Abstract
Treatment of schizophrenia has had limited success in treating core cognitive symptoms. The evidence of multi-gene involvement suggests that multi-target therapy may be needed. Meanwhile, the complexity of schizophrenia pathophysiology and psychopathology, coupled with the species-specificity of much of the symptomatology, places limits on analysis via animal models, in vitro assays, and patient assessment. Multiscale computer modeling complements these traditional modes of study. Using a hippocampal CA3 computer model with 1200 neurons, we examined the effects of alterations in NMDAR, HCN (Ih current), and GABAAR on information flow (measured with normalized transfer entropy), and in gamma activity in local field potential (LFP). We found that altering NMDARs, GABAAR, Ih, individually or in combination, modified information flow in an inverted-U shape manner, with information flow reduced at low and high levels of these parameters. Theta-gamma phase-amplitude coupling also had an inverted-U shape relationship with NMDAR augmentation. The strong information flow was associated with an intermediate level of synchrony, seen as an intermediate level of gamma activity in the LFP, and an intermediate level of pyramidal cell excitability. Our results are consistent with the idea that overly low or high gamma power is associated with pathological information flow and information processing. These data suggest the need for careful titration of schizophrenia pharmacotherapy to avoid extremes that alter information flow in different ways. These results also identify gamma power as a potential biomarker for monitoring pathology and multi-target pharmacotherapy.
Subject terms: Target identification, Neural circuits, Psychosis
Introduction
Schizophrenia is a chronic disease with a lifetime prevalence of around 4/10001, which usually produces life-long disability2. Cognitive impairment and information processing deficits are chief causes of disability3–5. The most affected cognitive domains are processing speed, working memory, episodic memory, and verbal learning and memory6–8. Current antipsychotic medications have limited impact on cognitive symptoms and information processing deficits9,10. Therefore, there are significant gaps in therapy and patients’ clinical care11.
Recent research has emphasized the role of glutamatergic transmission as an extension of the dopaminergic hypothesis for schizophrenia pathophysiology, especially to capture cognitive impairment associated with schizophrenia (CIAS) and information processing deficits12. The role of glutamatergic transmission has been supported by the psychotomimetic effect of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor (NMDAR) antagonists like phencyclidine (PCP) and ketamine in healthy volunteers13,14. Ketamine also worsened cognitive symptoms in schizophrenia patients15.
Oscillations are abnormal in schizophrenia patients, both in the low (delta and theta) and high (gamma) frequency ranges16,17. For example, a positive correlation was found between power of theta oscillations in the temporal lobe and positive symptoms using resting state MEG18. In a 40 Hz auditory steady-state response (ASSR) paradigm, theta power was found to be increased, and this was associated with impaired verbal memory19. Cortico-hippocampal theta oscillations play multiple roles in memory encoding and retrieval (reviewed in ref. 20), and mediate sequencing of events through theta/gamma coupling21,22.
Gamma oscillations are also involved in sensory integration23, enabling local computations within cortical microcircuits24 by providing binding among neurons belonging to a particular neuronal ensemble25,26, and routing information across brain regions27. Multiple studies revealed reduction in induced and evoked gamma power28–31 in schizophrenia patients. By contrast, presymptomatic, clinically high-risk individuals demonstrated an increase in resting gamma power32. Ketamine increased resting gamma power in healthy volunteers33. Interestingly, acute ketamine increased resting and evoked gamma power in rodent hippocampal CA334, while chronic ketamine reduced gamma power35, similar to what has been reported in patients with chronic schizophrenia32.
Combining evidence from gamma studies and ketamine studies suggests a role for NMDAR dysfunction in gamma abnormalities and in CIAS. Additional evidence comes from the schizophrenia genome-wide association study (GWAS) published in 2014, which identified GRIN2A (glutamate ionotropic NMDA-type receptor subunit 2A) on chromosome location 16p13 as being close to one of the 108 gene loci identified36. Clinically, agonists for NMDARs that have been studied for the treatment of CIAS include glycine and D-serine, both of which bind to an allosteric site on the GluN1 subunit of NMDAR and are obligatory for NMDAR activation by glutamate37. D-cyclo-serine, a partial agonist at the glycine site38, has also shown efficacy in some studies. However, these studies yielded mixed results; a recent meta-analysis found no overall significant effect on cognition, although young patients aged 30–39 years old showed some benefit39.
Another molecular aspect that we consider in this paper is the involvement of the HCN1 gene (hyperpolarization-activated cyclic nucleotide-gated channel type 1) that codes for Ih channels (hyperpolarization-activated current, also known as anomalous rectifier, If, Iq)40–42. Along with NMDAR, this channel plays a role in the generation and modulation of neuronal oscillations43,44. The HCN1 gene on chromosome location 5p21 is also close to one of the GWAS loci associated with schizophrenia36. The role of Ih in oscillations, and evidence from the GWAS study, suggest HCN1 product as another potential therapeutic target for CIAS45.
Manipulating the GABAergic system provides another potential treatment target46. The GABAergic system shapes synchronized neuronal activity during oscillations47–51, and is impaired in schizophrenia. Postmortem studies have revealed low inhibitory interneuron glutamic acid decarboxylase enzyme (GAD67), low GABA transporter, and low pyramidal cell GABAA receptor (GABAAR) α1 subunit mRNA transcripts in the frontal lobe of patients52. Similar findings were also demonstrated in the hippocampus, where a reduction in numbers of somatostatin-positive and parvalbumin-positive interneurons was also found53. The GABAergic deficit hypothesis for schizophrenia is further supported by reduced postmortem immunoreactivity of GAD65/67 in interneuronal neuropil in the hippocampus54. Reduced GABA tone has been suggested to mediate hippocampal hyperactivity in these patients55–59.
Schizophrenia pathology spans the multiple hippocampal subfields, as well as affecting adjacent para-hippocampal areas. We focus here on area CA3, an area that shows increased activity in schizophrenia but is relatively preserved in comparison to CA1. CA3 hyperactivity is seen in schizophrenia patients56, and in animal models of psychosis60,61. CA3 projects strongly to CA1, which also shows evidence of hyperactivity62,63, presumably as a consequence of these projections. CA1 shows shrinkage, even early in the illness64–66, suggesting early and prominent cell loss in CA1. A recent postmortem subfield transcriptome analysis of schizophrenia suggested that immune-mediated changes in CA3 might be one cause of CA3 excitability, which could subsequently produce synaptic scaling after cell loss in CA167, thereby further augmenting CA1 neuron loss68. Another reason to focus on CA3 is its possible role in pattern completion69–71, a component of cognitive processing. In this context, a hyperactive CA3 might alter pattern completion through activity spill-over59,72,73.
As described above, schizophrenia pathophysiology is extremely complex, with abnormalities at multiple scales, from genes, second messenger cascades, and cells, up to local networks and inter-areal communication. Given this complexity, it is reasonable to consider that multi-target pharmacotherapeutic approaches will be useful74. Targets in multi-target pharmacotherapy interact in highly non-linear ways, making it impossible to intuitively predict the effects75–77. We, therefore, use simulations to study these interactions. In this study, we investigated how alterations in the three targets mentioned above—NMDAR, Ih, GABAAR—will affect (1) oscillations and (2) information flow in a biophysically-realistic computer model of the CA3 region of the hippocampus. This builds on our prior results which identified the effects of blocking NMDARs on oriens-lacunosum moleculare (OLM) interneurons on gamma oscillations and information flow78.
Results
Baseline network activity produced theta with nested gamma oscillations
More than 2000 simulations were run using NEURON 7.479,80. These included 175 simulations for the NMDAR augmentation (5 input random seeds × 5 connectivity random seeds × 7 NMDAR scalings), and 1575 simulations for the tri-scaling interactions of NMDAR, Ih, and GABAAR (3 input random seeds × 3 connectivity random seeds × 7 NMDAR scalings × 5 Ih simulations × 5 GABAAR simulations), with additional preliminary simulations to tune the model. Multiple versions of individual simulations with different randomizations were run to ensure that results were reliable and not due to specific network architecture or background input. An individual simulation of 7 s model time required about 5 min computer time using an integration timestep of 0.1 ms on a Linux system with eight 2.67-GHz Intel Xeon quad-core CPUs. The full study utilized ~160 core-hours of high-performance computing time. Simulations were also run with finer timestep to assess numerical stability. We investigated the correlations between ion channel manipulations, scaling , , and , with information flow and gamma oscillation strength. We present the baseline simulations first, then NMDAR augmentation simulations, followed by the results from the simultaneous manipulation of NMDAR, Ih, and GABAAR.
In baseline simulations (Fig. 1), firing of the three neuronal populations was synchronized at two primary frequencies: gamma (~25–50 Hz) and theta (~6–8 Hz). Synchronized activity resulted from the following sequence of events: pyramidal neuronal firing triggered firing of PV basket interneurons, which in turn turned off the pyramidal neurons until inhibition wore off (pyramidal interneuron network gamma, PING oscillations) (Fig. 1c). Pyramidal neurons also triggered the firing of OLM interneurons, which provided further feedback inhibition to the pyramidal neurons, but at a slower rhythm (theta) due to OLM interneurons’ longer-lasting inhibition from longer GABAAR time constants. Synchronization at the theta frequency was also mediated by inhibitory input from the medial septum to both OLM and PV interneurons, but was not dependent on it. While pyramidal neurons were under the inhibitory influence of the OLM interneurons, the firing and reciprocal inhibition between PV basket interneurons generated interneuron network gamma (ING) oscillations (Fig. 1d). The synchronized firing of the pyramidal neurons, PV basket, and OLM interneurons was reflected in the local field potential (LFP) (Fig. 1b). The LFP showed both gamma and theta frequencies. The power spectral density (PSD) (Fig. 1e) showed the peaks of gamma (~35 Hz) and theta (~7 Hz) oscillations in this representative simulation.
Augmenting OLM NMDARs modulated circuit oscillations and information flow
Our first step was studying the effects of augmenting NMDAR of OLM interneurons on gamma oscillations and information flow across the model (Fig. 2). OLM interneurons were the location where NMDAR antagonism produced theta and gamma oscillatory changes similar to what was seen with ketamine in hippocampal CA334,78. Taking the simulations in Fig. 1 as the baseline, we then scaled of OLM interneurons from 1.5× to 30× the control simulations. This scaling could reflect a conductance increase due to phosphorylation or insertion of different isoforms with or without an increase in the number of NMDAR channels. Scaling of OLM interneurons reduced gamma power ~10-fold until it disappeared at above 30× control (Fig. 2b, left y-axis).
Scaling OLM upward increased drive onto OLM interneurons and therefore increased the firing rate of these inhibitory cells (Fig. 2b, blue line), which reduced the firing of the other two cell types that received OLM projections (PV, green; PYR, red). Information transfer (nTE) across the excitatory (PYR) population (Fig. 2a black) peaked at an intermediate level of PYR and PV firing, and showed far lower values with firing rate extremes, low or high. Cross-frequency coupling between theta phase and gamma amplitude, measured by modulation index (MI), also showed the inverted-U relationship with NMDAR scaling, correlating with information flow (Fig. 2a red). We found a similar inverted-U that related information flow to gamma power, a measure of synchronized firing (Fig. 2c). Note that the values of are reversed in Fig. 2c relative to 2a and 2b, with high on the left associated with low PYR firing and low gamma. There were two causes for low gamma with low PYR rate: (1) reduced number of spikes, which also reduced the strength of theta; (2) reduction in spiking coherence which was no longer as sharply shaped by PING and ING (Fig. 2d, scale = 30).
Modulating information flow with multiple channel manipulations
Consideration of multi-target pharmacotherapy requires determining how actions at different targets combine. We, therefore, looked at alterations in Ih and GABAAR, both of which are considered possible factors in schizophrenia pathophysiology36,53, along with . Having previously demonstrated strong effects on gamma of HCN channels of pyramidal and PV neurons43, we focused here on Ih at these two locations. The inverted-U configuration of the nTE peak for augmentation was seen at all Ih scalings except for 10× (Fig. 3). An inverted-U pattern could also be seen for GABAAR scaling with 10×, again at all but the highest Ih value. Similarly, the inverted-U peak can be detected for Ih scaling around the point marked H1, with a less well-defined, broader surface peaking around the point marked by H2. Thus, we have identified parameter ranges that identify a set of peaks which are points of strong nTE in a 4-D space based on this 3D parameter space.
Firing patterns from regions of low (L1–2; Fig. 4) and high (H1–2; Fig. 4) nTE were easily distinguishable. Two patterns of neuronal firing were seen which produced low nTE: (L1) low firing; (L2) over-synchrony. L1 exhibited low oscillatory power in addition to low firing. This was because of the combined inhibitory effect of high NMDAR scaling on OLM interneurons (30×) and high GABAAR scaling on pyramidal neurons (10×), leading to inhibition of pyramidal neurons and PV basket interneurons. In L2, highly synchronized phase-locked pyramidal neuron firing was driven by strong PV basket feedback inhibition, permitting little of the variation required to transfer information. This highly synchronized dynamics resulted from high Ih scaling (10×) on pyramidal neurons and PV interneurons, increasing their excitability and their synchronous activity, similar to what we reported before43. The different dynamics between L1 and L2 were reflected in their gamma power (bottom panel of Fig. 4).
Examination of rasters from points of high information flow-through (H1–H2 in Fig. 3) showed moderate levels of activity (H1–H2 in Fig. 4). Firing here was intermediate between the low firing of L1 and the strong phase-locking of L2, consistent with the inverted-U pattern. This intermediate synchronization allowed the pyramidal neurons to be excitable enough for the timing of their firing to be appropriately biased by driving input to allow information flow. Gamma power of H1 and H2 in the bottom panel of Fig. 4 was also intermediate between gamma power of L1 and L2. This pattern generalized across all manipulations, where maximum nTE was found at a mid-gamma-power range. As shown in Fig. 5, gamma power from the tri-scaling simulations was in the range of 0.01–10.8 mV2/Hz. However, the simulations with nTE in the top 10th percentile had gamma power at an intermediate range of 0.02–2.12 mV2/Hz.
The interaction between excitability and synchrony could be seen in the inverted-U shaped relationship of information flow with the firing rate of pyramidal neurons and PV interneurons (Fig. 6). At low-firing rate of pyramidal neurons, reflecting low excitability, there was reduced information flow (Fig. 6a, left). As firing rate increased, information flow increased. However, after a certain degree of excitability, information flow started decreasing. This could be explained by the limiting effect of highly synchronized firing on information flow, as reflected in the population synchrony of pyramidal neurons (red markers in Fig. 6c). Population synchrony was estimated using the coefficient of variation from the interspike intervals across all the neurons in a population81. Population synchrony ~1 represents interspike intervals that belong to a Poisson distribution, reflecting random population firing driven by the Poisson driving input. In a synchronized population, the upper range of population synchrony approaches , where N is the number of neurons firing synchronously at each firing cycle. At highly synchronous firing of pyramidal neurons (red markers in Fig. 6c) and PV interneurons (red markers in Fig. 6d), information flow was reduced. The coloring in panels a–d of Fig. 6 is for quintiles of pyramidal neuronal population synchrony, to keep track of the simulations with similar manipulations across firing rate and population synchrony. There was no clear relationship between OLM interneuronal firing pattern and information flow (Supplementary Fig. 1).
Discussion
Using a computer model of the CA3 microcircuit, we found an inverted-U pattern which related information flow both to potential therapeutic targets for schizophrenia—NMDAR, GABAAR, Ih, and to gamma activity. These findings suggest an interesting interaction between pyramidal neuronal excitability and synchrony (Fig. 7). At low excitability, pyramidal neurons were below firing threshold and showed low activity. A driving input was less likely to alter spike timing. Therefore, information flow was low. As excitability increased, more pyramidal neurons were closer to the firing threshold, and the driving input could more strongly bias firing, increasing information flow. At the other extreme of excitability, high pyramidal neuronal activity increased activity of basket interneurons, increasing gamma power via both ING and PING mechanisms (Fig. 1). With increased gamma power, pyramidal cell firing was increasingly locked into the oscillation, reducing the ability of inputs to influence firing times and reducing information flow. These findings provide a mechanistic explanation connecting pyramidal neuronal excitability and activity, with population dynamics and oscillations, in the context of potential therapeutic targets in schizophrenia.
Our study makes the following testable predictions: (1) Augmenting NMDARs on OLM interneurons, for example using photo-switchable NMDAR GluN2A or GluN2B subunits82 expressed specifically in the OLM interneurons, will show alterations in gamma activity in vivo. (2) The effect of augmenting NMDARs on information flow will depend on the dynamic state of the network reflected by gamma power. Optogenetic stimulation of channelrhodopsin-2 expressed in the PVs at gamma frequency could be used to manipulate gamma power83. Information flow could be measured by nTE using LFPs84. (3) Manipulations of Ih with photo-activated adenylate cyclase expressed specifically in PVs and PYRs, or by using norepinephrine, will produce our observed changes in gamma and information flow.
Either extreme of information flow will interfere with information processing in the brain. Brain organization is thought to be grossly hierarchical85. As a first approximation, we can ignore feedback circuits and consider the processing of information at each level from the prior processing stage86,87. We have previously hypothesized, and demonstrated in a model, that proper information processing at each stage requires a balance between information flow-through and information from the area itself, related to local resonance properties88. In the present model, where there is relatively low information flow-through, flow-through maximization would be optimal. Decreased flow-through across the CA3 microcircuit would provide decreased information at CA1, the next step in processing, with activity primarily related to CA3 dynamical state rather than to input from dentate gyrus.
Excessive dependence on internal activity has been proposed as a possible pathophysiological mechanism underlying the development of delusions in schizophrenia. Similar to our conclusions, Tamminga et al.59 suggested that CA3 hyperactivity in schizophrenia patients might indicate that the hypothesized pattern completion function of CA3 is excessive, resulting in faulty pattern completion that is not based on environmental input, producing delusions. In the cognitive domain, decreased information flow would impair performance because information being conveyed would be related more strongly to the dynamical patterns intrinsic to the CA3 microcircuit, rather than being related to the current task or to sensory input.
Coordination between different neural processes (neural coordination), including neuronal firing and oscillations, has been proposed to be important in cognitive coordination required to coordinate two or more frames of reference (e.g., visual and olfactory frames of reference in rodents), whether maintained in separate ensembles simultaneously or provided by alternating ensemble activation at delta or theta frequency89. Neural discoordination would then underlie cognitive symptoms (cognitive discoordination) in schizophrenia16,90,91. Failure to coordinate multiple frames could result in difficulty separating frames reflecting internal processing (expectations, Bayesian priors, imagining, planning) from frames reflecting external realities (stimulus associated). In our model, moderately-synchronized pyramidal neuronal firing was needed to increase information flow.
Impaired excitation/inhibition balance has been proposed to be one of the pathophysiological mechanisms in schizophrenia. In a combined fMRI and computer modeling study, Starc et al.92 showed impaired working memory deficits suggestive of cortical disinhibition. Impaired PV functioning in schizophrenia suggested the shift of the excitation/inhibition balance toward more excitable microcircuit as well93–95. Our model suggested how more excitable microcircuit can reduce information flow through increased synchrony.
Other groups have studied the relationship between gamma power and information flow outside the context of schizophrenia. Buehlmann et al.96 showed increased transfer entropy with increased gamma power between two sets of excitatory and inhibitory integrate-and-fire neurons. In addition to having a simpler model, they performed a modulation different than what we used here, the ratio, which may not have permitted them to explore as broad a range of gamma power. Akam et al.97 described a model where oscillations provided an additional channel for information flow besides the rate of neuronal firing. They studied the flow of information from four input populations of excitatory and inhibitory neurons into one output population. Switching the dynamics of one input population from irregular to oscillatory firing resulted in additional information flow, consistent with our study.
The major limitations of this study are the limitations that are inherent in all modeling studies—we necessarily made choices as to what to include and what to leave out. The things left out include both “known unknowns ... and unknown unknowns.” With respect to the unknown unknowns, we have limitations that are comparable, though more severe, than those of other studies—once you move to an animal disease model (in vivo) or remove tissue from the organism (in vitro) or simply extract parameters (in silico), you are eliminating much of the clinical phenomenology. With respect to the known unknowns, we are progressively adding detail and specifics using the best information available, but we continue to have computing and research limitations that reduce the fidelity of the model. In particular, (1) we omitted interneuron populations other than PV, OLM cells; (2) we omitted dopaminergic and serotonergic receptors, the targets of most current antipsychotic medications; (3) we modeled PV and OLM interneurons as having the same proportions; (4) we modeled PV and OLM as single compartments without 3D details; (5) there is inadequate information regarding the distribution of voltage- and calcium-sensitive ion channels in PYR dendrites.
Our multiscale modeling study suggests that network synchronization and pyramidal neuronal excitability are potential dynamical targets for the treatment of cognitive symptoms and information processing deficits in schizophrenia. We showed how manipulating multiple molecular elements in a multi-target pharmacotherapy approach explains some of the inverted-U shaped phenomena seen in schizophrenia98. Therefore, each patient may have to be carefully “tuned” to the middle range (either with DBS or pharmacology). Such an approach, a personalized medicine approach, fits the heterogeneity of the symptoms and genetic risk for the illness.
Multi-target pharmacotherapy is necessarily already used in schizophrenia treatment. Clozapine, often used after the failure of multiple other antipsychotic medications99, is considered a “dirty” medication that targets a wide array of receptors100. A multi-target pharmacotherapy approach is also supported by the evidence that multiple genes and proteins have been identified in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia (e.g., see ref. 101).
A number of FDA approved pharmacological agents act on the molecular targets we investigated here, and so could be explored for multi-target drug therapy complementing NMDAR augmentation in the treatment of cognitive symptoms in schizophrenia. GABAAR modulators include benzodiazepines, which are not subunit-specific, as well as subunit-specific agents, e.g., zolpidem102. But until now, GABAAR modulators are not neuronal subpopulation specific. In regard to Ih, two medications, lamotrigine and gabapentin, used for various neurological and psychiatric disorders, upregulate HCN1 levels103.
Our study points to a possible explanation of a non-intuitive relationship between gamma power and symptoms of schizophrenia. In schizophrenia and pharmacological models of schizophrenia, gamma power has been found to be decreased16,104,105 or increased32,106,107, depending on the study. Our results suggest a way to resolve the paradox: both extremes of gamma-power interfere with information processing. Psychotic symptoms, such as delusions of control and hallucinations, as well as cognitive symptoms, have been conceptualized as being due to “dysconnection” syndromes108, where the communication between different brain regions is disrupted. We would instead suggest that the complexity and variability of psychotic manifestations might instead be due to re-connection or re-wiring syndromes, where areas of decreased oscillatory strength will be disconnected and areas of increased oscillatory strength hyper-connected but with little variability and responsiveness to outside inputs (stereotypic thoughts and behaviors). This rewiring would be due to areas being pushed out of an essential central functional regime of gamma power. This further suggests the importance of careful titration when using schizophrenia medication; it would be easy to overshoot a target gamma-power zone.
Methods
Model
The full model is available on modelDB#258738 as http://modeldb.yale.edu/258738. It is based on refs. 43,78, modelDB#139421. The model was implemented in NEURON 7.479 running in parallel on eight 2.67 GHz Intel Xeon quad-core CPUs80. Result robustness was tested by using multiple random wirings and random stimulation input patterns for each parameter set, resulting in around 1900 simulations.
The model consisted of 800 pyramidal (PYR) neurons, 200 oriens-lacunosum moleculare (OLM) interneurons, and 200 parvalbumin-positive (PV) fast-spiking basket interneurons (Fig. 8). We used equal numbers of PV and OLM interneurons as PV interneurons provide the most prevalent feedback inhibition to pyramidal soma, while OLM interneurons provide the most dominant feedback inhibition targeting distal dendrites (reviewed in chapter 2 of ref. 109, and similar to other models43,78,110). These two interneurons are also hypothesized to generate theta-modulated gamma oscillations110–112. All neurons were modeled using multiple channels defined using Hodgkin–Huxley parameterizations. PYRs had five compartments comprising soma, apical dendrite, and basal dendrite. OLM and PV cells were single compartment. All neurons contained leak, fast sodium, delayed rectifier, and Ih. PYRs also had A-current, and had increasing Ih conductance up apical dendrite113. OLM added Ca2+-gated K2 current and high-threshold Ca2+ current with intracellular calcium dynamics.
PYRs projected both AMPARs and NMDARs on all cell types, with mid-apical projections onto other PYRs. PVs and OLMs projected to GABAAR: PV → PYR soma; OLM → PYR distal apical. OLM, PV received GABAAR input from medial septum (MS) at 6.7 Hz (theta). Synapses were modeled as double exponential mechanisms with parameters from ref. 110 (Table 1). Background activity was simulated low amplitude Poisson input to all populations114 (Table 2). Five seconds of simulation time were used for analysis, after discarding the first two seconds to allow network dynamics to stabilize. Local field potential (LFP) was calculated as voltage difference between apical and basal dendrites of all PYRs.
Table 1.
Presynaptic | Postsynaptic | Receptor | τ1 (ms) | τ2 (ms) | Conductance (nS) | Convergence |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pyramidal | Pyramidal | AMPA | 0.05 | 5.3 | 0.02 | 25 |
Pyramidal | Pyramidal | NMDA | 15 | 150 | 0.004 | 25 |
Pyramidal | Basket | AMPA | 0.05 | 5.3 | 0.36 | 100 |
Pyramidal | Basket | NMDA | 15 | 150 | 1.38 | 100 |
Pyramidal | OLM | AMPA | 0.05 | 5.3 | 0.36 | 10 |
Pyramidal | OLM | NMDA | 15 | 150 | 0.7 | 10 |
Basket | Pyramidal | GABAA | 0.07 | 9.1 | 0.72 | 50 |
Basket | Basket | GABAA | 0.07 | 9.1 | 4.5 | 60 |
Basket | OLM | GABAA | 0.07 | 9.1 | 0.023 | 1 |
OLM | Pyramidal | GABAA | 0.2 | 20 | 72 | 20 |
Table 2.
Cell | Section | Synapse | τ1 (ms) | τ2 (ms) | Conductance (nS) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pyramidal | Soma | AMPA | 0.05 | 5.3 | 0.05 |
Pyramidal | Soma | GABAA | 0.07 | 9.1 | 0.012 |
Pyramidal | Dend | AMPA | 0.05 | 5.3 | 0.05 |
Pyramidal | Dend | NMDA | 15 | 150 | 6.5 |
Pyramidal | Dend | GABAA | 0.07 | 9.1 | 0.012 |
Basket | Soma | AMPA | 0.05 | 5.3 | 0.02 |
Basket | Soma | GABAA | 0.07 | 9.1 | 0.2 |
OLM | Soma | AMPA | 0.05 | 5.3 | 0.0625 |
OLM | Soma | GABAA | 0.07 | 9.1 | 0.2 |
Manipulations
Target locations for NMDAR and Ih pathological manipulations were based on our prior work: NMDAR on OLM: modelDB#13942134,78; Ih on PYR, PV modelDB#15128243. We manipulated GABAARs on PYRs based on localization evidence from mRNA study in schizophrenia52 (Table 3).
Table 3.
Channel | Cell | Manipulation |
---|---|---|
NMDAR | OLM | ⇑ |
GABAA | Pyramidal | ⇕ |
Ih | Pyramidal & Basket | ⇕ |
Output measures
LFP power spectrum density (PSD) was calculated using Welch method (Python Scipy signal module)115 after removing DC component. Gamma power was measured as a 25–50 Hz band. Normalized transfer entropy (nTE) was used to measure information flow with 15 ms binning88,116; see ModelDB#136095 and Fig. 9. We measured phase-amplitude coupling between theta phase and gamma amplitude using modulation index (MI)117,118. Firing synchrony was measured using population coefficient of variance (popCV): for interspike intervals (isi) of the neuronal populations81.
Reporting summary
Further information on experimental design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
Supported by The department of Veterans Affairs, Veterans Health Administration, VISN 1 Career Development Award, and VA Special Psychopharmacology Research Fellowship Program, Leet and Patterson Trust Mentored Research Award, and New York State Tuition scholarship for graduate students to M.A.S.; IMAG (U01EB017695) and NIMH (R01MH086638) to W.W.L.; and NIDCD (R01DC012947-06A1) and Army Research Office Grant (W911NF-19-1-0402) to S.A.N. We would like to thank Larry Eberle (SUNY Downstate) for Neurosim lab support; Tom Morse and Ted Carnevale (Yale) for ModelDB support; and the Shepherd lab (Yale) for helpful comments. The views and conclusions contained in this document are those of the authors and should not be interpreted as representing the official policies, either expressed or implied, of the Army Research Office or the U.S. Government. The U.S. Government is authorized to reproduce and distribute reprints for Government purposes notwithstanding any copyright notation herein.
Author contributions
Conception of the work: M.A.S., S.A.N., and W.W.L. Data acquisition and analysis: M.A.S. Interpretation of data: M.A.S., S.A.N., and W.W.L. Preparation and revision of manuscript: M.A.S., S.A.N., and W.W.L.
Data availability
The model full code is available on modeldb: http://modeldb.yale.edu/258738. It includes the code that could be used to run the simulations.
Code availability
The model full code will be available on modeldb: http://modeldb.yale.edu/258738.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41537-020-00109-0.
References
- 1.Saha S, Chant D, Welham J, McGrath J. A systematic review of the prevalence of schizophrenia. PLoS Med. 2005;2:e141. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0020141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Knapp M, Mangalore R, Simon J. The global costs of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bull. 2004;30:279–293. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Matza LS, et al. Measuring changes in functional status among patients with schizophrenia: the link with cognitive impairment. Schizophrenia Bull. 2006;32:666–678. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kitchen H, Rofail D, Heron L, Sacco P. Cognitive impairment associated with schizophrenia: a review of the humanistic burden. Adv. Ther. 2012;29:148–162. doi: 10.1007/s12325-012-0001-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, Mintz J. Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizophrenia Bull. 2000;26:119–136. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Dickinson D, Harvey PD. Systemic hypotheses for generalized cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a new take on an old problem. Schizophrenia Bull. 2009;35:403–414. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Cognition in schizophrenia: impairments, determinants, and functional importance. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 2005;28:613–33. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2005.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Harvey PD, Rosenthal JB. Cognitive and functional deficits in people with schizophrenia: Evidence for accelerated or exaggerated aging? Schizophrenia Res. 2018;196:14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Carbon M, Correll CU. Thinking and acting beyond the positive: the role of the cognitive and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. CNS Spectr. 2014;19:38–52. doi: 10.1017/S1092852914000601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Citrome L. Unmet needs in the treatment of schizophrenia: new targets to help different symptom domains. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2014;75:21–26. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13049su1c.04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Forray C, Buller R. Challenges and opportunities for the development of new antipsychotic drugs. Biochemical Pharmacol. 2017;143:10–24. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2017.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Moghaddam B, Adams B, Verma A, Daly D. Activation of glutamatergic neurotransmission by ketamine: a novel step in the pathway from NMDA receptor blockade to dopaminergic and. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:2921–2927. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-08-02921.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Javitt DC, Zukin SR. Recent advances in the phencyclidine model of schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1991;148:1301–1308. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.10.1301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Krystal JH, Karper LP, Seibyl JP, Freeman GK, Delaney R, et al. Subanesthetic effects of the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist, ketamine, in humans. psychotomimetic, perceptual, cognitive, and neuroendocrine responses. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1994;51:199–214. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950030035004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Malhotra AK, Pinals DA, Adler CM, Elman I, Clifton A, et al. Ketamine-induced exacerbation of psychotic symptoms and cognitive impairment in neuroleptic-free schizophrenics. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997;17:141-150. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00036-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Oscillations and neuronal dynamics in schizophrenia: the search for basic symptoms and translational opportunities. Biol. Psychiatry. 2014;77:1001–1009. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Javitt DC, et al. A roadmap for development of neuro-oscillations as translational biomarkers for treatment development in neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45:1411–1422. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-0697-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Siekmeier PJ, Stufflebeam SM. Patterns of spontaneous magnetoencephalographic activity in patients with schizophrenia. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2010;27:179–190. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0b013e3181e0b20a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kirihara K, Rissling AJ, Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Light GA. Hierarchical organization of gamma and theta oscillatory dynamics in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2012;71:873–880. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Karakas, S. A review of theta oscillation and its functional correlates. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.04.008 (2020) (ahead of print). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 21.Buzsáki G, Wang X-J. Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;35:203–225. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lisman J. The theta/gamma discrete phase code occuring during the hippocampal phase precession may be a more general brain coding scheme. Hippocampus. 2005;15:913–922. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wang X-J. Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol. Rev. 2010;90:1195–1268. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00035.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bartos M, Vida I, Jonas P. Synaptic mechanisms of synchronized gamma oscillations in inhibitory interneuron networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:45–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.de Almeida L, Idiart M, Lisman JE. A second function of gamma frequency oscillations: an E%-max winner-take-all mechanism selects which cells fire. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:7497–7503. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6044-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Lisman J, Buzsáki G. A neural coding scheme formed by the combined function of gamma and theta oscillations. Schizophr. Bull. 2008;34:974–980. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Colgin LL, Denninger T, Fyhn M, Hafting T, Bonnevie T, et al. Frequency of gamma oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus. Nature. 2009;462:353–357. doi: 10.1038/nature08573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kissler J, Müller MM, Fehr T, Rockstroh B, Elbert T. MEG gamma band activity in schizophrenia patients and healthy subjects in a mental arithmetic task and at rest. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2000;111:2079–2087. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00425-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Krishnan GP, Vohs JL, Hetrick WP, Carroll CA, Shekhar A, et al. Steady state visual evoked potential abnormalities in schizophrenia. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2005;116:614–624. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Light GA, Hsu JL, Hsieh MH, Meyer-Gomes K, Sprock J, et al. Gamma band oscillations reveal neural network cortical coherence dysfunction in schizophrenia patients. Biol. Psychiatry. 2006;60:1231–1240. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Thuné H, Recasens M, Uhlhaas PJ. The 40-Hz auditory steady-state response in patients with schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73:1145–1153. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Grent T, et al. Resting-state gamma-band power alterations in schizophrenia reveal E/I-balance abnormalities across illness-stages. eLife. 2018;7:e37799. doi: 10.7554/eLife.37799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rivolta D, Heidegger T, Scheller B, Sauer A, Schaum M, et al. Ketamine dysregulates the amplitude and connectivity of high-frequency oscillations in cortical-subcortical networks in humans: Schizophrenia Bull. 2015;41:1105–1114. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lazarewicz MT, et al. Ketamine modulates theta and gamma oscillations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010;22:1452–1464. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kittelberger K, Hur EE, Sazegar S, Keshavan V, Kocsis B. Comparison of the effects of acute and chronic administration of ketamine on hippocampal oscillations: relevance for the NMDA receptor. Brain Struct. Funct. 2012;217:395–409. doi: 10.1007/s00429-011-0351-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature511, 421–427 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 37.Mothet JP, Parent AT, Wolosker H, Brady RO, Linden DJ, et al. D-serine is an endogenous ligand for the glycine site of the n-methyl-d-aspartate receptor. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2000;97:4926–4931. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.9.4926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Sheinin A, Shavit S, Benveniste M. Subunit specificity and mechanism of action of NMDA partial agonist D-cycloserine. Neuropharmacology. 2001;41:151–158. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(01)00073-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Chang C-H, et al. Effect of n-methyl-d-aspartate-receptor-enhancing agents on cognition in patients with schizophrenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis of double-blind randomised controlled trials. J. Psychopharmacol. 2019;33:436–448. doi: 10.1177/0269881118822157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Accili EA, Proenza C, Baruscotti M, DiFrancesco D. From funny current to HCN channels: 20 years of excitation. Physiology. 2002;17:32–37. doi: 10.1152/physiologyonline.2002.17.1.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Chen S, Wang J, Siegelbaum SA. Properties of hyperpolarization-activated pacemaker current defined by coassembly of HCN1 and HCN2 subunits and basal modulation by cyclic. J. Gen. Physiol. 2001;117:491–504. doi: 10.1085/jgp.117.5.491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Santoro B, Baram TZ. The multiple personalities of h-channels. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:550–554. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2003.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Neymotin SA, et al. Ih tunes theta/gamma oscillations and cross-frequency coupling in an in silico CA3 model. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e76285. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Neymotin SA, et al. Optimizing computer models of corticospinal neurons to replicate in vitro dynamics. J. Neurophysiol. 2017;117:148–162. doi: 10.1152/jn.00570.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kauppi K, et al. Revisiting antipsychotic drug actions through gene networks associated with schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2018;175:674–682. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17040410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Xu M-Y, Wong AHC. GABAergic inhibitory neurons as therapeutic targets for cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Acta Pharmacologica Sin. 2018;39:733–753. doi: 10.1038/aps.2017.172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Lytton WW, Sejnowski TJ. Simulations of cortical pyramidal neurons synchronized by inhibitory interneurons. J. Neurophysiol. 1991;66:1059–1079. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.3.1059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Chen X, Dzakpasu R. Observed network dynamics from altering the balance between excitatory and inhibitory neurons in cultured networks. Phys. Rev. E. 2010;82:031907. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.82.031907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Skinner FK. Cellular-based modeling of oscillatory dynamics in brain networks. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2012;22:660–669. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Pittman-Polletta BR, Kocsis B, Vijayan S, Whittington MA, Kopell NJ. Brain rhythms connect impaired inhibition to altered cognition in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2015;77:1020–1030. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Roux L, Buzsáki G. Tasks for inhibitory interneurons in intact brain circuits. Neuropharmacology. 2015;88:10–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Hashimoto T, Arion D, Unger T, Maldonado-Avilés JG, Morris HM, et al. Alterations in gaba-related transcriptome in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of subjects with schizophrenia. Mol. Psychiatry. 2008;13:147–161. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Konradi C, Yang CK, Zimmerman EI, Lohmann KM, Gresch P, et al. Hippocampal interneurons are abnormal in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Res. 2011;131:165–173. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Steiner J, Brisch R, Schiltz K, Dobrowolny H, Mawrin C, et al. GABAergic system impairment in the hippocampus and superior temporal gyrus of patients with paranoid schizophrenia: A post-mortem study. Schizophrenia Res. 2016;177:10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Heckers S, Konradi C. GABAergic mechanisms of hippocampal hyperactivity in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Res. 2015;167:4–11. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.09.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Li W, Ghose S, Gleason K, Begovic A, Perez J, et al. Synaptic proteins in the hippocampus indicative of increased neuronal activity in CA3 in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2015;172:373–382. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14010123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Tamminga CA. Psychosis is emerging as a learning and memory disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:247. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Tamminga CA, et al. Bipolar and schizophrenia network for intermediate phenotypes: outcomes across the psychosis continuum. Schizophr. Bull. 2014;40:S131–S137. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Tamminga CA, Southcott S, Sacco C, Wagner AD, Ghose S. Glutamate dysfunction in hippocampus: relevance of dentate gyrus and CA3 signaling. Schizophrenia Bull. 2012;38:927–935. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Bygrave AM, Jahans-Price T, Wolff AR, Sprengel R, Kullmann DM, et al. Hippocampal-prefrontal coherence mediates working memory and selective attention at distinct frequency bands and provides a causal link. Transl. Psychiatry. 2019;9:142. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0471-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Segev, A. et al. Reduced GluN1 in mouse dentate gyrus is associated with CA3 hyperactivity and psychosis-like behaviors. Mol. Psychiatry10.1038/s41380-018-0124-3 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 62.Talati P, Rane S, Kose S, Blackford JU, Gore J, et al. Increased hippocampal CA1 cerebral blood volume in schizophrenia. NeuroImage. Clin. 2014;5:359–364. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Schobel SA, Lewandowski NM, Corcoran CM, Moore H, Brown T, et al. Differential targeting of the CA1 subfield of the hippocampal formation by schizophrenia and related psychotic disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2009;66:938–946. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Nakahara S, Matsumoto M, van Erp TGM. Hippocampal subregion abnormalities in schizophrenia: A systematic review of structural and physiological imaging studies. Neuropsychopharmacol. Rep. 2018;38:156–166. doi: 10.1002/npr2.12031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Ho NF, Iglesias JE, Sum MY, Kuswanto CN, Sitoh YY, et al. Progression from selective to general involvement of hippocampal subfields in schizophrenia. Mol. Psychiatry. 2017;22:142–152. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Lieberman JA, et al. Hippocampal dysfunction in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia: a selective review and hypothesis for early detection and intervention. Mol. Psychiatry. 2018;23:1764–1772. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Perez, J. M. et al. Hippocampal subfield transcriptome analysis in schizophrenia psychosis. Mol. Psychiatry10.1038/s41380-020-0696-6 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 68.Rowan MS, Neymotin SA, Lytton WW. Electrostimulation to reduce synaptic scaling driven progression of alzheimer’s disease. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 2014;8:39. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2014.00039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Rolls ET. The mechanisms for pattern completion and pattern separation in the hippocampus. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2013;7:74. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Grande X, et al. Holistic recollection via pattern completion involves hippocampal subfield CA3. J. Neurosci. 2019;39:8100–8111. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0722-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Witter MP. Intrinsic and extrinsic wiring of CA3: indications for connectional heterogeneity. Learn Mem. 2007;14:705–713. doi: 10.1101/lm.725207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Tamminga CA, Stan AD, Wagner AD. The hippocampal formation in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2010;167:1178–1193. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09081187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Fellini L, Florian C, Courtey J, Roullet P. Pharmacological intervention of hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors impairs acquisition and long-term memory retrieval of spatial pattern. Learn. Mem. 2009;16:387–394. doi: 10.1101/lm.1433209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Kondej M, Stepnicki P, Kaczor AA. Multi-target approach for drug discovery against schizophrenia. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2018;19:3105. doi: 10.3390/ijms19103105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Neymotin SA, Dura-Bernal S, Lakatos P, Sanger TD, Lytton WW. Multitarget multiscale simulation for pharmacological treatment of dystonia in motor cortex. Front. Pharmacol. 2016;7:157. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2016.00157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Lytton WW, et al. Multiscale modeling in the clinic: diseases of the brain and nervous system. Brain Inform. 2017;4:219–230. doi: 10.1007/s40708-017-0067-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Neymotin SA, Dura-Bernal S, Moreno H, Lytton WW. Computer modeling for pharmacological treatments for dystonia. Drug Discov. Today Dis. Models. 2016;19:51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.ddmod.2017.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Neymotin SA, et al. Ketamine disrupts theta modulation of gamma in a computer model of hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:11733–11743. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0501-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Carnevale, N. T. & Hines, M. L. The NEURON Book (Cambridge University Press, 2006).
- 80.Hines ML, Davison AP, Muller E. Neuron and python. Front. Neuroinformatics. 2009;3:1. doi: 10.3389/neuro.11.001.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Tiesinga PHE, Sejnowski TJ. Rapid temporal modulation of synchrony by competition in cortical interneuron networks. Neural Comput. 2004;16:251–275. doi: 10.1162/089976604322742029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Berlin S, et al. A family of photoswitchable NMDA receptors. eLife. 2016;5:e12040. doi: 10.7554/eLife.12040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Cardin JA, et al. Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature. 2009;459:663–667. doi: 10.1038/nature08002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Kale, P., Acharya, J. V., Acharya, J., Subramanian, T. & Almekkawy, M. Normalized transfer entropy as a tool to identify multisource functional epileptic networks. In 40th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 1218–1221 (IEEE, 2018). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 85.Van Essen DC, Anderson CH, Felleman DJ. Information processing in the primate visual system: an integrated systems perspective. Science. 1992;255:419–423. doi: 10.1126/science.1734518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Himberger KD, Chien H-Y, Honey CJ. Principles of temporal processing across the cortical hierarchy. Neuroscience. 2018;389:161–174. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.04.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Linde-Domingo J, Treder MS, Kerrén C, Wimber M. Evidence that neural information flow is reversed between object perception and object reconstruction from memory. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:179. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-08080-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Neymotin SA, Jacobs KM, Fenton AA, Lytton WW. Synaptic information transfer in computer models of neocortical columns. J. Comput. Neurosci. 2011;30:69–84. doi: 10.1007/s10827-010-0253-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Lee H, Dvorak D, Fenton AA. Targeting neural synchrony deficits is sufficient to improve cognition in a schizophrenia-related neurodevelopmental model. Front Psychiatry. 2014;5:15. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Lee H, et al. Early cognitive experience prevents adult deficits in a neurodevelopmental schizophrenia model. Neuron. 2012;75:714–724. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Olypher AV, Klement D, Fenton AA. Cognitive disorganization in hippocampus: a physiological model of the disorganization in psychosis. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:158–168. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2064-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Starc M, et al. Schizophrenia is associated with a pattern of spatial working memory deficits consistent with cortical disinhibition. Schizophr. Res. 2016;181:107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Beneyto M, Lewis DA. Insights into the neurodevelopmental origin of schizophrenia from postmortem studies of prefrontal cortical circuitry. Int. J. Develop. Neurosci. 2011;29:295–304. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2010.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Lewis DA, Hashimoto T, Volk DW. Cortical inhibitory neurons and schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005;6:312–324. doi: 10.1038/nrn1648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Ferguson BR, Gao W-J. PV interneurons: critical regulators of E/I balance for prefrontal cortex-dependent behavior and psychiatric disorders. Front. Neural Circuits. 2018;12:37. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Buehlmann A, Deco G. Optimal information transfer in the cortex through synchronization. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2010;6:e1000934. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Akam T, Kullmann DM. Oscillations and filtering networks support flexible routing of information. Neuron. 2010;67:308–320. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.06.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Krystal JH, et al. Impaired tuning of neural ensembles and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia: A translational and computational neuroscience perspective. Biol. Psychiatry. 2017;81:874–885. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Nucifora FC, Mihaljevic M, Lee BJ, Sawa A. Clozapine as a model for antipsychotic development. NeuroTherapeutics: J. Am. Soc. Exp. NeuroTherapeutics. 2017;14:750–761. doi: 10.1007/s13311-017-0552-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Roth BL, Sheffler DJ, Kroeze WK. Magic shotguns versus magic bullets: selectively non-selective drugs for mood disorders and schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 2004;3:353–359. doi: 10.1038/nrd1346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Yang AC, Tsai S-J. New targets for schizophrenia treatment beyond the dopamine hypothesis. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2017;18:1689. doi: 10.3390/ijms18081689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Sanna E, et al. Comparison of the effects of zaleplon, zolpidem, and triazolam at various GABA(A) receptor subtypes. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2002;451:103–110. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(02)02191-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Postea O, Biel M. Exploring HCN channels as novel drug targets. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 2011;10:903–914. doi: 10.1038/nrd3576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. High-frequency oscillations and the neurobiology of schizophrenia. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2013;15:301–313. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2013.15.3/puhlhaas. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Rutter L, et al. Magnetoencephalographic gamma power reduction in patients with schizophrenia during resting condition. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:3254–3264. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Hong LE, et al. Gamma and delta neural oscillations and association with clinical symptoms under subanesthetic ketamine. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:632–640. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Hirano Y, et al. Spontaneous gamma activity in schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:813–821. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Stephan KE, Friston KJ, Frith CD. Dysconnection in schizophrenia: from abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring. Schizophr. Bull. 2009;35:509–527. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Cutsuridis, V., Graham, B., Cobb, S. & Vida, I. Hippocampal Microcircuits: A Computational Modeler’s Resource Book, 5 (Springer Science & Business Media, 2010).
- 110.Tort ABL, Rotstein HG, Dugladze T, Gloveli T, Kopell NJ. On the formation of gamma-coherent cell assemblies by oriens lacunosum-moleculare interneurons in the hippocampus. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:13490–13495. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705708104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Wang XJ, Buzsáki G. Gamma oscillation by synaptic inhibition in a hippocampal interneuronal network model. J. Neurosci. 1996;16:6402–6413. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-20-06402.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Wang X-J. Pacemaker neurons for the theta rhythm and their synchronization in the septohippocampal reciprocal loop. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;87:889–900. doi: 10.1152/jn.00135.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Kole MHP, Hallermann S, Stuart GJ. Single Ih channels in pyramidal neuron dendrites: properties, distribution, and impact on action potential output. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:1677–1687. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3664-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Destexhe A, Rudolph M, Paré D. The high-conductance state of neocortical neurons in vivo. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2003;4:739–751. doi: 10.1038/nrn1198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Jones, E., Oliphant, T., & Peterson, P. SciPy: Open source scientific tools for Python http://www.scipy.org/. (2001).
- 116.Gourévitch B, Eggermont JJ. Evaluating information transfer between auditory cortical neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;97:2533–2543. doi: 10.1152/jn.01106.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Tort ABL, et al. Dynamic cross-frequency couplings of local field potential oscillations in rat striatum and hippocampus during performance of a t-maze task. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:20517–20522. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810524105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Tort ABL, et al. Measuring phase-amplitude coupling between neuronal oscillations of different frequencies. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;104:1195–1210. doi: 10.1152/jn.00106.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The model full code is available on modeldb: http://modeldb.yale.edu/258738. It includes the code that could be used to run the simulations.
The model full code will be available on modeldb: http://modeldb.yale.edu/258738.