Abstract
Introduction
It is unclear whether the association between Mediterranean‐type Diet (MeDi) and cognition is similar across different racial/ethnic groups or between women and men.
Methods
The current study included 2435 participants (≥60 years of age) of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2011‐2014. Linear regression models were applied to examine the association between diet and cognition, adjusted for multiple demographic variables. Interaction between MeDi and race/ethnicity (non‐Hispanic White, non‐Hispanic Black, Hispanic, other), and between MeDi and gender, were examined, followed by stratified analyses.
Results
One score increase in MeDi was associated with 0.039 (95%confidence interval [CI] = 0.016‐0.062) higher global cognitive z‐score. Significant associations between MeDi and global cognition and between MeDi and immediate recall were found in the non‐Hispanic Whites only (P‐interaction = 0.057 and 0.059, respectively). MeDi was associated with increased score of animal fluency score in men but not in women (P‐interaction = 0.082).
Discussion
The positive association between MeDi and cognition might be dependent on race/ethnicity and gender.
Keywords: aging, cognition, diet, epidemiology, NHANES
1. INTRODUCTION
With an aging population, cognitive impairment and dementia have emerged as a public health challenge worldwide. 1 Currently there is no effective medical treatment for dementia; primary prevention through modifiable risk factors, such as healthy dietary habits, is thus an urgent priority to reduce the incidence of cognitive impairment and dementia. 2
There is emerging evidence supporting protective roles of certain dietary patterns on cognitive aging and dementia prevention. 3 , 4 , 5 , 6 Mediterranean diet (MeDi) refers to a traditional diet in the Mediterranean countries, characterized by a high consumption of olive oil, leafy green vegetables, fruits, cereals, legumes, and fish; low intake of meat and dairy products; and moderate consumption of alcohol. 3 MeDi is the most commonly examined dietary pattern in the cognitive aging literature and shows a promising beneficial role on cognitive function in older adults, 7 , 8 , 9 , 10 , 11 , 12 , 13 , 14 , 15 , 16 , 17 , 18 , 19 , 20 although the findings are not always consistent. 21 , 22 , 23 , 24 , 25
The inconsistency might be due to the discrepancies in study design, sample size, population, statistical methods, and assessments of diet and cognition. 3 In particular, cross‐race variation in dietary habits and quality may have contributed to the inconsistency. 26 Non‐Hispanic Blacks (NHBs) and Hispanics have lower consumption of fruits and vegetables than non‐Hispanic Whites (NHWs). 26 Similarly, men and women also tend to have different dietary preference, appetite sensations, and overall dietary habits. 27 , 28 Meanwhile, there are sex/gender and racial/ethnic disparities in dementia risk and cognitive decline in late life, such that women have higher risk than men and NHBs and Hispanics have higher risk than NHWs, but the exact mechanism is unknown. 29 , 30 To reduce cognitive health disparities through healthier lifestyles, association between dietary pattern and cognitive health needs to be examined further in national representative samples with male and female participants of multiple racial/ethnic groups. Identifying the potential effect modifiers for the diet‐cognition association may also help with developing subpopulation‐specific preventive measures for cognitive aging.
However, there is a paucity of research examining the role of sex and race/ethnicity as modifiers of the diet‐cognition association in later life, and the existing data have been inconsistent. Although the cognitive benefits of MeDi have been reported across regions and multiple races/ethnic groups, 6 , 8 most studies were based on predominantly White populations. 14 , 15 , 20 , 22 , 23 , 25 A few studies with multiple racial/ethnic groups did not examine 14 or did not find an interaction between race/ethnicity and MeDi on cognition. 7 , 9 , 11 , 31 In one study, stronger adherence to the MeDi reduced the rate of cognitive decline among African Americans, but not in White older adults. 13 Gender difference in the association between dietary factors and cognition has been rarely tested, and only one previous study found a stronger association of MeDi with cognition in men than in women. 16
In the present study, we examined whether the associations of MeDi with cognition vary by race/ethnicity and gender, using data from the nationally representative sample, the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2011‐2014.
2. METHODS
2.1. Participants and setting
The NHANES study, an ongoing cross‐sectional survey, is conducted biennially to assess the health and nutritional status of the noninstitutionalized U.S. population. Details of the study design and data collection were reported elsewhere. 32 Briefly, individuals who completed the in‐home interview were then invited to participate in the physical examination. The interview survey included demographic, dietary, lifestyle, and health‐related factors; the examination component consisted of medical and physical measurements and laboratory tests administered by highly trained medical personnel. The protocol of NHANES was approved by the National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS) Research Ethics Review Board. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant.
HIGHLIGHTS
In the current cross‐sectional study of elderly (age 60 and older) participants from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2011‐2014, high adherence to Mediterranean‐type Diet (MeDi) was associated with better cognition.
Race/ethnicity and gender modified the positive association between MeDi and cognition.
Higher adherence to MeDi was associated with higher cognitive scores in non‐Hispanic Whites, but not in other racial/ethnic groups including non‐Hispanic Blacks, Hispanics, and others.
There was a significant association between MeDi and executive function in men but not in women.
RESEARCH IN CONTEXT
Systematic review: The author reviewed previously published findings on the Mediterranean‐type Diet (MeDi) and cognition. Previous findings have suggested a positive association between MeDi and cognitive health, but it is unclear whether the association is similar across different racial/ethnic groups or between women and men.
Interpretation: Our study showed significant protective effects of MeDi on cognitive health in a nationally representative U.S. elderly population, and the positive association between MeDi and cognition seemed to be dependent on race/ethnicity and gender, with a stronger association in men than in women, and in non‐Hispanic Whites than in other racial/ethnic groups.
Future directions: Future prospective studies with a large sample of minority older adults and with both men and women are warranted to confirm the findings.
The present study analyzed cognitive data from NHANES 2011‐2014 in which a series of tests were conducted to assess the cognitive function (n = 3472) (Figure 1). Participates were excluded for the missing data on cognition assessment (n = 538), MeDi score (n = 228), and covariates (n = 271). Finally, a total of 2435 subjects were included in the primary data analyses (Table S1).
FIGURE 1.
Flow chart of subject selection
2.2. Assessment of MeDi
Dietary information was obtained through validated computer‐assisted 24‐hour dietary recall interviews using the U.S. Department of Agriculture's Automated Multiple‐Pass Method; the interviews were conducted by trained dietary interviewers. 33 The degree of adherence to MeDi was assessed by the MeDi scale constructed with nine components. 7 The sex‐specific median of energy‐adjusted intake was used as the cut‐off value for each of the nine MeDi components. One point was given for intake equal or greater than the cut‐off value for the beneficial components (vegetables, fruits, legumes, cereals, fish, and mono‐unsaturated fatty acids: saturated fatty acids ratio), or for intake less than the cut‐off value for the detrimental components (dairy products, and meat and poultry), or for a mild‐to‐moderate consumption of alcohol (> 0 to ≤1 drink/day for women, and > 0 and ≤2 drink/day for men). The total MeDi ranged from 0 to 9, with higher score indicating a better adherence to MeDi.
2.3. Assessment of cognition
There were three tests administered to assess the cognitive function among the subjects age 60 and older: the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD) Word Learning subset to evaluate immediate and delayed recall of new verbal information (memory sub‐domain); the Animal Fluency (AF) test to examine verbal semantic fluency (component of executive function); and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) to assess attention, processing speed, and working memory. 32
The module from CERAD consisted of 10‐item word list learning trials. The participants were asked to recall as many words as possible immediately after 10 unrelated words were read. Three consecutive immediate learning trials and one delayed trial were conducted in CERAD. The immediate recall (CERAD‐IR) score, calculated as the sum of the three immediate recalls, ranged from 0 to 30, and the delayed recall (CERAD‐DR) score ranged from 0 to 10.
Participants taking the AF test were asked to name as many animals as possible in 1 minute, with 1 point given for each correct answer. The score corresponds to the total number of correct answers.
The DSST was a module from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition (WAIS‐III). Participants were asked to copy in 2 minutes as many number‐corresponding symbols as possible in 133 boxes on paper with keys at the top containing nine numbers paired with symbols. The total score was summarized as the number of the correct matches (ranging from 0 to 133).
To assess the global cognitive function, which was more consistently associated with MeDi compared to other individual cognitive tests, 5 we calculated a composite cognitive z‐score by averaging the standardized scores of each cognitive test (CERAD‐IR, DERAD‐DR, AF, and DSST).
2.4. Covariates
The information on demographics including age, self‐reported gender (male, female), race/ethnicity (NHW, NHB, Hispanic, (Mexican‐American, and other Hispanics), Others (including Asians, others, and mixed race/ethnicities), education attainment (under high school, high school, above high school), marital status (married/cohabiting, divorced/widowed/separated, never married), and ratio of family income to poverty (PIR; ≤1, > 1) were collected via questionnaires. Smokers were the subjects who reported at least 100 lifetime cigarettes, and smoking status was categorized into three groups of never, current, and past. Dietary caloric (kcal) intake was obtained from the 24‐hour dietary recall. Recreational physical activity was classified into four levels: sedentary (no regular physical activity), insufficient (regular activity, <500 metabolic equivalent [MET] minutes/week), moderate (500 to 1000 MET minutes/week), and high (>1000 MET‐minutes/week). The body mass index (BMI) was divided into normal weight of <25, overweight of 25 to <30, and obesity of ≥30 kg/m2. Depression was measured by the brief Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ‐9), and a cut‐off score of 10 was applied to identify clinically relevant depression. Co‐morbidity burden was calculated as the total number (ranging 0‐4) of self‐reported comorbidities including hypertension, hyperlipidemia, diabetes, and cardiovascular disease (any history of heart failure, coronary heart disease, angina pectoris, heart attack).
2.5. Statistical analysis
Weighted analyses were applied to account for the complex sampling design of NHANES. The 2‐year weights (WTMEC2YR) were selected for the cognitive testing subsamples in NHANES 2011‐2012 and 2013‐2014. The final weight (WTMEC2YR/2) for NHANES 2011‐2014 was recalculated when the two survey cycles were combined. Taylor linearization 34 was used for the variance estimation.
The characteristics of participants were presented as means and proportions for continuous and categorical variables, respectively. Subjects were categorized into three groups according to the tertiles of MeDi. Differences of characteristics across the tertiles of MeDi, race/ethnicity, and gender, were tested with the chi‐square test for categorical variables and a linear regression model for continuous variable.
The association between adherence to MeDi and cognitive performance was examined by linear regression models with global cognitive z‐score as dependent variables and tertiles of MeDi as predictors (with the lowest tertile as the reference), first unadjusted and then adjusted for age, gender, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, PIR, smoking status, BMI category, physical activity, depression, and dietary calories. The linear trend of associations across tertiles of MeDi was tested by assigning the median values to each tertile and treating as a continuous variable in the adjusted models. The MeDi was also treated as a continuous variable to fit the models.
To examine the potential effect modification, a product term between MeDi (continuous score) and race/ethnicity (categorical, NHW as the reference group) or gender (men as the reference group) was added separately in the adjusted models, in which the product term was tested by the adjusted Wald test, with P values ≤0.10 considered as significant for the interaction. Stratification analyses were subsequently performed. Marginal effects of MeDi (continuous score) on cognition function were separately calculated by race/ethnicity and by gender.
Post hoc analyses were performed for each individual cognitive score, and for nine MeDi components (simultaneously in the model), similar to the main analysis for global cognitive score. Sensitivity analyses were performed: (1) Subjects who had difficulties in thinking and remembering, based on the question “During the past 12 months, have you (or has she/has he) experienced confusion or memory loss that is happening more often or is getting worse?”, were excluded from the data analyses; (2) co‐morbidity burden was additionally adjusted; and (3) using an alternative dietary pattern score, Healthy Eating Index‐2015 (HEI‐2015).
3. RESULTS
3.1. Characteristics of the study participants
Participant characteristics are presented in Table 1. The study sample included a total of 46,488,866 U.S. older subjects. Among all the subjects, the mean age was 68.99 years, and male subjects accounted for 46.25%. The proportions of NHW, NHB, Hispanic, and Others were 81.16%, 7.83%, 6.50%, and 4.51%, respectively. Participants with higher adherence to MeDi were more likely to be older, to be women, to have higher education levels, to be married/cohabiting, to have better economic conditions, to be never smokers, to have normal BMI, and to have higher levels of physical activity (P < 0.05, Table 1). Mean scores of all the cognitive tests, except for CERAD‐IR and CERAD‐DR, increased with increasing MeDi (P < 0.05).
TABLE 1.
Characteristics of study subjects from NHANES 2011‐2014
Tertiles of MeDi | P values | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Characteristics | Total | T1 (0 to 3) | T2 (4 to 5) | T3 (6 to 9) | Overall | T1 vs T2 a | T1 vs T3 a | T2 vs T3 a |
Sample size, n (%) | 2435 | 841 | 1129 | 465 | ||||
Representative population size | 46,488,866 | 15,620,163 | 21,740,153 | 9,128,550 | ||||
MeDi, mean (SD) | 4.15 (1.55) | 2.43 (0.76) | 4.44 (0.50) | 6.39 (0.63) | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 |
Age (years), mean (SD) | 68.99 (6.58) | 68.02 (6.45) | 69.45 (6.60) | 69.57 (6.62) | 0.001 | 0.003 | 0.003 | 0.803 |
Gender, n (%) | ||||||||
Male | 1190 (46.25) | 399 (43.97) | 543 (44.66) | 248 (53.94) | 0.049 | 0.850 | 0.027 | 0.044 |
Female | 1245 (53.75) | 442 (56.03) | 586 (55.34) | 217 (46.06) | ||||
Race/ethnicity, n (%) | ||||||||
Non‐Hispanic White | 1224 (81.16) | 415 (81.27) | 574 (81.21) | 235 (80.86) | 0.004 | 0.047 | 0.002 | 0.178 |
Non‐Hispanic Black | 566 (7.83) | 214 (8.56) | 252 (7.67) | 100 (6.97) | ||||
Hispanic | 445 (6.50) | 179 (7.75) | 201 (6.22) | 65 (5.03) | ||||
Others | 200 (4.51) | 33 (2.42) | 102 (4.90) | 65 (7.15) | ||||
Education, n (%) | ||||||||
Less than high school | 574 (14.84) | 240 (18.66) | 263 (14.59) | 71 (8.87) | < 0.001 | 0.142 | < 0.001 | 0.002 |
High school or equivalent | 567 (21.40) | 226 (23.65) | 263 (22.23) | 78 (15.56) | ||||
Above high school | 1294 (63.76) | 375 (57.68) | 603 (63.17) | 316 (75.57) | ||||
Marital status, n (%) | ||||||||
Married/cohabiting | 1415 (65.69) | 459 (62.76) | 658 (64.66) | 298 (73.17) | 0.018 | 0.738 | 0.010 | 0.035 |
Devoiced/widowed/separated | 885 (30.18) | 330 (33.26) | 419 (31.48) | 136 (21.82) | ||||
Never married | 135 (4.13) | 52 (3.98) | 52 (3.87) | 31 (5.00) | ||||
PIR, n (%) | ||||||||
≤1 | 398 (8.70) | 169 (10.91) | 180 (8.82) | 49 (4.63) | < 0.001 | 0.088 | < 0.001 | 0.001 |
>1 | 2037 (91.3) | 672 (89.09) | 949 (91.18) | 416 (95.37) | ||||
Smoking, n (%) | ||||||||
Never | 1189 (49.26) | 376 (46.3) | 572 (49.97) | 241 (52.61) | 0.001 | 0.025 | 0.001 | 0.025 |
Current | 298 (10.69) | 149 (15.88) | 116 (9.46) | 33 (4.73) | ||||
Past | 948 (40.05) | 316 (37.81) | 441 (40.57) | 191 (42.66) | ||||
BMI category, n (%) | ||||||||
Normal | 637 (25.82) | 200 (23.05) | 287 (24.22) | 150 (34.38) | 0.008 | 0.735 | 0.002 | 0.011 |
Overweight | 854 (36.04) | 288 (35.50) | 399 (36.68) | 167 (35.45) | ||||
Obese | 944 (38.13) | 353 (41.45) | 443 (39.09) | 148 (30.17) | ||||
Physical activity, n (%) | ||||||||
Sedentary | 1381 (53.49) | 569 (64.78) | 617 (52.82) | 195 (35.78) | < 0.001 | 0.016 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 |
Insufficient | 353 (14.16) | 103 (11.76) | 185 (15.98) | 65 (13.94) | ||||
Moderate | 275 (12.01) | 70 (9.25) | 131 (12.08) | 74 (16.57) | ||||
High | 426 (20.34) | 99 (14.21) | 196 (19.12) | 131 (33.71) | ||||
Depression, n (%) | ||||||||
No | 2218 (92.89) | 747 (91.37) | 1036 (92.79) | 435 (95.7) | 0.077 | 0.358 | 0.075 | 0.162 |
Yes | 217 (7.11) | 94 (8.63) | 93 (7.21) | 30 (4.30) | ||||
Dietary calories (kcal), mean (SD) | 1905.61 (794.54) | 2011.84 (797.26) | 1846.56 (749.70) | 1864.49 (873.09) | 0.001 | 0.001 | 0.120 | 0.799 |
Co‐morbidities, mean (SD) b | 1.67 (1.21) | 1.65 (1.20) | 1.72 (1.20) | 1.56 (1.23) | 0.391 | 0.434 | 0.434 | 0.434 |
NHANES wave, n (%) | ||||||||
2011‐2012 | 1130 (47.46) | 417 (49.35) | 541 (49.84) | 172 (38.53) | 0.030 | 0.883 | 0.066 | 0.031 |
2013‐2014 | 1305 (52.54) | 424 (50.65) | 588 (50.16) | 293 (61.47) | ||||
Global cognitive score, mean (SD) | 0.00 (0.79) | –0.05 (0.81) | –0.02 (0.77) | 0.14 (0.80) | 0.006 | 0.641 | 0.022 | 0.005 |
CERAD‐IR, mean (SD) | 19.84 (4.42) | 19.76 (4.42) | 19.60 (4.40) | 20.55 (4.41) | 0.003 | 0.553 | 0.026 | 0.002 |
CERAD‐DR, mean (SD) | 6.30 (2.27) | 6.31 (2.30) | 6.19 (2.20) | 6.53 (2.33) | 0.040 | 0.445 | 0.344 | 0.037 |
Animal Fluency, mean (SD) | 18.38 (5.67) | 17.77 (5.68) | 18.40 (5.56) | 19.34 (5.78) | 0.017 | 0.114 | 0.014 | 0.041 |
DSST, mean (SD) | 52.78 (16.43) | 51.47 (16.77) | 52.76 (16.05) | 55.05 (16.50) | 0.119 | 0.254 | 0.124 | 0.140 |
Abbreviations: %, weighted proportion.; BMI, body mass index (kg/m2); CERAD, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's disease; CERAD‐DR, delayed recall in CERAD trial; CERAD‐IR, immediate recall in CERAD trial; DSST, Digit Symbol substitution test; MeDi, Mediterranean Diet score; PIR, ratio of family income to poverty; SD, standard deviation; T1‐T3, the 1st to the 3rd tertiles.
P values of multiple comparisons were corrected by the False Discovery Rate method.
Total number of self‐reported co‐morbidities including hypertension, hyperlipidemia, diabetes, and cardiovascular disease (heart failure, coronary heart disease, angina pectoris, heart attack). There were 149 subjects with missing data on comorbidities.
The mean MeDi score in NHWs was similar to that in NHBs or Hispanics, but lower than in Others (Table S2). NHWs had higher global cognitive score than other racial/ethnic groups (Table S2). NHWs were more likely to have above high school education, be above poverty level, and be past smokers; were less likely to have depression compared to NHBs and Hispanics; and were more likely to have normal BMI and be married and cohabiting than NHBs (data not shown). NHWs consumed more fish, vegetables, cereal, and dairy but less legumes compared to Hispanics, consumed more vegetables, cereal, alcohol, and dairy but less meat compared to NHBs, and consumed more dairy and alcohol but less legumes compared to Others (data not shown).
Men had similar MeDi scores but had lower global cognitive score than women (Table S3). Compared to men, women had a lower percentage of being married or cohabiting with partners, and were less likely to be smokers but more likely to be below poverty level or to have depression (data not shown). Men consumed less fruits but more legumes, meat, and alcohol than women (data not shown).
3.2. Associations between MeDi and cognitive function
Compared with subjects in the first tertile (low adherence) of MeDi, those in the third tertiles (high adherence) (β = 0.162, 95% CIs = 0.050 to 0.275) had significantly higher global cognitive score (P trend = 0.006) in the adjusted model. An increase of 1 point in MeDi was significantly associated with an average increase of 0.039 in global cognitive score. Significantly positive associations were also observed for all individual cognitive test scores except for DSST (Table 2).
TABLE 2.
Associations between MeDi and cognitive scores
MeDi levels | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cognition test | Low (0 to 3) (n = 841) | Middle (4 to 5) (n = 1129) | High (6 to 9) (n = 465) | P trend b | Continuous MeDi |
Global cognition | |||||
Crude model | Reference | 0.027 (–0.088, 0.141) | 0.193 (0.041, 0.344) | 0.012 | 0.043 (0.011, 0.074) |
Adjusted model a | Reference | 0.058 (–0.015, 0.131) | 0.162 (0.050, 0.275) | 0.006 | 0.039 (0.016, 0.062) |
CERAD‐IR | |||||
Crude model | Reference | –0.151 (–0.666, 0.363) | 0.795 (0.152, 1.439) | 0.013 | 0.178 (0.032, 0.324) |
Adjusted model a | Reference | 0.062 (–0.292, 0.415) | 0.922 (0.386, 1.458) | 0.001 | 0.219 (0.102, 0.336) |
CERAD‐DR | |||||
Crude model | Reference | –0.113 (–0.410, 0.184) | 0.219 (−0.145, 0.582) | 0.200 | 0.051 (–0.033, 0.135) |
Adjusted model a | Reference | –0.002 (–0.237, 0.233) | 0.296 (0.015, 0.577) | 0.031 | 0.075 (0.004, 0.146) |
Animal fluency | |||||
Crude model | Reference | 0.632 (–0.160, 1.423) | 1.569 (0.519, 2.619) | 0.004 | 0.361 (0.155, 0.566) |
Adjusted model a | Reference | 0.750 (0.149, 1.351) | 1.078 (0.123, 2.033) | 0.035 | 0.276 (0.084, 0.468) |
DSST | |||||
Crude model | Reference | 1.294 (–0.974, 3.562) | 3.578 (0.151, 7.004) | 0.040 | 0.729 (–0.012, 1.470) |
Adjusted model a | Reference | 1.433 (–0.058, 2.923) | 1.982 (–0.233, 4.198) | 0.090 | 0.389 (−0.136, 0.914) |
Abbreviations: CERAD, consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer's disease; CERAD‐DR, delayed recall in CERAD trial; CERAD‐IR, immediate recall in CERAD trial; DSST, the Digit Symbol Substitution test; MeDi, Mediterranean Diet score; T1‐T3, the 1st to the 3rd tertiles.
The models were adjusted for age, gender, race, education, marital status, PIR, smoking, BMI category, physical activity, depression, dietary calories.
P values for the medians of each tertile of MeDi included in the linear regression models.
Values in bold text mean statistically significant (P < 0.05).
3.3. Effect modification analyses
The marginal effects of MeDi on the cognitive function varied across race/ethnicity and gender (Figure 2). Potential interaction effects were found between MeDi and race/ethnicity on global cognitive function (p interaction = 0.057), and in CERAD‐IR test (P interaction = 0.059). For both global cognitive function and CERAD‐IR, significant results were found only in NHWs—not in other racial/ethnic groups (Table 3)—and the difference in the MeDi‐cognition association was mainly between NHWs and NHBs (P interaction n = 0.055 for global score, and 0.085 for CERAD‐IR), and between NHWs and Hispanics (P interaction = 0.020 for global score, and 0.033 for CERAD‐IR).
FIGURE 2.
Marginal effects of MeDi on cognitive function by race/ethnicity and by gender. Predicted values of the scores of cognitive tests (y‐axis) were plotted against the MeDi (x‐axis) by race/ethnicity or gender based on the adjusted models. The product term between MeDi and race/ethnicity or between MeDi and gender was tested by the method of adjusted Wald test
TABLE 3.
Stratification analyses on the associations between MeDi and cognitive scores
Tertiles of MeDi | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cognitive score and stratification a | T1 (0 to 3) | T2 (4 to 5) | T3 (6 to 9) | P trend b | Continuous MeDi | P‐interaction c |
(n = 841) | (n = 1129) | (n = 465) | ||||
Global cognition | ||||||
By sex | – | |||||
Male | Reference | 0.088 (–0.026, 0.202) | 0.240 (0.079, 0.402) | 0.004 | 0.049 (0.017, 0.080) | – |
Female | Reference | 0.030 (–0.060, 0.120) | 0.091 (–0.034, 0.216) | 0.148 | 0.028 (–0.004, 0.060) | 0.239 |
By Race/ethnicity | 0.057 | |||||
Non‐Hispanic White | Reference | 0.072 (–0.015, 0.160) | 0.201 (0.065, 0.337) | 0.005 | 0.047 (0.020, 0.074) | – |
Non‐Hispanic Black | Reference | –0.027 (–0.151, 0.097) | –0.051 (–0.222, 0.121) | 0.554 | –0.005 (–0.046, 0.036) | 0.055 |
Hispanic | Reference | –0.004 (–0.107, 0.099) | –0.133 (–0.301, 0.036) | 0.131 | –0.016 (–0.058, 0.026) | 0.020 |
Others | Reference | 0.112 (–0.082, 0.307) | 0.162 (–0.106, 0.430) | 0.223 | 0.040 (–0.027, 0.106) | 0.882 |
CERAD‐IR | ||||||
By sex | ||||||
Male | Reference | 0.121 (–0.454, 0.696) | 1.191 (0.299, 2.082) | 0.006 | 0.258 (0.086, 0.429) | – |
Female | Reference | –0.029 (–0.604, 0.546) | 0.649 (0.054, 1.244) | 0.033 | 0.173 (0.008, 0.339) | 0.654 |
By Race/ethnicity | 0.059 | |||||
Non‐Hispanic White | Reference | 0.153 (–0.267, 0.574) | 1.117 (0.482, 1.752) | < 0.001 | 0.264 (0.124, 0.404) | – |
Non‐Hispanic Black | Reference | –0.604 (–1.517, 0.308) | –0.349 (–1.679, 0.981) | 0.620 | –0.053 (–0.334, 0.228) | 0.085 |
Hispanic | Reference | –0.385 (–1.077, 0.306) | –0.912 (–2.303, 0.480) | 0.176 | –0.137 (–0.422, 0.148) | 0.033 |
Others | Reference | 0.776 (–0.665, 2.217) | 1.455 (–0.097, 3.006) | 0.068 | 0.312 (–0.060, 0.684) | 0.613 |
CERAD‐DR | ||||||
By sex | ||||||
Male | Reference | –0.032 (–0.374, 0.311) | 0.330 (–0.130, 0.790) | 0.124 | 0.068 (–0.029, 0.164) | – |
Female | Reference | 0.000 (–0.341, 0.341) | 0.244 (–0.079, 0.567) | 0.134 | 0.078 (–0.014, 0.171) | 0.868 |
By Race/ethnicity | 0.233 | |||||
Non‐Hispanic White | Reference | 0.023 (–0.249, 0.295) | 0.385 (0.051, 0.719) | 0.021 | 0.095 (0.014, 0.177) | – |
Non‐Hispanic Black | Reference | –0.060 (–0.436, 0.316) | –0.112 (–0.598, 0.375) | 0.646 | –0.018 (–0.124, 0.087) | 0.439 |
Hispanic | Reference | –0.120 (–0.525, 0.285) | –0.625 (–1.156, –0.093) | 0.026 | –0.114 (–0.249, 0.021) | 0.053 |
Others | Reference | 0.143 (–0.322, 0.609) | 0.551 (–0.388, 1.489) | 0.249 | 0.156 (–0.110, 0.423) | 0.461 |
Animal Fluency | ||||||
By sex | ||||||
Male | Reference | 1.285 (0.335, 2.234) | 2.009 (0.594, 3.425) | 0.013 | 0.436 (0.132, 0.741) | – |
Female | Reference | 0.299 (–0.602, 1.200) | 0.259 (–0.893, 1.410) | 0.651 | 0.107 (–0.151, 0.365) | 0.082 |
By Race/ethnicity | 0.238 | |||||
Non‐Hispanic White | Reference | 0.790 (0.016, 1.564) | 1.285 (0.118, 2.453) | 0.037 | 0.318 (0.086, 0.549) | – |
Non‐Hispanic Black | Reference | 0.273 (–0.939, 1.486) | –0.421 (–2.009, 1.167) | 0.578 | –0.002 (–0.384, 0.380) | 0.104 |
Hispanic | Reference | 0.409 (–0.616, 1.435) | –0.167 (–1.644, 1.310) | 0.885 | 0.065 (–0.331, 0.461) | 0.089 |
Others | Reference | 1.831 (–0.630, 4.292) | 1.393 (–1.167, 3.954) | 0.267 | 0.227 (–0.353, 0.808) | 0.527 |
DSST | ||||||
By Sex | ||||||
Male | Reference | 1.825 (–0.794, 4.444) | 3.161 (0.155, 6.167) | 0.045 | 0.476 (–0.197, 1.149) | – |
Female | Reference | 1.227 (–0.621, 3.075) | 1.053 (–1.429, 3.535) | 0.395 | 0.315 (–0.338, 0.968) | 0.249 |
By Race/ethnicity | 0.472 | |||||
Non‐Hispanic White | Reference | 1.738 (–0.013, 3.488) | 2.561 (–0.137, 5.260) | 0.070 | 0.491 (–0.140, 1.122) | – |
Non‐Hispanic Black | Reference | 0.124 (–1.513, 1.761) | –0.005 (–3.287, 3.276) | 0.995 | –0.003 (–0.751, 0.745) | 0.107 |
Hispanic | Reference | 0.852 (–1.330, 3.033) | –0.332 (–4.543, 3.879) | 0.919 | 0.089 (–0.742, 0.921) | 0.600 |
Others | Reference | –1.851 (–7.093, 3.391) | –2.804 (–9.853, 4.246) | 0.421 | –0.343 (–1.896, 1.210) | 0.333 |
Abbreviations: MeDi, Mediterranean Diet score; T1‐T3, the 1st to the 3rd tertiles.
Stratification analyses were conducted when the P values of product term between Medi (continuous score) and each cognitive function were <0.10. The models were adjusted for age, gender, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, PIR, smoking, BMI category, physical activity, depression, and dietary calories, except for gender or race/ethnicity when it was under stratification.
P values for the medians of each tertile of MeDi included in the linear regression models.
A product term between tertiles of MeDi and race/sex were included in the adjusted models.
Values in bold text mean statistically significant (P < 0.05 for MeDi, P < 0.10 for interaction between MeDi and race/sex).
Potential interaction effects were identified between MeDi and gender in the AF test (P interaction = 0.082). MeDi was significantly associated with increased score of the AF test in men, but not in women (Table 3).
3.4. Sensitivity analyses
When excluding subjects with difficulties in thinking or remembering (Table S4), the associations were attenuated slightly, but remained significant for global score, CERAD‐IR, and AF in the adjusted models. The results remained almost unchanged when additionally adjusting for the self‐reported co‐morbidities (Table S5). We found similar results using alternative healthy dietary pattern HEI‐2015, with each 1 unit increase in HEI‐2015 associated with global cognition (β [95% CI] = 0.003 [0.000‐0.006]) and AF (0.025 [0.004‐0.046]). We found potential interactions between HEI‐2015 and sex for CERAD‐DR (P interaction = 0.080), between HEI‐2015 and race/ethnicity for AF (P interaction = 0.025) and global cognition (P interaction = 0.041). Among the food components of MeDi, higher consumption of fish (β = 0.20, 95% CI = 0.12‐0.29, P < 0.0001) and vegetables (β = 0.10, 95% CI = 0.02‐0.17, P = 0.015), and moderate consumption of alcohol (β = 0.09, 95% CI = 0.006‐0.18, P = 0.036) were associated with higher global cognition. The stratified results in sensitivity analyses were similar to the main analyses (data not shown).
4. DISCUSSION
In the current cross‐sectional study based on data from NHANES 2011‐2014, we found that high adherence to MeDi was associated with better cognition in older adults, age 60 years and older. Furthermore, the positive association between MeDi and cognition seemed to be dependent on race/ethnicity and gender, with a stronger association in men than in women, and in NHWs than in other racial/ethnic groups. Our results confirmed a previous report on adherence to a Mediterranean‐type diet and cognition in NHANES study population, 35 despite different ways in constructing MeDi score. 4 Our findings are also in line with previous reports on beneficial role of MeDi on cognition in population‐based studies, 3 , 4 , 5 , 6 but with a nationally representative U.S. population, are more generalizable.
We found a protective role of MeDi on cognition in NHWs but not in other race/ethnic groups, consistent with previous studies 31 , 36 on diet and cognition. It has been well documented that minorities are disproportionately burdened by cognitive impairment and risk of AD compared to NHWs. 30 Although the exact mechanisms are unclear, potential factors leading to such racial/ethnic disparities in cognition may include differences in socioeconomic status, early life adversities, educational attainment and quality of education, stress levels, perceived discrimination, and other psychosocial factors. 37 In addition to these distal factors, NHBs and Hispanics also have a higher prevalence of more proximal risk factors for AD, including cardiovascular risk factors, 38 heightened oxidative stress, 39 and higher inflammatory status. 39 Finally, dietary habit and quality can be affected by many of the distal factors and are also associated with downstream cardiovascular factors, and thus may play an important role in modifying or mediating the pathway from the contextual factors to cognition. 30 Our results suggest that following a healthy dietary pattern may have limited the effect to mitigate the overall heightened risk burden for cognitive impairment in minorities. Alternatively, as NHWs consumed more fish and vegetables (two key beneficial components), but less legumes than NHBs and Hispanics, the differences in the MeDi‐cognition association across racial/ethnic groups might be due to the subtle difference in how a high MeDi score was comprised. Improving neighborhood food environment, eliminating food deserts, and promoting nutritional education may provide a unique venue for reducing racial/ethnic disparities in diet quality and subsequent health status. 40 Nevertheless, some studies have found MeDi 13 or other dietary factors 41 were associated with slower cognitive decline for NHB but not for NHW participants, and others did not find a significant difference across racial/ethnic groups in terms of MeDi‐cognition/AD association. 7 , 9 , 11 , 31 Previous prospective studies investigating the association between the MeDi and cognition have been in predominately white populations 14 , 15 , 20 , 22 , 23 , 25 and rarely in minorities. 8 Therefore, current evidence is insufficient to conclude whether MeDi offer benefits for cognitive performance or AD prevention differently across racial/ethnic groups. With the growing and aging Black and Hispanic population in the United States, further studies are urgently needed to improve our understanding of preventive/risk factors in minorities that may inform interventions. 42
We found a significant association between MeDi and AF, a measure of executive function, in men but not in women. Gender differences in eating habits, taste perception, and preference have been well documented. 27 , 28 It is possible that the differences in the MeDi‐cognition association between men and women in the current study might be due to the different ways of reaching a high MeDi score, that is, by high intake of fruits and low intake of meat in women and by high intake of legumes in men. Furthermore, multiple lines of evidence suggest that women had a higher risk of AD than men. 29 Our findings are in line with a Greek study, which found that adherence to MeDi was associated with a lower risk of cognitive impairment in men but an increased risk in women. 16 Of interest, among existing studies reporting null associations between MeDi and cognition, several of them are large‐scale studies including women participants only. 21 , 22 , 23 , 24 Animal studies also support that males tend to be more susceptible to dietary stimuli for physiological changes such as weight gain, insulin resistance, hyperglycemia, metabolic alterations, deficits of learning, and hippocampal synaptic plasticity. 43 Sex differences in multiple physiobiological parameters, such as vascular dysfunction, inflammation, hypertension, circulating triglycerides and homocysteine, and microbiome responses, in response to special diets have also been reported in humans, 44 , 45 , 46 and may be the mechanisms underlying the observed sexual dimorphism in MeDi‐cognition association. Along this line, a recent study found genetic polymorphisms (rs34331204), associated with neurofibrillary tangles only among males, was positively associated with hippocampal volume, executive function, and delayed onset in males to a larger extent than in females. 47 Nevertheless, contrasting results, with diet‐cognition stronger in women than in men, have also been reported in a few studies. 48 , 49 , 50 Moreover, several studies observed no evidence for effect modification by gender on the association between diet and cognition. 11 To date, the gender‐diet interaction on cognition has not been routinely tested in previous epidemiological studies. 9 , 10 , 12 , 14 , 17 , 18 , 19 In the current era of precision medicine, further understanding of gender differences in the potential dietary intervention on cognitive aging should be prioritized to develop individualized prevention and treatment for cognitive aging and dementia.
The outstanding advantage of the current study lies in the study population, a large nationally representative sample of multiethnic older adults in the United States. It serves as an ideal population to explore the racial/ethnic differences and gender differences in diet‐cognition association. Furthermore, standard and detailed information on many potential confounders such as socioeconomic factors and physical activity were available from the survey and were controlled for in our study, and the results remained significant. Dietary consumption was estimated using USDA's Dietary Intake Data System 24‐hour dietary recall, which by nature was able to capture a wide range of foods and dietary habits including race and culturally specific foods, and thus was particularly appropriate for this multiethnic population with diverse cultural backgrounds. Multiple sensitivity analyses were performed, and the results were robust.
Our study has some limitations. Due to the nature of cross‐sectional study design, the temporal relationship between diet and cognition cannot be confirmed. There could be selection bias, as only slightly over half of older adults responded to the cognitive survey invitation, and some subjects were excluded due to missing data. However, a non‐response bias analysis showed similar cognitive scores with and without weighting adjustments. 32 The 24‐hour dietary recall might not be the best measure of long‐term dietary exposures and might be subject to recall bias. Genetic information such as apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 status was not available in NHANES 2011‐2014 cycles. Cognitive measures in NHANES were chosen for ease of administration, availability, and use in other surveys, so it may not be comprehensive enough to capture the entire cognitive profile of a subject and not sufficient for diagnosing cognitive impairment or dementia.
5. CONCLUSIONS
Higher adherence to MeDi is associated with better cognitive function in the U.S. elderly population. The positive association between MeDi and cognition is stronger in NHWs and men, compared to other racial/ethnic groups and women, respectively. Prospective studies with a large sample of minority older adults and with both men and women are warranted to confirm the findings.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
All authors have nothing to disclose.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
J. Guo conducted the statistical analyses, and contributed to data interpretation and critical revision of the manuscript; A. J. Moshfegh contributed to data interpretation and the critical review of the manuscript; Y. Gu designed the study, contributed to data interpretation, supervised the study process, and drafted the manuscript; and all authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Supporting information
Supplemental materials
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We acknowledge the researchers, the staff, and the participants of NHANES for their valuable contribution. The study is supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants R01AG059013, R56AG060156, and R01AG061008.
Gu Y, Guo J, Moshfegh AJ. Race/ethnicity and gender modify the association between diet and cognition in U.S. older adults: National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2011‐2014. Alzheimer's Dement. 2021;7:e12128 10.1002/trc2.12128
REFERENCES
- 1. World Health Organization , Alzheimer's Disease International. Dementia: A Public Health Priority. 2015.
- 2. Kivipelto M, Mangialasche F, Snyder HM, et al. World‐Wide FINGERS Network: a global approach to risk reduction and prevention of dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16:1078‐1094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. van den Brink AC, Brouwer‐Brolsma EM, Berendsen AAM, van de Rest O. The Mediterranean, Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH), and Mediterranean‐DASH Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay (MIND) Diets Are Associated with Less Cognitive Decline and a Lower Risk of Alzheimer's Disease‐A Review. Adv Nutr. 2019;10:1040‐1065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Aridi YS, Walker JL, Wright ORL. The association between the Mediterranean dietary pattern and cognitive health: a systematic review. Nutrients. 2017;9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Loughrey DG, Lavecchia S, Brennan S, Lawlor BA, Kelly ME. The Impact of the mediterranean diet on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Adv Nutr. 2017;8:571‐586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Scarmeas N, Anastasiou CA, Yannakoulia M. Nutrition and prevention of cognitive impairment. Lancet Neurol. 2018;17:1006‐1015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Tang MX, Mayeux R, Luchsinger JA. Mediterranean diet and risk for Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol. 2006;59:912‐921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Ye X, Scott T, Gao X, Maras JE, Bakun PJ, Tucker KL. Mediterranean diet, healthy eating index 2005, and cognitive function in middle‐aged and older Puerto Rican adults. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2013;113:276‐281.e271‐273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Tsivgoulis G, Judd S, Letter AJ, et al. Adherence to a Mediterranean diet and risk of incident cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2013;80:1684‐1692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Wengreen H, Munger RG, Cutler A, et al. Prospective study of dietary approaches to stop hypertension‐ and mediterranean‐style dietary patterns and age‐related cognitive change: the cache county study on memory, health and aging. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;98:1263‐1271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Tangney CC, Kwasny MJ, Li H, Wilson RS, Evans DA, Morris MC. Adherence to a Mediterranean‐type dietary pattern and cognitive decline in a community population. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;93:601‐607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Gardener S, Gu Y, Rainey‐Smith SR, et al. Adherence to a Mediterranean diet and Alzheimer's disease risk in an Australian population. Transl Psychiatry. 2012;2:e164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Koyama A, Houston DK, Simonsick EM, et al. Association between the Mediterranean diet and cognitive decline in a biracial population. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2015;70:354‐359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. McEvoy CT, Guyer H, Langa KM, Yaffe K. Neuroprotective diets are associated with better cognitive function: the health and retirement study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2017;65:1857‐1862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Gardener SL, Rainey‐Smith SR, Barnes MB, et al. Dietary patterns and cognitive decline in an Australian study of ageing. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20:860‐866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Katsiardanis K, Diamantaras AA, Dessypris N, et al. Cognitive impairment and dietary habits among elders: the Velestino Study. J Med Food. 2013;16:343‐350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Anastasiou CA, Yannakoulia M, Kosmidis MH, et al. Mediterranean diet and cognitive health: initial results from the hellenic longitudinal investigation of ageing and Diet. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0182048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Tanaka T, Talegawkar SA, Jin Y, Colpo M, Ferrucci L, Bandinelli S. Adherence to a Mediterranean diet protects from cognitive decline in the Invecchiare in Chianti Study of Aging. Nutrients. 2018;10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Galbete C, Toledo E, Toledo JB, et al. Mediterranean diet and cognitive function: the SUN project. J Nutr Health Aging. 2015;19:305‐312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Tangney CC, Li H, Wang Y, et al. Relation of DASH‐ and Mediterranean‐like dietary patterns to cognitive decline in older persons. Neurology. 2014;83:1410‐1416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Samieri C, Grodstein F, Rosner BA, et al. Mediterranean diet and cognitive function in older age. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass). 2013;24:490‐499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Samieri C, Okereke OI, E ED, Grodstein F. Long‐term adherence to the Mediterranean diet is associated with overall cognitive status, but not cognitive decline, in women. J Nutr. 2013;143:493‐499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Haring B, Wu C, Mossavar‐Rahmani Y, et al. No Association between dietary patterns and risk for cognitive decline in older women with 9‐year follow‐up: data from the women's health initiative memory study. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2016;116:921‐930.e921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Vercambre MN, Grodstein F, Berr C, Kang JH. Mediterranean diet and cognitive decline in women with cardiovascular disease or risk factors. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2012;112:816‐823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Cherbuin N, Anstey KJ. The Mediterranean diet is not related to cognitive change in a large prospective investigation: the PATH Through Life study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;20:635‐639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Hiza HA, Casavale KO, Guenther PM, Davis CA. Diet quality of Americans differs by age, sex, race/ethnicity, income, and education level. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2013;113:297‐306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Painchaud Guérard G, Lemieux S, Doucet É, Pomerleau S, Provencher V. Influence of nutrition claims on appetite sensations according to sex, weight status, and Restrained Eating. J Obes. 2016;2016:9475476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Baker AH, Wardle J. Sex differences in fruit and vegetable intake in older adults. Appetite. 2003;40:269‐275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Podcasy JL, Epperson CN. Considering sex and gender in Alzheimer disease and other dementias. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2016;18:437‐446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Manly J, Mayeux R. Ethnic differences in dementia and Alzheimer's disease Critical Perspectives on Racial and Ethnic Differences in Health in Late Life. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (U.S.); 2004. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Gu Y, Nieves JW, Stern Y, Luchsinger JA, Scarmeas N. Food combination and Alzheimer's disease risk: a protective diet. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:699‐706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Brody DJ, Kramarow EA, Taylor CA, McGuire LC. Cognitive performance in adults aged 60 and over: national health and nutrition examination survey, 2011‐2014. Natl Health Stat Report. 2019;126:1‐23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Moshfegh AJ, Rhodes DG, Baer DJ, et al. The U.S. Department of agriculture automated multiple‐pass method reduces bias in the collection of energy intakes. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;88:324‐332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Li Y, Yu M, Zhang J. Statistical inference on health disparity indices for complex surveys. Am J Epidemiol. 2018;187:2460‐2469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Taylor MK, Mahnken JD, Sullivan DK. NHANES 2011‐2014 reveals cognition of us older adults may benefit from better adaptation to the mediterranean diet. Nutrients. 2020;12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Peeri NC, Egan KM, Chai W, Tao MH. Association of magnesium intake and vitamin D status with cognitive function in older adults: an analysis of US National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2011 to 2014. Eur J Nutr. 2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Zahodne LB, Manly JJ, Smith J, Seeman T, Lachman ME. Socioeconomic, health, and psychosocial mediators of racial disparities in cognition in early, middle, and late adulthood. Psychol Aging. 2017;32:118‐130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Pool LR, Ning H, Lloyd‐Jones DM, Allen NB. Trends in racial/ethnic disparities in cardiovascular health among U.S. adults from 1999‐2012. J Am Heart Assoc. 2017;6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Feairheller DL, Park JY, Sturgeon KM, et al. Racial differences in oxidative stress and inflammation: in vitro and in vivo. Clin Transl Sci. 2011;4:32‐37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Bower KM, Thorpe RJ Jr, Rohde C, Gaskin DJ. The intersection of neighborhood racial segregation, poverty, and urbanicity and its impact on food store availability in the United States. Prev Med. 2014;58:33‐39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Christensen K, Gleason CE, Mares JA. Dietary carotenoids and cognitive function among U.S. adults, NHANES 2011‐2014. Nutr Neurosci. 2020;23:554‐562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Babulal GM, Quiroz YT, Albensi BC, et al. Perspectives on ethnic and racial disparities in Alzheimer's disease and related dementias: update and areas of immediate need. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15:292‐312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Hwang LL, Wang CH, Li TL, et al. Sex differences in high‐fat diet‐induced obesity, metabolic alterations and learning, and synaptic plasticity deficits in mice. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2010;18:463‐469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Suzuki Y, Ikeda K, Sakuma K, et al. Association between yogurt consumption and intestinal microbiota in healthy young adults differs by host gender. Front Microbiol. 2017;8:847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Luo L, Yang M, Hao Q, Yue J, Dong B. Cross‐sectional study examining the association between metabolic syndrome and cognitive function among the oldest old. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2013;14:105‐108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Taylor LE, Ramirez LA, Musall JB, Sullivan JC. Tipping the scales: are females more at risk for obesity‐ and high‐fat diet‐induced hypertension and vascular dysfunction?. Br J Pharmacol. 2019;176:4226‐4242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Dumitrescu L, Barnes LL, Thambisetty M, et al. Sex differences in the genetic predictors of Alzheimer's pathology. Brain. 2019;142:2581‐2589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Chan R, Chan D, Woo J. A cross sectional study to examine the association between dietary patterns and cognitive impairment in older Chinese people in Hong Kong. J Nutr Health Aging. 2013;17:757‐765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Beydoun MA, Fanelli‐Kuczmarski MT, Kitner‐Triolo MH, et al. Dietary antioxidant intake and its association with cognitive function in an ethnically diverse sample of U.S. adults. Psychosom Med. 2015;77:68‐82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Hossain S, Beydoun MA, Weiss J, Kuczmarski MF, Evans MK, Zonderman AB. Longitudinal associations between dietary quality and Alzheimer's disease genetic risk on cognitive performance among African American adults. Br J Nutr. 2020:1‐38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supplemental materials