Abstract
Background
The default mode network (DMN) is an important mediator of passive states of mind. Multiple cortical areas, such as the anterior cingulate cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, and lateral parietal lobe, have been linked in this processing, though knowledge of network connectivity had limited tractographic specificity.
Methods
Using resting‐state fMRI studies related to the DMN, we generated an activation likelihood estimation (ALE). We built a tractographical model of this network based on the cortical parcellation scheme previously published under the Human Connectome Project. DSI‐based fiber tractography was performed to determine the structural connections between cortical parcellations comprising the network.
Results
Seventeen cortical regions were found to be part of the DMN: 10r, 31a, 31pd, 31pv, a24, d23ab, IP1, p32, POS1, POS2, RSC, PFm, PGi, PGs, s32, TPOJ3, and v23ab. These regions showed consistent interconnections between adjacent parcellations, and the cingulum was found to connect the anterior and posterior cingulate clusters within the network.
Conclusions
We present a preliminary anatomic model of the default mode network. Further studies may refine this model with the ultimate goal of clinical application.
Keywords: ALE, anatomy, default mode network, parcellation, tractography, white matter
Using attentional task‐based fMRI studies to generate an activation likelihood estimation (ALE), we built a tractographical model of this network. Seventeen cortical regions were found to be part of the DMN: 10r, 31a, 31pd, 31pv, a24, d23ab, IP1, p32, POS1, POS2, RSC, PFm, PGi, PGs, s32, TPOJ3, and v23ab. These regions showed consistent interconnections between adjacent parcellations, and the cingulum was found to connect the anterior and posterior cingulate clusters within the network.
1. INTRODUCTION
Advances in human neuroimaging techniques have elucidated complex neural networks which execute key functions (Beckmann et al., 2005; Cole et al., 2014; De Luca et al., 2006; Razlighi, 2018; Smith et al., 2013; Thirion et al., 2006). A collection of interacting networks has been described in the literature, consisting of the brain's default mode network, salience network, and executive control network. In contrast to primary cortical areas which can be preserved during brain surgery, preservation of higher cognitive networks has proven more difficult due to their complex anatomy (Burks et al., 2018). Improvements in the understanding of the connectivity and anatomy of higher‐order cerebral networks are therefore likely to manifest in advances in brain tumor surgery.
Several studies have characterized the anatomy of the default mode network (DMN) since its discovery in 2001 (Andrews‐Hanna et al., 2010; Buckner et al., 2008; Horn et al., 2014; Raichle et al., 2001). It serves a primary role in passive states of mind, though it is also active during some goal‐oriented tasks (Bressler & Menon, 2010; Chand et al., 2017; Fox, Snyder, et al., 2005; Greicius et al., 2003). The network is typically described as consisting of the anterior and posterior cingulate cortices, and the lateral parietal lobe bilaterally (Alves et al., 2019; Andrews‐Hanna et al., 2010; Buckner et al., 2008). While important, existing descriptions of the DMN offer limited anatomical specificity, making it difficult to compare findings among different papers. This study instead relies on newly published parcellated brain maps to study the network anatomy of the DMN using a standard cortical atlas and nomenclature (Glasser et al., 2016).
In this study, a new cortical model of the DMN was constructed based on the parcellation scheme previously published under the Human Connectome Project (HCP) (Glasser et al., 2016). The HCP atlas is among the most detailed in vivo parcellation scheme constructed by combining automated machine learning approaches with extant neuroanatomical literature. It allows consistent and detailed delineation of cortical areas which we employed for its potential for reproducibility across studies and in clinical contexts. After identifying the cortical regions of interest involved in the network, we performed DSI‐based fiber tractography to demonstrate the structural connections between parcellations within the network. Our goal is to move toward a more precise anatomic model of the DMN for use in future studies.
2. METHODS AND MATERIALS
2.1. Literature search
We initially searched for relevant fMRI studies related to the DMN in BrainMap Sleuth 2.4 (Fox, Laird, et al., 2005; Fox & Lancaster, 2002; Laird et al., 2005). No research articles were identified using this software. Other literature software's were not queried as PubMed was subsequently queried to cover the literature gap on October 1, 2020, for fMRI studies relevant to the default mode network. We used the following search algorithm: (default mode OR default mode network OR DMN) AND “resting‐state fMRI” AND controls.” Studies relevant network were reviewed and included in our analysis if they fulfilled the following search criteria: (a) peer‐reviewed publication, (b) resting‐state fMRI study examining the DMN, (c) based on whole‐brain, voxel‐wise imaging, (d) including standardized coordinate‐based results in the Talairach or Montreal Neuroimaging Institute (MNI) coordinate space, and (e) including at least one healthy human control cohort. Only coordinates from healthy subjects were utilized in our analysis. Twenty‐eight papers met criteria for inclusion in this study (Anderson et al., 2011; Che et al., 2014; Chen et al., 2017; Chiong et al., 2013; Clemens et al., 2017; Crittenden et al., 2015; De Luca et al., 2006; Doll et al., 2015; Fransson, 2006; Greicius et al., 2003; Horn et al., 2014; Kennedy & Courchesne, 2008; Konishi et al., 2015; Laird et al., 2009; Lin et al., 2017; Luo et al., 2016; Maresh et al., 2014; Mason et al., 2007; Piccoli et al., 2015; Pletzer et al., 2015; Poerio et al., 2017; Spreng & Schacter, 2012; Stawarczyk et al., 2011; Taruffi et al., 2017; Utevsky et al., 2014; Vatansever et al., 2015; J. Xu et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2012). The details of these studies are summarized in Table 1.
TABLE 1.
Study | Task | Number of participants | MNI/Talairach | Coordinates | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Anderson et al., 2011 | Subjects were instructed, “Keep your eyes open and remain awake and try to let thoughts pass through your mind without focusing on any particular mental activity.” | 57 (100%) | MNI | −4 | −52 | 32 |
4 | −53 | 35 | ||||
−2 | 55 | −13 | ||||
2 | 55 | −13 | ||||
−49 | −62 | 34 | ||||
50 | −57 | 36 | ||||
Che et al., 2014 | Perceived social support at resting state. | 333 (100%) | MNI | 0 | −45 | 36 |
6 | 48 | −18 | ||||
−51 | −66 | 30 | ||||
48 | −63 | 39 | ||||
−6 | 54 | 45 | ||||
6 | 45 | −9 | ||||
−36 | −69 | 42 | ||||
39 | −66 | 42 | ||||
−6 | −54 | 45 | ||||
−3 | 54 | 3 | ||||
−51 | −69 | 33 | ||||
48 | −66 | 45 | ||||
Crittenden et al., 2015 | Switch between similar and dissimilar tasks, within a relatively large set of six tasks. | 18 (100%) | MNI | −30 | −36 | −6 |
33 | −36 | −9 | ||||
−21 | −42 | 9 | ||||
30 | −39 | 6 | ||||
−9 | −48 | 12 | ||||
9 | −51 | 12 | ||||
−12 | −54 | 24 | ||||
12 | −51 | 24 | ||||
−9 | 51 | −6 | ||||
9 | 48 | −3 | ||||
−39 | −75 | 33 | ||||
Luo et al., 2016 | To rest with eyes closed. | 148 (100%) | MNI | 0 | 51 | 39 |
−6 | −42 | 42 | ||||
−33 | −60 | 54 | ||||
Utevsky et al., 2014 | Three reward‐based decision tasks requiring externally focused attention and resting state. | 209 (100%) | MNI | 15 | −63 | 18 |
0 | −54 | 45 | ||||
−12 | −69 | 18 | ||||
6 | −42 | 3 | ||||
−3 | −45 | 6 | ||||
Vatansever et al., 2015 | Finger opposition paradigm with task and fixation periods versus. independent resting state. | 22 (100%) | MNI | 0 | 52 | 26 |
−6 | 52 | −2 | ||||
0 | 26 | −18 | ||||
−44 | −74 | 32 | ||||
44 | −74 | 32 | ||||
−54 | −54 | 28 | ||||
54 | −54 | 28 | ||||
−60 | −24 | −18 | ||||
60 | −24 | −18 | ||||
0 | −58 | 27 | ||||
−14 | −52 | 8 | ||||
14 | −52 | 8 | ||||
−27 | −15 | 26 | ||||
27 | −15 | 26 | ||||
−28 | −15 | −12 | ||||
28 | −15 | −12 | ||||
Doll et al., 2015 | To remain still with eyes closed and to not fall asleep during acquisition. | 26 (100%) | MNI | −6 | 53 | 1 |
−6 | 47 | −8 | ||||
6 | 47 | 7 | ||||
0 | −52 | 28 | ||||
6 | 61 | 28 | ||||
−6 | −52 | 28 | ||||
−6 | −61 | 34 | ||||
12 | −49 | 34 | ||||
12 | −67 | 34 | ||||
9 | −70 | 37 | ||||
−6 | −76 | 34 | ||||
Clemens et al., 2017 | Participants believe that they play a virtual ball tossing game with two other participants to whom they are supposedly connected via a computer, resting‐state data taken before and after game. | 89 (100%) | MNI | −6 | 28 | 32 |
50 | −14 | 26 | ||||
42 | 36 | 8 | ||||
22 | 8 | 50 | ||||
48 | 12 | 12 | ||||
4 | −62 | 18 | ||||
18 | −74 | 26 | ||||
Pletzer et al., 2015 | Four different tasks in a paper and pencil design to evaluate mathematical skills. Independent resting state. | 36 (100%) | MNI | 27 | −42 | −15 |
−24 | −45 | −15 | ||||
57 | −3 | −15 | ||||
−60 | −9 | −15 | ||||
−51 | −51 | 18 | ||||
−27 | 45 | 36 | ||||
33 | 42 | 30 | ||||
−18 | 66 | 3 | ||||
9 | 54 | −6 | ||||
39 | −15 | −6 | ||||
−42 | −57 | 21 | ||||
−18 | −6 | 21 | ||||
−6 | 57 | 3 | ||||
54 | 33 | 3 | ||||
−9 | −45 | 36 |
Lin et al., 2017 | Visual‐based attention task and then a resting‐state scan. | 14 (100%) | MNI | 0 | −53 | 26 |
−3 | 55 | 22 | ||||
−3 | 59 | −7 | ||||
−24 | 33 | −27 | ||||
30 | −33 | −27 | ||||
−52 | 69 | 26 | ||||
48 | −67 | 36 | ||||
−21 | 32 | 47 | ||||
12 | 44 | 48 | ||||
−61 | −17 | −30 | ||||
61 | −5 | −25 | ||||
Taruffi et al., 2017 | Listening to 4‐min blocks of sad and happy music (with the same tempo) with eyes closed. | 24 (100%) | MNI | −6 | 39 | −8 |
15 | 39 | −2 | ||||
0 | 45 | 31 | ||||
0 | −36 | 52 | ||||
21 | −51 | 16 | ||||
−48 | −69 | 43 | ||||
54 | −63 | 28 | ||||
48 | 12 | 25 | ||||
Stawarczyk et al., 2011 | Participants reported their conscious experiences in terms of both task‐relatedness and stimulus‐dependency while they performed the SART (Sustained Attention to Response Task). Independent resting state | 22 (100%) | MNI | 0 | 58 | −2 |
−8 | −62 | 20 | ||||
−38 | −80 | 36 | ||||
−16 | −48 | 0 | ||||
48 | 38 | 36 | ||||
−60 | −48 | 48 | ||||
2 | 58 | −2 | ||||
−2 | 70 | 20 | ||||
−2 | −50 | 22 | ||||
−44 | −72 | 48 | ||||
−64 | −22 | −26 | ||||
52 | −14 | −32 | ||||
−64 | −44 | −8 | ||||
−52 | 26 | 24 | ||||
De Luca et al., 2006 | Resting state, instructed to relax with their eyes closed, without falling asleep | 26 (100%) | Talairach | 6 | −78 | −3 |
24 | −78 | −10 | ||||
−30 | −89 | 20 | ||||
−2 | −51 | 27 | ||||
53 | −57 | 23 | ||||
2 | 54 | −3 | ||||
−20 | −19 | −18 | ||||
6 | −19 | 6 | ||||
−4 | −6 | 40 | ||||
−51 | −7 | 8 | ||||
−55 | −18 | 8 | ||||
57 | −5 | 20 | ||||
12 | −17 | 0 | ||||
22 | −16 | −13 | ||||
−2 | −21 | 43 | ||||
46 | 6 | 34 | ||||
44 | −48 | 46 | ||||
−38 | −56 | 48 | ||||
−55 | −58 | −9 | ||||
62 | −37 | −3 | ||||
52 | 26 | −4 | ||||
6 | 46 | 18 | ||||
8 | −46 | 41 | ||||
8 | −16 | 40 | ||||
Greicius et al., 2003 | Keep their eyes closed and to not think of anything in particular for 4 min. | 14 (100%) | Talairach | −2 | −51 | 27 |
−51 | −65 | 27 | ||||
−2 | 55 | −18 | ||||
−16 | 49 | 38 | ||||
−44 | 20 | 41 | ||||
−12 | −35 | 0 | ||||
18 | 54 | 32 | ||||
−58 | −18 | 14 | ||||
2 | 38 | −2 | ||||
2 | −51 | 27 | ||||
4 | −14 | 34 | ||||
4 | 9 | −6 | ||||
4 | −16 | −3 | ||||
−4 | −55 | 25 | ||||
0 | −49 | 30 | ||||
−4 | −49 | 26 | ||||
Horn et al., 2014 | Participants were asked to close their eyes. | 19 (100%) | Talairach | 6 | −61 | 34 |
−36 | −63 | 47 | ||||
45 | −43 | 52 | ||||
57 | −28 | 25 | ||||
6 | −58 | 34 | ||||
−54 | −66 | 32 | ||||
45 | −43 | 49 | ||||
60 | −28 | 25 | ||||
9 | −52 | 31 | ||||
−39 | −46 | 43 | ||||
48 | −46 | 28 | ||||
−3 | 62 | −8 | ||||
−30 | −100 | 4 | ||||
15 | −64 | 32 | ||||
−36 | −45 | 49 | ||||
39 | −40 | 46 | ||||
0 | 68 | 0 | ||||
−30 | 50 | 7 | ||||
−31 | −100 | 8 | ||||
Maresh et al., 2014 | Underwent two runs of the MID task, designed to assess one's neural response to the anticipation and receipt of rewarding or punishing monetary stimuli. | 84 (100%) | Talairach | −18 | −40 | 36 |
−30 | −56 | 36 | ||||
−8 | −54 | 48 | ||||
0 | −78 | 52 | ||||
−34 | −56 | 42 | ||||
−6 | −54 | 36 | ||||
−20 | 44 | 18 | ||||
−26 | 34 | 42 | ||||
−20 | 42 | 10 | ||||
−28 | 56 | 12 | ||||
−22 | 34 | 28 | ||||
−22 | 16 | 36 | ||||
−8 | −56 | 48 | ||||
−10 | −66 | 34 | ||||
−8 | −42 | 48 | ||||
−6 | −54 | 38 | ||||
−6 | −50 | 34 | ||||
−10 | −52 | 38 | ||||
−36 | −42 | 54 | ||||
−52 | −32 | 50 | ||||
8 | −48 | 60 | ||||
−36 | −54 | 54 | ||||
−42 | −48 | 56 | ||||
−46 | −50 | 56 | ||||
−50 | −50 | 50 | ||||
Fransson, 2006 | Two 10‐min scans during which they performed a resting‐state, low‐level baseline task (eyes open, visual fixation on a hair‐cross‐centered in the screen) and a sequential two‐back verbal working memory task. | 14 (100%) | Talairach | −39 | −75 | 45 |
−3 | −63 | 48 | ||||
3 | −66 | 30 | ||||
−33 | 18 | 57 | ||||
33 | 33 | 51 | ||||
−6 | 39 | 39 | ||||
9 | 45 | −12 | ||||
−12 | 45 | −12 | ||||
−60 | −27 | −15 | ||||
−57 | −36 | −6 | ||||
−27 | −27 | −24 | ||||
−42 | 9 | −42 | ||||
63 | −18 | −21 | ||||
48 | 9 | −42 | ||||
63 | −27 | −12 | ||||
39 | −72 | −42 | ||||
6 | −60 | −51 | ||||
−6 | −57 | −48 | ||||
30 | −18 | −24 | ||||
−24 | −90 | −27 | ||||
−27 | 27 | −24 | ||||
57 | 9 | 12 | ||||
45 | 12 | 6 | ||||
66 | −21 | 33 | ||||
63 | −18 | 24 | ||||
69 | −15 | 18 | ||||
−66 | −27 | 18 | ||||
−36 | 3 | 9 | ||||
Piccoli et al., 2015 | Subjects were asked to judge whether a given target stimulus had been part of a previous memory stimulus set or not. | 14 (100%) | Talairach | 2 | −44 | 31 |
−2 | 25 | −8 | ||||
41 | −56 | 33 | ||||
−54 | −63 | 20 | ||||
28 | −56 | 47 | ||||
−25 | −59 | 45 | ||||
44 | 3 | 24 | ||||
−55 | 5 | 30 | ||||
−2 | −48 | 24 | ||||
2 | 46 | 9 | ||||
50 | −59 | 23 | ||||
−44 | −71 | 30 | ||||
35 | −58 | 50 | ||||
−18 | −70 | 46 | ||||
40 | 18 | 35 | ||||
−27 | 31 | 28 | ||||
−10 | −53 | 13 | ||||
2 | 46 | 21 | ||||
46 | −70 | 26 | ||||
−50 | −65 | 38 | ||||
40 | −53 | 48 | ||||
−33 | −53 | 47 | ||||
43 | 22 | 27 | ||||
−38 | 16 | 24 | ||||
−4 | −37 | 36 | ||||
−3 | 28 | −3 | ||||
58 | −53 | 24 | ||||
−55 | −64 | 25 | ||||
33 | −56 | 44 | ||||
−36 | −38 | 37 | ||||
35 | 21 | 38 | ||||
−45 | 22 | 32 | ||||
2 | −44 | 30 | ||||
−7 | 37 | 9 | ||||
59 | −29 | 24 | ||||
−61 | −38 | 15 | ||||
30 | −53 | 30 | ||||
−40 | −50 | 33 | ||||
32 | 43 | 28 | ||||
−55 | 16 | 33 | ||||
−3 | −48 | 26 | ||||
−3 | 41 | 30 | ||||
30 | −81 | 36 | ||||
−43 | −74 | 32 | ||||
44 | −32 | 36 | ||||
−36 | −54 | 44 | ||||
42 | 38 | 39 | ||||
−41 | 30 | 38 | ||||
−1 | −40 | 28 | ||||
1 | 29 | −6 | ||||
41 | −77 | 38 | ||||
−50 | −74 | 20 | ||||
23 | −67 | 48 | ||||
−34 | −62 | 42 | ||||
35 | 29 | 25 | ||||
−21 | 35 | 28 | ||||
−3 | −54 | 20 | ||||
4 | 52 | 11 | ||||
56 | −58 | 32 | ||||
−53 | −58 | 21 | ||||
24 | −50 | 47 | ||||
−29 | −50 | 41 | ||||
22 | 28 | 28 | ||||
−36 | 25 | 26 | ||||
0 | −50 | 31 | ||||
−1 | 27 | −13 | ||||
54 | −63 | 24 | ||||
−46 | −65 | 26 | ||||
17 | −66 | 54 | ||||
−22 | −64 | 51 | ||||
44 | 35 | 27 | ||||
−43 | 24 | 33 | ||||
−3 | −42 | 29 | ||||
0 | 37 | −3 | ||||
39 | −69 | 28 | ||||
−52 | −70 | 24 | ||||
30 | −57 | 39 | ||||
−38 | −44 | 36 | ||||
44 | 30 | 33 | ||||
−28 | 28 | 33 | ||||
−9 | −51 | 26 | ||||
−2 | 31 | −4 | ||||
53 | −64 | 21 | ||||
−46 | −75 | 24 | ||||
26 | −66 | 36 | ||||
−37 | −37 | 38 | ||||
34 | 19 | 36 | ||||
−31 | 19 | 26 | ||||
−10 | −55 | 32 | ||||
−4 | 30 | −5 | ||||
39 | −71 | 34 | ||||
−48 | −77 | 30 | ||||
31 | −44 | 37 | ||||
−31 | −50 | 33 | ||||
51 | 28 | 32 | ||||
−49 | 25 | 33 | ||||
−8 | −35 | 33 | ||||
10 | 34 | 5 | ||||
50 | −65 | 30 | ||||
−54 | −65 | 28 | ||||
37 | −74 | 35 | ||||
−31 | −71 | 39 | ||||
35 | 36 | 38 | ||||
−45 | 32 | 31 | ||||
8 | −47 | 30 | ||||
2 | 42 | 2 | ||||
44 | −74 | 22 | ||||
−48 | −71 | 24 | ||||
26 | −68 | 45 | ||||
−43 | −38 | 45 | ||||
38 | 34 | 30 | ||||
−40 | 40 | 24 | ||||
Laird et al., 2009 | Investigating the Functional Heterogeneity of the Default Mode Network Using Coordinate‐Based Meta‐Analytic Modeling. | 840 (100%) | Talairach | −4 | −58 | 44 |
−4 | −52 | 22 | ||||
2 | 32 | −8 | ||||
52 | −28 | −24 | ||||
−2 | 50 | 18 | ||||
46 | −66 | 16 | ||||
−26 | 16 | 44 | ||||
−56 | −36 | 28 | ||||
−42 | −66 | 18 |
Chiong et al., 2013 | Moral reasoning task. | 16 (control subjects only) | MNI | 50 | −6 | 26 |
0 | −78 | 42 | ||||
4 | 34 | −14 | ||||
56 | −64 | 26 | ||||
4 | −58 | 24 | ||||
−20 | 4 | −12 | ||||
Poerio et al., 2017 | Viewed a fixation cross with eyes open. | 80 (100%) | MNI | −12 | −92 | −24 |
−48 | −72 | −28 | ||||
−48 | 24 | 32 | ||||
Spreng & Schacter, 2012 | Visuospatial planning was investigated by presenting participants with 2 configurations on a single screen: the “goal” position and the “initial” position. The objective of this task was for the participant to determine the minimum number of moves to accomplish the goal state. | 36 (100%) | MNI | −22 | −22 | −22 |
47 | −71 | 29 | ||||
Yang et al., 2012 | Subjects were asked to “relax and rest as we take pictures of your brain.” | 18 (100%) | Talairach | −10 | 40 | 50 |
10 | 66 | 10 | ||||
14 | 44 | −8 | ||||
−64 | −6 | −8 | ||||
−60 | −14 | −20 | ||||
64 | −8 | −26 | ||||
−30 | −22 | −20 | ||||
36 | −34 | −18 | ||||
Konishi et al., 2015 | Participants alternated between task blocks in which they either make decisions about the location of shapes as they are presented on screen or with respect to their location on the prior trial. | 29 (100%) | MNI | 4 | −64 | 30 |
18 | −52 | 16 | ||||
10 | −46 | 0 | ||||
32 | 14 | 42 | ||||
22 | 42 | 40 | ||||
28 | 20 | 46 | ||||
0 | 36 | 22 | ||||
16 | 64 | 0 | ||||
28 | 56 | 8 | ||||
44 | −58 | 36 | ||||
36 | −62 | 26 | ||||
40 | −68 | 44 | ||||
2 | −32 | 36 | ||||
10 | −46 | 34 | ||||
8 | −16 | 30 | ||||
−24 | −20 | −18 | ||||
−22 | −34 | −8 | ||||
−16 | −34 | 2 | ||||
−54 | −8 | −18 | ||||
−62 | −18 | −10 | ||||
−52 | −16 | −22 | ||||
−38 | −72 | 36 | ||||
−48 | −62 | 42 | ||||
−38 | −74 | 28 | ||||
−8 | −60 | 38 | ||||
−10 | −38 | 28 | ||||
−2 | −48 | 34 | ||||
−6 | −66 | 6 | ||||
−18 | −66 | 18 | ||||
−18 | −64 | 8 | ||||
Mason et al., 2007 | Minds could wander when they performed practiced versus novel task sequences. | 19 (100%) | Talairach | 6 | 62 | 11 |
0 | 62 | 16 | ||||
−12 | 49 | 45 | ||||
12 | 49 | 47 | ||||
21 | 40 | 45 | ||||
−38 | 20 | 48 | ||||
−9 | −39 | 35 | ||||
6 | −15 | 39 | ||||
24 | −7 | −20 | ||||
6 | 41 | 4 | ||||
−18 | −36 | −13 | ||||
−45 | −68 | 37 | ||||
−9 | −51 | 30 | ||||
42 | −11 | 3 | ||||
45 | −28 | 18 | ||||
−42 | −18 | −2 | ||||
−45 | −63 | 31 | ||||
−45 | −6 | −7 | ||||
50 | −57 | 33 | ||||
−30 | −39 | −13 | ||||
6 | 51 | −9 | ||||
3 | 57 | 42 | ||||
3 | −9 | 51 | ||||
−3 | 51 | 51 | ||||
−6 | 54 | 27 | ||||
−18 | 39 | 51 | ||||
15 | 48 | 42 | ||||
15 | 60 | 39 | ||||
21 | −24 | 72 | ||||
6 | 48 | 9 | ||||
−6 | −45 | 18 | ||||
−9 | −42 | 27 | ||||
3 | −45 | 18 | ||||
0 | −9 | 39 | ||||
54 | −57 | 36 | ||||
−48 | −66 | 30 | ||||
6 | −41 | 60 | ||||
45 | −24 | 18 | ||||
−42 | 0 | 9 | ||||
30 | −15 | 3 | ||||
36 | −3 | 9 | ||||
−36 | −9 | 9 | ||||
60 | −6 | 3 | ||||
−45 | −60 | 15 | ||||
−42 | −37 | 3 | ||||
Kennedy & Courchesne, 2008 | Subjects made true/false judgments for various statements about themselves (SELF condition) or a close other person (OTHER condition). | 13 (control only) | Talairach | −6 | 39 | 48 |
−2 | 51 | 28 | ||||
2 | −49 | 28 | ||||
−42 | 19 | 16 | ||||
−3 | −97 | 24 | ||||
−42 | 3 | 48 | ||||
−50 | −65 | 36 | ||||
−2 | 31 | 0 | ||||
−42 | 3 | −28 | ||||
58 | −65 | 28 | ||||
−50 | −33 | 4 | ||||
55 | −32 | 20 | ||||
7 | 39 | −1 | ||||
26 | −44 | 59 | ||||
51 | −8 | 12 | ||||
6 | −45 | 35 | ||||
10 | −28 | 44 | ||||
−53 | −61 | 24 | ||||
−37 | −12 | 0 | ||||
−2 | −89 | 20 | ||||
−6 | −73 | 0 | ||||
22 | −68 | 4 | ||||
7 | −57 | 27 | ||||
−18 | −45 | 4 | ||||
22 | −45 | 7 | ||||
18 | −25 | 17 | ||||
−38 | 28 | 0 | ||||
2 | −16 | 32 | ||||
−1 | 51 | 24 | ||||
−2 | 20 | 20 | ||||
−5 | −48 | 8 | ||||
−21 | 20 | 48 | ||||
3 | −33 | 39 | ||||
−18 | −33 | −12 | ||||
−38 | −73 | 29 | ||||
38 | 20 | 52 | ||||
47 | −68 | 24 | ||||
Xu et al., 2014 | Nondirective meditation. | 14 (100%) | MNI | 8 | 37 | 10 |
8 | 6 | 31 | ||||
−3 | 42 | −3 | ||||
Chen et al., 2017 | Resting state | 24 (control subjects only) | MNI | 3 | −54 | 27 |
−45 | −57 | 30 | ||||
−21 | −33 | −12 | ||||
−60 | −9 | −18 | ||||
−3 | 63 | 18 |
2.2. Creation of 3D Regions of Interest
In the original HCP study, parcellation data were analyzed using the CIFTI file format. CIFTI files use a surface‐based coordinate system termed greyordinates, which localizes regions of interest (ROIs) on inflated brains (Van Essen & Glasser, 2016). This is in contrast to traditional file formats, such as NIFTI, which denote regions based on volumetric dimensions (Larobina & Murino, 2014). As a result, it was difficult to perform deterministic tractography using ROIs in CIFTI file format. To convert the parcellations files to volumetric coordinates, the greyordinate label parcellation fields were standardized to the three‐dimensional volumetric working spaces of DSI Studio (Carnegie Mellon, http://dsi‐studio.labsolver.org) using the structural imaging data provided by the HCP. This operation was performed using Workbench Command within Connectome Workbench (Glasser et al., 2013). This allowed us to convert all 180 parcellations from surface‐based coordinates to volumetric coordinates and perform deterministic fiber tractography.
2.3. Activation likelihood generation and identification of relevant cortical regions
We used BrainMap Ginger ALE 2.3.6 to extract the relevant fMRI data for creation of an activation likelihood estimation (ALE; Eickhoff et al., 2009, 2012; Turkeltaub et al., 2012). All Talairach coordinates identified during literature review were converted to the MNI coordinate space using icbm2tal transform SPM Conversion in GingerALE. We subsequently performed a single study analysis using cluster‐level inference in the MNI coordinate space (cluster level of 0.05, threshold permutations of 1,000, uncorrected p‐value of 0.001). The ALE coordinate data were displayed on an MNI‐normalized template brain using the Multi‐image Analysis GUI (Mango) 4.0.1 (ric.uthscsa.edu/mango). The preconstructed ROIs of the HCP parcellations were overlaid on the ALE and two investigators independently compared the foci with the parcellations visually for inclusion in the DMN. Each investigator repeated the process to minimize intra‐observer variability. A third investigator reviewed the images and was available to resolve disputes in instances of interobserver variability, though this was seldom encountered.
2.4. Network tractography
Publicly available imaging data from the Human Connectome Project were obtained for this study from the HCP database (http://humanconnectome.org, release Q3). Diffusion imaging with corresponding T1‐weighted images from 25 healthy, unrelated subjects were analyzed during fiber tracking analysis (Subjects IDs: 100307, 103414, 105115, 110411, 111312, 113619, 115320, 117112, 118730, 118932, 100408, 115320, 116524, 118730, 123925, 148335, 148840, 151526, 160123, 178950, 188347, 192540, 212318, 366446, 756055). We used 25 brains as it is comparable to the number of subjects used in studies of a similar aim. We have previously tested the variability of tractography results above utilizing 25 subjects, and however, it was too small to justify using additional subjects as it is unlikely to alter the findings of the study. Often, beyond 10 subjects, the results do not change significantly. The demographics of the patients used in this study are detailed in Table 2. A multi‐shell diffusion scheme was used, and the b‐values were 990, 1985, and 1980 s/mm2. Each b‐value was sampled in 90 directions. The in‐plane resolution was 1.25 mm. The diffusion data were reconstructed using generalized q‐sampling imaging with a diffusion sampling length ratio of 1.25 (Yeh et al., 2010).
TABLE 2.
Variable (n = 25) | |
---|---|
Age (mean, SD, in years) | 29.5 (3.8) |
Gender | |
Female (n, %) | 13 (52) |
All brains were registered to the Montreal Neurologic Institute (MNI) coordinate space (Evans et al., 1992), wherein imaging is warped to fit a standardized brain model comparison between subjects (Evans et al., 1992). Tractography was performed in DSI Studio (Carnegie Mellon, http://dsi‐studio.labsolver.org) using a region of interest approach to initiate fiber tracking from a user‐defined seed region (Martino et al., 2013). A two‐ROI‐approach was used to isolate tracts (Kamali et al., 2014).
Voxels within each ROI were automatically traced with a maximum angular threshold of 45 degrees. When a voxel was approached with no tract direction or a direction change of greater than 45 degrees, the tract was halted. Tractography was terminated after reaching a maximum length of 800 mm. In some instances, exclusion ROIs were placed to exclude obvious spurious tracts that were not involved in the white matter pathway of interest.
2.5. Measuring connection strength
To quantify the strength of the connections identified within the DMN across all subjects, the tracking parameters used within DSI Studio were modified such that the program would count the total number of tracts between any two ROIs based on a random seed count of 2.5 million. Working sequentially through ROI pairs in the network, the number of tracts between regions was recorded for each subject after fiber tractography was terminated under these new conditions. The connection strength between ROI pairs within the DMN was calculated by averaging the number of tracts between each ROI pair across all subjects.
3. RESULTS
3.1. ALE regions and their corresponding parcellations
Figure 1 demonstrates the ALE of the twenty‐seven MRI experiments included in our meta‐analysis. Highlighted areas include the bilateral posterior cingulate cortices, the anterior cingulate cortices, and the lateral parietal lobes. Seventeen regions of interest were found to overlap the fMRI data, including areas 10r, a24, p32, and s32 in the anterior cingulate cortex; areas 31a, 31pd, 31pv, d23ab, POS1, POS2, RSC, and v23ab in the posterior cingulate cortex and parieto‐occipital sulcus; and areas IP1, PFm, PGi, PGs, and TPOJ3 in the lateral parietal lobe. Comparison overlays between the cortical parcellations and the ALE data are shown in Figure 2.
3.2. Structural connections within the default mode network
Deterministic tractography was utilized to show the basic structural connectivity of the DMN. These results are shown in Figure 3. Individual connections within the network are presented in Table 3 which tabulates the strengths of individual connections and lists the type‐specific white matter connections identified between regions.
TABLE 3.
Connection | N | Average strength weighted by all subjects | Average strength weighted by identified subjects | Connection type |
---|---|---|---|---|
Anterior and Posterior Cingulate Connections | ||||
10r to 31pv | 4 | 5.2 | 32.2 | cingulum fiber |
10r to a24 | 23 | 34.6 | 37.6 | U‐shaped fiber |
10r to d23ab | 7 | 1.6 | 5.7 | cingulum fiber |
10r to p32 | 25 | 236.7 | 236.7 | U‐shaped fiber |
10r to POS1 | 13 | 18.4 | 35.4 | cingulum fiber |
10r to POS2 | 4 | 1.1 | 7.0 | cingulum fiber |
10r to RSC | 14 | 13.0 | 23.2 | cingulum fiber |
10r to s32 | 21 | 128.7 | 153.2 | U‐shaped fiber |
10r to v23ab | 10 | 13.2 | 32.9 | cingulum fiber |
31a to 31pd | 25 | 177.4 | 177.4 | U‐shaped fiber |
31a to 31pv | 24 | 133.9 | 139.5 | U‐shaped fiber |
31a to a24 | 5 | 3.7 | 18.4 | cingulum fiber |
31a to d23ab | 24 | 61.0 | 63.5 | U‐shaped fiber |
31a to p32 | 6 | 2.0 | 8.3 | cingulum fiber |
31a to POS1 | 7 | 0.6 | 2.0 | U‐shaped fiber |
31a to POS2 | 14 | 18.7 | 33.4 | U‐shaped fiber |
31a to RSC | 14 | 7.1 | 12.6 | U‐shaped fiber |
31pd to 31pv | 25 | 203.6 | 203.6 | U‐shaped fiber |
31pd to a24 | 6 | 2.2 | 9.2 | cingulum fiber |
31pd to d23ab | 13 | 7.6 | 14.7 | U‐shaped fiber |
31pd to p32 | 2 | 0.9 | 11.5 | cingulum fiber |
31pd to POS1 | 6 | 7.4 | 30.8 | U‐shaped fiber |
31pd to POS2 | 20 | 19.1 | 23.8 | U‐shaped fiber |
31pd to RSC | 14 | 7.9 | 14.1 | U‐shaped fiber |
31pd to s32 | 1 | 0.04 | 1.0 | cingulum fiber |
31pd to v23ab | 7 | 13.5 | 48.3 | U‐shaped fiber |
31pv to a24 | 16 | 10.7 | 16.7 | cingulum fiber |
31pv to d23ab | 25 | 223.1 | 223.1 | U‐shaped fiber |
31pv to p32 | 11 | 18.8 | 42.8 | cingulum fiber |
31pv to POS1 | 20 | 34.9 | 43.6 | U‐shaped fiber |
31pv to POS2 | 20 | 25.4 | 31.8 | U‐shaped fiber |
31pv to RSC | 23 | 28.1 | 30.5 | U‐shaped fiber |
31pv to s32 | 1 | 0.1 | 3.0 | cingulum fiber |
31pv to v23ab | 25 | 120.6 | 120.6 | U‐shaped fiber |
a24 to d23ab | 15 | 4.8 | 7.9 | cingulum fiber |
a24 to p32 | 23 | 191.6 | 208.3 | U‐shaped fiber |
a24 to POS1 | 16 | 25.4 | 39.6 | cingulum fiber |
a24 to POS2 | 5 | 2.4 | 12.2 | cingulum fiber |
a24 to RSC | 22 | 42.5 | 48.3 | cingulum fiber |
a24 to s32 | 18 | 58.4 | 81.1 | U‐shaped fiber |
a24 to v23ab | 15 | 8.6 | 14.3 | cingulum fiber |
d23ab to p32 | 14 | 7.2 | 12.8 | cingulum fiber |
d23ab to POS1 | 24 | 28.2 | 29.3 | U‐shaped fiber |
d23ab to POS2 | 15 | 29.4 | 49.0 | U‐shaped fiber |
d23ab to RSC | 24 | 267.8 | 278.9 | U‐shaped fiber |
d23ab to v23ab | 25 | 131.2 | 131.2 | U‐shaped fiber |
p32 to POS1 | 18 | 36.9 | 51.3 | cingulum fiber |
p32 to POS2 | 10 | 3.4 | 8.6 | cingulum fiber |
p32 to RSC | 19 | 68.0 | 89.5 | cingulum fiber |
p32 to s32 | 20 | 43.4 | 54.3 | U‐shaped fiber |
p32 to v23ab | 19 | 81.2 | 106.9 | cingulum fiber |
POS1 to POS2 | 25 | 174.4 | 174.4 | U‐shaped fiber |
POS1 to RSC | 25 | 252.0 | 252.0 | U‐shaped fiber |
POS1 to s32 | 2 | 0.1 | 1.5 | cingulum fiber |
POS1 to v23ab | 24 | 223.2 | 232.5 | U‐shaped fiber |
POS2 to RSC | 17 | 15.1 | 22.2 | U‐shaped fiber |
POS1 to v23ab | 11 | 2.3 | 5.3 | U‐shaped fiber |
RSC to s32 | 2 | 1.8 | 23 | cingulum fiber |
RSC v23ab | 24 | 136.4 | 142.1 | U‐shaped fiber |
s32 to v23ab | 2 | 0.2 | 2.0 | cingulum fiber |
Lateral Parietal Connections | ||||
IP1 to PFm | 23 | 149.4 | 162.4 | U‐shaped fiber |
IP1 to PGi | 12 | 19.9 | 41.4 | U‐shaped fiber |
IP1 to PGs | 20 | 145.6 | 182.0 | U‐shaped fiber |
IP1 to TPOJ3 | 7 | 1.0 | 3.7 | U‐shaped fiber |
PFm to PGi | 25 | 418.5 | 418.5 | U‐shaped fiber |
PFm to PGs | 17 | 142.8 | 210.0 | U‐shaped fiber |
PFm to TPOJ3 | 5 | 1.0 | 4.8 | U‐shaped fiber |
PGi to PGs | 24 | 220.3 | 229.5 | U‐shaped fiber |
PGi to TPOJ3 | 25 | 211.2 | 211.2 | U‐shaped fiber |
PGs to TPOJ3 | 18 | 39.8 | 55.3 | U‐shaped fiber |
The cortical areas identified as part of the DMN can be grouped into three distinct clusters: an anterior cingulate cluster (10r, a24, p32, s32), a posterior cingulate cluster (31a, 31pd, 31pv, d23ab, POS1, POS2, RSC, v23ab), and a lateral parietal cluster (IP1, PFm, PGi, PGs, TPOJ3). U‐shaped fibers form a majority of the connections between ROI pairs of the network. These fibers generally have the same morphology, arising within one part of the cortex before curving 180 degrees to terminate in a part of the brain immediately adjacent to its origin. These U‐shaped fibers represent the local connections between anterior cingulate, posterior cingulate, and lateral parietal areas in close proximity.
The cingulum was also identified during fiber tracking analysis. This white matter bundle was found to connect the anterior and posterior cingulate clusters within the DMN. These fibers arise from the anterior cingulate cortex, and curve posteriorly to run within the deep white matter adjacent to the cingulate gyrus. The fibers course along the length of the corpus callosum, until they terminate in the posterior cingulate cortex and parieto‐occipital sulcus (Figure 3).
All four parcellations of the anterior cingulate cluster (10r, a24, p32, and s32) contribute to the cingulum, though with variable frequency. Areas a24 and p32 demonstrated consistent connections across all 25 subjects to all parcellations of the posterior cingulate cluster (31a, 31pd, 31pv, d23ab, POS1, POS2, RSC, and v23ab). In contrast, the connections from areas 10r and s32 were occurred infrequently, and the parcellations were found to connect to fewer regions of the posterior cingulate cortex (Table 3).
No long‐association fiber bundle was found to connect the lateral parietal regions to either the anterior cingulate or posterior cingulate cortices. This was expected, as no such connection has been described previously. However, IP1, PFm, PGi, PGs, and TPOJ3 all showed consistent interconnections between one another in the form of U‐shaped fibers. Figure 4 is a simplified schematic of the connections discussed above. Lines in this schematic represent individual connections of the DMN and are labeled with their corresponding strength measured by averaging the number of tracts between ROI pairs across all subjects.
There was no relationship between demographic data and network anatomy within our cohort. Our cohort was not diverse enough to observe significant differences as this was not a primary aim of this study and we wanted to produce a model from healthy controls to avoid confounding factors. It may, however, be interesting to study changes in the DMN under different demographic characteristics in the future.
4. DISCUSSION
In this study, we utilized meta‐analytic software and deterministic tractography to construct an anatomic model of the DMN based on the cortical parcellation scheme previously published under the Human Connectome Project (Glasser et al., 2016). The DMN is a critical resting‐state brain network involved in mind‐wandering, and mental time travel, though it is also active during moral reasoning, autobiographical and episodic memory retrieval, and semantic processing (Alves et al., 2019; Buckner et al., 2008; Horn et al., 2014; Murphy et al., 2018). It is also implicated in several disorders including depression, autism, and dementia (Dutta et al., 2019; Ouchi & Kikuchi, 2012; Padmanabhan et al., 2017). Accurately defining regions of the DMN are pivotal to understanding its function which may then offer insights for clinicians into the mechanism and potential therapies for these disorders. In addition, a precise anatomic and connectomic description of the network will allow surgeons to make better judgments during brain surgery. The anatomic constituents of this network are discussed below.
4.1. The anterior cingulate cluster
Cortical areas 10r, a24, p32, and s32 overlap with the ALE in the region of the anterior cingulate cortex, which has been identified as a component of the DMN in multiple studies (Andrews‐Hanna et al., 2014; Buckner et al., 2008; Mars et al., 2012; Schilbach et al., 2008). Area 10r is one of several newly described divisions of the original Brodmann Area 10 (Glasser et al., 2016), which expanded the entire frontal polar cortex from the medial superior frontal gyrus to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Burgess & Wu, 2013; Peng et al., 2018). Little is known about this region; however, it is located in the anterior inferior portion of the medial superior frontal gyrus. Just posterior to area 10r is area s32 which lies in the subcallosal cortex. This region is interconnected to other areas of the limbic system and is known to play a role in emotional response regulation and reward expectation (Beckmann et al., 2009; Palomero‐Gallagher et al., 2008).
Superior to areas 10r and s32 are areas p32 and a24, respectively. Area p32 is located in the antero‐medial superior frontal gyrus, bordering the inferior bend of the callosal sulcus. This region plays a role in the integration of emotional and cognitive information during social interaction tasks to assist in error monitoring (Beckmann et al., 2009; Palomero‐Gallagher et al., 2008). Area a24 is located in the anterior cingulate gyrus proper, lying anterior to the genu of the corpus callosum. This region has been implicated as part of the “affect division” of the anterior cingulate cortex and has been linked to the analysis of internal and external states of mind to assist in emotional expression and motivation (Devinsky et al., 1995; Drevets et al., 2008).
All four areas are interconnected by U‐shaped fibers to one other and demonstrate fiber projections consistent with the cingulum to the posterior cingulate cortex.
4.2. The posterior cingulate cluster
Cortical areas 31a, 31pd, 31pv, d23ab, POS1, POS2, RSC, and v23ab overlap with the ALE in the region of the posterior cingulate cortex, which has also been identified consistently as a component of the DMN across multiple studies (Che et al., 2014; Leech & Sharp, 2014; Lin et al., 2017; Luo et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2019). Based on their functionality and anatomic distribution, these regions can be further classified into dorsal and ventral posterior cingulate regions.
Areas 31a and d23ab comprise the dorsal division of parcellations of the posterior cingulate cortex within the DMN. Area 31a is located on the anterior half of the subparietal gyrus, directly posterior to the marginal sulcus. Area d23ab is located on the posterior cingulate gyrus proper, superior to the splenium of the corpus callosum. Both areas are considered part of the dorsal posterior cingulate cortex which is highly active during tasks that require an external focus, especially concerning visuospatial orientation of the body (Aggleton et al., 2014; Bzdok et al., 2015; Glasser et al., 2016; Leech & Sharp, 2014). Task functional magnetic resonance imaging also indicates that these regions are involved in the working memory processes related to places and body images (Aggleton et al., 2014; Bzdok et al., 2015; Glasser et al., 2016; Leech & Sharp, 2014).
In contrast to areas 31a and d23ab, areas 31pd, 31pv, and v23ab comprise the ventral division of parcellations of the posterior cingulate cortex within the DMN. Area 31pd is located on the posterior superior portion of the subparietal gyrus, while area 31pv is located on the posterior inferior portion of the subparietal gyrus where it extends across the cingulate sulcus onto the posterior cingulate gyrus. Area v23ab is located in along the caudal aspect of the posterior cingulate cortex, near the superior portion of the cingulate isthmus. These areas are considered part of the ventral posterior cingulate cortex which is active during self‐relevant tasks, including retrieval of semantic and episodic memories (Aggleton et al., 2014; Bzdok et al., 2015; Glasser et al., 2016; Leech & Sharp, 2014). Task fMRI studies indicate that these regions are also involved in the working memory processes of body and face images (Aggleton et al., 2014; Bzdok et al., 2015; Glasser et al., 2016; Leech & Sharp, 2014).
Three other areas were identified as forming part of the posterior cingulate cluster within our anatomic model of the DMN: RSC, POS1, and POS2. These regions do not sort easily into either the ventral or dorsal divisions of the DMN. The RSC, or retrosplenial cortex, is a thin region of the cortex that occupies the inferior aspect of posterior cingulate cortex. It lies immediately adjacent to the callosal sulcus and wraps around the splenium of the corpus callosum. This region is primarily responsible for transitioning between allocentric or view‐independent spatial perspectives and egocentric or view‐dependent spatial perspectives (Aggleton et al., 2014; Glasser et al., 2016; Leech & Sharp, 2014; Vann et al., 2009). The RSC is implicated in spatial navigation, episodic memory, future planning, and imagination. In addition, the RSC has been suspected of being involved in the retrieval of recent autobiographical information from memory (Aggleton et al., 2014; Glasser et al., 2016; Leech & Sharp, 2014; Vann et al., 2009).
Regions POS1 and POS2 occupy the inferior and superior halves of the anterior bank of the parieto‐occipital sulcus, respectively. Task fMRI studies demonstrate that areas POS1 and POS2 are activated during the working memory processes of place images (Glasser et al., 2016), and it has been suggested that both regions have a strong, coupled functional correlation with area RSC related to scene comprehension (Glasser et al., 2016).
All eight regions are interconnected by U‐shaped fibers to one other and demonstrate fiber projections consistent with the cingulum to the anterior cingulate cortex. Other studies have reported that a component of the superior longitudinal fasciculus, SLF‐I, connects regions of the precuneus to more anterior regions within the frontal lobe. We were unable to identify fibers of the superior longitudinal fasciculus on tractography. We suspect this may have been due to the SLF‐I’s greater involvement in connecting the precuneus to more dorsal regions of the frontal lobe. There are proposed fibers of the SLF‐I which run closely parallel to the cingulum, namely the paracingulate fascicle, though the validity of performing such segregation on DTI, and whether this segregation has purposeful meaning on function is not clear.
4.3. The lateral parietal cluster
Cortical areas PFm, PGi, PGs, IP1, and TPOJ3 overlap with the ALE in the region of the lateral parietal lobe, which, as for the cingulate cortices, has been identified consistently as a component of the DMN across multiple studies (Alves et al., 2019; Buckner et al., 2008; Lei et al., 2014; Sestieri et al., 2011; Xu et al., 2016; Zanchi et al., 2017). Many of these parcellations are located in the inferior parietal lobule, including areas PFm, PGs, and PGi.
Area PFm is located on the anterior superior surface of the angular gyrus and extends across the sulcus onto the posterior superior bank of the supramarginal gyrus. In contrast, areas PGs and PGi are contained entirely within the angular gyrus, occupying its superior and inferior surfaces, respectively. The functions attributed to these regions are numerous and are summarized below:
-
‐
Area PFm has been shown to be active during nonspatial attention tasks, during decision making tasks when individuals change choices, during rule changes during visually guided attention tasks, and during attentional reorientation (Caspers et al., 2006). The inferior parietal lobule is also involved in the syntactical components of language processing (Ben Shalom & Poeppel, 2008).
-
‐
Area PGs has been shown to be active when individuals change their visuospatial attention from one focus to another (Mars et al., 2011). Specifically, area PGs is involved in the response to biological motion (Mars et al., 2011). The region is also relevant in number processing (Caspers et al., 2011).
-
‐
Area PGi has a functional profile similar to that of area PGs. For example, area PGi has been shown to be active when individuals change their visuospatial attention from one focus to another (Mars et al., 2011). Within the original parcellations study, the Human Connectome Project authors discuss both areas PGi and PGs as major nodes in the DMN (Glasser et al., 2016).
The other two areas identified as part of the DMN were areas IP1 found on the inferior bank of the intraparietal sulcus, and area TPOJ3 found on the posterior inferior portion of the inferior parietal lobule. Area IP1 shows significant activation during mental arithmetic activities, and, as part the intraparietal sulcus, supports more complex parts of numeric and mathematical information processing (Uddin et al., 2010; Wu et al., 2009). The functional profile of area TPOJ3 is not as well characterized.
The three clusters of the DMN we identified in our study aligns with other studies defining the DMN. Our aim was indeed to define the DMN using standardized and precise anatomical nomenclature. Recently, however, one study extended the definition of the DMN to include the thalamus and basal forebrain through aligning structural MRI data with functional maps (Alves et al., 2019). Moreover, we were also unable to identify previously defined components of the DMN in the temporal lobe, including the parahippocampal region and middle temporal gyrus. We suspect this may have been due to our inclusion criteria relying on the complete reporting of MNI coordinates by original studies, which was not uniformly done. Nevertheless, these regions, combined with the basal forebrain and thalamus, may explain the role of the DMN in memory, as reported by previous studies showing DMN activation during memory tasks (Huo et al., 2018; Spreng & Grady, 2010). This may consequently provide new avenues for exploring pathogenesis and treatment in pathological states such as Alzheimer's disease, where a decreased functional connectivity in the DMN has been observed (Qi et al., 2018).
4.4. The strength of connections within the default mode network
The strength of the connections identified between parcellations of the DMN is reported in Table 3. Two different values for strength are recorded based on the average number of tracts across all subjects versus the average number of tracts across subjects in which the connection was actually identified. It is certainly the case that the structural connectivity of the DMN varies to some degree between individuals, and by presenting both sets of average connectional strengths, one can see how connections can vary in the network. For example, the cingulum projection from area 10r to area 31pv has an average strength of 5.2 across all 25 subjects (meaning one would expect to find 5.2 streamlines using the fiber tracking algorithm discussed in the methods) versus an average strength of 32.2 in the four individuals in which the connection was identified. By reporting both numbers, we can see that, while the connection between 10r and 31pv occurs infrequently in the network, in individuals who have such a connection, it is relatively strong.
It should also be noted that we did not set a threshold for the strength that might limit the connections shown for the DMN. For example, assessing the connection between a24 and d23ab via the cingulum, one sees that the average strength across all 25 subjects used in this study was 4.8 versus 7.9 in the fifteen subjects for whom such a connection was actually identified. If we had set a threshold of an average strength of 10.0 or set a threshold related to the frequency by which we saw the connection, that is, in at least 20/25 subjects, then we would not report this connection at all. In our mind, this is incorrect. Instead, it more appropriate to say that the connection between a24 and d23ab, while relatively weak compared to other connections in the network, occurs relatively frequently within the DMN. This is as opposed to reporting that no such connection exists between the two parcellations.
Despite not setting a threshold for network connectivity, the frequency and strength associated with certain connections identified as part of the DMN (e.g., the connection between 31pd and s32 which was identified in one subject with an overall strength of 0.04) raise an important question of which connections are critical for the functionality of the network. Answering this question is beyond the scope of this study, and further research is needed to understand which connections within the DMN are most critical for the successful functioning of the DMN.
5. CONCLUSIONS
We present a preliminary anatomic model of the default mode network. Further studies may refine this model with the ultimate goal of clinical application.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
Dr. Sughrue is the Chief Medical Officer of Omniscient Neurotechnologies. No products directly related to this were discussed in this paper. All other authors have no financial interest or potential conflicts of interest.
ETHICAL STATEMENT
The current study does not have any ethical considerations as it is a meta‐analysis on previously completed data.
Peer Review
The peer review history for this article is available at https://publons.com/publon/10.1002/brb3.1976.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Data were provided [in part] by the Human Connectome Project, WU‐Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 NIH Institutes and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research; and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University.
Sandhu Z, Tanglay O, Young IM, et al. Parcellation‐based anatomic modeling of the default mode network. Brain Behav.2021;11:e01976 10.1002/brb3.1976
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author (MS), upon reasonable request.
REFERENCES
- Aggleton, J. P. , Saunders, R. C. , Wright, N. F. , & Vann, S. D. (2014). The origin of projections from the posterior cingulate and retrosplenial cortices to the anterior, medial dorsal and laterodorsal thalamic nuclei of macaque monkeys. European Journal of Neuroscience, 39(1), 107–123. 10.1111/ejn.12389 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alves, P. N. , Foulon, C. , Karolis, V. , Bzdok, D. , Margulies, D. S. , Volle, E. , & Thiebaut de Schotten, M. (2019). An improved neuroanatomical model of the default‐mode network reconciles previous neuroimaging and neuropathological findings. Communications Biology, 2, 370 10.1038/s42003-019-0611-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson, J. S. , Ferguson, M. A. , Lopez‐Larson, M. , & Yurgelun‐Todd, D. (2011). Connectivity gradients between the default mode and attention control networks. Brain Connectivity, 1(2), 147–157. 10.1089/brain.2011.0007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews‐Hanna, J. R. , Reidler, J. S. , Sepulcre, J. , Poulin, R. , & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional‐anatomic fractionation of the brain's default network. Neuron, 65(4), 550–562. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews‐Hanna, J. R. , Smallwood, J. , & Spreng, R. N. (2014). The default network and self‐generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316(1), 29–52. 10.1111/nyas.12360 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann, C. F. , DeLuca, M. , Devlin, J. T. , & Smith, S. M. (2005). Investigations into resting‐state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 360(1457), 1001–1013. 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann, M. , Johansen‐Berg, H. , & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2009). Connectivity‐based parcellation of human cingulate cortex and its relation to functional specialization. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(4), 1175–1190. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3328-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ben Shalom, D. , & Poeppel, D. (2008). Functional anatomic models of language: Assembling the pieces. The Neuroscientist, 14(1), 119–127. 10.1177/1073858407305726 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bressler, S. L. , & Menon, V. (2010). Large‐scale brain networks in cognition: Emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci, 14(6), 277–290. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner, R. L. , Andrews‐Hanna, J. R. , & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain's default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38. 10.1196/annals.1440.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess, P. , & Wu, H.‐C. (2013). Rostral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann area 10): metacognition in the brain. In (pp. 524–534).
- Burks, J. D. , Conner, A. K. , Bonney, P. A. , Glenn, C. A. , Baker, C. M. , Boettcher, L. B. , Briggs, R. G. , O’Donoghue, D. L. , Wu, D. H. , & Sughrue, M. E. (2018). Anatomy and white matter connections of the orbitofrontal gyrus. Journal of Neurosurgery, 128(6), 1865–1872. 10.3171/2017.3.JNS162070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bzdok, D. , Heeger, A. , Langner, R. , Laird, A. R. , Fox, P. T. , Palomero‐Gallagher, N. , Vogt, B. A. , Zilles, K. , & Eickhoff, S. B. (2015). Subspecialization in the human posterior medial cortex. NeuroImage, 106, 55–71. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspers, S. , Eickhoff, S. B. , Rick, T. , von Kapri, A. , Kuhlen, T. , Huang, R. , Shah, N. J. , & Zilles, K. (2011). Probabilistic fibre tract analysis of cytoarchitectonically defined human inferior parietal lobule areas reveals similarities to macaques. NeuroImage, 58(2), 362–380. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspers, S. , Geyer, S. , Schleicher, A. , Mohlberg, H. , Amunts, K. , & Zilles, K. (2006). The human inferior parietal cortex: Cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. NeuroImage, 33(2), 430–448. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chand, G. B. , Wu, J. , Hajjar, I. , & Qiu, D. (2017). Interactions of the salience network and its subsystems with the default‐mode and the central‐executive networks in normal aging and mild cognitive impairment. Brain Connectivity, 7(7), 401–412. 10.1089/brain.2017.0509 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Che, X. , Zhang, Q. , Zhao, J. , Wei, D. , Li, B. , Guo, Y. , Qiu, J. , & Liu, Y. (2014). Synchronous activation within the default mode network correlates with perceived social support. Neuropsychologia, 63, 26–33. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen, H. J. , Lin, H. L. , Chen, Q. F. , & Liu, P. F. (2017). Altered dynamic functional connectivity in the default mode network in patients with cirrhosis and minimal hepatic encephalopathy. Neuroradiology, 59(9), 905–914. 10.1007/s00234-017-1881-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiong, W. , Wilson, S. M. , D’Esposito, M. , Kayser, A. S. , Grossman, S. N. , Poorzand, P. , Seeley, W. W. , Miller, B. L. , & Rankin, K. P. (2013). The salience network causally influences default mode network activity during moral reasoning. Brain, 136(Pt 6), 1929–1941. 10.1093/brain/awt066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clemens, B. , Wagels, L. , Bauchmuller, M. , Bergs, R. , Habel, U. , & Kohn, N. (2017). Alerted default mode: Functional connectivity changes in the aftermath of social stress. Scientific Reports, 7, 40180 10.1038/srep40180 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole, M. W. , Bassett, D. S. , Power, J. D. , Braver, T. S. , & Petersen, S. E. (2014). Intrinsic and task‐evoked network architectures of the human brain. Neuron, 83(1), 238–251. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crittenden, B. M. , Mitchell, D. J. , & Duncan, J. (2015). Recruitment of the default mode network during a demanding act of executive control. Elife, 4, e06481 10.7554/eLife.06481 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Luca, M. , Beckmann, C. F. , De Stefano, N. , Matthews, P. M. , & Smith, S. M. (2006). fMRI resting state networks define distinct modes of long‐distance interactions in the human brain. NeuroImage, 29(4), 1359–1367. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devinsky, O. , Morrell, M. J. , & Vogt, B. A. (1995). Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain, 118(1), 279–306. 10.1093/brain/118.1.279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doll, A. , Holzel, B. K. , Boucard, C. C. , Wohlschlager, A. M. , & Sorg, C. (2015). Mindfulness is associated with intrinsic functional connectivity between default mode and salience networks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 461 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00461 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drevets, W. C. , Savitz, J. , & Trimble, M. (2008). The subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in mood disorders. CNS Spectrums, 13(8), 663–681. 10.1017/S1092852900013754 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dutta, A. , McKie, S. , Downey, D. , Thomas, E. , Juhasz, G. , Arnone, D. , Elliott, R. , Williams, S. , Deakin, J. F. W. , & Anderson, I. M. (2019). Regional default mode network connectivity in major depressive disorder: Modulation by acute intravenous citalopram. Translational Psychiatry, 9(1), 116 10.1038/s41398-019-0447-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff, S. B. , Bzdok, D. , Laird, A. R. , Kurth, F. , & Fox, P. T. (2012). Activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis revisited. NeuroImage, 59(3), 2349–2361. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff, S. B. , Laird, A. R. , Grefkes, C. , Wang, L. E. , Zilles, K. , & Fox, P. T. (2009). Coordinate‐based activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis of neuroimaging data: A random‐effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Human Brain Mapping, 30(9), 2907–2926. 10.1002/hbm.20718 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans, A. C. , Marrett, S. , Neelin, P. , Collins, L. , Worsley, K. , Dai, W. , Milot, S. , Meyer, E. , & Bub, D. (1992). Anatomical mapping of functional activation in stereotactic coordinate space. NeuroImage, 1(1), 43–53. 10.1016/1053-8119(92)90006-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox, M. D. , Snyder, A. Z. , Vincent, J. L. , Corbetta, M. , Van Essen, D. C. , & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(27), 9673–9678. 10.1073/pnas.0504136102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox, P. T. , Laird, A. R. , Fox, S. P. , Fox, P. M. , Uecker, A. M. , Crank, M. , Koenig, S. F. , & Lancaster, J. L. (2005). Brainmap taxonomy of experimental design: Description and evaluation. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 185–198. 10.1002/hbm.20141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox, P. T. , & Lancaster, J. L. (2002). Mapping context and content: The BrainMap model. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(4), 319–321. 10.1038/nrn789 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fransson, P. (2006). How default is the default mode of brain function? Further evidence from intrinsic BOLD signal fluctuations. Neuropsychologia, 44(14), 2836–2845. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasser, M. F. , Coalson, T. S. , Robinson, E. C. , Hacker, C. D. , Harwell, J. , Yacoub, E. , Ugurbil, K. , Andersson, J. , Beckmann, C. F. , Jenkinson, M. , Smith, S. M. , & Van Essen, D. C. (2016). A multi‐modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature, 536(7615), 171–178. 10.1038/nature18933 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasser, M. F. , Sotiropoulos, S. N. , Wilson, J. A. , Coalson, T. S. , Fischl, B. , Andersson, J. L. , Xu, J. , Jbabdi, S. , Webster, M. , Polimeni, J. R. , Van Essen, D. C. , & Jenkinson, M. (2013). The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage, 80, 105–124. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greicius, M. D. , Krasnow, B. , Reiss, A. L. , & Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(1), 253–258. 10.1073/pnas.0135058100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horn, A. , Ostwald, D. , Reisert, M. , & Blankenburg, F. (2014). The structural‐functional connectome and the default mode network of the human brain. NeuroImage, 102(Pt 1), 142–151. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huo, L. , Li, R. , Wang, P. , Zheng, Z. , & Li, J. (2018). The default mode network supports episodic memory in cognitively unimpaired elderly individuals: Different contributions to immediate recall and delayed recall. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 10, 6 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamali, A. , Sair, H. I. , Radmanesh, A. , & Hasan, K. M. (2014). Decoding the superior parietal lobule connections of the superior longitudinal fasciculus/arcuate fasciculus in the human brain. Neuroscience, 277, 577–583. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.07.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy, D. P. , & Courchesne, E. (2008). Functional abnormalities of the default network during self‐ and other‐reflection in autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3(2), 177–190. 10.1093/scan/nsn011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konishi, M. , McLaren, D. G. , Engen, H. , & Smallwood, J. (2015). Shaped by the past: The default mode network supports cognition that is independent of immediate perceptual input. PLoS One, 10(6), e0132209 10.1371/journal.pone.0132209 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird, A. R. , Eickhoff, S. B. , Li, K. , Robin, D. A. , Glahn, D. C. , & Fox, P. T. (2009). Investigating the functional heterogeneity of the default mode network using coordinate‐based meta‐analytic modeling. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(46), 14496–14505. 10.1523/jneurosci.4004-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird, A. R. , Lancaster, J. L. , & Fox, P. T. (2005). BrainMap: The social evolution of a human brain mapping database. Neuroinformatics, 3(1), 65–78. 10.1385/NI:3:1:065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larobina, M. , & Murino, L. (2014). Medical image file formats. Journal of Digital Imaging, 27(2), 200–206. 10.1007/s10278-013-9657-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leech, R. , & Sharp, D. J. (2014). The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain, 137(1), 12–32. 10.1093/brain/awt162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lei, X. , Wang, Y. , Yuan, H. , & Mantini, D. (2014). Neuronal oscillations and functional interactions between resting state networks. Human Brain Mapping, 35(7), 3517–3528. 10.1002/hbm.22418 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin, P. , Yang, Y. , Gao, J. , De Pisapia, N. , Ge, S. , Wang, X. , Zuo, C. S. , Jonathan Levitt, J. , & Niu, C. (2017). Dynamic default mode network across different brain states. Scientific Reports, 7, 46088 10.1038/srep46088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo, Y. , Kong, F. , Qi, S. , You, X. , & Huang, X. (2016). Resting‐state functional connectivity of the default mode network associated with happiness. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(3), 516–524. 10.1093/scan/nsv132 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maresh, E. L. , Allen, J. P. , & Coan, J. A. (2014). Increased default mode network activity in socially anxious individuals during reward processing. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 4, 7 10.1186/2045-5380-4-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mars, R. B. , Jbabdi, S. , Sallet, J. , O'Reilly, J. X. , Croxson, P. L. , Olivier, E. , Noonan, M. P. , Bergmann, C. , Mitchell, A. S. , Baxter, M. G. , Behrens, T. E. J. , Johansen‐Berg, H. , Tomassini, V. , Miller, K. L. , & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2011). Diffusion‐weighted imaging tractography‐based parcellation of the human parietal cortex and comparison with human and macaque resting‐state functional connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(11), 4087–4100. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5102-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mars, R. , Neubert, F.‐X. , Noonan, M. , Sallet, J. , Toni, I. , & Rushworth, M. (2012). On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain”. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 189 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martino, J. , De Witt Hamer, P. C. , Berger, M. S. , Lawton, M. T. , Arnold, C. M. , de Lucas, E. M. , & Duffau, H. (2013). Analysis of the subcomponents and cortical terminations of the perisylvian superior longitudinal fasciculus: A fiber dissection and DTI tractography study. Brain Structure and Function, 218(1), 105–121. 10.1007/s00429-012-0386-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason, M. F. , Norton, M. I. , Van Horn, J. D. , Wegner, D. M. , Grafton, S. T. , & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus‐independent thought. Science, 315(5810), 393–395. 10.1126/science.1131295 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy, C. , Jefferies, E. , Rueschemeyer, S.‐A. , Sormaz, M. , Wang, H.‐T. , Margulies, D. S. , & Smallwood, J. (2018). Distant from input: Evidence of regions within the default mode network supporting perceptually‐decoupled and conceptually‐guided cognition. NeuroImage, 171, 393–401. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ouchi, Y. , & Kikuchi, M. (2012). A review of the default mode network in aging and dementia based on molecular imaging. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 23(3), 263–268. 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Padmanabhan, A. , Lynch, C. J. , Schaer, M. , & Menon, V. (2017). The default mode network in autism. Biological Psychiatry. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 2(6), 476–486. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.04.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palomero‐Gallagher, N. , Mohlberg, H. , Zilles, K. , & Vogt, B. (2008). Cytology and receptor architecture of human anterior cingulate cortex. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 508(6), 906–926. 10.1002/cne.21684 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peng, K. , Steele, S. C. , Becerra, L. , & Borsook, D. (2018). Brodmann area 10: Collating, integrating and high level processing of nociception and pain. Progress in Neurobiology, 161, 1–22. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2017.11.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piccoli, T. , Valente, G. , Linden, D. E. , Re, M. , Esposito, F. , Sack, A. T. , & Di Salle, F. (2015). The default mode network and the working memory network are not anti‐correlated during all phases of a working memory task. PLoS One, 10(4), e0123354 10.1371/journal.pone.0123354 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pletzer, B. , Kronbichler, M. , Nuerk, H. C. , & Kerschbaum, H. H. (2015). Mathematics anxiety reduces default mode network deactivation in response to numerical tasks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 202 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00202 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poerio, G. L. , Sormaz, M. , Wang, H. T. , Margulies, D. , Jefferies, E. , & Smallwood, J. (2017). The role of the default mode network in component processes underlying the wandering mind. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(7), 1047–1062. 10.1093/scan/nsx041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qi, H. , Liu, H. , Hu, H. , He, H. , & Zhao, X. (2018). Primary disruption of the memory‐related subsystems of the default mode network in Alzheimer's disease: Resting‐state functional connectivity MRI study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 10, 344 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00344 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle, M. E. , MacLeod, A. M. , Snyder, A. Z. , Powers, W. J. , Gusnard, D. A. , & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(2), 676 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Razlighi, Q. R. (2018). Task‐evoked negative BOLD response in the default mode network does not alter its functional connectivity. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 12, 67 10.3389/fncom.2018.00067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schilbach, L. , Eickhoff, S. B. , Rotarska‐Jagiela, A. , Fink, G. R. , & Vogeley, K. (2008). Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the "default system" of the brain. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(2), 457–467. 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sestieri, C. , Corbetta, M. , Romani, G. L. , & Shulman, G. L. (2011). Episodic memory retrieval, parietal cortex, and the default mode network: Functional and topographic analyses. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(12), 4407–4420. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3335-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, S. M. , Beckmann, C. F. , Andersson, J. , Auerbach, E. J. , Bijsterbosch, J. , Douaud, G. , Duff, E. , Feinberg, D. A. , Griffanti, L. , Harms, M. P. , Kelly, M. , Laumann, T. , Miller, K. L. , Moeller, S. , Petersen, S. , Power, J. , Salimi‐Khorshidi, G. , Snyder, A. Z. , Vu, A. T. , … Glasser, M. F. (2013). Resting‐state fMRI in the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage, 80, 144–168. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng, R. N. , & Grady, C. L. (2010). Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the default mode network. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(6), 1112–1123. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21282 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng, R. N. , & Schacter, D. L. (2012). Default network modulation and large‐scale network interactivity in healthy young and old adults. Cerebral Cortex, 22(11), 2610–2621. 10.1093/cercor/bhr339 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stawarczyk, D. , Majerus, S. , Maquet, P. , & D'Argembeau, A. (2011). Neural correlates of ongoing conscious experience: Both task‐unrelatedness and stimulus‐independence are related to default network activity. PLoS One, 6(2), e16997 10.1371/journal.pone.0016997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taruffi, L. , Pehrs, C. , Skouras, S. , & Koelsch, S. (2017). Effects of sad and happy music on mind‐wandering and the default mode network. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 14396 10.1038/s41598-017-14849-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thirion, B. , Dodel, S. , & Poline, J.‐B. (2006). Detection of signal synchronizations in resting‐state fMRI datasets. NeuroImage, 29(1), 321–327. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turkeltaub, P. E. , Eickhoff, S. B. , Laird, A. R. , Fox, M. , Wiener, M. , & Fox, P. (2012). Minimizing within‐experiment and within‐group effects in activation likelihood estimation meta‐analyses. Human Brain Mapping, 33(1), 1–13. 10.1002/hbm.21186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uddin, L. Q. , Supekar, K. , Amin, H. , Rykhlevskaia, E. , Nguyen, D. A. , Greicius, M. D. , & Menon, V. (2010). Dissociable connectivity within human angular gyrus and intraparietal sulcus: Evidence from functional and structural connectivity. Cerebral Cortex, 20(11), 2636–2646. 10.1093/cercor/bhq011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Utevsky, A. V. , Smith, D. V. , & Huettel, S. A. (2014). Precuneus is a functional core of the default‐mode network. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(3), 932–940. 10.1523/jneurosci.4227-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen, D. C. , & Glasser, M. F. (2016). The Human Connectome Project: Progress and prospects. Cerebrum: the Dana Forum on Brain Science, 2016, cer‐10‐16. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5198757/ [Google Scholar]
- Vann, S. D. , Aggleton, J. P. , & Maguire, E. A. (2009). What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(11), 792–802. 10.1038/nrn2733 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vatansever, D. , Menon, D. K. , Manktelow, A. E. , Sahakian, B. J. , & Stamatakis, E. A. (2015). Default mode network connectivity during task execution. NeuroImage, 122, 96–104. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, R. W. Y. , Chang, W.‐L. , Chuang, S.‐W. , & Liu, I. N. (2019). Posterior cingulate cortex can be a regulatory modulator of the default mode network in task‐negative state. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 7565 10.1038/s41598-019-43885-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu, S. S. , Chang, T. T. , Majid, A. , Caspers, S. , Eickhoff, S. B. , & Menon, V. (2009). Functional heterogeneity of inferior parietal cortex during mathematical cognition assessed with cytoarchitectonic probability maps. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 2930–2945. 10.1093/cercor/bhp063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu, J. , Vik, A. , Groote, I. R. , Lagopoulos, J. , Holen, A. , Ellingsen, Ø. , Håberg, A. K. , & Davanger, S. (2014). Nondirective meditation activates default mode network and areas associated with memory retrieval and emotional processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 86 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00086 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu, X. , Yuan, H. , & Lei, X. (2016). Activation and connectivity within the default mode network contribute independently to future‐oriented thought. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 21001 10.1038/srep21001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang, X. F. , Bossmann, J. , Schiffhauer, B. , Jordan, M. , & Immordino‐Yang, M. H. (2012). Intrinsic default mode network connectivity predicts spontaneous verbal descriptions of autobiographical memories during social processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 592 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00592 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeh, F.‐C. , Wedeen, V. J. J. , & Tseng, W.‐Y.‐I. (2010). Generalized q‐sampling imaging. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 29(9), 1626–1635. 10.1109/TMI.2010.2045126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanchi, D. , Montandon, M.‐L. , Sinanaj, I. , Rodriguez, C. , Depoorter, A. , Herrmann, F. R. , Borgwardt, S. , Giannakopoulos, P. , & Haller, S. (2017). Decreased fronto‐parietal and increased default mode network activation is associated with subtle cognitive deficits in elderly controls. Neurosignals, 25(1), 127–138. 10.1159/000486152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author (MS), upon reasonable request.