Skip to main content
Molecular Autism logoLink to Molecular Autism
. 2020 Oct 21;11:81. doi: 10.1186/s13229-020-00371-0

Face individual identity recognition: a potential endophenotype in autism

Ilaria Minio-Paluello 1,2,3,, Giuseppina Porciello 1,2, Alvaro Pascual-Leone 4,5,6, Simon Baron-Cohen 7
PMCID: PMC7576748  PMID: 33081830

Abstract

Background

Face individual identity recognition skill is heritable and independent of intellectual ability. Difficulties in face individual identity recognition are present in autistic individuals and their family members and are possibly linked to oxytocin polymorphisms in families with an autistic child. While it is reported that developmental prosopagnosia (i.e., impaired face identity recognition) occurs in 2–3% of the general population, no prosopagnosia prevalence estimate is available for autism. Furthermore, an autism within-group approach has not been reported towards characterizing impaired face memory and to investigate its possible links to social and communication difficulties.

Methods

The present study estimated the prevalence of prosopagnosia in 80 autistic adults with no intellectual disability, investigated its cognitive characteristics and links to autism symptoms’ severity, personality traits, and mental state understanding from the eye region by using standardized tests and questionnaires.

Results

More than one third of autistic participants showed prosopagnosia. Their face memory skill was not associated with their symptom’s severity, empathy, alexithymia, or general intelligence. Face identity recognition was instead linked to mental state recognition from the eye region only in autistic individuals who had prosopagnosia, and this relationship did not depend on participants’ basic face perception skills. Importantly, we found that autistic participants were not aware of their face memory skills.

Limitations

We did not test an epidemiological sample, and additional work is necessary to establish whether these results generalize to the entire autism spectrum.

Conclusions

Impaired face individual identity recognition meets the criteria to be a potential endophenotype in autism. In the future, testing for face memory could be used to stratify autistic individuals into genetically meaningful subgroups and be translatable to autism animal models.

Keywords: Autism, Individual identity recognition, Face memory, Prosopagnosia, Endophenotype, Heterogeneity, Social memory, Theory of mind, Emotion recognition

Background

Autism is a highly hereditable, lifelong, neurodevelopmental condition characterized by difficulties in social communication and interaction, alongside unusually restricted and repetitive behavior and interests, sensory hypersensitivity, and difficulties adjusting to unexpected change [1]. Autism occurs in at least 1% of the population [2] and is associated with high levels of poor mental health which could be reduced by a better and earlier intervention [3]. Clinical, etiological, and genetic heterogeneity in autism poses challenges to the discovery of causes and the development of effective interventions for autism [46]. Heterogeneity could be addressed via an endophenotype-based stratification approach which would accelerate the identification of genetic underpinnings and specific interventions [7, 8]. An endophenotype approach in autism is complicated by the fact that phenotypic expression in autism changes developmentally over time, depends on symptom severity, and varies in the presence of intellectual disability (ID) which commonly co-occurs with autism [9, 10]. Researchers have proposed interesting behavioral and neural endophenotypes in autism [8, 11], including language delay [12], gazing at social scenes [13], scores on the Social Responsiveness Scale [14], and white matter structure [15]. Many other proposed endophenotypes in autism do not meet the definition of endophenotype they adopt [16] as, for example, they lack evidence of heritability [1720]. Weaknesses of many endophenotypes proposed so far include the unknown prevalence in autism or relevance to a small subgroup of autistic individuals [15, 21, 22], reduced proximity to gene action (i.e., being under genetic influences of unknown or small effect sizes that are comparable to those of autism itself) [23], unknown or high genetic complexity [14, 24], lack of known or envisaged neurobiological bases [13], and limited translatability to animal models [12, 14, 23]. Such weaknesses are not specific to autism endophenotype literature but apply to endophenotypes of psychiatric conditions in general [2527]. Individual identity recognition (IIR) could be a potential new autism endophenotype devoid of most of the weaknesses described above.

The ability to recognize another individual is crucial for social interaction [28], emerges very early in development [29, 30], is conserved across species [28], and is linked to the oxytocin (OXY) system [31, 32]. Humans, like other primates, recognize other individuals mostly by their face [3335], which has evolved to signal individual identity [36]. Identity recognition is challenged by a face’s intrinsic (e.g., age, facial expressions/movements) and extrinsic (e.g., visual perspective, luminosity) identity-invariant changes and might benefit from plastic face representations [3739]. Humans show large individual differences in their face IIR ability [40], including 2–3% of individuals in the general population who report severe difficulties recognizing identity from faces in everyday life [41, 42]. This face-blindness condition, known as developmental prosopagnosia (DP), is not associated with brain damage or deficits in low-level vision, can run in families, is likely polygenic [43], and the OXY system seems to play a role in it [44, 45]. Although there are no formal diagnostic criteria for DP, it is generally agreed diagnostic assessment should primarily involve objective measures of face IIR. The Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT) [46] is considered the gold standard memory test for unfamiliar faces [4749] with a clinical cut-off score for prosopagnosia (see the “Methods” section).

While face perception in general, ranging from face detection to emotion recognition, has been extensively studied in autism [50, 51], face memory/IIR has been the focus of few well-controlled studies. In fact, studies that attempted to investigated face memory/IIR in autism often used tasks that suffered from (1) familiarity confounds (i.e., the task could be solved by recognizing whether an individual is seen before or novel) [5255], (2) no (or very short) retention time of the memory trace related to facial identity [5658], (3) face stimuli that include non-facial features (e.g., hair, clothes) [56, 59, 60] which allow correct performance even when facial internal features are covered [61], and (4) matching tasks with identical target and test face images, that therefore could be solved based on feature matching strategies [54, 62].

Overall, recent reviews of the available evidence have shown that, whenever memory is involved, autistic individuals as a group have reduced face identity recognition skills compared to matched neurotypical controls [51, 63] and that their face memory difficulties seem both process (e.g., memory vs. perception) and social-domain (e.g., faces and bodies vs. houses) specific [64]. To the best of our knowledge, no study has reported the prevalence of clinical impairment in face memory (i.e., prosopagnosia) in autism. In fact, those studies that used the CFMT, in many cases did not report group means and standard deviations but just case-control statistics [6569]. When they did report group means, however, they did not report the percentage of participants meeting the CFMT prosopagnosia cut-off [7074]. An exception is the study by Hedley and colleagues [75] who reported that 8 out of their 34 AUT adults (i.e., 24%) were prosopagnosic according to the CFMT.

No study has tested face memory in a larger group of autistic adults, that is, at an age when difficulties in face memory, if present, are most evident [70]. Further, no previous study has considered face memory difficulties in terms of autism within-group variability rather than mean case-control differences, addressing the heterogeneity in performance found in autism.

Face IIR has the potential to be an autism endophenotype because it is highly heritable [7678], independent of intellectual ability [78, 79], and difficulties in IIR are more common in family members of autistic individuals [59, 80, 81]. Future autism research could benefit from the fact that IIR is measurable early in development [30, 82] and in individuals with ID [83], may be linked to OXY polymorphisms in families with an autistic child [60] (for different findings in neurotypical participants, see [84]), and is translatable to mice [85, 86].

Here, we estimated the prevalence of prosopagnosia in autism, characterized it with respect to related face perceptual processes, and investigated its links to autism symptom severity, personality traits, and difficulties in mental state understanding from the eye region.

Methods

Participants

We tested 80 autistic (AUT) adults (16 females, mean age = 31.3 ± 11.9 years, age range = 18–73 years) with no ID (IQ- percentile = 78 ± 29, range = 4–100, N = 74 of which Raven’s SPM mean percentile = 91 ± 15, range = 38–100, N = 31; Wechsler scales mean full-scale IQ percentile = 69 ± 32, range = 4–100, N = 43), and 80 neurotypical controls (NT) (16 females, mean age = 28.8 ± 9.1 years, age range = 18–56 years) with no ID (IQ percentile = 92 ± 13, range = 21–100, N = 69, of which Raven’s SPM mean percentile = 93 ± 14, range = 21–100, N = 47; Wechsler scales mean full-scale IQ percentile = 89 ± 12, range = 50–100, N = 22) matched for age (t test with separate variance estimates, t(147.7) = 1.47, p = 0.14, 95% CI [− 5.75, 0.85]; Levene’s test F(1,158) = 4.43, p = 0.04), sex, and country of residency (UK, USA, Italy). Although we did not have IQ measures for 6 AUT and 11 NT participants, given that they all completed high school and in some cases were enrolled in college-level education, we could assume they too did not have ID.

Autism diagnosis was made by a professional expert in autism according to DSM-IV criteria and confirmed via the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) [87] and the Autism Diagnostic Interview Revised (ADI-R) [88] by a certified clinician (Table 1). Intellectual ability was assessed via either the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence [89], the Wechsler Adult Scale of Intelligence IV [90], or the Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices [91] (Table 1). NT participants did not have a neurological or psychiatric condition, autistic first-degree relatives, or an autism spectrum quotient questionnaire score above the autism cut-off [92]. Participants were recruited because they took part in other experimental studies [9395]; therefore, they were not preselected based on their interest or skills in identity recognition. The study was approved by local Institutional Review Boards, adhered to the declaration of Helsinki, and all participants gave informed consent before participation.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the autistic sample

ADOS CSS ADOS
SA + RRB
ADOS
Comm + SocInt
ADI Comm ADI
Soc Int
ADI
RRB
ADI
Abn Dev
IQ percentile AQ
Cut-off 8 7 8 10 3 3 31
AUT

7 ± 3

N = 67

13 ± 5

N = 67

10 ± 4

N = 69

16 ± 5

N = 55

19 ± 6

N = 55

6 ± 3

N = 55

2 ± 1

N = 55

78 ± 29

N = 74

31 ± 9

N = 78

ADOS Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, CSS Calibrated Severity Score, SA Social Affective, RRB Restricted Repetitive Behavior, Comm Communication, Soc Int Social Interaction, ADI Autism Diagnostic Interview, Abn Dev abnormal development, IQ intelligence quotient, AQ autism quotient

Procedure

Participants performed computer-based versions of a standardized battery of tests and questionnaires either onsite or online (6% of NT and 14% of AUT participants completed between one and three questionnaires/tests online). All participants completed the Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT) [46] upright, and almost all autistic participants completed the other tests and questionnaires (sample size is reported for each test).

Tests

The Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT) [46] upright and inverted

The CFMT is a computer-based test that uses a three-alternative forced-choice paradigm with the unlimited response time. Participants have to memorize and recognize 6 individuals. The test is divided into 3 parts of increasing difficulty. In the first part (18 trials), participants view each individual from three different angles and have to recognize him in three consecutive trials (correct images are identical to the studied ones). In the second part (30 trials), the to-be-recognized individual can be any of the 6 previously learned ones, now presented with different angles and/or lighting. The third part (24 trials) has the same structure as the second one but Gaussian visual noise is added to the images in order to make participants rely more on holistic (vs. feature-based) face processing [96]. A number of correct answers at or below 42 (out of 72), which corresponds to 2 standard deviations from the mean, are indicative of prosopagnosia, while chance level corresponds to 24 correct responses [46, 47]. The CFMT has well-controlled stimuli (e.g., including only facial features, the same individual is presented with identity invariant changes), is unidimensional, highly reliable, has high discriminant and convergent validity, is precise over a wide range of ability levels [47, 97], and can be reliably administered online [98, 99]. Performance on the CFMT is heritable [100] and has little or no correlation with general intelligence [78, 79]. The AUT group also took the CFMT with face stimuli presented upside down [47, 101]. AUT participants took the upright and the inverted CFMT tests in counterbalanced order, one at the beginning and one at the end of the experiment, to reduce carry-over effects. Better performance for upright vs. inverted faces is known as the face inversion effect, which is interpreted in favor of typical holistic (vs. feature-based) processing of faces [102]. In order to account for age-dependent decline in performance, we used Bowles and colleagues [47] second-order polynomial fit in conjunction with the standard deviations of the residuals of that fit to calculate age-standardized prosopagnosia cut-off scores for participants over 49 years of age.

The Cambridge Face Perception Test (CFPT) [103]

The CFPT is a computer-based test where each of its 16 trials involves limited time (i.e., 40 s in the version used here) to sort 6 front-facing faces according to their similarity to a target face presented with a ¾ profile. Each of the 6 faces was created by morphing a different individual with the target face by varying degrees (28%, 40%, 52%, 64%, 76%, and 88%). In half of the trials, faces are presented upright, while in the other half of the trials, faces are presented inverted. For each trial, the order in which the participant arranged the morphed faces is scored by summing the deviations of each morphed face from its correct position. The CFPT error score corresponds to the sum of scores of upright trials. The higher the error score, the worse the participants’ performance. Perfect performance corresponds to an error score of 0, while chance performance corresponds to an error score of 93.3. The CFPT is considered a measure of face perception skills with no memory demand, as the target and the morphed faces are visible during the task and, therefore, do not need to be memorized. Not all individuals with DP perform worse than controls on the CFPT [47, 104, 105], just those with apperceptive prosopagnosia.

The Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) [106]

The RMET is a test where participants have to recognize mental states (including complex emotions) from 36 photographs of the eye region of individuals varying in sex and age. The RMET has good reliability [107], autistic individuals consistently perform less well than matched neurotypical controls [108112] and do not show the neurotypical advantage of female vs. male participants [108]. A large online study of the RMET in over 80.000 individuals confirmed the neurotypical sex difference (female advantage) and identified a single nucleotide polymorphism associated with performance [113]. Individuals with DP perform similarly to controls on the RMET [103, 114116].

Questionnaires

The Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) [92]

The AQ is a 50-item self-report questionnaire measuring the number of autistic traits across five domains: communication, social skills, attention switching, imagination, and attention to detail. The respondent rates how strongly they agree or disagree with each statement, using a four-point scale. Total score ranges from 0 to 50 and scores above 31 are indicative of autism. Scores between 23 and 28 are considered Broad Autism Phenotype (BAP), between 29 and 34 Medium Autism Phenotype (MAP), and above 34 Narrow Autism Phenotype (NAP) [117]. A large online study of the AQ confirmed the sex difference (typical males score higher on average than typical females) and the STEM effect (those working in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Math score higher on average than those who do not) in half a million people [118]. A recent, even larger online study of over 600,000 people from the general population and 36,000 autistic people, confirmed the case-control difference, the sex difference, and the STEM effect [119], using a short form of the AQ.

The twenty-item Prosopagnosia Index (PI20) [120]

The PI20 is a 20-items self-report questionnaire proposed by its authors as a measure of prosopagnosic traits [121]. Respondents indicate on a five-point Likert scale how much they agree with statements describing their face identity recognition abilities and experience in everyday life. Total score ranges from 20 to 100 and a score over 64 is considered indicative of prosopagnosia [120]. The PI20 score correlates with performance on the CFMT in the general population and in DP [120122] and distinguishes DPs from controls [120, 123, 124]. The need for DP diagnostic assessment to include a self-report questionnaire assessing awareness of everyday difficulties in face memory is debated [48, 49, 125].

The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) [126]

The IRI is a 28-item self-report questionnaire with four subscales each said to be measuring an independent empathy component. Subscales include perspective taking, which measures the ability to adopt another person’s view point; empathic concern, which measures the tendency to respond with warm, compassionate feelings for others; fantasy, which measures the tendency to identify with fictional characters; and personal distress, which measures a self-oriented negative arousal/discomfort response to another person’s distress/negative experience. Participants indicate on a five-point Likert scale how much they agree with each statement.

The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS) [127]

The TAS is a 20-item self-report questionnaire with three subscales each tapping a component of alexithymia: difficulty identifying feelings, difficulty describing feelings, and externally oriented thinking. Total scores range between 20 and 100, with higher scores indicating more alexithymic traits. Sixty-one is the cut-off score for high alexithymia [128]. The TAS has good internal consistency and good test-retest reliability [127]. Alexithymia seems highly prevalent in people on the autistic spectrum compared to the general population, and it seems to play a role in autism emotion recognition skills and empathic response [129].

Statistical analysis

Before running each statistical test, we checked whether its assumptions were met. Before running Student’s t tests, we checked for homogeneity of variances via Levene’s test and when significant we considered separate variances estimates. Before running moderation analysis, we checked for multicollinearity (via variance inflation factor and tolerance), independence of residuals (via Durbin–Watson statistic), linearity and homoscedasticity (via visual inspection of the standardized residual vs. standardized values scatter plot), and homogeneity of variances (via Levene’s test). We considered a dependent variable to be normally distributed if Shapiro-Wilks test was not significant (p > 0.05) or if its absolute SME-standardized Z-Skewness and Z-Kurtosis were considered normal (i.e., Z-Skewness and Z-Kurtosis < 1.96 at p < 0.05) [130]. In case of normal distribution, we reported parametric tests; otherwise, non-parametric tests were used. Specifically, when data distribution was not normal and/or sample sizes were different, we used robust statistics [131]. For each statistical test, we reported relevant statistical indices including the distribution’s parameters and their degrees of freedom (df), the sample size used (N), mean or median, standard deviation (SD), probability (p), confidence intervals (CI), and effect size. We did not randomize the recruitment selection of autistic (nor matched NT) participants as recruitment of autistic participants was not easy, and our main goal was to have the largest possible sample size.

We did not find outliers in correlation and regression analyses as no participant met at least two between Mahalanobis distance, Cook’s distance and Leverage cut-offs [130]. Statistical analysis was run via SPSS (IBM) and PROCESS plugin [132], STATISTICA (StatSoft, Inc. 2007), and R (R Development Core Team 2013, packages WRS2, RVAideMemoire, ggplot2).

Results

Intellectual ability and autistic traits in autistic and neurotypical participants

Comparison of available autistic (74 out of 80) and neurotypical (69 out of 80) IQ percentile scores via robust Yuen’s test (WRS2, R-package) with default trimmed value of 0.2 provided Ty(54.53) = 2.42, p = 0.02, trimmed mean difference = 7.74, 95% CI [1.33, 14.15], and explanatory measure of effect size = 0.45, 95% CI [0.18, 0.71]. Therefore, based on the available data, NT (92 ± 13) had higher IQ percentiles than AUT (78 ± 29) participants.

Results of the AQ showed that 41% of AUT participants scored within the narrow autism phenotype range (vs. 0% of NT), 27% within the medium autism phenotype range (vs. 1% of NT), 13% within the broad autism phenotype range (vs. 8% of NT) and 19% outside the broad autism phenotype (vs. 91% of NT) [117]. As expected, AUT (31.01 ± 9.02, N = 78) had higher AQ scores than NT (14.86 ± 6.01, N = 78) participants, t test with separate variance estimate t (134) = 13.17, p = 6.20e-26, 95% CI [13.73, 18.58]; Levene’s test F(1,154) = 9.21, p = 0.003.

Prosopagnosia is more common in autism than in controls

We found that prosopagnosia was more common in autism: 36% of AUT met the clinical cut-off for prosopagnosia, while this was the case for only 6% of NT. Autism diagnosis was significantly associated with prosopagnosia χ2(1) = 21.51, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.18, 0.42], that is, based on the odds ratio, the odds of being prosopagnosic were 8.5 times higher for AUT individuals than for NT (Fig. 1). Please see Table 2 for groups’ performance scores on the CFMT and note from Fig. 1 that all groups showed some degree of variation in the number of correct responses on the CFMT. As evident in every figure, although one participant scored below the CFMT cut-off (i.e., 42), they were assigned to the AUT-NP group as they did not meet their age-standardized prosopagnosia cut-off.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The prevalence of prosopagnosia in autism. Performance (number of correct responses) at the Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT) for 160 participants plotted as a function of their diagnostic group (autistic-AUT vs. neurotypical-NT) and prosopagnosia (prosopagnosic-P vs. non-prosopagnosic-NP). Thicker horizontal lines represent the medians, boxes the interquartile ranges, and whiskers the maximum and minimum values. The solid horizontal black line is the CFMT clinical cut-off for prosopagnosia (i.e., 42). AUT-P: dark blue, AUT-NP: light blue, NT-P: dark red, NT-NP: light red boxes. Black dots represent individual data points

Table 2.

Group performance at the Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT) of prosopagnosic (P) and non-prosopagnosic (NP) autistic (AUT) and neurotypical (NT) participants

CFMT (prosopagnosia cut-off = 42) N mean number of correct responses (over a total of 72) SD Range
NT (NT-NP + NT-P) 80

56.20

95% CI [54.03, 58.37]

9.76

95% CI [8.45, 11.56]

25–72
NT-NP 75

57.45

95% CI [55.48, 59.42]

8.57

95% CI [7.38, 10.21]

43–72
NT-P 5

37.40

95% CI [28.42, 46.38]

7.23

95% CI [4.33, 20.78]

25–42
AUT (AUT-NP + AUT-P) 80

46.85

95% CI [44.36, 49.34]

11.20

95% CI [9.69, 13.26]

18–72
AUT-NP* 51

53.64

95% CI [51.49, 55.79]

7.55

95% CI [6.31, 9.41]

43–72
AUT-P 29

35.52

95% CI [33.28, 37.75]

5.87

95% CI [4.66, 7.94]

18–42

NT neurotypical participants, AUT autistic participants, P prosopagnosic, NP non-prosopagnosic, CFMT Cambridge Face Memory Test, SD standard deviation, CI confidence interval

*AUT-NP mean, SD, and range do not include a participant who scored 36 and was assigned to the AUT-NP group as they did not meet their age-standardized prosopagnosia cut-off

Clinical and personality trait measures do not distinguish between autistic individuals with and without prosopagnosia

Autistic prosopagnosic (AUT-P) and autistic non-prosopagnosic (AUT-NP) individuals did not differ in their diagnostic symptom severity, assessed via the ADOS Calibrated Severity Score (CSS) [133] and the ADI-R total score (i.e., Communication, Social Interaction, Restricted Repetitive Behaviors, and Developmental Abnormalities sum of scores), number of autistic traits assessed via the AQ questionnaire [92], level of general intelligence assessed via Raven’s progressive matrices or Wechsler adult intelligence scales, mental state recognition from the eye region assessed via the RMET [106], perspective taking (PT) and empathic concern (EC) assessed via the IRI [126], alexithymia assessed via the TAS [127], and self-report prosopagnosia assessed via the PI20 questionnaire [120] (see Table 3).

Table 3.

Comparisons between autistic participants with (AUT-P) and without (AUT-NP) prosopagnosia

Group N Mean SD SE t (df)/Ty(df) p 95% CI Cohen’s d/Yuen’s effect size
ADOS CSS AUT-NP 43 6.40 2.55 0.39
AUT-P 24 7.08 2.60 0.53 − 1.05(65) 0.30 − 1.99,0.62 − 0.27
ADI TOT AUT-NP 38 43.16 11.28 1.83
AUT-P 17 45.77 12.95 3.14 − 0.76 (53) 0.45 − 9.52, 4.31 − 0.22
AQ AUT-NP 51 30.86 9.53 1.33
AUT-P 27 31.30 8.12 1.56 0.13 (39) 0.89 − 3.54, 4.04 0.07
IQ AUT-NP 49 80.56 26.86 3.84
AUT-P 25 73.68 31.73 6.35 0.87 (17.94) 0.35 − 8.52, 23.09 0.21
RMET AUT-NP 50 0.65 0.16 0.02
AUT-P 26 0.60 0.19 0.04 0.95 (22.3) 0.35 − 0.07, 0.17 0.18
PI20 AUT-NP 42 55.00 15.51 2.39
AUT-P 21 56.57 16.07 3.51

0.40

(23.59)

0.69 − 12.11, 819 0.1
PT AUT-NP 50 13.22 6.07 0.86
AUT-P 26 12.46 5.57 1.09

0.69

(74)

0.49 − 1.84, 3.79 0.17
EC AUT-NP 50 16.08 5.30 0.75
AUT-P 26 16.27 6.27 1.23 − 0.004 (74) 1.00 − 2.70, 2.69 − 9.37e−4
TAS AUT-NP 44 50.32 10.60 1.60
AUT-P 23 59.09 10.99 2.29

1.85

(65)

0.07 − 0.41, 10.57 − 0.44

Cohen’s d effect size is interpreted as 0.2 (small), 0.5 (medium), 0.8 (large); (*) Yuen’s effect size is interpreted as 0.10 (small), 0.30 (medium), and 0.50 (large)

t = Student t, (*) Ty = Yuen’s T, SD standard deviation, SE standard error, df degrees of freedom, CI confidence interval, AUT-P autistic prosopagnosic, AUT-NP autistic non-prosopagnosic, ADOS Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, ADI TOT Autism Diagnostic Interview Total score (i.e., Communication + Social Interaction + Restricted Repetitive Behaviour + Developmental Abnormalities), AQ* autism quotient, IQ* Intelligence Quotient, RMET* Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test, PI20 Twenty-item Prosopagnosia Index, PT Perspective Taking, EC Empathic Concern, TAS Toronto Alexithymia Scale

Face individual identity recognition is linked to mental state recognition only in autistic individuals with prosopagnosia

We completed a moderation analysis to investigate the role of face IIR over mental state recognition in the two groups of prosopagnosic and non-prosopagnosic AUT participants. Moderation analysis, R2 = 0.16, F (3, 72) = 4.48, p = 0.006, revealed that the interaction between identity recognition, assessed via the CFMT, and whether AUT participants were prosopagnosic or non prosopagnosic, predicted participants’ ability to infer another person's mental states by looking at their eye region, assessed via the RMET, b = 0.02, SE = 0.006, t = 3.00, p = 0.004, 95% CI [0.006, 0.030], N = 76 (Fig. 2). The increase in R2 due to the interaction was 0.10, F(1,72) = 8.86, p = 0.004. While in AUT-P, face memory skill on the CFMT predicted their ability to understand another person’s mental states on the RMET (b = 0.02, SE = 0.005, t = 3.45, p = 0.001, 95% CI [0.008, 0.029], N = 26), this was not the case for AUT-NP (b = 0.0004, SE = 0.003, t = 0.15, p = 0.88, 95% CI [− 0.005, 0.006], N = 50).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Prosopagnosia moderates the influence of identity recognition on mental state understanding. In autistic participants who are prosopagnosic (AUT-P, dark blue circles), face memory skill at the CFMT predicted their ability to understand another person’s mental states at the RMET, while this was not the case for autistic participants who are not prosopagnosic (AUT-NP, light blue circles). The dark blue solid line represents a significant regression line for the AUT-P group, while the light blue line represents nonsignificant regression line for the AUT-NP group. The black solid line represents CFMT cut-off score (i.e., 42)

The relation between identity and mental state recognition does not depend on individuals’ basic face perception skills

We ran a moderated moderation model in order to investigate the role of face perception, assessed via the CFPT (see Table 4 for performance scores), on the interaction between identity recognition and prosopagnosia over participants’ ability to recognize another person’s mental state from the eye region. The CFPT was not normally distributed (significant Shapiro-Wilks test) due to a positive skew, which was resolved by square root transformation. None of the main effects nor interactions were significant. In particular, the three-way interaction between identity recognition × face perception × prosopagnosia was not significant, b = − 0.003, t (54) = − 0.41, p = 0.68, 95% CI [− 0.02, 0.01], N = 62.

Table 4.

Group performance at the CFPT

N Error score SD Range
CFPT upright AUT 62

51.97

95% CI [45.61, 58.32]

25.02

95% CI [21.26, 30.41]

18–116
AUT-NP 41

44.44

95% CI [37.19, 51.69]

22.97

95% CI [18.86, 29.39]

20–96
AUT-P 21

66.67

95% CI [56.37, 76.96]

22.61

95% CI [17.30, 32.65]

18–116
CFPT inverted AUT 62

73.94

95% CI [69.97, 77.90]

15.61

95% CI [13.27, 18.98]

36–102
AUT-NP 41

71.61

95% CI [66.41, 76.81]

16.48

95% CI [13.53, 21.08]

36–100
AUT-P 21

78.48

95% CI [72.58, 84.38]

12.96

95% CI [9.82, 18.72]

50–102
CFPT inversion effect (Inv-Up)
AUT-NP 41

27.17

95% CI [21.88, 32.47]

16.77 − 10–58
AUT-P 21

11.81

95% CI [2.99, 20.62]

19.37 − 32–44
Controls from Bowles et al. 2009 118 26.43 14.41

CFPT Cambridge Face Perception Test, AUT autistic participants, P prosopagnosic, NP nonprosopagnosic, SD standard deviation, CI confidence interval

Autistic individuals with prosopagnosia do not show face memory inversion effect and have no general memory difficulties

To further investigate differences in face processing and memory skills, we compared AUT-P and AUT-NP performance on the CFMT with upright and inverted faces (Fig. 3). Type III mixed ANOVA revealed significant main effects of prosopagnosia, F(1,62) = 54.30, p = 4.94e-10, ηp2 = 0.47, and face orientation, F(1,62) = 98.82, p = 1.88e-14, ηp2 = 0.61, which were explained by a significant prosopagnosia × face orientation interaction, F(1,62) = 41.01, p = 2.29e-08; ηp2 = 0.40. Tukey HSD post hoc tests evidenced that while AUT-NP (N = 42) showed a face inversion effect, AUT-NP upright = 53.14 ± 7.90, AUT-NP inverted = 35.53 ± 5.55, p = 0.0002, AUT-P (N = 21) did not perform better on upright (35.14 ± 6.28) than inverted faces (31.33 ± 8.06, p = 0.15). AUT-P performed worse than AUT-NP on upright faces (p = 0.0001), but they did not differ on inverted faces (p = 0.11). All other post hoc comparisons were not significant (ps > 0.14). To test whether group performance differed from the chance level of responding, for each condition, we ran one-sample t tests against the chance level and found that both groups performed differently from the chance level in all conditions: upright CFMT, AUT-P: t (20) = 8.13, p = 9.04e-8, 95% CI [32.28, 38.00]; AUT-NP: t(42) = 24.20, p = 2.76e-26, 95% CI [50.71, 55.57] and inverted CFMT, AUT-P: t(20) = 4.17, p = 4.72e-4, 95% CI [27.67, 35.00]; AUT-NP: t(42) = 13.63, p = 5.08e-17, 95% CI [33.83, 37.24]. The latter result, together with the fact that only 3 AUT-P and 1 AUT-NP participants performed below the chance level on the inverted CFMT and only 1 AUT-P on the upright CFMT (Fig. 3), excluded the presence of a floor effect in performance.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Autistic prosopagnosics do not show face inversion effect and have no general memory impairment. Non-prosopagnosic autistic participants (AUT-NP, N = 42, light blue dots) showed a face inversion effect, that is they performed better on upright (gray bars) vs. inverted (white bars) faces, while prosopagnosic autistic participants (AUT-P, N = 22, dark blue dots) did not. AUT-P did not show a general memory impairment, that is, although they performed worse than AUT-NP on upright faces, they did not differ from AUT-NP on inverted faces. The red asterisk indicates that AUT-NP performance at the upright CFMT differs from all other conditions (ps < 0.05). The dashed horizontal line indicates the CFMT chance level which corresponds to 24 correct responses

AUT participants completed both the CFMT upright and inverted (in counterbalanced order) while NT participants completed only the CFMT Upright. Results of a 2 × 2 mixed ANOVA showed a non-significant main effect of order, F(1,62) = 0.18, p = 0.67, ηp2 = 0.003, and a significant main effect of orientation, F(1,62) = 102.5, p = 9.21e-15, ηp2 = 0.62, with a more accurate performance for upright compared to inverted faces. Notably, the order × orientation interaction was non-significant, F(1,62) = 1.62, p = 0.29, ηp2 = 0.02, assuring that the order in which AUT participants completed the upright or inverted CFMT did not influence their performance at the upright CFMT.

Autistic individuals with and without prosopagnosia show face perception inversion effect

To extend our investigation about differences in identity processing between AUT-P and AUT-NP to their perceptual abilities, we compared AUT-P and AUT-NP performance on the CFPT with upright and inverted faces. Type III mixed ANOVA on the CFPT error scores revealed significant main effects of prosopagnosia, F(1,60) = 9.75, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.14, and face orientation, F(1,60) = 67.49, p = 2.11e−11, ηp2 = 0.53, which were explained by a significant prosopagnosia × face orientation interaction, F(1,60) = 10.48, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.15. Tukey HSD post hoc tests showed that both groups performed better with upright vs. inverted faces (AUT-NP: p = 0.0002; AUT-P: p = 0.02) and that only in the case of upright faces, AUT-P performed worse than AUT-NP (upright: p = 0.0004; inverted: p = 0.56). Finally, AUT-P showed a smaller perceptual face inversion effect than AUT-NP, t(60) = − 3.24, p = 0.004, 95% CI [− 24.85, − 5.87] (see Table 4).

Face individual identity recognition is not linked to general intelligence

Level of intelligence (IQ percentile) did not significantly correlate with face memory (number of correct responses on the CFMT) both in NT (Spearman ρ = 0.11, p = 0.36, 95% CI [− 0.12, 0.34], N = 69) and AUT (Spearman ρ = 0.20, p = 0.09, 95% CI [− 0.06, 0.42], N = 74) participants (Fig. 4a).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

a General intelligence does not correlate with face memory skill in autism. In autistic participants, IQ level (percentile) does not significantly correlate with face memory skill (number of correct responses) at the Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT). Light blue dots represent nonprosopagnosic autistic participants (AUT-NP) while the dark blue dots represent prosopagnosic autistic participants (AUT-P). The dashed black line represents the nonsignificant regression line, while the surrounding gray-shaded area represents a 95% confidence interval. b Subjective face memory awareness does not predict objective face memory performance in autistic individuals. In autistic participants self-reported prosopagnosia traits at the prosopagnosia index questionnaires (PI20) do not correlate with their objective performance at the Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT). The light blue dots represent non-prosopagnosic autistic participants (AUT-NP) while the dark blue dots represent prosopagnosic autistic participants (AUT-P). The dashed black line represents the nonsignificant regression line, while the surrounding gray-shaded area represents a 95% confidence interval

Subjective face memory awareness does not predict objective face memory performance in autistic individuals

Regression analysis showed that AUT participants self-reported prosopagnosic traits (assessed via the PI20 questionnaire) did not predict their objective face memory/individual identity recognition skills (assessed via the CFMT), b = − 0.16, t(61) = − 1.29, p = 0.20, R2 = 0.03, F(1,61) = 1.66, p = 0.20, N = 63 (Fig. 4b).

Discussion

Our results show that prosopagnosia potentially occurs in more than one third (i.e., 36%) of autistic adults without ID. Importantly, our prosopagnosia-based stratification into two groups, AUT-P and AUT-NP, was independent of symptom severity, autistic traits, IQ, general memory skills, empathy, and alexithymia. This is in keeping with our idea that face memory difficulties do not interact with autism at a necessary and sufficient aetiological level, an idea that is also supported by the fact that DP individuals do not have difficulties in social skills [134], nor, to the best of our knowledge, they are known to have higher rates of autism. We speculate that difficulties in face IIR may therefore not lie on the causal pathway from genes to autism. In keeping with the polygenic nature of autism [135] and probably of DP [43], difficulties in IIR may rather contribute to autism’s genetic background liability [136] in a manner resembling a multivariate correlated liability model [137] including multiple genes, endophenotypes, and environmental factors. In such a schematic model, the association between genetic risk factors of autism and those of prosopagnosia could vary from reflecting mere spatial proximity of the implicated alleles, which therefore remain linked over generations, to functional impact on the same neural circuits (e.g., the OXY system). Face IIR might be valuable to help focus research on new genetically meaningful autistic subgroups, neurobiological pathways, and neural systems. Its role may be similar to what proposed by Constantino [138] for other symptoms which are not specific to autism but which are highly prevalent in autism and are strongly genetically influenced.

We investigated the relationship between face memory and mental state recognition from the eye region as both are impaired in autism [51, 108] and rely on extracting information from the same (eye) region [139] which has strong diagnostic [87, 88] and predictive [140, 141] value to autism. Our data show that identity recognition was associated with the ability to recognize another person’s mental state by looking at their eyes, both essential skills to navigate the social world. In particular, face identity recognition was linked to mental states understanding exclusively in autistic participants who were prosopagnosic. Such association was unlikely due to reduced expertise with faces, as non-autistic individuals with DP are not impaired in mental state recognition from the eye region [103, 114116]. Alternatively, reduced attention to the eyes may affect both identity [139, 142144] and mental state recognition and may be linked to altered OXY-mediated social processing of identity sensory cues [31, 45]. The chances of a common neurobiological mechanism underlying both face identity and mental state recognition in AUT-P increase the potential relevance of IIR as an endophenotype, as it stratifies autistic individuals in a way that is meaningful to distinctive difficulties in social interaction. The differential relationship we found in AUT-P and AUT-NP between face IIR and mental state recognition from the eye region supports the idea that AUT-P and AUT-NP might be two separate subgroups, and not just that AUT-NP were autistic individuals with poor identity recognition skills [125]. We included performance on the Cambridge Face Perception Test as an additional moderator to investigate whether difficulties in face identity and emotion recognition in AUT-P were both due to perceptual failure in face processing. We found that the relationship between identity and mental state recognition was not moderated by altered basic face perception skills that impair face recognition even when face memorization is not needed (i.e., apperceptive prosopagnosia).

To further investigate between-group differences in face processing and general memory skills, we tested autistic participants’ memory for inverted faces, which, unlike from upright faces, are typically not processed holistically [145]. In their review, Weigelt and colleagues [63] reported that only 2 out of 14 case-control studies showed no face inversion effect in autistic participants, and so, the majority of studies suggest better face recognition for upright vs. inverted faces [102] also in autistic individuals. However, none of these studies controlled for prosopagnosia. Our results show that while AUT-NP remembered faces better when presented upright vs. inverted, AUT-P did not show such face inversion effect. This suggests that upright faces may not be a special class of stimuli to remember for AUT-P and that presence of prosopagnosia, not autism, drove the lack of face memory inversion effect as both groups had an autism diagnosis. Interestingly instead, both AUT-NP and AUT-P showed face inversion effect when their face perception skills were assessed with no memory demands (i.e., on the CFPT), again suggesting that retention of facial identity information is crucial in differentiating AUT-P from AUT-NP. Nonetheless, interpretation is not so straight forward as results on the presence of face inversion effects in DP are mixed, evidencing holistic face processing is not always impaired in DP [43, 101]. The absence of face memory inversion effect and a positive correlation between face memory and mental state understanding are instances in which AUT-P differed from DP. It remains an open question whether prosopagnosia in AUT shares similar characteristics and neurobiological correlates with prosopagnosia in non-autistic, DP individuals. In addition, we found that, while AUT-P performed worse than AUT-NP with upright faces (at both perceptual and memory levels), they did not differ with inverted faces, suggesting our results were not due to unspecific memory impairments.

As IQ varies greatly along the autistic spectrum [146], we checked whether IQ correlated with IIR and found that this was not the case both for our AUT and NT participants, a result similar to that found in the general population [78, 79]. Face memory may therefore be potentially relevant to the entire autistic spectrum, across all levels of intellectual ability. It remains to be tested whether the independence between face memory skills and IQ holds true also with individuals with ID.

Lastly, we explored autistic participants’ awareness of their face IIR skills and whether a self-report questionnaire, here the PI20 questionnaire, could be used for screening autistic individuals with difficulties in face memory. Contrary to the general population and to DP individuals [120122], subjective scores of autistic participants on the PI20 did not predict their objective performance on the CFMT. Therefore, a self-report questionnaire does not seem a reliable prosopagnosia screening tool for autism, which is at odds with previous findings showing that adult autistic participants had similar face memory awareness compared to neurotypical individuals [147].

Limitations

This was not an epidemiological sample and therefore may not be representative of the prevalence of prosopagnosia in autism. However, it is the largest sample available to date and it was not biased with respect to face processing skills, so it may indeed reflect the true prevalence of prosopagnosia in autistic adults with no ID. We do not know whether our results generalize to the entire autism spectrum. Future work is needed to determine whether prosopagnosia is equally prevalent and similarly associated with mental state recognition skills also in other autistic individuals not represented in the current sample, such as in children, individuals with ID and females (our sample included only 20% of females).

Our proposal that prosopagnosia might be a potential endophenotype in autism would benefit from additional findings supporting co-segregation of autism and prosopagnosia within families and higher rate of prosopagnosia in non-autistic family members compared to the general population.

Future perspectives

Autism research benefits from parallel human and animal studies and our results, taken in the context of the current literature, open various avenues of research. Future human studies could investigate whether face memory difficulties can subgroup autism high-risk infants in prospective meaningful ways. Given that intranasal OXY (INOXY) was shown to normalize identity recognition in DP [44], correctly increase familiar judgments of previously seen faces in controls [148], and improve eye contact in autistic individuals [149], researchers could investigate its effects on face memory in autism. Researchers could also investigate, as a downstream cascade, the effects of INOXY on other face memory-related social behaviors relevant to autism, such as social anxiety and attentional preference for faces [150, 151]. Further, since face memory could be a proxy to autistic participants’ OXY-relevant genetic background, researchers could investigate whether it predicts responders to INOXY. This face IIR-based stratification of participants may help addressing the failure of many [152, 153], often underpowered [154], INOXY intervention studies with autistic participants.

Recent evidence showing that assessment of IIR in autistic individuals may extend beyond visual-face to other sensory systems and identity-conveying cues such as auditory-voice [155] and olfactory-body odor [156], increases the possibility to directly translate experimental paradigms and research questions to autism rodent models [85]. Animal studies, overcoming limitations intrinsic to human research, could examine if and which genetic autism mouse models show IIR deficits, to then uncover their neuro-biological correlates with a focus on OXY’s possible modulatory role in generating states for optimized information extraction [157] and in attributing salience and reward to identity-relevant sensory cues [158].

In conclusion, we found that difficulties in face individual identity recognition are highly prevalent in autism and are linked to difficulties in mental state understanding from the eye region independently from face perception skills. Further, they stratify autistic individuals irrespective of intellectual ability, diagnostic symptoms, and personality traits. Because of its potential role as an endophenotype, we believe individual identity recognition may be important to advance our understanding of autism within a translational framework informed by, and informative to, neurobiological non-human animal research.

Acknowledgements

We thank all participants who took part in this study. Particular thanks go to autistic participants and their relatives who, with their interest and dedication, make autism research possible.

Abbreviations

ADI-R

Autism Diagnostic Interview Revised

ADOS

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

AQ

Autism spectrum quotient

AUT

Autism

AUT-NP

Autistic non-prosopagnosic

AUT-P

Autistic prosopagnosic

CFMT

Cambridge Face Memory Test

CFPT

Cambridge Face Perception Test

CI

Confidence interval

CSS

Calibrated Severity Score

EC

Empathic concern

ID

Intellectual disability

IIR

Individual identity recognition

INOXY

Intranasal oxytocin

IQ

Intelligence quotient

IRI

Interpersonal Reactivity Index

NT

Neurotypical

OXY

Oxytocin

PI20

Twenty-item Prosopagnosia Index

PT

Perspective taking

RRB

Restricted Repetitive Behaviors

SA

Social affective

SD

Standard deviation

SE

Standard error

TAS

Twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale

VIF

Variance inflation factor

Authors’ contributions

IMP designed the study, run clinical assessments, collected and analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript. GP analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. APL and SBC supervised the project and wrote the manuscript. The authors read and approved the manuscript.

Funding

IMP was supported by the USA—Italy Fulbright Commission and the Italian Ministry of Health (GR-2009-1607360) during the period of this work.

APL was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH, primarily RO1MH100186); the National Science Foundation, Sidney R. Baer, Jr. Foundation, Harvard Catalyst The Harvard Clinical and Translational Science Center (NCRR and the NCATS NIH, UL1 RR025758), and by the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency (DARPA) via HR001117S0030. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of Harvard Catalyst, Harvard University, and its affiliated academic health care centers, the National Institutes of Health, National Science Foundation, the Sidney R. Baer Jr. Foundation, or DARPA.

SBC was supported by the MRC (grant number RNAG/128, award number RG46450), the Wellcome Trust (grant number RNAG/235, award number RG58828), and the Autism Research Trust, during the period of this work. He was also supported by the Innovative Medicines Initiative 2 Joint Undertaking (JU) under grant agreement No 777394. The JU receives support from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program and EFPIA and AUTISM SPEAKS, Autistica, SFARI. He was also supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care East of England at Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, NIHR, or Department of Health and Social Care’. Finally, he received support from the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre (BRC).

Availability of data and materials

The anonymized datasets analyzed in the current study are available from the corresponding author on request.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical clearance was granted by the local ethics committees at IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy; University College London, London, UK; Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Boston, MA, USA, and the study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Consent for publication

Participants gave informed consent to participate in the study and to publish their anonymized individual data prior to participation in the study. Participants were paid for their time.

Competing interests

A.P.L. serves on the scientific advisory boards for the Starlab Neuroscience, Neuroelectrics, Neosync, NovaVision, Magstim, and Cognito and is listed as an inventor on several issued and pending patents on the real-time integration of transcranial magnetic stimulation with electroencephalography and magnetic resonance imaging. The other authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders : DSM-5. Fifth edition. Arlington: American Psychiatric Association, [2013]; 2013. Available from: https://search.library.wisc.edu/catalog/9910187853902121.
  • 2.Elsabbagh M, Divan G, Koh Y, Kim YS, Kauchali S, Marcín C, et al. Global prevalence of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. Autism Res. 2012;5(3):160–179. doi: 10.1002/aur.239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lai M, Lombardo MV, Baron-Cohen S. Autism. Lancet. 2013;6736(13):1–15. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Geschwind DH, State MW. Gene hunting in autism spectrum disorder: on the path to precision medicine. Lancet Neurol. 2015;14(11):1109–1120. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00044-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Auyeung B, Chakrabarti B, Baron-Cohen S. Sex/Gender Differences and autism: setting the scene for future research. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015;54:11–24 Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0890856714007254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 6.Szatmari P, Georgiades S, Duku E, Bennett TA, Bryson S, Fombonne E, et al. Developmental trajectories of symptom severity and adaptive functioning in an inception cohort of preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(3):276–283. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kapur S, Phillips AG, Insel TR. Why has it taken so long for biological psychiatry to develop clinical tests and what to do about it? Mol Psychiatry. 2012;17(12):1174–1179. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Viding E, Blakemore S-J. Endophenotype approach to developmental psychopathology: implications for autism research. Behav Genet. 2007;37(1):51–60. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9105-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Christensen DL, Braun KVN, Baio J, Bilder D, Charles J, Constantino JN, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years — autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 Sites, United States, 2012. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2018;65(13):1–23. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6513a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Volkmar FR, McPartland JC. From Kanner to DSM-5: autism as an evolving diagnostic concept. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2014;10(1):193–212. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.DiLalla LF, McCrary M, Diaz E. A review of endophenotypes in schizophrenia and autism: the next phase for understanding genetic etiologies. Am J Med Genet Part C Semin Med Genet. 2017;175(3):354–361. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.c.31566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Alarcón M, Yonan AL, Gilliam TC, Cantor RM, Geschwind DH. Quantitative genome scan and ordered-subsets analysis of autism endophenotypes support language QTLs. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(8):747–757. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Constantino JN, Kennon-McGill S, Weichselbaum C, Marrus N, Haider A, Glowinski AL, et al. Infant viewing of social scenes is under genetic control and is atypical in autism. Nature. 2017;547(7663):340–344. doi: 10.1038/nature22999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lowe JK, Werling DM, Constantino JN, Cantor RM, Geschwind DH. Social responsiveness, an autism endophenotype: genomewide significant linkage to two regions on chromosome 8. Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172(3):266–275. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14050576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lisiecka DM, Holt R, Tait R, Ford M, Lai MC, Chura LR, et al. Developmental white matter microstructure in autism phenotype and corresponding endophenotype during adolescence. Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5(3):e529. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(4):636–645. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Spencer MD, Holt RJ, Chura LR, Suckling J, Calder AJ, Bullmore ET, et al. A novel functional brain imaging endophenotype of autism: the neural response to facial expression of emotion. Transl Psychiatry. 2011;1(7):e19–e17. doi: 10.1038/tp.2011.18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Spencer MD, Holt RJ, Chura LR, Calder AJ, Suckling J, Bullmore ET, et al. Atypical activation during the embedded figures task as a functional magnetic resonance imaging endophenotype of autism. Brain. 2012;135(11):3469–3480. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Gabard-Durnam L, Tierney AL, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Alpha asymmetry in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(2):473–480. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1926-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Tierney AL, Gabard-Durnam L, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Developmental trajectories of resting EEG power: an endophenotype of autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One. 2012;7(6). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 21.Kaiser MD, Hudac CM, Shultz S, Su Mei Lee, Cheung C, Berkena AM, et al. Neural signatures of autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(49):21223–21228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 22.Moseley RL, Ypma RJF, Holt RJ, Floris D, Chura LR, Spencer MD, et al. Whole-brain functional hypoconnectivity as an endophenotype of autism in adolescents. NeuroImage Clin. 2015;9:140–152. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.07.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Liu XQ, Paterson AD, Szatmari P. Genome-wide linkage analyses of quantitative and categorical autism subphenotypes. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(7):561–570. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Chaste P, Klei L, Sanders SJ, Hus V, Murtha MT, Lowe JK, et al. A genome-wide association study of autism using the Simons simplex collection: does reducing phenotypic heterogeneity in autism increase genetic homogeneity? Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77(9):775–784. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Geschwind DH, Flint J. Genetics and genomics of psychiatric disease. Science (80- ) 2015;349(6255):1489–1494. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa8954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Flint J, Timpson N, Munafò M. Assessing the utility of intermediate phenotypes for genetic mapping of psychiatric disease. Trends Neurosci. 2014;37(12):733–741. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Krol A, Wimmer RD, Halassa MM, Feng G. Thalamic reticular dysfunction as a circuit endophenotype in neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuron. 2018;98(2):282–295. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Tibbetts EA, Dale J. Individual recognition: it is good to be different. Trends Ecol Evol. 2007;22(10):529–537. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2007.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bushnell IWR. Mother’s Face Recognition in Newborn Infants: Learning and Memory. Infant Child Dev. 2001;10(1–2):67–74.
  • 30.Turati C, Bulf H, Simion F. Newborns’ face recognition over changes in viewpoint. Cognition. 2008;106(3):1300–1321. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Johnson ZV, Young LJ. Oxytocin and vasopressin neural networks: implications for social behavioral diversity and translational neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;76:87–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Yokoi S, Naruse K, Kamei Y, Ansai S, Kinoshita M, Mito M, et al. Sexually dimorphic role of oxytocin in medaka mate choice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020;117(9):4802–4808. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1921446117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Perrodin C, Kayser C, Abel TJ, Logothetis NK, Petkov CI. Who is that ? Brain networks and mechanisms for identifying individuals. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015;xx:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Chang L, Tsao DY. The code for facial identity in the primate brain. Cell. 2017;169(6):1013–1028.e14. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Stevens CF. Conserved features of the primate face code. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2018;201716341 Available from: http://www.pnas.org/lookup/doi/10.1073/pnas.1716341115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 36.Sheehan MJ, Nachman MW. Morphological and population genomic evidence that human faces have evolved to signal individual identity. Nat Commun. 2015;5(4800):1–21. doi: 10.1038/ncomms5800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Porciello G, Bufalari I, Minio-Paluello I, Di Pace E, Aglioti SM. The ‘enfacement’ illusion: a window on the plasticity of the self. Cortex. 2018;104:261–275. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bufalari I, Porciello G, Sperduti M, Minio-Paluello I. Self-identification with another person’s face: the time relevant role of multimodal brain areas in the enfacement illusion. J Neurophysiol. 2015;113(7):1959–1962. doi: 10.1152/jn.00872.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Minio-Paluello I, Porciello G, Gandolfo M, Boukarras S, Aglioti SM. The enfacement illusion boosts facial mimicry. Cortex. 2019;123:113–123. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Tardif J, Morin Duchesne X, Cohan S, Royer J, Blais C, Fiset D, et al. Use of face information varies systematically from developmental prosopagnosics to super-recognizers. Psychol Sci. 2018:1–9 Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0956797618811338. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 41.Kennerknecht I, Grueter T, Welling B, Wentzek S. First report of prevalence of non-syndromic hereditary prosopagnosia (HPA) Am J Med Genet Part A. 2006;140A:1617–1622. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.31343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kennerknecht I, Ho NY, Wong VCN. Prevalence of hereditary prosopagnosia (HPA) in Hong Kong Chinese population. Am J Med Genet Part A. 2008;146A(22):2863–2870. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.32552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Susilo T, Duchaine B. Advances in developmental prosopagnosia research. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23(3):423–429. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bate S, Cook SJ, Duchaine B, Tree JJ, Burns EJ, Hodgson TL. Intranasal inhalation of oxytocin improves face processing in developmental prosopagnosia. Cortex. 2014;50:55–63. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Cattaneo Z, Daini R, Malaspina M, Manai F, Lillo M, Fermi V, et al. Congenital prosopagnosia is associated with a genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene: an exploratory study. Neuroscience. 2016;339:162–173. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.09.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Duchaine B, Nakayama K. The Cambridge Face Memory Test: results for neurologically intact individuals and an investigation of its validity using inverted face stimuli and prosopagnosic participants. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(4):576–585. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Bowles DC, McKone E, Dawel A, Duchaine B, Palermo R, Schmalzl L, et al. Diagnosing prosopagnosia: effects of ageing, sex, and participant-stimulus ethnic match on the Cambridge face memory test and Cambridge face perception test. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2009;26(5):423–455. doi: 10.1080/02643290903343149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Bate S, Tree JJ. The definition and diagnosis of developmental prosopagnosia. Q J Exp Psychol. 2017;70(2):193–200. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1195414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Dalrymple KA, Palermo R. Guidelines for studying developmental prosopagnosia in adults and children. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2016;7(1):73–87. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Nomi JS, Uddin LQ. Neuropsychologia face processing in autism spectrum disorders: from brain regions to brain networks. Neuropsychologia. 2015;71:201–216. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Webb SJ, Neuhaus E, Faja S. Face perception and learning in autism spectrum disorders. Q J Exp Psychol. 2017;70(5):970–986. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1151059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Dawson G, Carver L, Meltzoff AN, Panagiotides H, McPartland J, Webb SJ. Neural correlates of face and object recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development. Child Dev. 2002;73(3):700–717. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Sterling L, Dawson ÆG, Webb ÆS, Murias ÆM, Munson J, Panagiotides ÆH, et al. The role of face familiarity in eye tracking of faces by individuals with autism spectrum disorders. 2008. pp. 1666–1675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Chawarska K, Shic F. Looking but not seeing: atypical visual scanning and recognition of faces in 2 and 4-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord [Internet]. 2009;39(12):1663–1672. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0803-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Eussen MLJM, Louwerse A, Herba CM, Van Gool AR, Verheij F. Verhulst FC, et al. Childhood facial recognition predicts adolescent symptom severity in autism spectrum disorder. 2015:1–11. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 56.Barton JJS, Cherkasova MV, Hefter R, Cox TA, O’Connor M, Manoach DS. Are patients with social developmental disorders prosopagnosic? Perceptual heterogeneity in the Asperger and socio-emotional processing disorders. Brain. 2004;127(8):1706–1716. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Wolf JM, Tanaka JW, Klaiman C, Cockburn J, Herlihy L, Brown C, et al. Specific impairment of face-processing abilities in children with autism spectrum disorder using the Let’s Face It! skills battery. Autism Res [Internet]. 2008;1(6):329–340. doi: 10.1002/aur.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Wilson CE, Palermo R, Burton AM, Brock J. Recognition of own- and other-race faces in autism spectrum disorders. Q J Exp Psychol. 2011;64(10):1939–1954. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2011.603052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Oerlemans AM, Droste K, Van Steijn DJ, De Sonneville LMJ, Buitelaar JK, Rommelse NNJ. Co-segregation of social cognition, executive function and local processing style in children with ASD, their siblings and normal controls. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(12):2764–2778. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1807-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Skuse DH, Lori A, Cubells JF, Lee I, Conneely KN, Puura K, et al. Common polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene ( OXTR ) is associated with human social recognition skills. Proc Natl Acad Sci [Internet]. 2014;111(5):1987–1992. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302985111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Duchaine BC, Weidenfeld A. An evaluation of two commonly used tests of unfamiliar face recognition. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41(6):713–720. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00222-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Wilson CE, Palermo R, Brock J. Visual scan paths and recognition of facial identity in autism spectrum disorder and typical development. PLoS One. 2012;7(5):e37681. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Weigelt S, Koldewyn K, Kanwisher N. Face identity recognition in autism spectrum disorders: a review of behavioral studies [Internet]. Vol. 36, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. Elsevier Ltd; 2012. p. 1060–1084. Available from: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 64.Weigelt S, Koldewyn K, Kanwisher N. Face recognition deficits in autism spectrum disorders are both domain specific and process specific. Pavlova M, editor. PLoS One [Internet]. 2013 Sep 11;8(9):e74541. Available from: http://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0074541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 65.Whyte EM, Behrmann M, Minshew NJ, Garcia NV, Scherf KS. Animal, but not human, faces engage the distributed face network in adolescents with autism. Dev Sci. 2016;19(2):306–317. doi: 10.1111/desc.12305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.O’Hearn K, Tanaka J, Lynn A, Fedor J, Minshew N, Luna B. Brain and cognition developmental plateau in visual object processing from adolescence to adulthood in autism. BRAIN Cogn [Internet]. 2014;90:124–134. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Scherf KS, Elbich D, Minshew N, Behrmann M. Individual differences in symptom severity and behavior predict neural activation during face processing in adolescents with autism. NeuroImage Clin [Internet]. 2015;7:53–67. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Ewbank MP, Pell PJ, Powell TE, Von EAH, Baron-cohen S, Calder AJ. Repetition suppression and memory for faces is reduced in adults with autism spectrum conditions. Cereb Cortex [Internet]. 2016;1–12. Available from: https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/cercor/bhw385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 69.Lynn AC, Padmanabhan A, Simmonds D, Foran W, Hallquist MN. Luna B, et al. Functional connectivity differences in autism during face and car recognition: underconnectivity and atypical age-related changes. 2016:1–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 70.O’hearn K, Schroer E, Minshew N, Luna B. Lack of developmental improvement on a face memory task during adolescence in autism. Neuropsychologia [Internet]. 2010;48(13):3955–60 Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0028393210003787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 71.Kirchner JC, Hatri A, Heekeren HR. Dziobek I. Autistic symptomatology, face processing abilities, and eye fixation patterns. 2011:158–67. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 72.Fedor J, Lynn A, Foran W, DiCicco-Bloom J, Luna B, O’Hearn K. Patterns of fixation during face recognition: differences in autism across age. Autism [Internet]. 2017;136236131771498. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1362361317714989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 73.Dwyer P, Xu B, Tanaka JW. Investigating the perception of face identity in adults on the autism spectrum using behavioural and electrophysiological measures. Vision Res [Internet]. 2018;(February):0–1. Available from: 10.1016/j.visres.2018.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 74.Schelinski S, Roswandowitz C, von Kriegstein K. Voice identity processing in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res [Internet] 2017;10(1):155–68. doi: 10.1002/aur.1639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Hedley D, Brewer N, Young R. Face recognition performance of individuals with Asperger syndrome on the Cambridge face memory test. Autism Res. 2011;4(6):449–455. doi: 10.1002/aur.214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Zhu Q, Song Y, Hu S, Li X, Tian M, Zhen Z, et al. Heritability of the specific cognitive ability of face perception. Curr Biol [Internet]. 2010;20(2):137–142. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Wilmer JB, Germine L, Chabris CF, Chatterjee G, Williams M, Loken E, et al. Human face recognition ability is specific and highly heritable. Proc Natl Acad Sci [Internet]. 2010;107(11):5238–5241. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913053107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Shakeshaft NG, Plomin R. Genetic specificity of face recognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci [Internet]. 2015;112(41):12887–12892. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1421881112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Wilmer JB, Germine LT, Nakayama K. Face recognition: a model specific ability. Front Hum Neurosci [Internet]. 2014;8(October):1–5. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Wilson CE, Freeman P, Brock J, Burton AM, Palermo R. Facial identity recognition in the broader autism phenotype. Miles J, editor. PLoS One [Internet]. 2010 22;5(9):e12876. Available from: https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 81.de Klerk CCJM, Gliga T, Charman T, Johnson MH. Face engagement during infancy predicts later face recognition ability in younger siblings of children with autism. Dev Sci [Internet]. 2014;17(4):596–611. doi: 10.1111/desc.12141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.de Haan M, Nelson CA. Brain activity differentiates face and object processing in 6-month-old infants. Dev Psychol [Internet]. 1999;35(4):1113–1121. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.1113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Key AP, Dykens EM. Incidental memory for faces in children with different genetic subtypes of Prader-Willi syndrome. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci [Internet]. 2017;12(6):918–927. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Verhallen RJ, Bosten JM, Goodbourn PT, Lawrance-Owen AJ, Bargary G, Mollon JD. The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and face recognition. Psychol Sci [Internet]. 2017;28(1):47–55. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0956797616672269. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 85.Minio-Paluello I, Porciello G, Mandillo S, Golini E, Aglioti SM, Gross CT, D'Amato FR. Poster at the workshop: understanding the neuroregulatory actions of oxytocin and its potential clinical applications. Erice, Italy. 22-27/05/ 2018. Poster title: Identity Recognition A Promising Oxytocin-relevant Endophenotype in Autism. 2018.
  • 86.Cheetham SA, Thom MD, Jury F, Ollier WER, Beynon RJ. The genetic basis of individual-recognition signals in the mouse. Curr Biol. 2007;17(October 23):1771–1777. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord [Internet]. 2000 Jun;30(3):205–23. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11055457. [PubMed]
  • 88.Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994;24(5):659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence WASI: Manual [Internet]. Pearson/PsychCorpl; 1999. Available from: https://books.google.it/books?id = adTXtwAACAAJ.
  • 90.Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV). [Internet]. Pearson, The Psychological Corporation; 2008. Available from: https://books.google.it/books?id = aZhltAEACAAJ.
  • 91.Raven JC. Standard Progressive Matrices Sets A, B, C, D & E [Internet]. Harcourt; 2006. Available from: https://books.google.it/books?id = V3HxPgAACAAJ.
  • 92.Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord [Internet]. 2001;31(1):5–17. doi: 10.1023/A:1005653411471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Minio-Paluello I, Baron-Cohen S, Avenanti A, Walsh V, Aglioti SM. Absence of embodied empathy during pain observation in Asperger syndrome. Biol Psychiatry [Internet]. 2009;65(1):55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Minio-Paluello I, Lombardo M V., Chakrabarti B, Wheelwright S, Baron-Cohen S. Response to smith’s letter to the editor “Emotional empathy in Autism spectrum conditions: Weak, Intact, or heightened?” J Autism Dev Disord [Internet]. 2009;39(12):1749–54. Available from: https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%2Fs10803-009-0800-x. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 95.Curioni A, Minio-Paluello I, Sacheli LM, Candidi M, Aglioti SM. Autistic traits affect interpersonal motor coordination by modulating strategic use of role-based behavior. Mol Autism [Internet]. 2017;8(1):23. Available from: http://molecularautism.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s13229-017-0141-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 96.McKone E, Martini P, Nakayama K. Categorical perception of face identity in noise isolates configural processing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2001;27(3):573–599. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.3.573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Cho S, Wilmer J, Fiset D, Gulick AE Van, Ryan KF. Item Response Theory Analyses of the Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT). 2015;27(2):552–566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 98.Germine L, Nakayama K, Duchaine BC, Chabris CF, Chatterjee G, Wilmer JB. Is the Web as good as the lab? Comparable performance from Web and lab in cognitive/perceptual experiments. Psychon Bull Rev. 2012;19(5):847–857. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0296-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Bate S, Frowd C, Bennetts R, Hasshim N, Murray E, Bobak AK, et al. Applied screening tests for the detection of superior face recognition. Cogn Res Princ Implic [Internet]. 2018 27;3(1):22. Available from: https://cognitiveresearchjournal.springeropen.com/articles/10.1186/s41235-018-0116-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 100.Wilmer JB, Germine L, Chabris CF, Chatterjee G, Williams M, Loken E, et al. Human face recognition ability is specific and highly heritable. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010;107(11):5238–5241. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913053107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Duchaine B, Yovel G, Nakayama K. No global processing deficit in the Navon task in 14 developmental prosopagnosics. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2(2):104–113. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Valentine T. Upside-down faces: a review of the effect of inversion upon face recognition. Br J Psychol [Internet]. 1988;79(4):471–491. Available from: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1988.tb02747.x. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 103.Duchaine B, Germine L, Nakayama K. Family resemblance: ten family members with prosopagnosia and within-class object agnosia. Cogn Neuropsychol [Internet]. 2007;24(4):419–430. doi: 10.1080/02643290701380491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Bobak AK, Parris BA, Gregory NJ, Bennetts RJ, Bate S. Eye-movement strategies in developmental prosopagnosia and “super” face recognition. Q J Exp Psychol [Internet]. 2017;70(2):201–217. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1161059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Garrido L, Duchaine B, Nakayama K. Face detection in normal and prosopagnosic individuals. J Neuropsychol [Internet]. 2008;2(1):119–140. doi: 10.1348/174866407X246843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry [Internet]. 2001;42(2):241–251. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Fernández-Abascal EG, Cabello R, Fernández-Berrocal P, Baron-Cohen S. Test-retest reliability of the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test: a one-year follow-up study. Mol Autism [Internet]. 2013;4(1):33. Available from: http://molecularautism.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/2040-2392-4-33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 108.Baron-Cohen S, Bowen DC, Holt RJ, Allison C, Auyeung B, Lombardo M V, et al. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test: complete absence of typical sex difference in ~400 men and women with autism. Yamasue H, editor. PLoS One [Internet]. 2015 27;10(8):e0136521. Available from: http://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 109.Losh M, Adolphs R, Poe MD, Couture S, Penn D, Baranek GT, et al. Neuropsychological profile of autism and the broad autism phenotype. Arch Gen Psychiatry [Internet]. 2009 1;66(5):518. Available from: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/article.aspx?doi=10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 110.Holt RJ, Chura LR, Lai MC, Suckling J, Von Dem Hagen E, Calder AJ, et al. “Reading the Mind in the Eyes”: an fMRI study of adolescents with autism and their siblings. Psychol Med [Internet]. 2014;44(15):3215–3227. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Lai M-C, Lombardo M V., Ruigrok AN V., Chakrabarti B, Wheelwright SJ, Auyeung B, et al. Cognition in males and females with autism: similarities and differences. Botbol M, editor. PLoS One [Internet]. 2012 17;7(10):e47198. Available from: http://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0047198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 112.Wilson CE, Happé F, Wheelwright SJ, Ecker C, Lombardo M V., Johnston P, et al. The neuropsychology of male adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome. Autism Res [Internet]. 2014 Oct;7(5):568–81. Available from: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/aur.1394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 113.Warrier V, Grasby KL, Uzefovsky F, Toro R, Smith P, Chakrabarti B, et al. Genome-wide meta-analysis of cognitive empathy: heritability, and correlates with sex, neuropsychiatric conditions and cognition. Mol Psychiatry [Internet]. 2018;23(6):1402–1409. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Duchaine BC, Parker H, Nakayama K. Normal recognition of emotion in a prosopagnosic. Perception [Internet]. 2003;32(7):827–838. doi: 10.1068/p5067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Palermo R, Willis ML, Rivolta D, McKone E, Wilson CE, Calder AJ. Impaired holistic coding of facial expression and facial identity in congenital prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia [Internet]. 2011;49(5):1226–1235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Lee Y, Duchaine B, Wilson HR, Nakayama K. Three cases of developmental prosopagnosia from one family: detailed neuropsychological and psychophysical investigation of face processing. Cortex [Internet]. 2010;46(8):949–964. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Wheelwright S, Auyeung B, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S. Defining the broader, medium and narrow autism phenotype among parents using the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Mol Autism [Internet]. 2010;1(1):10. Available from: http://molecularautism.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/2040-2392-1-10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 118.Ruzich E, Allison C, Chakrabarti B, Smith P, Musto H, Ring H, et al. Sex and STEM occupation predict Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) Scores in half a million people. Taniike M, editor. PLoS One [Internet]. 2015 21;10(10):e0141229. Available from: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 119.Greenberg DM, Warrier V, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S. Testing the empathizing–systemizing theory of sex differences and the extreme male brain theory of autism in half a million people. Proc Natl Acad Sci [Internet]. 2018;115(48):12152–12157. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1811032115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Shah P, Gaule A, Sowden S, Bird G, Cook R, Cook R. The 20-item prosopagnosia index (PI20): a self-report instrument for identifying developmental prosopagnosia. R Soc Open Sci [Internet]. 2015;2(140343):1–11. doi: 10.1098/rsos.140343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Gray KLH, Bird G, Cook R. Robust associations between the 20-item prosopagnosia index and the Cambridge Face Memory Test in the general population. R Soc Open Sci [Internet]. 2017 Mar;4(3):160923. Available from: https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rsos.160923. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 122.Livingston LA, Shah P. People with and without prosopagnosia have insight into their face recognition ability. Q J Exp Psychol. 2017:1–3. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 123.Rubino C, Corrow SL, Corrow JC, Duchaine B, Barton JJS. Word and text processing in developmental prosopagnosia. Cogn Neuropsychol [Internet]. 2016;33(5–6):315–328. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1204281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Biotti F, Cook R. Impaired perception of facial emotion in developmental prosopagnosia. Cortex [Internet]. 2016;81:126–36. Available from: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 125.Barton JJS, Corrow SL. The problem of being bad at faces. Neuropsychologia [Internet]. 2016;89:119–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Davis MH. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. J Pers Soc Psychol [Internet]. 1983;44(1):113–26. Available from: http://www.uv.es/~friasnav/Davis_1980.pdf%5Cn. http://www.uv.es/~friasnav/Davis_1980.pdfhttp://content.apa.org/journals/psp/44/1/113%5Cn.
  • 127.Bagby RM, Parker JDA, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia scale—I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res [Internet]. 1994 Jan;38(1):23–32. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/0022399994900051. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 128.Parker JD, Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. The 20-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. J Psychosom Res [Internet]. 2003;55(3):269–275. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00578-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Bird G, Cook R. Mixed emotions: the contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Transl Psychiatry [Internet]. 2013;3(7):e285. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Field A. Discovering Statistics Using SPSS. Third. Los Angeles: SAGE; 2009. v–821.
  • 131.Field AP, Wilcox RR. Robust statistical methods: a primer for clinical psychology and experimental psychopathology researchers. Behav Res Ther [Internet]. 2017;98:19–38. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Hayes AF. PROCESS: a versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling. Unpubl Manuscr [Internet]. 2012;1–39. Available from: http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf.
  • 133.Gotham K, Pickles A, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS Scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord [Internet]. 2009;39(5):693–705. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Duchaine B, Murray H, Turner M, White S, Garrido L. Normal social cognition in developmental prosopagnosia. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2009;26(7):620–634. doi: 10.1080/02643291003616145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.de la Torre-Ubieta L, Won H, Stein JL, Geschwind DH. Advancing the understanding of autism disease mechanisms through genetics. Nat Med [Internet]. 2016;22(4):345–361. doi: 10.1038/nm.4071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Bourgeron T. From the genetic architecture to synaptic plasticity in autism spectrum disorder [Internet]. Vol. 16, Nature Reviews Neuroscience. Nature Publishing Group; 2015. p. 551–563. Available from: 10.1038/nrn3992. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 137.Kendler KS, Neale MC. Endophenotype: a conceptual analysis. Mol Psychiatry [Internet]. 2010;15(8):789–797. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Constantino JN. Deconstructing autism: from unitary syndrome to contributory developmental endophenotypes. Int Rev Psychiatry [Internet]. 2018;30(1):18–24. doi: 10.1080/09540261.2018.1433133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Peterson MF, Eckstein MP. Looking just below the eyes is optimal across face recognition tasks. Proc Natl Acad Sci [Internet]. 2012;109(48):E3314–E3323. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1214269109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Jones W, Klin A. Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2–6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature [Internet]. 2013;504(7480):427–431. doi: 10.1038/nature12715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Elsabbagh M, Mercure E, Hudry K, Chandler S, Pasco G, Charman T, et al. Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic eye gaze is associated with later emerging autism. Curr Biol [Internet]. 2012;22(4):338–342. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.DeGutis J, Cohan S, Mercado RJ, Wilmer J, Nakayama K. Holistic processing of the mouth but not the eyes in developmental prosopagnosia. Cogn Neuropsychol [Internet]. 2012;29(5–6):419–446. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2012.754745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Royer J, Blais C, Charbonneau I, Déry K, Tardif J, Duchaine B, et al. Greater reliance on the eye region predicts better face recognition ability. Cognition [Internet]. 2018;181(August):12–20. Available from. 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 144.Davis J, Mckone E, Zirnsak M, Moore T, Kearney RO, Apthorp D, et al. Social and attention-to-detail subclusters of autistic traits differentially predict looking at eyes and face identity recognition ability. 2017:191–219. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 145.Maurer D, Le Grand R, Mondloch CJ. The many faces of configural processing. Trends Cogn Sci. 2002;6(6):255–260. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01903-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Christensen DL, Braun KVN, Baio J, Bilder D, Charles J, Constantino JN, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years — autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 Sites, United States, 2012. MMWR Surveill Summ [Internet]. 2018;65(13):1–23. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6513a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Wilkinson DA, Best CA, Minshew NJ, Strauss MS. Memory awareness for faces in individuals with autism. J Autism Dev Disord [Internet]. 2010;40(11):1371–1377. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0995-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Rimmele U, Hediger K, Heinrichs M, Klaver P. Oxytocin makes a face in memory familiar. J Neurosci [Internet]. 2009;29(1):38–42. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4260-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Auyeung B, Lombardo M V, Heinrichs M, Chakrabarti B, Sule A, Deakin JB, et al. Oxytocin increases eye contact during a real-time , naturalistic social interaction in males with and without autism. 2015;5(2):e507-e506. Available from: 10.1038/tp.2014.146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 150.Davis JM, McKone E, Dennett H, O’Connor KB, O’Kearney R, Palermo R. Individual differences in the ability to recognise facial identity are associated with social anxiety. Baune BT, editor. PLoS One [Internet]. 2011 14;6(12):e28800. Available from: https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 151.Kanat M, Spenthof I, Riedel A, Elst LT Van, Heinrichs M, Domes G. Restoring effects of oxytocin on the attentional preference for faces in autism. 2017;7(4):e1097-e1098. Available from: 10.1038/tp.2017.67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 152.Alvares GA, Quintana DS, Whitehouse AJO. Beyond the hype and hope: critical considerations for intranasal oxytocin research in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res [Internet]. 2017;10(1):25–41. doi: 10.1002/aur.1692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Bakermans-kranenburg MJ, Van Ijzendoorn MH. Sniffing around oxytocin: review and meta-analyses of trials in healthy and clinical groups with implications for pharmacotherapy. Transl Psychiatry [Internet]. 2013;3(5):e258–e214. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Walum H, Waldman ID, Young LJ. Statistical and methodological considerations for the interpretation of intranasal oxytocin studies. Biol Psychiatry [Internet]. 2016;79(3):251–257. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.06.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Schelinski S, Borowiak K, von Kriegstein K. Temporal voice areas exist in autism spectrum disorder but are dysfunctional for voice identity recognition. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci [Internet]. 2016;11(11):1812–1822. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Endevelt-Shapira Y, Perl O, Ravia A, Amir D, Eisen A, Bezalel V, et al. Altered responses to social chemosignals in autism spectrum disorder. Nat Neurosci [Internet]. 2018;21(1):111–122. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0024-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Oettl L-L, Ravi N, Schneider M, Scheller MF, Schneider P, Mitre M, et al. Oxytocin enhances social recognition by modulating cortical control of early olfactory processing. Neuron [Internet]. 2016;90(3):609–621. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Grinevich V, Stoop R. Interplay between oxytocin and sensory systems in the orchestration of socio-emotional behaviors. Neuron [Internet]. 2018 Sep;99(5):887–904. Available from: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The anonymized datasets analyzed in the current study are available from the corresponding author on request.


Articles from Molecular Autism are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES