. Ricco’s area (RA) is the largest area for which complete spatial summation occurs, with incomplete summation being exhibited for stimuli larger than RA. The size of RA has been shown to increase in the healthy visual system with retinal eccentricity2226, and reduced background illuminance2729, as well as in some forms of ocular disease such as glaucoma3032. It has been hypothesized that such dynamic changes may serve as a mechanism to maintain the input of a constant number of functional RGCs to cortical receptive fields, thus ensuring a constant sensitivity in the presence of visual noise24,31,33,34. We hypothesise that similar changes in spatial summation are likely to occur in non-pathological myopia to compensate for reduced RGC density secondary to ocular growth and retinal stretch.

Two previous studies have investigated spatial summation in myopia. Jaworski et al.35 restricted their measurements to the foveal region only, comparing emmetropes to high myopes (mean refractive error −10D). The authors observed a 55% and 43% increase in the size of what was defined as the ‘critical area at maximum summation’ in myopia for S-cone and achromatic stimuli respectively. However, the increase noted for the achromatic stimulus failed to reach statistical significance, likely due to the small sample size. Spatial summation was subsequently measured by Atchison et al.19 for a larger cohort of myopes, with refractive errors ranging from −0.50D to −12.5D, at a range of visual field eccentricities from 0 to 30 degrees along the horizontal meridian. An increase in RA was observed at the fovea and in the temporal visual field in myopia, but no significant changes were observed nasally. Both studies however used a constrained fitting technique, whereby the slope of the first and second lines in a bi-linear summation function were fixed, assuming either complete or a fixed degree of partial or no summation, this method being known to bias estimates of RA36. In addition, neither study investigated the effect of prospectively controlling axial-length induced alterations in retinal image size (RIS) on measures of spatial summation despite the fact that RIS is larger in axial myopes compared to emmetropic or hyperopic observers. Indeed, it has been proposed that a ‘neural minification’, occurring secondary to an increased spacing of retinal elements and possibly reflective of altered spatial summation, likely accounts for an enlarged RIS in axial-myopia and may serve to optimize visual function in myopic observers37,38. Considering this, we propose that the presence of altered neural processing in the myopic visual system may only be manifest when a constant inter-observer RIS, which is independent of axial length, is employed. To-date no study has investigated this.

The purpose of this study was to determine if RA is enlarged in non-pathological axial myopia and to quantify the relative contribution of local neural elements (e.g., RGC layer thickness, RGC number) to measures of spatial summation. The effect of higher-order aberrations and axial length induced differences in RIS on spatial summation was also investigated with a view to isolating optical and neural induced changes on this neurophysiological process.

Methods

Participants

Twenty-four participants with axial myopia (mean 26.9, range 18–58 years) and twenty age-similar non-myopic controls (mean 26.4, range 19–53 years) were recruited for this study. All participants had a best corrected Snellen visual acuity of 20/20 (6/6) or better in both eyes, astigmatism < 1.50DC in the test eye, no visual field defect measured with the 24–2 SITA standard threshold test (Humphrey Visual Field Analyser, Carl Zeiss Meditec, Dublin, CA) and intraocular pressure ≤ 21 mmHg as measured using Goldmann applanation tonometry. Peripapillary retinal-nerve-fibre-layer (RNFL) scans also revealed RNFL thickness to be within normal limits and macular OCT scans revealed no abnormalities (Spectralis OCT, Heidelberg Engineering Gmbh., Heidelberg, Germany). A clinical examination identified no media opacities or concurrent ophthalmic disease, and participants did not have any systemic conditions or take any medications that could affect vision.

Refractive error was measured objectively in each participant using a binocular open-field autorefractor (Shin Nippon NVision-K 5001, Japan) following the instillation of Tropicamide Hydrochloride 1.0%. Participants fixated on a Maltese cross target positioned on a flat wall at a distance of six meters, with an average of three measures being taken. Myopia was defined as a spherical equivalent refractive error ≤ −0.50DS39. The myopic group had central refractive errors ranging from −0.50DS to −9.75DS (mean −4.14 DS), with refractive errors ranging from −0.25DS to +1.75DS (mean +0.71 DS) in the control group. Based on the World Health Organisation (2015) definitions, nine participants were defined as having high-myopia (≤ –5.00DS), with the remainder (n = 15) having myopia in the range −0.50DS to −4.75DS (low-moderate myopia). The characteristics of each group, along with biometric measurements, are displayed in Table 1.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the myopic and control groups.

Controls (n = 20) Myopes (n = 24) Low-moderate myopes (n = 15) High myopes (n = 9)
Age (years) 22.50 (20.00 to 31.00) 23.00 (20.00 to 28.50) 22.00 (19.50 to 28.00) 23.00 (22.00 to 27.00)
Refractive error BVS (DS)  +0.50 (0.00 to +1.25) −3.63 (−2.00 to −6.00) −2.50 (−1.75 to −3.75) −7.00 (−5.63 to −7.88)
Astigmatism (DC) −0.25 (0.00 to −0.75) −0.50 (0.00 to −1.00) −0.50 (0.00 to −1.00) −0.50 (0.00 to −1.00)
Axial length (mm) 23.64 (23.01 to 24.01) 25.20 (24.56 to 26.00) 24.61 (24.24 to 25.41) 26.33 (25.64 to 27.95)
Anterior chamber depth (mm) 3.60 (3.45 to 3.86) 3.73 (3.53 to 3.90) 3.72 (3.52 to 3.80) 3.93 (3.62 to 4.05)
Average corneal curvature (mm) 7.91 (7.81 to 8.07) 7.79 (7.62 to 7.91) 7.84 (7.67 to 8.00) 7.64 (7.41 to 8.14)

Summary values are presented as median (IQR).

This study received ethical approval from the Ulster University Biomedical Sciences Research Ethics Filter Committee and the research adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed, written consent was given by all subjects prior to data collection.

Refractive correction

For all participants, refractive correction was achieved by (i) full aperture trial lenses placed at the anterior focal point of the eye (15.2 mm) such that Knapp’s law, minimizing relative spectacle magnification, was satisfied (i.e., RIS equal to that in an emmetropic eye was maintained for all participants with varying axial ametropia) and, (ii) soft contact lenses where Knapp’s law was not satisfied (i.e., RIS was not equal with varying axial ametropia)40. The power of trial lens for correction was determined by non-cycloplegic objective refraction (Shin Nippon NVision-K 5001 binocular open field autorefractor, Shin-Nippon, Tokyo, Japan) and subjective refraction at a 6 m viewing distance. For all experimental tests an appropriate, subjectively refined near addition was incorporated to account for reduced accommodative facility post pupil-dilation and the monitor viewing distance. The correct back vertex adjustment was made for refractive errors ≤ −4.00DS when calculating the power of contact lens correction to use. The order in which participants undertook the spectacle corrected and contact lens corrected measurements of spatial summation was randomized to minimize any bias due to learning effects or fatigue. Refractive correction was provided to the test eye only, with the fellow eye occluded using an opaque eye-patch.

Apparatus and stimuli

All stimuli were presented on a gamma-corrected CRT display (SONY 420GS; Sony Corp., Tokyo, Japan; pixel resolution, 1,280 × 965, refresh rate 75 Hz, viewing distance 620 mm) after a 1.5 h warm up period. The achromatic background had a mean luminance of 10 cd/m2 and the maximum luminance of the test stimuli was 126.6 cd/m2. The chromaticity co-ordinates of both the background and stimuli were x = 0.258 and y = 0.257 as measured using a colorimeter (ColorCAL-II, Cambridge Research Systems, Rochester, UK). Stimuli were generated using MATLAB (2016b, The MathWorks Inc., USA) with Psychtoolbox (v3.0) and a Bits-# (Cambridge Research Systems, Rochester, UK). Participant responses were collected using a Cedrus RB-540 response pad (Cedrus Corporation, San Pedro, CA).

Experimental measurements were either completed on the same day as the screening tests, or on a separate day depending upon individual preference. All experimental measurements were carried out on one eye only with the pupil of the test eye being dilated with Tropicamide Hydrochloride 1.0% to maintain a constant photopic inter-observer retinal illuminance. Contrast thresholds for six, achromatic, circular stimuli of area ranging from 0.01–2.07 deg2 and Bridgeman41 duration 187.8 ms (15 frames) were measured at four peripheral locations at 10º eccentricity (along 90º, 180º, 270º and 360º meridians). Participants were asked to fixate on a central cross target throughout all measurements. To account for spatial luminance inhomogeneity of the CRT display, localized contrast thresholds were determined for each test location using luminance values for the background and stimulus measured at each test location using a colorimeter (ColorCAL-II, Cambridge Research Systems, Rochester, UK).

To determine if higher order aberrations (HOAs) influence measures of spatial summation these were measured using an aberrometer (Imagine Eyes irx3 Wavefront Aberrometer, France) in the test eye, post dilation, both with and without a contact lens in situ. All measures were captured immediately post-blink such that habitual tear film and optical quality were reflected in the measures. Accurate alignment between the pupillary plane of the eye and the instrument lenslet array was obtained through the adjustment of an internal graticule over the pupil and focusing of the Purkinje images. The participant was asked to fixate on the internal target, a black 6/12 (20/40) letter ‘E’ on a white background. Three measurements were taken under each condition and an average obtained. HOAs were analysed over a 6-mm pupil using Zernike polynomials (ZPs) from third to sixth order. The root mean square (RMS) of the total HOAs (3rd–6th order ZPs) was used in further analysis.

Psychophysical procedure

Contrast thresholds for the six achromatic stimuli were determined using a randomly interleaved 1–1 ‘YES–NO’ staircase procedure, with a 0.05 log unit (0.5 dB) step size. Each stimulus area was considered in a separate run in a randomized order, with thresholds for the four locations being measured within each stimulus run in a randomly interleaved fashion. Each staircase terminated after six reversals with the threshold being calculated as the mean of the final four reversals. False positive rate was monitored using the presentation of stimuli of 0% contrast, with tests being rejected and repeated if the false positive rate was above 20%. Following each stimulus presentation, a listening window of two seconds for the collection of participant responses was permitted. If, following the closure of this listening window, no response was collected the stimulus was assumed to be unseen.

Structural measurements

Co-localized structural measures of peripheral ocular length and retinal-ganglion-cell-layer (RGCL) thickness were obtained following the instillation of Tropicamide Hydrochloride 1%. Peripheral ocular length measurements were captured using an IOL Master (Carl-Zeiss Meditec, USA). A custom-built four-LED ring target was affixed to the front of the instrument to allow peripheral measurements at 10° along the four primary meridians. Three measurements were taken at each position, with an average peripheral ocular length being calculated for each participant. Possible confounding effects of ocular rotation on measurements of peripheral ocular length were presumed insignificant due to the small eccentricity measured and short duration of eccentric fixation required to obtain the measurement.42,43 Previous work has also reported that the IOL-Master is capable of repeatable and reliable off-axis measurements up to 40° eccentricity44.

RGCL thickness values were obtained by taking a 24° × 24° posterior pole scan centred on the fovea with the Spectralis OCT (Heidelberg Engineering Gmbh., Heidelberg, Germany). Participant mean keratometry values were input to minimize the effects of inter-individual variations in ocular magnification on transverse measures captured45. An 8 × 8 grid was then centred over the fovea with any errors in the automated segmentation being manually corrected. Mean RGCL thickness across the measurement grid squares (3° × 3°) within which the corresponding locations examined in the visual field fell (after correction for retinal ganglion offset from underlying photoreceptors46) was used to examine the relationship between functional measures and underlying retinal structure.

The number of RGCs underlying RA in each observer was also estimated using two methods in this study. In method one histological RGC counts from an age-similar cohort47 were used to produce normative values of RGC/mm2 over the central retina (4 mm eccentricity). These values were subsequently scaled to simulate a global expansion (‘balloon’) model of myopia, whereby RGC density proportionally changed for axial length values that departed from that expected in an emmetropic eye (23.3 mm)48, assuming a constant number of RGCs. The number of RGCs underlying a given stimulus area was subsequently calculated as the product of the mean histologically derived RGC/mm2 values over the area of stimulus presentation and stimulus area in mm2 (histology method, RGCHist). The second method utilized the technique described by Raza and Hood49 to infer the RGC number underlying a stimulus in a given observer from OCT data (RGCOCT, Eq. 1). In short, this used OCT derived RGCL thickness (mm) in a given observer (RGCL), co-localized stimulus area (Sarea, mm2), and normative RGC volumetric density (RGC/mm3) of RGCL tissue (GCD, calculated by dividing the mean RGC/mm2 across the area of the stimulus extrapolated from unscaled, age-similar histological data47 with co-localized OCT derived RGCL thickness [mm] values in healthy, non-myopic observers).

RGCOCT=RGCL·GCD·Sarea 1

For all calculations, an observer specific conversion factor (qp) was calculated using the abbreviated axial length method50 to translate degrees of visual space to mm on the retina at the test eccentricity. This value was a constant when considering spectacle corrected data, and proportional to axial length with contact lens correction in this study. Further details on both models to estimate RGC number are available in the Supplementary materials.

Statistical analysis

For each participant, an average contrast threshold for each stimulus size was calculated across the four peripheral locations, a spatial summation function then being plotted using these average values. In the case of the contrast threshold at a given location being greater than the maximum output of the display monitor used (ceiling effect) these data were excluded from analysis. Summation functions were fit using iterative two-phase regression analysis where the slope of the first line in the function was constrained to −1 (reflecting complete summation), but the slope and intercept of the second line (representing partial summation) was free to vary. The intersection of the two lines was taken as the upper limit of complete summation or RA. Data were excluded from further analysis if the bilinear model had a poor fit (R2 < 0.9), or if RA was smaller than the smallest stimulus used. If the estimated RA value was greater than the largest stimulus used, RA was taken to be the largest stimulus area.

To investigate the relationship between the size of RA and co-localized ocular length and RGCL thickness measures, Passing-Bablok regression (transformation method) was used. This technique was chosen as it is suitable for a non-parametric data set, permits error in both the x and y variables, is less influenced by the presence of outliers and has been demonstrated to yield more precise estimates of slope and intercept compared to ordinary least squares or Deming regression51,52. A central assumption of this analysis is that the relationship between the x and y variables is linear. This was tested using a cumulative sum (cusum test), with a null hypothesis that the variables are linear. The other prior assumption is that there is a significant positive correlation between the two variables, as determined by Kendall’s tau correlation53. If a significant, positive, linear correlation exists, then a regression line was plotted using the Passing-Bablok procedure. For all analyses, the strength of any correlation was obtained with Kendall’s tau correlation coefficient where a linear relationship between variables was demonstrated with a cusum test.

Statistical analysis was carried out using MATLAB (2019a, The MathWorks Inc., USA) and R (Version 3.6.2). For all statistical tests an alpha of 0.05 was considered statistically significant, with Holm-Bonferroni correction applied where indicated. In all cases a Shapiro–Wilk test was used to determine if data sets followed a normal distribution and the appropriate parametric or non-parametric statistical tests were applied accordingly.

Results

Contribution of axial elongation to refractive error

To determine if Knapp’s Law may be invoked in the study cohort, and thus ensure that only neural contributions to RA were investigated, it was necessary to demonstrate that the refractive error of participants was axial in origin. This was achieved by calculating the spherical equivalent refractive error from measures of axial length assuming the ametropia was solely axial in origin (DP, based upon the method of Chui et al.18 using the Bennett and Rabbetts three-surface schematic eye54, Eq. 2 where AL = axial length in mm) and comparing these estimates with ground truth values (DObs, objectively measured refractive error) for the whole study cohort (i.e., myopes and non-myopes).

DP=1.531/AL/1000-63.8 2

Spearman’s rank correlation analysis revealed there to be a strong and statistically significant relationship between the estimated and predicted refractive error values (rho = 0.81, P < 0.001, Fig. 1). No statistically significant difference between the measured and predicted refractive error values were also observed when examined using a Wilcoxon-Signed Rank test (P = 0.12).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Plot of predicted refractive error (based on all refractive error being axial in origin) and objectively measured refractive error. The line of equality (yellow) is included for reference.

Higher-order aberrations

Unaided, no significant differences in the Root Mean Square (RMS) for total HOA were observed between the myopia and control groups (control: mean 0.33 µm ± 0.12; myopia: mean 0.34 µm ± 0.10, unpaired t-test P = 0.95). In addition, no significant relationship existed between RMS values and either refractive error (Kendall’s tau = 0.08, P = 0.46) or axial length (Kendall’s tau = −0.13, P = 0.20). For both study groups, the mean RMS for total HOA increased with the contact lens in situ (control: mean 0.36 ± 0.13; myopia: mean 0.39 ± 0.10), but this increase was only found to be statistically significant for the myopic group (myopia: P = 0.02; control: P = 0.23, paired t-test). There were no statistically significant differences in HOAs between the myopia and control groups with contact lenses in situ (P = 0.36, unpaired t-test), and no significant relationship between HOA with contact lenses and either axial length (Kendall’s tau = −0.04, P = 0.70) or refractive error (Kendall’s tau = −0.02, P = 0.89).

Spatial summation in myopes vs non-myopes

For spectacle corrected measurements, an average peripheral RA value was obtained for all participants in the myopia group and 19 out of 20 participants in the control group (one control participant was excluded as RA was smaller than the smallest stimulus examined). For contact lens corrected measurements, an average peripheral RA was obtained for 23 out of the 24 participants in the myopia group (one participant excluded as R2 < 0.9 for fitted summation function) and all control observers.

For spectacle corrected measurements, median RA was significantly larger (P = 0.03, Mann Whitney U-test) in the myopia group (−0.81 log deg2, IQR −0.97 to −0.72) compared to the control group (−1.13 log deg2, IQR −1.34 to −0.88). For contact lens corrected measurements, no significant difference (P = 0.42, Mann Whitney U-test) was observed between the myopia (−1.10 log deg2, IQR −1.27 to −0.91) and control groups (−0.97 log deg2, IQR −1.22 to −0.83). Data are displayed graphically as boxplots in Fig. 2 and summary summation functions (using median thresholds) in Fig. 3. When comparing spectacle and contact lens measures for the same individual, RA was found to be significantly smaller in the myopia group when corrected with CL compared to spectacles (P = 0.02, Wilcoxon signed-rank) (Fig. 2). In contrast, no significant difference in RA was observed for the controls when measured with contact lenses and when measured with spectacles (P = 0.62, Wilcoxon signed-rank).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Average peripheral Ricco’s area measured for myopes and controls with spectacle and contact lens correction. Spot markers representing data from individual observers are included for reference.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Summary spatial summation functions constructed using median thresholds for (A) controls—spectacle corrected, (B) controls—contact lens corrected, (C) myopes—spectacle corrected, and (D) myopes—contact lens corrected.

Interestingly, the contrast at threshold for a stimulus equal to RA was found to be lower in the myopia group (median 0.27 log ∆I, IQR 0.12–0.53) compared to controls (median 0.42 log ∆I, IQR 0.12–0.48) for the spectacle corrected data; this difference however failed to reach statistical significance (P = 0.15, Mann Whitney U-test, Fig. 4). No difference in the threshold at RA was observed between the groups when the contact lens corrected data were examined (Myope median 0.41 log ∆I, IQR 0.22–0.56; Control median 0.48 log ∆I, IQR 0.21–0.53; P = 0.99, Mann Whitney U-test, Fig. 4).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Contrast thresholds for a stimulus equal to Ricco’s area in the control and myopia groups as measured with spectacle and contact lens correction.

Relationship between Ricco’s area and structural measures

For spectacle corrected measurements, a weak yet statistically significant positive linear relationship was observed between peripheral RA and corresponding peripheral ocular length values (Kendall’s tau = 0.23, P = 0.03, Fig. 5A). Passing-Bablok regression revealed that RA (log deg2) increases by a factor of 13.5 per log unit increase in co-localized peripheral ocular length. For the contact lens corrected measurements, no significant relationship between peripheral RA and co-localised peripheral ocular length was observed (Kendall’s tau = −0.05, P = 0.62, Fig. 5B).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Peripheral Ricco’s area plotted as a function of peripheral ocular length for (A) spectacle corrected measurements and (B) CL corrected measurements.

Mean peripheral RGCL thickness was significantly thinner in the myopia group compared to the controls in the locations examined (P < 0.01, unpaired t-test). There was also a significant (p = 0.02) negative relationship (Kendall’s tau = −0.24) between mean peripheral ocular length (log mm) and mean peripheral RGCL-thickness (log µm). When considering the relationship between peripheral RGCL thickness and spectacle-corrected RA, a weak, negative correlation was observed (Kendall’s tau = −0.16). This relationship however failed to reach statistical significance (P = 0.13). No relationship between RGCL thickness and contact-lens-corrected RA data was observed (Kendall’s tau = −0.03, P = 0.81). The results for spectacles and contact lens corrected data are displayed in Figs. 6A and B respectively.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Peripheral Ricco’s area plotted against peripheral RGCL thickness for (A) spectacle corrected measurements and (B) CL corrected measurements.

Retinal ganglion cell number underlying Ricco’s area

Using both methods of calculation, no statistically significant difference in RGC number underlying RA was observed between the myopia and control groups with spectacle or contact lens correction (histology method: Kruskal–Wallis χ2(3) = 6.3, P = 0.10; OCT method: Kruskal–Wallis χ2(3) = 6.6, P = 0.09). Despite this, estimates of RGC number underlying RA (median, IQR) were found to be higher in the myopia cohort (histology: 81.7 cells, IQR 57.6 to 103.9; OCT: 78.4 cells, IQR 58.4 to 102.1) compared to control observers (histology: 43.4 cells, IQR 26.3 to 71.7; OCT: 42.9 cells, IQR 25.1 to 73.7) when examined with spectacle correction (Fig. 7). Conversely, RGC number was lower in the myopia cohort (histology: 46.9 cells, IQR 33.3 to 82.8; OCT: 44.8 cells, IQR 33.9 to 83.8) compared to controls (histology: 60.8 cells, IQR 36.1 to 89.5; OCT: 58.9 cells, IQR 36.3 to 93.9) with contact lens correction.

Figure 7.

Figure 7

Boxplots reporting the number of RGCs underlying Ricco’s area in the control and myopia cohorts with spectacle and contact lens correction as estimated using (A) scaled histological data, and (B) OCT derived RGCL thickness values.

Discussion

When inter-observer differences in the projected retinal image size are controlled for (Knapp’s law invoked), peripheral RA was found to be larger in the myopia group compared to non-myopic controls. Such differences were not present when identical psychophysical measures were performed with contact lens correction where RIS varied proportionally with axial length (i.e., Knapp’s Law was not satisfied). To our knowledge, this is also the first study to observe a statistically significant, positive correlation between peripheral RA (spectacle corrected) and co-localized measurements of peripheral ocular length.

The finding of altered spatial summation in myopia is in agreement with the two previous studies that have investigated this topic. Jaworski et al.35 reported the foveal ‘critical area’ to be 0.16 log units larger in a high-myopia cohort (refractive error above −8.50DS) compared to non-myopic controls for an achromatic stimulus in the fovea. Atchison et al.19 also considered spatial summation in a large cohort both centrally and out to 30º along the horizontal meridian, the authors reporting a 0.03 log unit increase in RA per diopter increase in myopia. While such trends point towards altered spatial summation in axial myopia, differences in functional testing methodology and statistical analyses severely limit inter-study comparisons. For example, only the fovea was examined by Jaworski et al.35 compared with a region at 10º eccentricity in the current study, it being known that spatial summation varies with visual field eccentricity.22,26 Another key difference is the use of contrasting summary values to reflect the extent of spatial summation being exhibited. Jaworski et al.35 compared ‘critical area at maximum summation’ in myopes and non-myopic controls, defining this metric as the transition from partial summation to no summation. In the present study and that of Atchison et al.19, the upper limit of complete spatial summation (RA) was used to describe the extent of spatial summation. Furthermore, a constrained fitting technique was used by both Jaworski et al.35 and Atchison et al.19 to generate summation functions; a methodology which can lead to inaccuracies when extracting summary values from summation data36. Other inter-study differences include stimulus chromaticity, background luminance and psychophysical test setup (e.g., staircase step-size, auditory stimuli, etc.)

Other studies have previously presented evidence in support of changes in spatial vision, and by inference spatial summation, in myopia. For example, it has been reported that visual acuity16,17 and peripheral resolution acuity4,18,19 are reduced in myopia. Other work quantifying aniseikonia in participants with anisomyopia also provides evidence for altered spatial summation. Bradley and colleagues37 used a dichoptic size matching test with identical inter-eye RIS (i.e., Knapp’s law invoked) to reveal large degrees of residual aniseikonia (22%) with spectacle lens correction, such differences being proportional to the degree of axial elongation. Interestingly, in two observers with measures repeated with contact lens correction (where Knapp’s law did not hold) aniseikonia was markedly reduced (3.9%) in their study. Such results closely reflect the observations made in the present study whereby RA was related to axial length when RIS was optically controlled, this relationship not being apparent with CL correction. Bradley et al.37 propose that such findings may be related to perceptual minification of the retinal image in the myopic eye, potentially arising secondary to inter-eye differences in retinal stretching. Similar work undertaken by Rabin et al.38 proposed that axial anisometropia-induced aniseikonia reflects differences in the spatial density of ‘retinal elements’.

Physiological basis of altered spatial summation in myopia

Much debate surrounds the physiological basis of spatial summation in the human visual system. It has been proposed that the density of retinal neurons (e.g., photoreceptors, RGCs)31,55, RGC receptive field organization34,56,57 and higher visual centers55,58 each contribute to the measured RA or ‘perceptive field’, with changes to the functional or structural integrity of these features potentially inducing alterations in spatial summation. Previous work examining photopic spatial summation in observers with no eye disease, found RA to enlarge as a function of visual field eccentricity22, this change being attributed to variations in the density of retinal neurons moderating stimulus detection. Work examining spatial summation in glaucoma reported similar changes to occur secondary to reductions in functional RGC density31, it being hypothesized that such alterations in spatial summation occur to maintain input to cortical receptive fields from a constant number of functionally intact RGCs, thus maintaining a constant signal-to-noise ratio. It has also been proposed31,34,55 that a fixed number of RGCs underlie RA across the visual field, accounting for changes in spatial summation area as a function of visual field eccentricity. In the case of the current study, it is possible that a similar hypothesis is applicable in myopia, where ocular growth and subsequent retinal stretch leads to reductions in localized RGC density4,18,19 and an enlarged RA serves to maintain a constant number of RGCs underlying RA and a constant signal–noise ratio for contrast detection. This hypothesis may be further supported by the fact we observed no statistically significant difference in RGC number estimated to underlie RA when modelled using both normative histological and OCT data (Fig. 7).

While considering changes in the density of RGCs in myopia as the sole source of alterations in RA is convenient, it is likely that multiple loci in the visual pathway play a role. For example, a strong relationship between co-localized RGCL thickness and RA would be expected if the density of RGCs was the sole factor determining the size of RA. However, in the present study only a weak negative relationship was observed between these variables, similar to previous findings relating RA to co-localized RGC number derived from psychophysical measures in glaucoma31. Furthermore, despite there being no statistically significant differences in estimated RGC number underlying RA in myopia and control participants, marked variability in these values was observed (Fig. 7). In the context of the myopic visual system, alterations in the density of RGCs4,18,19 and function of higher-visual centers59,60 have been reported previously, with changes in the organisation of RGC receptive fields also being hypothesized to occur in response to altered chemical balance in the body. For example, dopamine and dopamine antagonists are known to alter the balance between the center and surround components of center-surround antagonistic receptive fields of retinal neurons by altering the degree of electrical coupling between cells6165. This role has been demonstrated in rabbit on-bipolar cells whereby dopamine concentration was increased in photopic conditions, leading to an increase in the weighting of the off-surround, whereas maintained darkness and/or blocking dopamine receptors led to diminished receptive field surrounds66. Looking specifically at RGCs, Jensen and Daw67 found dopamine antagonists to cause a reduction in the antagonistic surround input to the off-center RGC receptive field, leading to a shift in the center-surround arrangement in favour of the center (i.e., larger central receptive field size). Previous authors have proposed RA to be a psychophysical correlate of the relationship between RGC receptive field centre and surrounds in the retina22,27,57, with the potential that dopamine alters this balance and thus RA. Much evidence points towards reduced retinal dopamine levels in myopia61,68,69, with light exposure (which stimulates dopamine release in the retina70) associated with a reduction in myopia onset and progression7173. It is therefore conceivable that the larger RA found in myopia may be a consequence of lower dopamine levels in this group.

Other work points to the role of the visual cortex in moderating spatial summation. Redmond et al.58 found changes in RA with background luminance for the S-cone pathway, where retinal center-surround receptive field organisation is known not to exist, the authors proposing this to point to the influence of higher visual centers. Indeed, a cortical contribution22 or basis31,74,75 to RA has been suggested by several authors. Such changes may take the form of alterations in the spatial tuning of cortical filters or an active remodelling of the visual cortex in response to changes in the density of retinal neurons as demonstrated in in vivo animal studies76,77. More recent functional MRI work has also identified altered structure59 and functional-connectivity deficits78 within the visual pathway of patients with high myopia. It is therefore possible that the changes in RA observed in this study may reflect changes to multiple loci of the visual pathway, including higher visual centres, in myopia.

Whilst the neurological underpinnings of RA are still debated, it is clear from this study and others79,80 that optical factors can also profoundly influence measurements of spatial summation. Specifically, it is evident that when optically induced changes in RIS, occurring secondary to axial elongation in myopia, are accounted for a perceptual ‘minification’ remains, manifesting as an enlarged RA in axial-myopes relative to controls. By contrast, such differences in RA were not observed when contact lens correction was used and Knapp’s Law not satisfied. In this instance, a lack of minifcation of the retinal image by the refractive correction leads to RIS proportionally increasing with axial elongation and RA being ‘filled’ more rapidly; this relationship breaking down when Knapp’s law is satisfied and RIS remains constant with axial length. Similar results were reported by Atchison et al.19 who observed a stronger relationship between RA and refractive error after post-hoc correction for inter-observer differences in RIS. This interplay between neural and optical factors is thought to account for residual perceptual aniseikonia in anisometropia when measured with a constant inter-eye RIS37,38. Indeed such findings may be a consequence of increased spatial summation in the axially myopic eye, these neural changes serving to compensate for an enlarged RIS and thus optimize visual function.

Implications for the clinical assessment of spatial vision

The outcomes of the present work may have implications for both the assessment of spatial vision in observers with myopia, but also for the development and interpretation of tests of spatial vision designed to detect ophthalmic diseases (e.g., perimetry for glaucoma). Considering the association between ocular length and measures of RA observed in this study, it is possible that changes in RA may act as a non-invasive, functional marker of global or localized (i.e., equatorial or posterior pole elongation) globe expansion in progressive myopia81. For example, in the absence of biometric measures and concurrent disease, RA values may be measured at multiple locations and reflect the extent of local retinal stretch/axial elongation present when RIS is carefully controlled. Measurements of RA could also potentially be combined with structural measures in myopia (e.g., axial length, retinal thickness) to enable progressive myopia to be detected and monitored more robustly. Combining different sources of information, from both structural and functional measures, has been demonstrated to be more effective than considering just one clinical measure in isolation for other ocular conditions where monitoring and predicting progression is important (e.g., glaucoma, ocular hypertension)8284.

The results of the present study also have potential implications for the design of perimetric test strategies used to detect functional deficits in glaucoma. Specifically, those tests (e.g., area-modulation perimetry85) intended to probe alterations in spatial summation in glaucoma may need to incorporate a normative database stratified according to AL if the balance between AL induced changes in RIS and neural minification is not maintained (i.e., spectacle lens used to correct refractive error) in axial myopes. Incorporating such information will serve to increase the specificity of such a test to detect true glaucoma related changes in RA and not those secondary to axial expansion of the globe.

Conclusions

In summary, our novel observation of an increased RA in axial-myopia when RIS is invariant of AL suggests spatial summation to be altered in the myopic, but otherwise healthy, visual system. We propose that this finding represents a functional adaptation of the myopic visual system to an enlarged RIS in the axially-elongated globe. The implications of this research are three-fold in that it, (i) builds our knowledge of the structure/function relationship in myopia, (ii) provides ‘normal myopic control’ information for similar research in glaucoma, and (iii) creates the potential for the development of a non-invasive functional test for myopic progression. Further work is however necessary to determine if the ratio of measurement variability to changes in RA in myopia (i.e., myopia signal-to-noise ratio) is favourable across all stages of myopia.

Supplementary information

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a PhD studentship from the Department for the Economy, Northern Ireland (VS).

Author contributions

Study concept and design: P.J.M., R.S.A., V.S., K.J.S. Acquisition of data: V.S. Analysis and interpretation of data: V.S., P.M., R.A. Drafting of the manuscript: V.S., P.J.M. Critical review of the manuscript: All authors.

Data availability

Supporting data will be made available upon request from the corresponding author.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-67893-8.

References

  • 1.Ogawa A, Tanaka M. The relationship between refractive errors and retinal detachment–analysis of 1,166 retinal detachment cases. Jpn. J. Ophthalmol. 1988;32:310–315. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Wong TY, Klein BE, Klein R, Knudtson M, Lee KE. Refractive errors, intraocular pressure, and glaucoma in a white population. Ophthalmology. 2003;110:211–217. doi: 10.1016/S0161-6420(02)01260-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wong TY, Ferreira A, Hughes R, Carter G, Mitchell P. Epidemiology and disease burden of pathologic myopia and myopic choroidal neovascularization: An evidence-based systematic review. Am. J. Ophthalmol. 2014;157:9–25.e12. doi: 10.1016/j.ajo.2013.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Wolsley CJ, Saunders KJ, Silvestri G, Anderson RS. Investigation of changes in the myopic retina using multifocal electroretinograms, optical coherence tomography and peripheral resolution acuity. Vis. Res. 2008;48:1554–1561. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cheng SC, Lam CS, Yap MK. Retinal thickness in myopic and non-myopic eyes. Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2010;30:776–784. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2010.00788.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Myers CE, et al. Retinal thickness measured by spectral domain optical coherence tomography in eyes without retinal abnormalities: The Beaver Dam eye study. Am. J. Ophthalmol. 2015;159:445–456.e441. doi: 10.1016/j.ajo.2014.11.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Chui TYP, Song H, Burns SA. Individual variations in human cone photoreceptor packing density: Variations with refractive error. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2008;49:4679–4687. doi: 10.1167/iovs.08-2135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Li K, Tiruveedhula P, Roorda A. Intersubject variability of foveal cone photoreceptor density in relation to eye length. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2010;51:6858–6867. doi: 10.1167/iovs.10-5499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Dabir S, et al. Axial length and cone density as assessed with adaptive optics in myopia. Indian J. Ophthalmol. 2015;63:423–426. doi: 10.4103/0301-4738.159876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Seo S, et al. Ganglion cell-inner plexiform layer and retinal nerve fiber layer thickness according to myopia and optic disc area: A quantitative and three-dimensional analysis. BMC Ophthalmol. 2017;17:22. doi: 10.1186/s12886-017-0419-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Leung CK, et al. Retinal nerve fiber layer measurements in myopia: An optical coherence tomography study. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2006;47:5171–5176. doi: 10.1167/iovs.06-0545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Westall C, et al. Values of electroretinogram responses according to axial length. Doc. Ophthalmol. 2001;102:115–130. doi: 10.1023/A:1017535207481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kader MA. Electrophysiological study of myopia. Saudi J. Ophthalmol. 2012;26:91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.sjopt.2011.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hidajat R, et al. Influence of axial length of normal eyes on PERG. Doc. Ophthalmol. 2003;107:195–200. doi: 10.1023/a:1026282425885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Luu CD, Lau AI, Lee S. Multifocal electroretinogram in adults and children with myopia. Arch. Ophthalmol. 2006;124:328–334. doi: 10.1001/archopht.124.3.328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Strang NC, Winn B, Bradley A. The role of neural and optical factors in limiting visual resolution in myopia. Vis. Res. 1998;38:1713–1721. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00303-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Coletta NJ, Watson T. Effect of myopia on visual acuity measured with laser interference fringes. Vis. Res. 2006;46:636–651. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.05.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Chui TYP, Yap MKH, Chan HHL, Thibos LN. Retinal stretching limits peripheral visual acuity in myopia. Vis. Res. 2005;45:593–605. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Atchison DA, Schmid KL, Pritchard N. Neural and optical limits to visual performance in myopia. Vis. Res. 2006;46:3707–3722. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Liou S-W, Chiu C-J. Myopia and contrast sensitivity function. Curr. Eye Res. 2001;22:81–84. doi: 10.1076/ceyr.22.2.81.5530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Riccò, A. Relazione fra il minimo angolo visuale e l'intensità luminosa. Mem. Soc. Degli Spettrosc. Ital.6 (1877).
  • 22.Wilson ME. Invariant features of spatial summation with changing locus in the visual field. J. Physiol. 1970;207:611–622. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Inui T, Mimura O, Kani K. Retinal sensitivity and spatial summation in the foveal and parafoveal regions. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 1981;71:151–163. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.71.000151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Volbrecht V, Shrago EE, Schefrin BE, Werner J. Spatial summation in human cone mechanisms from 0 degrees to 20 degrees in the superior retina. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. 2000;17:641–650. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.17.000641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Vassilev A, Mihaylova MS, Racheva K, Zlatkova M, Anderson RS. Spatial summation of S-cone ON and OFF signals: Effects of retinal eccentricity. Vis. Res. 2003;43:2875–2884. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2003.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Khuu SK, Kalloniatis M. Spatial summation across the central visual field: Implications for visual field testing. J. Vis. 2015;15:6–6. doi: 10.1167/15.1.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Barlow HB. Temporal and spatial summation in human vision at different background intensities. J. Physiol. 1958;141:337–350. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1958.sp005978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lelkens AM, Zuidema P. Increment thresholds with various low background intensities at different locations in the peripheral retina. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 1983;73:1372–1378. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.73.001372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Cornsweet TN, Yellott JI., Jr Intensity-dependent spatial summation. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. 1985;2:1769–1786. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.2.001769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Fellman, R. L., Lynn, J. R., Starita, R. J. & Swanson, W. H. In Perimety Update 1988/1989 (ed A. Heijl) 313–324 (Kugler and Ghedini, 1989).
  • 31.Redmond T, Garway-Heath DF, Zlatkova MB, Anderson RS. Sensitivity loss in early glaucoma can be mapped to an enlargement of the area of complete spatial summation. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2019;51:6540–6548. doi: 10.1167/iovs.10-5718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Mulholland PJ, Redmond T, Garway-Heath DF, Zlatkova MB, Anderson RS. Spatiotemporal summation of perimetric stimuli in early glaucoma. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2015;56:6473–6482. doi: 10.1167/iovs.15-16921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Fischer B. Overlap of receptive field centers and representation of the visual field in the cat's optic tract. Vis. Res. 1973;13:2113–2120. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(73)90188-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kwon, M. & Liu, R. Linkage between retinal ganglion cell density and the nonuniform spatial integration across the visual field. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 201817076. 10.1073/pnas.1817076116 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 35.Jaworski A, Gentle A, Zele AJ, Vingrys AJ, McBrien NA. Altered visual sensitivity in axial high myopia: a local postreceptoral phenomenon? Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2006;47:3695–3702. doi: 10.1167/iovs.05-1569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Mulholland PJ, Redmond T, Garway-Heath DF, Zlatkova MB, Anderson RS. Estimating the critical duration for temporal summation of standard achromatic perimetric stimuli. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2015;56:431–437. doi: 10.1167/iovs.14-15304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bradley A, Rabin J, Freeman RD. Nonoptical determinants of aniseikonia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 1983;24:507–512. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Rabin J, Bradley A, Freeman RD. On the relation between aniseikonia and axial anisometropia. Am. J. Optom. Physiol. Opt. 1983;60:553–558. doi: 10.1097/00006324-198307000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.World-Health-Organization. The Impact of Myopia and High Myopia. (2015).
  • 40.Knapp H. The influence of spectacles on the optical constant and visual acuteness of the eye. Arch. Ophthalmol. 1869;1:377–410. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bridgeman B. Durations of stimuli displayed on video display terminals: (n − 1)/f + persistence. Psychol. Sci. 1998;9:232–233. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00045. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Radhakrishnan H, Charman WN. Peripheral refraction measurement: Does it matter if one turns the eye or the head? Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 2008;28:73–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2007.00521.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Mathur A, et al. The influence of oblique viewing on axial and peripheral refraction for emmetropes and myopes. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 2009;29:155–161. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2008.00623.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Ding X, He M. Measurement of peripheral eye length. Ophthalmology. 2012;119:1084–1085. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2012.01.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ctori I, Gruppetta S, Huntjens B. The effects of ocular magnification on spectralis spectral domain optical coherence tomography scan length. Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 2015;253:733–738. doi: 10.1007/s00417-014-2915-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Drasdo N, Millican CL, Katholi CR, Curcio CA. The length of Henle fibers in the human retina and a model of ganglion receptive field density in the visual field. Vis. Res. 2007;47:2901–2911. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Curcio CA, Allen KA. Topography of ganglion cells in human retina. J. Comp. Neurol. 1990;300:5–25. doi: 10.1002/cne.903000103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Atchison DA, et al. Eye shape in emmetropia and myopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2004;45:3380–3386. doi: 10.1167/iovs.04-0292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Raza AS, Hood DC. Evaluation of the structure-function relationship in glaucoma using a novel method for estimating the number of retinal ganglion cells in the human retina. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2015;56:5548–5556. doi: 10.1167/iovs.14-16366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Bennett AG, Rudnicka AR, Edgar DF. Improvements on Littmann's method of determining the size of retinal features by fundus photography. Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 1994;232:361–367. doi: 10.1007/BF00175988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bilic-Zulle L. Comparison of methods: Passing and Bablok regression. Biochem. Med. 2011;21:49–52. doi: 10.11613/BM.2011.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Passing H, Bablok W. A new biometrical procedure for testing the equality of measurements from two different analytical methods. J. Clin. Chem. Clin. Biochem. 1983;21:709–720. doi: 10.1515/cclm.1983.21.11.709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Bablok W, Passing H, Bender R, Schneider B. A general regression procedure for method transformation. Application of linear regression procedures for method comparison studies in clinical chemistry, Part III. J. Clin. Chem. Clin. Biochem. 1983;26:783–790. doi: 10.1515/cclm.1988.26.11.783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Bennett, A. G. & Rabbetts, R. B. Bennett & Rabbetts' Clinical Visual Optics. (Butterworth-Heinemann, 1998).
  • 55.Swanson WH, Felius J, Pan F. Perimetric defects and ganglion cell damage: Interpreting linear relations using a two-stage neural model. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2004;45:466–472. doi: 10.1167/iovs.03-0374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Barlow HB. Summation and inhibition in the frog's retina. J. Physiol. 1953;119:69–88. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1953.sp004829. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Glezer VD. The receptive fields of the retina. Vis. Res. 1965;5:497–525. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(65)90084-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Redmond, T., Zlatkova, M., Vassilev, A., Garway-Heath, D. & S Anderson, R. Changes in Ricco's area with background luminance in the S-cone pathway. Optom. Vis. Sci.90. 10.1097/OPX.0b013e318278fc2b (2013). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 59.Huang X, et al. Altered whole-brain gray matter volume in high myopia patients: A voxel-based morphometry study. NeuroReport. 2018;29:760–767. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000001028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Malecaze FJ, Boulanouar KA, Demonet JF, Guell JL, Imbert MA. Abnormal activation in the visual cortex after corneal refractive surgery for myopia: Demonstration by functional magnetic resonance imaging1. Ophthalmology. 2001;108:2213–2218. doi: 10.1016/s0161-6420(01)00843-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Feldkaemper M, Schaeffel F. An updated view on the role of dopamine in myopia. Exp. Eye Res. 2013;114:106–119. doi: 10.1016/j.exer.2013.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Mangel SC, Dowling JE. Responsiveness and receptive field size of carp horizontal cells are reduced by prolonged darkness and dopamine. Science. 1985;229:1107–1109. doi: 10.1126/science.4035351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Hampson EC, Vaney DI, Weiler R. Dopaminergic modulation of gap junction permeability between amacrine cells in mammalian retina. J. Neurosci. 1992;12:4911–4922. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-12-04911.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Hare WA, Owen WG. Similar effects of carbachol and dopamine on neurons in the distal retina of the tiger salamander. Vis. Neurosci. 1995;12:443–455. doi: 10.1017/S0952523800008348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Zhang A-J, Jacoby R, Wu SM. Light- and dopamine-regulated receptive field plasticity in primate horizontal cells. J. Comp. Neurol. 2011;519:2125–2134. doi: 10.1002/cne.22604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Chaffiol A, Ishii M, Cao Y, Mangel SC. Dopamine regulation of GABA(A) receptors contributes to light/dark modulation of the ON-cone bipolar cell receptive field surround in the retina. Curr. Biol. CB. 2017;27:2600–2609.e2604. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.07.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Jensen RJ, Daw NW. Effects of dopamine antagonists on receptive fields of brisk cells and directionally selective cells in the rabbit retina. J. Neurosci. 1984;4:2972–2985. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-12-02972.1984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Zhou X, Pardue MT, Iuvone PM, Qu J. Dopamine signaling and myopia development: What are the key challenges. Prog. Retin. Eye Res. 2017;61:60–71. doi: 10.1016/j.preteyeres.2017.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Kearney S, O'Donoghue L, Pourshahidi LK, Cobice D, Saunders KJ. Myopes have significantly higher serum melatonin concentrations than non-myopes. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 2017;37:557–567. doi: 10.1111/opo.12396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Brainard GC, Morgan WW. Light-induced stimulation of retinal dopamine: A dose-response relationship. Brain Res. 1987;424:199–203. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)91211-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Guggenheim JA, et al. Time Outdoors and physical activity as predictors of incident myopia in childhood: A prospective cohort study. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2012;53:2856–2865. doi: 10.1167/iovs.11-9091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Read SA, Collins MJ, Vincent SJ. Light exposure and eye growth in childhood. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2015;56:6779–6787. doi: 10.1167/iovs.14-15978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.French AN, Ashby RS, Morgan IG, Rose KA. Time outdoors and the prevention of myopia. Exp. Eye Res. 2013;114:58–68. doi: 10.1016/j.exer.2013.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Pan F, Swanson WH. A cortical pooling model of spatial summation for perimetric stimuli. J. Vis. 2006;6:1159–1171. doi: 10.1167/6.11.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Je S, Ennis FA, Woodhouse JM, Sengpiel F, Redmond T. Spatial summation across the visual field in strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:3858. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-21620-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Eyding D, Schweigart G, Eysel UT. Spatio-temporal plasticity of cortical receptive fields in response to repetitive visual stimulation in the adult cat. Neuroscience. 2002;112:195–215. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00039-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Pizzorusso T, et al. Reactivation of ocular dominance plasticity in the adult visual cortex. Science. 2002;298:1248–1251. doi: 10.1126/science.1072699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Zhai L, et al. Altered functional connectivity density in high myopia. Behav. Brain Res. 2016;303:85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.01.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Redmond T, Zlatkova MB, Garway-Heath DF, Anderson RS. The effect of age on the area of complete spatial summation for chromatic and achromatic stimuli. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2010;51:6533–6539. doi: 10.1167/iovs.10-5717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Dalimier E, Dainty C. Role of ocular aberrations in photopic spatial summation in the fovea. Opt. Lett. 2010;35:589–591. doi: 10.1364/ol.35.000589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Strang NC, Winn B, Bradley A. The role of neural and optical factors in limiting visual resolution in myopia. Vis. Res. 1998;38:1713–1721. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00303-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Medeiros FA, Leite MT, Zangwill LM, Weinreb RN. Combining structural and functional measurements to improve detection of glaucoma progression using Bayesian hierarchical models. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2011;52:5794–5803. doi: 10.1167/iovs.10-7111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Medeiros FA, et al. A combined index of structure and function for staging glaucomatous damage. Arch. Ophthalmol. 2012;130:1107–1116. doi: 10.1001/archophthalmol.2012.827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Russell RA, Malik R, Chauhan BC, Crabb DP, Garway-Heath DF. Improved estimates of visual field progression using Bayesian linear regression to integrate structural information in patients with ocular hypertension. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2012;53:2760–2769. doi: 10.1167/iovs.11-7976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Rountree L, et al. Optimising the glaucoma signal/noise ratio by mapping changes in spatial summation with area-modulated perimetric stimuli. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:2172. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-20480-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

Supporting data will be made available upon request from the corresponding author.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES