Skip to main content
Frontiers in Neurology logoLink to Frontiers in Neurology
. 2020 Sep 23;11:562275. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.562275

Menopause and Brain Health: Hormonal Changes Are Only Part of the Story

Pauline M Maki 1,*, Rebecca C Thurston 2
PMCID: PMC7538803  PMID: 33071945

Abstract

Most studies of menopause and brain aging have focused on the role of the sex steroid hormone, estradiol, as a key mechanisms contributing to cognitive and brain aging in women. An emerging literature demonstrates that beyond endogenous estradiol levels, menopausal symptoms, particularly vasomotor symptoms (VMS), are also key determinants of menopause-related changes in cognition and brain function. Critically, that literature shows the importance of using objective techniques to identify associations of VMS with memory performance, brain structure, and brain function. While self-report measures are important patient-centered outcomes in women's health research, objective measures of VMS typically relate more strongly to indices of cognitive and brain health. Currently, it is premature to make a causal claim about VMS and memory dysfunction, but initial findings raise the possibility that women with VMS might experience an improvement in cognition with VMS treatment. More generally, these findings underscore the utility of investigating female-specific risk factors for cognitive decline.

Keywords: menopause, cognition, vasomotor, brain, neuroimaging, cardiovascular

Introduction

The burgeoning field of research in menopause and brain health has its roots in the discovery, now more than 30 years ago, by Catherine Woolley in the laboratory of Bruce McEwen that the structure and function of the hippocampus is influenced by changes in physiological levels of the sex steroid hormone, estradiol (1, 2). This foundational discovery propelled the growth of basic science research on the role of sex steroid hormones in brain function and brain structure, establishing a protective role in both rodent and non-human models. While there is a clear role for research on the role of sex steroid hormones in brain aging, comparatively little work has been conducted on the role of menopausal symptoms in brain aging. The hallmark symptom of the menopause is vasomotor symptoms (VMS), hot flashes and night sweats. These symptoms persist for many women well beyond the final menstrual period, into periods of risk for dementia, when levels of estradiol have plateaued (3). The continuity of VMS beyond the menopausal transition raises important questions about their potential role in cognition and brain health. This focus is important not only mechanistically but also clinically. If VMS contribute to cognitive dysfunction in women, any effective treatment for VMS regardless of whether the treatment is hormonal, non-hormonal, or lifestyle, could potentially confer cognitive benefit.

In this review, we present an overview of VMS in relation to cognition and brain function at midlife and beyond. We consider the methodologies used to measure VMS and the importance of using objective VMS measures in research studies. We draw on observational studies and clinical trials to show initial evidence of a relationship between VMS and memory dysfunction—the cognitive domain that appears most sensitive to female reproductive factors (4). We consider the mechanisms by which VMS can influence memory, drawing on our neuroimaging studies and on studies showing linkages to other risk factors for cognitive aging, particularly cardiovascular disease, and sleep dysfunction. We conclude that emerging evidence suggests a role for VMS in cognitive aging in women.

VMS Epidemiology

VMS are the classic symptom of the menopause transition. VMS are experienced by most midlife women at some point during the menopause transition (5). For a third of women, VMS are frequent or severe. Whereas, VMS have long been assumed to be an incidental midlife symptom, more recent research has brought a wealth new knowledge about this prevalent midlife experience that has challenged long-held assumptions, elucidating their epidemiology and implications for women's health and functioning.

Whereas, VMS were once thought to be time-delimited events isolated to a few years around the onset on the postmenopause, recent data indicate that VMS persist for an average of 7–10 years for moderate-severe or frequent VMS, and much longer for milder symptoms (3, 6). Major cohort studies from the United States (US) and around the globe have further indicated that not all women follow the same trajectories of VMS over the transition (7, 8). For example, in the United States, 18% of women have VMS primarily when they are still cycling early in the transition, 29% of women have VMS have VMS primarily postmenopausally once their cycles have stopped, 27% of women have few or no VMS, and 25% of women have VMS that persist from early in the transition through the later postmenopausal years (7).

A growing body of research also underscores the pronounced racial/ethnic differences in VMS. In the United States, African American women have the most frequent, persistent, and bothersome VMS of any racial/ethnic groups, with over 80% of African American women reporting VMS at some point during the transition (9, 10). Approximately 50–70% of non-Hispanic White, Asian (Japanese, Chinese), and Hispanic report VMS during the menopause, with slightly (non-significantly) lower rates among Asian women. Similarly, in an Australian sample, Asian women have lower rates of VMS than other groups, and in an international consortium of menopause studies, Japanese women were less likely to report severe VMS than were Caucasian women (11, 12). Further, independent of race/ethnicity, less educated women and women in lower socioeconomic positions have more VMS than their more educated and affluent counterparts in both the United States (5) and in the United Kingdom (8).

VMS can have a profound impact on women's lives. They are a consistent predictor of depressed mood, sleep problems, and poorer quality of life during the menopause transition across social, emotional, and physical domains of functioning (1315). In addition to these important implications for quality of life and functioning, recent data have also linked VMS to key indices of physical and neurocognitive health.

VMS Physiology

Insights into the physiology of VMS has expanded greatly in recent years with the accumulation of clinical and basic science studies elucidating the critical role of hypothalamic KNDy (kisspeptin, neurokinin B and dynorphin) neurons (16). These neurons are located in the arcuate nucleus (infundibular nucleus in humans) of the hypothalamus, and play a critical role in the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal (HPG) axis. They are hypothesized to function as the gonadotropin releasing hormone (GnRH) pulse generator. In that role, KNDy neurons are thought to control the release of follicle stimulating hormone (FSH) and luteinizing hormone (LH), which stimulate ovarian production of estradiol. In turn, estradiol acts on KNDy neurons through negative feedback. As levels of estrogen decrease at menopause, KNDy neurons undergo hypertrophy (enlargement) and are reversed with estrogen supplementation (17). With respect to thermoregulatory function, KNDy neurons project to the median preoptic nucleus (MnPO) of the hypothalamus. There they bind to neurokinin 3 receptors (NK3R) which in turn project to heat dissipation effectors. The ligand for NK3R receptors is the endogenous neuropeptide, neurokinin B (NKB). As levels of estrogen decline, levels of NKB rise and activate NK3R receptors in the MnPO. This overactivation results in rapid heat dissipation response that women experience symptomatically as VMS. Targeting this mechanism, pharmacologic antagonists of NK3R are a new line of therapeutics to treat VMS in women, and lower VMS frequency and intensity more rapidly than conventional menopausal hormone therapy (HT) (18).

It has been known for quite some time that while declines in estradiol in the menopausal transition set the general stage for VMS and are therefore a necessary factor, changes in estradiol are not a sufficient proximal cause of individual VMS events (19). All women transition through the menopause but not all women have VMS. From a mechanistic perspective, the view that excessive NK3 signaling by NKB in the MnPO causes VMS accounts for the necessary role of estrogen withdrawal and for findings of a temporal, but not causal relationship of VMS and pulses of LH (16). However, other physiologic systems have been linked to VMS including the autonomic nervous system (particularly vagal withdrawal) (20, 21), the thermoregulatory system (22), and the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis (more fully elucidated below). Thus, significant advances have been made, yet further research is required to fully understand the underlying physiology of VMS, which is turn can advance the science on VMS and brain health.

VMS Measurement: What Objective Techniques Tell Us That Self-Report Does Not

VMS are measured in several ways. Self-report approaches include questionnaires that ask women to recall their VMS from weeks, months, or years prior. In other work, diaries to report VMS are employed, and are completed at the end of the day, upon waking, or at the time of the VMS (23). However, these self-report measures have important limitations. It is well-established that the self-report of physical symptoms is subject to memory, mood, and other reporting processes (2427). Important, VMS can also be assessed physiologically. The most well-established physiologic measure of VMS is sternal skin conductance (28). Early work supported the validity of this approach, which has been used in multiple subsequent observational studies and even clinical trials (29, 30). These physiologic measures are particularly important to understanding the etiology of the VMS and their relationships to key health indices apart from the influence of psychological, memory, and other reporting factors.

Psychological, sleep quality, and other non-VMS factors have a well-established impact on reporting of VMS. Typically, 62–72% of reported VMS are associated with an objective VMS, and 47–70% of objective VMS accompanied by a subjective report (31), with estimates varying by factors including whether the VMS occurred during sleep or wake, mood at the time of reporting, and the quality of the prior night's sleep. For example, more negative mood was associated with increased reporting of VMS not detected physiologically (31). Women with more negative mood also appraise their VMS to be more bothersome, apart from the occurrence of the VMS (32). Notably, physiologic VMS do not show placebo responses (in contrast to the 30% placebo response with diary-reported VMS (28, 30). Such evidence validates the physiologic VMS measures, as a “true” VMS measure that would not be expected to change with a placebo intervention. Further, physiologic VMS are particularly important when assessing nocturnal VMS. Accurately reporting VMS during sleep can be challenging and influenced by the quality of the sleep itself. For example, several studies with self-report and physiologic VMS measures show that reporting of VMS upon waking is influenced by the quality of sleep the prior night (27, 33). Some research indicates that reports of nighttime VMS recalled upon waking may be more related to sleep quality than to the occurrence of VMS themselves (33).

Finally, use of physiologic VMS measures is particularly important when investigating the links between VMS and cognition. Poorer memory performance may be associated with less accurate recall of VMS, thereby introducing a significant bias into the study when using self-reported VMS measures only. Despite this methodological limitation of self-report VMS, most of the literature relating VMS to memory performance relies on self-report measures of VMS. Ambulatory VMS monitors allow for real-time self-report of VMS events and therefore do not rely on recall for self-report VMS. In this way, studies using ambulatory monitors allow for the investigation of both self-report and physiologic VMS in relation to memory, without the influence of recall bias.

VMS and Memory

In addition to VMS, cognitive complaints and objective cognitive difficulties are common in the menopausal transition. For example, among 12,425 healthy women 40–55 years of age in a baseline study of the SWAN, 39% of women complained of forgetfulness (34). Among those women, complaints of forgetfulness (yes/no) varied by menopausal status; compared to the premenopause, the odds of forgetfulness increased by 44% in the early perimenopause, 43% in the late perimenopause, 27% in the postmenopause, and 27% odds in surgical menopause (34). In the Seattle Midlife Women's Health Study, predictors of memory complaints included age, hot flashes, anxiety, depressed mood, perceived stress, perceived health and history of sexual abuse (35). Subjective memory complaints relate to objective cognitive difficulties in midlife women. In the Rochester Investigation of Cognition and Menopause (RICAM), memory complaints on a standardized questionnaire, the Memory Function Questionnaire, were associated with decreased encoding on a verbal memory test and with working memory (36) and in later work with working memory and attention/concentration (37). In our own work, subjective ratings of current memory were associated with performance on a measure of delayed verbal memory (38). Large-scale prospective studies of healthy women followed across the menopausal transition demonstrate subtle but reliable changes in verbal learning and memory, as well as processing speed (39, 40). Notably, initial evidence from these studies indicate that performance declines in the perimenopause but appears to normalize in the postmenopause (39, 40). If this pattern of change in memory across the menopausal transition is valid, it is difficult to envisage how declines in estradiol alone can account for changes in memory, as memory appears to bounce back while declines in estradiol persist.

A growing body of evidence suggests that menopausal symptoms, particularly VMS, may account in part for changes in memory in midlife women. Importantly, subjective VMS generally are unrelated to memory performance, including a study of 6-year longitudinal cohort study of 1,903 midlife women in SWAN (41). The studies showing evidence of a relationship between VMS and memory performance relied on the use of the ambulatory monitors (4244). The first evidence emerged from a cross-sectional investigation of women with moderate-to-severe VMS (43). Women in that study wore ambulatory VMS monitors, recorded subjective VMS on that monitor and/or on a diary (useful for sleep VMS), and performed a battery of neuropsychological tests. A higher frequency of physiologic VMS but not subjective VMS, particularly during sleep, were associated with lower performance on a verbal memory test. Self-reported sleep, age, race, and mood did not account for those relationships. The association between VMS and worse memory was recently replicated in breast cancer survivors, again with associations evident only in physiologic VMS (42). In breast cancer survivors, effects were not differentially stronger during sleep. Importantly, these associations were independent of actigraphy-based measures of sleep, mood and other factors (42).

The findings from these cross-sectional studies of physiologic VMS and memory difficulties, raised the possibility that treating VMS may result in improvements in memory. To address that question, we examined changes in memory performance and physiological VMS in a pilot randomized, sham-control study of stellate ganglion blockade (SGB) (44). SGB is an anesthesia procedure used in pain medicine and involves administration of a local anesthetic to the C6 ganglion. Consistent with findings from open-label trials (4548), SGB reduced moderate-to-severe VMS and reduced physiologic VMS compared to sham (30). While SGB did not significantly improve memory, the magnitude of improvement in physiologic VMS was significantly associated with the magnitude of improvement in memory performance (44). Those findings suggested a linear relationship between improvements in VMS frequency and improvements in memory, and raise the possibility that other hormonal and non-hormonal interventions for VMS may confer memory benefits.

VMS and Brain Health

In a series of neuroimaging studies in midlife women, physiological VMS but not subjective VMS, were associated with brain health. In MsHeart, the frequency of physiologic VMS was positively associated with increases in white matter hyperintensities (49). White matter hyperintensities (WMH) are risk factors for AD and vascular dementia. Whether VMS are causally related to WMH is unknown, but the pathophysiological basis of this association is likely to involve cardiovascular disease (CVD) risk factors, as CVD risk factors are linked to both VMS and WMH (50).

In addition to brain structure, physiologic VMS are associated with alterations in brain function measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), including resting state functional connectivity (51) and blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) fMRI during performance of a verbal memory task (52). In Ms. Heart, we investigated the relationship of VMS with alterations in the default mode network (DMN) during the resting state (52). The DMN is an organized network in the brain that is active during rest and suppressed during tasks requiring attention to the external world. The DMN includes regions along the anterior and posterior midline, the lateral parietal cortex, prefrontal cortex, and medial temporal lobe, including the hippocampus (53). In Ms. Heart, the frequency of VMS was associated with alterations in this network at rest as measured with fMRI (51). The pattern of alterations was characterized by hyperconnectivity of the hippocampus with multiple frontal regions, and this hyperconnectivity was especially pronounced for sleep VMS. Importantly, these associations persisted after controlling for age, race, education, and sleep. Hyperconnectivity in the DMN has been viewed as reflecting a fundamental response to neurological disruption (54), often viewed as compensatory in nature. As in the memory studies, physiologic VMS but not reported VMS were associated with alterations in the DMN, underscoring the importance of objective methods for VMS ascertainment.

In a recent fMRI study, we examined the association between VMS and brain activation during performance of a verbal memory task, including word encoding and word recognition conditions (52). Physiologic VMS but not reported VMS were associated with decreased memory and with alterations in brain activity during the memory task. During word encoding, more frequent physiologic VMS were positively associated with activation in the left orbitofrontal cortex, left middle frontal gyrus/superior frontal gyrus, right superior frontal gyrus, and right parahippocampus. During recognition, more frequent VMS were positively associated with activation in bilateral middle frontal and superior frontal regions and bilateral hippocampus/parahippocampus. Overall, these data suggest that VMS-related declines in memory may be due to alterations in the function of the hippocampus, parahippocampus, and multiple regions of the prefrontal cortex.

Together, the two fMRI studies present a consistent pattern that drives ongoing work in this area. The areas of the DMN associated with physiologic VMS—the hippocampus, medial frontal and orbitofrontal cortex—overlapped considerably with the brain areas associated with VMS in the memory study. In both cases, the patterns were of increased connectivity or activity, consistent with a compensatory response. Neither study found an association between brain function and subjective VMS.

VMS and Brain Health Putative Mechanisms

Cardiovascular

VMS have been linked to multiple indicators of CVD risk. In both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies, self-reported VMS have been associated with a more adverse CVD risk factor profile, including high blood pressure and hypertension risk, higher lipids (total cholesterol, LDL cholesterol, triglycerides, ApoB), a more insulin resistant profile, diabetes risk, and in some studies, a more pro-inflammatory or pro-coagulant profile (55). Controlling for traditional CVD risk factors, both self-reported. In the National Institutes of Health-funded MsHeart study, physiologically assessed presence or frequency of VMS have also been linked to poorer endothelial function, particularly for younger midlife women (56, 57). In MsHeart and other studies, more frequent self-reported and physiologically assessed VMS have been associated with greater carotid atherosclerosis (intima media thickness and plaque) (5860). Other work suggests that reported VMS may also be associated with increased risk of future clinical CVD events (e.g., myocardial infarction, heart failure, stroke) and CVD mortality, which may depend on the timing of the VMS (61, 62). These associations between VMS and CVD risk generally are not explained by endogenous estradiol levels when these measurements are available. Notably, poorer cardiovascular health (e.g., adverse CVD risk factors, subclinical and clinical CVD) is a well-established risk factor for dementia (6366). Thus, CVD risk should be considered as one mechanism linking VMS to poorer cognitive health and dementia risk.

Cortisol and Blood Flow

VMS are associated with elevated levels of cortisol, a steroid hormone that is released by the adrenal glands in response to physical and psychological stress and activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (6769). Cortisol levels increase 20 min after a VMS, as measured objectively by changes in finger skin temperature and skin resistance (67). Increased cortisol may be one factor contributing to cognitive difficulties in midlife women with VMS. Elevated cortisol has been linked to decreases in memory and executive functioning, particularly in women (7073). Levels of glucocorticoids at both high and low levels, in an inverted-U dose response curve, are associated with suboptimal performance on learning and memory retrieval tasks (74). Other work demonstrates that VMS frequently cause a reduction in blood flow to the brain (~5% reduction) (75).

Sleep

In addition to VMS, sleep problems are common during the menopause transition, with up to half of midlife women reporting sleep problems (14). In longitudinal cohort studies, reported VMS are one of the most consistent predictors and robust of reported sleep problems during the menopause transition (76). Although findings employing objective measures of sleep and VMS initially produced somewhat mixed findings, several recent studies using objective measures of both VMS and sleep have further supported the importance of VMS to sleep (77). For example, we found a 5-fold odds of actigraphy-detected wakening with an objective VMS than during times without objective VMS; these wake episodes were observed irrespective of whether the woman reported the VMS or even knew she was having VMS (78). This poor sleep can have implications for women's brain health. We have found that women with greater objectively-detected wake after sleep onset had increased WMH even after considering multiple covariates including VMS (79). Sleep disturbance negatively affects cognitive function during healthy aging (80) and is associated with an increased risk of Alzheimer's disease (81). VMS may therefore negatively impact cognition and brain health through disrupted sleep quality.

Discussion

An emerging body of work indicates that VMS may be an important determinant of cognition at midlife and beyond. This association emerged with the use of objective techniques for measuring VMS, and was not evident with self-report. About 70% of women will experience VMS and it is now recognized that this hallmark menopausal symptom persists in many women for more than a decade after the final menstrual period. While the focus on estradiol and brain health is clearly justified on the basis of a wealth of basic science studies, this emerging body of work supports the need to expand the focus beyond hormonal effects in menopause to menopausal symptoms. This focus is all the more important because of the possibility that VMS might be a modifiable risk factor for memory dysfunction.

Initial findings from a pilot study raise the possibility that women with VMS might experience an improvement in cognition once treated. It is clear that currently no strong claim can be made about a causal relationship between VMS and memory decline, but at minimum VMS may help to identify women at risk for memory decline at midlife.

At the systems level, our ongoing work in MsBrain funded by the National Institute on Aging explores relationships between physiologic VMS and brain health across a range of neuroimaging outcomes, including fMRI measures of verbal memory, resting state, and emotion perception; WMH; diffusion tensor imaging; and brain volume in a large sample of women who also contribute neuropsychological assessments, actigraphy-based measures of sleep, measures of subclinical CVD and CVD risk factors, hormone measures, and a wide range of sociodemographic and clinical variables. Findings from that study will not only provide insights into the reliability of initial findings in MsHeart, but will also allow for a more comprehensive assessment of the complex array of factors that link VMS to cognitive health. Ultimately, this line of research will contribute to a growing literature identifying female-specific risk factors for cognitive aging.

Author Contributions

PM and RT each contributed to the writing of this review. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

PM has served as a consultant to: Abbvie, Balchem, and Pfizer. RT has served as a consultant/advisor to Astellas, Pfizer, Procter & Gamble, and Virtue Health.

Footnotes

Funding. This work was funded by the 1RF1AG053504-01 to RT and PM (Multi-PI).

References

  • 1.Woolley CS, Gould E, Frankfurt M, McEwen BS. Naturally occurring fluctuation in dendritic spine density on adult hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci. (1990) 10:4035–9. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-12-04035.1990 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Woolley CS, McEwen BS. Estradiol mediates fluctuation in hippocampal synapse density during the estrous cycle in the adult rat. J Neurosci. (1992) 12:2549–54. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-07-02549.1992 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Avis NE, Crawford SL, Greendale G, Bromberger JT, Everson-Rose SA, Gold EB, et al. Study of Women's Health Across the, duration of menopausal vasomotor symptoms over the menopause transition. JAMA Intern Med. (2015) 175:531–9. 10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.8063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Maki PM. Verbal memory and menopause. Maturitas. (2015) 82:288–90. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2015.07.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gold EB, Colvin A, Avis N, Bromberger J, Greendale GA, Powell L, et al. Longitudinal analysis of the association between vasomotor symptoms and race/ethnicity across the menopausal transition: study of women's health across the nation. Am J Public Health. (2006) 96:1226–35. 10.2105/AJPH.2005.066936 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Freeman EW, Sammel MD, Lin H, Liu Z, Gracia CR. Duration of menopausal hot flushes and associated risk factors. Obstet Gynecol. (2011) 117:1095–4. 10.1097/AOG.0b013e318214f0de [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Tepper PG, Brooks MM, Randolph JF, Jr, Crawford SL, El Khoudary SR, Gold EB, et al. Characterizing the trajectories of vasomotor symptoms across the menopausal transition. Menopause. (2016) 23:1067–74. 10.1097/GME.0000000000000676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mishra GD, Kuh D. Health symptoms during midlife in relation to menopausal transition: British prospective cohort study. BMJ. (2012) 344:e402. 10.1136/bmj.e402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Thurston RC, Joffe H. Vasomotor symptoms and menopause: findings from the study of Women's Health across the Nation. Obstet Gynecol Clin North Am. (2011) 38:489–501. 10.1016/j.ogc.2011.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Freeman EW, Grisso JA, Berlin J, Sammel M, Garcia-Espana B, Hollander L. Symptom reports from a cohort of African American and white women in the late reproductive years. Menopause. (2001) 8:33–42. 10.1097/00042192-200101000-00007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mishra G, Lee C, Brown W, Dobson A. Menopausal transitions, symptoms and country of birth: the Australian Longitudinal Study on Women's Health. Aust N Z J Public Health. (2002) 26:563–70. 10.1111/j.1467-842X.2002.tb00367.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Chung HF, Pandeya N, Dobson AJ, Kuh D, Brunner EJ, Crawford SL, et al. The role of sleep difficulties in the vasomotor menopausal symptoms and depressed mood relationships: an international pooled analysis of eight studies in the InterLACE consortium. Psychol Med. (2018) 48:2550–61. 10.1017/S0033291718000168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Avis NE, Colvin A, Bromberger JT, Hess R, Matthews KA, Ory M, et al. Change in health-related quality of life over the menopausal transition in a multiethnic cohort of middle-aged women: Study of Women's Health Across the Nation. Menopause. (2009) 16:860–9. 10.1097/gme.0b013e3181a3cdaf [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kravitz HM, Joffe H. Sleep during the perimenopause: a SWAN story. Obstetr Gynecol Clin North Am. (2011) 38:567–86. 10.1016/j.ogc.2011.06.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bromberger JT, Kravitz HM, Chang YF, Cyranowski JM, Brown C, Matthews KA. Major depression during and after the menopausal transition: Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN). Psychol Med. (2011) 41:1879–88. 10.1017/S003329171100016X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Rance NE, Dacks PA, Mittelman-Smith MA, Romanovsky AA, Krajewski-Hall SJ. Modulation of body temperature and LH secretion by hypothalamic KNDy (kisspeptin, neurokinin B and dynorphin) neurons: a novel hypothesis on the mechanism of hot flushes. Front Neuroendocrinol. (2013) 34:211–27. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.07.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sandoval-Guzman T, Stalcup ST, Krajewski SJ, Voytko ML, Rance NE. Effects of ovariectomy on the neuroendocrine axes regulating reproduction and energy balance in young cynomolgus macaques. J Neuroendocrinol. (2004) 16:146–53. 10.1111/j.0953-8194.2004.01143.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Prague JK, Roberts RE, Comninos AN, Clarke S, Jayasena CN, Nash Z, et al. Neurokinin 3 receptor antagonism as a novel treatment for menopausal hot flushes: a phase 2, randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Lancet. (2017) 389:1809–20. 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30823-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Freedman RR. Biochemical, metabolic, and vascular mechanisms in menopausal hot flashes. Fertil Steril. (1998) 70:332–7. 10.1016/S0015-0282(98)00137-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.de Zambotti M, Colrain IM, Sassoon SA, Nicholas CL, Trinder J, Baker FC. Vagal withdrawal during hot flashes occurring in undisturbed sleep. Menopause. (2013) 20:1147–53. 10.1097/GME.0b013e31828aa344 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Thurston RC, Matthews KA, Chang Y, Santoro N, Barinas-Mitchell E, von Kanel R, et al. Changes in heart rate variability during vasomotor symptoms among midlife women. Menopause. (2016) 23:499–505. 10.1097/GME.0000000000000586 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Freedman RR, Krell W. Reduced thermoregulatory null zone in postmenopausal women with hot flashes. Am J Obstet Gynecol. (1999) 181:66–70. 10.1016/S0002-9378(99)70437-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Fisher WI, Thurston RC. Hot flashes: phenomenology and measurement. In: Sievert LL, Brown DE, editors. Biological Measures of Human Experience Across the Lifespan: Making Visible the Invisible. Cham: Springer; (2016). p. 233-54. 10.1007/978-3-319-44103-0_11 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Erskine A, Morley S, Pearce S. Memory for pain: a review. Pain. (1990) 41:255–65. 10.1016/0304-3959(90)90002-U [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Stone AA, Broderick JE, Schwartz JE, Shiffman S, Litcher-Kelly L, Calvanese P. Intensive momentary reporting of pain with an electronic diary: reactivity, compliance, patient satisfaction. Pain. (2003) 104:343–51. 10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00040-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, Fireman P, Gwaltney JM, Jr, Newsom JT. State and trait negative affect as predictors of objective and subjective symptoms of respiratory viral infections. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1995) 68:159. 10.1037/0022-3514.68.1.159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Fu P, Matthews KA, Thurston RC. How well do different measurement modalities estimate the number of vasomotor symptoms? Findings from the study of women's health across the nation FLASHES study. Menopause. (2014) 21:124–30. 10.1097/GME.0b013e318295a3b9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Carpenter JS, Andrykowski MA, Freedman RR, Munn R. Feasibility and psychometrics of an ambulatory hot flash monitoring device. Menopause. (1999) 6:209–15. 10.1097/00042192-199906030-00006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Carpenter JS, Storniolo AM, Johns S, Monahan PO, Azzouz F, Elam JL, et al. Randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover trials of venlafaxine for hot flashes after breast cancer. Oncologist. (2007) 12:124–35. 10.1634/theoncologist.12-1-124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Walega DR, Rubin LH, Banuvar S, Shulman LP, Maki PM. Effects of stellate ganglion block on vasomotor symptoms: findings from a randomized controlled clinical trial in postmenopausal women. Menopause. (2014) 21:807–14. 10.1097/GME.0000000000000194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Thurston RC, Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA, Sherwood A. Emotional antecedents of hot flashes during daily life. Psychosom Med. (2005) 67:137–46. 10.1097/01.psy.0000149255.04806.07 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Thurston RC, Bromberger JT, Joffe H, Avis NE, Hess R, Crandall CJ, et al. Beyond frequency: who is most bothered by vasomotor symptoms? Menopause. (2008) 15:841–7. 10.1097/gme.0b013e318168f09b [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Thurston RC, Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA, Sherwood A. Association between hot flashes, sleep complaints, and psychological functioning among healthy menopausal women. Int J Behav Med. (2006) 13:163–72. 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1302_8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Gold EB, Sternfeld B, Kelsey JL, Brown C, Mouton C, Reame N, et al. Relation of demographic and lifestyle factors to symptoms in a multi-racial/ethnic population of women 40-55 years of age. Am J Epidemiol. (2000) 152:463–73. 10.1093/aje/152.5.463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Mitchell ES, Woods NF. Cognitive symptoms during the menopausal transition and early postmenopause. Climacteric. (2011) 14:252–61. 10.3109/13697137.2010.516848 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Weber M, Mapstone M. Memory complaints and memory performance in the menopausal transition. Menopause. (2009) 16:694–700. 10.1097/gme.0b013e318196a0c9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Weber MT, Mapstone M, Staskiewicz J, Maki PM. Reconciling subjective memory complaints with objective memory performance in the menopausal transition. Menopause. (2012) 19:735–41. 10.1097/gme.0b013e318241fd22 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Drogos LL, Rubin LH, Geller SE, Banuvar S, Shulman LP, Maki PM. Objective cognitive performance is related to subjective memory complaints in midlife women with moderate to severe vasomotor symptoms. Menopause. (2013) 20:1236–42. 10.1097/GME.0b013e318291f5a6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Greendale GA, Huang MH, Wight RG, Seeman T, Luetters C, Avis NE, et al. Effects of the menopause transition and hormone use on cognitive performance in midlife women. Neurology. (2009) 72:1850–7. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a71193 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Epperson CN, Sammel MD, Freeman EW. Menopause effects on verbal memory: findings from a longitudinal community cohort. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. (2013) 98:3829–38. 10.1210/jc.2013-1808 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Greendale GA, Wight RG, Huang MH, Avis N, Gold EB, Joffe H, et al. Menopause-associated symptoms and cognitive performance: results from the study of women's health across the nation. Am J Epidemiol. (2010) 171:1214–24. 10.1093/aje/kwq067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Fogel J, Rubin LH, Kilic E, Walega DR, Maki PM. Physiologic vasomotor symptoms are associated with verbal memory dysfunction in breast cancer survivors. Menopause. (2020). 10.1097/GME.0000000000001608. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Maki PM, Drogos LL, Rubin LH, Banuvar S, Shulman LP, Geller SE. Objective hot flashes are negatively related to verbal memory performance in midlife women. Menopause. (2008) 15:848–56. 10.1097/gme.0b013e31816d815e [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Maki PM, Rubin LH, Savarese A, Drogos L, Shulman LP, Banuvar S, et al. Stellate ganglion blockade and verbal memory in midlife women: evidence from a randomized trial. Maturitas. (2016) 92:123–9. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2016.07.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lipov E, Lipov S, Stark JT. Stellate ganglion blockade provides relief from menopausal hot flashes: a case report series. J Womens Health. (2005) 14:737–41. 10.1089/jwh.2005.14.737 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Lipov EG, Joshi JR, Sanders S, Wilcox K, Lipov S, Xie H, et al. Effects of stellate-ganglion block on hot flushes and night awakenings in survivors of breast cancer: a pilot study. Lancet Oncol. (2008) 9:523–32. 10.1016/S1470-2045(08)70131-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Haest K, Kumar A, Van Calster B, Leunen K, Smeets A, Amant F, et al. Stellate ganglion block for the management of hot flashes and sleep disturbances in breast cancer survivors: an uncontrolled experimental study with 24 weeks of follow-up. Ann Oncol. (2012) 23:1449–54. 10.1093/annonc/mdr478 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.van Gastel P, Kallewaard JW, van der Zanden M, de Boer H. Stellate-ganglion block as a treatment for severe postmenopausal flushing. Climacteric. (2013) 16:41–7. 10.3109/13697137.2012.709889 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Thurston RC, Aizenstein HJ, Derby CA, Sejdic E, Maki PM. Menopausal hot flashes and white matter hyperintensities. Menopause. (2016) 23:27–32. 10.1097/GME.0000000000000481 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Debette S, Schilling S, Duperron MG, Larsson SC, Markus HS. Clinical significance of magnetic resonance imaging markers of vascular brain injury: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Neurol. (2019) 76:81–94. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.3122 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Thurston RC, Maki PM, Derby CA, Sejdic E, Aizenstein HJ. Menopausal hot flashes and the default mode network. Fertil Steril. (2015) 103:1572–8.e1. 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2015.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Maki PM, Wu M, Rubin LH, Fornelli D, Drogos LL, Geller S, et al. Hot flashes are associated with altered brain function during a memory task. Menopause. (2020) 27:269–77. 10.1097/GME.0000000000001467 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2008) 1124:1–38. 10.1196/annals.1440.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Hillary FG, Roman CA, Venkatesan U, Rajtmajer SM, Bajo R, Castellanos ND. Hyperconnectivity is a fundamental response to neurological disruption. Neuropsychology. (2015) 29:59–75. 10.1037/neu0000110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Thurston RC. Vasomotor symptoms: natural history, physiology, and links with cardiovascular health. Climacteric. (2018) 21:96–100. 10.1080/13697137.2018.1430131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Thurston RC, Chang Y, Barinas-Mitchell E, Jennings JR, von Kanel R, Landsittel DP, et al. Physiologically assessed hot flashes and endothelial function among midlife women. Menopause. (2017) 24:886–93. 10.1097/GME.0000000000000857 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Thurston RC, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Everson-Rose SA, Hess R, Matthews KA. Hot flashes and subclinical cardiovascular disease: findings from the study of women's health across the nation heart study. Circulation. (2008) 118:1234–40. 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.108.776823 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Thurston RC, Chang Y, Barinas-Mitchell E, Jennings JR, Landsittel DP, Santoro N, et al. Menopausal hot flashes and carotid intima media thickness among midlife women. Stroke. (2016) 47:2910–5. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.116.014674 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Thurston RC, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Everson-Rose SA, Hess R, Powell LH, Matthews KA. Hot flashes and carotid intima media thickness among midlife women. Menopause. (2011) 18:352–8. 10.1097/gme.0b013e3181fa27fd [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Ozkaya E, Cakir E, Kara F, Okuyan E, Cakir C, Ustun G, et al. Impact of hot flashes and night sweats on carotid intima-media thickness and bone mineral density among postmenopausal women. Int J Gynaecol Obstet. (2011) 113:235–8. 10.1016/j.ijgo.2010.12.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Thurston RC, Johnson BD, Shufelt CL, Braunstein GD, Berga SL, Stanczyk FZ, et al. Menopausal symptoms and cardiovascular disease mortality in the Women's Ischemia Syndrome Evaluation (WISE). Menopause. (2017) 24:126–32. 10.1097/GME.0000000000000731 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Szmuilowicz ED, Manson JE, Rossouw JE, Howard BV, Margolis KL, Greep NC, et al. Vasomotor symptoms and cardiovascular events in postmenopausal women. Menopause. (2011) 18:603–10. 10.1097/gme.0b013e3182014849 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Zeki Al Hazzouri Vittinghoff E, Sidney S, Reis JP, Jacobs DR, Jr, et al. Intima-media thickness and cognitive function in stroke-free middle-aged adults: findings from the coronary artery risk development in young adults study. Stroke. (2015) 46:2190–6. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.115.008994 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Kivipelto M, Helkala EL, Laakso MP, Hanninen T, Hallikainen M, Alhainen K, et al. Midlife vascular risk factors and Alzheimer's disease in later life: longitudinal, population based study. BMJ. (2001) 322:1447–51. 10.1136/bmj.322.7300.1447 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Whitmer RA, Sidney S, Selby J, Johnston SC, Yaffe K. Midlife cardiovascular risk factors and risk of dementia in late life. Neurology. (2005) 64:277–81. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000149519.47454.F2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Romero JR, Beiser A, Seshadri S, Benjamin EJ, Polak JF, Vasan RS, et al. Carotid artery atherosclerosis, MRI indices of brain ischemia, aging, and cognitive impairment: the Framingham study. Stroke. (2009) 40:1590–6. 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.535245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Meldrum DR, Defazio JD, Erlik Y, Lu JK, Wolfsen AF, Carlson HE, et al. Pituitary hormones during the menopausal hot flash. Obstet Gynecol. (1984) 64:752–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Woods NF, Carr MC, Tao EY, Taylor HJ, Mitchell ES. Increased urinary cortisol levels during the menopausal transition. Menopause. (2006) 13:212–21. 10.1097/01.gme.0000198490.57242.2e [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Gibson CJ, Thurston RC, Matthews KA. Cortisol dysregulation is associated with daily diary-reported hot flashes among midlife women. Clin Endocrinol. (2016) 85:645–51. 10.1111/cen.13076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Greendale GA, Kritz-Silverstein D, Seeman T, Barrett-Connor E. Higher basal cortisol predicts verbal memory loss in postmenopausal women: Rancho Bernardo study. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2000) 48:1655–8. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.tb03878.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Seeman TE, McEwen BS, Singer BH, Albert MS, Rowe JW. Increase in urinary cortisol excretion and memory declines: MacArthur studies of successful aging. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. (1997) 82:2458–65. 10.1210/jc.82.8.2458 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Newcomer JW, Selke G, Melson AK, Hershey T, Craft S, Richards K, et al. Decreased memory performance in healthy humans induced by stress-level cortisol treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1999) 56:527–33. 10.1001/archpsyc.56.6.527 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.McCormick CM, Lewis E, Somley B, Kahan TA. Individual differences in cortisol levels and performance on a test of executive function in men and women. Physiol Behav. (2007) 91:87–94. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.01.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Lupien SJ, Gaudreau S, Tchiteya BM, Maheu F, Sharma S, Nair NP, et al. Stress-induced declarative memory impairment in healthy elderly subjects: relationship to cortisol reactivity. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. (1997) 82:2070–5. 10.1210/jc.82.7.2070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Lucas RA, Ganio MS, Pearson J, Crandall CG. Brain blood flow and cardiovascular responses to hot flashes in postmenopausal women. Menopause. (2013) 20:299–304. 10.1097/gme.0b013e31826e45f0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Kravitz HM, Zhao X, Bromberger JT, Gold EB, Hall MH, Matthews KA, et al. Sleep disturbance during the menopausal transition in a multi-ethnic community sample of women. Sleep. (2008) 31:979–90. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.de Zambotti M, Colrain IM, Javitz HS, Baker FC. Magnitude of the impact of hot flashes on sleep in perimenopausal women. Fertil Steril. (2014) 102:1708–15.e1. 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2014.08.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Thurston RC, Chang Y, Buysse DJ, Hall MH, Matthews KA. Hot flashes and awakenings among midlife women. Sleep. (2019) 42:zsz131. 10.1093/sleep/zsz131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Thurston RC, Wu M, Aizenstein HJ, Chang Y, Mitchell EB, Derby CA, et al. Sleep characteristics and white matter hyperintensities among midlife women. Sleep. (2019) 43:zsz298. 10.1093/sleep/zsz298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Scullin MK, Bliwise DL. Sleep, cognition, and normal aging: integrating a half century of multidisciplinary research. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2015) 10:97–137. 10.1177/1745691614556680 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Bubu OM, Brannick M, Mortimer J, Umasabor-Bubu O, Sebastiao YV, Wen Y, et al. Sleep cognitive impairment, and alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep. (2017) 40:zsw032. 10.1093/sleep/zsw032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Neurology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES