Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2020 Jun 18;34(4):1017–1028. doi: 10.1111/cobi.13495

Effects of body size on estimation of mammalian area requirements

Michael J Noonan 1,2,, Christen H Fleming 1,2, Marlee A Tucker 3,4,5, Roland Kays 6,7, Autumn‐Lynn Harrison 8, Margaret C Crofoot 9,10, Briana Abrahms 11, Susan C Alberts 12, Abdullahi H Ali 13, Jeanne Altmann 14, Pamela Castro Antunes 15, Nina Attias 16, Jerrold L Belant 17, Dean E Beyer Jr 18, Laura R Bidner 9,19, Niels Blaum 20, Randall B Boone 21,22, Damien Caillaud 9, Rogerio Cunha de Paula 23, J Antonio de la Torre 24, Jasja Dekker 25, Christopher S DePerno 7, Mohammad Farhadinia 26,27, Julian Fennessy 28, Claudia Fichtel 29, Christina Fischer 30, Adam Ford 31, Jacob R Goheen 32, Rasmus W Havmøller 9, Ben T Hirsch 33, Cindy Hurtado 34,35, Lynne A Isbell 9,19, René Janssen 36, Florian Jeltsch 20, Petra Kaczensky 37,38, Yayoi Kaneko 39, Peter Kappeler 29, Anjan Katna 40,41, Matthew Kauffman 42, Flavia Koch 29, Abhijeet Kulkarni 40, Scott LaPoint 43,44, Peter Leimgruber 1, David W Macdonald 26, A Catherine Markham 45, Laura McMahon 46, Katherine Mertes 1, Christopher E Moorman 7, Ronaldo G Morato 23,47, Alexander M Moßbrucker 48, Guilherme Mourão 49, David O'Connor 4,50,51, Luiz Gustavo R Oliveira‐Santos 52, Jennifer Pastorini 53,54, Bruce D Patterson 55, Janet Rachlow 56, Dustin H Ranglack 57, Neil Reid 58, David M Scantlebury 59, Dawn M Scott 60, Nuria Selva 61, Agnieszka Sergiel 61, Melissa Songer 1, Nucharin Songsasen 1, Jared A Stabach 1, Jenna Stacy‐Dawes 50, Morgan B Swingen 7,62, Jeffrey J Thompson 63,64, Wiebke Ullmann 20, Abi Tamim Vanak 40,65,66, Maria Thaker 67, John W Wilson 68, Koji Yamazaki 69,70, Richard W Yarnell 71, Filip Zieba 72, Tomasz Zwijacz‐Kozica 72, William F Fagan 2, Thomas Mueller 3,4, Justin M Calabrese 1,2
PMCID: PMC7496598  PMID: 32362060

Abstract

Accurately quantifying species’ area requirements is a prerequisite for effective area‐based conservation. This typically involves collecting tracking data on species of interest and then conducting home‐range analyses. Problematically, autocorrelation in tracking data can result in space needs being severely underestimated. Based on the previous work, we hypothesized the magnitude of underestimation varies with body mass, a relationship that could have serious conservation implications. To evaluate this hypothesis for terrestrial mammals, we estimated home‐range areas with global positioning system (GPS) locations from 757 individuals across 61 globally distributed mammalian species with body masses ranging from 0.4 to 4000 kg. We then applied block cross‐validation to quantify bias in empirical home‐range estimates. Area requirements of mammals <10 kg were underestimated by a mean approximately15%, and species weighing approximately100 kg were underestimated by approximately50% on average. Thus, we found area estimation was subject to autocorrelation‐induced bias that was worse for large species. Combined with the fact that extinction risk increases as body mass increases, the allometric scaling of bias we observed suggests the most threatened species are also likely to be those with the least accurate home‐range estimates. As a correction, we tested whether data thinning or autocorrelation‐informed home‐range estimation minimized the scaling effect of autocorrelation on area estimates. Data thinning required an approximately93% data loss to achieve statistical independence with 95% confidence and was, therefore, not a viable solution. In contrast, autocorrelation‐informed home‐range estimation resulted in consistently accurate estimates irrespective of mass. When relating body mass to home range size, we detected that correcting for autocorrelation resulted in a scaling exponent significantly >1, meaning the scaling of the relationship changed substantially at the upper end of the mass spectrum.

Keywords: allometry, animal movement, area‐based conservation, autocorrelation, home range, kernel density estimation, reserve design, scaling, alometría, autocorrelación, conservación basada en áreas, diseño de reserva, distribución local, escalamiento, estimación de densidad del núcleo, movimiento de mamíferos, 异速增长, 动物移动, 区域保护, 自相关, 家域, 核密度估计, 保护区设计, 标度

Short abstract

Article impact statement: Due to autocorrelation‐induced bias, conventional methods severely underestimate the area requirements of GPS‐tracked large mammals.

Introduction

Globally, human‐altered landscapes are restricting animal movement (Fahrig 2007; Tucker et al. 2018), and habitat loss and fragmentation are the principal threats to terrestrial biodiversity (Brooks et al. 2002; Wilson et al. 2016). A key component to conserving species in increasingly human‐dominated landscapes is understanding how much space is required to maintain stable, interconnected populations (Brashares et al. 2001; Pe'er et al. 2014). Area requirements are typically quantified via home‐range analysis (Burt 1943). This routinely involves collecting tracking data on species of interest (Kays et al. 2015) and then applying a home‐range estimator to these data (Fleming et al. 2015; Noonan et al. 2019). These range estimates can then be used to inform recommendations on reserve sizes (Linnell et al. 2001), to advocate for specific land‐tenure systems (Johansson et al. 2016; Farhadinia et al. 2018), and to make conservation policy recommendations (Barton´ et al. 2019). However, tracking data are often strongly autocorrelated, whereas conventional home‐range estimators are based on the assumption of independent and identically distributed data (Noonan et al. 2019).

When data are autocorrelated, the total number of data points does not reflect the total amount of information in the data set (i.e., effective sample size) (Fleming & Calabrese 2017). Although the idea that autocorrelation may affect home‐range estimates is not new (e.g., Swihart & Slade 1985; Fieberg 2007; Fleming et al. 2015), only recent analyses have demonstrated the seriousness of the problem. Using the largest empirical tracking data set assembled to date, Noonan et al. (2019) found conventional estimators significantly negatively biased when used on autocorrelated data. Although any form of bias is undesirable, the systematic underestimation of home‐range areas is a worst‐case scenario from a conservation perspective. Any policy or management decisions informed by underestimated home‐range estimates could result in failed conservation initiatives (Brashares et al. 2001; Gaston et al. 2008) or exacerbate negative human–wildlife interactions at reserve boundaries (Van Eeden et al. 2018).

Noonan et al. (2019) noticed that large‐bodied species tended to exhibit more negatively biased conventional home‐range estimates than small‐bodied species. However, the species included in their study were not selected to provide the broad range of body masses required to investigate allometric trends. We compiled an extensive empirical data set of global positioning system (GPS) locations from 757 individuals across 61 terrestrial mammalian species with body masses ranging from 0.4 to 4000 kg. We used these data to investigate whether the underestimation of home‐range size scales with body mass. To see the potential for this, consider that large species have large home ranges (Jetz et al. 2004) that tend to take longer to cross than smaller home ranges (Calder 1983). In addition, range crossing time (τp) interacts with the sampling interval (dt) in determining the amount of autocorrelation in tracking data (Fleming & Calabrese 2017; Noonan et al. 2019). When dtτp, the resulting data are autocorrelated, whereas dt τp results in effectively independent data. Finally, the magnitude of the negative biases in conventional home‐range estimates increases in proportion to the strength of autocorrelation in the data (Noonan et al. 2019). Combining these facts, we arrived at the hypothesis that an allometry in τp drives autocorrelation and negative estimation bias to scale with body size.

We examined this hypothesis in 2 ways. First, we tested whether the chain of relationships that would drive bias to scale with mass holds for empirical tracking data. Second, we explored how well 2 methods of home‐range estimation for autocorrelated data eliminate the scaling of home‐range estimation bias. These methods were model‐informed data thinning, which removes autocorrelation from the data prior to home‐range estimation, and autocorrelation‐informed home‐range estimation, which statistically accounts for autocorrelation in movement data. We then used model selection to determine whether significant allometry bias remains in the data for each approach and identified whether one of these corrections offers improved performance over the other. Finally, in light of our findings, we revisited the concept of home‐range allometry (e.g., McNab 1963; Jetz et al. 2004; Tucker et al. 2014). Mammalian home‐range area (H) scales positively with body mass (M) as H = B 0 Mb, where B 0 is a normalization constant and b is the scaling exponent (McNab 1963). Despite decades of research, however, there has been little consensus on whether the allometry is linear (i.e., M 1), or superlinear (i.e., M> 1). Historically, this scaling relationship has been calculated by compiling home‐range areas estimated via conventional estimators, which are subject to varying levels of autocorrelation‐induced bias (Noonan et al. 2019), whereas no one has assessed this relationship directly from tracking data. Although consistent bias across the mass spectrum would lead only to a change in the normalization constant, differential bias across the mass spectrum could alter the scaling exponent, fundamentally changing the properties of the relationship. As such, we tested for any significant deviations from linear (M 1) scaling.

Methods

All analyses were based on precollected tracking data sets obtained under appropriate permits and that were based on Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee approved protocols.

Data Compilation

To investigate whether biases in home‐range estimation scale with body size, we compiled GPS tracking data for 61 globally distributed terrestrial mammalian species, comprising 6.94 × 106 locations for 757 individuals collected from 2000 to 2019 (Fig. 1). Individual data sets were selected based on the criterion of range resident behavior (i.e., area‐restricted space use), as evidenced by plots of the semivariance in positions as a function of the time lag separating observations (i.e., variograms) with a clear asymptote at large lags (Calabrese et al. 2016). When data do not indicate evidence of range residency, home‐range estimation is not appropriate (Calabrese et al. 2016; Fleming & Calabrese 2017), so we excluded data from migratory or nonrange resident individuals. The visual verification of range residency via variogram analysis was conducted using the R package ctmm (version 0.5.3) (Calabrese et al. 2016). Further details on these data are given in Supporting Information.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Distribution of study sites for the empirical global positioning system tracking data set spanning 757 individuals across 61 mammalian species.

For each of the species in our data set, we compiled covariate data on that species’ mean adult mass in kilograms. We also identified the main food source for each species and classified them as carnivorous or omnivorous or frugivorous or herbivorous. Data from these 2 dietary classes were analyzed separately. Mass and dietary data were from the EltonTraits database (Wilman et al. 2014).

Tracking‐Data Analyses

Our conjecture that the underestimation of home‐range areas increases as body size increases was based on 2 well‐established biological and one methodological relationship: the positive correlation between body mass and home‐range area (Jetz et al. 2004); the positive correlation between home‐range area and range crossing time, τp (Calder 1983); and the negative correlation between range crossing time and the effective sample size for area estimation, N area (i.e., equivalent number of statistically independent locations [Noonan et al. 2019]). We hypothesized that these conspire to drive 2 previously untested relationships: a potential negative correlation between body mass and N area and a potential negative correlation between body mass and home‐range estimator accuracy.

Testing for these relationships first required estimating the autocorrelation structure in each of the individual tracking data sets. To accomplish this, we fitted a series of range‐resident, continuous‐time movement models to the data with the estimation methods developed by Fleming et al. (2019). The fitted models included the independent and identically distributed process, which features uncorrelated positions and velocities; the Ornstein–Uhlenbeck (OU) process, which features correlated positions but uncorrelated velocities (Uhlenbeck & Ornstein 1930); and an OU‐foraging (OUF) process, featuring both correlated positions and velocities (Fleming et al. 2014). We used model selection to identify the best fitting model given the data (Fleming et al. 2014) from which τp and N area were extracted. To fit and select the movement models, we used the R package ctmm and applied the workflow described by Calabrese et al. (2016).

We estimated home‐range areas for each of the 757 individuals in our tracking database via kernel density estimation (KDE) with Gaussian reference function bandwidth optimization because this is one of the most commonly applied home‐range estimators in ecological research (Noonan et al. 2019). The KDE home ranges were estimated via the methods implemented in ctmm, and the further small‐sample‐size bias correction that was introduced in area‐corrected KDE (Fleming & Calabrese 2017).

Our primary aim was to determine the extent to which autocorrelation‐induced bias in conventional home‐range estimation might increase with body size. This required an objective and statistically sound measure of bias. We applied the well‐established technique of block cross‐validation (Noonan et al. 2019) to quantify bias in empirical home‐range estimates.

By determining the extent to which the results of an analysis generalize to a statistically independent data set, cross‐validation is an effective tool for quantifying bias (Pawitan 2001). For this approach, each individual data set was split in half, and a home‐range area was estimated from the first half of the data only (i.e., training set). Next, the percentage of observations in the second half of the data (i.e., held‐out set) that fell within the specified contour (here 50% and 95%) of the estimated home range was calculated. If the percentage of points included came out consistently higher or lower than the specified contour, then it would suggest positive or negative bias, respectively.

As a further measure of bias, we identified the contour of the home range estimated from the training set that contained the desired percentage of locations in the held‐out set (i.e., 50% and 95%) and compared the area within that contour to the estimated area at the specified quantile. For example, consider that the 95% area estimated on the training data contained only 90% of the locations in the held‐out set, whereas the 97% contour contained 95% of the locations. To measure bias, we would take the ratio between the 97% area and the 95% area. Cross‐validating home‐range estimates in this way can also be seen as providing a measure of how well a home‐range estimate can be expected to capture an animal's future space use, assuming no substantial changes in movement behavior.

Block cross‐validation is based on the assumption that data from the training and held‐out sets are generated from the same processes. To confirm this assumption, we used the Battacharryya distance implementation in ctmm (Winner et al. 2018) as a measure of similarity (range 0–) between the mean area and covariance parameters of movement models fitted to the training and held‐out data sets and determined whether the confidence intervals on this distance contained 0 (details are given in Appendix S1 in Noonan et al. [2019]). Using this method, we determined that 160 of 757 individuals had movement models with significantly different parameter estimates between the first and second halves of the data, so we excluded these from our cross‐validation analyses. We found no significant relationship between whether or not a data set was excluded from our analyses and which species the data were from (p = 0.52) or between exclusion and how long an individual was tracked (p = 0.39). This confirmed that the subsampling required to meet the assumptions of half‐sample cross‐validation did not bias our sample.

Correction Factors

We explored 2 potential solutions to the allometric scaling of autocorrelation and home‐range estimation bias: thinning data to minimize autocorrelation and using autocorrelation‐informed home‐range estimation.

Conventional kernel methods are based on an assumption of independence; however, they can provide accurate estimates for autocorrelated processes when the sampling is coarse enough that the data appear uncorrelated over time (Hall & Hart 1990). Thus, data thinning presents a potentially straightforward solution to autocorrelation‐induced bias, but requires a balance between reducing autocorrelation and retaining sample size. We, therefore, explored model‐informed data thinning as a means of mitigating size‐dependent home‐range bias. As noted above, the parameter τp relates to an individual's range‐crossing time and quantifies the time scale over which positional autocorrelation decays to insignificance. More specifically, because positional autocorrelation decays exponentially at rate 1p, the time required for the percentage of the original velocity autocorrelation to decay to α is τα = τpln(1). Conventionally, data are thinned to independence with a 95% level of confidence, and approximately3τp is the time it takes for 95% of the positional autocorrelation to decay. Consequently, we thinned each individual's tracking data to a sampling frequency of dt = 3τp. We then used autocorrelation functions to quantify how much autocorrelation remained in the thinned data and evaluated the performance of KDEs on these thinned data.

As opposed to manipulating the data to meet the assumptions of the estimator, the second potential solution was to use an estimator that explicitly modeled the autocorrelation in the data. Autocorrelated‐KDE (AKDE) is a generalization of Gaussian reference function KDE that conditions upon the autocorrelation structure of the data when optimizing the bandwidth (Fleming et al. 2015). Following the workflow described by Calabrese et al. (2016), AKDE home‐range areas were estimated conditioned on the selected movement model for each data set, via the methods implemented in ctmm, with the same small‐sample‐size bias correction applied to the conventional KDE area estimates (Fleming & Calabrese 2017). The AKDE is available via the web‐based graphical user interface at ctmm.shinyapps.io/ctmmweb/(Dong et al. 2017).

Correction Factor Performance

To test for body‐size‐dependent biases in cross‐validation success, we fitted 3 regression models to the cross‐validation results as a function of log10‐scaled mass. The models included an intercept‐only model (i.e., no change in bias with mass), linear model, and logistic model. We then identified the best model for the data via small‐sample‐size corrected quasi‐Akaike information criterion (Burnham et al. 2011).

Species may exhibit similarities in traits due to phylogenetic inertia and the constraints of common ancestry; thus, controlled comparisons are required (Harvey & Pagel 1991). Accordingly, we did not treat species data records as independent; rather, we used the phylogenetic distances among species to construct a variance–covariance matrix and defined the correlation structure in our allometric regressions with the R package nlme (version 3.1‐137) (Pinheiro et al. 2018). Phylogenetic relationships between eutherian mammalian orders were based on genetic differences and taken from Liu et al. (2001). Intraorder relationships were taken from more targeted studies aimed at resolving species‐level relationships, including Price et al. (2005) for Artiodactyla, Matthee et al. (2004) for Lagomorpha, Steiner and Ryder (2011) for Perissodactyla, Barriel et al. (1999) for Proboscidea, Perelman et al. (2011) for Primates, and Agnarsson et al. (2010) for Carnivora. For Canidae, however, we took relationships from Lindblad‐Toh et al. (2005), due to better coverage of the species in our data set. The phylogenetic tree was built with the R package ape (version 5.2) (Paradis & Schliep 2019), and branch lengths were computed following Grafen (1989). Phylogenies are given in Supporting Information.

Results

Allometric Scaling of Bias

Out of 757 data sets, only one was independent and identically distributed and free from significant autocorrelation. Conventional KDE 95% home‐range areas cross‐validated at a median rate of 88.3% (95% CI 87.2–90.1), which was below the target 95% quantile and demonstrated a tendency to underestimate home‐range areas on average. Similarly, KDE 50% home‐range areas cross‐validated at a median rate of 41.5% (95% CI 39.4–43.3), which was again below the target 50% quantile. The magnitude of KDE's underestimation worsened as body mass increased (t = 2.30, p = 0.02) (Fig. 2a), carnivores and herbivores did not differ significantly (t = 0.31; p = 0.75). Cross‐validation success of 50% home‐range areas across the mass spectrum was best described by a linear decay model with an intercept of 47.2 (95% CI 39.9–54.5) and a slope of –3.9 (95% CI –7.0 to –0.8). In other words, for every order of magnitude increase in body mass, home‐range estimates captured approximately4% less of an individual's future space use.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Cross‐validation of conventional kernel density estimation (KDE) across the mammalian body‐mass spectrum: (a) percentage of locations from the second half of the data (held‐out set) included in KDE 50% home ranges estimated from the first half of the data (training set) as a function of body mass (dashed line, target 50% quantile; solid line, phylogenetically controlled regression model fit to cross‐validation results; shading, 95% CI of the fit) and (b) regression model describing the accuracy of 95% KDE area estimates across the mass spectrum. Accuracy was quantified as the ratio between estimated 95% area of the training set and the area contained within the contour that encompassed 95% of locations in the held‐out set. The horizontal dashed line represents an unbiased area estimate. The x‐axes in are log scaled.

When comparing the 95% area estimates with the area estimates for the contours that contained 95% of locations, KDE accuracy across the mass spectrum was best described by linear decay (Fig. 2b). Consequently, whereas the home‐range areas of mammals weighing <10 kg were underestimated by 13.6% (95% CI 6.3–18.6), those of species weighing >100 kg were underestimated by 46.0% on average (95% CI 36.7–51.4).

Mechanisms Driving Body Size‐Dependent Estimation Bias

We found significant positive relationships between body mass and home‐range area (regression parameter: β = 1.18, 95% CI 0.92–1.43, t = 9.09, p <0.0001) (Fig. 3a) and between home‐range area and range crossing time, τp (β = 7.09, 95% CI 4.78–9.41, t = 6.00, p < 0.0001) (Fig. 3b) and a negative relationship between τp and the effective sample size, N area (β = −0.65, 95% CI –0.70 to –0.60, t = 25.46, p <0.0001) (Fig. 3c). The former 2 scaling relationships differed significantly between carnivorous and herbivorous mammals (t = 3.08, p <0.005 and t = 2.37, p = 0.02, respectively). Carnivores tended to have larger home ranges and shorter range crossing times than comparably sized herbivores, and herbivores tended to have longer range crossing times. The relationship between N area and mass did not differ between dietary classes (t = 0.82, p = 0.06). The N area was governed by both τp and sampling duration, T, such that N area T/τp. Although we noted a positive correlation between body mass and T in the studies we sampled (β = 0.24, 95% CI 0.09–0.39, t = 3.17, p < 0.005), this was not enough to counter the positive correlation between mass and τp. Consequently, the net result was a negative relationship between body mass and N area (β = 0.23, 95% CI –0.39 to –0.08, t = 2.98, p < 0.005) (Fig. 3d).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Mechanisms driving body‐size‐dependent estimation bias: (a) positive allometry of home‐range areas, (b) correlation between home‐range area and range‐crossing time (τp), (c) negative correlation between τp and effective sample size (N area) governed by duration of observation period (T) and τp such that N area T/τp, and (d) resulting negative allometry of N area (axes, log scaled; lines, phylogenetically controlled fitted regression models). From (a) to (d), 1 axis is preserved from the previous panel to demonstrate the inherent link between each of these relationships (arrows, visual aid of link; top‐left arrow, end of the chain).

Correction Factors

Model‐informed data thinning served to reduce the mean autocorrelation at lag 1 from 0.96 (95% CI 0.96–0.97) to 0.32 (95% CI 0.30–0.35) (Fig. 4). Hence, an independent and identically distributed model was the best fit for 167 of the 463 individuals for which sufficient data (>2 locations) remained after data thinning. The remaining individuals were best described by OU and OUF processes whose autocorrelation parameters were not significant. Although thinning mitigated the correlation between bias and body mass (β = –2.41, 95% CI –6.08 to 1.26, t = 1.29, p = 0.20), the median cross‐validation rate of 95% home ranges estimated using the thinned data was only 85.1% (95% CI 83.6–86.5). This approximately3% decrease in performance, as compared with conventional KDE on the full data, was likely the result of the small sample size. Model‐informed data thinning resulted in a mean data loss of 93.2% (95% CI 92.1–94.3), and the median number of approximately independent locations left in each data set after thinning was only 23 (95% CI 18–26). Furthermore, in approximately20% of the individuals, ≤2 locations remained after thinning, making it impossible to estimate a home‐range area on the thinned data.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Frequency of amounts of autocorrelation at lag 1 in the full tracking data sets for each of the 757 individuals used to estimate home ranges via conventional kernel density estimation (KDE), compared with the thinned data sets for individuals for which sufficient data remained after thinning to apply KDE.

Autocorrelation‐Informed Home‐Range Estimation

Like model‐informed data thinning, autocorrelation‐informed home‐range estimation via AKDE also eliminated the correlation between cross‐validation success and body mass (β = –0.51, 95% CI –1.88 to 0.86, t = 0.73, p = 0.47). However, without the data loss required by the thinning approach, AKDE resulted in a median cross‐validation rate of 95.2% (95% CI 94.2–95.9) for 95% home ranges and 51.3% (95% CI 49.26–54.36) for 50% home ranges. In other words, AKDE exhibited consistent accuracy across species, irrespective of the allometries in autocorrelation time scales and effective sample sizes.

Scaling of Mammalian Space Use

When regressing home‐range area against mass with conventional KDE estimates, we documented no significant difference from linear scaling for either herbivores or carnivores (Table 1). For AKDE‐derived area estimates, however, we detected that the scaling exponent was significantly >1 for both taxonomic groups, suggesting home‐range area scales with mass according to a power function.

Table 1.

Estimates of the scaling exponent (b) of mass to home‐range area relationship *

Category KDE (95% CI) AKDE (95% CI)
All mammals 1.20 (0.95–1.45) 1.28 (1.01–1.54)
Herbivores and frugivores 1.26 (0.99–1.52) 1.38 (1.09–1.66)
Carnivores and omnivores 1.23 (0.95–1.50) 1.27 (1.01–1.56)

*Abbreviations: KDE, kernel density estimation; AKDE, autocorrelated‐kernel density estimation.

Discussion

The importance of autocorrelation in animal‐tracking data has been an active area of research for decades (Swihart & Slade 1985; Fieberg 2007; Fleming et al. 2015). We, however, are the first to demonstrate that mass‐specific space requirements driven by autocorrelation‐induced underestimation of home‐range areas are worse for larger species. From a fundamental perspective, the continuous nature of animal movement means quantities, such as positions, velocities, and accelerations, are necessarily autocorrelated (Fleming et al. 2014). Autocorrelation time scales (τ) should, therefore, be viewed as explicit attributes of an animal's movement process (Gurarie & Ovaskainen 2015) that are revealed when the temporal resolution of measurement becomes ≲τ. As technological advances continue to permit ever‐finer sampling (Kays et al. 2015), persistent autocorrelation is likely to become the norm in animal‐tracking data. Pairing data from inherently autocorrelated processes with statistical approaches that ignore autocorrelation not only risks biasing any derived quantities, but also effectively negates the technological advances that are improving data quality. Unless analyses that are informed by autocorrelation become adopted by movement ecologists and conservationists, the issue of autocorrelation‐induced bias will only worsen. Conversely, properly harnessing the wealth of information provided by autocorrelation can dramatically improve the accuracy of tracking‐data‐derived measures (see also Fleming & Calabrese 2017; Winner et al. 2018; Noonan et al. 2019). Our findings, therefore, highlight the need for more statistical estimators that can handle biologically induced variance without introducing bias.

Implications of Size‐Dependent Bias

From a conservation perspective, the underestimation of home‐range areas is a worst‐case scenario. When reserves are too small, relative to their target species’ area requirements, the probability of local populations undergoing declines or extirpations increases significantly (Brashares et al. 2001; Gaston et al. 2008). Undersized protected areas resulting from poorly estimated space needs also risk exacerbating the issue of negative human–wildlife interactions at reserve boundaries (Van Eeden et al. 2018) as animals move beyond reserve boundaries to meet their energetic requirements (Farhadinia et al. 2018). It is thus of critical importance that policy actions be well informed about species’ spatial requirements. To this end, we analyzed a broad taxonomic and geographic range of data and identified a strong correlation between home‐range underestimation and body size when autocorrelation was ignored; average bias was approximately 50% at the upper end of the mass spectrum. In this regard, the majority of home ranges are estimated via methods based on the assumption of statistically independent data (Noonan et al. 2019). Combined with the facts that humans are the dominant mortality source for terrestrial vertebrates globally (Hill et al. 2019), that this mortality is higher for large‐bodied species (Hill et al. 2020), and that megafauna are experiencing more severe range contractions (Tucker et al. 2018) and extinction risk (Cardillo et al. 2005), the most threatened species are also likely to be those with the least accurate home‐range estimates, a worrying combination.

Based on these findings, we suggest that any conservation initiatives or policy based on home‐range estimates derived from estimators based on the assumption of statistically independent data be revisited, especially where large‐bodied species are involved. To facilitate this, we developed HRcorrect, an open‐access application that allows users to correct a home‐range area estimate for their focal species’ body‐mass‐specific‐bias with a correction factor calculated from our cross‐validation regression models. The current version of HRcorrect is freely available from https://hrcorrect.shinyapps.io/HRcorrect/. However, there are numerous factors beyond body mass that influence an individual's home‐range size. For instance, mammalian home‐range areas are well known to covary with the spatial distribution of resources (Litvaitis et al. 1986; Boutin 1990), social structure (Lukas & Clutton‐Brock 2013), sex (Cederlund & Sand 1994; Lukas & Clutton‐Brock 2013; Noonan et al. 2018), age (Cederlund & Sand 1994), population density (Adler et al. 1997), and reproductive status (Rootes & Chabreck 1993; Noonan et al. 2018). Furthermore, if an individual's space use changes over time (e.g., interseasonal and ‐annual variation), a home‐range area estimated from a single observation period may not be representative of its long‐term area requirements. As such, the deterministic trend‐based correction provided by HRcorrect is not a substitute for more rigorous data collection and home‐range estimation and should only be used for cases where the underlying tracking data are not accessible.

Allometries and Conservation Theory

The metabolic theory of ecology (West et al. 1997) suggests that body mass represents a super trait that governs a wide range of ecological processes. Prime among these is the relationship between body mass and home‐range area, an allometry that has guided ecological theory for more than 50 years (McNab 1963; Calder 1983; Jetz et al. 2004). More recently, attempts have been made to integrate this allometry into conservation theory. For instance, Hilbers et al. (2016) incorporated the home‐range allometry into a method for quantifying mass‐specific extinction vulnerability, and Hirt et al. (2018) highlighted how allometries in movement and space use can be used to make testable predictions of movement and biodiversity patterns at the landscape scale. Similarly, Pereira et al. (2004) used allometries of space use and movement rates to predict species‐level vulnerability to land‐use change. If the underlying allometries are biased, however, hypothesis testing and conservation planning in this context can fail even if the logic behind the experimental design is perfectly sound. Although the earliest derivation of the home‐range allometry proposed a metabolically determined M 0 . 75 allometry (McNab 1963), subsequent revisions showed no support for a purely energetic basis for home‐range scaling (Calder 1983; Kelt & Van Vuren 2001; Jetz et al. 2004; Tucker et al. 2014; Tamburello et al. 2015). Although all these studies concluded that home‐range area should scale with an exponent greater than the 0.75 predicted by metabolic requirements alone, there has been little consensus on whether the allometry is linear (M 1) or superlinear (M> 1). Our results suggest that at least part of the confusion can be attributed to the increasing bias in underestimating home ranges with increasing body size. Ours is the first study to estimate this relationship directly from tracking data by applying a consistent estimator across all individuals and, crucially, correcting for any potential autocorrelation‐induced bias (Noonan et al. 2019). In doing so, we documented a super‐linear relationship between body mass and home‐range area (exponent of approximately 1.25 for M). This shift from linear to power‐law scaling fundamentally changes the behavior of the relationship, particularly at the upper end of the mass spectrum. Although we did not investigate the mechanisms behind the deviation from the metabolically determined M 0 . 75, we encourage future work on this subject be based on the assumption of a superallometry, as opposed to linear allometry. Accurately quantifying species’ area requirements is a prerequisite for successful, area‐based conservation planning. Our results highlight an important yet hitherto unrecognized aspect of home‐range estimation: autocorrelation‐induced negative bias in home‐range estimation that is systematically worse for large species. Crucially, however, our findings also outline a readily applicable solution to the problem of size‐dependent bias. We demonstrated that home‐range estimation that properly accounts for the autocorrelation structure of the data is currently the only consistently reliable solution for eliminating allometric biases in home‐range estimation (see also Noonan et al. 2019). We emphasize that the differential scaling of autocorrelation across the mass spectrum be a key consideration for movement ecologists and conservation practitioners and suggest avoiding home‐range estimators that assume statistically independent data.

Supporting information

Data set summary statistics (Appendix S1), individual tracking data set summaries (Appendix S2), and mammalian phylogenetic relationships (Appendix S3) are available online. The authors are solely responsible for the content and functionality of these materials. Queries (other than absence of the material) should be directed to the corresponding author.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a Smithsonian Institution Scholarly Studies Award to M.J.N., J.M.C., and A.L.H. and by the U.S. NSF Advances in Biological Informatics program (ABI‐1458748 to J.M.C., W.F.F., and C.H.F.). N.B., F.J., and W.U. were supported by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft in the framework of the BioMove Research Training Group (DFG‐GRK 2118/1). T.M. and M.T. were funded by the Robert Bosch Foundation. S.L. was supported by Animals on the Move (NNX15AV92A), a NASA Arctic Boreal Vulnerability Experiment‐funded project. This work was supported in part by the Wellcome Trust/DBT India Alliance Fellowship to A.T.V. (grant IA/CPHI/15/1/502028) and an IISc‐ISRO Space Technology Cell Grant to M.T. Any use of trade, firm, or product names is for descriptive purposes only and does not imply endorsement by the U.S. Government.

Article impact statement: Due to autocorrelation‐induced bias, conventional methods severely underestimate the area requirements of GPS‐tracked large mammals.

Literature Cited

  1. Abrahms B. 2017. Data from: Suite of simple metrics reveals common movement syndromes across vertebrate taxa. MovebankData Repository. 10.5441/001/1.hm5nk220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adler G, Endries M, Piotter S. 1997. Spacing patterns within populations of a tropical forest rodent, Proechimys semispinosus, on five Panamanian islands. Journal of Zoology 241:43–53. [Google Scholar]
  3. Agnarsson I, Kuntner M, May‐Collado LJ. 2010. Dogs, cats, and kin: a molecular species‐level phylogeny of carnivora. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 54:726–745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barriel V, Thuet E, Tassy P. 1999. Molecular phylogeny of Elephantidae. Extreme divergence of the extant forest African elephant. Comptes Rendus de l'Acad´emie des Sciences‐Series III‐Sciences de la Vie 322:447–454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barton´ KA, Zwijacz‐Kozica T, Zieba F, Sergiel A, Selva N. 2019. Bears without borders: long‐distance movement in human‐dominated landscapes. Global Ecology and Conservation 17:e00541. [Google Scholar]
  6. Boutin S. 1990. Food supplementation experiments with terrestrial vertebrates: patterns, problems, and the future. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68:203–220. [Google Scholar]
  7. Brashares JS, Arcese P, Sam MK. 2001. Human demography and reserve size predict wildlife extinction in West Africa. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 268:2473–2478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brooks TM, et al. 2002. Habitat loss and extinction in the hotspots of biodiversity. Conservation Biology 16:909–923. [Google Scholar]
  9. Burnham KP, Anderson DR, Huyvaert KP. 2011. AIC model selection and multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: some background, observations, and comparisons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65:23–35. [Google Scholar]
  10. Burt WH. 1943. Territoriality and home range concepts as applied to mammals. Journal of Mammalogy 24:346–352. [Google Scholar]
  11. Calabrese JM, Fleming CH, Gurarie E. 2016. ctmm: an R package for analyzing animal relocation data as a continuous‐time stochastic process. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 7:1124–1132. [Google Scholar]
  12. Calder WA, III . 1983. Ecological scaling: mammals and birds. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 14:213–230. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cardillo M, Mace GM, Jones KE, Bielby J, Bininda‐Emonds OR, Sechrest W, Orme CDL, Purvis A. 2005. Multiple causes of high extinction risk in large mammal species. Science 309:1239–1241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cederlund G, Sand H. 1994. Home‐range size in relation to age and sex in moose. Journal of Mammalogy 75:1005–1012. [Google Scholar]
  15. Cross PC, Bowers JA, Hay CT, Wolhuter J, Buss P, Hofmeyr M, du Toit JT, Getz WM. 2016. Data from: Nonparameteric kernel methods for constructing home ranges and utilization distributions. Movebank Data Repository. 10.5441/001/1.j900f88t. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dong X, Fleming CH, Calabrese JM. 2017. ctmm webapp: a graphical user interface for the ctmm R package. Available from http://ctmm.shinyapps.io/ctmmweb/ (accessed February 2020).
  17. Fahrig L. 2007. Non‐optimal animal movement in human‐altered landscapes. Functional Ecology 21:1003–1015. [Google Scholar]
  18. Farhadinia MS, Johnson PJ, Macdonald DW, Hunter LTB. 2018. Anchoring and adjusting amidst humans: ranging behavior of Persian leopards along the Iran–Turkmenistan borderland. PLOS ONE 13 (e0196602). 10.1371/journal.pone.0196602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fieberg J. 2007. Kernel density estimators of home range: smoothing and the autocorrelation red herring. Ecology 88:1059–1066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fleming CH, Calabrese JM. 2017. A new kernel density estimator for accurate home‐range and species‐range area estimation. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 8:571–579. [Google Scholar]
  21. Fleming CH, Calabrese JM, Mueller T, Olson KA, Leimgruber P, Fagan WF. 2014. Non‐Markovian maximum likelihood estimation of autocorrelated movement processes. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 5:462–472. [Google Scholar]
  22. Fleming CH, Fagan WF, Mueller T, Olson KA, Leimgruber P, Calabrese JM. 2015. Rigorous home range estimation with movement data: a new autocorrelated kernel density estimator. Ecology 96:1182–1188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fleming CH, Noonan MJ, Medici EP, Calabrese JM. 2019. Overcoming the challenge of small effective sample sizes in home‐range estimation. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 10:1679‐1689. [Google Scholar]
  24. Gaston KJ, Jackson SF, Cantu´‐Salazar L, Cruz‐Piñón G. 2008. The ecological performance of protected areas. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 39:93–113. [Google Scholar]
  25. Grafen A. 1989. The phylogenetic regression. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, Biological Sciences 326:119–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gurarie E, Ovaskainen O. 2015. Characteristic spatial and temporal scales unify models of animal movement. The American Naturalist 178:113–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hall P, Hart JD. 1990. Convergence rates in density estimation for data from infinite‐order moving average processes. Probability Theory and Related Fields 87:253–274. [Google Scholar]
  28. Harvey PH, Pagel MD. 1991. The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, United Kingdom. [Google Scholar]
  29. Hilbers J, Schipper A, Hendriks A, Verones F, Pereira H, Huijbregts M. 2016. An allometric approach to quantify the extinction vulnerability of birds and mammals. Ecology 97:615–626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hill J, DeVault T, Wang G, Belant J. 2020. Anthropogenic mammal mortality increases with the human footprint. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 18:13–18. [Google Scholar]
  31. Hill JE, DeVault TL, Belant JL. 2019. Cause‐specific mortality of the world's terrestrial vertebrates. Global Ecology and Biogeography 28:680–689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hirt MR, Grimm V, Li Y, Rall BC, Rosenbaum B, Brose U. 2018. Bridging scales: allometric random walks link movement and biodiversity research. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 33:701–712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jetz W, Carbone C, Fulford J, Brown JH. 2004. The scaling of animal space use. Science 306:266–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Johansson Ö, Rauset GR, Samelius G, McCarthy T, Andr´en H, Tumursukh L, Mishra C. 2016. Land sharing is essential for snow leopard conservation. Biological Conservation 203:1–7. [Google Scholar]
  35. Kays R, Crofoot MC, Jetz W, Wikelski M. 2015. Terrestrial animal tracking as an eye on life and planet. Science 348:aaa2478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kays R, Hirsch BT. 2015. Data from: Stink or swim: techniques to meet the challenges for the study and conservation of small critters that hide, swim or climb and may otherwise make themselves unpleasant. Movebank Data Repository. 10.5441/001/1.8d8385j0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kelt DA, Van Vuren DH. 2001. The ecology and macroecology of mammalian home range area. The American Naturalist 157:637–645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lindblad‐Toh K, et al. 2005. Genome sequence, comparative analysis and haplotype structure of the domestic dog. Nature 438:803–819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Linnell JD, Andersen R, Kvam T, Andr´en H, Liberg O, Odden J, Moa PF. 2001. Home range size and choice of management strategy for lynx in Scandinavia. Environmental Management 27:869–879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Litvaitis JA, Sherburne JA, Bissonette JA. 1986. Bobcat habitat use and home range size in relation to prey density. Journal of Wildlife Management 50:110–117. [Google Scholar]
  41. Liu F‐GR, Miyamoto MM, Freire NP, Ong PQ, Tennant MR, Young TS, Gugel KF. 2001. Molecular and morphological supertrees for eutherian (placental) mammals. Science 291:1786–1789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lukas D, Clutton‐Brock TH. 2013. The evolution of social monogamy in mammals. Science 341:526–530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Matthee CA, van Vuuren BJ, Bell D, Robinson TJ. 2004. A molecular supermatrix of the rabbits and hares (Leporidae) allows for the identification of five intercontinental exchanges during the Miocene. Systematic Biology 53:433–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. McMahon LA, Rachlow JL, Shipley LA, Forbey JS. 2017. Data from: Evaluation of micro‐GPS receivers for tracking small‐bodied mammals. Movebank Data Repository. 10.5441/001/1.b0g6rq1t. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. McNab BK. 1963. Bioenergetics and the determination of home range size. The American Naturalist 97:133–140. [Google Scholar]
  46. Morato RG, et al. 2018. Jaguar movement database: a GPS‐based movement dataset of an apex predator in the neotropics. Ecology 99:1691–1691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Noonan MJ, et al. 2018. Data from: A comprehensive analysis of autocorrelation and bias in home range estimation. Dryad Digital Repository. 10.5061/dryad.v5051j2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Noonan MJ, et al. 2019. A comprehensive analysis of autocorrelation and bias in home range estimation. Ecological Monographs 89:e01344. [Google Scholar]
  49. Noonan MJ, Newman C, Markham A, Bilham K, Buesching CD, Macdonald DW. 2018. In situ behavioral plasticity as compensation for weather variability: implications for future climate change. Climatic Change 149:457–471. [Google Scholar]
  50. Paradis E, Schliep K. 2019. ape 5.0: an environment for modern phylogenetics and evolutionary analyses in R. Bioinformatics 35:526–528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Pawitan Y. 2001. In all likelihood: statistical modelling and inference using likelihood. Clarendon Press, Oxford, United Kingdom. [Google Scholar]
  52. Pe'er G, et al. 2014. Toward better application of minimum area requirements in conservation planning. Biological Conservation 170:92–102. [Google Scholar]
  53. Pereira HM, Daily GC, Roughgarden J. 2004. A framework for assessing the relative vulnerability of species to land‐use change. Ecological Applications 14:730–742. [Google Scholar]
  54. Perelman P, et al. 2011. A molecular phylogeny of living primates. PLOS Genetics 7:e1001342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D; R Core Team . 2018. nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3.1‐137. Available from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme (accessed February 2020).
  56. Price SA, Bininda‐Emonds ORP, Gittleman JL. 2005. A complete phylogeny of the whales, dolphins and even‐toed hoofed mammals (Cetartiodactyla). Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 80:445–473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Rootes WL, Chabreck RH. 1993. Reproductive status and movement of adult female alligators. Journal of Herpetology 27:121–126. [Google Scholar]
  58. Steiner CC, Ryder OA. 2011. Molecular phylogeny and evolution of the Perissodactyla. Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society 163:1289–1303. [Google Scholar]
  59. Swihart RK, Slade NA. 1985. Testing for independence of observations in animal movements. Ecology 66:1176–1184. [Google Scholar]
  60. Tamburello N, Côté IM, Dulvy NK. 2015. Energy and the scaling of animal space use. The American Naturalist 186:196–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Tucker MA, et al. 2018. Moving in the Anthropocene: global reductions in terrestrial mammalian movements. Science 359:466–469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Tucker MA, Ord TJ, Rogers TL. 2014. Evolutionary predictors of mammalian home range size: body mass, diet and the environment. Global Ecology and Biogeography 23:1105–1114. [Google Scholar]
  63. Uhlenbeck GE, Ornstein LS. 1930. On the theory of the Brownian motion. Physical Review 36:823–841. [Google Scholar]
  64. Van Eeden LM, Crowther MS, Dickman CR, Macdonald DW, Ripple WJ, Ritchie EG, Newsome TM. 2018. Managing conflict between large carnivores and livestock. Conservation Biology 32:26–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. West GB, Brown JH, Enquist BJ. 1997. A general model for the origin of allometric scaling laws in biology. Science 276:122–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wilman H, Belmaker J, Simpson J, de la Rosa C, Rivadeneira MM, Jetz W. 2014. Eltontraits 1.0: species‐level foraging attributes of the world's birds and mammals. Ecological archives E095‐178. Ecology 95:2027–2027. [Google Scholar]
  67. Wilson MC, et al. 2016. Habitat fragmentation and biodiversity conservation: key findings and future challenges. Landscape Ecology 31:219–227. [Google Scholar]
  68. Winner K, Noonan MJ, Fleming CH, Olson KA, Mueller T, Sheldon D, Calabrese JM. 2018. Statistical inference for home range overlap. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 9:1679–1691. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data set summary statistics (Appendix S1), individual tracking data set summaries (Appendix S2), and mammalian phylogenetic relationships (Appendix S3) are available online. The authors are solely responsible for the content and functionality of these materials. Queries (other than absence of the material) should be directed to the corresponding author.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material


Articles from Conservation Biology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES