Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2020 Nov 11;10:19602. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-76757-0

A test of the lateral semicircular canal correlation to head posture, diet and other biological traits in “ungulate” mammals

J Benoit 1,, L J Legendre 2, A A Farke 3, J M Neenan 4, B Mennecart 5, L Costeur 5, S Merigeaud 6,7, P R Manger 8
PMCID: PMC7658238  PMID: 33177568

Abstract

For over a century, researchers have assumed that the plane of the lateral semicircular canal of the inner ear lies parallel to the horizon when the head is at rest, and used this assumption to reconstruct head posture in extinct species. Although this hypothesis has been repeatedly questioned, it has never been tested on a large sample size and at a broad taxonomic scale in mammals. This study presents a comprehensive test of this hypothesis in over one hundred “ungulate” species. Using CT scanning and manual segmentation, the orientation of the skull was reconstructed as if the lateral semicircular canal of the bony labyrinth was aligned horizontally. This reconstructed cranial orientation was statistically compared to the actual head posture of the corresponding species using a dataset of 10,000 photographs and phylogenetic regression analysis. A statistically significant correlation between the reconstructed cranial orientation and head posture is found, although the plane of the lateral semicircular canal departs significantly from horizontal. We thus caution against the use of the lateral semicircular canal as a proxy to infer precisely the horizontal plane on dry skulls and in extinct species. Diet (browsing or grazing) and head-butting behaviour are significantly correlated to the orientation of the lateral semicircular canal, but not to the actual head posture. Head posture and the orientation of the lateral semicircular canal are both strongly correlated with phylogenetic history.

Subject terms: Palaeontology, Auditory system

Introduction

The need for a reliable and reproducible way of orienting dry skulls for cranial measurements has led to a considerable amount of literature suggesting that the plane of the lateral semicircular canal (LSC) of the bony labyrinth (the osseous capsule of the inner ear) is horizontal when the head is held in its “habitual” (i.e. not actively attained) or “alert” positions112. This is backed by the hypothesis that a horizontal orientation of the LSC would mechanically maximize the recording of rotational and linear head movements made in the horizontal plane by placing the sensory hair cells of the semicircular canal and its associated ampulla perpendicular to the horizontal plane1216. The subsequent use of the orientation of the plane of the LSC as a proxy to infer head posture in fossil vertebrates has grown more popular among paleontologists, as it is being applied to dozens of extinct taxa such as archosaurs, including dinosaurs, and synapsids, including mammals12,1729. This has raised discussion on some crucial paleobiological questions, such as the evolution of bipedalism in ancient hominin14,18 and paleodiets. As browsers are expected to hold their head higher than grazers30, head posture has been invoked in reconstructing ancient diet in fossil herbivorous species20,24. Semi-aquatic species, on the other hand, would hold their head tilted upward27 (but see Neenan and Scheyer31). In addition, head posture is directly involved in discussions about the origin of endothermy, as blood pressure to perfuse the head, and particularly the brain, directly depends on head posture and thermophysiology (species with low metabolism have a lower blood pressure than species with a high metabolism, and therefore cannot perfuse their brain if their head is held far above their heart)32. Head posture may thus be crucial for inferring the evolution of endothermy in birds, mammals, and their respective ancestors, the non-avian dinosaurs, and non-mammalian synapsids32. Finally, because of the remodeling of the skull and musculature to accommodate cranial appendages and to absorb shocks, head posture is also central to discussions regarding the practice of display and head-butting24,29,33. Head-butting correlates with a hierarchical ranking system and social organisation in herbivores, which makes it a direct proxy of complex behavior in extinct species3440. As such, head posture is relevant to many crucial paleobiological, behavioural, and physiological inferences, and the validity of the use of the LSC orientation as a proxy to reconstruct it requires scrutiny.

Although theoretically sound13, and supported by field observations of some reptiles (turtles, crocodiles, and squamates) showing a 0°–5° difference only between the plane of the LSC and the horizontal during “habitual” head posture12,4143, the assumption that the plane of the LSC is horizontal when the head is at rest has been repeatedly challenged in archosaurs and mammals6,11,43,44. Most published accounts of head posture in mammals evidence an anterior upward pitching of the LSC averaging 20°–30° to the horizontal (e.g. in humans) when the head is at rest or in alert posture6,11,12,30,43,45. In birds, the orientation of the plane of the LSC in alert posture varies between − 15° and 50° around the horizontal8,43. All of these studies used different methodological approaches to quantify head posture (e.g. by keeping animals on a leash or distracted, using field observation of wild animals, using photographs from open access banks, using pictures from the literature) and access the orientation of the LSC (e.g. using X-ray radiography, CT scanning, sawed skulls or dissection), thus making their results difficult to compare12,43. In addition, these studies were made on a small number of individuals, (domestic animals and rodents usually) even though studies in humans have shown that the orientation of the plane of the LSC can vary a lot within species (e.g. Caix and Outrequin, 1979). As such, though generally accepted, robust evidence in support of the hypothesis that the orientation of the plane of the LSC is horizontal when the animal is at rest, and can, therefore, be used to reconstruct head posture in extinct species, is still pending.

This study implements a large scale and methodologically homogenous critical assessment of the question using a statistical approach in modern “ungulates” (Perissodactyla, Artiodactyla, and Paenungulata). The aim is to document the actual, neutral head posture in life of modern species (using field observations) in order to compare this to the head posture inferred from LSC orientation in a dry skull (using CT scanning). This will enable us to address if the orientation of the plane of the LSC is a good proxy to reconstruct the head posture of extinct species, in order to ultimately make future paleobiological reconstructions more reliable. We will also test if some variables such as diet, body size, habitat, and head-butting are significantly correlated to head posture and/or LSC orientation as is usually believed24,30,45,47.

Materials and methods

Sampling

As the inclusion of a statistically-significant number of taxa was essential to this study, we chose to focus primarily on ungulate-grade mammals (i.e. Paenungulata, Artiodactyla, Perissodactyla, and Tubulidentata). Ungulates are more abundant than carnivores or primates in zoos, easily identifiable, and well represented in institutional dry skull collections. They display a wide array of body sizes, a greater variety of documented head postures47, a wider range of expected inner ear orientations (as hypothesized from the inclination of the snout compared to that of the brain-case48), and more varied degrees of adaptation to head-butting34 than any other mammalian group. Moreover, they are the ideal target group to address if diet (browsing v. grazing) plays a significant role in the orientation of the LSC, as previously suggested in the literature20,24. Finally, they usually display an elongated snout, which makes it easier to compare the orientation of the head in live animals to that of the corresponding dry skulls.

Head posture in live animals

Head posture was documented by taking pictures of zoo animals in lateral view using a camera equipped with a spirit level (Fig. 1) to ensure that pictures were taken as close to the horizontal plane as possible. The animals were photographed in 2018 and 2019 at the National Zoological Garden, Pretoria (South Africa), Johannesburg Zoo (South Africa), Montecasino Bird Garden, Fourways (South Africa), Lory Park Animal and Owl Sanctuary, Midrand (South Africa), Ménagerie du Jardin des Plantes, Paris (France), Parc Zoologique de Paris (France), Prague Zoo (Czech Republic), Chester Zoo (United Kingdom), Zoologischer Garten Berlin (Germany), Tierpark Berlin (Germany), and Zooparc of Beauval (France). The saiga antelope pictures were kindly provided by K.H. Vogel. The dataset represents about 10,000 pictures documenting the head posture of 129 species and is available here: https://osf.io/4vpnj/?view_only=3dc987012fcd44a6a64ad7d8949ec01f (https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/4VPNJ). The pictures were taken from outside the enclosures to avoid interaction with the animals. It was essential for this study that the animals remain calm and act naturally, so their environment was not disturbed, and the animals were not put on leash or isolated. As such, individual identification was not possible. Representatives of both sexes are mixed in the dataset as sexes could not always be determined. The typical photography set up is illustrated in Fig. 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Protocol to photograph animal head posture. (a) An animal stands or slowly walks The camera is positioned to photograph the animal in lateral view. (b) A spirit-level mounted on top of the camera ensures that the picture is captured when the camera is held horizontally. (c) The borders of the resulting picture are parallel to the horizontal plane (which enables measurement of neutral head posture).

To ensure that the photographed head postures were comparable between individuals and species, all pictures were taken by one of the authors only (J.B., except for the saigas, which were taken by Alexander Sliwa from the Kölner Zoo). The pictures used for this study were selected to reflect as closely as possible what will hereafter be referred to as the “neutral” head posture. The neutral posture of an “ungulate” is here defined as the angle between the main axis of the head and the horizontal when an animal’s head remains still, its attention is not attracted by a moving or immobile target, and it is not foraging, drinking, or performing any other identifiable activity involving head movements (e.g. sniffing). The animal can be standing or lying down. “Neutral” head posture differs from head posture “at rest” as it encompasses ruminating animals and individuals slowly walking with their head steady (not pitching up and down while moving). Alert postures49 were included only if the animal's attention was not directed toward an identifiable direction, and were avoided as much as possible. That is why this study focuses on zoo animals, which are accustomed to human presence. For consistency and to enable comparisons, pictures of semiaquatic “ungulates” (e.g. hippos) were taken when the animal’s head was not immersed so that their head posture was not influenced by buoyancy.

The orientation of the head compared to the horizontal plane was measured by J.B. using ImageJ as the angle between the horizontal border of the picture (horizontality of which was ensured by the use of a spirit level on the camera, Fig. 1) and the main axis of the head (traced as the axis running from just above the upper lip to the middle of the occiput on the back of the head) in strict lateral view (Fig. 2a). An average neutral head posture was then calculated for each species (Table 1). The intraspecific standard deviation (measurement error) for neutral head posture is ± 1.6°.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Measurement protocols illustrated on a white rhinoceros (Ceratotherium simum). (a) Neutral head posture measured from the photographs (see Fig. 1). (b) Reconstructed cranial orientation from the CT-scans. (c) A combination of the two measurements that illustrates how the orientation of the plane of the laterals semicircular canal compares to the horizontal.

Table 1.

Dataset used for the statistic analyses.

Species Order Sub-order Average reconstructed cranial orientation (in °) Average neutral head posture (in °) Body mass (in kg) Diet: browser/grazer/mixed/other Frontal head to head butting: yes/no Habitat: open/closed/mixed/rocky/semi-aquatic
Elephas maximus Afrotheria Paenungulata ? 39 (n = 105) 2720 Mixed Yes Mixed
Heterohyrax brucei Afrotheria Paenungulata ? 32 (n = 95) 2 Browser No Rocky
Loxodonta africana Afrotheria Paenungulata 28 (n = 3) 48 (n = 103) 4000 Mixed Yes Open
Procavia capensis Afrotheria Paenungulata 21 (n = 4) 19 (n = 121) 4 Grazer No Rocky
Orycteropus afer Afrotheria Tubulidentata 41 (n = 2) 42 (n = 37) 70 Other No Open
Hexaprotodon liberiensis Artiodactyla Hippopotamidae 27 (n = 2) 50 (n = 27) 262 Browser No Semi-aquatic
Hippopotamus amphibius Artiodactyla Hippopotamidae 20 (n = 3) 59 (n = 74) 2300 Grazer No Semi-aquatic
Addax nasomaculatus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 45 (n = 1) 48 (n = 75) 93 Grazer Yes Open
Aepyceros melampus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 53 (n = 2) 24 (n = 41) 58 Mixed Yes Open
Alcelaphus buselaphus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 68 (n = 8) 55 (n = 67) 185 Grazer Yes Open
Alces alces Artiodactyla Ruminantia 44 (n = 2) 33 (n = 28) 543 Browser Yes Closed
Ammotragus lervia Artiodactyla Ruminantia 51 (n = 1) 43 (n = 71) 103 Mixed Yes Rocky
Antidorcas marsupialis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 42 (n = 3) 38 (n = 61) 43 Mixed Yes Open
Antilocapra americana Artiodactyla Ruminantia 42 (n = 1) 36 (n = 140) 55 Browser Yes Open
Antilope cervicapra Artiodactyla Ruminantia 35 (n = 3) 23 (n = 121) 37 Grazer Yes Open
Axis axis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 46 (n = 1) 27 (n = 122) 78 Grazer Yes Open
Axis kuhlii Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 24 (n = 8) 36 Grazer ? Open
Axis porcinus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 35 (n = 1) 28 (n = 117) 43 Grazer ? Open
Babyrousa babyrussa Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 38 (n = 13) 90 Other No Closed
Babyrousa celebensis Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 29 (n = 18) 90 Other No Closed
Bison bison Artiodactyla Ruminantia 70 (n = 3) 51 (n = 119) 613 Grazer Yes Mixed
Bison bonasus Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 49 (n = 44) 610 Mixed Yes Mixed
Bos gaurus Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 33 (n = 37) 825 Mixed No Closed
Bos grunniens Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 49 (n = 13) 775 Grazer Yes Open
Bos javanicus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 32 (n = 1) 29 (n = 71) 650 Grazer ? Closed
Bos taurus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 36 (n = 2) 39 (n = 89) 650 Grazer Yes Open
Boselaphus tragocamelus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 36 (n = 4) 28 (n = 127) 205 Mixed No Open
Bubalus bubalis Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 31 (n = 112) 771 Mixed Yes Closed
Bubalus depressicornis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 51 (n = 3) 30 (n = 81) 250 Mixed ? Closed
Bubalus mindorensis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 49 (n = 1) ? 240 Grazer No Closed
Budorcas taxicolor Artiodactyla Ruminantia 54 (n = 2) 46 (n = 121) 300 Mixed Yes Closed
Capra caucasica Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 36 (n = 60) 96 Mixed Yes Rocky
Capra falconeri Artiodactyla Ruminantia 52 (n = 1) 30 (n = 48) 71 Mixed Yes Closed
Capra hircus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 45 (n = 4) 32 (n = 164) 80 Mixed Yes Open
Capra ibex Artiodactyla Ruminantia 56 (n = 3) 25 (n = 72) 99 Grazer Yes Rocky
Capreolus capreolus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 43 (n = 1) 28 (n = 92) 28 Mixed No Mixed
Cephalophus dorsalis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 40 (n = 3) ? 20 Browser No Closed
Cephalophus leucogaster Artiodactyla Ruminantia 38 (n = 4) ? 18 Browser No Closed
Cephalophus natalensis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 30 (n = 3) 25 (n = 68) 13 Browser No Closed
Cephalophus niger Artiodactyla Ruminantia 36 (n = 3) 15 (n = 100) 20 Browser No Closed
Cephalophus nigrifrons Artiodactyla Ruminantia 31 (n = 5) ? 17 Browser No Closed
Cephalophus sylvicultor Artiodactyla Ruminantia 43 (n = 3) 31 (n = 148) 63 Browser No Closed
Cervus albirostris Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 30 (n = 61) 165 Grazer Yes Mixed
Cervus elaphus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 41 (n = 5) 22 (n = 186) 128 Mixed Yes Mixed
Cervus nippon Artiodactyla Ruminantia 35 (n = 1) 19 (n = 20) 95 Mixed Yes Closed
Connochaetes gnou Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 62 (n = 14) 134 Mixed Yes Open
Connochaetes taurinus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 68 (n = 4) 60 (n = 74) 213 Grazer Yes Open
Dama dama Artiodactyla Ruminantia 48 (n = 3) 25 (n = 69) 35 Grazer Yes Mixed
Damaliscus lunatus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 55 (n = 6) ? 155 Grazer Yes Open
Damaliscus pygargus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 54 (n = 2) 50 (n = 81) 71 Grazer Yes Open
Elaphodus cephalophus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 27 (n = 1) 33 (n = 15) 22 Mixed No Closed
Elaphurus davidianus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 46 (n = 2) 38 (n = 73) 183 Grazer No Open
Gazella dorcas Artiodactyla Ruminantia 30 (n = 3) ? 15 Browser No Open
Gazella spekei Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 28 (n = 154) 20 Mixed ? Open
Gazella subgutturosa Artiodactyla Ruminantia 42 (n = 2) ? 28 Mixed No Open
Gazella thomsonii Artiodactyla Ruminantia 46 (n = 3) ? 21 Grazer Yes Open
Giraffa camelopardalis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 32 (n = 5) 24 (n = 187) 1317 Browser No Open
Hemitragus jemlahicus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 59 (n = 2) 40 (n = 79) 75 Mixed Yes Rocky
Hippocamelus sp. Artiodactyla Ruminantia 56 (n = 1) ? 55 Browser ? Mixed
Hippotragus equinus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 46 (n = 1) 36 (n = 78) 262 Grazer Yes Open
Hippotragus niger Artiodactyla Ruminantia 44 (n = 2) 40 (n = 140) 228 Grazer Yes Open
Hydropotes inermis Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 28 (n = 5) 12 Mixed No Open
Kobus ellipsiprymnus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 42 (n = 4) 31 (n = 76) 211 Grazer Yes Mixed
Kobus leche Artiodactyla Ruminantia 46 (n = 2) 27 (n = 59) 91 Grazer Yes Open
Kobus megaceros Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 17 (n = 68) 90 Grazer Yes Open
Kobus vardoni Artiodactyla Ruminantia 39 (n = 2) ? 66 Grazer Yes Open
Litocranius walleri Artiodactyla Ruminantia 33 (n = 3) 14 (n = 181) 41 Browser No Open
Madoqua sp. Artiodactyla Ruminantia 25 (n = 3) 24 (n = 97) 6 Browser No Open
Mazama americana Artiodactyla Ruminantia 35 (n = 1) ? 17 Mixed No Closed
Moschus moschiferus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 25 (n = 1) 32 (n = 16) 12 Mixed No Closed
Muntiacus reevesi Artiodactyla Ruminantia 43 (n = 2) 30 (n = 78) 21 Mixed No Closed
Naemorhedus crispus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 50 (n = 1) ? 38 Mixed No Rocky
Naemorhedus goral Artiodactyla Ruminantia 41 (n = 2) 38 (n = 33) 29 Mixed No Rocky
Naemorhedus griseus Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 38 (n = 10) 29 Mixed ? Rocky
Naemorhedus sumatraensis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 47 (n = 3) ? 29 Mixed ? Closed
Nanger dama Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 23 (n = 41) 46 Mixed Yes Open
Nanger granti Artiodactyla Ruminantia 39 (n = 4) ? 56 Mixed Yes Open
Nanger soemmeringii Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 25 (n = 108) 42 Mixed Yes Open
Neotragus batesi Artiodactyla Ruminantia 28 (n = 3) ? 4 Browser No Closed
Nesotragus moschatus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 26 (n = 2) ? 4 Browser No Closed
Nilgiritragus hylocrius Artiodactyla Ruminantia 61 (n = 2) ? 90 Grazer Yes Rocky
Odocoileus virginianus Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 19 (n = 22) 79 Mixed Yes Closed
Okapia johnstoni Artiodactyla Ruminantia 40 (n = 2) 34 (n = 193) 283 Browser No Closed
Oreamnos americanus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 50 (n = 3) 52 (n = 52) 91 Grazer No Rocky
Oreotragus oreotragus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 34 (n = 3) 15 (n = 262) 14 Browser Yes Rocky
Oryx beisa Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 48 (n = 48) 163 Mixed Yes Open
Oryx dammah Artiodactyla Ruminantia 46 (n = 1) 44 (n = 87) 137 Grazer Yes Open
Oryx gazella Artiodactyla Ruminantia 49 (n = 2) 45 (n = 60) 163 Grazer Yes Open
Oryx leucoryx Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 43 (n = 60) 75 Grazer Yes Open
Ourebia ourebi Artiodactyla Ruminantia 31 (n = 3) ? 12 Grazer No Open
Ovibos moschatus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 42 (n = 2) 46 (n = 21) 475 Mixed Yes Open
Ovis ammon Artiodactyla Ruminantia 36 (n = 1) 23 (n = 53) 163 Mixed Yes Rocky
Ovis aries Artiodactyla Ruminantia 37 (n = 2) 22 (n = 104) 110 Mixed Yes Open
Ovis canadensis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 55 (n = 2) ? 98 Browser Yes Rocky
Ovis orientalis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 31 (n = 1) 28 (n = 11) 48 Grazer Yes Rocky
Panolia eldii Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 20 (n = 60) 93 Grazer ? Open
Pantholops hodgsonii Artiodactyla Ruminantia 46 (n = 3) ? 33 Grazer Yes Open
Pelea capreolus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 45 (n = 3) ? 25 Mixed No Open
Philantomba monticola Artiodactyla Ruminantia 16 (n = 1) 26 (n = 23) 7 Browser No Closed
Procapra gutturosa Artiodactyla Ruminantia 38 (n = 3) ? 30 Grazer No Open
Pseudois nayaur Artiodactyla Ruminantia 65 (n = 1) 28 (n = 73) 55 Grazer Yes Rocky
Pudu puda Artiodactyla Ruminantia 22 (n = 1) 27 (n = 30) 10 Browser No Closed
Rangifer tarandus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 45 (n = 2) 35 (n = 189) 99 Grazer Yes Mixed
Raphicerus campestris Artiodactyla Ruminantia 28 (n = 5) 28 (n = 9) 11 Mixed No Open
Redunca arundinum Artiodactyla Ruminantia 34 (n = 2) 23 (n = 128) 73 Grazer Yes Open
Redunca fulvorufula Artiodactyla Ruminantia 39 (n = 3) 23 (n = 33) 29 Grazer Yes Open
Rucervus duvauceli Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 23 (n = 17) 180 Grazer ? Open
Rupicapra rupicapra Artiodactyla Ruminantia 42 (n = 2) 36 (n = 84) 37 Grazer Yes Rocky
Rusa alfredi Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 24 (n = 10) 53 Mixed ? Closed
Rusa timorensis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 47 (n = 1) 29 (n = 32) 117 Grazer ? Open
Rusa unicolor Artiodactyla Ruminantia 38 (n = 1) 21 (n = 17) 273 Mixed Yes Mixed
Saiga tatarica Artiodactyla Ruminantia 43 (n = 2) 38 (n = 2) 48 Grazer Yes Open
Sylvicapra grimmia Artiodactyla Ruminantia 30 (n = 4) ? 19 Browser No Mixed
Syncerus caffer caffer Artiodactyla Ruminantia 51 (n = 2) 27 (n = 70) 648 Grazer Yes Open
Syncerus caffer nanus Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 24 (n = 72) 320 Mixed No Closed
Taurotragus derbianus Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 27 (n = 55) 700 Browser Yes Closed
Taurotragus oryx Artiodactyla Ruminantia 49 (n = 4) 30 (n = 53) 470 Browser Yes Mixed
Tetracerus quadricornis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 31 (n = 3) ? 17 Browser No Mixed
Tragelaphus angasii Artiodactyla Ruminantia 29 (n = 3) 22 (n = 76) 78 Mixed Yes Closed
Tragelaphus euryceros Artiodactyla Ruminantia 25 (n = 1) 26 (n = 197) 330 Browser No Closed
Tragelaphus imberbis Artiodactyla Ruminantia 46 (n = 1) 21 (n = 252) 82 Browser Yes Closed
Tragelaphus scriptus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 26 (n = 3) 25 (n = 59) 53 Browser Yes Closed
Tragelaphus spekei Artiodactyla Ruminantia 35 (n = 1) 20 (n = 125) 98 Mixed Yes Closed
Tragelaphus strepsiceros Artiodactyla Ruminantia 41 (n = 4) 23 (n = 78) 214 Browser Yes Mixed
Tragulus javanicus Artiodactyla Ruminantia 33 (n = 2) 15 (n = 144) 4 Browser No Closed
Tragulus nigricans Artiodactyla Ruminantia ? 12 (n = 50) 4 Browser No Closed
Catagonus wagneri Artiodactyla Suoidea ? 46 (n = 47) 35 Browser No Closed
Phacochoerus africanus Artiodactyla Suoidea 55 (n = 12) 46 (n = 75) 73 Grazer Yes Open
Potamochoerus porcus Artiodactyla Suoidea 45 (n = 1) 53 (n = 30) 85 Other Yes Closed
Sus barbatus Artiodactyla Suoidea ? 55 (n = 7) 97 Other ? Closed
Sus cebifrons Artiodactyla Suoidea ? 51 (n = 14) 28 Browser ? Closed
Sus scrofa Artiodactyla Suoidea 37 47 (n = 79) 200 Other No Closed
Tayassu pecari Artiodactyla Suoidea ? 43 (n = 21) 33 Other No Closed
Camelus bactrianus Artiodactyla Tylopoda 15 (n = 2) 2 (n = 136) 570 Mixed No Open
Camelus dromedarius Artiodactyla Tylopoda 13 (n = 3) -1 (n = 127) 450 Browser No Open
Lama guanicoe Artiodactyla Tylopoda ? 15 (n = 89) 120 Grazer No Open
Lama lama Artiodactyla Tylopoda ? 9 (n = 48) 94 Grazer No Rocky
Vicugna pacos Artiodactyla Tylopoda 23 (n = 1) 17 (n = 55) 66 Grazer No Open
Vicugna vicugna Artiodactyla Tylopoda 17 (n = 1) 20 (n = 66) 50 Grazer No Rocky
Equus asinus Perissodactyla Equidae 53 (n = 2) 58 (n = 88) 275 Mixed No Open
Equus burchelli Perissodactyla Equidae 47 (n = 2) 59 (n = 109) 280 Grazer No Open
Equus caballus Perissodactyla Equidae 44 (n = 4) 59 (n = 135) 250 Grazer No Open
Equus grevyi Perissodactyla Equidae ? 66 (n = 182) 401 Grazer No Open
Equus hemionus Perissodactyla Equidae ? 58 (n = 117) 325 Grazer No Open
Equus zebra Perissodactyla Equidae 50 (n = 1) 61 (n = 156) 274 Grazer No Open
Ceratotherium simum Perissodactyla Rhinocerotidae 38 (n = 9) 57 (n = 140) 1930 Grazer No Open
Dicerorhinus sumatrensis Perissodactyla Rhinocerotidae 25 (n = 1) ? 1400 Browser ? Closed
Diceros bicornis Perissodactyla Rhinocerotidae 36 (n = 4) 35 (n = 80) 1129 Browser No Open
Rhinoceros sondaicus Perissodactyla Rhinocerotidae 28 (n = 2) ? 1890 Browser ? Semi-aquatic
Rhinoceros unicornis Perissodactyla Rhinocerotidae 34 (n = 2) 17 (n = 83) 1416 Mixed No Open
Tapirus bairdii Perissodactyla Tapiridae ? 21 (n = 34) 200 Browser No Semi-aquatic
Tapirus indicus Perissodactyla Tapiridae 14 (n = 1) 28 (n = 117) 263 Browser No Semi-aquatic
Tapirus terrestris Perissodactyla Tapiridae 25 (n = 3) 23 (n = 149) 200 Browser No Semi-aquatic

Data on body mass, diet, head-butting behaviour, and habitat are from the literature (see Supplementary Table S1). n represents the number of measurements.

The bony labyrinth is one of the first organs to completely ossify in mammals as its adult size and shape are reached at mid-gestation50,51. However, the orientation of the LSC seems to show age-related variations in some tetrapod species, including humans, which may impact their head posture26,52,53. As such, juveniles were excluded from the dataset.

Head posture from dry skulls

A total of 285 medical quality CT-scans and micro-CT scan representing 118 species mostly from the American Museum of Natural History (AMNH), Ditsong Museum (AZ and TM), Evolutionary Studies Institute of the University of the Witwatersrand (BP), Wits Life Science Museum (WLSM), School of Anatomical Science of the University of the Witwatersrand (MS and ZA), Natural History Museum of Basel (NMB), Yale Peabody Museum of Natural History (YPM), and Zoological Museum of the University of Zurich (ZM) (see the Supplementary Table S1 for details), were used.

The bony labyrinths of each skull were segmented manually and reconstructed in 3D using the software AVIZO 9 (FEI VSG, Hillsboro OR, USA) at the virtual imaging labs of the Evolutionary Studies Institute and the Natural History Museum of Basel. The skull was reconstructed using either the Isosurface or threshold functions under the same software. The angle between the plane of the LSC and the main axis of the skull was then measured in lateral view in 2D (Fig. 2b). The plane of the LSC was determined visually in lateral view, following most previous authors19,20,22,2426. The main axis of the skull was traced as the axis running from just above the premaxilla (approximately at the level of the centre of the nasal opening) to the middle of the occiput (Fig. 2b) in order to maximize the homology with the measurements taken on living animals. This angle represents the anterior tilting of the head if the LSC is considered horizontal. This angle is hereafter referred to as “the reconstructed cranial orientation” or “reconstructed head posture”. Measurements were taken bilaterally when both bony labyrinths were available and then averaged for each species (Table 1). For consistency, all measurements were taken by the same author (J.B.). None of the samples expressed strong lateral tilting of the LSC or an undulating morphology that could impede taking this measurement or affect its accuracy. The intraspecific standard deviation (measurement error) for reconstructed head posture is ± 2.1°.

The complete dataset of reconstructed head postures is available in the Supplementary Table S1. This dataset was complemented by measurements made on the published pictures from Girard and Schellhorn5,30 (see Supplementary Table S1).

As for the picture dataset, only the individuals showing reasonable signs of maturity (e.g. cranial bone fusion, erupted molars) were considered.

Data processing

The dataset was analyzed using phylogenetic comparative methods to control for the non-independence of observations5456. We used the time-calibrated phylogenetic tree of mammals of Bininda-Emonds et al.57 because it encompasses all the species in our dataset and fossils can be easily added to it in future analyses. The tree was pruned to match the species in our dataset using function ‘drop.tip’ in R package ape58. All subsequent analyses were performed in R v3.6.3 (R Core Team, 2020). The phylogenetic signal of individual variables was estimated using Pagel’s lambda59 for continuous features (reconstructed cranial orientation, neutral head posture, body mass) using function ‘phylosig’ in package phytools60. Lambda was chosen over the other commonly used estimator K61 because of the latter’s poor performance for trees with small sample sizes and polytomies62,63, both of which can be found in our dataset. For binary traits (head-butting; see below), phylogenetic signal was estimated with the D-statistic64 using function ‘phylo.d’ in package caper65. To test whether the plane of the LSC can be used as a reliable proxy to reconstruct the neutral head posture, we regressed the neutral head posture of living animals on the reconstructed cranial orientation using data in Table 1, and phylogenetic generalized least squares (PGLS) regressions66,67. PGLS were compiled using the ‘gls’ function in package nlme68, with correlation structures for each evolutionary model specified in ape58. A model selection procedure based on the corrected Akaike information criterion (AICc) was applied to the regressions using the package AICcmodavg69. Five evolutionary models were considered for this selection procedure (see70): Brownian Motion, Pagel’s Lambda, Ornstein–Uhlenbeck, Early Burst, and White Noise – i.e. non-phylogenetic, ordinary least squares (OLS) regression. All regressions were performed using raw and log-transformed data (natural logarithm). Both variables in the models are in the same unit and order of magnitude, and the models built with raw data showed a higher significance and met parametric assumptions better than models built with log-transformed data. For this reason, we used the former to assess the relationship between the two variables.

Because of the high degree of body mass allometry in neuroanatomical features7173, body mass measurements for all species in the sample were taken from the literature (Supplementary Table S1) and included as a co-predictor to be tested against models built with only the reconstructed and neutral head postures as predictors in the AICc-based model selection procedures. The coefficient of determination and p-value for generalized least squares regressions cannot be compiled straightforwardly due to the autocorrelated structure of the residuals67. Following Paradis55, we compiled a pseudo-R-squared and p-value based on McFadden’s formula74, based on a likelihood ratio test between our model and a null model. Normality and homoscedasticity of the residuals were assessed using a Shapiro–Wilk test and a Q-Q plot, and graphically using residuals v. fit plots, respectively75.

Finally, phylogenetic one-way Analyses of Variance (phylANOVA)76 with False Discovery Rate posthoc corrections77 were used to test for a difference between groups in three separate factors, for both reconstructed cranial orientation and neutral head posture (Table 1). The first factor is diet, for which species were categorized as “browser”, “grazer”, “mixed” (for a mixed diet between browsing and grazing), or “other” (for omnivorous and myrmecophagous species). The second predictor is whether a species practices head-to-head combat. The head-butting category includes wrestling and ramming species (hereafter referred to simply as head-butting species) but excludes flank-butting species (e.g. giraffes). The last predictor is the habitat, which was scored between open (savannah or steppes), closed (forest or jungle), mixed (mix of open and closed habitats), rocky (for species living on steep, rocky slopes), or semi-aquatic. The scoring of all three predictors was done using the literature (see the list in Table 1). PhylANOVAs were performed using function ‘phylANOVA’ in phytools60.

Ethics declarations

As the animals were not approached or armed, no ethical clearance was necessary for this study.

Results

All the data in the dataset for which a phylogenetic signal could be measured (neutral head posture, reconstructed cranial orientation, body mass, and head-butting) carry a strong phylogenetic signal (lambda > 0.8 for the first three variables; D = − 0.2841056 for head-butting) (See Supplementary Table S1).

Species with body mass under 100 kg have an average neutral head posture of 30° and reconstructed cranial orientation of 39°, whereas species larger than 100 kg have a neutral head posture averaging 37° and average cranial orientation of 40°. This suggests an effect of body mass on head posture as was hypothesized by Köhler47, but not on the orientation of the LSC (Fig. 3). This is consistent with statistical analyses, which identify a very weak effect of body mass on neutral head posture (R2 = 0.040; p-value = 0.014), and none on reconstructed cranial orientation (R2 = 0.023; p-value = 0.054) using OLS. However, once corrected for phylogeny using PGLS, the effect of body mass on head posture (R2 = 0.030, p-value = 0.06025) and cranial orientation (R2 = 0.012, p-value = 0.9903) is no longer significant.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Boxplot of neutral head posture (left) and reconstructed cranial orientation (right) in degrees (°) in species with body mass below 100 kg (< 100 kg) and species with body mass superior or equal to 100 kg (≥ 100 kg). The average angle for each category is indicated in the corresponding boxplot. n represents the number of species in each category.

Phylogenetic regressions (Fig. 4a) identify a statistically significant (p-value = 4.519e−07) but relatively low correlation (R2 = 0.261) between neutral head posture and the reconstructed cranial orientation. This supports that the orientation of the LSC in life is correlated to the neutral head posture in “ungulates”. The equation of the linear model is:

Neutral head posture=0.384×reconstructed cranial orientation+13.468. 1

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Phylogenetic regressions of neutral head posture of living mammals plotted against the reconstructed cranial orientation (in °) without body mass (a) and with body mass included as a co-predictor (b). Abbreviations: R2, coefficient of determination.

The 95% confidence interval for the slope (0.242–0.526) is significantly different from 1, which means that this model cannot be approximated to an isometric relationship (which would be expected if the LSC was held horizontally).

The model including body mass, neutral, and reconstructed head postures, and the interaction term of the three as co-predictors was selected by AICc as fitting our data best (Fig. 4b). This model shows results very similar to those of the simple regression model, being significant with a slightly stronger correlation (R2 = 0.325; p-value = 3.235e−07). The equation of the resulting model is written:

Neutral head posture=11.677+0.328×reconstructed cranial orientation-0.001×body masskg-0.0002×reconstructed cranial orientation×body mass(kg). 2

The slope confidence interval is slightly lower than that of the previous model (for reconstructed cranial orientation: 0.148–0.508), which removes it even further from an isometric relationship (Fig. 4b). The very low coefficients for body mass and reconstructed orientation × body mass are not significantly different from zero (see Supplementary Table S1), and are reported here to ensure full transparency of our results.

Surprisingly, for both simple and multiple regression models, the evolutionary model selected by AICc was the Early Burst (EB) model61, representing a rapid adaptive radiation followed by stasis. EB models are known to be rarely selected as the best evolutionary model in such selection procedures78.

On average, browsers tend to hold their heads less tilted anteriorly (26°) than mixed feeders (32°), and grazers (36°) in neutral posture (Fig. 5). This seems to reflect on the reconstructed cranial orientation as browsers have a higher reconstructed head posture (33°), than mixed feeders (40°) and grazers (44°) (Fig. 5). Phylogenetic ANOVAs indicate a significant difference between browsers and grazers for the reconstructed orientation of the skull (F = 7.723; p-value = 0.046), but not for the neutral head posture (F = 2.663; p-value = 0.516). Mixed feeders are statistically indiscernible from both browsers and grazers in any case.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Boxplot of neutral head posture (left) and reconstructed cranial orientation (right) in degrees (°) in browsers (green), mixed feeders (orange), grazers (yellow), and omnivorous + myrmecophagous species (other) (grey). The average angle for each category is indicated in the corresponding boxplot. n represents the number of species in each category.

Variations of reconstructed cranial orientation do not show any trend with habitat preference (Fig. 6), but a slight trend toward more downwardly tilted head postures seems to occur in species living in a more open habitat (Fig. 6); however, this trend is not significant (F = 1.343; p-value = 0.792). Semi-aquatic species seem to have a more posteriorly tilted LSC resulting in a higher reconstructed cranial orientation (23°) than fully terrestrial species, but this does not reflect on the neutral head posture (Fig. 6). Unfortunately, the sample size for this category was too low to effectively test if this difference was significant or simply the result of the scarcity of semi-aquatic species in the dataset.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Boxplot of neutral head posture (left) and reconstructed cranial orientation (right) in degrees (°) in species living in closed (green), mixed (orange), open (yellow), rocky (grey), and semiaquatic (S-Aq) (blue) habitats. The average angle for each category is indicated in the corresponding boxplot. n represents the number of species in each category.

Head-butting is found to have a highly significant statistical effect on reconstructed head posture (F = 39.467; p-value = 0.002), with a difference of 13° between head-butting and non-head-butting species on average (Fig. 7); however, with only 1° difference on average (Fig. 7), the same is not true for the neutral head posture (F = 3.126; p-value = 0.591).

Figure 7.

Figure 7

Boxplot of neutral head posture (left) and reconstructed cranial orientation (right) in degrees (°) in non-head-butting (No H-B) and head butting (H-B) species. The average angle for each category is indicated in the corresponding boxplot. n represents the number of species in each category.

Discussion

LSC orientation is correlated with head posture, but is not horizontal

The assumption that the plane of the LSC can be used as a reliable indicator of the horizontal plane on dry skulls and thus can serve as a proxy to reconstruct the “habitual” or “alert” head posture in extinct species has been a long-held17, yet insufficiently tested hypothesis in mammals. Attempts to test this hypothesis have highlighted that the plane of the LSC is often tilted upward compared to the horizontal in most mammalian species5,6,11,30,45. A famously baffling example is that of humans, in which aligning the plane of the LSC to the horizontal plane results in a “habitual” posture of the head inclined 30° down anteriorly16,8,11,12,18,46. In the current study, the steenbuck (Raphicerus campestris) is the only species in which the average neutral and reconstructed head postures are the same (Table 1), which means that, on average, the LSC is parallel to the horizontal plane when the steenbuck’s head posture is neutral. Among the species for which the neutral head posture and reconstructed cranial orientation could be compared, only half of them show a difference between the averages of the two that is below 10°. As such, even though the plane of the LSC should be horizontal on theoretical grounds13, this is not the rule in “ungulates”.

The two phylogenetic regressions provided here (Fig. 4) are the first large sample size attempts to address the existence and nature of a correlation between the orientation of the plane of the LSC and the neutral head posture in mammals across a large taxonomic sampling. We find that whether corrected or not for body mass, the correlation between the reconstructed cranial orientation and the neutral head posture is significant (p-value < 0.0001); however, if the plane of the LSC was held horizontally in neutral head posture, the regression line should not differ significantly from an isometric line. Instead, both regression lines have slopes that significantly differ from 1 (Fig. 4), which means that they cannot be approximated by isometric lines. For this reason, though there is a significant correlation between the orientation of the LSC and that of the head in “ungulates”, the plane of the LSC should not be considered horizontal when reconstructing ancient head posture.

According to the phylogenetic regressions, the equation that describes the relationship between the reconstructed cranial orientation and the neutral head posture is given in Eq. (1), and that between cranial orientation, head posture, and body mass is given in Eq. (2). As estimating body mass in extinct species is always contentious7982, the first of these equations may seem more practical to estimate the actual head posture of a given extinct ungulate species. In both cases, the variance of the residuals is high, which might indicate a low predictive power of these models (R2 equals 0.26 and 0.33, respectively).

The misalignment between the plane of the LSC and the horizontal is consistent with the results obtained by Marugán-Lobón et al.43 on birds using Duijm’s dataset8, though their approach to the study of head posture was different from the one presented here, which limits comparisons. The reason why the LSC would not be aligned with the horizontal in the neutral posture is still unclear. It may be explained by the very function of the canals and ampullae, which are meant to record head movements and play crucial roles in the vestibulo-ocular and vestibulo-collic reflexes to compensate for the movements and accelerations of the head compared to the eyes and the rest of the body6,13,14,43. As such, recording movements and monitoring reflexes during locomotion and head movements along the predominant axis of yaw would be the main drivers of LSC adaptations83, which would result in a relaxed selection on the functions performed when the head remains still, such as aligning the plane of the LSC to the horizontal plane during neutral head posture. In support of this hypothesis, a recent study by Dunbar et al.84 on the orientation of the LSC during locomotion in horses found a 66° inclination of the head below the horizontal during slow walk, and a higher head posture of 56° and 55° during trot and canter. They argued that fast locomotion brought the plane of the LSC to about 5° around the horizontal in their specimens84. According to Zubair et al.85, domestic cats keep their LSC about 10° to the horizontal during locomotion, although Hullar12 reports a tilting up to 60° of the LSC in cats during “normal activities”. Primates appear to keep the plane of their LSC within a 20° range about the horizontal during locomotion84, which is consistent with its orientation at rest12. Published data about the orientation of the LSC during locomotion are scarce and some are contradictory. Additionally, they are difficult to acquire as animals rarely maintain a static head posture during locomotion as they often pitch their heads repeatedly84,85. In the future, such data could nevertheless enable testing whether the orientation of the LSC would be a better predictor of the head posture during locomotion rather than at rest.

Another hypothesis is that an overall misalignment of all three semicircular canals would enable all semicircular canals to record a component of horizontal and vertical accelerations43.

The effect of phylogeny

Both the neutral and reconstructed head postures carry an important phylogenetic signal (Lambda equals 0.97 and 0.84, respectively). For paleontologists, this strong phylogenetic signal implies that the best way to predict the head posture of an extinct “ungulate” is to look at the neutral head posture of its modern relatives. In comparison, once the data are corrected for phylogeny, diet is found to have only a weak correlation with the reconstructed cranial orientation (F = 7.723; p-value = 0.046), and no significant effect on the neutral head posture (F = 2.663; p-value = 0.516). This reflects well in the dataset.

The Tylopoda is the group in which the head is the most consistently tilted upward, with an average neutral head posture of 10°. This reflects on their reconstructed cranial orientation which averages 17°. Tylopods nevertheless include grazers (genus Vicugna), mixed feeders (Camelus bactrianus), and browsers (Camelus dromedarius) that all keep their heads relatively high (Figs. 4, 8a). On the other end of the spectrum, pigs display remarkable consistency at holding their head low (average neutral head posture for Suoidea = 46°), with for example the grazing Phacochoerus and the browsing Catagonus both keeping their head 46° below the horizontal plane on average (Table 1, Fig. 4).

Figure 8.

Figure 8

Neutral head posture in three species of Tylopoda (a) and Alcelaphinae (b) contrasting the almost horizontal head posture assumed by Camelus, Lama, and Vicugna with the almost vertical head posture of Alcelaphus, Connochaetes, and Damaliscus.

The species that holds their head the lowest below the horizontal belong to the Equidae (average neutral head posture = 60°; average reconstructed cranial orientation = 49°) and Alcelaphinae (average neutral head posture = 57°; average reconstructed cranial orientation = 61°) (Figs. 4, 8b). The species with the most tilted head posture is the Grevy’s zebra (Equus grevyi), with a 66° tilt on average (Table 1), comparable to the extreme 67° reconstructed cranial orientation of the sauropod dinosaur Nigersaurus20. Tilting of the head was hypothesized to be correlated to body size in “ungulates”47, with small, forest-dwelling species holding their head higher than large species adapted to savannah. A similar trend is found here, with species above 100 kg having their head tilted 37° anteriorly on average whereas species below 100 kg hold their heads 30° below the horizontal on average (Fig. 3). Statistical tests on our dataset find a significant correlation between body mass and head posture (p-value = 0.014), but this correlation is no longer significant once the data are corrected for the effect of phylogeny (p-value = 0.060). This strongly suggests that the trend observed here and by Köhler47 actually reflects the fact that large savannah herbivores belong to just a few clades (e.g. equids, alcelaphins, and hippotragins) whereas the small ones mostly belong to the Antilopinae.

A more significant effect of body mass might nevertheless be found while including very small-bodied species (e.g. rodents, shrews) because their head posture would be more constrained by its proximity to the substrate. Similarly, species with a sprawling posture would also have to keep their head higher, as already observed in many reptiles12,4143.

The relationship between LSC orientation and phylogeny has been empirically anticipated as the way the LSC enters the vestibule (either directly above the posterior ampulla or at different levels within the ampulla) is distinctive between different clades of ruminant50,51,8688.

The selection of an Early Burst model as the best fit for the whole dataset suggests that the evolution of head posture might represent a classic example of adaptive radiation61,78. This may be the effect of the abundance of bovids in our dataset, which originated in the early Neogene and rapidly adapted to a wide variety of ecological niches, diet, body mass, social behavior, and habitat48,89. However, the presence of a strong phylogenetic signal for both head posture variables cannot be directly interpreted as evidence for such a radiation in terms of evolutionary process, which would require further analyses of diversification rates90. The increase of phenotypic divergence resulting in different niches for a given character in a clade does not always correspond to an adaptive radiation, even when it closely matches the underlying phylogeny. This is due to ecological interactions between distantly related species, which can result in a similar timing of evolutionary shifts for distinct clades, the detail of which is often very difficult to decipher without exhaustively sampling each of these clades91. Indeed, even if an early radiation in head posture diversity would be consistent with the high discrepancy between taxonomic groups for both variables (Fig. 4), the small sample size for all groups except Ruminantia prevents a straightforward discussion of specific evolutionary constraints in that context. Furthermore, other factors such as a high proportion of sympatric species in the sample may also artificially increase the fit of an EB evolutionary model78. A larger and broader sampling among mammals is thus likely to blur such a signal and could result in a more homogeneous distribution of residuals that would not necessarily match so closely the observed pattern of relative phylogenetic proximity.

Diet

As grazers have to keep their head low while foraging on grass, whereas browsers have to catch leaves higher in bushes and trees, and because herbivores spend most of their time acquiring low-energetic food49,92,93, it is expected on an evolutionary scale that the skull, neck musculature, and vestibular apparatus of herbivores would adapt to these different feeding strategies and that it would reflect in their head posture, even at rest20,30,45. As such, a gradually more anteriorly tilted neutral head posture is expected as moving from browsing species to mixed feeders, and finally grazers20,30. Schellhorn30 was the first to compare the reconstructed cranial orientation of Rhinocerotidae to their actual head posture (using an open-access database of photographs) and found results consistent with such a gradient.

Measuring the reconstructed cranial orientation from Schellhorn’s published figures and adding our own observations of neutral head posture, we do find a more tilted reconstructed cranial orientation in the grazing Cerathotherium (average reconstructed cranial orientation = 38°), than in the mixed feeder Rhinoceros (average reconstructed cranial orientation = 34°), and the browsing Dicerorhinus and Diceros (average reconstructed cranial orientation = 31°) (Fig. 9); however, this gradient does not reflect on the neutral head posture that shows no particular trend among rhinocerotids (Fig. 9). Despite the large difference between the average head posture of browsers and grazers, the very low head posture of mixed feeder rhinocerotids casts some doubts on the validity of the correlation between head posture and diet (Fig. 9). The Cervidae constitutes a more striking example (Fig. 10). In cervids, the head is consistently kept within about 10° around the average neutral head posture (20°) regardless of diet, and browser and grazers are not distinguishable based on neutral head posture (average = 30° for browser; average = 28° for grazers) or reconstructed cranial orientation (average = 41° for browsers; average = 45° for grazers) (Table 1).

Figure 9.

Figure 9

Boxplot of neutral head posture (left) and reconstructed cranial orientation (right) in degree (°) in browsing (green), mixed feeding (orange), and grazing (yellow) Rhinocerotidae. The average angle for each category is indicated in the corresponding boxplot. n represents the number of measurements in each category.

Figure 10.

Figure 10

Boxplot of neutral head posture (left) and reconstructed cranial orientation (right) in degrees (°) in browsing (green), mixed feeding (orange), and grazing (yellow) Cervidae. The average angle for each category is indicated in the corresponding boxplot. n represents the number of measurements in each category.

The situation in rhinocerotids illustrates that for the whole dataset. Diet is found to have no statistical effect on neutral head posture (p-value = 0.516), and its relationship to the reconstructed cranial orientation is barely significant (p-value = 0.046). Average values for neutral and reconstructed head postures show a visible increase in tilting with a more grass-rich diet, but there is a strong overlap between dietary groups for both variables (Fig. 5). This suggests that even though diet could potentially be reconstructed in extinct species using the orientation of the LSC, caution should be taken as i) browsers and grazers could be statistically discriminated, but mixed feeders could not; ii) the correlation between reconstructed cranial orientation and diet does not seem to reflect on the neutral head posture. The reason why remains unknown; and iii) the high p-value suggests that adding more data (particularly CT data) in the future may affect this correlation.

Semi-aquatic adaptation

No significant result indicative of a correlation between habitat and head posture or reconstructed cranial orientation was found; however, semi-aquatic species show a noticeably high reconstructed posture of the skull on average (23°) compared to other species (Fig. 6). The low number of semi-aquatic species in the dataset (one rhinocerotid, three tapirids, and two hippopotamids, Table 1) likely prevents this trend to be identified as significant in our sample. A high head posture is not observed in semi-aquatic species (Fig. 6), even though it would be expected of species that have to keep breathing above water level most of the time while immersed27,30. Recently, a semi-aquatic habit for the Triassic archosaur Proterosuchus and the therapsid Lystrosaurus has been hypothesized as these two species would have their head tilted upward anteriorly when the plane of the LSC is horizontal27,94 (Fig. 11c). In Proterosuchus this upward tilting would be about 17°27, whereas in Lystrosaurus it would be between 19° and 23°94. However, an upward tilting of the neutral head posture is also habitually observed in many fully terrestrial species, such as Camelus dromedarius and was occasionally spotted in Capra ibex (Fig. 11a; see Supplementary Table S1). Among modern archosaurs, an upward tilting of the head is observed in some sea birds and the common starling Sturnus vulgaris8. An upward tilting of the beak when the dry skull is held with the plane of the LSC horizontal seems to be observed in the razorbill (Alca torda) and the Heron (Ardea cinerea) (as inferred from the figures and measurements in Duijm8, see Supplementary Table S1); however, the reconstructed cranial orientations in the razorbill and heron vary between 0° and -4° only (Fig. 11b).

Figure 11.

Figure 11

Examples of modern and extinct species in which the neutral or reconstructed head posture is tilted upward anteriorly. (a) The neutral head posture of two specimens of the tylopod Camelus dromedarius and caprin Capra ibex. (b) The reconstructed cranial orientation of two birds, Ardea cinerea and Alca torda, according to Duijm8.

Among fossil species, the likely terrestrial sauropod Ngwevu intloko (Fig. 11c) would also have had its reconstructed cranial orientation tilted 17° above the horizontal (identified as Massospondylus carinatus in Sereno et al.20; see Chapelle et al.95). The peculiar orientation of the LSC in Lystrosaurus, Proterosuchus, and Massospondylus is remarkable as such a downward tilting of the LSC superior to 15° has never been found in any modern species to date, particularly not in the semi-aquatic species studied here which all have a posteriorly tilted LSC as all other “ungulates” (average reconstructed cranial orientation of semi-aquatic species = 23°) (Fig. 6; Table 1). The heron and razorbill mentioned above would have an anterior tilting of less than 4° (Supplementary Table S1), and Duijm’s8 dataset includes mostly semi-aquatic species, which limits comparisons. Overall, a correlation between an upward tilting of the LSC and semi-aquatic lifestyle is not supported by current data.

Noteworthily, the most aquatic species of the dataset, Hippopotamus amphibius, stands out on the scatter-plots as an outlier (Fig. 4). This is due to the large difference between its strongly anteriorly tilted neutral head posture and its almost horizontal reconstructed cranial orientation (difference = 39°). Hippopotamus amphibius normally spends very little time on land49, and it can be hypothesized that its bony labyrinth morphology would be more adapted to life in water than on land96. This hypothesis is supported by the orientation of the head while swimming in H. amphibius, which is more consistent with its reconstructed cranial orientation (Fig. 12), and the fact that the difference between the neutral and reconstructed head postures in the more terrestrial Hexaprotodon liberiensis (23°) falls more within the range of variation of “ungulates” (Fig. 4). Further field observations of underwater hippopotamus head posture will be necessary to address this hypothesis.

Figure 12.

Figure 12

The neutral head posture (a), reconstructed cranial orientation (b), and head posture underwater (c) in Hippopotamus amphibius.

The influence of head-butting

Taxa that engage in head-to-head combat have almost the same average neutral orientation of their head as non-head-butting taxa (34° and 33° respectively) (Fig. 7). In sharp contrast, they show a significantly more anteriorly tilted reconstructed cranial orientation (average = 46°) compared to non-head-butting taxa (average = 33°). Unlike what is observed between browsers and grazers, the values here are markedly different (Fig. 7) and the difference is highly statistically significant (p-value = 0.002). It is unlikely that the reconstructed cranial orientation reflects the posture during head-butting, as animals keep their head extremely low during this activity49,92,93, much lower than their corresponding reconstructed cranial orientation (Fig. 13b). A phenomenon of re-orientation of the braincase and basicranium in head-butting “ungulates” that would not affect head posture overall seems more probable. Such cranial flexure would result in a misalignment of the main axis of the braincase with that of the snout (Fig. 13), a condition termed cyptocephaly and commonly encountered in head-butting “ungulates”33,48,92,9799. This implies that natural selection on the alignment of the plane of the LSC to the horizontal was muted by another, likely more important adaptation to head-butting. This may be the necessity to re-orientate the braincase and basicranium in order to align the fighting surface of the skull, occipital condyles, and vertebral column to help dissipate the energy of the impact to the body and away from the brain24,100,101. Another hypothesis would be that the re-organization of the braincase serves to accommodate the development of the large cranial apparatuses found in most head-butting species (e.g. horns and antlers)29. In our dataset, 56% of species without horns do not head-butt, and 83% of species with horns do head-butt (Supplementary Table S1), which would partly support this hypothesis. In contrast, intraspecific variations in the presence or absence of horns appear to have no impact on neutral head posture on average (Fig. 14; Supplementary Table S1). These two hypotheses will need further observations and biomechanical modeling to be addressed adequately.

Figure 13.

Figure 13

Comparison of the cranial (transparent), endocranial (pink), and LSC (green) orientations in a non-head-butting species (Tapirus indicus, a) and a head-butting species (Connochaetes taurinus, b).

Figure 14.

Figure 14

Boxplot of intraspecific variation of neutral head posture in degrees (°) in individuals in which horns, antlers, or ossicones are present or large (Horns) and absent or small (No horns). The average angle for each category is indicated in the corresponding boxplot. n represents the number of measurements in each category.

Concluding remarks

Neutral head posture is here found to be significantly correlated to the orientation of the plane of the LSC in “ungulate” mammals, but this relationship is loose, and it appears that diet and head-butting have an effect on LSC orientation although not on neutral head posture as would be expected. This suggests an overall relaxed constraint on the alignment of the plane of the LSC to the horizontal at rest. Head posture during locomotion and/or adaptation to head-butting might play a more significant role in the orientation of the LSC than its horizontality at rest, two possibilities that will have to be addressed further.

In this contribution, some noteworthy trends between the orientation of the LSC, body mass, diet, adaptation to a semi-aquatic environment, and head-butting are pointed out, although many of these ecological components are difficult to disentangle. “Ungulates” living in closed habitats are often smaller than in open habitats, more solitary, browsers and tend to fight for mates by stabbing each other with their horns and teeth, whereas species from more open habitats usually graze in large herds and perform head-butting to ascertain dominance and attract mates34,36,40,47.

Finally, although this study finds that there is some interesting ecological and behavioral signal in the orientation of the LSC of ungulates that could be exploited by paleontologists, it is crucial to highlight that the phylogenetic signal was highly significant for all the variables examined here and as such, what the orientation of the LSC reflects the best in “ungulates” is their phylogeny more than anything else. Further understanding of the evolutionary processes associated with such a strong phylogenetic disparity will require investigating each subclade in the sample individually and a more exhaustive sample for each of them.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information (877.5KB, xlsx)

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to the zoos that supported this study: SANBI National Zoological Garden, Pretoria (Tracy Rehse), Johannesburg City Parks and Zoo (Dr. Arnold Kanengoni), Montecasino Bird Garden (Elaine Bratt), Lory Park Animal and Owl Sanctuary, Midrand (Kara Heynis), Ménagerie du Jardin des Plantes et Parc Zoologique de Paris, CNRS (Dr. Michel Saint-Jalme and Dr. Alexis Lécu) with the participation of the National Museum of Natural History- France, Prague Zoo (Schneiderová Irena), Conservation and Science, North of England Zoological Society (Chester Zoo), Dr. Tobias Rahde (Zoologischer Garten Berlin), Tierpark Berlin (Dr. Florian Sicks), Zooparc de Beauval, Kölner Zoo (K.H. Vogel for the pictures of the saïga and Alexander Sliwa). The authors are grateful to the following people for enabling access to their CT-scanning facilities: Dr. Elrentia Busy, MD, and Dr. Mark Haagensen, MD, Donald Gordon Medical Centre, Johannesburg, South Africa; the radiology staff at Radiologist Groupe CRP, Clinique du Parc, Castelnau le Lez, and Tridilogy, Saint-Gely-du-Fesc, France; Dr. Renaud Lebrun, Montpellier RIO Imaging, France; the radiology staff of the Zurich University Hospital, Switzerland; and staff at the Department of Radiology, Stony Brook University Hospital, USA. The authors are grateful to the following institutions for access to their collections: Ditsong Museum (Dr. Heidi Fourie), Evolutionary Studies Institute (Dr. Bernhard Zipfel and Dr. Sifelani Jirah), Wits Life Science Museum (Dr. James Harrison), Institut des Sciences de l’Evolution de Montpellier (Suzanne Jicquel, Dr. Rodolphe Tabuce), Zoological Museum of the University of Zurich (Martina Schenkel), American Museum of Natural History (Eileen Westwig), Yale Peabody Museum of Natural History (Kristof Zyskowski). This study was made possible by the support of the DST-NRF Centre of Excellence in Palaeosciences (CoE in Palaeosciences). Opinions expressed and conclusions arrived at, are those of the authors and are not necessarily to be attributed to the CoE in Palaeosciences. MB and LC acknowledge Georg Schulz and Bert Müller for scanning the specimens and the financial support from the Swiss National Science Foundation through projects SNF Project 200021_159854/1 and 200021_178853/1. AF acknowledges financial support from a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship and Society of Integrative and Comparative Biology. JMN was funded by a Leverhulme Trust Early Career Fellowship (ECF-2017-360).

Author contributions

J.B., L.J.L., and P.R.M. designed the study, J.B., A.A.F., L.C., J.M.N., B.M., S.M., and P.R.M. provided the data, J.B. and L.J.L. analysed the data, J.B. and L.J.L. wrote the paper, C.L., A.A.F., B.M., J.M.N., S.M., and P.R.M. provided critical comments. All authors gave final approval for publication.

Data availability

The datasets analysed during the current study are available in the Supplementary Table S1 and online at this URL: https://osf.io/4vpnj/?view_only=3dc987012fcd44a6a64ad7d8949ec01f (10.17605/Osf.Io/4vpnj). Data replicated in the Wits Data Archive and can be guaranteed to be in the holding of the University of Witwatersrand.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-76757-0.

References

  • 1.Schoenemann, A. Schläfenbein und Schädelbasis, eine anatomisch-otiatrische Studie. (Gesellsch., 1906).
  • 2.Benoit-Gonin L, Lafite-Dupont P. Destinée du canal semi-circulaire externe dans le passage de la station quadrupède à la station bipède. C R Soc. Biol. Paris. 1907;62:98–99. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Girard LL. plan des canaux semi-circulaires horizontaux considéré comme plan horizontal de la tête. Bull. Mem. Soc. Anthropol. Paris Ser. 1923;7:14–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Ledebkin L. Über die Lage des Canalis semicircularis lateralis bei Säugern. Anatonzischer Anzeiger. 1924;58:449–460. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Girard L. L’attitude normale de la tête déterminée par le labyrinthe de l’oreille. Bull. Mem. Soc. Anthropol. Paris Serie. 1929;10:79–99. doi: 10.3406/bmsap.1929.9309. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.de Beer GR. How animals hold their heads. Proc. Linn Soc. Lond. 1947;159:125–139. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1947.tb00491.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Van Der Klaauw CJ. Size and position of the functional components of the skull: a contribution to the knowledge of the architecture of the skull, based on data in the literature. Arch. Néerl. Zool. 1948;9:1–556. doi: 10.1163/187530152X00017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Duijm MJ. On the head posture in birds and its relation features. Proceedings of the Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen C. 1951;54:202–271. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Saban, R. Fixité du canal-semicirculaire externe et variations de l’angle thyridien. Mammalia16, (1952).
  • 10.Anthony, J. Justification des principes de la méthode véstibulaire. Mammalia17, (1953).
  • 11.Vidal PP, Graf W, Berthoz A. The orientation of the cervical vertebral column in unrestrained awake animals I. Resting position. Exp. Brain Res. 1986;61:549–559. doi: 10.1007/BF00237580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hullar TE. Semicircular canal geometry, afferent sensitivity, and animal behavior. Anat. Rec. 2006;288A:466–472. doi: 10.1002/ar.a.20304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Graf W, Klam FL. système vestibulaire : anatomie fonctionnelle et comparée, évolution et développement. C R Palevol. 2006;5:637–655. doi: 10.1016/j.crpv.2005.12.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Fitzpatrick RC, Butler JE, Day BL. Resolving Head Rotation for Human Bipedalism. Curr Biol. 2006;16:1509–1514. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.05.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cox PG, Jeffery N. Semicircular canals and agility: the influence of size and shape measures. J Anat. 2010;216:37–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2009.01172.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Malinzak MD, Kay RF, Hullar TE. Locomotor head movements and semicircular canal morphology in primates. PNAS. 2012;109:17914–17919. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206139109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hotton, N. I. Dicynodonts and their role as primary consumers. in The Ecology and Biology of Mammal-like Reptiles (eds. Hotton, N. I., MacLean, P. D., Roth, J. J. & Roth, E. C.) 71–82 (Smithsonian Institution Press, 1986).
  • 18.Spoor F, Wood B, Zonneveld F. Implications of early hominid labyrinthine morphology for evolution of human bipedal locomotion. Nature. 1994;369:645–648. doi: 10.1038/369645a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Witmer LM, Chatterjee S, Franzosa J, Rowe T. Neuroanatomy of flying reptiles and implications for flight, posture and behaviour. Nature. 2003;425:950–953. doi: 10.1038/nature02048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sereno PC, et al. Structural extremes in a cretaceous dinosaur. PLoS ONE. 2007;2:e1230. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Witmer, L. M., Ridgely, R. C., Dufeau, D. L. & Semones, M. C. Using CT to Peer into the Past: 3D Visualization of the Brain and Ear Regions of Birds, Crocodiles, and Nonavian Dinosaurs. in Anatomical Imaging (eds. Endo, H. & Frey, R.) 67–87 (Springer Japan, 2008). doi:10.1007/978-4-431-76933-0_6.
  • 22.Witmer LM, Ridgely RC. New insights into the brain, braincase, and ear region of tyrannosaurs (Dinosauria, Theropoda), with implications for sensory organization and behavior. Anat Rec. 2009;292:1266–1296. doi: 10.1002/ar.20983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Badlangana NL, Adams JW, Manger PR. A comparative assessment of the size of the frontal air sinus in the Giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) Anat. Rec. 2011;294:931–940. doi: 10.1002/ar.21401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Benoit J, Manger PR, Norton L, Fernandez V, Rubidge BS. Synchrotron scanning reveals the palaeoneurology of the head-butting Moschops capensis (Therapsida, Dinocephalia) PeerJ. 2017;5:e3496. doi: 10.7717/peerj.3496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Araújo R, Fernandez V, Polcyn MJ, Fröbisch J, Martins RMS. Aspects of gorgonopsian paleobiology and evolution: insights from the basicranium, occiput, osseous labyrinth, vasculature, and neuroanatomy. PeerJ. 2017;5:e3119. doi: 10.7717/peerj.3119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bullar CM, Zhao Q, Benton MJ, Ryan MJ. Ontogenetic braincase development in Psittacosaurus lujiatunensis (Dinosauria: Ceratopsia) using micro-computed tomography. PeerJ. 2019;7:e7217. doi: 10.7717/peerj.7217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Brown EE, Butler RJ, Ezcurra MD, Bhullar B-AS, Lautenschlager S. Endocranial anatomy and life habits of the Early Triassic archosauriform Proterosuchus fergusi. Palaeontol. 2020;63:255–282. doi: 10.1111/pala.12454. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Schade M, Rauhut OWM, Evers SW. Neuroanatomy of the spinosaurid Irritator challengeri (Dinosauria: Theropoda) indicates potential adaptations for piscivory. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:9259. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-66261-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Sakagami R, Kawabe S. Endocranial anatomy of the ceratopsid dinosaur Triceratops and interpretations of sensory and motor function. PeerJ. 2020;8:e9888. doi: 10.7717/peerj.9888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Schellhorn R. A potential link between lateral semicircular canal orientation, head posture, and dietary habits in extant rhinos (Perissodactyla, Rhinocerotidae) J. Morphol. 2018;279:50–61. doi: 10.1002/jmor.20753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Neenan JM, Scheyer TM. The braincase and inner ear of Placodus gigas (Sauropterygia, Placodontia)—a new reconstruction based on micro-computed tomographic data. J. Vert. Paleontol. 2012;32:1350–1357. doi: 10.1080/02724634.2012.695241. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Seymour RS. Cardiovascular physiology of dinosaurs. Physiology. 2016;31:430–441. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00016.2016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Schaffer, W. M. & Reed, C. A. The co-evolution of social behavior and cranial morphology in sheep and goats (Bovidae, caprini). 1–112 (Field Museum of Natural History, 1972). doi:10.5962/bhl.title.2828.
  • 34.Geist V. The evolution of horn-like organs. Behav. 1966;27:175–214. doi: 10.1163/156853966X00155. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Pérez-Barbería FJ, Gordon IJ. Gregariousness increases brain size in ungulates. Oecologia. 2005;145:41–52. doi: 10.1007/s00442-005-0067-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Emlen DJ. The evolution of animal weapons. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2008;39:387–413. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.39.110707.173502. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Boucot, A. J. & Poinar, G. O. Jr. Fossil Behavior Compendium. (CRC Press, 2011).
  • 38.Snively E, Theodor JM. Common functional correlates of head-strike behavior in the pachycephalosaur Stegoceras validum (Ornithischia, Dinosauria) and combative artiodactyls. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e21422. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Peterson JE, Vittore CP. Cranial pathologies in a specimen of Pachycephalosaurus. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e36227. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Cabrera D, Stankowich T. Stabbing slinkers: tusk evolution among artiodactyls. J. Mammal. Evol. 2018 doi: 10.1007/s10914-018-9453-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Brichta AM, Acuña DL, Peterson EH. Planar relations of semicircular canals in awake, resting turtles Pseudemys scripta. BBE. 1988;32:236–245. doi: 10.1159/000116551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Boistel R, et al. Assisted walking in Malagasy dwarf chamaeleons. Biol. Lett. 2010;6:740–743. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Marugán-Lobón J, Chiappe LM, Farke AA. The variability of inner ear orientation in saurischian dinosaurs: testing the use of semicircular canals as a reference system for comparative anatomy. PeerJ. 2013;1:e124. doi: 10.7717/peerj.124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Taylor MP, Wedel MJ, Naish D. Head and neck posture in sauropod dinosaurs inferred from extant animals. Acta Palaeontol. Pol. 2009;54:213–220. doi: 10.4202/app.2009.0007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Coutier F, Hautier L, Cornette R, Amson E, Billet G. Orientation of the lateral semicircular canal in Xenarthra and its links with head posture and phylogeny. J. Morphol. 2017;278:704–717. doi: 10.1002/jmor.20665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Caix M, Outrequin GL. variabilité des canaux semi-circulaires osseux. Anat. Clin. 1979;1:259–266. doi: 10.1007/BF01654582. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Köhler, M. Skeleton and Habitat of Recent and Fossil Ruminants. (F. Pfeil, 1993).
  • 48.Solounias, N. Family Bovidae. in The Evolution of Artiodactyls (eds. Prothero, D. R. & Foss, S. E.) 278–291 (University of The Johns Hopkins Press, 2007).
  • 49.Estes, R. D. The Behavior Guide to African Mammals: Including Hoofed Mammals, Carnivores, Primates. (Univ of California Press, 2012).
  • 50.Mennecart B, Costeur L. Shape variation and ontogeny of the ruminant bony labyrinth, an example in Tragulidae. J. Anat. 2016;229:422–435. doi: 10.1111/joa.12487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Costeur L, Mennecart B, Müller B, Schulz G. Prenatal growth stages show the development of the ruminant bony labyrinth and petrosal bone. J. Anat. 2017;230:347–353. doi: 10.1111/joa.12549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Lyu H-Y, et al. The age-related orientational changes of human semicircular canals. Clin. Exp. Otorhinolaryngol. 2016;9:109–115. doi: 10.21053/ceo.2014.02012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Neenan JM, Chapelle KEJ, Fernandez V, Choiniere JN. Ontogeny of the Massospondylus labyrinth: implications for locomotory shifts in a basal sauropodomorph dinosaur. Palaeontology. 2019;62:255–265. doi: 10.1111/pala.12400. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Felsenstein J. Phylogenies and the comparative method. Am. Nat. 1985;125:1–15. doi: 10.1086/284325. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Paradis, E. Analysis of Phylogenetics and Evolution with R. (Springer New York, 2012). doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-1743-9.
  • 56.Modern phylogenetic comparative methods and their application in evolutionary biology: concepts and practice. (Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2014). doi:10.1007/978-3-662-43550-2.
  • 57.Bininda-Emonds ORP, et al. The delayed rise of present-day mammals. Nature. 2007;446:507–512. doi: 10.1038/nature05634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Paradis, E. & Schliep, K. ape 5.0: an environment for modern phylogenetics and evolutionary analyses in R. Bioinformatics35, 526–528 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 59.Pagel M. Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution. Nature. 1999;401:877–884. doi: 10.1038/44766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Revell LJ. phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things) Methods Ecol. Evol. 2012;3:217–223. doi: 10.1111/j.2041-210X.2011.00169.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Blomberg SP, Garland T, Ives AR. Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution. 2003;57:717–745. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00285.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Münkemüller T, et al. How to measure and test phylogenetic signal. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2012;3:743–756. doi: 10.1111/j.2041-210X.2012.00196.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Molina-Venegas R, Rodríguez MÁ. Revisiting phylogenetic signal; strong or negligible impacts of polytomies and branch length information? BMC Evol. Biol. 2017;17:53. doi: 10.1186/s12862-017-0898-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Fritz SA, Purvis A. Selectivity in mammalian extinction risk and threat types: a new measure of phylogenetic signal strength in binary traits. Conserv. Biol. 2010;24:1042–1051. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2010.01455.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Orme D, et al. CAPER: comparative analyses of phylogenetics and evolution in R. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2013;3:145–151. doi: 10.1002/ece3.439. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Grafen A, Hamilton WD. The phylogenetic regression. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1989;326:119–157. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1989.0106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Symonds, M. R. E. & Blomberg, S. P. A primer on phylogenetic generalised least squares. In Modern Phylogenetic Comparative Methods and Their Application in Evolutionary Biology: Concepts and Practice (ed. Garamszegi, L. Z.) 105–130 (Springer, 2014). doi:10.1007/978-3-662-43550-2_5.
  • 68.Pinheiro, J. C., Bates, D. J., DebRoy, S. & Sakar, D. The Nlme package: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models, R Version 3. R package version vol. 6 (2012).
  • 69.Mazerolle, M. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference Based on (Q)AIC(c). R package version vol. 2 (2013).
  • 70.Benoit J, et al. Brain evolution in Proboscidea (Mammalia, Afrotheria) across the Cenozoic. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-37186-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Jerison, H. Evolution of The Brain and Intelligence. (Elsevier Science, 1973).
  • 72.Spoor F, et al. The primate semicircular canal system and locomotion. PNAS. 2007;104:10808–10812. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704250104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Spoor F, Bajpai S, Hussain ST, Kumar K, Thewissen JGM. Vestibular evidence for the evolution of aquatic behaviour in early cetaceans. Nature. 2002;417:163–166. doi: 10.1038/417163a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Veall MR, Zimmermann KF. Pseudo-R2 measures for some common limited dependent variable models. J. Econ. Surveys. 1996;10:241–259. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6419.1996.tb00013.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Mundry, R. Statistical issues and assumptions of phylogenetic generalized least squares. in Modern Phylogenetic Comparative Methods and Their Application in Evolutionary Biology: Concepts and Practice (ed. Garamszegi, L. Z.) 131–153 (Springer, 2014). doi:10.1007/978-3-662-43550-2_6.
  • 76.Garland T, Dickerman AW, Janis CM, Jones JA. Phylogenetic analysis of covariance by computer simulation. Syst. Biol. 1993;42:265–292. doi: 10.1093/sysbio/42.3.265. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B (Methodological)57, 289–300 (1995).
  • 78.Harmon LJ, et al. Early bursts of body size and shape evolution are rare in comparative data. Evolution. 2010;64:2385–2396. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01025.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Romano, M., Manucci, F. & Palombo, M. R. The smallest of the largest: new volumetric body mass estimate and in-vivo restoration of the dwarf elephant Palaeoloxodon ex gr. P. falconeri from Spinagallo Cave (Sicily). Hist Biol1, 1–14 (2019).
  • 80.Romano, M. & Manucci, F. Resizing Lisowicia bojani: volumetric body mass estimate and 3D reconstruction of the giant Late Triassic dicynodont. Hist Biol0, 1–6 (2019).
  • 81.Campione, N. E. & Evans, D. C. The accuracy and precision of body mass estimation in non‐avian dinosaurs. Biol. Rev. brv. 12638 (2020). doi:10.1111/brv.12638. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 82.Hopkins, S. S. B. Estimation of Body Size in Fossil Mammals. in Methods in Paleoecology: Reconstructing Cenozoic Terrestrial Environments and Ecological Communities (eds. Croft, D. A., Su, D. F. & Simpson, S. W.) 7–22 (Springer International Publishing, 2018). doi:10.1007/978-3-319-94265-0_2.
  • 83.Spoor F, Zonneveld F. Comparative review of the human bony labyrinth. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1998;107:211–251. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(1998)107:27+&#x0003c;211::AID-AJPA8&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-V. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Dunbar DC, Macpherson JM, Simmons RW, Zarcades A. Stabilization and mobility of the head, neck and trunk in horses during overground locomotion: comparisons with humans and other primates. J. Exp. Biol. 2008;211:3889–3907. doi: 10.1242/jeb.020578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Zubair HN, Beloozerova IN, Sun H, Marlinski V. Head movement during walking in the cat. Neuroscience. 2016;332:101–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.06.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Mennecart B, Costeur L. A Dorcatherium (Mammalia, Ruminantia, Middle Miocene) petrosal bone and the tragulid ear region. J. Vert. Paleontol. 2016;36:e1211665. doi: 10.1080/02724634.2016.1211665. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Mennecart B, et al. Bony labyrinth morphology clarifies the origin and evolution of deer. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:13176. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12848-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Aiglstorfer M, Costeur L, Mennecart B, Heizmann EPJ. Micromeryx? eiselei—a new moschid species from Steinheim am Albuch, Germany, and the first comprehensive description of moschid cranial material from the Miocene of Central Europe. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0185679. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0185679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Prothero, D. R. & Schoch, R. M. Horns, Tusks, and Flippers: The Evolution of Hoofed Mammals. (JHU Press, 2002).
  • 90.Ackerly D. Conservatism and diversification of plant functional traits: evolutionary rates versus phylogenetic signal. PNAS. 2009;106:19699–19706. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901635106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Cadotte MW, Davies TJ, Peres-Neto PR. Why phylogenies do not always predict ecological differences. Ecol. Monogr. 2017;87:535–551. doi: 10.1002/ecm.1267. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Geist, V. Deer of the world: their evolution, behaviour, and ecology. (Stackpole Books, 1998).
  • 93.Walker, E. P., Nowak, R. M. & Paradiso, J. L. Walker’s Mammals of the world. (Johns Hopkins University Press, 1983).
  • 94.Benoit J, Manger PR, Fernandez V, Rubidge BS. The bony labyrinth of late Permian Biarmosuchia: palaeobiology and diversity in non-mammalian Therapsida. Palaeontol. Afr. 2017;52:58–77. [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Chapelle KEJ, Barrett PM, Botha J, Choiniere JN. Ngwevu intloko: a new early sauropodomorph dinosaur from the Lower Jurassic Elliot Formation of South Africa and comments on cranial ontogeny in Massospondylus carinatus. Peer J. 2019;7:e7240. doi: 10.7717/peerj.7240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Mourlam MJ, Orliac MJ. Infrasonic and ultrasonic hearing evolved after the emergence of modern whales. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:1776–1781.e9. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Osborn, H. F. The titanotheres of ancient Wyoming, Dakota, and Nebraska. (Washington [D.C.] : Dept. of the Interior, U.S. Geological Survey, 1929).
  • 98.Osborn, H. F. Proboscidea : a monograph of the discovery, evolution, migration and extinction of the mastodonts and elephants of the world. vol. I (New York : Published on the J. Pierpont Morgan Fund by the trustees of the American Museum of Natural History : American Museum Press, 1936).
  • 99.Osborn, H. F. Proboscidea : a monograph of the discovery, evolution, migration and extinction of the mastodonts and elephants of the world. vol. II (New York : Published on the J. Pierpont Morgan Fund by the trustees of the American Museum of Natural History : American Museum Press, 1942).
  • 100.Stanley SM. Relative growth of the titanothere horn: a new approach to an old problem. Evolution. 1974;28:447–457. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1974.tb00766.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Barghusen HR. A review of fighting adaptations in dinocephalians (Reptilia, Therapsida) Paleobiol. 1975;1:295–311. doi: 10.1017/S0094837300002542. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information (877.5KB, xlsx)

Data Availability Statement

The datasets analysed during the current study are available in the Supplementary Table S1 and online at this URL: https://osf.io/4vpnj/?view_only=3dc987012fcd44a6a64ad7d8949ec01f (10.17605/Osf.Io/4vpnj). Data replicated in the Wits Data Archive and can be guaranteed to be in the holding of the University of Witwatersrand.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES